Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    1/16

    Shyness, Self-Confidence, and Social InteractionAuthor(s): Philip Manning and George RaySource: Social Psychology Quarterly, Vol. 56, No. 3 (Sep., 1993), pp. 178-192Published by: American Sociological AssociationStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2786777

    Accessed: 02/12/2009 14:04

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless

    you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you

    may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

    Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asa.

    Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed

    page of such transmission.

    JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of

    content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms

    of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    American Sociological Associationis collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to

    Social Psychology Quarterly.

    http://www.jstor.org

    http://www.jstor.org/stable/2786777?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asahttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asahttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/2786777?origin=JSTOR-pdf
  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    2/16

    Social Psychology Quarterly

    1993, Vol. 56, No. 3, 178-192

    Shyness, Self-Confidence,

    and

    Social Interaction*

    PHILIPMANNING

    GEORGE

    RAY

    Cleveland

    State University

    Thispaper describes

    the behavior of

    shy

    and

    self-confident

    ndividuals.

    Althoughthere

    is

    a

    literature

    concerning

    the causes and

    consequences of shyness

    and

    self-confidence, nobody

    has systematically investigated

    what such

    people actually do.

    This

    paper reportsfindings

    obtained in a

    laboratory

    about

    conversational

    strategies

    used

    by shy

    and

    self-confident

    men

    and

    women. Participants

    were

    chosen

    from introductory

    ourses at a midwestern

    university

    and were asked

    to

    complete

    the Personal

    Report of

    Communication

    Apprehension PRCA)

    questionnaire. Those with

    either

    high

    or

    low scores

    then

    were asked

    to

    participate in a

    laboratory experiment

    n which

    dyads got

    to know each

    other.

    These

    interactionswere

    audio- and

    videotaped. Throughconversation-analytic echniques,

    verbal

    patterns of both

    shy and self-confidentbehavior

    were

    identified.

    In this paperwe discuss

    the

    management

    of

    conversational encounters by

    both

    shy

    and

    self-confident people. Although

    there exists

    a

    large literature about the causes and conse-

    quences of shyness and self-confidence, very

    little is known about how such people conduct

    mundane encounters (for

    a

    recent overview,

    see

    Crozier 1990).

    Thus we

    focus

    here on the

    conversationalprocedures used by people to

    display either shyness or self-confidence.

    These proceduresmake shyness observable-

    and-reportable. as Garfinkel puts

    it

    (1984,

    p. 1). As

    a

    starting point for

    this

    study we

    examine the typical procedures used by

    unacquaintedpeople

    to

    get

    to

    know each

    other. We

    will

    suggest

    that

    shy

    and self-

    confident

    speakers modify

    these basic

    proce-

    dures, albeit

    in

    different ways.

    Maynard

    and Zimmerman

    (1984) con-

    ducted

    an

    importantexperiment designed to

    specify the procedures used by acquainted

    and unacquainted dyads to initiate topical

    talk. These authorsbelieve that unacquainted

    dyads

    must resolve three issues

    in

    order to

    introduce

    a

    new topic. The first issue is

    relevance, which suggests that

    a

    preferred

    *

    Address

    all

    correspondence

    to

    Philip Manning,

    Department of Sociology, Cleveland

    State

    University,

    Cleveland,

    OH 44115. An earlier version of this

    paper

    was presented at the annual meeting of the Speech

    CommunicationAssociation, held in Chicago in Novem-

    ber 1992. We are grateful to Robert Hopper,

    Bill

    Morgan,

    Sarah

    Matthews,

    and

    Wendy

    Leeds-Hurwitz or

    their critical

    readings

    of

    various drafts

    of this

    paper.

    We

    also

    wish

    to

    thank

    he

    three

    anonymous

    reviewers

    at

    SPQ

    for their

    insightful

    comments.

    Beverly

    Shankman

    helped

    considerably

    with

    data

    collection.

    Financialassistancefor

    this

    study

    was

    provided by

    the

    Department

    of

    Sociology

    at

    Cleveland State University.

    topic must be withheld until it

    can be

    fitted

    naturally nto

    a

    conversation.

    The new

    topic

    must answer the question

    Why that now?

    (Maynard and Zimmerman

    1984, p.

    301;

    Schegloff and Sacks

    1973). Second,

    topics

    introduced by

    unacquainted dyads must

    be

    sensitive to the

    unsaid,

    taken-for-granted

    backgroundknowledgeof the

    speakerand the

    hearer (also see Beach

    1983; Hopper 1981).

    Maynard and Zimmerman (1984, p. 302)

    suggest

    that

    this background

    knowledge

    must

    be handledcarefully so as to

    avoid sensitive

    areas. Hence these

    territoriesof the self

    (Goffman

    1971) require

    ritual

    care. The third

    issue to be resolved

    by unacquainted

    dyads

    is

    the

    display

    of

    an

    appropriate

    elationship

    in

    their talk.

    Both speaker and hearer wish

    to

    appear

    riendly

    but

    not

    overfriendly;although

    they do not want to

    appeardistant,neitherdo

    they want

    to

    be

    too self-exposed

    or too

    inquisitive(AltmanandTaylor 1973;also see

    Knappand

    Vangelisti 1992). Giddens

    (1990)

    suggests

    that

    friendship

    is the

    process

    of

    gradually

    revealing informationabout

    oneself

    to

    another person.

    This

    process

    of

    gradual

    self-exposure

    involves

    a

    very

    complicated

    procedure

    that must be

    enacted

    during

    the

    demands of

    unscripted,

    sequential,

    conversa-

    tional

    interaction.

    The

    participants

    n

    Maynard and Zimmer-

    man's

    experiment were

    44

    students recruited

    from a freshman

    sociology

    class.

    Although

    they

    were

    not

    a

    random

    sample,

    the

    conver-

    sational

    procedures

    they employed

    are

    likely

    to

    be

    typical

    of

    the

    procedures

    generally

    used

    to introduce

    topics

    in

    social

    interaction.

    Maynard

    and Zimmerman

    discovered that

    in

    an

    experimental

    setting, unacquainted

    dyads

    178

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    3/16

    SHYNESS AND SELF-CONFIDENCE 179

    usually generated topical talk either by

    drawing

    on their immediate environment

    ( setting talk ) or by

    the

    use

    of

    pretopical

    sequences. These pretopical sequences used

    question-answer pairs

    in which the

    speaker

    asked the hearer either to categorize himself

    or herself

    in

    some way, or

    to

    specify preferred

    activities (1984, p. 305). Maynard and

    Zimmermanargued

    that

    pretopical

    sequences

    often were extended

    in

    unacquainteddyads

    because both parties were searching for a

    topic that would not lead to the loss

    of face by

    either

    participant.

    In the

    experimentreported

    n this

    paper, we

    attempt o extend Maynardand

    Zimmerman's

    argument by showing

    that at least two

    psychological traits shyness and self-

    confidence-can be

    understood

    as manipula-

    tions of the

    basic

    interactional procedures

    used to initiate topical talk.

    DEFINITIONALROBLEMS

    Personalcharacteristics

    uch as shyness are

    hardto define, both for the lay person

    and

    for

    the social

    psychologist.

    In

    everyday

    life

    people can easily identify the meaning of a

    social exchange,

    even

    though

    they

    cannot

    explain

    this

    meaning

    in

    words. As

    in

    many

    other

    aspects

    of social

    life, people

    are skilled

    practitioners

    without

    being capable

    teachers

    of

    social interaction.Giddens (1984)

    refers to

    this situation as

    a

    distinction between prac-

    tical and discursive consciousness:

    the

    former refers to

    the

    ability

    to

    perform

    the

    mundane

    skills of

    everyday

    interaction, such

    as

    greetings

    and

    polite exchanges;

    the

    latter

    refers to the ability to describe these skills

    verbally. Typically we

    cannot

    give

    a

    discur-

    sive amount even of such

    apparently

    simple

    actions

    as ending telephone

    calls

    (see

    Sche-

    gloff andSacks 1973). In

    this

    paper

    we intend

    to translatepracticalknowledge

    about

    shyness

    and self-confidence

    into discursive knowl-

    edge.

    Shyness

    and self-confidence

    have been

    difficult to define

    in

    the

    literature.

    Philip

    Zimbardo,

    for

    example,

    acknowledged

    that

    his pioneering

    Stanford

    survey

    of

    shyness

    allowed each

    person

    to

    adopt

    his

    or her

    definition

    (1977, p. 13).

    Another

    prominent

    researcher, JonathanCheek,

    defines

    shyness

    as

    a

    temporary

    emotional

    reaction

    triggered

    by encountering

    new

    people

    and situations

    (1989, p. xv). Writing

    with

    Stephen

    Briggs

    in

    a

    recent volume devoted

    to

    shyness

    and

    embarrassment,

    Cheek

    refined this

    definition,

    stating

    that

    shyness

    is the

    tendency

    to

    feel

    tense, worried,

    or

    awkward

    during social

    interactions, especially

    with unfamiliarpeo-

    ple (Cheek and Briggs

    1990, p. 321). The

    authors also suggested that shyness is the

    opposite of social self-confidence

    (1990,

    p.

