47
The novel language-systematic aphasia screening SAPS: Screening- based therapy in combination with computerised home-training Franziska Krzok 1 , Verena Rieger 1 , Katharina Niemann 1 , Ruth Nobis- Bosch 2 , Irmgard Radermacher 1 , Walter Huber 1 , Klaus Willmes 1 , Stefanie Abel 1,3 1 Department of Neurology, Medical Faculty, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany 2 Section of Continuous Learning, German Association of Logopedics (dbl), Germany 3 Neuroscience and Aphasia Research Unit, Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, UK Correspondence to: PD Dr. Stefanie Abel, Neuroscience and Aphasia Research Unit (NARU), School of Biological Sciences, University of Manchester, 1

Theoretischer Hintergrund - research.manchester.ac.uk  · Web viewThe SAPS-tasks for this cell constitutes auditory word-to-picture matching: ... PWA might already reach ceiling

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

The novel language-systematic aphasia screening SAPS: Screening-based therapy in

combination with computerised home-training

Franziska Krzok1, Verena Rieger1, Katharina Niemann1, Ruth Nobis-Bosch2, Irmgard Radermacher1,

Walter Huber1, Klaus Willmes1, Stefanie Abel1,3

1 Department of Neurology, Medical Faculty, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany

2 Section of Continuous Learning, German Association of Logopedics (dbl), Germany

3 Neuroscience and Aphasia Research Unit, Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, UK

Correspondence to:

PD Dr. Stefanie Abel,

Neuroscience and Aphasia Research Unit (NARU),

School of Biological Sciences,

University of Manchester,

Brunswick Street, Manchester, M13 9PL, UK

E-mail: [email protected]

1

The novel language-systematic aphasia screening SAPS: Screening-based therapy in

combination with computerised home-training

Background: SAPS is a novel language-systematic aphasia screening developed for the German language, which

already had been positively evaluated. It offers a fast assessment of modality-specific psycholinguistic

components at different levels of complexity and the derivation of impairment-based treatment foci from the

individual performance profile. However, the SAPS has not been evaluated in combination with the new SAPS-

based treatment yet.

Aims: We set out to replicate the practicality of the SAPS and investigate the effectiveness of a SAPS-based face-

to-face therapy combined with computerised home-training in a feasibility study. We aimed to examine the

soundness of the treatment design, to determine treatment-induced changes in patient performance as measured

by the SAPS, to assess parallel changes in communicative abilities, and to differentiate therapy effects achieved

by face-to-face therapy versus add-on effects achieved by later home-training.

Methods and Procedures: 16 participants with post-stroke aphasia (PWA) were included into our study. They

were administered the SAPS and communicative testing before and after the treatment regimen. Each PWA

received one therapy session followed by home-training per day, with the individual treatment foci being

determined according to initial SAPS-profile, and duration of treatment and possible change of focus dependent

on performance assessed by continuous therapy monitoring.

Outcomes and Results: The combination of therapy and home-training based on the SAPS was effective for all

participants. We were able to show significant improvements for impairment-based SAPS performance and, with

high inter-individual variability, in everyday communication. These two main targets of speech and language

therapy were correlated and SAPS-improvements after therapy were significantly higher than after home-training.

Conclusions: The SAPS offers the assessment of an individual performance profile in order to derive sufficiently

diversified, well-founded and specific treatment foci and to follow up changes in performance. The appending

treatment regimen has shown to be effective for our participants. Thus, the study revealed feasibility of our

approach.

2

Keywords: Aphasia, Speech and Language Therapy, Language processing, Neuropsychology, Diagnosis

What is already known: The efficacy of aphasia therapy in the chronic recovery stage previously has been confirmed and

various diagnostic instruments are available to assess language performance and its therapy-related changes. However,

there is no standardised aphasia test that, in one go, covers the multitude of language-systematic impairments and directs

subsequent therapy. To fill this gap, the language-systematic aphasia screening SAPS has been developed for the German

language, but it has not been not been evaluated in combination with the new SAPS-based treatment yet.

What this study adds: We were able to show that the SAPS and its appending treatment regimen are feasible. SAPS offers a

sufficiently diversified, well-founded and specific guidance on the choice of treatment foci and allows the assessment of

treatment-induced changes in performance. The treatment regimen consisting of SAPS-based therapy and home-training was

effective for our patient group: We found significant improvements for impairment-based SAPS performance and for everyday

communication. Thus, the SAPS-based treatment approach is feasible and offers a solid basis for larger treatment studies.

Clinical implications: SAPS is a novel diagnostic instrument which allows the assessment of language-systematic impairments

for clinical practice of Speech and Language Therapy. The screening offers a rationale for selection of therapy foci, and thereby

can serve as starting point for therapy planning; the SAPS-based therapy integrates home-training into the therapy regimen, and

changes in performance can be tightly monitored. In rehabilitation settings, the SAPS may prove to be an important complement

to current assessment and treatment of aphasia.

Introduction

The efficacy of aphasia therapy has been confirmed in numerous studies (Brady et al. 2016, Breitenstein et al. 2017).

It is well-known that therapy leads to the most favourable outcomes when delivered at high frequency (Bhogal et al.

2003b, Brady et al. 2016) and in short but intensive intervals (Bhogal et al. 2003a). With intensive aphasia therapy,

not only persons with aphasia (hereinafter referred to as PWA) in the acute stage of aphasia, but also those in the

chronic stage can achieve further improvements (Rose et al. 2013) – although the latter might be limited and smaller

than those gained during the earlier recovery stages which are assisted by spontaneous improvements (Basso and

3

Macis 2011). In order to deliver high-dose aphasia therapy and maximise intensity, computer-based home-training

can supplement face-to-face therapy. The integration of computer training into a treatment regimen has shown to be

effective (Allen et al. 2012, Stark and Warburton 2016, Stachowiak 1993, van de Sandt-Koenderman 2011, Zheng et

al. 2016).

