115
Acquisiaions and Direction des acquisitions et BiMiographicServices Branch des setvices biMiographiques NOTICE AVIS Tiie quality of this microform is La qualite de cette microforme heavily dependent upon the depend grandement de la qualit6 quality of the original thesis de ta thhse soumise au submitted for microfilming. microfilmage. Nous avons tout Every effort has been made to fait pour assurer une qualit6 - ensure the highest quality of sup6rieure de reproduction. reproduction possible. If pages are missing, contact the S'il manque des pages, veuillez university which granted the communiquer avec I'universite degree. qui a confer6 le grade. Some pages may have indistinct print especially if the original pages were typed with a poor typewriter ribbon or if the university sent us an inferior photocopy. Reproduction in full or in part of this microform is governed by the Canadian Copyright Act, R.S.C. 1970, c. C-30, and subsequent amendments. La qualit6 d'impression de certaines pages peut laisser a desirer, surtout si les pages originales ont ete dactyiographi6es a I'aide d'un ruban us6 ou si I'universite nous a fait parvenir une photocopie de qualit6 inferieure. La reproduction, mBme partielle, de cette microforme est soumise a la Loi canadienne sur le droit d'auteur, SRC 1970, c. C-30, et ses amendements subsequents.

Visual cover and site selection by mule deer · 2018. 8. 25. · iii Mty cover has 3 functional components: auditory, olfactory, and visual cover. In this study, I examined visual

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Acquisiaions and Direction des acquisitions et BiMiographicServices Branch des setvices biMiographiques

NOTICE AVIS

Tiie quality of this microform is La qualite de cette microforme heavily dependent upon the depend grandement de la qualit6 quality of the original thesis de ta thhse soumise au submitted for microfilming. microfilmage. Nous avons tout Every effort has been made to fait pour assurer une qualit6 -

ensure the highest quality of sup6rieure de reproduction. reproduction possible.

If pages are missing, contact the S'il manque des pages, veuillez university which granted the communiquer avec I'universite degree. qui a confer6 le grade.

Some pages may have indistinct print especially if the original pages were typed with a poor typewriter ribbon or if the university sent us an inferior photocopy.

Reproduction in full or in part of this microform is governed by the Canadian Copyright Act, R.S.C. 1970, c. C-30, and subsequent amendments.

La qualit6 d'impression de certaines pages peut laisser a desirer, surtout si les pages originales ont ete dactyiographi6es a I'aide d'un ruban us6 ou si I'universite nous a fait parvenir une photocopie de qualit6 inferieure.

La reproduction, mBme partielle, de cette microforme est soumise a la Loi canadienne sur le droit d'auteur, SRC 1970, c. C-30, et ses amendements subsequents.

VISUAL COVER AND SITE SELECTDN BY MULE DEER

Ann H, Rahme

B. Sc. (W.), University of British Columbia, 1985

THESIS SUBMfITED IN PAR1IAL mJLmLLMENT OF

THE REQUlREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

MASTER OF SCIENCE

in the Department

of

Biological Sciences

0 Ann H. Rahme 1991

SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY

NOVEMBER 1991

All rights reserved. This work may not be reproduced in whole or in part, by photocopy

or other means, without permission of the author.

National Lrbrary 1+1 ,,,"a* BiWiheqw nationale du Canada

Acquisitions and Direction des acquisitians et Bibhgraphic Services Branch des sewices bibllographiques

3% Welsngton Sfreel 3%. rue Wethngt~n Omm. Onlam Oaawa (Ontaro) KIA ON4 KIA ON4

The author has granted an irrevocable non-exclusive licence allowing the National Library of Canada to reproduce, loan, distribute or sell copies of his/her thesis by any means and in any form or format, making this thesis available to interested persons.

L'auteur a accorde une licence irrevocable et non exclusive permettant a la Bibliothbque nationale du Canada de reproduire, priiter, distribuer ou vendre des copies de sa these de quelque maniere et sous quelque forme que ce soit pour mettre des exemplaires de cette these 5 la disposition des person nes interessees.

The author retains ownership of L'auteur conserve la propriete du the copyright in his/her thesis. droit d'auteur qui protege sa Neither the thesis nor substantial th8se. Ni la t h k e ni des extraits extracts from it may be printed or substantiels de celfe-ci ne otherwise reproduced without doivent &re imprimes ou &/her permission. autrement reproduits sans son

autorisation.

ISBN 0-315-76231-5

Wame :

Degree :

ANH HgLEH RAHME

Master of science

T i t l e of Thesis:

VISUAL COVER I W D SITE SELECTIONS BY MULE DEER

Examining Committee:

Chairman: Dr. R. C. Brooke

Dr. A.S. hbept*

- ~hacMeton, Associate Professor, imal Sciences, UBC, Vancouver, B.C.

- . D~.'M-L. Winston, Professor, Dept . Biologic,al Sciepces , SF'U

- - Dr. MID. Pitt, Associate Professor, Plant Science, Faculty of Agric, Sciences UBC, Vancouver, B. C, Public Examiner

I hereby grant to Simon Fraser University the rlght to lend

my thesis, proJect or extended essay-(the Xtle of whlch i s shown be low)

to users of the S imoh Frarer Unlversl ty 11 br&y, and to make part la t or

single coples only for such users or In response to a request from t h e

l ibrary of any other university, or other educational Fnstltutlon, on

i t s own behalf or for one of Its users. I further agree that permission for multfpfe copying of thls work for scholarly purposes may be gran+ed

by me or the Dean of Graduate Studles. I t Is understood that.copy1ng

or publIcat1on of this uork for ffnanclal galn shall not be allowed

wlthout my w r l tten perm~ssfon,

Tltfs of Thssls/Project/Extended Essay

Visual Cqver and S i t e Selection b y Mule Deer

Alfihor: . - [signature)

A n n 3. R a h m e

(name 1

N o v . 1 3 , 1991

(date)

iii

M t y cover has 3 functional components: auditory, olfactory, and

visual cover. In this study, I examined visual cover, as weU as crown closure and

other characteristics of sites used by Rocky Mountain mule deer (Odocoileus

Iremionus hemionus). I d e s c r i i the characteristics of visual cover and

evaluated the definition of adequate visual over used in ungulate habitat

studies. Visual cover reqrriremerrts for any species have not been well-defined,

but wildlife biologists assume that visual cover is adequate when an average of

10% of an adult ungulate is visible at a distance of 61 m or less. I found that 61 m

was too long of a sight distance to be used to describe quality of visual cover

available in different habitats, Also, 61 m was significantly longer than the

minimum distance at which 10% of a deer model was visible at sites used by

does. Thermal cover has been assumed to be adequate when crown closure

equals or exceeds 70% and tree height is at least 16 m. The mean crown closure at

sites used by does was 26.8% + 3.0 SE and was significantly less than 70%. The

ecrological validity of these assumptions about visual and thermal cover is

questioned a d discussed.

I measured visual cover w i d a profile board, a profile pole, and a deer

model, evaluated these apparatuses, and used them to desm'be characteristics of

visual cover for different habitats. I propose procedures that best discriminate

differences in visual cover among habitats. These procedures were used to

describe visual cover characteristics of 7 hiibitats. Unspaced sapling forests

provided the densest visual cover while open habitats provided the sparsest

v h d cover. Shrub, spaced sapling, immature forest and mature forest habitats

were intermediate in density of visual cover. The important vegetation

compents of visual aver were: crown closure, percent over of conifer trees

i v

in the top height strata, percent cover of amifas in the shrub layer, total percent

cover of non-conifers in the shrub layer, percent cover of deciduous trees in the

middle height strata, diameter (dbh) of deciduous trees of the middle height

strata, and percent cover of deciduous trees in the bottom height strata. Previous

modeis of visual cover used only characteristics of the coniferous overstory and

ignored the contribution of deciduous and under st or)^ vegetation to visual cover.

My results suggest that these omissions should be rectified.

I compared habitat characteristics at sites used by does to those at sites

randomly located within doe home ranges. I predicted that does used sites with

denser visual cover after parturition (to maximize survival of their fawns and

themselves) than that available at randomly located sites within their home

ranges, or than that at sites used before parturition. I examined visual cover,

crown dosure, and food and water resources at these sites to determine the

relative importance of these resources in site selection. Although climatic

conditions at the time of location were recorded, I was unable to determine

whether does required thermal cover. Availability of forb and minimum

distance to roads of sites used by does did not influence site selection, and there

was no apparent trade-off between either visual cover or crown closure and

percent cover of forbs. Visual cover and distance to water of sites influenced site

selection, but site selection behaviour differed significantly among does.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Financial support for the thesis project was provided by an NSERC grant

to Dr. A. S. Harestad, B-C. Ministry of Environment (Dave Jones and Bill Harper),

B.C. Ministry of Forests (Brian Nyberg), and B.C. Ministry of Transportation and

Highways (indirectly through Keith Sipson's contract). Personal funding was

provided by my parents, teaching assistantships, S.F.U.'s work study program, a

graduate bursary, stdent. loans, B.C.'s Chdenge program, and Dr. Harestad's

NSERC grant. The opportunity to study mule deer in the Okanagan was

provided by Keith Simpson. Keith also provided unlimited logistic support.

Allison Haney, Maureen Connelly, and Odilia were capable field

assistants, not to mention all my Peachland vistors that I put to work. I thank

Barry and Moira Rondeau for renting their truck to me. f must also thank the

girls for permitting me to follow them although they evaded my predictions:

Gabby, Easy, Brenda, Gimp, Sweetie, Shrimp, Trigger, Rodeo, Cindy, Fart, Myr,

Last, Atf&a, Daze, Wait, and Blaster. Fred Hovey, Alton, and the Midas

manuals guided me through data analyses. Editorial comments were supplied by

Alton, Dr. Mark Winston, Dr. Dave Shackleton, Dr. Mike Pitt, and Rob

Houtman. Moral support was provided by Rob, friends, colleagues, email pals,

r oomtes , and family. Thankfully, Melissa also took 4 years to finish her M.A.

The transitory crew in Peachland was great fun to live and work with:

Keith Simpson, Keith Hebert, Colleen Hodgson, Mike Demarchi, Graeme the

Scot, Jodi and the kids, ZRS Gyug and Sabeena. The folks at Hatheurne Lake and

Tepee Lakes Fishing Resorts provided hot beverages on cold days and an amused

tolerance of us tramping around their resorts. I thank Douglas Lake Ranch and

Brenda Mines for rescuing me that snowy and slippery New Year's Eve.

TABLE OF CONTENTS

. . ........................................................................................................................ Approval .ii

*.. Abstract ....................................................................................................................... .m

Acknowledgements ................... ... ........................................................................... v . . ................................................................................................................. List of Tables v 1 1

... ............................................................................................................. List of Figures v i i i

...................................................................................... 1. General Introduction ..I

.......................................................................................................... Study Area .4

2 Measurement and Characteristics of Visual Cover ................................. ..6

.................................................................................... Methods and Materials .8

Results and Discus6ort ....... ,.. ................................................................... 15 ..................................................................................................... Conclusions .38

3. Influences of Visual Cover and Other Factors on Diurnal Site

Selection by Doe Mule Deer .......................................................................... 41

................................................................................... Methods and Materids 44

............................................................................................................. Results.. .47

...................................................................................................... Discussion 7 4

...................................................................................................... Conclusions 86

4. Conclusions and Management Recommendations ............................... -88

............................................................................................................ Literature Cited 91

UST OF TABLES

Characteristics of 7 habitat t y p s in Thompson Plateau study area 1988-89- For each habitat type, mean percent cover of 3 plant groups, mean percent cover of 3 strata of coniferous and deciduous trees, mean tree diameters (dbh), percent crown dosure and sample sizes are indicated ........................................................ 14

Means of 8 variables that describe deer model visibility in 7 types of habitats ................................................................................................. 30

Independent variables in my multiple linear regression model and their mefkknts when the dependent variables are the visibility scores of the fourth pole section at 5 m and 15 m sequentially ....................................................................................................... 3 6

Mean percent cover of forbs at sites used by does and random sites ...................................................................................................................... 52

ANOVA table of percent cover of forb at sites used before and after parturition ........-......-..t.......-w..-................................................... ............ 53

Mean distances to water (m) from sites used by does and random sites ...................................................................................................... 54

Mean percent visibility of the deer model at coyote height and at 15 m for sites used by does and random sites ....-.................................. 5 7

Mean minimum distance (m) at which only 10% of the deer model can be seen at coyote height for sites used by does and

.......................... random sites .............................. ........................................... 6 0

Three micro-climate parameters estimated for does on the Thompson Plateau ................-.-.. ,.., ............................................................. 67

Mean percent crown closure for sites used by does and random sites ................................................................................................................... 73

Mean distance to the nearest road (m) for sites used by does and random sites .................................................................................................... 7 5

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure p a g ~

Study area on the Thompson Plateau in south-central British Columbia. Radicxallared deer winter in the Okanagan Valley near Peachland but spend spring and summer on the

........................................................................................... Thompson Plateau .5

Relationship between visibility of the profile board and pole at the fourth section (0.75-1.00 m). Visibility scores at 3 sites (n = 48) were measured at my height (1.5 m), 4 distances (5 m, 15 m, minimum distance for 10% deer model visibaty at coyote height and at my height), and 4 directions (updope, downslope, right and left across slope) ........................................................................... 16

Section numbers of the 8 pole sections and proportions of the deer model potentially visr'ble at different 25-cm sections of the

........................ pole are provided. Both apparatuses are drawn to scale 18

Relationship between mean percent of the deer model visible at 15 m at my height (IS m) and that at coyote height (0.75 m) ................ 20

Mean percent visibility of the deer m d d at my height (1.5 m) and 2 distances (5 m and 15 m) and the mean minimum distance for 10% deer model visibility for each habitat ........................tan.. 22

Mean and stambrd error of minimum distances (m) for 10% visibility of the deer model measured at my height (15 m) for 7 types of habitats ..............~...~......~...................................................................... 24

Mean and standard error of differences between habitats in visibility scores of each pole section, pole halves (where B is the bottom 4 sections of the pole and T is the top 4 sections of the pole) and total pole at 15 m (where BT is d l 8 sections of the pole) .............. --.. ........................................................................ ...-.......... -25

Mean and standard error of differences between habitats in visibility scores of the fourth pole section at 15 m for 4 observer positions and for the mean of the visibility scores recorded at

............................................................. the 4 positions ......-,...,,.........,.... ,. 27

i x Mean visibility score of the fourth pole section (0.75-1.00 m) at 15 m for each habitat and time period ...U.............1...od.ododododod.odod.-od.od.od....od..ododod..od.od.od 29

Mean and standard error percent visibility of the deer model measured at 15 m and my height (1-5 m) for 5 successional stages of a sere ................................................................................................... 32

Mean visibility score of each pole section for 7 habitats ......................... 33

Relationship b e e n percent visibility of the deer model measured at 15 rn and coyote height (0.75 m) and percent cover of forb ............................................ ... ................................................................ -49

Relationship between percent mown closure and percent cover of fork. .............................................................................................................. -50

Differences in mean percent visibility of the deer model at 15 m and coyote height (0.75 m) for random sites and sites used by 14 does where deer identification is ranked in order of difference. A positive value for a difference indicates that the mean visibility for random sites is greater than that for sites used by that doe ................................................................................................ ............. 58

Differences in mean minimum distance (m) at which only 10% of the deer model can be seen at coyate height (0.75 m) for random sites and sites used by 14 does where deer identification is ranked in order of difference. A positive value for a difference indicates that the mean visibility for random sites is greater than that for sites used by that doe.. ............................................... 62

Differences in mean minimum distance (m) at which only 10% of the deer model can be seen at coyote height (0.75 m) for random sites and bedding sites used by 11 does where deer identification is ranked in order of difference. A positive value for a difference indicates that the mean visibility for random

.............. sites is greater than that for bedding sites used by that doe..--.- 63

Diffkrences in mean minimum distance (m) at which only 10% ~f the deer m& can be seen at coyote height (0.75 m) for bedding and nun-bedding sites used by 11 does where deer identification is ranked in order of difference. A positive value for a difference indicates that the mean visibility for non- bedding sites is greater than that for bedding sites used by that doe-.- ..-....-...-- ............. ..,..,, ........................................................... .................... 64

X 18 Mean visibility of each pole section at IS m and my height (1.5

m) for doe and random sites .................................................................... 66

19 Frequency distribution for values of wind speeds recorded in 1988 (n = 42) ...................................................................................................... .a

20 Frequency distribution for values of short-wave radiation recorded in 1989 (n = 31) ................................................................................. 69

21 Frequency distributions of air temperature (n = 81) and operative temperature estimates for 1988 (when short-wave radiation was assumed to be U H ) ~ / m 2 ) and for 1989 (when

........................................................ wind speed was assumed to be zero) -70

CHAPTER1

GENERAL INTRODUCTION

All animals require fad , water and potentially cover to sunrive. Of these

requirements, cover is the least understood and only a few studies have

examined cover as a factor in habitat selection. Cover is best defined in the

context of the functiolls it perf-. Animals use cover in several ways:

thermal cover moderates climatic conditions which reduces thennoregulatory

energy requirements, security cover (i.e. hiding cover) reduces the risk of

detection and of attack by predators, escape cover provides a means of escape

from predators once the animal is detected, insect cover provides refuge from

harassing insecis, and snow-interception cover alters the depth and density of the

snowpack. The different functional types of cover are not structurally exclusive

because physical attributes of habitats may perform the different functions (Taber

1961, Peek et aI. 1982). f was able to avoid confounding use of security cover with

use of snow-interception cover by limiting my field season to Iate spring and

summer, but it was nttcessary to address h use of thermal, insect and escape

cover*

Security cave has 3 functional components: auditory, olfactory, and

visual cover- Only the visual tmmponent of security cover has been examined to

date because only visual cover can be measured effectively. Structurally, security

cover consists of vegetation and non-vegetation physical barriers that obstruct

predators' saw~y peK*eptim of prey- The bamers can operate to affect detection

by any of the following interfering with scent-laden air currents (oIfactory

cover), absorbkg or deaecoing sounds made by prey (auditory cover), or

obstnrcting the view of prey by predators (visual cover). The functional

compnenb of security cover may not be structurally exclusive. It is unlikely

that visual cover is physically distinct from olfactory and auditory cover.