    322). Although these

    definitions are impor-

    tant

    preliminaryclassifications, they do

    not

    capturethe

    day-to-day interactional

    practices

    of shy and self-confident

    individuals.

    Further definitional problems result from

    the use

    of vocabulariesthat are

    distinctive to

    academic

    disciplines. Social

    psychologists,

    psychiatrists,

    and

    communication specialists

    have

    analyzed

    the

    problem

    of

    anxiety about

    social interaction. Social psychologists usu-

    ally

    employ

    the

    everyday term shyness.

    Psychiatrists, however, prefer the

    expression

    social phobia,

    a

    term that

    suggests an

    underlyingmedical problem.

    The Diagnostic

    and Statistical Manual

    (DSM III-R) lists

    various symptoms of

    this disorder. The

    critical

    element of

    the

    psychiatricdefinition is

    persistent ear of one

    or

    more

    situations (the

    social

    phobic situations)

    in

    which

    the

    person

    is exposed to possible scrutiny by others and

    fears

    that

    he or she

    may

    do

    somethingor

    act

    in a

    way

    that will be

    humiliating

    or

    embarrassing AmericanPsychiatric

    Associ-

    ation

    1987, p. 243).

    At

    present, several

    efforts are

    being

    made to treat social

    phobia

    pharmacologically. By

    contrast,

    scholars in

    cgmmunication

    studies

    prefer

    the

    term

    com-

    munication

    apprehension,

    which

    they

    define

    as

    an

    individual's level of fear or

    anxiety

    associated with real or

    anticipated

    communi-

    cation with another person or persons

    (McCroskey 1982, p. 137). Although

    these

    scholars'

    interestoften is linked to

    the

    anxiety

    felt

    by

    people

    before

    public appearances

    and

    speeches,

    it also includes

    communication

    in

    small

    groups

    and

    dyadic

    encounters.

    In this

    paper

    we

    avoid the

    use

    of

    social

    phobia,

    which

    suggests

    a

    medicalization of

    the

    problem.

    We

    prefer

    either the

    everyday

    expression

    shyness

    or

    communication

    appre-

    hension,

    the

    term

    favored

    by

    communication

    specialists.

    We use these two terms inter-

    changeably

    to

    refer to the

    tendency

    to feel

    tense or awkward

    during

    social

    interaction,

    especially

    with

    strangers.

    In

    this

    paper

    we

    are

    interested

    in the

    conversational

    procedures

    that

    accompany

    or

    possibly

    constitute

    this

    condition. Self-confidence often has been

    analyzed

    in

    term of

    communication

    predispo-

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    4/16

    180

    SOCIAL

    PSYCHOLOGYQUARTERLY

    sitions

    such as talkativeness

    and extroversion

    (Ray

    1986).

    We define

    self-confidence

    as

    calmness

    and assertiveness during

    social

    interaction;

    once again,

    we are interested

    in

    specifying

    the conversational strategies

    that

    self-confidentpersonspractice.

    RESEARCH

    ESIGN

    The

    quasi-experiment

    reported

    here

    used

    dyads

    with

    varying

    degrees

    of communication

    apprehension.

    Participants

    were

    chosen by

    using the Personal

    Report of

    Communication

    Apprehension

    (PRCA).

    This

    measure has

    been

    shown

    to be reliable

    and valid for

    identifying

    decidedly apprehensive

    and non-

    apprehensive individuals (McCroskey 1982;

    Richmond

    and

    McCroskey 1983).

    Theoreti-

    cally,

    scores on the PRCA

    can

    range

    from

    24

    to

    120. Data from

    10,000 participants

    ielded

    a mean score

    of 65.6

    with a standard

    deviation

    of

    14.1

    (McCroskey

    1982, p.

    168).

    In

    order

    to ensure samples

    of decidedly shy

    and self-confident

    participants,

    we decided

    to

    recruit

    ndividuals

    with scores either below

    45

    (self-confident

    persons)

    or above

    85

    (shy

    persons). The mean scores for the self-

    confident

    and the

    shy participants

    were 31

    and 95

    respectively.

    Over

    a six-month

    period

    we administered

    the

    PRCA to more

    than

    500 undergraduate

    students

    at a midwestern

    university.

    Having

    identified

    a cohort

    of

    shy

    and self-confident

    persons,

    we then recruited

    participants

    or

    a

    laboratory

    experiment.

    This experiment

    is

    a modified

    replication

    of the

    study reported

    by

    Maynard

    and Zimmerman(1984).

    As

    in

    their project, we wanted to record spontane-

    ous interaction between unacquainted

    per-

    sons. We

    videotaped

    and transcribed

    nterac-

    tions between

    20

    dyads:

    10

    apprehensive

    and

    10 self-confident.

    Because we considered

    gender

    to be a

    potentially

    confounding

    variable,

    we matched only same-sex

    dyads.

    Fifteen

    female and five

    male

    dyads

    took

    part

    in this

    study.

    All

    participants

    were

    between

    18 and

    24

    years

    old.

    The facilities for recording

    the interactions

    consisted

    of

    a

    small-group

    discussion room

    with a one-way mirror. The

    room contained

    two chairs at

    right

    angles

    to each other,

    with a

    small table between. On

    the table was

    a

    clearly

    visible

    microphone.

    The control room

    on the other

    side of the mirror contained

    a

    videotape recording

    camera (VHS)

    to which

    was connected

    the audio input

    from the

    microphone. The ensuing recordings were of

    high audio and video quality.

    Participantsagreed to report at appointed

    times; each went to a separatewaiting room.

    The participants then were led to the

    small-group room, and the basic procedure

    was explained to them. They were told to take

    about 10 minutes to get to know one

    another. Participants understood that their

    anonymity would be protectedand that they

    could withdraw from the experimentat any

    time. We offered to erase the tapes of any

    participantswho were uncomfortablewith the

    experiment nobody was). Participants igned

    an informed consent form and, in returnfor

    their involvement, received extra credit in an

    introductory ourse.

    In the

    Maynard and

    Zimmerman

    experi-

    ment, participants were led to believe that

    they

    were

    waiting

    to discuss a

    topic,

    the title

    of which was enclosed

    in an

    envelope

    on the

    table

    in

    front of them. Participantswere told

    that a few minutes were needed to set

    up

    equipment

    before the

    experiment

    and that

    they

    should

    get

    to know one another until

    everything

    was

    ready.

    After

    10 or 12

    minutes

    the researchers revealed that the envelope

    had been a ploy and that the experiment was

    now complete.

    In some of the

    pilot

    tests we followed this

    format,

    but after

    examining

    the

    transcripts

    we

    discovered

    that misinformationwas unneces-

    sary

    for this

    experiment

    and

    did

    not alter

    the

    natureof the interactions.Thereafterwe gave

    all

    participants

    a

    straightforward xplanation

    of the basic

    procedure

    and

    goals

    of the

    study.

    This

    step

    involved

    telling participants

    to

    spend a few minutes getting to know each

    other. We transcribed ach

    dyadic

    interaction

    using

    the

    conversation-analytic ranscription

    outlined

    in

    Jefferson

    (1984, pp. ix-xvi) (see

    appendix).

    RESEARCH

    INDINGS

    Shyness

    and self-confidence

    obviously

    are

    not absolute states to which

    people

    are

    assigned

    in

    a fixed and

    permanentway.

    For

    example,

    some

    people

    are

    shy

    on some

    occasions and not on

    others;

    others are

    shy

    most

    of the time. From our

    perspective,

    shyness

    describes

    conversationaland interac-

    tional

    strategies

    that individuals

    employ

    (hence

    the idea that

    people

    do

    shyness).

    As stated

    earlier, Maynard

    and

    Zimmerman

    believe

    that

    unacquainteddyads

    use

    setting

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    5/16

    SHYNESS

    AND

    SELF-CONFIDENCE

    181

    talk before moving into extended topical

    talk

    by the use of pretopical

    sequences (1984, p.

    314). They inferred from this point

    that

    by

    doing pre-topical sequences,

    these

    parties

    demonstratedan orientation to achieving a

    more intimate sharedness by ritually and

    mutually mapping

    each other's information

    preserve, therebyprovidingfor the possibility

    of common territories of

    self usable for

    generating topical

    talk

    (1984, p. 314).

    Our

    findings reveal that shyness can

    be

    under-

    stood

    as a

    manipulation

    of

    the

    procedures

    outlined by Maynardand Zimmerman.

    Our

    sample of self-confident participants argely

    displayed the procedures

    indicated

    by

    those

    authors.

    Analysis of the transcripts evealedthatthe

    shy participants'

    talk had

    certain

    general

    characteristics.The opening sequences were

    dominated

    by setting

    talk-that

    is,

    talk

    about

    the immediate

    environment.

    Setting

    talk

    typically

    was extensive.

