13
Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Decit Hyperactivity Disorder Children Itai Berger 1, * , Ortal Slobodin 1 , Hanoch Cassuto 2 1 Pediatric Neurology Unit, The Neuro-Cognitive Center, Hadassah Hebrew University Medical Center, Jerusalem, Israel 2 Pediatric Neurology, Leumit and Clalit HMO, Jerusalem, Israel *Corresponding author at: Pediatric Neurology Unit, The Neuro-Cognitive Center, Hadassah-Hebrew University Medical Center, Mount Scopus Campus, POB 24035, Jerusalem 91240, Israel. Tel.: 972-2-5844903; fax: 972-2-5328963. E-mail address: [email protected] (I. Berger). Editorial Decision 23 October 2016; Accepted 28 October 2016 Abstract Objective: Despite the popularity of continuous performance tests (CPT) in supporting the diagnostic procedure of attention-decit hyper- activity disorder (ADHD), these measures are still controversial mainly due to limited sensitivity, specicity, and ecological validity. Thus, there continues to be a need for further validation of these objective attention measures. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the use- fulness of a CPT that includes environmental distracting stimuli, in supporting the diagnosis of ADHD in children. Method: Participants were 798 children aged 712 years (493 boys and 305 girls). The ADHD group included 339 children, whereas the control group included 459 children without ADHD. The study employed the MOXO-CPT, which incorporates visual and auditory stimuli serving as environmental distractors. Results: Compared to their unaffected peers, children with ADHD received signicantly lower scores in all 4 CPT indices: attention, tim- ing, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. Specically, ADHD children were less attended to the stimuli and performed fewer reactions on accurate timing. Furthermore, children with ADHD performed signicantly more impulsive and hyperactive responses than controls. Receiver operat- ing characteristic analysis revealed fair to excellent diagnostic ability of all CPT indices except impulsivity, which showed poor ability to distinguish ADHD children from controls. The tests total score yielded excellent diagnostic performance. Conclusions: MOXO-CPT consistently distinguished between children with ADHD and their unaffected peers, so that children with ADHD performed worse than controls in all study indices. Integration of CPT indices improves the diagnostic capacity of ADHD and may better reect the complexity and heterogeneity of ADHD. Keywords: ADHD; Assessment; Attention; Childhood neurologic disorders; Test construction Introduction Attention-decit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is an early onset, enduring neuro-developmental decit, which is among the most prevalent chronic health conditions (Polanczyk, de Lima, Horta, Biederman, Rohde, 2007). Given the wide heteroge- neity of symptoms, complex manifestation, and high comorbidity rates, there is an importance in a developmental perspective that views ADHD as a multifactorial disorder with multiple, causal processes and pathways (Berger, Remington, Leitner, & Leviton, 2015). The diagnosis of ADHD combines clinical observation with subjective reports. In clinical practice, the assessment process in children typically involves obtaining information from multiple sources, including parentsteachersinterviews or ques- tionnaires, clinical assessment, and, when needed, neuropsychological or other testing (American Academy of Pediatrics [AAP], 2001; Seixas, Weiss, & Muller, 2012; Wolraich et al., 2011). The vulnerability of the clinical assessment, interviews, and questionnaires to clinician and informant biases (Rousseau, Measham, & Bathiche-Suidan, 2008; Serra-Pinheiro, Mattos, & Regalla, 2008; Skounti, Philalithis, & Galanakis, 2007) may lead to underdiagnosis as well as to overdiagnosis © The Author 2016. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected] doi:10.1093/arclin/acw101 Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology Advance Access published November 23, 2016 at Hebrew University of Jerusalem on December 7, 2016 http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ Downloaded from

Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in theDiagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Children

Itai Berger1,*, Ortal Slobodin1, Hanoch Cassuto2

1Pediatric Neurology Unit, The Neuro-Cognitive Center, Hadassah Hebrew University Medical Center, Jerusalem, Israel2Pediatric Neurology, Leumit and Clalit HMO, Jerusalem, Israel

*Corresponding author at: Pediatric Neurology Unit, The Neuro-Cognitive Center, Hadassah-Hebrew University Medical Center,Mount Scopus Campus, POB 24035, Jerusalem 91240, Israel. Tel.: 972-2-5844903; fax: 972-2-5328963.

E-mail address: [email protected] (I. Berger).

Editorial Decision 23 October 2016; Accepted 28 October 2016

Abstract

Objective: Despite the popularity of continuous performance tests (CPT) in supporting the diagnostic procedure of attention-deficit hyper-activity disorder (ADHD), these measures are still controversial mainly due to limited sensitivity, specificity, and ecological validity. Thus,there continues to be a need for further validation of these objective attention measures. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the use-fulness of a CPT that includes environmental distracting stimuli, in supporting the diagnosis of ADHD in children.Method: Participants were 798 children aged 7–12 years (493 boys and 305 girls). The ADHD group included 339 children, whereas thecontrol group included 459 children without ADHD. The study employed the MOXO-CPT, which incorporates visual and auditory stimuliserving as environmental distractors.Results: Compared to their unaffected peers, children with ADHD received significantly lower scores in all 4 CPT indices: attention, tim-ing, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. Specifically, ADHD children were less attended to the stimuli and performed fewer reactions on accuratetiming. Furthermore, children with ADHD performed significantly more impulsive and hyperactive responses than controls. Receiver operat-ing characteristic analysis revealed fair to excellent diagnostic ability of all CPT indices except impulsivity, which showed poor ability todistinguish ADHD children from controls. The test’s total score yielded excellent diagnostic performance.Conclusions: MOXO-CPT consistently distinguished between children with ADHD and their unaffected peers, so that children withADHD performed worse than controls in all study indices. Integration of CPT indices improves the diagnostic capacity of ADHD and maybetter reflect the complexity and heterogeneity of ADHD.

Keywords: ADHD; Assessment; Attention; Childhood neurologic disorders; Test construction

Introduction

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is an early onset, enduring neuro-developmental deficit, which is amongthe most prevalent chronic health conditions (Polanczyk, de Lima, Horta, Biederman, Rohde, 2007). Given the wide heteroge-neity of symptoms, complex manifestation, and high comorbidity rates, there is an importance in a developmental perspectivethat views ADHD as a multifactorial disorder with multiple, causal processes and pathways (Berger, Remington, Leitner, &Leviton, 2015).