Physical barriers that obstruct one of a predator's senses could obstruct other

senses.

The visual cover requirements have not been identified or measured for

any species, but for elk (Cmus elaphus) and mule deer (Odocoileus hemionus)

visual cover is thought to be adequate when an average of 10% of an adult

efk/deer is visible at a distance of 61 m or less (Thomas et al. 1979). Thermal

cover is thought to be adequate when crown closure equals or exceeds 70% and

tree height is at least 16 m in a stand of conifers (Thomas et al. 1986). Although

both of these criteria are currently being used to manage forests for many species

of ungulates, neither has been tested for ecological validity. I examined visual

cover, crown closure, and other characteristics of habitats used by doe mule deer

and their fawns to investigate the characteristics and use of visual cover and to

evaluate the definition of adequate visual cover proposed by Thomas et al.

(1979).

I measured visual cover in several ways. In Chapter 2, I evaluate the

procedures used in measuring visual cover with a profile pole (and profile

board) and a deer model. I propose a set of procedures that best discriminate

among habitats by providing an effective way of detecting differences in visual

cover. I document the phenology of visual cover over late spring and summer,

describe visual cover characteristics for 7 habitat types, and identrfy the

mmponents of vegetation which contribute to visual cover.

I used a natural experiment to determine if doe mule deer use visual

cover (Chapter 3)- I predicted that does will use sites with denser visual cover

after parturition than that available at randomly located sites within their home

range or than that at sites usxi before parturition. Habitat characteristics at sites

used by does were compared to sites randomly located within doe home ranges. I

also compared sites used by does before parturition to those used after

parturition. I examined visual cover, crown closure, food and water resources at

these sites to determine which resources were important in site selection. The

micro-climate conditions at the time of location also were examined to assess the

need of thermal cover. In this way, I attempted to determine which resources

(food, water, visual cover and crown closure) were correlated with sites used by

does in summer.

STUDY AREA

My study was conducted on the Thompson Plateau (Fig. 1) in south-

central British Columbia (500 N, 1200 W). The plateau, with its rolling hills,

varies in elevation between 900 m and 1900 m. The Montane Spruce, Ponderosa

Pine, Engelmann Spruce-Subalpine Fir Biogeoclimatic Zones occur in this

area (B.C. Ministry of Forests 1988). The Montane Spruce Zone is the most

common zone and Engelmann spruce (Picea engelmnnii) and subalpine fir

(Abies lasiocarpa) are its climax conifer species. Because of fires, forest

harvesting and other disturbances much of the forest land in the study area is

not at the climax stage. Currently, the forests are dominated by lodgepole pine

(Pinus contorfa) with Engelmann spruce, subalpine fir, and rarely Douglas fir

(Pseudotsuga menziesii) as minor species. The landscape is interspersed with

lakes and sedge meadows, clear-cuts, roads, hydro-lines, the Brenda Mines open

pit mine, and the right-of-way for the new Okanagan Connector Highway.

The study site was chosen on the basis of the existing facilities. Keystone

Bio-Research was contracted by the B.C. Ministry of Transportatior. and

Highways in cooperation with the B.C. Ministry of Environment to conduct an

inventory of ungulates along the Okanagan Connector Highway. Their study

began in November 1986 and continues into 1992. Keystone Bio-Research had

15-30 adult male and female mule deer fitted with radio-collars. Deer were

trapped with baited Clover traps (Clover 1954) on their winter ranges during 4

consecutive winters (1986-87,1987-88,198&89,1989-90). Besides a radio-colJ.ar,

each deer was fifted with a unique pair of coloued ear tags. The deer use tht?

Thompson Plateau for their spring and summer ranges. This area has an

extensive mad system which facilitates relocation of deer by radio-telemetry.

Fig

ure

1. S

tudy

are

a on

the

Tho

mps

on P

late

au in

sou

th-c

entr

al B

ritis

h C

olum

bia.

Rad

io-c

olla

red

deer

win

ter

in t

he O

kana

gan

Val

ley

near

P

each

land

but

spe

nd s

prin

g an

d su

mm

er o

n th

e T

hom

pson

Pla

teau

(S

imps

on 1

988)

.

CHAPTER2

MEASUREMENT AND CHARACTERISTICS OF VISUAL COVER

Security cover consists of vegetation and non-vegetation physical barriers

that obstrud predators' sensory perception of prey. These barriers can operate to

affect detection by any of the following: interfering with scent-laden air currents

(olfactory cover), absorbing and deflecting sounds made by prey (auditory cover),

and by obstructing the view of prey by predators (visual cover). Although a

number of authors have claimed to have measured security cover (i.e. hiding

cover), they have measured only visual cover, which is the easiest of the 3 to

measure. However, it is probable that structural characteristics which determine

visual cover are the same characteristics which determine olfactory and auditory

cover.

Security cover is typically defined in terms of its visual cover component.

The visual cover requirement has not been determined for any species, but a

common assumption is that visual cover is adequate when an average of 10% of

a standing adult elk is visible at a distance of 61 m or less (Thomas et al. 1979).

This definition is used as a standard criterion for assessing visual cover in forest

stands for ungulates like elk and mule deer, although it remains untested for

ecological validity.

Visual cover has k n measured in a number of ways. Nudds (1977) first

suggested that the density board developed by Wight (1939) (see Gysel and Lyon

1980), and used to measure foliage structure in habitats of birds (MacArthur and

MacArthur 1961, Recher 1%9) and small mammals (Rosenzweig and Winakur

2969, WCloskey and Fieldwick 1973, muld be adapted to quantify vegetation

structure in large mammal habitats. In his critique of the use of the density

board, Nudds (1977) suggested that it is inadequate for measuring vegetation

structure because it does not allow vegetation density to be apportioned to

different heights above ground. Without this capability, density boards cannot be

used to disaiminate among habitats (i.e. detect differences in habitat structure).

Nudds (1977) developed a profile board with which he could describe vegetation

density at different heights and thus quantify habitat structure. Several

researchers have used Nudds' profile board with a variety of procedures for

studies of wildlife habitats (Riley 1982, Canfield et al. 1986, Krasowski and Nudds

1986, Loft et al. 1987, Griffith and Youtie 1988, *heen and Lyon 1989, Yeo and

Peek 1989, MacHutchon and Harestad 1990).

Several researchers also have measured visual cover with models or

targets (Canfield et al. 1986, Smith and Long 1987, Griffith and Youtie 1988).

Smith and Long (1987) used a model that simulated the broadside profile of an

elk. The model was divided into 98 equal-sized squares of 2 alternating colors

(unspecified) and an observer standing 61 m away counted the number of visible

squares to estimate the percent of an elk that would be concealed. Griffith and

Youtie (1988) used models of adult mule deer in bedded and standing positions.

Concealment of deer silhouettes was estimated as the percentage of 0.1-m squares

that were 225% concealed by vegetation. Canfield et al. (1986) used the "Hillis"

method for measuring visual cover in which a target individual moved

randomly along a transect while an observer on the opposite slope recorded the

percentage of 20 observations, taken at 5-sec intervals, that the target was not

visible as a human torso. Usefulness of models and targets is limited because

they are species-s-c and do not standardize descriptions of vegetation

structure. Profile boards, however, are not species-specific, and have broader

utility because measures of vegetation structure and density can be standardized.

While Nudds (19772 made a thorough critique of methods used to

measure visual cover up to 1977, Griffith and Youtie (1988) evaluated the

8

influence of the width of a profile device (board or pole) on estimates of visual

cover, and the repeatability of estimating visual cover by different observers.

They also correlated data collected with profile devices to that collected with a

model of a deer silhouette. My objectives were to confirm the findings of

Griffith and Youtie (1988) in their comparisons of a profile pole with both the

profile board and a deer model. I evaluate procedures used in measuring visual

cover. I propose a set of procedures that best discriminate among habitats by

providing an effective way of characterizing visual cover of different habitat

types. I present phenological changes of visual cover for 4 habitat types from late

spring through summer. I describe visual cover characteristics of 7 habitat types.

I idenhfy attributes of vegetation which contribute to visual cover. I comment

on models that have been developed to predict visual cover in different forest

types and on the use of the standard definition of adequate visual cover in forest-

wildlife management.

METHODS AND MATERIALS

Fifteen different does were radietracked regularly during the study: 11 in

1988 and 12 in 1989. Eight does were radio-tracked during both years. In both

1% and 1989, I began my field work in June and ended it in August. Radio-

tracking was conducted during daylight hours between 0800 and 2000 PST.

Triangulation was initiated from roads to obtain a rough estimate of the doe's

location. Triangulation was then continued on foot until I was very close to the

doe. I approached the does kom a downwind direction and as silently as

possible. Exact locations of does (doe sites) were determined by seeing or hearing

the does or coxifinning their location with physical signs such as backs, feces,

beds or browsed vegetation. Site centres were located in the centre of bedding

9

sites or at the point of greatest site dist.hubance or where the doe was sighted.

Some of the methodology used in the 1988 season was adapted for the 1989

season for increased efficiency and appropriateness. In 1988, random siies were

located by the following method. After I hished radio-tracking the does in

August, I joined ali the hat iom for each doe on an aerial photograph and used

the minimum convex polygon method Wohr 1947) to determine their home

ranges. I then used a grid and randomly chose coordinate pairs to locate random

sites within each home range. I followed this procedure for all does for which I

had at least 3 locations. In 1989, I used a method that allowed me to visit

random sites throughout the season rather than at the end of the season. When

I was at a dm site, I chose a random direction by spinning a pencil. Then, using a

hip chain, I walked 200 m in that direction. AU these random sites should be

within doe home ranges, which are 7 krn2 in summer (Simpson and Gyug 1991).

I measured visual cover by estimating horizontal visibility of a deer

model in 1988 and 1989, and using a vegetation profile board in 1988 and a

vegetation profi!e pole in 1989. Visual cover is an important component of

security cover for the system that I investigated. Coyotes, for which vision is an

effective sense for prey location (Wells and Lehner 19781, are a common predator

of fawns in my study area (Simpson 1988). Whereas other researchers measured

percent concealment of a model or profile device (Mudds 1977, Griffith and

Youtie 1988), I measured percent visibility. These 2 measures are complements

of each other and results are equivalent. Griffith and Youtie (1988)

demonstrated that visibility measures are repeatable because there were no

difftemcs in visibility values recorded by different observers. Nevertheless, I

estimated all deer model and pole visibilities myself to eliminate inter-observer

bias

A fullsized (i.e. the same size as an average doe mule deer, 1 m at the

1 0

shoulder) d e r mode1 was made of 7.5-an tltick foam rubber. The silhouette was

the largest one possible for a doe that is standing broadside with head and ears

facing the observer. The deer model was spray-painted orange and was not

intended to mimic natural colouring. I drew a 20-cm square grid on the deer

model to facilitate my estimation of percent visibility of the whole deer model

(e-g. head and ears = 1.5 squares = 10%). Griffith and Youtie (1988) used a model

of a standing deer silhouette that was the same size but a different shape than

mine; the head was in profile and only upper thighs of legs were represented.

Concealment of their deer model was estimated as the percentage of 0.1-m

squares, painted alternately black and white, that were 225% concealed by

vegetation.

I constructed both a vegetation profile board and pole to measure

vegetation structure and density. The board was made of l a thick plywood

that was 10 cm wide and 2 m high. It was divided into 8 25-cm long sections

which were painted alternately white and fluorescent orange. Nudds (1977) and

Griffith and Youtie (1988) used similar designs for their vegetation profile

boards. Both Nudds (1977) and Griffith and Youtie (1988) used profile boards

that were 30.5 cm wide with alternating 50-an long sections of black and white.

The board used by Nudds (1977) was 0.5 m higher than the 2-m board used by

Griffith and Youtie (1988). In 1989, I made a 2-m vegetation profile pole of 2.5-

an diameter woo& doweling. Like my board, it was divided into 8 25- long

sections that were painted alternately white and orange. Griffith and Youtie

(1988) also used a 2.- diameter pole that was 2 m high, but it was divided into

IO-an long intervals, within 0.5-m long sections (percent concealment was

m a t e d for the 0.5-m long sections using IO-an long intervals), that were

painted alternately black and white. I wed the board in 1988, but I used the less

crumbersome pole for the 1989 field season. To ensure continuity of data, I

11

measured visibility of both the pole and board at 3 sites (4 directiom/site, and 4

distances/direction). Two d the sites were in open habitats, and the other was in

a forested habitat

The model of a deer was placed at site centres and its visibility was

recorded at 4 distance: 5 m, 15 m, the minimum distance at which only 10% of

the deer model can be seen when the observer is at coyote height (0.75 m), and

the minimum distance at which only 10% of the deer model can be seen at my

height (1.5 m). At each site, the deer model was viewed at these 4 distances, in 4

directions (upslope, downslope, and right and left aaoss the slope), and at 2

observer heights. Six of 8 visibility variables for the deer model were measured

as percent visibility: percent visible at 5 m and coyote height, percent visible at 5

m and my height, percent visible at 15 rn and coyote height, percent visible at 15

m and my height, percent visible at coyote height when only 10% is visible at my

height, and percent visible at my height when only 10% is visible at coyote

height. The remaining 2 visibility variables were measured as distances rather

than percent visible: the minimum distance at which only 10% of the deer

model is visible at coyote height, and the minimum distance at which only 10%

of the deer model is visible at my heightt

Pole 6.e. board (1988) and pole (1989)) visibility was measured at 4

distances and 4 directions like deer model visibility, but unlike deer model

visibility, pole visibility was estimated at my height only. The proportion of

each 25-rrm interval visible through the vegetation was recorded as a single digit

visibility score fram 1 to 5 which corresponded to a range in percent visibility

(i-e. 1 = 0 to 20%,2 = 21 to 4U%, and so forth). Pole visibility was measured at 3

distances in 1988 and at 4 distances in 1989. In both years, pole visibility was

estimated at 5 m, 15 m and the minimum distance for 10% visib'ity of the deer

model at my height- In 1989, I also estimated pole visibility at the minimum

1 2

distance for l G % visibility of the deer model at coyote height. For each distance

that pole visibility was measured, there were 11 pole visibility variables: 1 for

each of the 8 intervals on the pale, the sum of visibility scores for the bottom 4

intervals, the sum of the visibility scores for the top 4 intervals and the sum of

visibility scores for all intervals.

A variety of understory Characteristics were assessed at random sites and

sites used by does. Within a 5-m radius of the site centre, ocular estimates of the

percent covers of shrubs and forbs were determined and recorded by species in

1988. In 1989, the percent cover of shrubs was examined by height class (<5 cm,

>5 cm - ~ 0 . 5 m, >0.5 m - ~ 1 . 0 m, 21.0 m - 4 . 5 m, 21.5 m - <2.0 m) to assess - - understory structure. In 1989, only total percent cover of forb was estimated.

The percent covers of grasses, sedges, and mosses were recorded both years.

For each tree species and height strata, I measured average diameter and

average height and estimated percent cover within in the 5-m radius plot with

one exception. I did not record the diameters of deciduous trees in the lower

strata because these diameters were too small to influence visibility. Within an

overstory canopy, there were potentially 3 height strata: a dominant upper strata

composed of the tallest trees (A), a middle strata of subordinate trees (8) and a

lower strata of trees that are 2 m to 10 m tall (C). Four species of conifers were

recorded in the study area: lodgepole pine, subalpine fir, en gel ma^ spruce,

and Douglas fir. There were also 3 species of deciduous trees: thin-leaved

mountain alder (Ainus incana), trembling aspen (Populus tremuloides), and

willow (Salix spp.). Crown closure was estimated with the simple ocular

method (Bunnell and Vales 1990).

Sites were classified as being 1 of 7 habitat types, defined by successional

stage open, shrub, riparian, spaced sapling forest, unspaced sapling forest,

immature forest, or mature forest Sedge meadows, recent clear-cuts, roads, and

13

hydro-line right-of-ways were A considered open habitat, while shrub habitat

(e.g 10-year-old clear-cuts) was all early successional stages that were comprised

mainly of shrubs rather than trees. Riparian habitat was characteristically

dominated by shrubs and associated with water. Sapling habitat was all early

forest successional stages with trees that were on average 6 m tall and 4-9 an in

diameter at breast height (dbh). Immature forest was comprised mainly of

conifer trees whose diameters (dbh) were less than 26 an. Mature forest was

comprised of older trees with diameters greater than 20 cm (dbh). The immature

and mature forests in my study area were uneven-aged and comprised of several

species of trees. The special and complex attributes of border habitats were not

addressed in this study. I included only random sites for habitat descriptions

because inclusion of sites used by does would be biased toward attributes selected

by deer. Habitat characteristics that were measured at random sites were

averaged for each habitat type (Table I).

Sample sizes for habitat types were unequal because sites were determined

randomly or by radiolocating the does. The only exception to this method was

used in locating sites of the 2 sapling habitat types. Sapling habitats were very

rare in my study area and were never used by does nor found within doe home

ranges. These sapling habitats did not have the same species composition: the

spaced sapling habitat was primarily lodgepole pine and the unspaced sapling

habitat was primarily subalpine fir. Five sites were located randomly within

each of these stands.