While the efficacy of aphasia therapy in general is evident, it remains relatively unresolved which therapy

method to choose for an individual PWA in order to obtain optimal therapy outcomes, and the exact intensity and

dose required to reach this outcome. According to the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and

Health (ICF; World Health Organization [WHO], 2001), the superior aim of aphasia therapy is to enable each PWA

to communicate in everyday life (Springer 2008, Weniger and Springer 2006, Worrall et al. 2011). Cognitive-

linguistic therapies have been recommended as a Practice Standard for the rehabilitation of language and

communication deficits, with the approach favourably being accompanied by participation-based approaches aiming

at amelioration of pragmatic communication and conversational skills, and potentially being assisted by supervised

computer-based training (Cicerone et al. 2000). A standardised aphasia test that, in one go, covers the multitude of

language-systematic impairments and directs subsequent therapy would be of high value. Moreover, the inclusion of

computer-based training into the impairment-based treatment regimen would be desirable, and some positive effects

on communicative performance would be expected even though not directly practiced. The current therapy approach

based on the German test instrument “Sprachsystematisches Aphasiescreening (SAPS)” (Blömer et al. 2013; SAPS

was initially translated as Speech-systematic Aphasia Screening, but was relabelled to Language-Systematic Aphasia

Screening to gain English unambiguousness) was developed to fill this significant gap.

The efficacy of aphasia therapy can be assessed using standardised diagnostic instruments. In Germany,

various language-systematic tests enable the evaluation of speech and language therapy for lexical and sub-lexical

disorders (e.g., LeMo: de Bleser et al. 2004, Hierarchische Wortlisten: Liepold et al. 2003). The ICF has underlined

the necessity to also disclose to what extent PWA can participate in everyday life and what impact aphasia therapy

exerts on participation. To ascertain whether the ultimate speech and language therapy (SLT) goal to improve

4

communicative abilities has been reached requires other diagnostic instruments than those which describe

improvement of underlying linguistic functions (Blomert 1990). Although participation-based diagnostic instruments

for assessment of everyday communication are rare, there is the Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test

(ANELT; Blomert et al. 1994, Blomert et al. 1987). This test (primarily) measures the ability to convey information

verbally in everyday situations, while the linguistic form of the message is of minor importance. For any new

language-systematic diagnostic instrument, information about its relation to participation-based measures is

mandatory.

While there are various diagnostic instruments for aphasia, to our knowledge none of them offers sufficiently

diversified, well-founded and specific guidance on the choice of therapy. For the Dutch language, ScreeLing (Visch‐

Brink et al. 2010) assesses sub-lexical, lexical and syntactic language components and thereby gives a short

overview. For the German language, Abel et al. (in preparation) developed the SAPS (see Blömer et al. 2013) whose

rationale to directly derive foci for aphasia treatment is to be further examined in the present study. Before study

initiation, it was unclear how best to choose individual treatment foci from a given SAPS performance profile, and

how much time to dedicate to each chosen focus given the restricted time frame for therapy deliverance. While the

former was considered to depend on locus and severity of impairment and affect therapy outcomes, the latter was

considered to depend on individual speed of learning and to affect time to reach mastery level per treatment focus.

Thus, in the present feasibility study, therapists were instructed to (1) freely choose two affected language

functions within the SAPS scheme at a time, (2) monitor and treat the trained items of the therapy foci chosen, using

SAPS-tasks (including appending home-training), (3) complement therapy with any SLT procedure and materials

deemed appropriate for the individual PWA and foci, and (4) comply with pre-defined sequences of item sets, based

on individual learning speed as assessed in the monitoring. Thus, the therapy design is both strict and flexible, also

allowing to assess its soundness and carve out an optimal rationale for the selection of therapy foci within the SAPS.

5

Overview of the SAPS (language-systematic aphasia screening)

The SAPS assesses the three different linguistic domains phonology/phonetics (sublexical functions),

lexicon/semantics (lexical functions) and morphology/syntax (morpho-syntactic functions) in both receptive and

expressive language modalities (see Figure 1 for an overview). Each of the six resulting modules on the horizontal

axis consists of three cells (language tasks) with different levels of demand on the vertical axis. Thus, taken as a

whole the SAPS comprises 18 cells, i.e. 18 different language tasks which can be selected as treatment foci for

therapy and home-training. While the SAPS covers the core language domains of phonology, lexicon, and syntax

with their ancillary components for both receptive and expressive spoken modalities, it does not assess performance

for written language or more complex text/discourse processing. The compilation of SAPS tasks with appendent

treatment regimen was founded on established knowledge on the components of language processing (especially the

Levelt model; overviews in Brown and Hagoort 2000, Hillis 2015) and evidence-based best practice in SLT (e.g.,

Cicerone et al. 2000, Hillis 2015, Brady et al. 2006) as also applied at the Aachen Aphasia Ward, a special aphasia

unit for intensive speech therapy and neuropsychological treatment for stroke patients since 1984 (Huber et al. 1999)

where the present study was conducted.

- Figure 1 about here -

The assessment with the present SAPS version is performed via computer. The SLT evaluates the responses

of the PWA online using keyboard keys, and the computer presents a summed score at the end of each cell. The

output of the screening is an individual performance profile, identifying linguistic strengths and weaknesses of each

PWA. Based on this profile, the language-systematic and controlled therapy can be planned and executed. By

focusing treatment at the impaired task (cell), the goal of improving the according linguistic function and underlying

processing stages is implied (Figure 1), with the broader goal of accomplishing generalisation to other items and tasks

6

as well as translation to everyday language. The face-to-face SAPS therapy is complemented by a computer-based

home-training, for both of which half of the test items (Training items or T-items, N=12 for receptive and N=8 for

expressive cells) can be used in SAPS-tasks. The remaining items (Control items or C-items; also N=12 and N=8 for

receptive and expressive cells, respectively) are reserved for assessments only (further information about the choice

of treatment foci and SAPS-based therapy are given in the methods section).

In 2013, Blömer et al. evaluated the original version of the SAPS. By and large, the properties of the test

construction currently were confirmed for the present sample, and the tasks which were insufficiently graded in

difficulty were successfully revised, yielding high internal consistency in all modules (Cronbach’s alpha >.90) and an

appropriate gradation in difficulty according to the levels of demand for the modified SAPS. The current feasibility

study built the basis for a larger aphasia trial (Breitenstein et al. 2017), and the successful final test construction and

evaluation will be addressed elsewhere using this larger sample.