The diagnosis of ADHD combines clinical observation with subjective reports. In clinical practice, the assessment processin children typically involves obtaining information from multiple sources, including parents–teachers’ interviews or ques-tionnaires, clinical assessment, and, when needed, neuropsychological or other testing (American Academy of Pediatrics[AAP], 2001; Seixas, Weiss, & Muller, 2012; Wolraich et al., 2011). The vulnerability of the clinical assessment, interviews,and questionnaires to clinician and informant biases (Rousseau, Measham, & Bathiche-Suidan, 2008; Serra-Pinheiro,Mattos, & Regalla, 2008; Skounti, Philalithis, & Galanakis, 2007) may lead to underdiagnosis as well as to overdiagnosis

© The Author 2016. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected]:10.1093/arclin/acw101

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology Advance Access published November 23, 2016 at H

ebrew U

niversity of Jerusalem on D

ecember 7, 2016

http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 2: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

(and consequently to under or over treatment) of ADHD, particularly in certain groups (e.g., girls) (Ginsberg, Quintero, Anand,Casillas, & Upadhyayam, 2014; Quinn & Madhoo, 2014). The “transparency” of ADHD, even among experienced profes-sionals, may cause misdiagnosis, harm the treatment aspects, and has potential non-beneficial prognostic aspects (Berger &Maeir, 2014). Thus, there has long been interest in developing objective laboratory-based measures that could support clinicaldiagnosis. Currently, the most popular objective laboratory-based measures that could support clinical diagnosis are the con-tinuous performance test (CPT) tasks (Edwards et al., 2007; Vogt & Williams, 2011). The majority of CPT tasks (e.g., CPT-Degraded Stimulus, Adler et al., 2001; Gordon Diagnostic System, Gordon & Mettelman, 1987; Test of Variables ofAttention, Greenberg, 1997; CPT identical-pairs, Cornblatt, Risch, Faris, Friedman, & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, 1988) measureselective attention, sustained attention, and impulsivity, which can be used alongside clinical evaluation to inform the diagnos-tic process (Hall et al., 2014, 2016; Sonuga-Barke, Sergeant, Nigg & Willcutt, 2008). Usually, the test involves the rapid pre-sentation of a series of visual or auditory stimuli over a period of time (typically numbers, letters, number/letter sequences orgeometric figures). Subjects are instructed to respond to the “target” stimulus and to avoid responding to “non-target” stimuli.Response to non-target stimuli is referred to as a “commission error,” whereas the absence of response to target stimuli isreferred to as an “omission error.” Omission errors have been empirically assumed to measure inattention, whereas commis-sion errors are considered to measure impulsivity. Other standard measures of CPT responses include the number of correctresponses, response time (RT), and the variability in RT.

Like other cognitive tests, the CPT may serve as an aiding tool in the diagnostic procedure but is still considered controver-sial by some authors mainly due to questions regarding limited sensitivity, specificity, and ecological validity (Arble, Kuentzel,& Barnett, 2014; Edwards et al., 2007; Nigg, Willcutt, Doyle, & Sonuga-Barke, 2005). The validity of a diagnostic test refersto what the test measures and how well it does so. Barkley (1991) observed that although CPTs present excellent face validity(measure the ability to sustain attention for an extended period of time), their psychometric validity are often low to moderate.However, psychometric properties of CPT vary considerably between different tests. An analysis of eight CPT studies revealeda wide heterogeneity in measures of sensitivity (9%–88%) and specificity (23%–100%) to ADHD. Notably, the studies withhigher specificities (100% and 94%) had low sensitivities (13% and 62%, respectively) (Pan, Ma & Dai., 2007).

The controversial psychometric qualities of various CPT tasks were often attributed to a low ecological validity, namelythe ability to simulate the difficulties of ADHD patients in everyday life (Negut, Jurma, & David, 2016; Pelham et al., 2011;Rapport, Chung, Shore, Denney, & Isaacs, 2000). In contrast to many practitioners’ view that CPT provides a snapshot ofchild’s typical behavior, most CPTs are actually free from external, environmental distracting stimuli, which are thought tosignificantly impair the cognitive performance of ADHD children (APA, 2000). Although non-target stimuli can be often con-sidered as distractors, they are actually an integral part of the go/no-go paradigm and could not and should not be filtered. Incontrast, everyday environment is characterized by an ongoing flow of external non-relevant stimuli that have to be ignoredor filtered in order to allow adaptive functioning. This lack of external stimuli in most CPTs is particularly crucial for olderchildren, where prediction to school behavior may be desired. Thus, several authors emphasized the need to improve the eco-logical validity of the CPT by evaluating the child’s behavior in more natural settings (Barkley 1991; Negut et al., 2016;Parsons, Bowerly, Buckwalter, & Rizzo, 2007).

The degree to which CPT corresponds to ADHD symptomatology remains unclear. For instance, the correlation betweenCPT performance and parent or teacher rating scales is low to moderate (Forbes, 1998; Rielly, Cunningham, Richards,Elbard, & Mahoney, 1999). It is possible that CPT is sensitive to only part of the core deficits of ADHD, such as inattentionand impulsivity but not to others, such as hyperactivity. At the same time, CPT probably assesses unique aspects of ADHD,which are not captured by rating scales (Hall et al., 2016).

It has also been shown that developmental changes in ADHD symptomatology were not consistently reflected in changesin CPT performance (Halperin, Trampush, Miller, Marks, & Newcorn, 2008; Vaughn et al. 2011). One of the explanationsfor this inconsistency considers ADHD as a result of various neurological dysfunctions with different developmental trajecto-ries. The first is a subcortical dysfunction that persists across development and the second is a frontal dysfunction thatimproves with time (Halperin & Schulz, 2006). CPT outcomes that are related to frontal lobe functionality (e.g., commissionerrors) should decrease over time concomitantly with ADHD symptoms, whereas CPT outcomes linked to subcortical func-tioning (e.g., RT variability) should remain relatively constant and unrelated to ADHD symptomatology.

Another limitation of many current versions of CPT is ceiling effect (Berlin, Bohlin, Nyberg, & Janols, 2004; Mahone,Pillion, & Hiemenz, 2001), which seems to be related to the ease of visual format CPTs (Lasee & Choi, 2013). Most CPTparadigms rely on cognitive stimuli that involve clearly distinctive target/non-target stimuli (e.g., letters or numbers). Thiseasy distinctiveness often lead to high hit rates and low false alarm rates, or even perfect performance (See, Howe, Warm, &Dember, 1995). Ceiling effect due to insufficient cognitive demands is even more pronounced in older children and adoles-cents (Epstein et al., 2003). Despite this, most studies constrain their analyses to error rates even in the presence of highlyaccurate performance (Huang-Pollock, Karalunas, Tam, & Moore, 2012). Finally, there are limitations in CPT ability to

2 I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 3: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

measure deficits in timing, which are often evidenced in ADHD (Castellanos & Tannock, 2002; Kuntsi, Oosterlaan, &Stevenson, 2001). Traditionally, difficulties in timing at a CPT task are evaluated by mean RT for correct responses to the tar-get (which is interpreted as a measure of information processing and motor response speed) and by the standard deviation ofRT for correct responses to the target (which is interpreted as a measure of variability or consistency) (Greenberg &Waldman,1993). Previous findings revealed inconsistent results regarding RT in children with ADHD. Although some studies reportedslower RT in ADHD than in healthy controls (Andreou et al., 2007; de Zeeuw et al., 2008), others found faster RT in ADHDchildren. Slow responses were attributed to lapses of attention (Hervey et al., 2006). Such results suggest that RT indicesreflect multiple interacting processes, including attention, speed–accuracy trade-off effects, rate of information processing,motor bias, and response criteria (Ratcliff, 2002). Accurate assessment of these multiple interacting processes and understand-ing of how they contribute to an ultimate RT score are therefore critical to the interpretability of RT data (Huang-Pollocket al., 2012).

Taken together, there continues to be a need for an objective measure of attention that addresses some of the known short-comings. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the usefulness of the MOXO-CPT as a supporting measure in the diagno-sis of ADHD in children aged 7–12 years. Specifically, the study explores the discriminative validity of the test betweenchildren with ADHD and their non-affected peers. The term MOXO is derived from the world of Japanese martial arts andmeans a “moment of lucidity.”