If visibility changed during the field season because of vegetation growth,

visibilities measured at random sites assessed in August 1988 may not have been

mmparabie to those measured at doe sites located in May through August (but

assessed in August) 1988. To estimate this potential bias, phenology plots were

established to justifv measuring visibility at doe sites and random sites at the end

Tab

le 1

. C

hara

cter

istic

s of

7 ha

bita

t typ

es in

Tho

mps

on P

late

au s

tudy

are

a 19

88-8

9. F

or e

ach

habi

tat t

ype,

mea

n pe

rcen

t cov

er o

f 3 p

lant

gro

ups,

mea

n pe

rcen

t cov

er o

f 3

stra

ta o

f co

nife

rous

and

dec

iduo

us tr

ees,

mea

n tr

ee d

iam

eter

s (d

bh),

perc

ent c

row

n cl

osur

e an

d sa

mpl

e si

zes

are i

ndic

ated

. T

ree

heig

ht st

rata

: A =

a

dom

inan

t upp

er st

rata

of

the

talle

st tr

ees,

B =

a m

iddl

e st

rata

of

subo

rdin

ate

tree

s, an

d C

= a

low

er st

rata

th

at a

re 2

m to

10 m t

au.

Varia

ble

Qpe

n S

hrub

R

ipar

ian

Uns

pace

d Sp

aced

Im

mat

ure

Mat

ure

sapl

ing

sapl

ing

fore

st

fore

st

Sam

ple

size

17

5

10

5 5

15

28

Cro

wn

1 11

7

9 7

56

41

clos

ure

(%)

Forb

s (9

6)

18

20

20

22

14

17

12

Gra

sses

and

43

3

53

0 32

1

4

13

sedg

es (%

) N

on-c

onife

r 14

55

73

67

63

38

4

5

shw

bs (%

) Tr

ee s

trata

A

,B,C

A

,B,C

A

IBIC

A

,B,C

A

,B,C

A

,f?

,C

A,B

,C

Con

ifer

(%)

1,

1,

0 7

,1

,2

1

0,5

,0

10

,24

,3

36,5,2

35

,20

,11

28,20,5

dbh

(cm

) 1,

0,

0

3,

1,

2 11

, 5,

2

16, 1

0,

1 9,

2,

1

13,

8,3

2

1,1

3,2

D

ecid

uous

(%I

0, 0,

7 Q,

0,25

0,

0,36

0,

0, O

0,

0,1

4

0,

1,1

2

0,

O,6

of the 1988 season. I estimated visibilities of the profile pole at standard

distances, directions, and my height. In 1989, visibility at phenology plots was

measured through the field season to monitor temporal changes in visibility.

Three plots in each of 4 habitat types (open, riparian, i m t u r e forest and

mature forest) were visited 5 times during the field season. The sites were

monitored 2-3 weeks apart, beginning in late May and ending in early August.

The plots were located in areas within home ranges of d e r that I had radio-

tracked in 1988.

RESIJL'l'S AND DISCUSSION

Evaluation of Apparatuses

Board - Pole Comparison

1 tested for the equivalency of the profile board, which was used in 1988,

and the profile pole, which was used in 1W, by comparing the visibility scores of

each section of the board with those of the pole. For all 8 height sections for both

the board and the pole (0-2 rn), I calculated the mean of visibility scores from the

4 directions at 15 m for 3 sites. With paired median test and Bonferroni's

conrections, I found that the visibility Sores of dl 8 sections of the board were not

significantly different from that of the pole (n = 3, P > 0.05). Figure 2 shows

visliity scores far the fourth d o n (0-75-1.00 m above the ground) for the 3

sites in 4 directions, and at 4 distances from site centre. Like Griffith and Youtie

t1988), 1 condude that the width of a pro•’ile device is unimportant. Thus, I have

nut distinguished between dab collected with the board or the pole in the rest of

my analyses, and so refer to pale and board data simply as pole data.

Visibility score for profile pole

Figure 2 Relationship between visibility of the profile board and pole at the fourth section (0-75-1 -00 m). Visibility scores at 3 sites (n=48) were measured at my height (1.5 m), 4 distances (5 m, 15 m, minimum distance for 10% deer model visibility at coyote height and at my height), and 4 directions (upslope, downslope, right and left across slope). The number of occasions for each data point are given as values of "nH-

1 7

Deer Model - Pole Comparison

Following the procedure of Griffith and Youtie (1988), I assigned the mid-

values to the visibility scores of the pole data (i-e. 1 = lo%, 2 = 30%, 3 = SO%, 4 =

70%, and 5 = 90%). I then weighted these visibility scores by multiplying them by

the proportion of the deer model potentially visible at that section of the pole

(Fig. 3), and correlated the sum of these weighted visibility scores for the pole

with the deer model visibility (n = 2102, df = 2100, r = 0.91, P < 0.05). Data for this

analysis were collected at 164 sites in 4 directions, at my height, and at 3 or 4

distances for the deer model and pole (n = 2102). Before using correlation

analyses, I examined the residuals from a regression between deer model

visibility and the transformed visibility of the pole for normality and found that

the residuals of deer model visibility have a nonnal distribution.

I obtained higher correlation coefficients (r = 0.91 overall and r = 0.87-0.96

for my 7 habitat types) than did Griffith and Youtie (1988) (r ranged between 0.62

and 0.85 for 6 habitat types). I used a finer scale, (25-cm intervals) than did

Griffith and Youtie (1988) (5(Fcm intervals). In discussing the results of their

bedded deer model and pole comparisons, Griffith and Youtie (1988) suggested

that a finer scale may improve the correlation. Although I did not use a bedded

deer model, I conclude that decreasing length of profile pole sections increased

the amount of variation i? deer model concealment that can be explained by

pole concealnrent. Canfield et al. (1986) tried to correlate the visual cover

measured with the "Hillis" method to that measured with the profile board and

found they were only moderately correlated.

Pole data can be transformed into deer model-like data without a loss of

information, and the visibility of a standing deer can be indexed by either device.

With a profile pole, it is possible to record vegetation structure and density

where this is not possible with the deer model. Also in most situations, the pole

Figure 3. Section numbers of the 8 pole sections and proportions of the deer model potentially visible at different 25-an sections of the pole are provided. Both apparatuses are drawn to scale.

is easier to use than is the deer model.

Evaluation of Procedures

Observer Height

I used the deer model to compare visibility estimated at the 2 observer

heights because all pole visibility data were recorded at my height only and not at

coyote height. I used Spearman's rank correlations to compare the percent

visibility of the deer model recorded at the 2 observer heights (coyote height, 0.75

m; my height, 1.5 m) and 2 distances (5 m and 15 m). I also compared the

minimum distance at which there was 10% visibility of the deer model at coyote

height with that at my height. Visibility at coyote height was highly correlated to

the visibility at my height: 5 m, n = 164, rs = 0.92, P < 0.01; 15 m, n = 164, rs = 0.96,

P < 0.01 (Fig. 4). The minimum distance at which 10% visibility was observed at

coyote height was also strongly correlated with the minimum distance at which

10% visibility was observed at my height (n = 164, rs = 0.95, P < 0.01).

I compared visibility of the deer model at the 2 observer heights to

determine which observer height dowed for discrimination among habitats.

The mean visibility of the deer model at 15 m for each habitat was compared to

that for a l l other habitats; there were 7 habitats, and 21 unique pairs of habitat

comparisons. When the 21 differences resulting from the 21 habitat comparisons

were averaged for both coyote height (0.75 m) and my height (1.5 m), the mean of

the differences was only slightly greater for my height (24.7 m + 4.6 SE) than that

for coyote height (23.6 m + 4.1 SE) and was not significant (Mann-Whitney U test,

n = 42, df = I, U = 214.5, P > 0.05).

Only one other researcher (Riley 1982) measured visual cover at coyote

height; however, he did not compare visibility at coyote height with visibility at

other observer heights. Visib'liity of the deer model at coyote height (0.75 m) and

I I I I I I I I

0 1

20 40 60 80 100 Percent visibility at coyote height

Figure 4. Relationship between mean percent of the deer model visible at 15 m at my height (1.5 m) and that at coyote height (0.75).

2 1

that at my height (1 -5 m) were highly correlated, thus one can be transformed to

the other. Also, the comparison between 2 observer heights demonstrated the

same magnitude of differences among habitats. I recommend that other

researchers measure visibility at their own height because estimating visibility is

easier when standing.

Distance of Observer from Site Centre

At my height, visibility of the deer model in each habitat type decreased

with distance from the deer model (Fig. 5). The rate of decrease was greater for

shrub through mature forest successional stages than that for open habitat. I

compared visibility of the pole at 5 m and 15 m to determine which distance

allowed for discrimination among habitats. The mean visibility score of the

fourth pole section (0.75-1.00 m) at 5 m and 15 m of each habitat was compared to

that of all other habitats. The mean of the differences in visibility scores was

slightly greater for 15 m (1.18 + 0.19 SE) than for 5 m (0.88 + 0.12 SE), but was not

significantly different (Mann-Whitney U test, n = 42, df = 1, U = 187.0, P > 0.05).

Nudcis (1977) found differences in profile board visibility among habitats

to be greater at 15 m than at 5,10,20,25, or 30 m, and recommended that

visibility of a profile board be estimated at a standard distance to ensure

comparability of visibility measures. He also suggested that the standard distance

may be species and geographically specific. Yeo and Peek (1989) used 10 m as a

standard distance for their study of Sitka black-tailed deer (0. h. sifkensis) in

Alaska while Riley (1982) used 6 m for his study of mule deer fawns in Montana,

but none of these researchers discussed why they chose these distances. I

conclude that 15 m is an appropriate distance to use as a standard in my study

area for mule deer.

It has been assumed that visual cover is adequate when 10% of a standing

+ open

+ shrub

+ riparian

qc_ unspaced sapling

4 spaced sapling

-6-- immature forest

+ mature forest

o ] I I I I I I I I I I I I

0 I

10 20 30 40 50 60 70 Distance from deer model (m)

Figure 5. Mean percent visibility of the deer model at my height (1.5 m) and 2 distances (5 m and 15 m) and the mean minimum distance for 10% deer model visibility for each habitat.

23

elk is visible at 61 m or less (Thomas et al. 1979). I measured the minimum

distance for 10% deer model visibility for each habitat at randomly located sites

within doe home ranges and at my height. I found that the mean of this distance

was much less than 61 m for all habitats except open habitats (Fig. 6). A t-test

performed on the entire dataset reveals that the minimum distance for 10% deer

model visibility was sigxuficantly different from 61 m (n = 164, df = I, t = -15.0, P <

0.05). When the data for each habitat were analyzed individually with t-tests and

Bonferroni's sequential correction, open habitat was the only habitat that was not

different than 61 m (n = 33, df = 1, t = 0.5, P > 0.05). Thomas et al.3 (1979)

definition is useful only in determining whether visual cover is present or not.

It does not allow for detecting differences in visual cover quality among habitats

with visual cover. While 61 m is too long to discriminate among habitats, a

distance of 15 m is more suitable in describing quality of visual cover.

Pole Section

I compared visibility of all 8 sections of the pole to determine which

section allowed for discrimination among habitats. The mean visibility of each

pole section at 15 m for each habitat was compared to that of all other habitats.

These same comparisons were done for the 2 pole halves (the sum of the bottom

4 sections and the sum of the top 4 sections) and total pole (the sum of all 8

sections). The mean of the differences between the pairs was divided by 4 for the

2 pole halves and divided by 8 for the total pole to standardize the magnitudes of

visibility scores. The means of the differences were greatest for the fourth (0.75-

1-00 m) and fifth (1.00-1.25 m) sections 7). Although a multisample Kruskal-

Wallis test revealed that differences between habitats were not sigruficantly

different for any of the pole sections (n = 231, df = 10, H = 7.7, P > 0.05), I used the

fourth pole section as the visib'ity variable in further analyses, because more of

opn shb rip usp ssp imm mat Habitat type

Figure 6. Mean and standard error of minimum distances (m) for 10% visibility of the deer model measured at my height (1.5 m) for 7 types of habitats: opn = open, shb = shrub, rip = riparian, usp = unspaced sapling, ssp = spaced sapling, imm = immature forest, mat = mature forest.

Figure 7. Mean and standard error of differences between habitats in visibility scores of each pole section, pole halves (where B is the bottom 4 sections of the pole and T is the top 4 sections of the pole) and total pole at 15 m (where BT is a11 8 sections of the pole.

2.0

1.8- cn

1.6- 6 0 -

1.4- 2. -- - .- n 1.2- cn .- > 1.0- C .- a 0.8- Q) - U r 0.6- Q, L

0.4- ii -

0.2 - - 0.0 1 1 1 1 I 1 I 1 1 1 1

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 B T B T Pole section

26

a standing deer could be seen at 0.75-1-00 m than at 1.00-1.25 m (Fig. 3.); therefore

the fourth section was the most biologically meaningful section for estimating

visibilities in habitats. However, lower sections of the pole should be used when

evaluating habitat use by fawns (Riley 1982) and perhaps for bedded adults.

Observer Position

I compared visibility of the pole at 4 different observer positions to

determine which observer position allowed for discrimination among habitats.

Visibility scores were recorded at 4 different positions relative to the centre of the

site where the pole was placed: upslope, downslope, right and left across slope.

The mean visibility of the fourth pole section at 15 m of each habitat in 4

observer positions was compared to that of all other habitats. The mean of the

differences was greatest for the upslope position (Fig. 8), but was not significantly

different from those for the oiher positions (I-way multisample Kruskal-Wallis

test, n = 105, df = 4, H = 2.7, P > 0.05).

Other researchers used the 4 cardinal directions or randomly chosen

directions instead of slope directions (Nudds 1977, Griffith and Youtie 1988) and

made no comment on the influence of observer position. Canfield et al. (1986)

and Yeo and Peek (1989) measured visibility from upslope and downslope

positions. Canfield et al. (1986) hund that visibility of a target increased as the

elevation of a viewer outside the target's stand increased, however, they did not

comment on the influence of elevation of the observer on visibility within a

stand. Position did not influence estimates of visual cover because my study area

was gently sloped. In steeply sloped areas, position could affect estimates of

visual cover. To ensure comparability of my results with those from other study

areas I chose to use the mean of visibility scores recorded at the 4 observer

positions for each site for the remainder of my malyses.

Figure 8. Mean and standard error of differences between habitats in visibility scores of the fourth pole section at 15 m for 4 observer positions and for the mean of the visibility scores recorded at the 4 positions.

- 0.2 - 0.0 I I I I 1

UP down right left mean Obsewer position

Characteristics of Visud Cover

Phenology of Visual Cover

In 1988 and 1989, I used different methods for locating random sites within

doe home ranges, and I measured the visibility of doe and random sites either

during (1 989) or at the end of the fiefd season (1988). Thus, 1988 and 1989 datasets

may not be directly comparable. To assess whether they were comparable, I

monitored 12 phenolcgy plots through the 1989 field season to record temporal

effects on visibility at sites. A series of I-way median tests for each habitat, with

date as the only effect and corrected with Bonferroni's sequential correction,

showed that visibility did not change significantly during the field season for any

of the 4 habitats (n = 15, df = 4, P > 0.05) (Fig. 9).

I concluded that visibility did not change from the end of May until early

August in 1989 for 4 habitat types, and assuming that no significant temporal

changes in visibility occurred for the same period in 1988, I pooled visibility data

from 1988 and 1989 . Loft et al. (1987) found that there was no loss of visual cover

in the first half of suxnrner in plots (ungrzed) that he monitored from 20 June to

28 September. Nudds (1977) measured interseasonal variation in visual cover

and found that visual cover decreased from summer to fall as deciduous trees

and shrubs lost their leaves. He did not d e changes in visibility during late

spring and summer.

Visual Cover in Various Habitat Types

Visibility of the deer modeI in 7 different habitats was described by 8

visibility variables (Table 2). Figure 5 also demonstrates deer model visibility in

each habitat type. Only 2 varhbks of deer d e 1 visibility did not show

signifcant differences between habitats with a 1-way multisample Kruskaf-

Wallis tesk percent visrile at coyote height when only 10% of the deer d e 1 is

+ riparian

+ immature forest

+ mature b e s t

Date

Figure 9. Mean visibility score of the fourth pole section (0.75-1.00 m) at 15 m for each habitat and time period.

Table 2. Means of 8 variables that describe deer model visibility in 7 types of 3 0 habitats. Krusal-Wallis test, n = 42, df = 6, *I? < 0.05. Variable labels: c = coyote height (0.75 m), a = my height (1.5 m), 5 = 5 m, 15 = 15 m, dl0 = minimum distance for 10% visibility, cad10 = percent visible at coyote height and adlO, and acdlO = percent visible at my height and cdlO (see page 9).

Habitat c5' a5" c15* al5' cd1O' adlo* cad10 acd10

Open 94.7% 98-0% 69.2% 77.4% 55.5rn 65.7m 5.3% 27.7%

Shrub 67.4% 73.2% 22.8% 23.4% 15.0m 18.2rn 4.8% 26.3%

Unspaced 46.6% 52.8% 7.2% 9.8% 10.6 rn 11.6 m 8.8% 14.4% sapling

'paced 68.4% 67.4% 12.8% 18.0% 13.8 m 15.4 rn 7.2% 18.2% sapling

immature 84.5% 86.8% 36.7% 39.1% 23.0 rn 23.6 rn 12.8% 14.5% forest

Mature 76.5% 79.6% 27.6% 31.8% 19.6m 2 1 . 8 ~ 1 8.5% 19.7% forest

visible at my height (n = 42, df = 6, H = 8.4, P > 0.051, and percent visible at my

height when only 10% of the deer model is visible at coyote height (n = 42, df = 6,

H = 10.0, P > 0.05). For the other 6 visibility variables, there were significant

differences among habitats (n = 85, df = 6, P c 0.05).