Aims and hypotheses of the present study

In the present pilot study, we investigated aphasia therapy based on the final, revised version of the SAPS (language-

systematic aphasia screening). We aimed to determine the modules/cells chosen for treatment (therapy and home-

training) and the impact of speed of learning in the group of PWA as featured by our treatment design, to examine its

soundness. We hypothesised that (hypothesis 1) our treatment approach which consists of SAPS-based therapy and

computer-based home-training resulted in improvements in PWA’s performance in the SAPS modules chosen, (2)

home-training led to further improvements in PWA’s performance over and above face-to-face therapy, (3) home-

training was well-constructed and feasible, and (4) language-systematic treatment affected communicative

performance as measured in a participation-based language test (ANELT).

7

Methods

Experimental design

The study was conducted on the Aachen Aphasia Ward at the RWTH Aachen University Hospital, where PWA

receive multidisciplinary, complex and high-intensity SLT as in-patients for typically 7 weeks, usually covered by the

German health system (https://www.ukaachen.de/kliniken-institute/klinik-fuer-neurologie/klinik/aphasiestation.html).

The design of the study is presented in Figure 2. In week 1, PWA were pre-tested with SAPS and ANELT. As a

current assessment with the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT; Huber et al., 1983) is a prerequisite for admission to the

ward, those scores were also available for each PWA. After that, each PWA received treatment for up to 16

consecutive weekdays. Subsequent to the treatment phase, the PWA were assessed with SAPS and ANELT again.

After termination of the study regimen, they received further intensive standard therapy for their remaining stay on

the ward. Therefore, we did not re-assess the PWA in a follow-up. Test results are shown in Supplementary Table S2

(SAPS), S3 and S4 (AAT), and S5 (ANELT).

- Figure 2 about here -

Language Assessment

The SAPS as implemented in the computer software Presentation by the senior author (version 0.70, 09.02.03) was

carried out on a laptop at both test times. In the computer-based SAPS version, the Speech and Language Therapist

(SLT) scored the responses of the PWA via coded button responses online. The computer automatically counted and

summed the scores reached by the PWA in real-time, assessed whether the PWA met pre-defined criteria for

termination of each cell/module, delivered summaries of scores, and determined the performance categories. The

PWAs’ outcome for each cell was attributed to three different performance categories, resulting in an individual

performance profile (see Figure 3 for an example). Based on the maximum scores of the cells and a confidence level

of 95% according to the binomial model (Willmes, 2000), the categories were calculated separately for receptive (24

8

items, max. 48 points) and expressive (16 items, max. 64 points) cells: “mastered” (≥90% correct according to the

binomial model), “mastered with uncertainties” (≥75%), and “not mastered” (<75%); assessment along the increasing

levels of demand per module could be terminated as soon as non-mastery of a cell became evident.

Embedded within clinical routine, SAPS pre-tests and therapy were carried out by one of the experienced

speech therapists from the aphasia ward, and SAPS post-tests were conducted by the authors who were blind to

treatment foci chosen (K. Niemann, V. Rieger and F. Krzok performed testing as SLT master students, supervised by

S. Abel, W. Huber and K. Willmes as experienced clinicians in aphasiology). For the repeated assessment with the

ANELT, we used its version A for pre-testing and version B for post-testing. The ANELT assessments were also

carried out by the authors as stated above, with pre- and post-test not being conducted by the same examiner.

- Figure 3 about here -

Participants

Within an interval of 5 months, we recruited in-patients from the Aachen Aphasia Ward. Persons with (i) post-acute

or chronic aphasia (≥ 6 months post-onset) (ii) of vascular genesis, (iii) with suitability for language-systematic

training and (iv) a stay at the ward for full 7 weeks were considered for inclusion. Moreover, (v) good comprehension

of task instructions (i.e., reasonable responses to tasks, e.g. for SAPS practice items), and (vi) at least two not

mastered cells in the SAPS pre-test were a prerequisite for final inclusion. Persons with progressive speech and

language disorders, a severe apraxia of speech, or other disorders which were not covered by our study but needed to

be prevailingly addressed in therapy, rendering language-systematic therapy not suitable (e.g., severe neuro-cognitive

impairments, participation-based treatment focus requested), were excluded. Figure 4 gives an overview of the

recruitment process with our inclusion/exclusion criteria. The characteristics of all 16 PWA included into the study

are presented in Table 1. The study was approved by the ethics committee at the Medical Faculty of the RWTH

Aachen University (EK 089/17).

9

- Figure 4 about here -

- Table 1 about here -

Construction of the home-training

The novel SAPS home-training featured supervised, self-guided computer-based training (Power Point Presentation,

Office 2003, Windows XP) covering all SAPS-tasks. The T-items of each SAPS cell were divided into two parallel

sets (set A and set B), rendering 36 presentations for home-training. All presentations were designed equally so PWA

could easily recognize recurring elements of the home-training. To keep handling as easy as possible, each home-

training presentation was kept visually and cognitively simple without distracting elements.

The home-training aimed at multi-modal stimulation of target tasks and items within five levels: three initial

stimulation levels, a consecutive level with SAPS-demands, and a final drill level. The stimulation levels either

offered some preparatory training to prime the target task/items or consisted of the target SAPS-task with additional

cues, modalities and feedback for the according items. On level 4, the PWA performed the task exactly as demanded

in SAPS, without further assistance. All in all, in the course of stimulation the cues vanished and the demands

increased. Each item passed through levels 1-4 before being repeatedly practiced at the drill level with SAPS

demands amongst all other items and in varying order. An example of the home-training (lexical-receptive cell,

easiest level of demand) is shown in in Supplementary Figure S3. The SAPS-tasks for this cell constitutes auditory

word-to-picture matching: On stimulation levels 1-3, the number of distractors is limited, and PWA can repeatedly

listen to the target item which is presented simultaneously in written modality.