With the purpose of improving ecological validity, the MOXO-CPT incorporates external interfering stimuli (auditory andvisual) that serve as distractors. Previous research has shown that incorporating environmental distractors improved the test’sutility in differentiating ADHD from control children (Cassuto, Ben-Simon, & Berger, 2013). Importantly, the negative effectof environmental distractors on CPT performance of ADHD children did not diminish with age, suggesting that distractibilityis a persistent deficit in ADHD (Slobodin, Cassuto, & Berger, 2015). Finally, the timing index of the MOXO-CPT is able todifferentiate between motor speed problems and inattention.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants in this study were 798 children aged 7–12 years, of them 493 were boys and 305 were girls. The study groupincluded 339 children diagnosed with ADHD (mean age (in years), 9.27, SD = 1.65), and the control group included 459 chil-dren without ADHD (mean age (in years) 9.71, SD = 1.64). Children were divided into six different age categories. The cate-gory of “8 years”, for example, included children who were 8 years or older but younger than 9 years. As can be seen inTable 1, within each age category, the two study groups did not differ in gender distributions.

Participants in the ADHD group were recruited from children referred to the outpatient pediatric clinics of a neuro-cognitive center, based in a tertiary care university hospital. The children were referred through their pediatrician, generalpractitioner, teacher, psychologist, or directly by the parents.

Inclusion criteria for participants in the ADHD group were: (a) The child met the criteria for ADHD according to DSM-IV- TR (APA, 2000), as assessed by a certified pediatric neurologist. The diagnostic procedure included a semistructuredinterview with the child and parents, completion of questionnaires, and medical/neurological examination that confirmedADHD diagnosis; (b) the child scored above the standard clinical cutoffs for ADHD symptoms on ADHD/DSM-IV scales(DuPaul, Power, Anastopouls, & Reid, 1998); and (c) the child was drug-naïve.

Participants in the control group were randomly recruited from typically developed children who study in regular classes atprimary schools. As a common practice in other similar studies (Advokat, Martino, Hill, & Gouvier et al., 2007; Cordier,Munro, Wilkes-Gillan, Speyer, &Pearce, 2014) and for the purpose of this article (evaluate the usefulness of a CPT), a typi-cally developing child was defined as a child who did not have childhood developmental disorder (i.e., scored below the clini-cal cutoff for any of the Conners’ Parent Rating Scales-Revised [CPRS-R] subscales and DSM-IV scales) and for whom noconcerns had been raised about development by a teacher or health professional (Berlin et al., 2004; de Zeeuw et al., 2008).Inclusion criteria for participants in the control group were: (a) the child scored below the clinical cutoff point for ADHDsymptoms on ADHD/DSM-IV scales. (b) the absence of academic or behavioral problems. For this purpose, the parents andteachers were asked to state (with the informed consent) that there are no behavioral/academic/social difficulties. Using parentand teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms is considered a common and reliable method for assigning participants to ADHD/non-ADHD groups (e.g., Uno et al., 2006; Van Mourik et al., 2007).

The control group of the study included only typically developed children and not referred children without ADHD. Thereason for this selection is that referred children who are diagnosed as non-ADHD in clinical settings are frequently diagnosed

3I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 4: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

with other comorbid conditions. Because the study focused on the ability of the MOXO-CPT to differentiate ADHD childrenfrom their non-affected peers, there was a rationale for preferring a control group who was rigorously screened for ADHD.Nevertheless, the limitations of this selection will be further discussed.

The exclusion criteria for all participants were: intellectual disability, other chronic condition, chronic use of medications,and primary psychiatric diagnosis (e.g., depression, anxiety, and psychosis). All participants in this study studied in regularclasses in regular schools.

Based on a community health center, our sample was multicultural and heterogeneous with regard to potentially confound-ing factors correlated with the diagnosis of ADHD (Wolraich et al., 2011). All participants agreed to participate in the study,and their parents provided a written informed consent to the study, approved by the Helsinki committee (IRB) of Hadassah-Hebrew University Medical Center, Jerusalem, Israel.

Measurements

Measurement of child behavior—The parent and teacher forms of the Conners Rating Scale (Conners, 1997a, 1997b) orADHD/DSM-IV Scales (DuPaul et al., 1998) were employed. These are of well-accepted validity and reliability, and both areregarded as standards in ADHD diagnosis (Barkley, 2014).

MOXO-CPT—This study employed the MOXO-CPT version (Berger & Goldzweig, 2010). The MOXO- CPT (NeuroTechSolutions Ltd) is a standardized computerized test designed to diagnose ADHD-related symptoms. The test consists of eightstages (levels). Each level consists of 53 trials and lasts 114.15s. The total duration of the test is 15.2 min. In each trial, a stimu-lus (target or non-target) is presented in the middle of the computer screen for durations of 0.5, 1, or 3s and is followed by a“void” of the same duration (Fig. 1). Fifty-three stimuli are presented in each level, of which 33 are target stimuli and 20 arenon-target. By implanting a void period after each stimulus and using variable presentation durations of the elements, theMOXO-CPT could distinguish accurate responses performed in “good timing” (quick and correct responses to the target per-formed during stimulus presentation) from accurate but slow responses (correct responses to the target performed after thestimulus presentation, during the void period). These two aspects of timing correspond to the two different problems of ADHD,responding quickly and accurately (National Institute of Mental Health, 2012).

Each stimulus remains on the screen for the full duration of the designated presentation time, regardless of whether aresponse was given. This practice allows the measuring of the timing of the response as well as its accuracy. The screen size

Table 1. Participants’ gender distribution

Age (years) ADHD (N = 339) Control (N = 459) Difference χ2 (1, N = 798)

7N 62 64 1.14Male 40 (64.52) 37 (57.81)female 22 (35.48) 30 (42.19)

8N 67 75 0.37Male 47 (70.15) 50 (66.67)female 20 (29.85) 25 (33.33)

9N 67 71 0.0004Male 37 (64.91) 46 (64.69)female 30 (35.09) 25 (35.31)

10N 62 87 0.15Male 35 (56.45) 47 (54.02)female 27 (44.55) 40 (45.98)

11N 51 87 0.01Male 32 (62.74) 55 (63.21)female 19 (37.26) 32 (27.79)

12N 40 74 0.01Male 24 (60) 44 (59.46)female 16 (40) 30 (40.54)

ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; N = number. *p < .05 **p < .01 ***p < .001.

4 I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 5: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

is 125 high and 166 wide. The child is located 60 cm from the screen and is instructed to respond to target stimulus as quicklyas possible by pressing the space bar once and only once. The child is also instructed not to respond to any other stimuli butthe target and not to press any other key but the space bar.

Test stimuli. Both target and non-target stimuli are cartoon pictures free of letters or numbers. The absence of letters andnumbers in the stimuli is important given the fact that ADHD children tend to have learning difficulties (e.g., dyslexia anddyscalculia), which may be confounded with CPT performance (Seidman, Biederman, Monuteaux, Doyle, & Faraone, 2001).Target stimulus is always a cartoon image of a child’s face. Non-target stimuli include five different images of animals. Bothtarget and non-target stimuli are 41 mm × 41 mm large and are always presented in the center of the screen (Fig. 2).