Visibility in different successional stages were examined to determine how

visibility changes during the process of succession (Fig. 10). Visual cover was

sparsest in the earliest successional stage (open habitats). Visual cover was

densest in the shrub and sapling (unspaced) stages and was intermediate in

density in immature and mature stages- Yeo and Peek (1989) measured visual

cover and found that visual cover was greatest in sapling habitats and least in

mixed conifer old growth habitats. They did not include open habitats in their

investigation. Six of the habitats they examined are similar to 4 of my 7 habitat

types: shrub, sapling, immature, and mature or old growth forest. The mean

percent visibilities of their pole at 10 m for these habitats falls between the means

of percent visibility of my deer model at 5 m and 15 m. Becker et al. (1990)

discussed visual cover values of different successional stages for coastal

biogeoclimatic zones. Herb stages (i.e. open habitat) had very poor visual cover

values, while shrub-seedling stages (i.e. shrub habitat), sapling-pole (i.e. sapling),

and young/mature (i.e. immature and mature forests) had poor to excellent

visual cover value depending on density, height, and bole size of conifers and

evergreen shrubs. The values of visual cover of different successional stages that

I obtained in the interior of B.C. appear to be comparable to results obtained by

Yeo and Peek (1989) in coastal Alaska and Becker ei al. (1990) in coastal B.C.

Visibility profiles for all habitats in my study area were similar in shape

=g. 11). A 1-way multisample Kruskal-Wallis test for the visibility of each pole

section showed that visibility of all pole d o n s were significantly different

among habitats (n = 85, df = 6, H = 36.7, P < 0.05). Visibility was least at the lowest

I I I I 1 OPn shb USP imm mat

Successional stages

Figure 10. Mean and standard error percent visibility of the deer model measured at 15 m and my height (1.5 m) for 5 successional stages of a sere. opn = open, shb = shrub, usp = unspaced sapling, imm = immature forest, mat = mature forest.

I

I I I I I I I I I I i 0 1 2 3 4 5

Mean visibility score

immature forest

mature forest

Figure 11. Mean visibility score of each pole section for 7 habitats.

3 4

sections of the pole and incheased with increasing height above the ground. The

pole was most visible at al l levels in the open habitat, less visible in immature

and mature forests and least visible in the remaining 4 habitats. Nudds (1977)

found that, structurally, vegetation was more similar betweex-, habitats in the first

meter above ground level than that in the second meter. In Nudds' study area,

the distribution of vegetation became more uniform at all heights as sera1 age

increased. The rank order of the habitat visibilities for the deer model was the

same as that for the profile pole (Fig. 10 and Fig. 11). The magnitude of

visibilities among habitats appear different because of the differences in shape of

the two apparatuses.

Nudds (1977) compared structural differences within habitat tykes and

concluded that forest habitats were not uniformly structured. I performed a 1-

way multisample Kruskal-Wallis test on deer model and pole visibility at 15 m

using site number as the effect and found no intra-habitat differences in visibility

for any of the 7 habitats (P > 0.05). I conclude that within each of my 7 habitats,

structure and density of their vegetation were uniform.

Components of Visual Cover

I performed a multiple linear regression to determine which physical and

vegetation characteristics contributed to visual cover. The frequency

distributions of the residuals of the regressions were normal, thus I did not

violale the assumption of normality for this parametric test. Because of

autocorrelation between the independent variables, I could not perform

regression analysis on data for each habitat separately. It is not surprising that,

within habitats, habitat characteristics were closely related.

The visibility of the fourth section of the pole (0.75-1.00 m) at 5 m and 15

rn were the 2 dependent variables used sequentially. There were 21 independent

variables used (Table 3). Because total percent cover of non-conifer shrubs was

directly related to the percent cover of non-conifer shrubs at each of 5 heights, I

ran 2 separate analyses, one with the total percent cover and the second with the

percent cover at the 5 heights. None of the percent cover variables for the 5

heights of non-conifer shrubs were significant in the regression, but total percent

cover of shrubs was significant (Tables 3). Thus the regression models presented,

used total percent cover sf shrubs rather than percent cover of non-conifer

shrubs at 5 heights.

Although all the habitat variables were measured for a 5-m-radius plot,

the regression of pole visibility at 15 m fitted the data better ( ~ 2 = 0.52) than that

at 5 m ( ~ 2 = 0.42). This may be further evidence that 15 m is a more appropriate

standard distance for measuring visual cover than 5 m. There were 4 significant

independent variables that the regression models had in common: aown

closure, total percent cover of non-conifers in the shrub layer, percent cover of

deciduous trees in the middle height strata, and percent cover of deciduous trees

in the bottom height strata. There were 3 other variables that were significant

only in the 15 m regression model: percent cover of conifer trees in the top

height strata, percent cover of conifers in the shrub layer, and the diameter of

deciduous trees in the middle height strata. There was 1 variable, percent cover

of conifer trees in the bottom height strata, that was sigruficant in the 5 m

regression model only. Of all the significant variables only 2 had positive

coefficients: aown closure and diameter of deciduous trees in the middle height

strata. In a multiple regression, the sign ~f a coefficient does not necessarily

indicate the nature of the relationship between the dependent and independent

variables.

It is not surprising that percent cover of moss or herbs were not sigmficant

variables because neither plant type occurred frequeny 0.75-1.00 m above the

Table 3. Independent variables in my multiple linear regression model and 3 6 their coefficients when the dependent variables are the visibility scores of the fourth pole section at 5 m and 15 m sequentially. (5 m: n = 155, P c 0.05, ~2 = 0.42; 15 m: n = 155, P < 0.05, ~2 = 0.52) ' P c 0.05.

Independent Variables Coefficient Coefficient

at 5 m at 15 m

Constant +4.90e +4.42'

Slope +0.003 -0.01 0

Crown closure +0.01' +0.014*

Percent cover of moss -0.001 -0.003

Percent cover of herbs -0.001 -0.001

Percent cover of conifer trees in the top height strata -0.01 -0.01 9*

Diameter of conifer trees of the top height strata +0.003 -0.01 4

Percent cover of conifer trees in the middle height strata -0.01 -0.01 3

Diameter of conifer trees of the middle height strata -0.01 -0.01 8

Percent cover of conifer trees in the bottom height strata -0.03' -0.020

Diameter of conifer trees of the bottom height strata +0.05 -0.01 2

Percent cover of conifer in the shrub layer -0.01 -0.030'

Total percent cover of non-conifer in the shrub layer -0.005' -0.006'

Percent cover of deciduous trees in the middle height strata -0.1 4' -0.1 36"

Diameter of deciduous trees of the middle height strata +0.06 +0.092*

Percent cover of deciduous trees in the bottom height strata -0.02' -0.030'

37

ground. As expected, slope did not influence visual cover because the terrain in

my study area was gently sloped. Canfield ei al. (1986) examined the visual cover

of stands when viewed from an opposing slope to the stand. They found that the

viewing angle explained 52% of the variation in visual cover but slope, tree

height, tree and shrub densities and distribution also affected the relationship

between viewing angle and visual cover.

My analyses suggest that shrub densities of coniferous and non-coniferous

plants were important components of visual cover. Although some researchers

(Taber 1961, Black et al. 1976, Loft et al. 1987, Becker et al. 1990) acknowledged that

understory vegetation provides both thennal and visual cover, researchers who

modeled visual cover have not. Two visual cover models for ungulates

proposed by Smith and Long (1987) and McTague and Patton (1989) were based

on the boles and live crowns of conifers and did not include shrubs. Modeling

the characteristics of shrubs is problematic, but their role in visual cover cannot

be ignored. Lyon (1987) used a model that included boles but did not include

shrubs, but later realized that tall shrubs can provide visual cover. At the shrub

densities he recorded in some treatments of lodgepole pine stands, visual cover

increased from less than 10% when shrubs were not considered to over 90%

when they were.

The importance of deciduous trees as a component of visual cover has not

been examined by other researchers because they attempted to simulate visibility

only in coniferous stands. My study examined visual cover for a spectrum of

successional stages, not all of which were pure coniferous forests. In my study

area, diameter of conifers was not an important predictor of visual cover, but

percent cover of conifers in the top strata was. Previous models of visual cover

(Smith and Long 1987, Lyon 1987, McTague and Patton 1989) tended to use

variables such as average diameters, crown closure, density, basal area and stand

3 8 density index, because they are common forestry measures; however, these may

have no direct relationship to visual cover. Smith and Long's (1987) model of

visual cover used only coniferous tree diameters (dbh), density and spatial

distribution of trees. When they tested their model in the field, they found that

observed visual cover did not agree with their simulated results. Smith and

Long (1987) attributed &is discrepancy to irregular spacing of trees in field

situations, but I suggest that this discrepancy may be attributed to the exclusion of

understory and deciduous components from their model.

My regression analyses provide important insights into the structure of

visual cover. Habitat characteristics that have not traditionally k n included in

models of visual cover are important components of visual cover: e.g. percent

cover of coniferous and non-coniferous shrubs, percent cover of deciduous trees

in the middle and lower height strata and the diameter of deciduous trees of the

middle height strata. My regression analyses also show that crown closure and

percent cover of conifers in the top height strata which are commonly used in

some form in models, are sigruficant factors affecting visual cover.

CONCLUSIONS

In evaluating the use of 3 apparatuses to estimate visual cover (profile

board, profile pole and deer model), I found that the board and the pole are

equivalent, and that data collected with the pole can be transformed into percent

visibility of a deer model. The pole is convenient to use and provides data on

vegetation structure and deflsify. In comparison, the deer model is less

convenient to lnse and does not provide adequate information on vegetation

structure. Therefore, the profile-pole is the most versatile and convenient

apparatus for measuring visual cover. My results confirmed those reported by

Griffith and Youtie (1988).

I evaluated procedures of measuring visual cover and proposed a set of

procedures that increase our ability to detect differences in visual cover values

among habitats. Visual cover can be measured at the observer's standing height

because there appears to be no advantage to h e l i n g at coyote height. It is best to

use a standard distance between observer and apparatus for habitat or site

comparisons, which probably will be species and geographically speafic. For

mule deer in my study area, it was more appropriate to measure visual cover

quality at 15 m than at 61 m. The fourth (0.75-1.00 m) and fifth section (1.00-1.25

m) were the best pole sections at which to measure visibility for conditions

encountered in my study area for adult mule deer. I suggest that lower sections

of the pole be used when evaluating habitat use by fawns and perhaps bedded

adults. In my study area, there was no significant advantage for the observer to

stand upslope, downslope or across-slope from the apparatus when estimating

visual cover.

Temporal changes in visibility in 4 habitats was not significant during late

spring and summer. Horizontal visibility for each habitat can be measured at

any time during late spring and summer to assess a representative value for

visual cover of the various biogeodirnatic zones present in my study area.

Visibility was greatest in open habitats and least in unspaced sapling stands.

Regression analysis revealed habitat characteristics that predict pole visibility and

thus visual cover. The most important habitat characteristics to measure appear

to be: crown closure, percent cover of conifer trees in the top height strata,

percent cover of coniferous shrubs, total percent cover of non-coniferous shrubs,

percent cover of deciduous trees in the middle and bottom height strata, and

diameter of deciduous trees of the middle height strata. Because there were

gentle slopes in my study area, vegetation characteristics rather than physical

characteristics such as slope were important predictors of visual cover.

Existing visual cover models that translated cover guidelines into forestry

prescriptions have 2 weaknesses. First, they do not include deciduous tree and

shrub densities. Second, they a l l use untested "standard" criteria (e.g. visual

cover is adequate when 10% of a standing elk is visible at 61 m or less) to

evaluate the value of visual cover in various forest types. Only Smith and Long

(1987) questioned this assumption. Wore evaluating visual cover, I recommend

that investigators conduct preliminary research to ascertain a standard distance

that is appropriate for their species and study area by examining visibility-

distance functions. Also, models that simulate visual cover should include

characteristics of shrubs and deciduous trees.

4 1

C W R THREE

INFLUENCES OF VISUAL COVER AND OTHER FACTORS ON DIURNAL SITE

SELECTION BY MULE DEER

Security cover is a structural resource that reduces the probability of

detection or attack by a predator (Elton 1939, MacHutchon 1988). Because

structural characteristics of habitats vary aaoss a forested landscape, security

cover also varies and could therefore affect use of forest lands by Rocky

Mountain mule deer. Although security cover appears to be an important

component of mule deer habitat, its role in habitat selection has not been clearly

demonstrated (Peek et al. 1982, Thomas et al. 1986). Several studies have assessed

visual cover (as the most easily measured structural component of security

cover) in various habitat types fox ungulates (Nudds 1977, Riley 1982, Canfield et

al. 1986, Loft et al. 1987, Smith and Long 1987, Griffith and Youtie 1988, Servheen

and Lyon 1989, Yeo and Peek 1989). None of these, however, determined the

importance of visual cover resources relative to other important resources in

habitat selection.

The selection of habitats is thought to occur at 3 levels (Johnson 1980).

First, the geographical range of a species is selected by individuals. Then, each

individual selects a mosaic of habitats for their home range (second level).

Lastly, sites for basic activities are chosen within home ranges (third level).

Individuals use habitats in which they are most likely to survive and reproduce

(Krebs and Davies 1984). Use of a particular site is believed to be a function of the

costs arrd benefits available to the animal at the site (Peek et al. 1982). For

example, a cost of using a particular site may De increased predation risk due to a

lack of visual cover. A benefit of using a particular site may be high availability

of a valuable resource such as food.

4 2

The availability and distribution of basic resources such as food, water and

cover could influence selection of sites by does. I compared the availability of

food, water and visual cover at sites used by does to that at randomly located sites

within doe home ranges to establish the relative importance of these resources

in site selection. There was also the possiwty that sites are used by d m to avoid

human disturbance, so minimum distance to roads of s i te is examined. Atso, I

investigated whether visual cover functioned for thermal or security cover. To

assess the n e d for t h e d cover, I examined micro-climate conditions (air

temperature, wind speeds, and light intensity) at each site used by does. Crown

closure was estimated as an index of thermal cover at random sites and sites used

by does (Black et al. 1976, Leckenby et a1 1982, Thomas et aL 1986, Lyon 1987,

Smith and Long 1987). Structural characteristics of visual cover for sites used by

does and random sites were described in terms of visibilities of the deer model

and profile pole. The minimum distance for 10% visibility of a deer model was

measured at sites used by does to further evaluate the validity of the common

definition of adequate visual cover (Thomas et al. 1979).

In m i n g wh&er deer were influenced by visual cover in site

selection, I proposed a null and an alternate hypothesis.

Hg Visual cover at sites used by does was not different than that at randomly

located sites within doe home ranges.

In this case, deer would have used sites on the basis of hbitat

characteristics other than visual cover or at random.

HI V i d cover at sites used by does was different than thzr at randomly

bcated sites wiihin doe b m e ranges.

More +ally, does should have used sites with less visual cover when

4 3

the costs of predation were low or the bedits available by using the site were

high, Does should have used sites with less visual mver in spring when their

immediate energy rqukments were high and if there is a trade-off between

f d resources and visual mver resources. During spring when their fat reserves

were depleted (Mautz 1978, Simpson 1988), deer should have risked foraging on

vernal vegetation which was first available in open habitats to avoid continued

weight loss. Access to high quality forage early in spring a d d have effettlvely

shorened the duration of low energy and nutrient intake and increased survival

(Stridand 1975). The l ~ s f s of predation would have been relatively low in

spring because adult does and yearlings would not have been as vulnerable to

predation as young-of-theyear in summer. Visual cover at sites used by does

before parturition should have been lower than that at randomly sites within

doe home ranges or than that at sites used after parturitioa Simpson (1988)

o b e ~ e d that some does used open habitats that provided high quality food

before moving to their summer ranges where they gave birth.

In another scenario, does should have used sites with more visual cover

than that which was available at randomly located sites when the costs of

predation were high. After parturition, if a doe was ammpanied by her

newborn fawn into an opening, she would be risking her fawn's life and her

own by using a site with W e visual cover. Fawns are more vulnerable to

predatioln than add& (Trainer 19751, therefore the costs of using an open habitat

d d be greater for a doe with a fawn than those for a solitary doe. I predicted

that sites used by a doe with a fawn would have greater visual cover than sites

used by a solitary doe Daa with fawns should have used edges (ecotones) and

small openings* whereas solitary does w d d have ventured further from visual

cover into openings to forage on high Quality food and to bed near forage sources.

Edwards (1983) found that moose (.Alas nlces) cows with calves "show a different

4 4

distribution and diet from solitary adults and yearling moose". During spring

just before calving, moose cows on Isle Royale moved from an area with high

quality food to a predator-free area with low quality food. Visual cover was not

investigated by Edwards (19831, but her data indicate that behaviour of females

with young was influenced by predation risk.

METHODS AND MATERIALS

In both 1988 and 1989, I began my field work in June and ended it in

August. Parturition occurred in June and July for the majority of does. By the

time the fawns were 6 weeks old, they were relatively mobile and tended to

follow their dams rather than hide. All radio-tracked females produced fawns.

Eleven females were radio-tracked regularly in 1988; 10 were adults and 1 was a

yearling. Twelve adult does were radbtracked in 1989. A total of 15 different

individuals were tracked during the study.

Radietracking was conducted dwing daylight between 0800 and 2000 PST.

Triangulation was initiated from roads to obtain a rough estimate of the doe's

location, Triangulation was then continued on foot until I was very close to the

doe. I approached the does from a downwind direction and as silently as

@ble to avoid detection. The exact location of the doe was determined by

seeing or hearing it or eonfinning its location with physical sign. I looked for

fawns and signs of feeding, bedding, running and defecating. Besides finding a

hiding fawn, small tracks, small pellets, and small beds were assumed to confirm

the presence of a fawn I a s s a d from the doe's behaviour whether the doe had

a fawn nearby (White et al. 1972).

I compared Ue behaviour of does before and after parturition. The

treatments for this analysis were does without fawns (i.e. before parturition) and

4 5

does with fawns (i.e. after parturition), and the control treatment was

represented by randomly located sites within doe home ranges. Thus, site types

were classified in 3 tiers. Sites were either where does were found, with or

without fawns, or where sites were randomly located within doe home ranges.

Sites used by does were classified as either being used before or after parturition.

Also, doe sites were classified as to what kind of activity the doe was engaged in

at the site: bedding, feeding or unknown. One random site was determined for

every doe site with only 6 exceptions. Methods for choosing random sites

within home ranges of does were described in Chapter 2.