10

SAPS-based treatment: face-to-face therapy in combination with home-training

One hour of SAPS-based therapy and one hour of home-training were performed each weekday, summing up to two

hours SAPS-treatment per weekday. For home-training, we supervised every home-training of a new cell, which

might result in up to three supervised sessions per week. SAPS-based therapy was conducted by SLTs from the ward,

who were experienced in impairment-based therapy for aphasia, and they received advise by S. Abel, W. Huber and

K. Willmes in case of any uncertainty. Based on the performance profile derived from the pre-test, the SLT decided

autonomously which foci to work on in therapy and chose two cells, which needed to be either not mastered or

mastered with uncertainties, as starting point for treatment. For SAPS-based therapy, the T-items of the chosen

SAPS-cells were trained using the according SAPS-tasks, and further materials and therapy tasks of similar demands

and structure could complement the approach at the discretion of the SLT, if time allowed. The same SAPS-items

used in therapy were trained during home-training, with each session taking about an hour and each of the two cells

occupying about half of each session. Training success was monitored by assessing performance for the SAPS-items

in the according task at the beginning and end of each one-to-one therapy session; the development of scores from

initial versus final monitoring of the same session was taken to reveal therapy effects, while those from final

monitoring of a session versus initial monitoring of the following session was supposed to reveal home-training

effects. If performance criteria were reached after two days of treatment as registered in initial monitoring of a

session (indicating fast learning for that cell) or after four days (indicating moderate or slow learning), a new

treatment focus was chosen (see also Figure 2). This procedure continued until 16 therapy days had been completed

or no more cells were left to choose from. An overview of the three types of learning as featured in the SAPS-design

is presented in Figure 5. There are three possible versions of progression along treatment days with varying set

sequences which are dependent on a PWA’s learning speed. Since there is only little evidence in the literature on

how often item (sets) need to be repeated until PWA master a certain level of demand, we chose a procedure which is

both simple for the SLT to conduct and flexible to adapt it to individual learning curves, and which allows us to

investigate this issue further. Our approach ensured that PWA could improve in as many cells as possible dependent

on their learning curves (limited by the maximum therapy duration of 16 days), and therefore cover different

11

treatment foci without having a drill effect from overly repeating the relatively small item set of a cell too often (a full

demonstration with criteria for progression is shown in Supplementary Table S1).

- Figure 5 about here -

The current SAPS-based treatment replaced the regular one-to-one therapy plus homework on the ward.

Please note that over and above obligatory group therapies (3x/week), PWA were still able to additionally attend low-

frequent offers at the ward, including music therapy or computer-based therapy. On the ward, the individualised

computer-based therapy did not cover SAPS-items, -task or -demands, and any task similar to SAPS was omitted in

computer-based therapy; instead, PWA focused on writing, internet searches, and the creation of presentations.

Moreover, the SLT had the opportunity to integrate other therapy goals such as writing or basic exercises (e.g. for

apraxia of speech) in the SAPS-therapy. However, it did not exceed the particular SAPS-demand or involve C-items.

Statistical analyses

For the statistical data evaluation we used SPSS Statistics 20 (IBM Corporation, 2011). We generally chose a

stricter threshold (p<.05) for group analyses with many items, and a more liberal threshold for analyses of limited

power, i.e. on the single case level or with fewer items involved. For our first aim and hypothesis on chosen treatment

foci and their effects we conducted group and single case analysis. On the group level, we calculated the performance

changes in the SAPS for all cells and modules as well as for the trained and untrained cells (exact Wilcoxon signed-

rank test, one-tailed, p<.05). Furthermore, we compared the performance changes between trained and untrained cells

(exact Mann-Whitney-U-test, one-tailed, p <. 05, Bonferroni-Holm corrected).

12

In order to investigate the impact of learning speed, we compared performance change in relation to type of

learning (exact Mann-Whitney-U-test, one-tailed, p<.05, Bonferroni-Holm corrected) in case previous tests (Kruskal-

Wallis-test, Jonckheere-Terpstra-test for monotone trends, one-tailed, p<.05) revealed trends per cells/modules. We

also correlated the extent of performance change and the amount of trained cells with the learning speed (Spearman

rank correlation, one-tailed, p<.05). We also examined possible differences in performance change for fast learners

(fast learning in ≥75% of the cells) compared to moderate/slow learners (exact Mann-Whitney-U-test, one-tailed,

p<.05, Bonferroni-Holm corrected). Furthermore, we correlated the percentage of fast learned cells and the

percentage of successfully completed cells with the performance changes in the SAPS (Spearman rank correlation,

one-tailed, p<.05). Finally, we used cross tables in order to consider initial performance over and above the impact of

training.

On the single case level we analysed the performance change for all items as well as for T-items and C-items

separately (exact Wilcoxon signed-rank test, one-tailed, p<.10). Moreover, differential changes for T- versus C-items

were examined (exact Mann-Whitney-U-test, one-tailed, p<.10).

Furthermore, we wanted to assess various factors that might influence the outcome. To investigate the

influence of the type of aphasia (fluent vs. non-fluent) and apraxia of speech on SAPS baseline performance and pre-

post changes in the expressive modules we used exact Mann-Whitney-U-tests (one-tailed, p<.05, Bonferroni-Holm

corrected). In the same way we compared baseline performance and changes in SAPS-performance along the three

recovery stages of aphasia. For revealed trends (Kruskal-Wallis-test, Jonckheere-Terpstra-test for monotone trends,

one-tailed, p<.05) we expected improvements to decrease with time post-stroke. We also investigated possible

monotone trends (expected: post-acute > early chronic > late chronic) related to extent and success of learning,

considering the number of learned cells, fast learned cells, successfully completed cells, and type of learning per

module.

In our second and third hypothesis we focused on the home-training. Therefore we assessed the learning

progress for therapy and home-training by calculating the difference between scores for initial and final therapy

13

monitoring within and between sessions. These differences were further analysed for all PWA (n=16) on the group

level (exact Wilcoxon signed-rank test, one-tailed, p<.10).

Communicative performance was addressed in our fourth hypothesis. We determined changes for the ANELT

along critical differences from Kawalla (2011) (+/- 7 points for scale A; +/- 4 points for scale B) and correlated data

from ANELT and SAPS (Spearman rank correlations, one-tailed, p<.05)

Results

Examination of soundness of treatment design

Choice of treatment foci. Table 2 shows how many cells mastered with uncertainties or not mastered during pre-

testing were chosen for therapy. If a PWA revealed uncertainties in a cell of the phonological-receptive or lexical-

expressive module, it was always chosen. All other imperfectly mastered modules were selected at lower frequencies,

as were those with non-mastered performance levels. While the syntactic-receptive module was regularly (>80%)

chosen if at all applicable for therapy, both lexical modules were regularly selected if not mastered; syntax-

expressive was chosen at lowest rates (41%). For completely not mastered modules, mostly the two lower levels of

demands (1 or 2) were chosen.