Distracting stimuli. To simulate everyday environment, the MOXO-CPT includes visual and auditory distracting stimuli thatare not part of the non-target stimuli. The distracting stimuli are of various degrees of similarity to the target stimulus.Distractors are short animated video clips containing visual and auditory features, which can appear separately or together.

Fig.1. Definition of the time line.

Fig. 2. MOXO-CPT target and non-target stimuli.

5I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 6: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

All distractors are typical elements in the child environment, a unique feature to the MOXO-CPT. Overall, six different dis-tractors are included, each of them could appear as pure visual (e.g., three birds moving their wings), pure auditory (e.g., birdssinging), or as a combination of them (birds moving their wings and singing simultaneously). Each distractor was presentedfor a different duration ranging from 3.5s to 14.8s, with a fixed interval of 0.5s between two distractors. Visual distractorsinclude six different stimuli: a gong (presented for 6.8s), a bowling ball (3.5s), birds (9.25s), a warrior (Jedi) with saber(14.8s), a saber (6.8s), and a flying airplane (8.6s).

Visual distractors appear at one of the four spatial locations on the sides of the screen: down, up, left, or right. Visual dis-tractors that appear on the left or right axis are 200–400 pixels high and 100–200 pixels wide, whereas those that appear onthe up or down axis are 100–200 pixels high and 100–600 wide. The distance between visual distractors and target or non-target stimuli is always 21 mm (Fig. 3).

Auditory distractors include the six corresponding sounds of all visual distractors (e.g., a gong sound, sound of a bowlingball, birds singing, etc.). The sound is delivered through loudspeakers located on both sides of the screen (about 60 cm dis-tance from the child’s ears). The sound intensity is about 70% of the maximal intensity of the loudspeakers.

Distractors’ onset is not synchronized with target or non-target’s onset and could be generated during target or non-targetstimulus or during the void period.

Test levels. The test consists of eight levels with 53 trials in each level. The stimuli and their presentation time are identicalacross all levels; however, the levels differ in the visual and auditory distractors present in the trials. Different levels of theMOXO-CPT are characterized by a different set of distractors: Levels 1 and 8 did not include any distractors but only targetand non-target stimuli, Levels 2 and 3 contained pure visual stimuli, Levels 4 and 5 contained pure auditory stimuli, andLevels 6 and 7 contained a combination of visual and auditory stimuli. The sequence of distractors and their exact position onthe display are constant for each level. The load of the distracting stimuli increases in the odd number levels. During the sec-ond, fourth, and sixth levels, only one distractor is presented at a time. During the third, fifth, and seventh levels, two distrac-tors are presented simultaneously.

Performance indices. The MOXO-CPT includes four performance indices:Attention The number of correct responses (pressing the key in response to a target stimulus), which were performedeither during the stimulus presentation on the screen or during the void period that followed. Thus, it was possible to evaluatewhether the participant responded correctly to the target (was attentive to the target) independently of how fast he was.Knowing how many responses are expected, it was also possible to calculate the number of times the target was presented,but the patient did not respond to it (omission errors).

The difference between the total number of the target stimuli and the number of correct responses produces the number ofomission errors. The score in this index was calculated as the average of correct responses throughout the eight test levels.Timing The number of correct responses (pressing the key in response to a target stimulus), which are given while the targetstimulus is still presented on the screen. This index did not include responses that were performed during the void period (afterthe stimulus has disappeared). In contrast to the attention index that measures correct responses independently of their speed, the

Fig. 3. MOXO-CPT visual distractors.

6 I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 7: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

current index measures correct responses only if they were performed on good timing. The score in this index was calculated asthe average of correct responses, while the target stimulus is still presented on the screen throughout the eight test levels.

Impulsivity The number of commission errors performed only during the time in which a non-target stimulus is presenton the screen. Usually, commission errors are coded in any case of inappropriate response to the target (e.g., pressing a ran-dom key) (Greenberg &Waldman ,1993). In contrast, the MOXO-CPT’s impulsivity index considers only the pressings onthe keyboard’s space bar in response to non-target stimulus as impulsive behavior . All other non-inhibited responses (e.g.,pressing the keyboard more than once) were not coded as impulsive responses (as will be described in Hyperactivity). Scorein this index was calculated as the average of impulsive responses throughout the eight test levels.

Hyperactivity The number of all types of commission responses that are not coded as impulsive responses (e.g., multiple re-sponses, random key pressing). Several examples are: (a) Multiple responses—pressing the keyboard’s space bar more than once(in response to target/ non-target), which is commonly interpreted as a measure of motor hyperresponsivity (Greenberg&Waldman, 1993). The MOXO-CPT considered only the second press and above (the first response would be considered as cor-rect response with good timing as multiple responses, as correct response with poor timing, or as impulsive response, depends onthe type of element appearing on the screen). (b) Random key pressing—pressing any keyboard button other than the space bar.Multiple and random key pressing was chosen as a measurement of hyperactivity for several reasons. First, finger tapping testsare widely used in the research of various neurological conditions (such as amyotrophic lateral sclerosis), for characterization andquantitative assessment of the motor function (Londral et al., 2016). Second, finger movements, as well as arm, head, and foot,are used to assess speed and quality deficits in motor activity of ADHD (Carte, Nigg, & Hinshaw, 1996; Schuerholz, Cutting,Mazzocco, Singer & Denckla, 1997). For instance, finger-sequencing task was recently used by Gaddis et al. (2015), as a measureof motor overflow in children with ADHD. ADHD children display increased motor activity than controls in low stimulation con-ditions (Antrop, Roeyers, Van Oost, & Buysse,2000; Kofler, Raiker, Sarver, Wells,& Soto, 2016).

Thus, measuring motor activity after the original response was performed and no novel information was presented, whichallow the test to evaluate increased activity precisely in such low stimulation conditions.

By separating commission errors due to impulsive behavior from commission errors due to motor hyperresponsivity, itwas possible to identify the multiple sources of response disinhibition. Score in this index was calculated as the average ofhyperactive responses along the eight test levels.

Procedure

The test was administered by a technician who ensured that the participant understood and followed the instructions. Thetechnician was present throughout the entire session. All participants (including the ADHD group) did not use any mediationbefore or during their participation in the study.

Data Analysis

The usefulness of the test for ADHD diagnosis was examined by comparing data along the four MOXO-CPT indicesbetween ADHD and controls. First, Chi-square analysis was used to examine group differences in gender distribution. Then,CPT performance of ADHD children was compared to that of typically developing children, using independent samplesT-tests with Tukey’s correction for multiple comparisons for each one of the four MOXO-CPT indices. Also, a total score ofthe test (consisted of all four indices together) was calculated by averaging the z-scores of the four single indices.