Visual cover was an important component of security cover for the

system that I investigated. Coyotes, for which vision is an effective sense for

prey location (Wells and Lehner 1978), were a common predator of fawns in my

study area (Simpson 1988). I measured visual cover by estimating the horizontal

visibility of a deer model (1988,1989) and of a profile board in 1988, and a profile

pole in 1989. Although visibility of the profile pole (or board) was positively

correlated to visibility of the deer model ( s e Chapter 2), I focussed on the

visibility of the deer model at coyote height to assess the use of visual cover by

deer and to assess the validity of the common definition of adequate visual

cover (Thomas et al. 1979) because a direct measure is better for experimental

purposes than a correlated measure.

Descriptions of the apparatuses (e.g. the deer model, the profile board, and

the profile pole) used to measure visual cover and of the procedures used in

measuring visual cover were given in Chapter 2. The profile board was used in

1988, but I used the less ambersome pole in the 1989 field season. I have not

distinguished between data collected with the board and the pole in my analyses

because the width of a visual cover device was unimportant (Griffith and Youtie

1988, Chapter 2) and so referred to pole and board data simply as pole data.

4 6

Where other researchers measured percent concealment of a model or profile

apparatus (Nudds 1977, Griffith and Youtie 19881, I measured percent visibility.

These 2 measures were complementary and results are equivalent (see Chapter

2). Griffith and Youtie (1988) demonstrated that visibility measures are

repeatable because there were no differences in visibility values recorded by

different observers. However, to eliminate inter-observer bias, 1 estimated all

deer model and pole visibilities myself.

A variety of habitat characteristics were assessed at sites used by deer and at

random sites. Within a 5-m radius of the site centre, total percent cover of forbs

was estimated in 1989, while a percent cover by each forb species was estimated

in 1988. Several other site characteristics were assessed that could affect use sf

visual cover: minimum distance to water and roads, air temperature,

occurrence of precipitation, incident light intensity (1989 only), wind speed (1988

only), and crown closure. Air temperature was measured in the shade just

above ground level with a thermometer. Incident light intensity was recorded at

shoulder height with a light meter at the site centre and in a nearby clearing.

Wind speed at shoulder height was measured with an anemometer at the site

centre. Crown closure was estimated with the simple ocular method (Bunnell

and Vales 1990). The same procedure for site description was followed at both

random and doe sites except micro-climate variables were measured only at doe

sites.

Understory characteristics also were measured in August for random sites

in 1988, whereas understory characteristics were measured throughout the

season at doe sites in 1988 and 1989 and at random sites in 1989. There was a

potential seasonal bias in comparing understory characteristics at doe sites with

those at random sites. I estimated percent cover of forb at phenology plots and

tested for seasonal &ects on percent cover of forbs with a multisample Kruskal-

4 7

Wallis test on the phenology dataset. Like Loft et al. (1987), I found that percent

cover of forb peaked in mid-summer and then declined for all four habitats, but

these changes in availability were insignificant whether I used date in a 1-way

analysis (n = 56, df = 4, H = 8.79, P 7 0.05) or date and habitat in a 2-way analysis (n

= 56, df = 19, H = 26.0, P > 0.05). Because there was no seasonal effect on percent

cover of forbs, the date on which data were collected does not confound forb

availability analyses. Although all of my 1988 random sites were assessed at the

end of the field season, I conclude that these sites can be compared without bias

to my doe site data and 1989 random site data which were collected throughout

the field season.

RESULTS

Sample Sizes

I located 81 sites used by does anci their fawns: 46 in 1988 and 35 in 1989.

During the study, 3-9 sites were identified for each of the 15 does that were

tracked. Among the 81 sites used by does, 56 were bedding sites and 25 were non-

bedding sites; 23 were used by does before parturition and 58 were used after

parturition. Of the 56 bedding sites, 12 were used before parturition and 44 were

used after parturition. f measured visibility at all but 2 of 81 doe sites. For 6 of 81

does sites, I did not have corresponding random sites (n = 75). Sample sizes for

my analyses of visibility data were 4 times greater than that of other datasets

because at sites, visibility variables were measured: upslope, downslope, and

right and left aaoss the slope. The sample size for percent cover of forbs was less

than 81 because I neglected to record it at 3 sites used by does (n = 781, and only 73

random-doe site pairs were available for forb analyses because not all of these 78

sites had random site partners. Light intensity was measured only in the first

4 8

field season because it did not occur to me to measure light intensity until after

my first field season (n = 31). I did measured wind speed in 1988 only because

there was no wind on 70% of deer locations in 1988, and when present it was

gusty and difficult to measure accurately (n = 42). Neither of these climate

variables was recorded on every occasion during their respective field seasons

because the equipment was not always functioning or available.

Percent Cover of Forbs

Although I measured the percent cover of several potential forage types,

my forage analyses focus on forbs because forb make up -97% of the diets of

Rocky Mountain mule deer during summer (Wilkins 1957, Lovaas 1958, Kufeld

et al. 1973, Willms et al. 1980, Collins and Urness 1983). Forb are also one of the

most digestible (65% digestibility) and most nutritious (13% crude protein) forage

types during summer (Schwartz and Hobbs 1985), and so they are considered to

be the most important forage type to deer.

Forb - Cover Tradeoff

I tested for the existence of a food - cover tradeoff with regression analyses.

If there was a tradeoff, then I expected to find a significant positive linear

regression between visibility and percent cover of forbs and a significant negative

linear relationship between crown closure and percent cover of forbs. A simple

linear regression between the visibility of the deer model at 15 m and coyote

height and forb coverage at doe and random sites (Fig. 12) was insignificant (n =

152, df = 151, r2 = 0.016, P = 059). A simple linear regression between crown

dosure and forb coverage was also insignrfiwt (n = 152, df = 151, r2 = 0.1 69, P =

0.11) (Fig. 13). There was no evidence that a forb - visual cover tradeoff existed in

my study area in summer.

Percent of model visible at 15 m

Figure 12. Relationship between percent visibility of the deer model measured at 15 m and coyote height (0.75 m) and percent cover of forb.

Percent crown closure

Figure 13. Relationship between percent crown closure and percent cover of forb.

Percent Cover of Forbs at Sites

Although does did not have to choose between good visual cover and

good forb availability, they may have used sites on the basis of percent cover of

forbs. I used 2 types of analyses to examine percent cover of forbs at doe sites.

First, a I-tailed Wilcoxon matched-pair rank-sum test demonstrated that percent

cover of forbs at doe sites was not sigruficantly (n = 73, P > 0.05) different from

that at random sites (Table 4). Also, percent cover of f o r b at sites used by does

before parturition was not significantly different from that at sites used by does

after parturition (n = 20, P > 0.05). Second, I performed 2 separate analyses of

variance for activity type and reproductive status because there was only one doe

for which I had all 4 combinations of activity type and reproductive status sites.

Although analysis of variance (ANOVA) data should k normally distributed, I

obtained the same results when I used data which had been normal rank

transformed. Becavse ANOVA is robust with respect to normality, results from

an ANOVA on untransformed data should be valid. The only signhcant main

effect was deer identity and there were no sigruficant interactions between

variables (Table 5). Does did not appear to use sites on the basis of percent cover

of forbs before or after parturition, but individual deer did use sites differently.

Water Availability

To test whether does used sites on the basis of their proximity to water, I

compared the minimum distance to water sowces (that I was able to identify) for

doe sites and random sites (Table 6). No significant results were found witk a 2-

tailed Wilcoxon matched-pair rank-sum test for the doe-random comparison (n

= 75, P > 0.05) or the beforeafter parturition comparison (n = 20; P > 0.05). With 2

3-way ANOVAs using site type (doe or random), deer identity and either activity

type (bedding or not) or reproductive status (fawn or not), both runs

Table 4. Mean percent cover of forbs at sites used by does and random sites. 5 2 Site types: R = random, D = doe, Bf = before parturition, A = after parturition, NBd = non-bedding, and Bd = bedding.

Site Type n R a n g e (%) M e a n (%) S E (%)

R 75 0-70 15.9 1.8

D 78 0-67 15.3 2.0

DBf 22 0-51 15.5 2.8

DA 56 0-67 15.3 2.5

DNBd 23 0-66 18.3 4.3

DBd 55 0-67 14.1 2.1

DBf Bd 12 0-28 12.9 2.7

D A M 43 0-67 14.4 2.6

Table 5. ANOVA table of percent cover of forbs at sites used before and after 5 3 parturition. Site = Site type, Id = Doe identification, Repro .=

Reproductive status.

- -

Factors Sum of df Mean F-ratio P Test term squares square

Site 49.9 1 49.9 0.32 0.58 Site x Id

Id 7350.1 1 1 668.2 2.97 0.00 Residual

Site x Id 1694.8 11 1 54.1 0.68 0.75 Residual

Repro 17.5 1 17.5 0.06 0.81 Id x Repro

Site x Repro 399.0 1 399.0 1.44 0.26 Site x Id x Repro

Id x Repro 3244.1 1 1 294.9 1.31 0.24 Residual

Site x Id x Repro 3052.5 1 1 277.5 1.23 0.28 Residual

Residual 16673.0 74 225.3

Total 32533.0 121

Table 6. Mean minimum distances to water (m) from sites used by does and 5 4 random sites. Site types: R = random, D = doe, Bf = before parturition, A = after parturition, NBd = non-bedding, and Bd = bedding.

Site Type n Range (m) Mean (m) SE (m)

DNBd 25 0 - 500 105 30

demonstrated that deer identity was the only sigruficant main effect and both

activity type (n = 120, df = 32, P < 0.05) and reproductive status (n = 126, df = 35, P

c 0.05) interacted with deer identity. Variation among individuals prevents

simple interpretation of results: some deer used sites near water sources while

others did not.

Deer may have responded to the proximity of water for reasons other than

as sources of drinking water. Other resources associated with riparian habitat,

such as lush forage and willow thickets which provided dense visual cover may

promote use of surrounding habitats. Also, areas with running or standing

water may provide non-visual (olfactory) security cover for deer by masking

their scent from predators (Sweeney et al. 1971, Simpson 1988). I tested for a

preference for riparian habitat by comparing the percent of sites used by does that

were in riparian habitats to the percent of random sites that were in riparian

habitats. Although a greater proportion of sites used by does are in riparian

habitats (24%) than that for random s i te (13%), a maximum likelihood test of

independence showed that site choice was independent of habitat type (n = 154,

df = 4, L = 5.69, P > 0.05). This result indicated site selection is independent of

habitat type (as I had classified them). Simpson (1988) suggested that does

switched from using forested or open habitats to using riparian areas during and

after parturition. I found this to be true for only 3 of 15 does and the opposite is

m e for 4 of 15 while I doe used riparian areas both before and after parturition (7

of 15 does were not located both before and alter parturition). It appears that

preference for riparian habitats varied among individuals.

Visual Cover

Visibility of the Deer Model at Sites used by Does

I tested for a preference for sites with low visibility by does with a I-tailed

Wilcoxon matched-pair rank-sum test with Bonferroni's sequential correction

for all 8 visibility variables. The test showed that only the minimum distance at

which 10% of the deer model could be seen at coyote height was significantly

different between sites used by does and random sites (n = 380, P < 0.05). The

visibility of the deer model at coyote height at 15 m, and the minimum distances

at which 10% of the deer model could be seen at coyote height and my height

were significantly lower at sites used after parturition than that at random sites

(n = 212, P < 0.05). No variables were significantly different between sites u s 4 by

does before parturition and random sites.

I chose 2 visibility variables among the 8 on which to conduct further

statistical tests. Because coyotes are a common predator of deer, I decided to use

the variables which were measured at coyote height. I used the visibility at 15 m

and coyote height because 15 m is a more biologically relevant distance than 5 m

for deer in terms of predator-prey interactions di.e. 5 m is too short). Also, 15 m is

better than 5 m for distinguishing among habitats (see Chapter 2). I also was

interested in the minimum distance at which only 10% of the deer model can be

seen at coyote height in order to compare it to 61 m which is used in the

definition of adequate visual cover proposed by Thomas et at. (1979).

Visibility of the Deer Model at 15 rn and Coyote Height

I tested the visibility of the deer model at 15 m and ooyote height (Table 7)

with 2 3way ANOVAs. I used 3 independent factors: site type (random or doe),

d e e ~ identity, and either activity type (bedding or not) or reproductive s t a m

before or after parturition). For both ANOVAs, deer identity was the only

significant variable and the only significant interaction was between site type and

deer identity (activity type run, n = 476, df = 10, P < 0.05; rqmiutive status ntn,

n = 500, df = 11, P < 0.05). In Figure 14, I plotted the interaction between site type

Table 7. Mean percent visibility of the deer model at coyote height and at 15 5 7 m for sites used by does and random sites. Site types: R = random, D = doe, Bf = before parturition, A = after parturition, NBd = non- bedding, and Bd = bedding.

Site Type n Range (%) Mean (%) SE (%)

R 75 0-1 00 36.6 3.2

D 79 0-1 00 30.9 2.9

DBf 22 0-1 00 38.9 6.2

DA 57 0-1 00 27.9 3.2

DNBd 25 0-1 00 28.6 5.0

Df3d 54 0-1 00 32.0 3.6

O B W 11 0-1 00 45.1 8.6

DAM 43 0-1 00 28.7 3.8

- 3 0 - 0 , 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16

Rank

Figure 14. Differences in mean percent visibility of the deer model at 15 m and coyote height (0.75 m) for random sites and sites used by 15 does where deer identification is ranked in order of difference. A positive value for a difference indicates that the mean visibility for random sites (R) is greater than that for sites used by that doe (D).

and deer identity. For 8 of 15 deer, mean visibility at random sites was greater

than that at the sites used by doe, but in 7 of 15 deer the mean visibility at

random sitc:; was less than that at sites used by does . When I added a categorical

variable that was rested in deer identity and either activity type or reproductive

status, the nested variable was the only sigruficant variable and the only

significant interaction was between it and site type for both runs (activity type, df

= 39, P < 0.05; reproductive status, df = 37, P < 0.05). When I plotted the

interaction terms, there were no particular deer acting as outliers. These results

were consistent for 3 forms of the data: untransformed, arcsin(square root)

transformed, and ranked-normal transformed. There was not a consistent trend

in site selection behaviow exhibited by does regarding visual cover available at

the sites they used.

Distance at Which Only 10% of the Deer Model Can be Seen at Coyote Height

Mean minimum distances at which only 10% of the deer model can be

seen at coyote height are described for 8 site types in Table 8. A t-test performed

on the entire dataset demonstrated that the minimum distance for 10% deer

model visibility was significantIy Merent than 61 m (n = 154, df = 1, t = -16.2, P <

0.05). When datasets for each site type were analyzed individually with t-tests

and Bonferroni's sequential correction, the minimum distance for 10% deer

model visibility for every site type was also significantly different from 61 m (df =

1, P a 0.05).

Minimum distance at which only 10% of the deer model can be seen at

coyote height was tested with 2 3-way anafyxs of variance. The 3 effects were:

site type (random or doe), deer identity, and either activity type (bedding or not)

or reproductive status Wore or after parturition). For both ANOVAs, deer

identity was the only significant variable. Only 11 of 15 does could be used in the

Table 8. Mean minimum distance (m) at which only 10% of the deer model 6 0 can be seen at coyote height for sites used by does and random sites. Site types: R = random, D = doe, Bf = before parturition, A = after parturition, N M = non-bedding, and Bd = bedding.

Site Type n Range (m) Mean (m) SE (in)

R 75 5-1 55 27.4 2.7

D 79 6-274 23.7 3.4

DBf 22 8 - 42 22.6 2.3

DA 57 6-274 24.4 4.7

DNBd 25 8-274 30.0 10.2

D M 54 6 - 66 20.8 1.6

DBfBd 11 8 - 42 25.4 3.2

DAM 43 6 - 66 19.7 1.8 - il

8 1

ANOVA for activity type because I identified both bedding and non-bedding sites

for only I1 of 15 does. Two sipficant interactions occurred between site type

and deer identity (n = 476, df = 43, P < 0.05) and between site type, deer identity

and activity type (n = 500, df = 47, P < 0.05)- Figure 15 demonstrates the

interaction between site type and deer identification while Figures 16 and 17

demonstrate the 3-way interaction between activity type, site type and deer

identification. These interactions were difficult to describe because there is no

regular pattern. For 8 of 15 does, the minimum distance was longer for random

sites than that fur sites used by does. Results were similar for bedding sites: for 7

of 11 does the minimum distance for 10% visibility is longer for random sites

than that for bedding sites used by does. When comparing non-bedding sites

with bedding sites, for 6 of I1 does, the minimum distance was longer for non-

bedding sites than that for bedding sites.

When I added a categorical variable that was nested in deer identity and

either activity type or reproductive status, the nested variable was the only

significant variable and the only signrficant interaction was between it and site

type for both runs (activity type, n = 476, df = 43, P < 0.05; reproductive status, n =

500, df = 47, P < 0.05). When I plotted the interaction tenns, there were no

particular deer acting as outliers. These results were not completely consistent

for 2 forms of the data: untransfonned and ranked-nod transformed.

It seems that individual does use sites differently with regard to visual

cover. The does that used sites with lower visibility at 15 m were the same does

that used sites with shorter minimum distances for 10% visibility than those

measured for random sites. One would srpect this result because these visibility

varhbfes are correlated (n = 154, df = 152, rs = 0.92, P < 0.01). k e is some

indication that does did not use sites r m d d y , but there was not a clear trend in

the relationship between the visibility at sites used by does and that at random

Q, U t E? a C s

Figure 15.

Rank

Differences in mean minimum distance (m) at which only 10% of the deer model can be seen at coyote height (0.75 m) for random sites and sites used by 15 does where deer identification is ranked in order of difference. A positive value for a difference indicates that the mean visibility for random sites (R) is greater than that for sites used by that doe (D).