    Pretopical sequences

    (usually consisting

    of

    question-and-answer

    pairs)

    also were

    used,

    but

    only rarely

    did

    they

    initiate

    a

    topic. Pretopical sequences

    often

    were replaced by new pretopical sequences,

    which typically also were rejected. After

    several

    rejections, participants

    often returned

    to setting

    talk. The

    linking

    of the

    turns

    of

    talk

    was

    very

    loose

    among

    the

    shy participants

    because

    they usually

    chose

    not

    to select the

    next speaker at each transition relevance

    place (TRP). Easy turn transition requires

    conversationalmaintenance throughback-

    channel feedback and other

    prompts.

    Mainte-

    nance

    of

    this

    kind,

    however

    was not

    generally

    evident

    in

    our

    transcripts. Shy participants

    could have initiated topical talk by drawing

    on their situated dentities

    as

    students,

    but

    this

    approach was conspicuous only by

    its

    ab-

    sence.

    The conversational nteraction

    of

    the self-

    confident

    participants

    was

    strikingly

    differ-

    ent. Instead of

    relying

    on extensive

    setting

    talk

    during opening sequences, they

    used

    a

    tripartitesequence

    of

    1)

    name

    exchange, 2)

    setting

    comment contained

    in a

    turn

    at

    talk,

    and

    3)

    a

    pretopical sequence.

    Self-confident

    participants developed almost all of the

    pretopicalsequences

    into

    topics.

    This

    finding

    suggests

    that

    topics

    are

    unfavored ;

    as a

    result, many

    more

    topics

    are available for

    discussion. These

    participants'

    turns

    at

    talk

    generally

    were

    connected

    tightly;

    often turns

    were latched (one speaker began

    as soon as

    the

    previous speakerstopped talking),

    and we

    observed

    noticeably more overlap.

    Self-

    confident

    participants often drew on

    their

    situated

    identities as students,

    normally by

    using pretopical sequences

    about the choice

    of

    courses and

    majors.

    Shy participantsgenerally used extensive

    setting talk,

    employed

    pretopical sequences

    that

    did not initiate topical

    talk, and left the

    floor

    open

    at TRPs

    by not

    specifying the next

    speaker.

    We

    noted

    exceptions,

    however;

    these involved the talk

    surrounding

    particu-

    lar topic. We observed

    that the procedure

    outlined above was

    suspended

    whenever

    a

    certain topic became

    available to

    a

    particular

    speaker.

    This

    finding leads us to

    suggest that

    shy participants

    have

    favored

    topics which

    they introduce whenever possible and then

    pursue

    enthusiasticallyindeed, overenthusi-

    astically.

    Opening Sequences

    One

    strategy

    used

    by

    both

    shy and

    self-confident

    participants

    was

    to

    use

    setting

    talk.

    Participants

    talked either

    about the

    physical

    details of the room or

    about their

    reactionsto this environment.Because all of

    the conversations

    reported

    n

    this

    paper were

    recorded

    n

    a

    social

    psychology

    laboratory,

    t

    is not

    surprising

    that

    all

    of the

    participants

    made

    at

    least

    passing reference to the

    artificiality

    of their immediate

    setting.

    This

    finding supports

    one of the

    findings

    of

    Maynard

    and

    Zimmerman,

    who

    quote

    the

    following exchange:

    (1)

    Sharon: Hi there

    Judy:

    This s

    no

    good

    Sharon: Thereare

    two-waymirrors,

    ne-way

    mirrors,

    whatever

    they're

    called

    (Maynard

    nd

    Zimmerman

    984, p.

    304).

    In this

    example

    Sharon's

    statement

    Hi

    there

    may

    be directed

    not

    at

    Judy

    but

    at the

    researchers

    who

    she

    assumes are

    standing

    on

    the other side of

    the

    one-way

    mirror.We

    say

    this because

    it is

    unlikely

    that

    Judy's

    comment, This is no good, is a reply to a

    standard

    greeting. Thus,

    Sharon's Hi there

    reminds us that these interactions

    are

    not

    strictly dyadic

    because at least

    during

    some

    periods,

    the

    participants

    acknowledge

    the

    presence

    of

    people

    who can

    overhear the

    conversation. This

    extract therefore

    gives

    provisional

    evidence

    that the

    researchersare

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    6/16

    182 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGYQUARTERLY

    not completely absent from the conversational

    and social interaction.Goffman (1981) makes

    a similar point when he suggests that social

    interaction ncludes both ratified and nonrati-

    fled

    participants.

    He states that we should

    consider the presence of nonratifiedpartici-

    pants (in this case, the researchers) to be

    typical:

    Now considerthat much talk takes place

    in

    the

    visual and aural range of persons who are not

    ratified participantsand whose access to the

    encounter, however minimal, is itself perceived

    by the official participants.

    These adventitious

    participants

    are

    bystanders.

    Their

    presence

    should be consideredthe rule, not the exception

    (Goffman 1981, p. 132).

    We discovered

    a

    tremendous

    variation in

    the use

    of

    setting

    talk

    by shy

    and

    by

    self-confident

    participants;

    he former use

    it

    extensively

    and

    the

    latter make

    only passing

    reference

    to the

    setting.

    On

    average,

    the

    shy

    participants

    devoted 13 turnsto

    setting

    talk

    at

    the

    beginning

    of the

    interaction.

    In

    addition,

    eight

    of the

    10

    shy dyads

    returned o

    setting

    talk

    at least once later in the conversation.

    Among the self-confident participants we

    observed only an average of five turns of

    setting talk,

    all of which were brief.

    Only

    one

    of the

    10

    self-confident

    dyads

    returned to

    setting

    talk later in the conversation.

    The

    following

    extract between two

    shy

    participants isplays an opening sequence that

    is dominated

    by setting

    talk:

    (2) (SP6, modified)

    01 A: ((nervous laughter))

    A

    microphone

    02 B:

    We're being tape recorded

    03 A: I

    know probably

    04 B:

    Huh

    05

    A:

    Okay

    06

    B: I

    guess they're going to observe how

    nervous

    we

    are

    07 ((laughs))

    08

    A: I

    know

    This

    extract exemplifies the reliance

    on

    setting

    talk

    by shy speakers.We

    notice

    that

    the

    participants who

    do not

    know

    each

    other)

    do not

    exchange

    either names

    or

    greetings,

    but insteadrely on short comments about the

    immediate

    setting. Participants

    craft their

    turns as minimal unit

    types.

    These

    speakers

    continued

    with

    setting

    talk for another five

    turns

    until

    the

    following exchange

    was made:

    (3) (SP6, modified)

    16 A:

    We should just

    sit

    here and

    not

    say

    anything

    17 B:

    ((laughs))No

    we'll

    be labeled

    shy.

    Y'know (.)

    18

    I dunno

    19

    ((laughs))

    20

    A:

    We'll start

    throwing

    around the

    chairs

    (.)

    we

    can

    21 pretend

    22

    fighting

    23

    (7)

    24 A:

    Hum:: .)

    Jeez(.)

    Elevenoh four.

    This

    extract shows both

    shy

    participants

    doing extensive

    setting

    talk.

    Speaker

    A

    is

    even

    willing

    to

    speculate

    on

    wild

    behavior

    such

    as

    throwing

    chairs

    around. After

    this

    utterance,

    B

    failed to

    respond,

    and a

    seven-second

    silence

    occurred.

    The

    setting

    talk ended only when Speaker A wistfully

    noted

    the

    time,

    Eleven

    oh four

    (line 24).

    A

    similar

    opening

    sequence

    occurred

    be-

    tween these two

    shy

    participants:

    (4)

    (SP4,

    modified)

    01

    A:

    Well there's a

    microphone

    .)

    in

    ront

    of the

    02 A:

    [glass

    window]

    03 B: [I didn't

    think

    I'd] get

    called

    I

    was

    like

    oh

    I'll

    04

    fill it

    ((the

    questionnaire))

    ut

    05

    (2)

    06 B: I

    guess

    08

    (8)

    09

    A: I

    wonderwhat

    would

    happenf

    we

    changed

    ur

    10

    seating

    11

    positions

    so our back

    was to the

    window

    12

    (2)

    As in

    (2),

    the

    participants

    do not

    introduce

    themselves, but instead comment on the

    setting.

    The turns

    are

    coupled

    only

    loosely

    and

    supply

    the

    minimum

    needed to

    secure

    sequential

    relevance. B's

    open-ended

    com-

    ment about

    filling

    out

    a

    questionnaire

    pro-

    duced a

    two-second silence in

    line 5.

    After

    she

    added

    I

    guess,

    another

    silence oc-

    curred,

    this

    one

    lasting eight

    seconds. As

    in

    (2), one

    of

    the

    speakersended a

    silence

    by

    speculating

    on

    the kinds of

    self-presentation

    they

    could

    practice

    ( I

    wonder what

    would

    happen f we changedourseatingpositionsso

    our back

    was

    to the

    window ).