The diagnostic value of the MOXO-CPT was assessed by calculating the areas under the receiver operating characteristic (ROC)curves, which was used to assess the best cutoff points to distinguish between ADHD and non-ADHD children. ROC curves givean indication of the diagnostic ability of a measure to distinguish between the cases and the controls. They yield an area under thecurve statistic (ROC-AUC), which ranges from 0 to 1. An ROC-AUC of 0 indicates incorrect classification of all cases, 0.5 indi-cates that the measure is no better than chance at distinguishing the cases and the controls, and an ROC-AUC of 1 indicates that allparticipants were correctly classified. Confidence intervals (95%) for an ROC-AUC that do not include 0.5 indicate that a measurecan significantly distinguish between the cases and the unaffected controls at the above chance level. Here, MOXO-CPT indiceswere analyzed for their ability to distinguish between ADHD children and controls. AUC results were considered excellent forAUC values between 0.9 and 1, good for AUC values between 0.8 and 0.9, fair for AUC values between 0.7 and 0.8, poor forAUC values between 0.6 and 0.7, and failed for AUC values between 0.5 and 0.6 (Obuchowski, Lieber,& Wians,2004). All analy-ses were conducted with SAS software for Windows version 9.2. A p-value of ≤.05 was considered statistically significant.

7I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 8: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Results

Differentiating between ADHD and Typically Developing Children

Results of t-test analyses for independent samples revealed significant differences between ADHD children and their non-affected peers (Table 2). ADHD children received significantly lower scores in the Attention and Timing indices than healthycontrols. ADHD children were less attended to the stimuli, made more omission errors, and performed fewer reactions onaccurate timing. Furthermore, ADHD children received significantly higher scores in the Hyperactivity and Impulsivity indi-ces than healthy controls. That is, they performed significantly more commission errors from all types: impulsive commissionerrors (responding to a non-target stimulus as if it was a target stimulus) and hyperactive commission errors that include allother sorts of non-inhibited responses.

Diagnostic Utility of the MOXO-CPT

To classify children based on the MOXO-CPT performance, the sensitivity and specificity in diagnosis of ADHD were cal-culated using optimal cutoff as the threshold for indices.

ADHD diagnosis was given according to DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000).Table 3 shows the cutoff points, sensitivity, and specificity rates based on the total scores of the MOXO-CPT (taking into

account all four indices). Different sensitivity and specificity rates could be used for different purposes of the test (e.g., screening,diagnosis). However, we used the optimal value that represents the maximized classification accuracy with the highest sensitivityand specificity rates. Using the MOXO-CPT, total score revealed that in all age categories, the optimal cutoff points were associ-ated with both high sensitivity and specificity rates. In all age categories, specificity and sensitivity rates were 85% or higher.

The discriminant ability of different MOXO-CPT indices was evaluated by an ROC curve analysis. Table 4 summarizesdiagnostic efficiency with the AUC statistic. With the exception of the impulsivity index, all MOXO-CPT indices showed fairto excellent ability to distinguish between the ADHD children and their unaffected peers. AUC values for attention parameterranged between 0.75 and 0.91, for the timing parameter 0.80 and 0.90, and for hyperactivity 0.73 and 0.82. The impulsivityparameter, however, showed AUC between 0.58 and 0.65, indicating low diagnostic performance when considered alone.The MOXO-CPT total score, which integrates all four CPT indices, consistently showed excellent discriminant ability for allobserved ages, with AUC values above 0.91. Figure 4 exemplifies this pattern in children aged 8 years.

Discussion

This study evaluates the usefulness of the MOXO-CPT (Berger & Goldzweig, 2010) in the diagnosis of ADHD in childrenaged 7–12 years. In line with the previous CPT literature, results showed that the MOXO-CPT consistently distinguishedbetween children with ADHD and their unaffected peers, so that children with ADHD performed worse than controls in allstudy indices (Corkum & Siegel, 1993; Losier, McGrath, & Klein, 1996; Uno et al., 2006). The largest difference betweenADHD and control children was found in the total score of the test that integrates all four indices. As compared to healthychildren, children with ADHD showed fewer accurate responses (more omission errors) and fewer reactions on accurate tim-ing. The findings that ADHD children demonstrated more inattentive responses both in quality and in timing provide evidencethat RT in CPT may reflect multiple interacting processes (Huang-Pollock et al., 2012) and highlight the need to separatelyaddress those two different deficiencies in ADHD.

Table 2. Group differences in MOXO-CPT performance indices

Age (years) Difference (ADHD− control)

Attention Timing Hyperactivity Impulsivity Total score

7 t(124) = −8.75*** t(124) = −10.66*** t(124) = 4.40*** t(124) = 2.51* t(124) = 11.01***8 t(140) = −7.79*** t(140) = –11.26*** t(140) = 6.14*** t(140) = 2.29* t(140) = 11.91***9 t(136) = −5.47*** t(136) = −8.04*** t(136) = 4.91*** t(136) = 2.90** t(136) = 10.44***10 t(147) = −5.12*** t(147) = −8.25*** t(147) = 6.21*** t(147) = 3.76*** t(147) = 11.69***11 t(136) = −5.17*** t(136) = −7.69*** t(136) = 6.38*** t(136) = 3.82*** t(136) = 12.18***12 t(112) = −4.50*** t(112) = −6.19*** t(112) = 4.40*** t(112) = 3.29** t(112) = 8.71***

ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

8 I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 9: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Moreover, children with ADHD showed significantly more commission errors from different types: impulsive commissionerrors (responding to a non-target stimulus as if it was a target stimulus) and hyperactive commission errors that include allother sorts of non-inhibited responses. Halperin and colleagues (1991) argued that CPT commission errors can stem frommultiple sources, which can be distinguished based on the specific nature of the errors. Using the A–X CPT, they noted thatcommission errors associated with rapid false responses are generally associated with impulsivity, whereas those associatedwith delayed responses (i.e., X-only errors) are typically associated with inattention. In line with this view, this study suggeststhat commission errors do not comprise a unitary measure and may reflect differential underlying psychological processes. Itsupports a multidimensional model of impulsivity and points to the importance of using multiple measures to assess its inter-related components (Lane, Cherek, Rhoades, Pietras & Tcheremissine, 2003; Reynolds, Ortengren, Richards & de Wit, 2006).

Results of the ROC analyses showed that although attention, timing, and hyperactivity indices showed fair to excellentability to distinguish between ADHD children and controls, the impulsivity index showed poor ability of doing so. Althoughpoor diagnostic capacity of commission errors was previously documented in other CPTs (Fazio, Doyle, & King, 2014), here,it may be attributed to the high cognitive load of the task. In contrast to other CPTs, the present task included environmentaldistracters that may increase the complexity of the task for both study groups. These higher cognitive demands, which areknown to increase commission error rates (Seymour, Mostofsky & Rosch, 2016), may explain why this index is not sensitiveand specific enough to ADHD. Further research is needed to elucidate how the index’s clinical utility is affected by differentcognitive demands (e.g., with or without distractors).

Table 4. Area under the curve from receiver operating characteristic curve analyses (+95% confidence intervals) for MOXO-CPT indices

Age (years) ROC-AUC

Attention Timing Hyperactivity Impulsivity Total score

7 0.91 0.86–0.96 0.90 0.85–0.96 0.73 0.64–0.82 0.61 0.51–0.71 0.94 0.84–0.988 0.85 0.76–0.92 0.91 0.86–0.96 0.78 0.71–0.86 0.58 0.48–0.67 0.96 0.92–0.999 0.77 0.68–0.85 0.85 0.77–0.92 0.74 0.65–0.83 0.62 0.53–0.72 0.91 0.86–0.9710 0.74 0.66–0.83 0.82 0.75–0.89 0.78 0.71–0.86 0.60 0.51–0.70 0.91 0.86–0.9611 0.78 0.70–0.87 0.82 0.74–0.90 0.80 0.72–0.88 0.65 0.55–0.74 0.95 0.91–0.9912 0.75 0.65–0.85 0.80 0.71–0.89 0.82 0.73–0.91 0.65 0.54–0.75 0.92 0.85–0.98

AUC, area under the curve; ROC, receiver operating characteristic curve.