Figure 16.

Rank

Differences in mean minimum dktance (m) at which only 10% of the deer model can be seen at coyote height (0.75 m) for random sites and bedding sites used by I1 does where deer identification is ranked in order of difference. A positive value for a difference indicates Uat the mean visibility for random sites (R) is greater than that for I d d i n g sites used by that doe (B).

Rank

Figure 17. Differences in mean minimum distance (m) at which only 10% of the deer model can be seen at coyote height (0.75 m) for bedding and non-bedding site used by I I does where deer identification is ranked in order of difference. A positive value for a difference indicates that the mean visibility for non-bedding sites (NB) is greater than that for bedding sites used by that doe (B).

sites. Activity type appears to have influenced the choice of sites regarding the

minimum distance for 10% deer model visibility at coyote height, but not for the

visibility of the deer model at 15 m and coyote height. Reproductive status did

not play a significant role in the selection of site visibility.

Visibility of the Profile Pole at Sites Used by Does

Two Sway multi-sample median tests adjusted with Bonferroni's

sequential correction were performed on 11 pole variables which were measured

at 15 m: visibilities of the 8 pole sections, sum of visibilities of all 8 pole sections,

sum of bottom 4 sections of the pole and sum of the top 4 sections of the pole.

Again, deer identity, site type, and either activity type or reproductive status were

used as sample strata. There were sig"ificant differences for 9 of the 11 variables,

according to both Sway median tests (activity type, n = 377, df = 43, P < 0.05;

reproductive status, n = 405, df = 47, P < 0.05); the exceptions were the visibilities

of the bktorn 2 sections of the pole (P > 0.05). Despite the si-cant differences

at 6 s f 8 pole sections, profiles of pole visibility for 6 site types look very similar

(Fig. 18). Significant differences could be attributed to variation in doe behaviour

rather than site type, activity type or reproductive status. Visibilties of the pole

and deer model were highly correlated (Chapter 2) so analysis of pole visibility

would duplicate analysis of deer model visibility.

Miao-Chate Con& tions

I measured air temperature, wind speed and incident light intensity to

assess the need for thermal cover by does. Air temperature, wind speed, and

light intensity at the time md place of doe locations are described in Table 9.

Figures 19,20 and 21 are frequency distn'butions of wind speed, short-wave

radiation, and air and operative temperature. Estimates of short-wave radiation

Random

Doe Before

Doe After

I . I t 1 , f . I t

0 1 2 3 4 5 1

Mean visibility score

Figure18. Meanvisibilityof eachpolesection at 15m and my height (1.5 m) for doe and random sites. I = 0-20%, 2 = 21-40%,3 = 41-60%, 4 = 61-8096,s = 81-100%.

Table 9. Three miaeclimate parameters estimated for does on the Thompson Plateau.

Factors n Range Mean SE

Air Temperature (C) 81 1.0-28.0 14.8 0.6

Wind Speed (rn/sec) 42 0.0-8.0 0.9 0.3

Light Intensity (lux) 31 15,000- 50,000 12,000 251,000

Wind speed (m/sec)

Figure 19. Frequency distribution for values of wind speeds recorded in 1988 (n = 42).

100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550

Short wave radiation (W/m *)

Figure 20. Frequency distribution for values of short-wave radiation recorded in 1989 (n = 31).

Air temperature

Operative temperature

Temperature (C)

Figure 21. Frequncy distributions of air temperature (n = 81) and operative temperature estimates for 1988 (when short-wave radiation was assumed to be 200 W/rn2) and for 1989 (when wind speed was assumed to be zero.

7 1

were dfulated from recorded light intensities using equations in Appendix 1

from Parker and Giklingharn (1986) assuming that a l l measurements were taken

at 1200 PST. Tkte equations used gave estimates of incident short-wave radiation

for standing, captive mule deer. Demarchi (unpubl. data) who measured global

radiation (which includes both long-wave and short-wave radiation) in an open

habitat in my study area reported 100-1000 ~ / m 2 from 1200-1400 PST. His

results confirmed that my values for short-wave radiation were reasonable.

I calculated estimates of operative temperature from air temperature,

wind speeds, and short-wave radiation, md compared them iu lower (+5.0 C)

and upper (+23.5 C ) aitid operative temperatures for adult mule deer in

summer pefage (Parker and Rdbbins 19&Q, Parker and Gillingham 1%).

Operative temperature describes the effective temperature experienced by a doe

by incorporating the effects of air temperature, wind speed and indent radiation

(Parker and GIUingham 1986). Because neither wind speed nor Iight intensity

were measured during both field seasons, the operative temperatures are only

approximate estimates. T'he operative temperatures for the 1989 dataset were

calculated for a zero wind speed which was the most ~OIIUIZD~ condition in 1988

(fig- 191 and in 1990 (Dernarchi, pers. anrm.), and the operative temperatures for

the 1988 dataset were estimated with 200 ~ / m 2 which was the mode of the 2989

short-wave radiation distribution W I ~ . 20).

In both 1388 and 1 W 8 there were d y 11 occasions of 81 when the

operative temperature fell outside the thermoneutral zone. On all of these

od=catSiofls operative temperatures were higher than the upper critical operative

teml~peraiture, and does were bedded on 10 of these 11 OCCaSiOm. Tfiere were

limitatio~ to the accnrav of opgative temperature estimates because they were

dculated h r standing adults. Values for aperative temperahues would have

Un ni least 8646 of the occasions, does used sites where operative

temperature was within the thennoneutral zone. However, it is not known if

the sites used were used for their moderate micro-climate conditions because I

neglected to measure m i a h t e conditions at random sites. It is possible that

the sites used experienced similar miaeclimates to that which was available

randomly, and no selection for sites with thermal cover occurred. Examination

of mown dosure at random and doe sites provided further insight into the

potential use of thermal cover.

Although my sample size was small with only 9 occurrences of rain, there

was no indication that rain affkted habitat selection during summer. I used a 2-

way multi-sarnple median test with rain occurrence and habitat type (n = 81, df =

4, M = 7.6, P P 0.05), d I-tailed 2-sample Mann-Whitney U test of deer model

visibility at 15 m and coyote height with rain occurrence (n = 79, U = 302.5, P >

0105). In both tests, the results were insignificant.

Crown Closure

The quality of thermal cover is commonly indexed by percent crown

damre (Black et al. 1976,l,edcenby et al. 1982, Thomas et d. 1986, Lyon 1987,

Smith and Long 19873. The average crown dosllres of sites used by does and

mdon sites are ckscdxd in Table 10- In comparing a m closufs at sites used

by does with that at random sites with 2 Sfactor ANOVAs, the only significant

main &kt was deer identifimrion (n = 75, Qf = 2, P < 0.007) for bottt runs. There

were no sigdkmt inferactions for the ANUVA wing site type, deer

identification and repradwtiwe status (RI = 75, df = 2, P > 0.05). Ali 3 fa-s

in- with each other for the ANOVA using site type8 deer identification

andactkvitytype(n=75,df=2,P<0.007). Fc~3of11daes,a0~md~~1rewas

greater at bedding s&s than at Ilon-bedding sites, while fbr 5 of 11 does, the

Table 10. Mean percent crown dosure for sites used by does and random 73 sites. Site types: R = random, D = doe, Bf = before parturition, A = after parturition, NBd = non-bedding, and Bd = bedding.

Type of Site n Range (%) Mean (%) SE (%)

DBf 23 0 - 70 23.0 4.8

7 4

aown dosure at bedding sites was less than that at non-bedding sites. For the

remaining 3 does, the crown closure at bedding sites was approximately equal to

that for non-bedding sites. Adequate thermal cover has been defined by different

researchers as different minimum crown closure values (6096, Black et al. 1976;

70%, Thomas et al. 1986, Lycm lW, Smith and Long 1987; 752, Black et ai. 1976,

Leckenby et d. 1982). Crown closure at random sites and at sites used by does

was siWmtly different from &I%, 7046, and 75% (random, t-test., n = 150, t =-

128, P < 0.05; doe, t-test, n = 150, t =-12.8, P < 0.05).

Human Disturbance

There was a large amount of variation in distance to the nearest road as

indicated in Table 11. The only significant main effect for 2 3-way ANOVAs was

doe identification (n = 75, df = 2, P > 0.051, and there were no significant

interactions. The factors used were doe identification, site type and either

activity type or reproductive status. A I h g h does behaved differently from

each other when using sites with respect to distance to the nearest road, there

was no indication that site selection by active or inactive does was influenced

before or after parhuitim by distmce of sites to the nearest mad.

Site choice d d have been influenced by the availability and quality of a

number of essential reso- food, waterI a m dosureI visual cover, and

mer. bxitr:'; 4 si ts to areas of htrman activity couid have

idlueQcced w k t i m of sites by does. Although I expeaed that both food and

visual cover rescrur.-es to be major inawnces on site selectionI tire results of my

analyses shamed o t k w k In fact, none of the basic resources mentioned above

Table 11. Mean distance to the nearest road (m) for sites used by does and 75 random sites. Site types: R = random, D = doe, Bf = before parturition, A = after parturition, NB; = non-bedding, and Bd = Mding.

Site Type n Range (m) Mean (m) SE (m)

R 75 0-2000 271 41

D 81 5-1800 31 6 40

DBf 23 5-1 500 277 73

DA 58 1 0-1 800 332 49

DNBd 25 5-1 500 271 74

DBd 56 10-1 800 336 48

OBfBd 12 30-900 319 76

DABd 44 10-1 800 342 58

76

appeared to strongiy influence diurnal site selection in late spring and summer.

The results contained considerable variation that was largely due to the different

behaviour of individuals.

Forage Resources

I could not test my prediction that deer used sites with less visuai cover

when percent cover d forbs associated with these areas was greater than that

d a t e d with areas with more visual cover because there was no apparent

trade-off between percent cover of forb and visual cover (or crown closlue).

Although open habitats may have had a higher average percent cover of forb

h did furest habitats8 &me was great variation in percent cover of forb within

each habitat type, and no signifbnt relationship betweem visual cover and food

resouces. Other researchers found that forage was more abundant in open

habitats than forest Wtats (Wallmo et al. lm, Collins and Umess 1983). In

Utah, C d h s and Urnes (1983) found a high percent cover of forage in aspen

forests and in open habitas such as dear-cuts. In Washingtun8 forage was most

abundant in stands less than 20-years-dd (Hanley 1984). Atthough I expected to

find a significant relationship between both horizontal visibility and crown

cbmre and percent cover of forbs8 it appears that these site characteristics were

not accurate predicbors of foabs It appears that it was not necessary for does to

between forage resources and aver r s a m e s when they selected sites.

Furthennope, there is no evidence that does were influenced by percent

cover of forbs when dwashg sites within their home ranges. Does were not

using sites with higher percent awer of forbs than that at random sites. There

a r e a t k a s t 3 r r x p l a n a ~ i o r t h i s ~ First,doesmaynothavehad todect

sites with high percent am- of f& because forb were not a iimited resource

a d were readily availaMe in sammer. WhiEe many re-cparckers correlated forb

7 7

availability with habitat seiecticm by deer in summer (Wallmo et d. 1972, Collins

and Umess 1983, W e y 1984, Harestad 1985, b f t et al. 1986, Ordway and

Krausman 1986, l3uduttha et al. 1989, Griffith and Peek 1989), d y Clary and

Larson (1971) found that the distribution of mule deer was not correlated to

forage abundance.

Second, other measures of quality and availability of food may have been

more appropriate. Forb quality, plant phenology and the distniution of

particular forage species may have influenced site selection, but I did not

examine these factors. There may be a more appropriate way of measuring forb

availability although the method I used had been used by other researchers

WEUms et ai. 1980, Harestad 1985). Forb availability also can be measured by

dipping and drying forbs within sample quadrants (Wallmo et al. 1972, Collins

and UIJL~SS 1983) or by cmmhg total number of plants per species or per forage

type in sample areas to calculate density (Grover and Thompson 1986, Urdway

and Krausman 1986)

Third, it may be that percent aver of f o r b is not an important factor in

choos'tng bedding sites which 709& of the sites that I idenW were. Percent

cover of fastlw at bedding sites were diffaent than that at non-bedding sites used

by deer, but rsot significantly di f ferent It is not known if does feed at or near

their Idding sitesI but it w d be ewqpthdy efficient if food resources were

h M d at or near their bedding sites. Several resear- described structural

chmaddst i~ of beddirrg siw lrsed by juvenile ungulates (Walher 1968, Fichter

1974, qrrah 1974, AutenrEsth 9nd Fichter I97?5, Auhmrieth 1976, Bmdey 1977,

Barrett 1981, Tucks and Ga;mer 1983, GaiUard and Ddonae 1989, and Alldredge

et d.1990, but none of these shtdies described the forage resou~as ai bedding

sites. E o a d a n d ~ c o ( r r e r ~ h a v e n o t ~ d ~ f o r b e d d i n g s i t e s

of adult unrgulstes;.

Water Resources

As indicated by significant interactions between individual deer and both

activity type and reproductive status, there was some evidence that site selection

by does was influend by the distribution of water. Behaviour varied betweex?

individuals; some individuals used sites close to water white others did not.

Local distributions of mule deer in deserts of the southwest US. (and possibly

Idaho, Griffith 1983) are determined by the distribution of water (Clark 1953,

Swank 1958, Vdood et al- 1970, Truett 1972, Ordway and Kritusman 1986). This is

a geographic phenomenon a d not the case in Montana (Mackie 1970) and

unlikdy to be true in my study area. The mean distance to water from the

locations where doe mule deer were found in Arizona (1.14 km + 0.03 SE)

(Ordway and Krausman 1986) is much greater than that for the Thompson

PIateau (0.13 km + 0.02 SE)- The same is true for the mean distances to water

from randomly bciited points, (Arizona, 1.28 km + 0.05 SE; Thompson Plateau,

0.14 km + 0.02 SE), which indicates that water souras are more common and

dispersed in my study area than in Arizona's desert. My study area was located

on a plateau -which has many small depressim that collect water and form lakes

and aeeks of various shes.

Some does may have been attracted to areas near water for reasons other

than access to chinking water. Deer do not rreciessarily need access to drinking

water because su&t fa- a n provide enough water to meet their

meOlbalic TeqzLireLztents d deer on mwt ranges ( w d h 1981). A&huugh does

did not s b w a prefeemce for ripariaa habitafs when choosing sites within their

bane rangeI Simpson a d Gyug (1991) who studied the same radio-c:*ed does

ckmmstrated drat dses used summer home ranges with a higher frequency of

~ k a b i t a t s t h a n t h a t w h i c h w a s a ~ e i n t k s t u d y a r e k Greateruseof

a r e e ~ n e a a w a t e r o ~ ~ b a s i n s h a s b e e n o b s e r v e d e i s e w ~ , ~ i ~ n a t u r e

79

of h e relationship betwem habitat choice and proximity to water is unclear

(Patton and Judd 1970, Black et al. 1976). Riparian habitats have several

characteristics that ma). appeal to does: dense visual cover, abundant forbs, and

w i n g or standing water (which provide olfactory cover by masking scents).

Other researchers found that does preferred habitats near water which had an

abundance of succulent forage in summer (Kauffman and Krueger 1!284, Gillen et

al. 1985, Loft et al. 1986, Carson and Peek 1987)- Collins and Umess (1983)

coffefated availability of high quaiity p h t s with high water tables of wet

meadows. Leckenby et a1 (1982) suggested that riparian areas are especially

important during fawn-rearing because all the basic resouras (Le. thermal cover,

security cover, sudent foragef and water) are concentrated within them.

Several researchers have mted the use of water by deer for escape cover

(Barkaiow and Kekr 1950, lhmmut and Taber 1956, Pimlott et al. 1%9, Sweeney

et id- 19n). Does king chased by dogs used a variety of escape patterns, one of

whichwasrmningthn,ughwata. h38of4Qchaseswhezethedeerusedthis

strategy, the dogs lost d&s &ail (Sweeniqr et al. 1971). In summaryf it is unlikely

that access to drinking water was important fix does in my study areaf but it may

have been important for does to have access to riparian habitats which offer a

variety of ~esources~ "Rmse doa that did not wse riparian habitats probably had

Im quality home ranges that did not indude riparian habitats.

v w covtr Individual deer differed in their use of visual aova- Sane deer used sites

with more visual mver than was present at random sites, while other deer used

sites with less visual ama than at random si-. Activity type influenced site

selection by does through an inters- with doe identity, but reproductive

status did not influence dte s e k c t i ~ ~ . This r d t was incunsistent with my

80

alternate hypothesis that does would use sites with dense visual cover when the

consequences of predation were high. I can offer severid possible explanations

for this inconsistency.

First, if the vulnerability of does to predation was low both before and after

parturition, and if the selection of sites by does was not tightly linked to fawn

survival then I expect no difference in the visual cover at the sites used before

and with that at sites used after parturition. There is some evidence that fawn

survival is correlated with use of visual cover at bedding sites (Kjos and

Montgomery 1969, Robinette et al. 1973, Bromley 1978, Dood 19713, Sheehy, 1978,

Smith and Mount 1979, Barrett 1981, Riley 1982, Bowyer and Bleich 1984), but

bedding sites are chosen by fawns not their dams &insdale and Tomich 1953,

Eant 1974). However, a dam may influence choice of bedding sites by its fawns in

potentially 2 ways. First, dams choose a general bedding area for its fawns before

they choose bedding sites &indate and TOlltjch 1953, Lmt 1974). Second, a fawn

may be influenced by its dam's site choices through habitat imprinting.