    Shy participants

    ypically

    relied on

    setting

    talk at

    the

    beginning

    of

    conversations and

    subsequently

    returned o it

    whenever

    topical

    talk failed. For

    example,

    after

    a

    conversation

    between two

    shy

    participants

    turned to

    the

    question

    of financial aid

    for

    students,

    uncer-

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    7/16

    SHYNESS

    AND SELF-CONFIDENCE

    183

    tainty

    about students' rights

    was

    used as

    an

    opportunity

    o return

    o setting

    talk:

    (5) (SP6,modified)

    92

    B: Theonly

    thing hey

    cando is

    justget

    a student

    93 loanout

    94

    A: Yeah

    t's so

    stupid

    95 B:

    Yeah

    96 (6)

    97

    A: Theyshould

    make

    he

    parents ay

    98 (3)

    99

    B: Yeah

    100

    (1)

    101 A:

    When hey

    gonnacome

    back n?

    102 (8)

    103

    A: It's nice table

    huh

    In this extract, the turns at talk are

    separated

    by long

    silences,

    and assertions

    are

    followed

    by

    simple agreement

    tokens.

    The

    failure

    of this topic

    is solved by

    a return

    o

    setting

    talk. The

    eight-second

    silence

    in line

    102

    occurs

    because Speaker

    B

    has not

    answered

    a direct

    question:

    When

    are

    they

    gonna

    come

    back

    in?

    Perhaps

    she did not

    reply

    because

    she heard

    it as a rhetorical

    question

    and hence

    one not

    requiring

    an

    answer. In any case, the conversation is

    jeopardized

    by

    the

    length

    of the

    silence,

    and

    is sustained

    only by

    a return

    o

    setting

    talk.

    Maynard

    and Zimmerman

    note that

    setting

    talk is

    a false

    topic

    in the sense

    that

    typically

    it

    is

    exhausted

    and

    so can

    be

    used

    only

    as a

    stepping-stone

    o other

    topics (1984,

    p.

    306).

    As we

    will

    discuss

    in

    detail

    later in

    the

    paper,

    we

    believe

    that the shy participants

    used

    setting

    talk

    as

    a

    safe

    topic,

    compara-

    ble to discussions

    about

    the weather.

    By

    contrast, as we will see, self-confident

    participants

    iewed

    setting

    talk

    as a

    dead end

    to be avoided.

    We consider

    setting

    talk to be a

    conversationalequivalent

    of what Goffman

    (1971)

    calls free supplies.

    These

    are

    goods

    or information,

    such as

    a match

    for a

    cigarette

    or

    directions

    to the

    subway,

    that

    anybody

    can

    ask for and

    expect

    to receive.

    Both

    free

    supplies

    and

    setting

    talk

    have a neutral and

    inconsequential aspect

    that

    makes them

    ap-

    propriate

    for situations

    in which individuals

    wish

    to disclose

    as little as possible

    about

    themselves.

    In addition, setting

    talk

    prevents

    the

    speaker

    romappearing

    unduly inquisitive

    about

    the

    other

    person's

    information

    pre-

    serve

    (Goffman

    1971, p. 39).

    In all

    but

    one

    case,

    the

    self-confident

    participants

    xchanged

    names. It is

    interesting

    that this

    small ritual

    was

    used

    by

    almost

    all of

    the self-confident participantsbut only once

    by the shy participants. Among the self-

    confident, the general strategy then was to

    make a fleeting reference to the setting and

    then

    to

    introduce

    the

    first pretopical se-

    quence. It is instructive that all initial

    pretopical sequences

    were

    accepted by the

    second speaker, and

    each

    developed into a

    topic.

    A

    typical example

    is

    the following

    exchange:

    (7) (SCP4)

    01

    A:

    I'm Julian

    02 B: I'm

    James

    03 A:

    Nice to

    meet

    you James

    04

    B:

    Nice

    to

    meet you (1) ((looks at

    mirror)) en bucks

    05 says

    06 it's on

    right

    now

    [((laughs))]

    07 A: [((laughs))It's] a

    possibility

    08

    a

    very

    distinct

    very distinct (.) what

    what're yuh

    09 taking up here?

    10

    B:

    uh

    musical theory

    In

    (7), the setting

    talk

    consists only of the

    brief comment

    ten bucks

    says

    it's on

    right

    now,

    which

    B

    appended

    to

    his

    type-

    connected second pair part in line 4. Notice

    that

    although

    A

    replies

    to B's

    setting talk,

    he

    refuses

    to dwell on the

    matter. Instead

    he

    chooses to ask a

    categorization question-

    that

    is,

    a

    question

    which invites

    recipients

    to

    classify

    themselves

    with

    respect

    to

    such

    membership

    devices

    as

    academic

    major

    or

    year

    in school

    (Maynard

    and Zimmerman

    1984, p. 305; Sacks 1972, p. 32).

    In

    (7) the

    categorizationquestion

    occurs

    in

    lines

    8

    and

    9, when A asks what're yuh taking

    [studying] up here? Whereas the shy partic-

    ipantstalkedextensively about

    the

    setting,

    in

    (7)

    the

    participantsdispensed

    with

    setting

    talk

    very quickly

    and

    never returned

    to

    it.

    Also

    consider the

    following example:

    (8) (SCP8,modified)

    01 A: Can

    they hear

    us=

    02 B:

    =

    I dunno

    ((laughs))

    03 A:

    Oh::kay

    04 B: Okay (3)

    05 A: What'syour name again?

    06 B: Lisa

    07 A: That's

    my

    nickname.

    In

    (8),

    both

    participants

    make

    a

    brief

    reference

    to the

    setting,

    and

    then

    after

    a

    gap

    begin exchanging

    basic information

    about

    themselves-their

    names,

    choice

    of

    major,

    and so on.

    Although

    both

    participants

    knew

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    8/16

    184

    SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

    QUARTERLY

    that

    their conversation

    was being recorded,

    A

    asks B

    whether

    a

    recording

    is

    being

    made,

    and

    B claims not to know (see

    lines 1-2).

    This reply

    further minimizes

    setting talk.

    Consider

    the following

    three

    examples

    of a

    simple name exchange:

    (9) (SCP3)

    01 A: ((extends

    hand))

    02 B: Awright

    03 A: Bob=

    04

    B: =Mike

    (10) (SCP7)

    01 A: My name's Jeff

    02 B:

    I'm Joe

    (.)

    How

    ya

    doin?=

    03

    A:

    =

    all right good

    (11) (SCP2)

    01 A: I'm Susan (.) what's your

    name?

    02 B: Olena

    Notice

    in

    these

    three

    cases

    that the

    name

    exchanges

    are

    unremarkable

    nd that the

    turns

    closely

    follow

    each

    other, including

    the first

    two cases

    in

    which latching

    occurs

    (no

    hesitation between turns).

    One effect of this

    exchange

    is to

    put participants

    on

    at least

    a

    putative

    first-name

    basis,

    which

    increases the

    degreeof acquaintance nd familiarity. The

    absence

    of

    a

    name

    exchange

    among

    shy

    participants

    s

    a

    way

    of

    achieving

    an amount

    of interactionaldistance

    and

    unfamiliarity.

    Perhaps

    an

    interactional slot

    or

    window

    of

    opportunity

    or

    name

    exchanges

    is

    present

    at

    the

    beginning

    of a

    conversation;

    if

    this

    is

    missed,

    it

    is difficult

    to

    return

    o it later.

    PretopicalSequences

    The term pretopical sequence refers to a

    preliminary

    sequence

    that introduces

    or

    in-

    vites an extended segment

    of topical talk

    (Maynard

    and

    Zimmerman

    1984, p. 306).

    Its

    function

    is to establish that

    a

    potentialtopic

    is

    acceptable,

    interesting,

    and unthreatening

    o

    the other

    person. Maynard

    and Zimmerman

    suggest

    that

    a

    pretopical

    sequence

    has

    two

    components:

    1)

    a

    categorization

    or

    category-

    activity

    question

    and

    2)

    an answer to

    this

    question.

    If a

    pretopical

    sequence

    is

    to

    develop

    into

    a

    topical

    sequence,

    the

    respon-

    dent must both answer the

    question

    and

    display

    a

    willingness

    to

    develop

    that answer

    over

    a

    series

    of

    subsequent

    turns.

    If

    the

    respondent

    only

    answers the

    question,

    espe-

    cially

    if that answer is

    brief,

    then

    typically

    the

    first

    speaker

    discontinues

    that pretopical

    sequence

    and

    begins

    another.

    Thus, brief

    replies

    sometimes are

    a

    means of

    rejectinga

    potential

    topic,

    although they also

    may be

    regarded as

    continuers

    (Maynard and

    Zimmerman 1984, p.

    308). When brief

    replies

    are accompanied

    by return

    questions,

    the pretopical bid is being rejected. By

    contrast,

    a

    lengthy reply

    to a

    question

    is

    heard

    as an

    acceptance of the

    pretopical

    sequence.