Table 3. Specificity and sensitivity rates of the MOXO-CPT total scores

Age category (years) Optimal

7Cutoff 184.60Sensitivity 86%Specificity 85%

8Cutoff 167.98Sensitivity 91%Specificity 88%

9Cutoff 144.98Sensitivity 90%Specificity 85%

10Cutoff 110.88Sensitivity 81%Specificity 85%

11Cutoff 107.89Sensitivity 86%Specificity 89%

12Cutoff 91.82Sensitivity 85%Specificity 85%

9I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 10: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Our results showed that the total score of the MOXO-CPT yielded the highest sensitivity and specificity rates, as comparedto any single index. MOXO-CPT’s total score was highly accurate in identifying participants with ADHD, based on theDSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) criteria. For all ages, optimal cutoff values were associated with both high sensitivity and specific-ity rates of 85% or more. AUC values of the MOXO-CPT total score were excellent, indicating a very high diagnostic perfor-mance in all observed ages. Comparing to AUC values of single MOXO-CPT indices, the total score had the greatest abilityto discriminate between ADHD children and controls.

These results lend support to the notion that most CPT tasks require multiple cognitive abilities (Coull, Frith, Frackowiak,& Grasby, 1996; Hall et al., 2016; Shalev, Ben-Simon, Mevorach, Cohen & Tsal, 2011; Straube, Bischoff, Nisch, Sauer, &Volz, 2002), and therefore an integration of CPT indices may better reflect the complexity and heterogeneity of ADHD etiol-ogy and clinical manifestations.

Several limitations of this study should be considered. The first limitation results from the study’s sampling method.Participation in the study was based on a voluntary agreement of children and their parents. This self-selected sampling strat-egy tends to be biased towards favoring more cooperative and motivated individuals. Therefore, it is not possible to determinewhether this sample also represents other children that were not recruited and whether cooperation is confounded with ADHDvariables (Gau et al., 2010; Lee & Ousley, 2006). Moreover, the clinics from which ADHD children were recruited werebased in tertiary care hospital. This population has heterogeneous background characteristics including correlates of ADHD.However, the fact that the control group was recruited from a random population supports our findings by showing that thetest is able to identify the ADHD children from a random population sample.

Another limitation of the study is the exclusion of ADHD children with severe comorbidities. ADHD is associated withmany psychiatric disorders (Gentile, Atiq, & Gillig, 2006), and current CPTs fail to distinguish between patients with ADHDand those with other psychiatric states (Advokat, et al., 2007; Suhr, Sullivan, & Rodriguez, 2011). Choosing a control groupof children without comorbidities reflects the study’s focus on the ability of the MOXO-CPT to differentiate ADHD childrenfrom their non-affected peers. Nevertheless, it clearly limits our knowledge about the test’s ability to differentiate ADHDfrom other comorbid conditions. This utility should be further examined. Finally, selection of a threshold for a screening testis best achieved according to the needs of the specific setting in which it is to be used. “Optimal” cutoff values vary depend-ing on the risk–benefit ratio between false-positive and false-negative test results and the base rate of the target disorder in thepopulation at hand. Nevertheless, important information may be lost when defining sensitivity and specificity in relation to asingle cutoff value of a continuous variable (Sox, 1986).

Overall, the results of this study indicate several strengths of the MOXO-CPT.The test is able to provide important information in assessing the core variables in ADHD, especially of attention, timing,

activity, and impulsivity and reduce reliance on subjective observer reports. Although symptoms of ADHD change consider-ably during maturation (Berger, Slobodin, Aboud, Melamed, & Cassuto, 2013; Faraone, Biederman, & Mick, 2006), the test’s

Fig. 4. Receiver operating characteristic curves for MOXO-CPT indices in children aged 8 years.

10 I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 11: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

ability to discriminate between ADHD children and typically developed controls was consistent in all age group. Specifically,the MOXO-CPT did not show age-related celling effect, suggesting that task demands are sufficient even for older children.

On the technical level, participants did not demonstrate any difficulties understanding the instructions or completing the task. Itsshort administration time may appeal for professionals working with young children who find it hard to cooperate with longer tasks.

There is a need for further research in order to explore the psychometric properties of the MOXO-CPT in other age groups,in samples with comorbid features, and in different subtypes of ADHD. Given the shortcomings of current CPT in differenti-ating between ADHD and psychiatric controls (McGee, Clark, & Symons, 2000), such study is necessary in further researchof the clinical utility of the MOXO-CPT in the diagnostic procedure of ADHD.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to acknowledge the dedicated help of Merav Aboud and Julia Melamed and thank them for their profes-sional, warm, and kind attitude towards the participating children and their families.

Conflict of Interest

IB and OS served on the scientific advisory board of NeuroTech Solutions Ltd. HC declares no conflicts of interests.

References

Adler, C. M., Sax, K. W., Holland, S. K., Schmithorst, V., Rosenberg, L., & Strakowski, S. M. (2001). Changes in neuronal activation with increasing atten-tion demand in healthy volunteers: an fMRI study. Synapse, 42, 266–272.

Advokat, C., Martino, L., Hill, B. D., & Gouvier, W. (2007). Continuous Performance Test (CPT) of college students with ADHD, psychiatric disorders, cog-nitive deficits, or no diagnosis. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10, 253–256.

American Academy of Pediatrics (2001). Subcommittee on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and committee on quality improvement. Clinical practiceguideline: treatment of the school-aged child with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 108, 1033–1044.

American Academy of Pediatrics (2011). ADHD: Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-order in children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 128, 1007–1023.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders text revision (DSM-IV-TR) (4th edn.-text revision).Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Andreou, P., Neale, B. M., Chen, W., Christiansen, H., Gabriels, I., Heise, A., et al. (2007). Reaction time performance in ADHD: Improvement under fast-incentive condition and familial effects. Psychological Medicine, 37, 1703–1715.

Antrop, L., Roeyers, H., Van Oost, P., & Buysse, A. (2000). Stimulation seeking and hyperactivity in children with ADHD. Attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder. Journal of Child Psychology Psychiatry, 41, 225–231.

Arble, E., Kuentzel, J., & Barnett, D. (2014). Convergent validity of the Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA+Plus):Associations with working memory, processing speed, and behavioral ratings. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 29, 300–312.

Barkley, R. A. (1991). The ecological validity of laboratory and analogue assessment methods of ADHD symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,19, 149–178.

Barkley, R. A. (2014). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (4th ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Berger, I., & Goldzweig, G. (2010). Objective measures of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder—A pilot study. Israel Medical Association Journal,

12, 531–535.Berger, I., & Maeir, A. (2014). ADHD: A transparent impairment, clinical, daily-life and research aspects in diverse populations. New York: Nova Science

Pub Incorporated.Berger, I., Remington, A., Leitner, Y., & Leviton, A. (2015). Brain development and the attention spectrum. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 23. doi:10.