Pronghorn fawns are found to use bedding sites with simiIar vegetation cover to

that at their birth sites which were chosen by their dams mchter 15)74). The same

~ v i o u r m a ) . o c c u r i n m u l e k *

Second, it is possible that some deer use visual VET while others do not;

deer may use m e r k~ a facultative way. Use of cover may be a behaviour that is

individd or SiaUationspeclfic Same individuals may prefer to use open sites

that aIlav them to be visually vigilant and detect predators quickly. In this

situation, the doe has ample time to decide when to flee and seek visual cover,

whenas other individuals prefer being inconspicuous at sites with visual cover

whae detection by predakcm (and of predators) is more difficult visually. This

maybetruewhether~av~isbeingusedprimarilyforswrurityornot, Ye0

and Peek (I=) suggested that dense vita& cover is not neaessarily better &an

8 1

sparse visual cover. They identified a e b I e tradeoff for prey that use visual

cover: when visual mver is dense then prey may not be detected by predators but

prey may not be able to deteet predators and an ambush may result This trade-

off could explain differences in doe behaviour.

In a study of alarm and flight responses of whitetailed deer in Florida,

Mary (1987) found that deer in dense vegetation fled even when a predator was

at a considerable distance. He hyp&w&d that deer exhibited this behaviour

because of the danger of losing sight of the predator. He further suggested that

deer sensed greater vulnerability to predators in dense vegetation, presmbly

because of the greater danger of surprise attack. Deer in forests seemed to

compensate for this increased danger by fleeing even when a predator was seen

at distances greater than 100 m. Consequently, solitary deer in the forest seemed

mare alert than those in bie pasfam Deer in open habitats could mnsenre

energy by mt attempting @ escape while the predator was at a s& distance.

AUdredge et aL (1991) found that w e n t pronghorns and fawns used denser

s h b cover than what was available at random sites, but the Mest, mat dense

coyer in their study area was avoided-

Third, it a h is possibk that visual over is so abundant in summer,

espedally for an animal that evdved on epm phim Gekt 19741, that most sites

cam meet the deer's cover mpimments- Perhaps selectian fix areas with visual

mis l x r c lu~ at the hame range level r a w than the site heL The random sites

that I used were randw\ within home ranges and therefore not truly randomly

1oc;ated. Sinrpn and Gyug (1991) evaluated summer hone range &ectictn by

mule deer does by comparing use and availability of various habitat types. They

found that does preferred riparian habitats and 2 immature hest types: spruce

alpine fir and mixed decidwus evergreen. Riparian habitats offer g o d visual

cover while immature folrests provide Illoderate visuaI cover <e Chapter 2).

82

While these use and availability comparisons suggest some preference for use of

visual cover at the home range level, this type of analyses have limited value

because of inherent biases against common habitats and biases for rare habitats

( J o h n 1980).

Fourth, visuai a v e r may have a stronger influence on site selection than

what is indicated by m y results because of a lack of power in my analyses. Small

sample sizes (i.e. less than 10 sites per doe) increase the probability that a null

hypothesis is wrongly accepted. Also, it may be that visual cover as I measured i t

is not equivalent to how deer perceive or use security cover. Auditory or

olfactory cover may be structurally different from visual cuver and more

important.

While several researchers have measured visual cover in ungulate

habitats (Nudds 1977, Riley 1982, Canfield et al. 1986, b f t et al. 1987, Smith and

b n g 1987, Griffith and Youtie 1988, Servheen and Lyon 1989, Yeo and Peek 1989,

Simpson and Gyug f992), no other study has compared visual cover at sites used

by deer to that at random sites located within home ranges. It has been observed

that habitat use of an area by deer increases when visual cover is available (Short

et aL 1977, Loft and M d c e 1984, toft et al. 1987). Bladc et al. (1976) suggested that

d t y cover may be required even in absence of predation risk if hLU use of a

habitat is to occur, implying psychological need. Dasmann and Taber (1956)

observed that adult h e s used dense brush during parturition and during the

first weeks of the fawn's life, while bucks fed in the open at this time. Yeo and

Pa& (1989) measwed visual cover at random sites within core areas and in non-

care areas within home ranges of Hack-tailed deer. Their d t s indicate that

lrisual cover was anfy in one habitat type when t h y ampared core

areas with mnum areas Visual mer was 16% greater within axe areas than

w i t h i n I u m ~ a r e a s o f ~ h r u b d ~ t a t

83

Aithough several researchers related LW of visual cover by fawns to fawn

survival by inferring that use of visual cover reduces predation risk and

vulnerability (Kjos and Montgomery 1969, Robinette et al. 1973, Brornley 1978,

Dood 1978, Sheehy, 1978, Smith and LeCount 1979, Barrett 1981, Riley 1982,

Bowyer and Bleich 1984), no one has attempted to correlate the use of visual

cover by adults to adult survival. Consequently, I am not convinced that does

are influenced by visual cover in their site selection within their home ranges.

Perhaps, visual cover is vital to fawns at the site selection level and important to

does at the home range selection ievel. If this is true, habitat managers should

focus on the understory component of visual cover which is important for fawn

bedding sites rather than conifer overstory. S h b s are a significant component

of visual cover for adult ungulates as well (see Chapte~ 2).

Universal use of the definition of adequate visual cover proposed by

Th- et al. (1979) may be inadequate and inappropriate. This definition is

inadequate because it gives no idonnation regarding quality of visual cover. It

only indicates whether visual cover is present or not Ah, 1 find that it is

inappropriate in my study area because the mean minimum distance for 10%

visibility of a standing dea IIbOdel at sites used by does was 24 m which is

sigdhntly less 61 m. Simpson and Gyug (1991) also found that the minimum

distancefbr 10% visibilittybfastandingperson) wasIessthan30mat78% of all

sites used by does. Yeo d Peek O W ) suggest that W% standing visual cover at

1ommaybeoptimaL LedEenbyetaL(1982)usedasimilarcriteriatoThomaset

aL (1979 that was Npposedly mare ampropriabe for their study area. in the Great

Basin Theyusedasightdis~of45mratherthan61 mandappliedittothe

view04abeddeddgerntherthanastandingW. Isuggestthatvisualwver

requitemen6 are species and geographicalIy specific Adapte visual cover as

-by ThomasetaL ~1979)maybevalidforeIkh theBlueMountainsof

8 4

Oregon and Washington, but not for deer in areas of denser vegetation like parts

of Alaska and Brtish Columbia.

Micro-Climate Conditions and Crown Closure

Although there is no conclusive evidence that does used visual cover, I

have examined the possibility that visual cover was used as thermal cover rather

than security cover. Peek et al. (1982) proposed that thermal cover is preferred

but not required by mule deer in summer when heat is extreme. kkenby et al.

(1982) noticed that forest stads that did not provide thermal or security mver

were rarely used during the day. Several investigators observed that habitat

selection can be influenced by micro-chate conditions (Dasmam 1954, Darling

1964, Loveless 1964, Staines 1976, Boyd 1981, Collins and Umess 1983, Brindley et

aL 1989)- Miao-climate conditions at most sites used by does were moderate in

terms of air temperature, wind, short-wave radiation, and rainfall which may

have resulted from moderating effects of thermal cover or may have indicative

of the climate of the general area.

There appears to be less than adequate thermal cover (as indexed by crown

closure> at both random sites and doe sites. There are 3 possible explanations for

tfre moderate conditions at sites used by does when crown dosure was low. First,

the climate in the general area was moderate and selection of sites with dense

a m ~~ was ll~~~ecessary. Second, sparse crown closures were sufficient

to provide t h d coyer. There is some dis<rrepancy as to which minimum

value of crow clamre provides adequate thermal cover in summer. Several

authors have defined adequate thermal cover as coniferous forests with an

average cravn ctusure exceding 70% and t n s at least 12 m Kgh (Wmas et al.

1986, Lyon 1987, Smith and Lcmg IP87) while other re~ea~chers used a greater

75% crown dosure dterhn for trees or shrubs taller than IS m (Black et ale

8 5

1976, Ledamby et at 1982). Black et aL (1976) also stated that for forest stands at

the sapling stage or older, a crown dasure of 60% provided adequate thermal

cover in summer. Third, understory vegetation provided ~~1 cover which

moderated the micrQ-climates at sites. Taber (1%1) acknowledged that

understory vegetation provides thermal cover. Dense aown closures do not

seem especially necessary to maintain high populations of mule deer because

topography and lower vegetation can act as thermal cover (Peek et al. 1982). I am

unable to determine whether thanaI cover was required by does in my study

area during summer, but I would suggest that the definition c' thermal cover

should not be limited to a standard aown closure value. The influence of

understory vegetation on rniao-climates should be widely acknowledged.

Jnsect Cover

It has been noticed by several researchers that harassment by insects can

influence habitat selection by ungulates (Darling 1964, Collins and Urness 1982,

Jakimchuk et al. 1987, B ~ d l e y et al. 1989). When harassed by insects, reindeer

and red deer seek open, efevated, windy places (Darling 19641, while feral goats

retreat to shady areas (Brindey et al. 1989). I did not measure levels of insect

harassment? but of sites used by does only 21% were in open habitats. Although

it is p s i b l e that insect harassment influend site selection by does, it is

unlikeiy that it was a major influence.

Disturbance

The distance from sites to nearest roads did not influence site selection.

Because the density of roads in my study area was relatively high, does may not

have had the opportunity to avoid roads. Deer may use visual cover to avoid

disturbane rather than avoiding habitats adjacent to roads. Habitat selection

86

strategies of elk appear to he based primarily on the availability of fwd and

hwnan bisturkce Peek et aL 1982). V d cover seems to be a requirement for

dk in the presence of human disturbance (Peek et al. 1982). Visual cover affects

responses of elk to hunting and road activities Perry and Overly 1976, Basile and

Lonner 1979, Irwin and Peek lm, Lyon 1979). Three researchers reported use of

visual cover by d- on north dopes during hunting season (Wilkins 1957,

lavaas 1958, and Madtie 1970), although they did not indicate whether deer used

visual cover outside of hunting season.

CONCLUSIONS

Density of visual cover and minimum distance to water influenced site

selection by doe muie deer, but perent cover of forb did not Does were not

confronted with a choice between food and visual cover (or aown closure)

because there was no apparent trade-off between these resources. The influence

of visual cover and water availability on site selection was ambiguous because of

variation in the behaviour between individuals. Although does did not show a

preference for riparian habitats at the site selection level of habitat choice,

Sipson and G p g (1W1) showed that does d riparian habitats more

frequently than what would be predicted from the availability of riparian habitats

when choosing their summer h e ranges. I have offered a number of

explanations for variation in doe behaviour with respect to visual cover. 1 was

unable to determine if either visual cover or crown closure were wed as thermal

cover- It is possible that does required insect cover, but I did not measure levels

of insect harassment Does did not appear to be influenced by disturbance from

roads in choosing sites.

I question universal use of the definition of adequate visual cover

87

proposed by Thomas et aL (19'7%. I find that 61 m is too long to be appropriate as

a standard sight dis-ce value for my study area. Perhaps it is inappropriate to

describe security mver which is functionally and structurally complex with a

single sight distance vatue. I suggest that more attention should be focussed on

the value of undmtory vegetation as visual cover for 2 reasons. First, because it

is more likely that fawns require visual cover than do adults and because fawns

primarily use understory structure for visual cover, understory vegetation

should be managed for its value as visual cover. Second, shrubs are structurally

an important component of visual cover for adults (see Chapter 2).

(zmwmR4

CONCLUSIONS AhTD MA3TAGEMENT RECOMMENDATIONS

In evaluating the use of 3 apparatuses to estimate visual cover (profile

bcrard, profile pole and deer model), I conclude that the profile pole is the most

versatile and convenient apparatus for measuring visual cover. The following

set of procedures for detecting differences in visual cover among habitats. Visual

cover can be measured at the observer's natural height because there is no

advantage to kneeling at coyote height For mule deer in my study area, the

optimal distance for measuring visibility is 15 m. The faurth (0.75-1.00 m) and

fifth section (1.00-1.25 m) are the best pole sections to measure visibility at for

conditions encountered in my study area for adult mule deer. I recommend that

lower sections of the pole be d when evaluating habitat use by fawns. There

is no significant advantage for the observer to stand upslope, downslope or

aaoss-slope from the apparatus when estimating visual cover.

Unspad sapling forests provided the densest visual cover while open

habitats provided the sparsest visual cover. Shrub, spaced sap;ng, immature

forest and mature forest habitats were intermediate in density of visual cover.

The important vegetation components of visual cover were: crown closure,

percent cover of conifer trees in the top height strata, percent cover of conifers in

the shrub layer, total percent cover of non-conifers in the shrub layer, percent

cover of dedduous trees in the middle height strata, diameter (dbh) of deciduous

trees of the middle height strata, and percent cover of decid~~)us trees in the

bottam height strata Mod& of visual cover use only characteristics of the

mnifmus overstory and ignore the contribution of deciduous and understory

vegetation to visual cover. This amision should be rectified.

The importance of Security mer to wildlife has long been acknowledged

89

1933, Egler 1m, 'I'bmas et d. lm, Leckenby et al. 1982. DeByle 1985). I

cannot claim that does required visual cover, although the distribution of visual

cover did influence site selection by does differently. Likewise, Wallmo and

Schoen (1980) found that while the structure of coniferous forests influenced

habitat selection, there was no evidence indicating that deer required forests (as

thermal and visual cover) for survival. Peek et al. (1982) suggested that thermal

cover is preferred but not required by mule deer in summer when heat is

extreme and when forage becomes desiccated. However, visual cover may be

required in the presence of human activity (Peek et al. 1982).

Perhaps, the distribution of visual cover influenced does more in their

selection of summer home ranges rather than in their se1don of sites within

summer home ranges. 5mm.I segregation and differential habitat use by

ungulates in summer is proposed to be a result of antipredator strategies by

maternal females (Skigas and Flinders 1980, Edwards 1983, Jakimchuk et al.

1987, Festa-Bianchet 1988). In these 3 studies, security cover was not visual cover

provided by vegetation but predator-free habitats in isolated areas. The

importance of visual cover in site selection is probably greater for young (that are

"hider" types like mule deer (Geist 1981)) than for adult ungulates. Several

researchers have correlated survival of young with the distribution and structure

of understory vegetation (Kjus and Montgomery 1%9, Robinettc et al. 1973,

Bromley 1978, Dood 1978, Sheehy, 1978, Smith and M o u n t 1979, Barrett 1981,

Riley 1982, Bowyer and BMch 1984). Because understory vegetation influences

survival of young and because it is an important component of visual cover for

adult deer, understory vegetation should be included in management plans and

for models of visual cover.

The universal application of the definition of adequate visual cover

pmposed by Thomas et aL (1979) seems inappropriate. Sixty-one meters is too

90 - . - long for dscmnmathn among habitats, a distance of 15 m is more appropriate

for this function for mule deer in my study area The mean minimum distance

for 10% vki'biility at sites used by dws was significantly less than 61 rn. It may be

neceSSafy to empirically redefine adequate visual cover for different species and

ecosystems like Leckenby et al. (1982) did for their mule deer study in the Great

Basin. However, security cover is functionally and structurally complex and it is

unlikeIy that a single sight distance value can describe security or visual aver

adequateIy.

Aildredge, A. W., R D. Deblinger, and J. Peterson. 1991. Birth and fawn bed site selection by pronghorns in a sagebrushsteppe community. J. Wildl.

Manage. 55222-227,

Autenrieth, R E. 1976. A study of birth sites selected by pronghorn does and the M sites of fawns. Pmc Bienn. Pronghorn Antdope Workshop 2127-134.

, and E. Fichter. 1m. On the behavior and socialization of pronghorn fawns. Wildl. Monogr. 42 llpp.

Barblow, F. S., Jr., and W. E. H e r . 1950. Escape behavior of the white-tailed deer. J. Wildl. Manage 1k246-247.

Barrett, M- W. 1981. Environmental characteristics and functional significance of pronghorn fawn bedding sites in Alberta. J. Wild. Manage 4S:l2O-l3l.

Basile, J. V., and T. N. Lomer. 1979. Vehicle restrictions influence elk and hunter distribution. Mont. J. For. 77355-159.

Beckr, D. Q, F. L. Bu.mdl, D. W. Janz, J. B. Nyberg, and E. L. Richardson. 1990. Techniques for managing habitat. Pages 133-195 in J. B. Nyberg and D. W. Janz, tech- eds. Deer and elk habitats in coastal forests of southern British

Columbia B.C. Minist For. Res. Branch Spec. Rep. Ser. 5. 310pp.

Black, H., R ScherzInger, and J. W. Thomas. 1976. Relationships of Rocky Mountain Elk and Rocky Mountain mule deer habitat to timber

management in the Blue Mountins of Oregon and Washington. Fages 11-31 in J. M. Peek and S. R Hieb, eds. Proc Elk-logging-roads Symp., Vniv. Idaho For. Wildl Range Exp. Stn.

BodPntha, T. S, j. M. Peek, and J. L Lam. 1989. Mule deer habitat use related to succession in a bcfigrass uxnmunity. J. Wid. Manage. 53314319-

Bowyer, R T-, and V. C. BIe id~ 1 9 4 . Weds of cattle grazing on selected habitats of southern mule deer. Calif. Fish Game 7&240-247.

Boyd, 1. L. 1981. Population changes and distribution of a herd of feral goats on Rhum, hner Hebrides 1960-78. J. Zool., Lond. 193287-304.

Brindley, E. L., D. J. Bullodr, and F. Maisels. 1989. Effects of rain and fly harassment on the feeding behavim of free-ranging feral goats. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2431-41.

B. C. Ministry of Fofests. 1!%8. Biogedimatic zones of British Columbia 1988. Map B.C. Minist For., Victoria, B.C.

Bromley, P. T. 1977. Aspects of the behavioural ecology and sociobiology of the pronghorn (Anf i lmup ameriuzna ). Ph.D. Thesis, Univ. Calgary, Alta.

370.8~.

. 1978. Ultimate factors, behavis-w and fawn recruitment. Proc Bienn. Pronghorn Antdope Workshop &449-471.

Bunneil, F. L., and D. J. Vales- 1990. Comparison of methods for estimating forest overstory cover: Me~ences among techniques. Can. J. For. Res. U3:IOI-107.