    For example:

    (12)

    01

    Al: What's

    ourmajor?

    02

    A2: Um, well my

    major's hysicsbut

    I

    03

    haven't

    really

    taken

    a

    physicsclass

    yet

    04

    so

    I

    have a good

    chance to change it

    05 Al: I've

    heard that's a good

    major

    (Maynard nd Zimmerman984,

    p. 306).

    Here the

    reply

    is

    clearly more than

    simply

    an answer

    to the

    question

    What's

    your

    major? It is also an

    acceptance

    of

    the

    topic

    of

    choosing

    a

    degree

    at the

    university. The

    merger

    of

    answers with

    acceptances of

    pretopical

    sequences

    is

    part

    of

    doing

    self-

    confidence.

    By contrast,

    shyness

    is

    displayed,

    made

    observable-and-reportable, hrougha

    separation of answers and acceptances to

    pretopicalsequences. Our data

    revealed that

    shy

    participants

    had

    a

    strong preference

    for

    rejectingpretopical

    sequences.

    The

    following

    extract

    is

    typical

    of this

    separation

    between

    answer and

    topic rejection:

    (13) (SPI)

    46

    A: How

    long wereyou

    in

    the

    army?

    47

    B:

    Three

    years

    48

    A:

    Were

    you ((spoken

    quietly)) .) you

    didn'tgo to

    49 Saudidid

    you?

    50

    B: No::o

    .)

    butI did

    go

    to

    Panama

    51 A:

    Did you?How was

    it?

    52 B:

    Yeah hat

    was

    pretty

    ool.

    In

    (13),

    B answers all

    of A's

    questions

    very

    preciselyuntil asked

    about

    Saudi. Her

    first

    reply,

    Three

    years

    (line

    47), displays

    the

    procedure

    for

    answering

    a

    question

    while

    simultaneously

    ejecting

    the

    topic.

    Notice that

    Speaker

    A

    accepts

    B's

    right

    to

    reject

    the

    potential

    topic,

    but

    introduces

    a

    new

    pretopi-

    cal

    sequence by

    a

    stepwise

    transition

    (Jeffer-

    son

    1984).

    When

    B is

    asked about

    Saudi,

    she

    initiallyrejects

    this

    potentialtopic by

    answer-

    ing

    the

    question

    with a

    simple

    No::o.

    The

    elongation, however,

    indicated

    hesitation

    about

    rejecting

    this

    topic,

    and after a

    micropause

    she offered the related informa-

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    9/16

    SHYNESS

    AND

    SELF-CONFIDENCE

    185

    tion

    thatshe had

    been to Panama

    while in

    the

    army.

    Her reference

    to Panamawas

    likely

    to

    refer

    not simply

    to Panama

    but to

    Panama

    during the

    American

    invasion

    of the

    country.

    Speaker

    A

    interpreted

    B's

    comment,

    No::o

    (.) butI did go to Panama, as an acceptance

    of a pretopical

    equence.

    In line

    52,

    however,

    B appears to reject

    this

    topic by giving

    a

    brief,

    abstract,

    and

    formulaic

    summary

    of the

    experience.

    When we turn

    to the self-confident

    partici-

    pants,

    we

    see that

    pretopical

    bids normally

    are

    successful

    and

    that acceptances

    are

    immediate.Indeed,

    one of the reasons

    for

    the

    interactional

    uccess

    of self-confident

    partici-

    pants

    was their general

    willingness

    to discuss

    a wide variety of topics. The discussion of

    topics

    requires

    considerable

    collaborative

    work.

    This is achieved

    interactionally

    by the

    first speaker's

    making

    invitations that

    the

    second speakercan

    accept

    readily.

    The rapid

    transition

    rom

    pretopical

    to topical sequence

    is a component

    of self-confidence. For

    example:

    (14) (SCP1O)

    173 A: that's

    that's

    exactly

    what I

    did

    um I

    decidedI jus

    174

    really

    like English

    better (.) but

    (.)

    what ya

    175

    wanna do?

    =

    176

    B:

    =well

    actuallyhuh

    it's

    a

    hardthing

    to say

    I

    mean

    177

    I

    like

    channel twenty

    [five]

    178 A:

    [oh good]

    179 B:

    oh

    like

    I

    have an independent

    study

    there

    I

    mean

    I

    The

    topic

    of

    future

    career goals

    continues

    for some time, butthe criticalpointis that this

    topic

    was introduced through

    a

    pretopical

    sequence

    and was

    pursued

    smoothly by

    both

    speakers.

    Notice

    that

    A's

    question

    what

    ya

    wanna do?

    is answered immediately

    by a

    latched reply, indicating

    no resistance

    to the

    topic.

    Further,

    both

    speakers'

    turns subse-

    quently

    overlap,

    revealing

    active pursuit

    of

    the

    topic.

    Pretopical

    sequences

    were

    accepted

    by

    self-confident participantseven when their

    content

    appeared

    to

    be

    potentially

    damaging

    to

    one

    of

    the

    participants.

    In the

    following

    extract,

    for

    example,

    one of the

    speakers

    asks

    questions

    about

    the other's

    girlfriend,

    consid-

    ering

    this

    topic

    to be

    freely

    available:

    (15) (SCP7,

    modified))

    39

    A:

    So

    your girlfriend=

    40

    B:

    =yeah the girl

    I'm

    dating=

    41 A:

    =

    okay

    her-her

    roommate

    is

    a com-

    munications

    major=

    42

    B:

    =

    yeah she's

    a

    master's student

    43 (1)

    44 A:

    o::h really

    45 B: = yeah

    46

    A:

    okay [she's a]

    47 B: [what do

    you]

    do with

    tha::t=

    48 A:

    =

    I have

    not a clue

    This extractcontains many

    featuresthat are

    typical

    of

    the

    self-confident

    participants'

    alk.

    The standardpretopical sequence

    is evident.

    In

    line 48, Speaker

    A affiliates to B's

    rhetorical

    query

    about

    the relevance of

    getting

    a master's

    degree

    in

    communication.

    Further,

    in line 40, in reply to the comment So your

    girlfriend,

    Speaker

    B does not

    wait to

    hear

    the

    question

    he is

    about

    to be

    asked;

    instead

    B

    acknowledges his willingness

    to

    discuss her,

    albeit

    by downgrading

    her status

    from

    girl-

    friend to the girl

    I'm

    dating.

    When it

    transpires

    hat

    Speaker

    A

    is

    actually

    nterested

    in the roommate

    of

    B's

    girlfriend,

    B

    latches

    his

    reply (line 42), showing

    his

    willingness

    to

    discuss

    her

    as

    well. At the end

    of

    this

    extract,

    both

    speakers

    are

    willing

    to

    comment that

    they do not see a vocationfor communication

    students;

    this

    finding

    is

    interesting

    because

    the

    comment

    could

    be

    viewed

    as

    potentially

    face-threatening.

    The

    Managementof

    Transition

    Relevance Places

    The term transition relevance

    place (TRP)

    derives

    from

    the seminal

    paper by Sacks,

    Schegloff, and Jefferson (1974). It refers to

    the

    structurally

    efined

    place during

    a conver-

    sation at which a

    chang-

    in

    speaker

    can

    occur.

    Hopper

    describes the TRP

    as the

    moment

    at

    which

    speakership

    s

    up for grabs

    (1992, p. 7). Although there are rules

    concerning

    the

    allocation

    of

    turns,

    turns at

    talk must be linked

    together duringthe flow

    of

    a

    conversation;

    hence this

    aspect

    of

    turntaking

    is

    achieved

    contingently. The

    success

    of

    TRPs is

    critical to

    the sequential

    organization of talk. Sacks et al. (1974)

    suggest

    the

    existence of three

    principal turn

    allocation

    options : 1) the speakerselects the

    next

    speaker, 2)

    the next

    speakerself-selects,

    and

    3)

    the

    current

    speaker continues (1984,

    pp. 703-706).

    We observed that

    shy partici-

    pants strongly preferred the third of these

    options;

    that

    is, they typically left the floor

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    10/16

    186

    SOCIAL

    PSYCHOLOGY

    QUARTERLY

    open

    and

    then continued

    speaking

    after

    a

    silence.

    This process is

    illustrated

    as

    follows

    by one pair

    of

    shy

    participants:

    (16) (SP6)

    43

    B: Yeah

    really

    44 (1)

    45

    B:

    Hmm::

    46 (2)

    47 B:

    That's

    a

    good

    question

    too.

    You

    know .) everybody

    48

    has different

    iews

    on

    it

    49

    (1)

    In this extract,

    B is producing

    urns

    that

    do

    not

    have

    an identifiable

    end,

    so he

    cannot

    signal

    to his

    partner

    that

    it is now

    the

    partner's

    turn to

    speak. Speaker

    B exercises

    the option of current speaker continuing

    across

    a

    series

    of

    completed

    unit

    types,

    none

    of

    which

    result

    in

    transfer

    of

    speakership.