3389/fnhum.2015.0002.Berger, I., Slobodin, O., Aboud, M., Melamed, J., & Cassuto, H. (2013). Maturational delay in ADHD: Evidence from CPT. Frontiers in Human

Neuroscience, 7, Article 691. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00691.Berlin, L., Bohlin, G., Nyberg, L., & Janols, L. (2004). How well do measures of inhibition and other executive functions discriminate between children with

ADHD and controls? Child Neuropsychology, 10, 1–13.Carte, E. T., Nigg, J. T., & Hinshaw, S. P. (1996). Neuropsychological functioning, motor speed, and language processing in boys with and without ADHD.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 481–498.Cassuto, H., Ben-Simon, A., & Berger, I. (2013). Using environmental distractors in the diagnosis of ADHD. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. doi:10.

3389/fnhum.2013.00805.Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews

Neuroscience, 3, 617–628.Conners, C. K. (1997a). Conners’ Parent RatingScale—Revised(L). New York, NY: Multi-Health Systems Inc.Conners, C. K. (1997b). Conners’ TeacherRatingScale—Revised (L). New York, NY: Multi-Health Systems Inc.Cordier, R., Munro, N., Wilkes-Gillan, S., Speyer, R., & Pearce, W. M. (2014). Reliability and validity of the Pragmatics Observational Measure (POM): A

new observational measure of pragmatic language for children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 1588–1598.

11I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 12: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Corkum, P., & Siegel, L. (1993). Is the continuous performance task a valuable research tool for use with children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disor-der? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1217–1239.

Cornblatt, B., Risch, N. J., Faris, G., Friedman, D., & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. (1988). The Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs version (CPT-IP):I. New findings about sustained attention in normal families. Psychiatry Research, 29, 65–68.

Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Grasby, P. M. (1996). A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: A PET study of sus-tained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia, 34, 1085–1095.

de Zeeuw, P., Aarnoudse-Moens, C., Bijlhout, J., Konig, C., Post Uiterweer, A., Papanikolau, A., et al. (2008). Inhibitory performance, response speed,intraindividual variability, and response accuracy in ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 808–816.

DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R. (1998). ADHD rating scale–IV. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Edwards, M. C., Gardner, E. S., Chelonis, J. J., Schulz, E. G., Flake, R. A., & Diaz, P. F. (2007). Estimates of the validity and utility of the Conner’s CPT in

the assessment of inattentive and/or hyperactive impulsive behaviors in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 393–404.Epstein, J. N., Erkanli, A., Conners, C. K., Klaric, J., Costello, J. E., & Angold, A. (2003). Relations between continuous performance test performance mea-

sures and ADHD behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 543–554.Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Mick, E. (2006). Decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychological

Medicine, 36, 159–165. doi:10.1017/S003329170500471X.Fazio, R., Doyle, L., & King, J. (2014). CPT-II versus TOVA: Assessing the diagnostic power of continuous performance tests. Archives of Clinical

Neuropsychology, 29, 540.Forbes, G. B. (1998). Clinical utility of the test of variables of attention (TOVA) in the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 54, 461–476.Gaddis, A., Rosch, K. S., Dirlikov, B., Crocetti, D., MacNeil, L., Barber, A. D., et al. (2015). Motor overflow in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder is associated with decreased extent of neural activation in the motor cortex. Psychiatry research, 233, 488–495.Gau, S., Lin, Y., Shang, C., Liu, S., Chiu, Y., & Soong, W. (2010). Emotional/ behavioral problems and functional impairment in clinic and community-

based children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Taiwan. Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 521–532.Gentile, J., Atiq, R., & Gillig, P. M. (2006). Adult ADHD: Diagnosis, differential diagnosis, and medication management. Psychiatry, 3, 25–30.Ginsberg, Y., Quintero, J., Anand, E., Casillas, M., & Upadhyayam, H. P. (2014). Underdiagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adult patients:

A review of the literature. The Primary Care Companion for CNS Disorders (pp. 16). doi:10.4088/PCC.13r01600.Gordon, M., & Mettelman, B. B. (1987). Technical guide to the Gordon Diagnostic System. Syracuse, NY: Gordon Systems.Greenberg, L. M. (1997). T.O.V.A visual continuous performance 15. Test. Los Alamitos, CA: Universal Attention Disorders Inc.Greenberg, M., & Waldman, I. (1993). Developmental normative data on The Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A™). Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 34, 1019–1030.Hall, C. L., Valentine, A. Z., Groom, M. J., Walker, G. M., Sayal, K., Daley, D., et al. (2016). The clinical utility of the continuous performance test and

objective measures of activity for diagnosing and monitoring ADHD in children: A systematic review. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,25, 677–699.

Hall, C. L., Walker, G. M., Valentine, A. Z., Guo, B., Kaylor-Hughes, C., James, M., et al. (2014). Protocol investigating the clinical utility of an objectivemeasure of activity and attention (QbTest) on diagnostic and treatment decision-making in children and young people with ADHD—‘Assessing QbTestUtility in ADHD’ (AQUA): A randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open, 4, e006838. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2014-006838.

Halperin, J., & Schulz, K. (2006). Revisiting the role of the prefrontal cortex in the pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PsychologicalBulletin, 132, 560–581.

Halperin, J. M., Sharma, V., Greenblatt, E., & Schwartz, S. T. (1991). Assessment of the continuous performance test: Reliability and validity in a non-referred sample. Psychological Assessment, 3, 803–808.

Halperin, J. M., Trampush, J. W., Miller, C. J., Marks, D. J., & Newcorn, J. H. (2008). Neuropsychological outcome in adolescents/young adults with child-hood ADHD: Profiles of persisters, remitters and controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 958–966.

Hervey, A. S., Epstein, J. N., Curry, J. F., Tonev, S., Eugene Arnold, L., Keith Conners, C., et al. (2006). Reaction time distribution analysis of neuropsycho-logical performance in an ADHD sample. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 125–140.

Huang-Pollock, C. L., Karalunas, S. L., Tam, H., & Moore, A. N. (2012). Evaluating vigilance deficits in ADHD: A meta-analysis of CPT performance.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 360–371.

Kofler, M. J., Raiker, J. S., Sarver, D. E., Wells, E. L., & Soto, E. F. (2016). Is hyperactivity ubiquitous in ADHD or dependent on environmental demands?Evidence from meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 46, 12–24. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.004.

Kuntsi, J., Oosterlaan, J., & Stevenson, J. (2001). Psychological mechanisms in hyperactivity: I. Response inhibition deficit, working memory impairment,delay aversion, or something else? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 199–210.

Lane, S. D., Cherek, D. R., Rhoades, H. M., Pietras, C. J., & Tcheremissine, O. V. (2003). Relationships among laboratory and psychometric measures ofimpulsivity: Implications in substance abuse and dependence. Addictive Disorders and their Treatment, (2), 33–40.

Lasee, M. J., & Choi, H. S. (2013). Evidence of reliability and validity for a children’s auditory continuous performance test. Sage Open, October-December:1–10. doi: 10.1177/2158244013511828.

Lee, D. O., & Ousley, O. Y. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a clinic sample of children and adolescents with pervasive develop-mental disorders. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 16, 737–746.