- Table 2 -

Based on these results, we further investigated the importance of modules as treatment foci: all but one PWA

received lexical training, in particular in the expressive modality. Moreover, the initial treatment foci regularly

combined a receptive and an expressive cell. In terms of the modalities, there was a trend for selection of receptive

14

cells prior to expressive cells in the phonological, but not in the lexical or syntactic domain, and receptive cells were

generally trained more often. An overview of the chosen cells for each PWA is shown in Supplementary Figure S1.

Types of learning. Due to the high variability of performance profiles at pre-testing, the number of treatment

days varied from 6 to 16 days (see Supplementary Table S2 for individual SAPS test scores). Our analysis of the

learning progress as measured by monitoring showed that, across all modules, more cells were finished after two

rather than four days of training. On average, the PWA completed 73% of the treatment foci with fast learning.

Whereas fast learning was always completed successfully, this was not obligatory for moderate and slow learning. On

average, 85% (range: 50-100%) of cells were finished successfully, while an unsuccessful termination of cells

occurred more often in expressive (15 cells in nine PWA) than receptive modules (3 cells in two PWA).

Changes in performance for PWA

Group analysis. The treatment regimen was effective for nearly all domains and cells of the SAPS (exact Wilcoxon

signed-rank test, one-tailed, p < .05), as illustrated in Figures 6 and 7, as well as Supplementary Table S6.

- Figure 6 and Figure 7 about here -

Moreover, Supplementary Table S6 shows that eight cells revealed a training effect indicated by significantly

higher pre-post changes if the cells were trained compared to not trained (Mann-Whitney U Test, one-tailed,

Bonferroni-Holm corrected, p <. 05). Considering each module separately, we found a similar pattern (see Table 3):

If a module was chosen for treatment, there were proportionally more significant improvements than without training.

For phonological and syntactic modules, the percentage ranged from 54 to 70%; in the lexical -receptive module half

15

of the trained cells improved significantly from pre- to post-test, and with a percentage of 27% the lexical- expressive

module revealed the most modest effect.

- Table 3 about here -

Table 4 shows the correlation between extent of change and training per module. It demonstrates that the more

cells the PWA trained and the faster they learned, the larger the improvements in the expressive phonological and

both syntactical modules (Spearman rank correlation, one-tailed, p < .01). Please note that for the syntactic- receptive

module, we found both improvements and deteriorations as depicted in Figure 6. Nevertheless, there was no

substantial difference in performance change when comparing fast or moderate/slow learners as a group (exact Mann-

Whitney-U-test, one-tailed).

- Table 4 about here -

Single case analysis. We found significant improvements in PWA performance for all modules (exact Wilcoxon

signed-rank test, one-tailed, p<.01 and p<.05) (see Supplementary Table S2 for individual SAPS test scores).

Depending on the module, there were significant changes in performance in nine to twelve PWA. For both

phonological modules as well as the lexical- receptive and syntactic-expressive one we rarely determined a decline of

performance (and if, mostly at p<.05 or p<.10). An exception occurred in the syntactic-receptive module, for which

we found an accumulation of significant declines for the easiest level, while there were many significant

improvements for the most difficult level. Out of eleven significant changes on the lowest level of demand in the

16

syntactic- receptive module, in eight PWA there were declines of performance (four times p<.01, two times p<.05

and two times p<.10) and improvements in three PWA (p<.01).

Furthermore, there were similar gains for T-items and C-items (exact Wilcoxon signed-rank test, one-tailed,

p<.01 and p<.05), whereas significant differences between these item pools, especially for the phonological-

receptive and both syntactic modules, turned out to be in favour of improvements for T-items (exact Mann-Whitney-

U Test, one-tailed, p<.10, p<.05 and p<.01).

Influencing factors. Considering influencing factors, we found that compared to participants with non-fluent

aphasia, those with fluent aphasia (n=6) improved significantly more in the phonological- expressive module (exact

Mann-Whitney U Test, one-tailed, p=.001), whereas the baseline performance was not influenced by this factor.

Apraxia of speech did neither influence baseline performance nor performance changes in expressive modules.

Regarding different stages of aphasia, in the phonological- expressive module persons with early chronic aphasia

showed significantly better baseline performance than persons with post-acute aphasia. After Bonferroni-Holm

correction this result did not reach statistical significance. While in general the stages of aphasia did not influence the

change in SAPS-performance or extent and success of learning for the syntactic-expressive module, extent and

success of learning increased continuously across all stages of aphasia (late chronic > early chronic > post-acute;

Jonckheere-Terpstra-test, one-tailed, p=.014).

Differential effects of therapy and home-training

We were able to show a significant therapy effect in most modules (exact Wilcoxon signed-rank test, one-tailed),

especially for medium and high levels of demand. In contrast, some add-on effect of home-training was evident for a

few modules only. The results for set A are illustrated in Figure 8 (Supplementary Figure S2 shows the results for set

B which revealed comparable effects). Comparing effects for therapy versus home-training for set A (respectively, B)

revealed a superiority of therapy (exact Wilcoxon signed-rank test, one-tailed, mostly p<.05 or p<.01). Again, we

17

found significant differences especially for medium and high levels.

- Figure 8 about here -

Construction and feasibility of home-training.

All but one PWA were able to accomplish the home-training, speaking for its feasibility and a good prior estimation

of required instruction comprehension. The exceptional case was excluded from the study during the course of

treatment due to serious problems in understanding and handling of the home-training. He achieved only a percentile

rank of 15 in the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT; Huber et al. 1983) subtest comprehension, although his comprehension

seemed less impaired in everyday situations. Three PWA had been excluded because of their poor comprehension

skills, corresponding to percentile ranks below 15. All other included PWA showed percentile ranks between 16 and

100, which can be seen in Supplementary Table S3 and S4.

The duration of home-training in the afternoon varied depending on PWA’s computer experience, the

different types of SAPS tasks, the amount of assistance requested by the PWA, as well as comorbidities and

neuropsychological impairments (e.g., apraxia or a reduced attention span). In 14 out of 16 PWA, the frequency of

supervision was sufficient, i.e. PWA became capable to perform the task all by themselves. This might be related to

the consistent structure of the home-training which enabled most of the PWA to easily train independently. In some

PWA, even a lower rate of supervision would have been adequate.

The home-training had been constructed so as to achieve a high number of repetitions at all steps to offer most

intensive stimulation. This procedure was not equally indicated for all PWA. Therefore, the home-training was

somewhat adapted throughout the course of the study to skip particular steps. PWA were asked for feedback to the

home-training and it appears that they were highly pleased with the home-training and enjoyed the possibility to

18

repeat the content of therapy independently on the same day.