    A

    similardifficulty

    occurs

    in the following:

    (17)

    (SP6)

    126

    A: yeah

    I have

    Rina

    Cathary .)

    She's

    really

    nice

    127 though

    128

    B: yeah

    (.) yeah

    It is certainly

    possible

    to

    hear

    the micro-

    pauseafter YeahI have RinaCathary as an

    invitation

    to the

    other

    person

    to

    speak.

    The

    additional

    comment,

    She's

    really

    nice

    though,

    also

    ends

    with the

    current

    speaker

    continuing

    at

    a

    TRP;though

    this

    TRP

    invites

    a

    reply,

    it

    does

    not

    require

    t,

    and thus

    creates

    the

    risk of

    an

    awkwardsilence.

    In the next example,

    the second speaker

    successfully

    negotiates

    the

    initial

    TRP,

    but

    then

    fails to connect

    her

    next comment

    back

    to the first speaker:

    (18) SP5)

    30

    B:

    We

    could

    discuss

    yourpin=

    31

    A:

    =

    ((laughs

    nervously))

    watch

    what

    yousay

    I made

    t

    32 ((laughs

    ervously))

    33

    (2)

    34

    B: Art?

    35

    A: Uh I'msorry

    The

    initial TRP

    linking

    the

    turns

    at lines 31

    and

    32 is

    managed

    through

    nervouslaughter.

    The laughter s latched to the comment We

    could

    discuss

    your

    pin.

    Up

    to

    this

    point,

    each

    TRP is

    accompanied

    by

    nervous laugh-

    ter.

    The second

    bout of nervous

    laughter,

    however,

    could

    not sustain

    an

    easy

    transition,

    and

    a

    two-second silence

    occurred.

    In line

    34,

    Speaker

    B then tried to

    encourage

    Speaker

    A

    to talk

    by uttering

    Art? as

    a

    question,

    but

    in

    a vague and abstract

    way.

    This was

    to no

    avail;

    a

    temporary,

    but

    nevertheless embar-

    rassing, silence followed.

    We suggest that when

    both speakers

    are

    shy, both

    tend to

    avoid

    exercising any

    of the

    speaker-selectionoptions at TRPs. The result

    is comparatively

    long

    and

    often

    awkward

    silences

    between

    turns.

    In contrast to the

    interactionbetween shy

    speakers,

    we

    observed

    that self-confident

    speakers

    do one of

    three

    things: 1) they

    negotiate

    turns by selecting

    the next

    speaker,

    2) they

    self-select, or 3) they respond

    quickly

    at

    silences.

    Consider

    this

    example:

    (19) (SCP4,

    modified)

    29 A: that'sall right.So what're outakin

    30

    this

    quarter?

    31

    B: Uhjeez two classes

    uh

    this

    oneand

    my saxophone

    32 class which

    s

    a

    littleahead

    well

    not

    this

    33

    interpersonalommunications

    33

    A:

    right

    dunnof

    you're

    n the

    same

    [class]

    34

    B:

    [yeah]

    35 B: yeah

    probably

    n a

    different lock

    This exchangecontainsone directquestion

    about

    what classes

    the

    other person

    is

    currentlytaking,

    and

    a

    second utterance

    ( I

    dunno

    if . .

    . )

    which is heard as

    a

    question.

    In

    both cases,

    the

    speaker

    selects

    the next

    speaker

    for the next

    turn. In

    the

    next

    example,

    we

    see

    both

    patterns

    exhibited:

    (20) (SCP3)

    177 A:

    I

    knowwhenya knowwhen

    I

    was

    178

    (.)

    eighteen

    .)

    I'm

    twenty-six

    now

    (.)

    when

    179 I waseighteen tooka yearoff

    180

    B: um hmm

    181

    A:

    partied

    my

    brains

    ut

    ya

    know

    182

    B: Um hmm

    183

    A:

    then

    started

    ack

    up (.)

    I

    I

    (.)

    didn't

    really

    184 find

    it

    difficult

    backthen but

    now

    I

    wouldya

    know

    185

    (.)

    ya

    know

    I

    lived on

    my

    own

    I

    worked ull

    time

    I

    186 went tuh

    school

    full time and

    my

    grades uffered

    187 because

    f

    it

    In this

    sequence, Speaker

    A

    makes several

    brief

    pauses

    that

    could have been taken

    as

    a

    TRP,

    but the

    potential

    for another turn

    was

    realized by the speakerexercising the

    cur-

    rent continues option. This example

    also

    shows

    Speaker

    B

    uttering back

    channel

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    11/16

    SHYNESS

    AND SELF-CONFIDENCE

    187

    responses, which

    serve

    as turn-yielding

    de-

    vices.

    The strategy

    used

    by shy participants

    produces both more silences and longer

    silences than does

    the

    strategy used

    by

    self-confident speakers. We also find evi-

    dence that shy speakers generally

    are reluc-

    tant

    to

    exercise

    the various

    speaker-selection

    options identified by

    Sacks et

    al.

    (1974);

    as

    a

    result, shy

    talk

    produces

    an

    organizedpattern

    of

    silences.

    Maynard 1980) suggested

    that the

    problem

    of speakertransitionmay

    be solved

    by

    the

    introduction of

    a

    new pretopical sequence.

    Shy participants

    in

    this study

    were

    more

    likely than self-confident participantsboth to

    have these problemsand to solve them in this

    way.

    The Favored Topic

    While

    analyzing

    the

    data,

    we noticed

    a

    consistent pattern: shy participants

    used

    setting talk extensively,

    then shifted to

    pretopical sequences that rarely,

    if

    ever,

    developed

    into

    topics.

    At

    breakdown

    points,

    such as silences following an unsuccessful

    TRP,

    a

    return o setting

    talk was

    common.

    As

    we collected more data, however, we noticed

    that a small

    subset

    of

    shy participants

    did not

    fit this

    pattern. Although

    extensive

    prelimi-

    nary setting

    talk

    was evident

    in

    the

    tran-

    scripts,

    in

    two cases

    we noticed

    that

    certain

    pretopical sequences

    were successful

    precur-

    sors

    to

    topical talk. Using

    a

    simplified

    version

    of

    analytic induction,

    we

    began

    to wonder

    whether our initial hypotheses were being

    disconfirmed or whether these exceptional

    cases hadanotherexplanation.As

    a

    result,we

    attempted o reformulateour initial hypothe-

    ses

    in

    order

    to

    accommodate hese

    discrepant

    eAamples.

    In one

    of

    these

    cases,

    the

    two

    shy

    participants egan

    in

    typical fashion, by using

    disconnected

    setting

    talk:

    (21) (SP2)

    01

    ((door loses))

    02

    (2.5)

    03 A: ptthisis weird=

    04 =

    I

    know

    t(hh)

    s::

    (hehhah)

    The

    two

    participants

    do not introduce

    themselves.

    Their conversation

    begins

    with

    a

    noticeable

    silence and

    then with

    setting

    talk.

    This

    continued for

    a considerable

    period.

    Eventually the

    first

    pretopical sequence

    was

    introduced:

    (22) (SP2)

    41

    A:

    What program are you in (.)

    where

    you majoring n '

    Because both participants were shy, we

    expected

    that

    the invitation o discuss possible

    majors would be declined by a short response

    and that another pretopical sequence would

    replace it. To our surprise, however, this

    pretopical sequence developed into an exten-

    sive discussion:

    (23) (SP2, modified)

    41 A: What

    program

    are

    you

    in

    (.) where

    you majoring

    n=

    42 B: =uh nursing

    43 A: Really

    =

    44 B: =Ye

    [ah]

    45 A: [are]

    46 A: you

    in the

    nursing program=

    47 B:

    =

    well I'm

    just gettin' I'm just

    getting

    into it I'm

    48

    still

    (.)

    I'm doin' the

    prerequisite

    classes before

    49

    I

    can y'know [actually get into n

    -]

    50 A: [what

    what

    else]

    51 do you need to do?

    The

    frequent overlaps

    and latched turns

    were quite unlike the typical conversations

    among shy participants.Silences at TRPs did

    not occur

    because both

    speakers

    were

    enthu-

    siastic about

    contributing

    o the conversation.

    The talk, one may argue, is

    indicative

    of

    the

    conversational

    style

    of

    very

    self-confident

    men

    and

    women. Indeed, both participants

    displayed

    an

    exaggerated

    commitment to

    the

    topic,

    over and above

    the

    expected require-

    ments for casual talk

    between strangers.

    It is

    as if

    too

    much

    personal identity

    can be

    detected

    in

    the

    sequential organization

    of

    these favored

    topics.

    The

    curious feature of

    this

    particular

    transcript

    was that

    the

    participants

    could

    continue

    in

    this manner

    only

    when

    talking

    about

    a

    certain

    topic-in

    this

    case, nursing.