Londral, A., Pinto, S., & de Carvalho, M. (2016). Markers for upper limb dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis using analysis of typing activity.Clinical Neurophysiology, 127, 925–931.

Losier, B. J., McGrath, P. J., & Klein, R. M. (1996). Error patterns on the continuous performance test in non-medicated and medicated samples of childrenwith and without ADHD: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 971–987.

Mahone, M. E., Pillion, J. P., & Hiemenz, J. R. (2001). Initial development of an auditory continuous performance test for preschoolers. Journal of AttentionDisorders, 5, 93–106.

McGee, R. A., Clark, S. E., & Symons, D. K. (2000). Does the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test aid in ADHD diagnosis? Journal of Abnormal ChildPsychology, 28, 425–424.

12 I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 13: Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in ......Usefulness and Validity of Continuous Performance Tests in the Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

National Institute of Mental Health. (2012). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Retrieved from: http://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/publications/attention-deficit-hyperactivity-disorder/complete-index.shtml.

Negut, A., Jurma, A.M., & David, D (2016). Virtual-reality-based attention assessment of ADHD: Clinical VR: Classroom-CPT versus a traditional continu-ous performance test. Child Neuropsychology, 3, 1–21.

Nigg, J. T., Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do we need neurop-sychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1224–1230.

Obuchowski, N. A., Lieber, M. L., & Wians, F. H. (2004). ROC curves in clinical chemistry: Uses, misuses, and possible solutions. Clinical Chemistry, 50,1118–1125.

Pan, X. X., Ma, H. W., & Dai, X. M. (2007). Value of integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test in the diagnosis of childhood attention defi-cit hyperactivity disorder. Chinese Journal of Contemporary Pediatrics, 9, 210–212.

Parsons, T. D., Bowerly, T., Buckwalter, J. G., & Rizzo, A. A. (2007). A controlled clinical comparison of attention performance in children with ADHD in avirtual reality class-room compared to standard neuropsychological methods. Child Neuropsychology, 13, 363–381.

Pelham, W. E., Waschbusch, D. A., Hoza, B., Gnagy, E. M., Greiner, A. R., Sams, S. E., et al. (2011). Music and video as distractors for boys with ADHDin the classroom: Comparison with controls, individual differences, and medication effects. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 1085–1098.

Polanczyk, G., de Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metare-gression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 942–948.

Quinn, P. O., & Madhoo, M. (2014). A review of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in women and girls: Uncovering this hidden diagnosis. The PrimaryCare Companion for CNS Disorders (pp. 16). doi:10.4088/PCC.13r01596.

Rapport, M. D., Chung, K. M., Shore, G., Denney, C. B., & Isaacs, P. (2000). Upgrading the science and technology of assessment and diagnosis: laboratoryand clinic-based assessment of children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 29, 555–568.

Ratcliff, R. (2002). A diffusion model account of response time and accuracy in a brightness discrimination task: Fitting real data and failing to fit fake butplausible data. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 278–291.

Reynolds, B., Ortengren, A., Richards, J. B., & de Wit, H. (2006). Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personality andIndividual Differences, 40, 305–315.

Rielly, N. E., Cunningham, C. E., Richards, J. E., Elbard, H. J., & Mahoney, W. J. (1999). Detecting attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a communica-tions clinic: Diagnostic utility of the Gordon Diagnostic System. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychologia, 21, 685–700.

Rousseau, C., Measham, T., & Bathiche-Suidan, M. (2008). DSM -IV, culture and child psychiatry. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child andAdolescent Psychiatry, 17, 69–75.

Schuerholz, L. J., Cutting, L., Mazzocco, M. M., Singer, H. S., & Denckla, M. ,. B. (1997). Neuromotor functioning in children with Tourette syndrome withand without attention-deficit–hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Neurology, 12, 438–442.

See, J. E., Howe, S. R., Warm, J. S., & Dember, W. N. (1995). Meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 230–249.Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M., Doyle, A. E., & Faraone, S. V. (2001). Learning disabilities and executive dysfunction in boys with attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 15, 544–556.Seixas, M., Weiss, M., & Muller, U. (2012). Systematic review of national and international guidelines on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of

Psychopharmacology, 26, 753–765.Serra-Pinheiro, M. A., Mattos, P., & Regalla, A. M. (2008). Inattention, hyperactivity, and oppositional-defiant symptoms in Brazilian adolescents: Gender

prevalence and agreement between teachers and parents in a non-English speaking population. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12, 135–140.Seymour, K. E., Mostofsky, S. H., & Rosch, K. S. (2016). Cognitive load differentially impacts response control in girls and boys with ADHD. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 141–154.Shalev, L., Ben-Simon, A., Mevorach, C., Cohen, Y., & Tsal, Y. (2011). Conjunctive Continuous Performance Task (CCPT)—A pure measure of sustained

attention. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2584–2591.Skounti, M., Philalithis, A., & Galanakis, E. (2007). Variations in prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder worldwide. European Journal of

Pediatrics, 166, 117–123.Slobodin, O., Cassuto, H., & Berger, I. (2015). Age-related changes in distractibility: Developmental trajectory of sustained attention in ADHD. Journal of

Attention Disorders. DOI:10.1177/1087054715575066.Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Sergeant, J. A., Nigg, J., & Willcutt, E. (2008). Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:

Nosologic and diagnostic implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17, 367–384.Sox, H. C. J. (1986). Probability theory in the use of diagnostic tests. Annals of Internal Medicine, 104, 60–66.Straube, E. R., Bischoff, N., Nisch, C., Sauer, H., & Volz, H. P. (2002). Input dysfunction and beyond: An evaluation of CPT components. Schizophrenia

Research, 54, 131–139.Suhr, J. A., Sullivan, B. K., & Rodriguez, J. L. (2011). The relationship of noncredible performance to continuous performance test scores in adults referred

for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder evaluation. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26, 1–7.Uno, M., Abe, J., Sawai, C., Sakaue, Y., Nishitani, A., Yasuda, Y., et al. (2006). Effect of additional auditory and visual stimuli on continuous performance

test (noise-generated CPT) in AD/HD children—Usefulness of noise-generated CPT. Brain and Development, 28, 162–169.Van Mourik, R., Oosterlaan, J., Heslenfeld, D. J., Konig, C. E., & Sergeant, J. A. (2007). When distraction is not distracting: A behavioral and ERP study on

distraction in ADHD. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 1855–1865.Vaughn, A., Epstein, J. N., Rausch, J., Altaye, M., Langberg, L., Newcorn, J. H., et al. (2011). Relation between outcomes on a continuous performance test

and ADHD symptoms over time. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 853–864.Vogt, C., & Williams, T. (2011). Early identification of stimulant treatment responders, partial responders and non-responders using objective measures in

children and adolescents with hyperkinetic disorder. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 16, 144–149.Wolraich, M., Brown, L., Brown, R. T., DuPaul, G., Earls, M., Feldman, H. M., et al. (2011). Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder;

Steering Committee on quality improvement and management. ADHD: Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of atten-tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 128, 1007–1022.

13I. Berger et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology (2016); 1–13

at Hebrew

University of Jerusalem

on Decem

ber 7, 2016http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/

Dow

nloaded from