Communicative performance (ANELT)

With regard to communicative performance, we found significant improvements for ANELT scales A

(understandability of the message) and B (intelligibility of the message) in three and four PWA, respectively (see

Supplementary Table S5 for individual ANELT test scores), summing up to 5 PWA improving significantly in the

ANELT. Significant deteriorations could only be found for the B-scale, namely in 3 PWA. Interestingly, considering

the whole sample, stable ANELT performance occurred for two PWA with poorest or very low pre-test performance,

respectively (10 and 21 points on scale A; 10 and 24 points on scale B) and for two PWA with best pre-test

performance (43 and 45 points on scale A). Regarding the relation between ANELT and SAPS both before and/or

after treatment (Table 5), there were especially high correlations for A scale performance and the lexical-expressive

and syntactic-expressive modules, with lower ones for the receptive modules of these domains. We also found high

correlations with the phonological modules. By and large, these results were also evident in comparison between the

B scale and SAPS, with the correlations with phonology being particularly strong as well. Finally, effects for both

scales were more pronounced in the post-test compared to the pre-test.

- Table 5 about here –

Discussion

In accordance to the first aim and hypothesis, the treatment regimen was effective for nearly all modules and cells as

featured in the SAPS. There were more significant improvements if a cell was trained, so performance was directly

modulated by training. At the same time, there were similar gains for T-items and C-items on the single case level. In

three modules, fast learning correlated with the extent of change – but overall, there was no substantial difference

19

between the three different types of learning.

We also confirmed supremacy of therapy sessions with regard to hypothesis 2, with some add-on effects of

home-training which demonstrates that it is a useful complement to face-to-face therapy. Whereas we got positive

feedback from PWA, length of training and need for supervision varied within the group. As expected, most of the

PWA coped well with the defined amount of supervision, and home-training was well feasible (hypothesis 3).

Finally, we were able to show significant improvements of everyday communication in the ANELT for some

PWA and positive correlations between communicative and linguistic performance, especially for the lexical-

expressive and syntactic- expressive SAPS-modules, revealing that hypothesis 4 was correct.

Evaluation of treatment design

Regarding the choice of treatment foci, there was large variation within our sample. However, we found that lexical

cells were frequently chosen, while the syntactic- receptive cell was rather neglected (see also Blömer et al., 2013).

Especially at the beginning of the treatment phase, receptive and expressive lexical cells were usually combined.

However, the syntactic module revealed the highest response to training if chosen (improvement in 67% of cases),

followed by phonology (62%), and lexicon being final (38.5%). According to Springer (2008), the relevance of a

lexically oriented therapy arises from the frequent presence of lexical disorders in aphasia. Regarding the sequence of

receptive and expressive cells, in our sample we found the trend to choose receptive prior to expressive demands only

for the phonological modules.

From these finding we can derive valuable recommendations: If a PWA presents with similar levels of

impairments across modules, we suggest starting with the syntactic module before training on the lexical and the

phonological one, in order to not oversee the need for sentence level training to improve functional communication.

Furthermore we suggest training receptive tasks prior to expressive ones, since comprehension is generally less

20

demanding and may ease later access in expressive tasks. If impairments only occur within one module, receptive and

expressive tasks can be combined. The suggested therapy rationale already has been considered in a randomised

controlled trial which applied SAPS as baseline and outcome measure, as basis for one-to-one treatment, and which

also integrated the appended computer-based SAPS home-training into the therapy regimen (Breitenstein et al. 2017).

The amount of trained cells was mainly influenced by two factors: the three different types of learning (fast,

moderate and slow learning) and the severity of symptoms - indicated by not mastered and insecurely mastered cells

in the SAPS baseline. Considering the types of learning, within our sample, there was a large scattering. About half

of the PWA completed all of their cells within two treatment days. Basically, in all modules more PWA finished their

cells after two days of treatment. As mentioned before, fast learning was always completed successfully for both item

sets, whereas in moderate learning at least the first set (A) was successfully terminated (so there is change to the

second set B), and in slow learning the first set was not terminated successfully (so A is trained again); a later

successful termination of set (B) did not influence the procedure (i.e., was not obligatory) (Figure 5). In our sample,

we found a higher percentage of unsuccessfully completed cells for expressive modules. A possible explanation

might be the presence of apraxia of speech within our sample. Even though apraxia of speech was not an influencing

factor on therapy outcomes in general, especially participants who suffered from both aphasia and varying degrees of

apraxia of speech might have profited from more prolonged expressive training, or from a stronger focus on

articulatory processes, which reveals a restriction of our study design. Another explanation for this finding may be the

fact that expressive tasks are generally more demanding than receptive ones; post-hoc analyses of pre-test

performance in SAPS-modules revealed that receptive (mean rank: 17) and expressive (mean rank: 16) modules

altogether did not differ significantly, again indicating the good construction of the SAPS (Mann-Whitney U, one-

tailed, p>.10); however, the syntactic module revealed a significantly better performance in the receptive compared to

expressive modality (p=.042).

Altogether, our findings revealed the soundness of the design, i.e. SAPS-based therapy with a maximum of 4

days per chosen focus is feasible and effective. However, given the variability of PWA responses, the flexible

21

adaption of treatment dose per treatment focus appears to be sensible, since fast learning occurred regularly but not

always.

Changes in SAPS-performance for PWA

Based on group and single case analyses, we found significant improvements in all modules of the SAPS.

Furthermore, we found a positive training effect for therapy and home-training. In nine cells there was a significant

superiority of training compared to no training in group analyses, revealing that the combination of therapy with

home-training and the construction of the home-training were successful. Along the same lines, there were

proportionately more significant improvements for all modules in case of training. These effects were particularly

distinct for the phonological and the syntactic modules. Given that seven out of eight PWA with apraxia of speech

had trained in the lexical-expressive module, apraxia of speech may contribute to the finding that especially the

lexical-expressive module improved less.