Canfield, J. E., L J. Lyan, and J. M. Hillis. 1986. The influence of viewing angle on elk hiding cover in young timber stands. U.S.D.A. For. Serv. Kes. Pap. INT-371. 5pp.

Carson, R G., and J M. Peek 1987. Mule deer habitat selection patterns in northcentral Washington. J. Wddl. Manage. 51:46-51.

Clark, E. D. 1953- A study of the behavior and movements of the Tucson Mountain mule deer. MS. Thesis, Univ. Arizona, Tucson. lllpp.

Ctary, W. P-, and F. R Latson. l97l. Elk and deer use ars related to food

93 resources in Arizona ponderosa pine. U.S.D.A. For. Serv. Res. Note M- 202 4pp.

Clover, M- R 2954. Single gate deer trap. Calif. Fish Game 40:367-373.

Collins, W. B., and P. J. Urnen. 19&2 Mule deer and elk responses to horsefly attacks. Northwest Sci. 56:299-302

A and - . 1983. Feeding behavior and habitat selectim of mule deer and eik on northern Utah summer range. J. Wild. Manage. 47646-663.

Darling, F. F. 1964. A herd of red deer: a study in the animal behaviour. Fourth ed- Oxford University Press, London.

Dasmann, R F. 1954. Emlogy and social behavior of a population of the Columbian black-tailed deer in California. Ph.D. Thesis, Univ. California, Berkeley. 155pp.

, and R D. Taber. 19%. Behavior of Colunnbian black-tailed deer with reference to population ecology. J. Mammal. 32143-164.

DeByle, N. V. 1985. Wildlife. Pages 135-152 in N. V. DeByle and R. P. Winokur, eds. Aspen: -logy and management in the western United States.

U.S.D.A. Fbr. Sem. Gen. Tech. Rep. RM-119.

Demarchi, M. W. personal communication. Faculty d Forestry, Univ. British Columbia.

Dood, k R 1978- Summer movements, habitat use, and mortality of mule deer fawns in the Missouri River Breaks, Montana. Mont. Dep. Fish Game Fed. Aid Job F d Rep. Mont. Proj. W-120-R-8,9, Job No. 9 . 5 5 ~ ~ .

Egler, F- E. 1977- Tlw n a v e of vegetation, its management and mismanagement: an introduction to vegetation science. Frank E. Egier, Norfolk, Conn. 527pp-

Elton, C. 1939. On the nature of cover. J. WW. Manage. 3532-338.

Festa-Bianm, M. 1988. SeasonaI range dxtion in bighorn sheep conflicts between forage Quaifity, forage quantity and predator avoidance. Owologia

?!kts&o-586.

Fichter, E. 1974. On the bedding bebviou~ of p~cfnghorn fawns. Pages 352-355 in V. GeiSt and F- R Wafther, eds. The beha- of ungulates and its relation to management. Vot I. IUCN New Series Publ. 24. Morges, Switzerland.

Gaiilard, J. M., and D. Delonne- 1989. Seleebn des sites de repas par les W e s

chevreuils (Coprwhw urprwk 1. Acta Oecologica 20:421-418.

Geist, V. 1974. On the relationship of social dutim and d o g y in ungulates. Am. &I. 14905-220-

- 1981. Behavior adaptive strategies in mule deer. Pages 157-223 in 0. C. W h o , ed- Mule deer and black-tailed deer of North America. Univ. Nebraska Press, Lincoln. 605pp.

Gillen, R L, W. C. Krueger, and R F. Miller. 1985. Cattle use of riparian meadows in the Hue Mountains of northeastern Oregon. J. Range Maltage. 38:sm.

Griffith, B., and J- M. Peek 1989. Mule deer use of sera1 stage and habitat type in bittttrbntsh communi~e- J. WddL Manage. 53636-642.

, and B. A, Youtie 1 s . Two ckvices for estimating foliage density and deer hiding cover. WM. SIC. BuIl- 16-206-210.

Griffith, D. S. 1983. E t ~ ) k @ d ~ ~ C S of mule k. aaters of the Moon

9 5 National Monument, Idaho. Univ. Idaho Coop. Park Stud. Unit, Moscow.

w?'. Grover, K E. and M. J- TRompson- 1986. Factors influencing spring feeding site

selection by elk in the Elkhum Mountains, Montana. J. Wildl. Manage. 50.466-470.

Gysel, L W., and L. J. Lyon. 1980. Habitat analysis and evaluation. Pages 305-327 in S. C. S d ~ d t z , ed. management techniques. Fourth ed. The Widlife Sodety, Washington, D.C.

Hanley, T. A. 1984. Habitat patches and tfi& selection by wapiti and black-tailed deer in a coastal montane coniferous forest. J. Appl. E d . 21:423-436.

Harestad, A. S. 1984. Seasonal movements of black-taiIed deer. Pages 403-409 in W. R Meehan, T. R Merrell, Jr. and T. A. Hanley, eds. Proc. Symp. on fish and wildlife relationships in old-gmwth forest. Am. Inst. Fish. Res. Biol.

- 1985. Habitat w by black-tailed deer on northern Vancouver Island. J. Wildl. Manage. 49946-950.

Irwin, L L, and J. M. Peek 1979. Relationship between road closures and elk behavior in northern W o . Pages 199-204 in M. S. Boy- and L. D. Hayden-Wing, eds. North American eik: ecology, behavior and management. Univ. Wyoming, Warnie.

Jakimchuk, R D., S. H. F&gman, and L. G. Sopuck. 1987. Differential habitat use and sexual segregation in the Central Arctic caribou herd. Can. J. Zool. 65-SM-541.

Jdmson, D. H. 1980. The comparison of usage and availability measurements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology 6 : M .

Kauffman, J. B., and W. C. Krueger. 1984. Livestock impacts on riparian easystems and &reamside management impiications ... a review. J. Range Minage 3E430-438.

K,&, C. G-, and G. G. M<mtpmery, 1%9. Daytime locations and bedsites of whitetailed deer fawns. Trans. Illinois State Acad. Sci. 621 17-119.

Krasowski, T. P., and T. D. Nudds. 1986. Mimhabitat structure of nest sites and nesting success of diving ducks. J. Wddl. Manage 9203-208.

Kr*, J- R, and N- B. Davies, eds. 1984- Second ed. Behavioural Ecology. Btaikwd Scientific, Oxford. 493pp.

Kufeld, R C., 0. C. W d h o 8 and C. Fetid- 1973. Foods of the Rocicy Mountain mule deer- US.D.A For. Serv. Res. Pap. RM-Ill. 31pp.

LaGay, IC E. 1987. The influence of habitat and group characteristics on the alarm and flight response of white-tailed deer. Anim. Behav. 3520-25.

Leckenby, D. A., D. P- Sheehy, C H. Nellis, R J. Scherzinger, L D. Luman, W. Elmore, J. C. Lemos, L Doughty, and C. E. Trainer. 1982 Mule deer. Wildlife habitats in managed rangelands - the Great Basin of southeast Oregon. U.S.D.A. For. Sem. Cen. Tech. Rep. PNW-139. 40pp.

Lent, P. C. 1974. Mother-infant relationships in ungulates. Pages 14-53 in V. Geist and F. R Walther, eds. The Maviour of ungulates and its relation to management Vol. 1. rUCN New Series Publ. 24. Morges, Switzerland.

-Id, A. 1933. Game managemt. Charles Scribner's Sons, New York, NY.

4 1 ~ ~ -

Linsdale, j. M=, and P. Q. T&ch 1953. A herd of mule deer: a record of

obseFvatioxs made on the Hastings Natural History Reservation. Univ. Calif. Press, l3erkeley. •̃ 67pp.

M, E R, and J. W- Mmke. 1984. DeeP use and habitat characteristics of transmission-he corridors in a Dou@as-fu forest. J. Wildl. Manage. 4&1311-1316.

I and J. 6. Kie 1%. fnteractkrn of cattle and deer an mountain rangeland. Cdif. AAgric 406-9-

, , . and R C- Bertraa 1987. Influence of cattle stocking rate on the structural profile of deer hiding cover. J. Wild. Manage. 51:655-661.

Lovaas, A. L 19%. Mule deer food habits and range use, Little Belt Mountains, Montana. J. WiU. Manage. 22275-283.

bveless, C. M. 1%4, Some relationships between wintering mule deer and the physical environment. Trans. North Am. Wildl. Nat. Resour. Conf. 29415-431,

Lyon, L J. 1979. Habitat &&v- for elk as influenced by roads and cover. J. For. 77:658-66U.

- 1987. Effects of thinning smallstem lodgepole pine stands on big game habitat. Pages 162-165 in R L. Garger, mpiler. Workshop Proc. Management of snall sfmn stands of ladgepole pine. U.S.D.A For. Serv. Gen. Tech. Rep. W-237.

MacArthur, R H, and J. W. MacArthu~~ 1961. On bird species diversity. Ecology 42594-598-

M'Qoskey, R T., and B. Fieldwick 1975. Ecological separation of sympatric rodents (Permyscus and Micro&). J. Mammal. 56:119-129.

MacHutchon, A G. 1988- Risk af predation and use of security cover and vigilance cover by foraging ~ I u m b i a n ground squirrels. U S Thesis, Simon Fraser UNV, Burnaby, B-C. 90pp.

, and A- S- Harestad- 1990. Vigilance behaviour and use of rocks by CoIumbian ground squirrels. Can. J, Zool. 6tk142&1432

McTague, J. P., and D. R Patton. 1989. Stamd density index and its application in

A&&, R J. 1970. Range ecology and relations of mule deer, elk, and cattle in the Missouri River Breaks, Montana. Wild. Monogr. 2 0 . 7 9 ~ ~ .

Mautz, W. W. 1978. Sledding on a bushy hillside: the fat cycle in deer. Wildl. Sac Bull. 6:8$-90.

Mob, C. 0. 1947. Table of equivalent populations of North American small mammals. Am, Midl. Nat. 37223-249-

Nudds, T. D. 19n. Quantifying the vegetative structure of wildlife cover. Wildl. Sac. Bull. 5113-117,

Ordway, L L., and P. R Kra- 1986. Habitat use by desert mule deer. J. WildI. Manage. W677-683.

Parkerf K. L, and M. P. G i g h a m . 1986. Estimating thermal environments for mule deer (Odclroiieus hemionus hemisnus ). B.C. Minist. For. Lands I&. B m c h WHR-24. 63pp.

. and C T. Wbifls. 1384. Thermoregulation in mule deer and elk. Can. J. Zeal. 621409-1422

Palton, D. R, and B- I- Jud& 1970. The roIe of wet meadows as wildlife habitat in the southwest J. Range Martage 23-272-275.

Peek, J. M, M D. ScottI L J- Ndsa, DD J. Piercef and L L Irwin. 1982. Role of amer in habitat miimagematt for big game in Northwestern United States. Trans, Nc&l Am, wild Conf- 47-372

P q , C, and R OvetEy- 1976. Impact of roads on big game distribution in Eportions of the Blue Mountains of Washington. Pages 62-68 in J. M. Peek and S. R Hieb, eds. Roc Elk-magging--roads Symp, Univ. Idaho For., WilaL, Range Exp. Stn,

Pimlott, D. H., J. A. Shannon, and G. B. Kolenosky. 1%9. The ecology of the

timber wolf in Algonquin Provincial Park Ontario Dep. Lands For. Res.

Rep. (Wildl.) 8 7 . 9 2 ~ ~ .

Pyrah, D. 1974. Antelope fawn bedding cover selection in central Montana.

Prm. Bienn. Pronghorn Antelope Workshop 6:113-121.

Recher, H. F. 1%9. Bird species diversity and habitat diversity in Australia and

North America. Am. Nat. 103:75-80.

Riley, S. J. 1982 Survival and behavior of radio-collared mule deer fawns during summers, 1978-1980, in the Missouri River breaks, Montana. M.S.

Thesis, Montana State Univ., Bozeman. S4pp.

Robinette, W. L., C. H. Baer, R E. Pillmore, and C. E. Knittle. 1973. Effects of nutritional change on captive mule deer. J. Wildl. Manage. 37:312-326.

Rawuweig, M. L., and J. Winakur. 1969. Population ecology of desert rodent communities: habitats and environemental complexity. Ecology 50:558- 572.

Schwartz, C. C., and N. T. Hobbs. 1985. Forage and range evaluation. Pages 25-51

in R J. Hudson and R G. White, eds. Bioenergetics of Wild Herbivores. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL.

Servheen, G., and L. J. Lyon. 1989. Habitat use by woodland caribou in the Sellcirk Mountains. J. WMl. Manage. 53:230-237.

Sheehy, D. P. 1978. Characteristics of shrubland habitat associated with mule deer fawns at birth and during early life in southeastern Oregon. Oreg. Dep. Fish Wad. Info. Rep. Ser. Wildl. No. 78-1. 31pp.

Short, EL L, W. Evans, and E. L h k e r . 1977. The use of natural and modified pinyon pinejuniper woodlands by deer and elk. J. Wildl. Manage. 41543-

559.

100 Simpson, K. 1988. Okanagan Connector Highway ungulate telemetry and

inventory. Consultant's report to the Minist. Environ. Parks. 25pp.

, and L. W. G p g . 1991. Environmental assessment of the Okanagan Connector Freway. Part iI. Pages 1-60 in Keystone Biersearch. Movements, habitats and status of moose and mule deer populations influenced by the Okanagan Comector Highway. Consultant's report to B.C. Minist. Highways and Minist. Environ.

Smith, F. W., and J. N. Long. 1987. Elk hiding and thermal cover guidelines in the context of lodgepole pine stand density. Western J. Appl. For. 2:6-10.

Smith, R H., and A. LeCount. 1979. Some factors affecting survival of desert mule deer fawns. f. Wildl. Manage. 43657-665.

Stains, B. W. 1976. The use of natural shelter by red deer in relation to weather in northeast Scotland. J. Zool., Land. 180:l-8.

Steigers, W. D. and J. T. Flinders. 1980. Mortality and movements of mule deer fawns in Washington. J. Wildl. Manage. 44381-388.

Strickland, D. 1975. Mule deer in the Medicine Bow Mountains, southeastern Wyoming. Wyo. Game Fish Dep. Wild. Tech. Rep. 2. 103pp.

Swank, W. G. 1958. The mule deer in Arizona chaparral and an analysis of other important deer herds: a research and management study. Ariz. Game Fish 9)ep. Wildif Bull. 3.109pp.

Sweeney, J. R, R L. Marchinton, and f. M. Sweeney. 19n. Responses of radio- monitored whitetailed deer chased by hunting dogs. J. Wildl. Manage. 35707-7l6.

Taber, R D. 1961. The black-tailed deer, a review of ecology and management. Terre Vie 2221-245.

Thomas, J. W., H. Black, Jr., It. J. Scherzinger, and R J. Pedersen. f 979. Deer and

101 elk. Pages 104-127 in J. W. Thomas, ed. Wildlife habiiats in managed forests - the Blue Mountains of Oregon and Washington. U.S.D.A. For. %IT. Agric. Handb. 9%. 512pp.

. D. A. W e n b y , L. J. Erickson, S- R Thomas, D. L. hacson, and R. J.

Murray. 19%. Wildlife habitat by design: National forests in the Blue Mountains of Oregon and Washington. Trans North Am. Wildl. Nat. Ftes Conf. 51:203-214.

Trainer, C. E. 1975. Direct causes of mortality in mule deer fawns during summer m d winter periods on Stem Mountain, Oregon. West. Assoc. State Fish Game Comm. Proc. 55163-170.

Truett, J. C. 1972. Ecology of the desert mule deer (Odocoifeus hemionus crooki ) in southeastern Arizona Ph.D. Thesisf Univ. Arizona, Tucson. 64pp.

Tucker, R D., and G. W. Garner. 1983. Habitat selection and vegetational characteristics of antelope fawn bedsites in west Texas. J. Range Manage. 36A10-113.

Wallmo, 0. C. 1981. Mule and black-tailed deer distribution and habitats. Pages 1-25 in 0. C. Wallmo, ed. Mule and black-tailed deer of North America. Univ. Nebraska Press, Lincoln. 605pp.

. W. L. Regelin, and D. W. Reichglrt. 1972. Forage use by mule deer relative to logging in Colorado. J. Wildl. Manage. 36:1025-1033.

, and J. W. Schoert. 1980. Response of deer to secondary forest succession in southeast Alaska. For. S' 26:44&462.

WaIther, F. 1968. Verhalt en der Gazellen. Die Neue Brehm Bucherei, no. 373.

Wittenberg-Lutherstadt A. Ziemsen Verlag. 144pp.

Wells, U C., and P. N. Lehner. 1978. The relative importance of the distance senses in coyote predatory behavior. Anim. Behav. 26251-258.

102 White, M., F. F. Knowlton, and W. C. Glamer. 1972. Effect of dam-newborn

fawn behavior on captive and mortality. J. Wildl. Manage. 36:897-906.

Wight, H. M. 1939- Field and laboratory technic in wildlife management. Univ. Michigan Press, Ann Arbor. 107pp.

Wilkins, B. T. 1957. Range use food habits and agricultural relationships of the mule deer, Bridger Mountains, Montana. J. Wildl. Manage. 21:159-169.

Wiilms, W. A., A. McLean, R Tucker, and R Ritcey. 1980. Deer and cattle diets on summer range in British Columbia. J. Range Manage. 33:55-59.

Wood, J. E., T. S. Bickle, W. Evans, J. C. Germany, and V. W. Howard, Jr. 1970. The Fort Stanton mule der herd: some ecological and life history characteristics with special emphasis on the use of water. New Mexico State Univ. A@C Exp. Stn. BdI. 567. 32pp.

Yeo, J. J., and J. M. Peek. 1989. Habitat characteristics selected by Sitka black-tailed deer in logged forests of southeastern Alaska. unpubl. report to U.S.D.A. For. Serv., Ketchikan, AM. 48pp.