    When

    other

    pretopical sequences

    were intro-

    duced,

    both

    participants

    reverted to

    a

    proce-

    dural

    pattern ypical of

    a

    shy person. At

    one

    point,

    for

    example,

    a

    participant

    evealed that

    previously

    she had

    attended another college

    (Tri C). This stepwise transition (Jefferson

    1984)

    in

    topics uncoupled the speakers'

    previously tightly linked turns

    at

    talk:

    (24) (SP2, modified)

    79 B:

    Oh

    you

    went to Tri

    C-

    80 A: Uhuh

    ((spoken very faintly))

    81

    (1.5)

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    12/16

    188 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGYQUARTERLY

    The introductionof Tri C as a

    potential topic

    disruptedthe

    conversationalflow. Both par-

    ticipantssuddenly

    were at

    a

    loss for words, as

    indicated by the silence

    following A's com-

    ment, Uhuh. The situation

    then was reme-

    died by a return to the previous topic,

    nursing:

    (25) (SP2)

    82

    B:

    So

    n

    do

    you

    have to

    go through

    all

    of

    the::

    (.)

    83

    classes

    ((prerequisites

    for nursing))

    over

    again

    84

    A: Not

    [the (.)

    four

    years]

    85

    B: [or some of the

    things]

    trans-

    ferred

    86

    A:

    they

    have a

    very

    different

    program

    In this extract

    we see that

    once the

    topic

    is

    understood

    to be related

    to

    nursing,

    both

    participants

    evertto

    swift turn

    transitionsand

    overlap.

    This finding suggests

    that

    shy

    participantshave

    a

    favored

    topic,

    which

    they are willing to discuss

    wheneverpossible.

    Therefore,to

    reach this topic, they

    will

    accept

    the invitation to

    speak

    offered

    in

    a

    pretopical

    sequence, despite

    their

    rejections

    of

    other

    pretopical sequences inviting

    them to discuss

    other topics. At the very least, the existence

    of

    favored

    topics

    shows that

    the

    shy partici-

    pants

    have only

    a narrow

    range

    of conversa-

    tionally acceptable

    interests.

    The Use of Situated

    Identities

    We

    noticed that self-confident

    participants

    tended

    to

    generate

    topical

    talk

    by drawing

    on

    their situated dentities.

    Alexander and

    Wiley

    (1981, p. 24) suggest that identities are

    situated by

    the relationship

    between the actor

    and

    the environment.

    In

    this

    study,

    the

    situated identity

    was

    clearly

    that

    of

    student.

    We

    observed

    that

    self-confident participants

    activated this identity very early

    in

    the

    conversation. By contrast,

    shy participants

    drew on

    their identity

    as

    students

    either

    very

    late or

    (in

    two

    cases)

    not

    at all.

    To

    obtain

    an

    accurate

    measure

    of

    this

    process, we

    needed

    a

    conversational index

    thatwould indicatethe use of the participants'

    situated dentity

    as

    student.

    We chose to look

    at

    the moment at

    which the

    question

    So what

    are you studying? occurred

    in

    the

    interac-

    tion. This

    question

    makes

    sense

    only

    in

    talk

    between

    two

    studentsbecause

    it

    draws on and

    assumes

    extensive

    knowledge

    about univer-

    sity

    life.

    Among the shy participants, the question

    usually was asked after extensive setting talk

    already had taken place. In the case of two

    dyads, the question So what are you

    studying?

    was not

    asked,

    so

    no provision

    was made for the relevance of those persons'

    student identities. This question appears to

    indicate

    an

    interactional failure by the shy

    participants, but, this interpretationmay be

    too limited. It is possible that the shy

    participants were willing to consider their

    situational identities not merely as students

    but also as experimental ubjects.

    This is a

    very radical view of situational identity;

    it

    may indicate that the shy participantswere

    relatively unwilling to relate on the basis of

    the more general category of fellow stu-

    dents.

    We found only one exception to this

    general

    observation about the

    shy partici-

    pants. On this occasion, the participants

    referred to their major very early

    in the

    conversation:

    (26) (SP3)

    10 B: so

    yer

    n

    UH

    (.)

    communicationsne

    eleven

    11 A: Ye::eah :eah

    12 (2)

    13 B: Hhmm

    .)

    What's

    yer major?

    This was

    very

    much the

    exception,

    how-

    ever. The other

    shy participantsmanaged

    to

    avoid this question

    until much later

    in

    the

    conversation.

    By contrast,

    the

    self-confident

    participants

    moved

    quickly

    to

    questions

    about

    classes, regarding

    hem

    as an

    obvious

    pretop-

    ical

    sequence:

    (27) (SCP5)

    13

    A: Do

    you go

    to CSU?

    14 B:

    Yeah

    (28) (SCP6)

    16 A:

    Okay(.)

    so

    you

    are

    in

    communica-

    tionsone eleven?

    (29) (SCP3)

    17 B: You

    go

    tuh schoolhere?

    (30) (SCP4)

    01 A: I'm

    Julian

    02 B: I'mJames

    03 A: Nice to

    meet

    you

    James

    04 B: Nice

    to

    meet

    you (1) ((looks

    at

    mirror))

    en bucks

    05

    says

    t's

    on

    right

    now

    [((laughs))]

    06

    A:

    [((laughs))It's]

    07

    possibility

    a

    very

    distinct

    very

    dis-

    tinct

    (.)

    what

    08 what're

    uh takingup

    here?

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    13/16

    SHYNESS

    AND SELF-CONFIDENCE

    189

    These examples

    show

    that for

    self-

    confident participants,

    a situated

    identity

    is a

    resource that is used

    to

    generate pretopical

    and then topical

    talk. We

    suggest

    that there

    is

    nothing special about the situated identity

    of

    student, and that it is reasonableto assume

    that self-confidentparticipants

    would drawon

    whatever situated identity was relevant

    for

    each particular

    onversationalexchange with

    an

    unacquaintedperson.

    DISCUSSION

    The findingsoutlined

    in

    this paper indicate

    the basic

    verbal

    procedures

    for

    both

    shyness

    and self-confidence. Shyness

    is

    achieved

    throughthe extensive display of setting talk,

    the recycling

    of

    rejected

    pretopicalsequences,

    the unwillingness to speak at TRPs, and

    the

    preference

    for

    and exaggerated commitment

    to a favored

    topic.

    Self-confidence

    is

    achieved

    through

    the

    minimization of

    setting

    talk,

    the

    exchange

    of

    names,

    the

    willingness

    to

    accept pretopical

    sequences,

    the

    willing-

    ness

    to speak at

    TRPs, and

    a

    display

    of

    neutralityregarding opic

    selection.

    Are these the

    symptoms

    of

    shyness and

    of

    self-confidence? (It

    is easier to think

    in

    terms of

    symptoms

    for

    the formerthan for

    the

    latter.) One

    can

    argue

    that these

    symptoms

    are

    described

    more

    accurately

    as

    procedures

    than as

    signs

    of

    an

    underlying psychological

    trait. Interactants do shyness and

    self-

    confidence (as

    observable features of their

    selves and interaction) through identifiable

    conversationalprocedures.It

    is

    possible

    that

    the

    symptoms

    of

    shyness and

    the state

    these symptoms indicate are one and the

    same. The existence of favored topics among

    shy participantsis strong evidence for this

    claim. These

    apparently

    anomalous pieces

    of

    data

    in

    fact are

    very revealing

    because

    they

    show

    that

    the

    hypothesis that shy people

    are

    inherently shy is probably empirically mis-

    taken; nstead, shy people

    probablyare shy

    only

    at certain times

    and under certain

    circumstances. Therefore it is reasonable to

    assume that there is an

    interactionalmecha-

    nism for doing shyness, which can be either

    activated

    or

    deactivated

    hrough

    collab-

    orative efforts to

    maintaintopical talk.

    This

    approach

    has a

    range of potential

    clinical

    applications

    because it

    suggests

    that

    the

    verbal

    components

    of

    shyness and

    self-

    confidence may

    be

    teachable through a

    conversation-analyticvocabulary. The

    find-

    ings outlined

    here could become

    a

    useful

    addition to the role-playingsimulationsused

    presentlyto help shy people (see Cheek 1989;

    Hartmanand Cleland 1990;

    Zimbardo

    1977).

    In addition to the verbal components of

    shyness and self-confidence, the motivation

    for

    this

    behavior

    is

    an

    issue.

    Although

    this

    topic

    is

    beyond

    the

    scope

    of this

    paper,

    we

    would like to offer a speculation.

    All

    social

    interaction nvolves a display of

    trust in

    the

    routinization and predictability

    of

    everyday

    life

    (Giddens 1984; Manning 1992). Despite

    the apparent cynicism

    of

    human behavior,

    interaction in the

    social world

    is

    made

    possible by individuals' tactfulness towards

    the failures

    of

    others

    (Goffman 1959, espe-

    cially p. 225). This argument s supportedby

    Shepperdand

    Arkin's claim

    that

    the

    socially

    anxious individual's

    presentation

    of self is

    not

    characteristically

    directed towards

    eliciting

    social

    approval.