Despite the high overall improvement in SAPS modules, we also found an exception within the syntactic-

receptive module. At the lowest level of demand, we found an accumulation of significant declines, while there were

many significant improvements at the highest level within this module. This might be explained by the different

sentence structures and strategies suitable to understand these sentences: At the lowest level of demand, the agent has

to be identified in agent-first sentences. On the moderate and difficult levels of demand, the agent has to be identified

in more complex agent-last sentences. In case that the PWA´s sensitivity and understanding of the variety of

underlying syntactic structures increases, they might abandon their agent-first strategy, leading to a decrease of

performance during post-testing of the lowest level. In case that PWA have already reached a more flexible way of

understanding sentences, they are able to stepwise amplify their syntactic repertoire and improve in their performance

at moderate or highest levels. For SAPS-therapy within the current study, we recommended not to go beyond the

level of demand that was to be treated; while this generally makes sense for clinical practice as well, it might be

worth including the variety of sentence structures for the syntax-receptive module as appropriate for the individual

PWA, including agent-first sentences for the first and agent-last sentences for the second level of demand. Moreover,

22

further picture sets for target and foil sentences could be integrated. All in all, by adding complementary material to

the SAPS-based regimen, the Speech and Language Therapists would become more flexible in using a broad variety

of exercises as necessary to meet the individual needs of the PWA and to retain their good motivation over a longer

period of time by using diverse and individualised material which is important for their everyday life.

While similar gains for T-items and C-items speak for training effects with generalisation to untrained items,

the significant differences between both pools reveal that the effects were specific, i.e. improvements were caused by

study-related training rather than unspecific spontaneous recovery or non-study experience. While for therapy

sessions, the trained material could be flexibly complemented by structurally similar material, the home-training only

focused on the trained items, showing its high drill character.

Regarding the changes in performance within all six modules, there were no differences between the three

recovery stages of aphasia. This confirms great potential for improvement for PWA even in the chronic stage of

aphasia (Rose et al. 2013, Basso and Macis 2011). For the syntactic-expressive module we found an increase in

extent and success of training over the different stages of aphasia (late chronic > early chronic > post-acute). These

results suggest that in the course of recovery from aphasia, those language skills which are important for conducting

those with more complex demands, e.g. the production of complex sentences, become increasingly available and

thereby responsive to treatment.

The three different types of learning (fast, moderate and slow) did not generally influence changes in

performance. Only for the phonological-expressive and both syntactic modules, the changes increased with number

of cells trained and with faster learning. Calculations regarding the influence of apraxia of speech on performance

during baseline or on pre-post changes in the expressive modules revealed no significant differences between

participants with and without this additional disorder.

Overall, the modified version of SAPS revealed to be sensitive to changes in performance caused by

treatment. These results are especially promising since the PWA only trained for the relatively short duration of

23

maximal 16 days. The prolongation of treatment might have further enhanced the outcome, a conclusion which is

supported by the finding that the PWA had not generally reached ceiling as measured by session monitoring.

Effects of therapy and home-training

The analysis of the monitoring of therapy and home-training revealed significant and highly significant effects for

therapy in all modules, especially for medium and high levels of demand. One reason for this is the fact that those

levels were trained more often. Although we also found significant add-on effects by home-training in some SAPS-

cells, therapy generally was superior. However, it remains unclear whether therapy would have been as effective

without home-training, as both were combined throughout.

We conclude that SAPS-based home-training was a useful complement to therapy sessions. It stabilises and

strengthens therapy effects by drill and thereby supports the consolidation of therapy content. In this respect we agree

that computer-based home-training can be integrated effectively into aphasia therapy (Allen et al. 2012, Stachowiak

1993, van de Sandt-Koenderman 2011, Zheng et al. 2016). However, due to the drill character, the procedure is

relatively rigid and inflexible; thus, PWA might already reach ceiling effects after therapy sessions, as measured by

monitoring, which might have prevented further increase of performance during home-training. Even though we

revealed superiority of therapy compared to consecutive home-training as an add-on, the two approaches influence

each other and therefore do not allow for an independent comparison; thus we cannot resolve the question whether

supervised computer-based home-training could replace therapy or not (Aftonomos et al. 1999, Stachowiak 1993).

Nevertheless, we show that therapy frequency can be increased by home-training to reach a sufficient amount of

therapy as required by Bhogal et al. (2003b), Kelly et al. (2011) and Brady et al. (2016).

Construction and feasibility of home-training

Our results show that we included and excluded PWA reliably in line with the fixed criteria. In the course of the

study, only one participant needed to be excluded from participation due to low comprehension performance. Thus,

24

we expect that for PWA with a percentile rank of at least 25 as measured by AATs subtest comprehension, the SAPS-

based home-training is well feasible. This accords with the recommendations for the B.A.Bar home-training from

Nobis-Bosch et al. (2011) for which low language comprehension (at least percentile rank 30 in the AAT subtest

comprehension) also was an exclusion criteria. Although the home-training was constructed to take about 30 minutes

per cell and run, some participants completed the daily home-training sessions faster, and some of them even repeated

the home-training tasks on the same day.

Except for two participants with global aphasia, apraxia and inexperience in computer applications, the

participants coped well with the home-training and were satisfied with the pre-defined amount of supervision. These

findings conform to Nobis-Bosch et al. (2011) and Nobis-Bosch et al. (2006) who determined a comparable amount

of supervision (1-3 hours per week) for the B.A.Bar home-training.

The construction of a computer-based home-training offered the utilization of audio and video files for most

tasks (except for lexical-expressive cells and syntactic-expressive cells levels 1 and 2). Based on clinical observations

and feedback from PWA, an adaption of the home-training appears suitable, including multimodal stimulation (audio,

video) for all tasks and higher flexibility to meet the divergent needs of each PWA (e.g., option to choose the number

of repetitions or levels of stimulation).

Changes in communicative performance (ANELT)

In the ANELT, three and four PWA improved significantly on understandability (scale A) and intelligibility scores

(scale B), respectively. Thus, overall the ANELT results do not show the same level of improvement as the SAPS

results, which is attributed to the challenge in SLT to reach translation to everyday language, over and above

generalisation to other tasks and items. Three PWA deteriorated in intelligibility; however, since phonetic processing

and speech output were no SAPS treatment foci, positive changes in intellibility were not expected to occur. Two

PWA (P33, P19) with very poor (scale A or both) and two PWA (P2, P10) with very good (scale A) pre-test

performance in the ANELT remained stable (Table S5). While these PWA did not improve in the ANELT, post-hoc

25

analysis showed that all of them improved in the SAPS. The ANELT assesses communicative performance and

therefore focusses on successful verbal conveyance of information with the linguistic form being of secondary

interest. In contrast, the SAPS emphasises phonological, lexical and syntactic impairments on different levels of

demand in the receptive and expressive modality. Hence it is possible that PWA reveal high scores in the ANELT

(ceiling-effect) although there are language-systematic impairments, which become evident in the SAPS.