    Instead

    the

    pre-eminentgoal

    of

    the

    anxious

    individualordinarily

    s

    to

    avoid

    disapproval

    rather

    than

    to seek

    approval

    (1990, p. 287).

    It

    may

    be

    reasonable to

    assume

    that

    in

    order to practice

    the

    proce-

    dures which

    display self-confidence,

    one

    must have a large degree of trust

    in

    the

    predictability

    of

    everyday

    life.

    By contrast,

    the

    procedures

    for

    doing shyness may

    be

    appropriate

    or

    people

    who lack trust

    in

    the

    supportive aspect

    of social interaction. The

    initiation

    of

    topical talk

    entails

    some

    risk

    because

    discrediting

    or

    embarrassing

    nforma-

    tion about

    a

    participantmay

    be

    uncovered.

    Shy participants

    eemed

    to

    perceive

    this

    risk

    to

    be

    great,

    whereas

    self-confident

    partici-

    pants perceived

    the

    risks of

    topical

    talk

    to be

    small.

    Goffman's

    analysis

    of embarrassment

    up-

    ports

    this

    argument:

    embarrassment

    occurs

    whenever

    a projected

    self

    cannot

    be sus-

    tained. The fact

    that

    we are

    not

    constantly

    embarrassed

    s

    testimony

    to

    the

    strength

    of

    the ritual

    order and to the

    prevalence

    of

    tact.

    We

    routinely exaggerate

    our own

    importance

    in

    ways

    that could make loss

    of

    face a

    common

    problem.

    It

    is

    possible

    that

    shy

    people anticipate

    embarrassment and that

    their anticipationhas consequences for the

    sequential organization

    of their

    talk.

    When

    viewed

    in

    this

    way, the preference

    for

    setting

    talk and the

    rejection

    of

    pretopical sequences

    can

    be

    understoodas

    a

    reaction

    to

    the fear of

    losing face,

    of

    becoming embarrassedby

    a

    self-revelation

    in

    a

    still-undeveloped topical

    exchange.

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    14/16

    190

    SOCIAL

    PSYCHOLOGY

    QUARTERLY

    Brown and

    Levinson (1987)

    use

    Goffman's

    argument

    as the basis

    for a

    theory

    of

    politeness.

    They

    suggest

    that

    politeness

    is

    largely

    an attemptto avoid

    face-threatening

    acts (FTAs).

    In social interaction, partici-

    pantsact politely and do nothingthat could be

    interpreted

    s an

    FTA. This

    view

    is

    supported

    by

    Maynard

    and Zimmerman,

    who noted that

    autobiographical

    talk among unacquainted

    dyads

    is

    always

    solicited

    with

    propriety

    and

    that pretopical

    sequences

    are handled

    with

    sensitivity.

    Many

    of the procedures

    used

    by

    the shy participants

    could

    be

    interpreted

    as

    strategies

    designed

    to avoid possible

    disap-

    proval.

    Everyday talk is oriented towards agree-

    ment, politeness,

    and the protection

    of face.

    Exchanges

    are

    constrained

    by

    a

    preference

    system

    that encouragesparticipants

    o express

    agreement

    immediately

    and

    to defer

    and

    soften

    disagreement

    (Pomerantz

    1984).

    Self-

    confident dyads

    showed

    an immediate

    rust n

    the

    preference

    system

    of

    everyday

    talk,

    assuming

    that embarrassment

    was unlikely

    to

    occur.

    This

    trust

    was

    missing

    from

    the

    behavior of the shy participants. Self-

    confident

    participants

    were

    happy

    to allow

    autobiographical

    information

    to

    leak

    out

    slowly,

    without fear

    of

    losing

    face. For

    example:

    (31)

    (SCP4)

    126 B: Yeah (1) yeah

    (1) I

    was

    just

    at

    Severance

    Hall the

    127 other

    day it

    I

    was there

    with

    my

    friend she'd just

    128 come back from school in Washing-

    ton

    D.C.

    she

    goes

    129

    to the

    American

    Universityin

    Wash-

    ingtonD.C. yes

    130 and

    uh

    we

    were walkin

    around she

    wanted

    to

    go

    to

    131 the art

    museum we went

    there and

    they

    had some

    kind

    132 of 75th anniversary

    or something

    In

    this

    example,

    Speaker B

    takes

    an

    extended turn of talk in which he begins a

    narrative concerning an experience

    with

    a

    friend.

    This development

    suggests that

    the

    selection

    of

    differentprocedures

    or

    managing

    face-to-face interactionmay

    be motivatedby

    assumptions

    about

    the

    degree

    of trust and tact

    that can

    be

    credited

    safely

    to the interaction

    order

    (Goffman

    1983).

    CONCLUSIONS

    Our analysis suggests that identifiable

    verbal 'procedures exist, which produce or

    perhapsconstituteboth shy and self-confident

    behavior. In this paper we offer a skeletal

    account of these procedures,as observed

    in

    a

    social psychology laboratory.We show that

    conversationsby shy participantswere domi-

    nated initially by setting talk. There were

    many pretopical sequences, most

    of

    which

    did not develop into topics.

    Turn

    transitions

    usually were difficult, primarilybecause at

    each transitionrelevance place the floor was

    left

    open; as

    a

    result, it

    was uncertainwhich

    speaker

    should

    speak

    next.

    Overlaps

    were

    rare, as were latched turns. Yet althoughthis

    account of

    shy

    conversational

    nteractionwas

    true in

    general,

    it was not

    always

    true.

    This

    procedurewas not used whenever one

    or

    both

    speakers found themselves discussing what

    we call a

    favoredtopic.

    When

    such

    a

    topic

    was broached by

    a

    pretopical sequence,

    overlaps

    and latched turns were evident and

    setting

    talk

    disappeared.

    If the conversation

    drifted from

    the

    favored

    topic,

    either

    by

    a

    stepwise transition or by a topic shifter, the

    shy participants ypically

    reverted to

    setting

    talk.

    This

    finding

    leads

    us to

    the conclusion

    that the

    shy participants strongly preferred

    their

    favored

    topic.

    Among

    self-confident

    participants,

    a

    very

    different set

    of

    procedures

    was

    evident.

    Setting

    talk was

    minimized;

    the

    speakers

    began

    with a

    name

    exchange

    and

    then

    quickly

    introduced he first pretopical sequence. This

    was acceptedalmost invariably,regardlessof

    the proposed topic. This finding leads us to

    suggest

    that

    topics among

    self-confident

    participantsare generally unfavored. Top-

    ics often were chosen by activatinga situated

    identity-in

    this

    study,

    that

    of

    student.

    Self-confident

    participants

    also

    performed

    routine conversational maintenance

    work

    through

    back channel

    feedback,

    verbal

    prompts,

    and so oIn.

    These results

    support

    the

    general

    eth-

    nomethodological finding

    that

    social interac-

    tion can be understood as a series of

    interactional

    procedures. Perhaps, then,

    cre-

    ativity

    in

    social interaction

    is

    not

    explicitly

    about

    spontaneous

    or

    idiosyncraticbehavior,

    but instead

    is

    a matter

    of

    modifying

    and

    manipulating

    these well-known

    and exten-

    sively

    used

    procedures

    for

    achieving

    various

    presentations

    of

    self.

  • 7/21/2019 Syness,Self-confidence and Social Interaction

    15/16

    SHYNESS AND

    SELF-CONFIDENCE

    191

    APPENDIX

    Adapted Transcribing

    Conventions from

    Jefferson 1984)

    1.

    [ A

    left-hand bracket

    marks the initial

    point

    of

    overlap.

    A

    right-handbracket

    indicates the point where the overlap

    ends.

    2. (2)

    Numbers in bracketsindicate elapsed

    time, in seconds.

    3.

    okay::y

    Colons indicate the

    elongation

    of

    a

    word.

    4.

    YES

    Capital letters

    indicate

    stress, usually

    through ncreased volume.

    5.

    ((coughs))

    Double parentheses

    contain informa-

    tion

    that is hard to transcribe.They are

    also used to clarify an indexical

    expression.

    6.

    ( )

    Single parentheses

    ndicate that

    part

    of

    the transcripts missing.

    7. I-I The

    dash

    indicates

    the cutoff of a prior

    word.

    8.

    No

    way

    Words are italicized to indicate em-

    phasis.

    REFERENCES

    Alexander,

    C.N.

    and M.G. Wiley. 1981.

    Situated

    Activity and Identity

    Formation. Pp.

    269-89 in

    Social Psychology:

    Sociological Perspectives,

    edited

    by M. Rosenberg

    and R.H. Turner. New

    York: Basic

    Books.

    Altman,I. andD. Taylor. 1973. Social Penetration:The

    Development of Interpersonal

    Relationships. New

    York: Holt,

    Rinehartand Winston.

    American Psychiatric

    Association. 1987. Diagnostic

    and

    Statistical

    Manual

    of

    Mental

    Disorders (DSM III-R).

    Washington,

    DC: American Psychiatric

    Association.

    Beach,

    W. 1983. Background

    Understandingsand the

    Situated Accompl