Consequently, the ANELT might not be sensitive to further improvements resulting from language-systematic

treatment. On the other hand, poor performance for individuals with severe aphasia often is not well captured in the

ANELT as well. These PWA might indeed benefit from treatment with a language-systematic focus, but their

improvements were not apparent in the ANELT because the demands in this test are too high (floor effect). For future

research it could be helpful to use the Scenario Test (van der Meulen et al. 2010, van der Meulen et al. 2008) as an

additional diagnostic instrument, as this test allows assessment of communicative skills in individuals with severe

aphasia.

Correlations analyses between SAPS and ANELT revealed the highest positive correlations between

understandability and performance in lexical-expressive and syntactic modules in pre- and post-test. This finding

aligns with Blomert and Buslach (1994) who adduced the importance of specific skills besides general linguistic ones

for reaching a good result in the ANELT. We were able to show improved ANELT-performance after language-

systematic treatment in around one-third of PWA; thus, there is high inter-individual variability in transfer to

everyday communication. For future investigations, it would be desirable to complement our therapy procedure by a

communicative approach as stated by Cicerone et al. (2000). By this, it could be evaluated whether PWA might

further improve their communicative abilities.

Conclusion and relevance

We revealed effectiveness of SAPS based therapy and supervised computer-based home-training as well as their

practicability. Despite the small patient sample size, we found positive effects for impaired language components as

26

measured by the SAPS and in around one-third of PWA also for everyday communication as measured by the

ANELT. Based on the SAPS, an individual performance profile can be assessed and psycholinguistically defined

treatment foci can be derived for therapy and home-training with subsequent evaluation of therapy outcomes. The

current feasibility study builds a firm basis for later controlled trials on aphasia therapy with larger sample sizes.

Acknowledgements

We thank Luise Springer for advice on study design and materials, as well as Katja Halm, Nina Scholtes, Kerstin

Schattka and the SLT team at the Aachen Aphasia Ward for support during conduction of the study.

References

ABEL, S., HUBER, W., LONGONI, F., SCHLENCK, K.-J. AND WILLMES, K., in preparation, Sprachsystematisches Aphasiescreening

(SAPS). [Language-systematic aphasia screening (SAPS)] (German test).

AFTONOMOS, L. B., APPELBAUM, J. S. AND STEELE, R. D., 1999, Improving outcomes for persons with aphasia in advanced

community-based treatment programs. Stroke, 30, 1370–1379.

ALLEN, L., MEHTA, S., MCCLURE, A. AND TEASELL, R., 2012, Therapeutic interventions for aphasia initiated more than six month post

stroke: A Rreview of the evidence. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 19, 523–535.

BASSO, A. AND MACIS, M., 2011, Therapy efficacy in chronic aphasia. Behavioral Neurology, 24, 317–325.

BHOGAL, S. K., TEASELL, R. W., FOLEY, N. C. AND SPEECHLEY, M. R., 2003a, Rehabilitation of aphasia: More is better. Topics in

Stroke Rehabilitation, 10, 66–76.

BHOGAL, S. K., TEASELL, R. W. AND SPEECHLEY, M. R., 2003b, Intensity of aphasia therapy, impact on recovery. Stroke, 34, 987-93.

27

BLÖMER, F., PESCH, A., WILLMES, K., HUBER, W., SPRINGER, L. AND ABEL, S., 2013, Das sprachsystematische Aphasiescreening

(SAPS): Konstruktionseigenschaften und erste Evaluierung. [Speech-systematic aphasia screening (SAPS): Properties and initial

evaluation] (German paper). Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 24, 139–148.

BLOMERT, L., 1990, What functional assessment can contribute to setting goals for aphasia therapy. Aphasiology, 4, 307–320.

BLOMERT, L. AND BUSLACH, D., 1994, Funktionelle Aphasiediagnostik mit dem Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test

(ANELT) – Deutsche Fassung. [Functional aphasia diagnostic with the Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test (ANELT) –

German version] (German Paper). Forum Logopädie, 2, 3–6.

BLOMERT, L., KEAN, M.-L., KOSTER, C. AND SCHOKKER, J., 1994, Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test: construction,

reliability and validity. Aphasiology, 8, 381–407.

BLOMERT, L., KOSTER, C., VAN MIER, H. AND KEAN, M.-L., 1987, Verbal communication abilities of aphasic patients: The everyday

language test. Aphasiology, 1, 463–474.

BRADY, M. C., KELLY, H., GODWIN, J., ENDERBY, P. AND CAMPBELL, P., 2016, Speech and language therapy for aphasia following

stroke. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews, 6, CD000425.

BREITENSTEIN, C., GREWE, T., FLÖEL, A., ZIEGLER, W., SPRINGER, L., MARTUS, P., HUBER, W., WILLMES, K.,

RINGELSTEIN, E. B., HAUSLER, K. G., ABEL, S., GLINDEMANN, R., DOMAHS, F., REGENBRECHT, F., SCHLENCK, K.-

J., THOMAS, M., OBRIG, H., DE LANGEN, E., ROCKER, R., WIGBERS, F., RÜHMKORF, C., HEMPEN, I., LIST, J.,

BAUMGAERTNER, A. AND THE FCET2EC STUDY GROUP, 2017, Intensive speech and language therapy in patients with

chronic aphasia after stroke: a randomised, open-label, blinded-endpoint, controlled trial in a health-care setting. Lancet, 389, 1528-

1538.

BROWN, C. M. and HAGOORT, P. (2000). The neurocognition of language (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

CICERONE, K. D., LANGENBAHN, D. M., DAHLBERG, C., KALMAR, K., MALEC, J. F., BERGQUIST, T. F., FLEICETTI, T.,

GIACINA, J. T., HARLEY, J. P., HARRINGTON, D. E., HERZOG, J., KNEIPP, S., LAASCH, L. AND MORSE, P., 2000,

Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: Recommendations for clinical practice. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,

81, 1596–1615.

DE BLESER, R., CHOLEWA, J., STADIE, N. AND TABATABAIE, S., 2004, LEMO - Lexikon modellorientiert: Einzelfalldiagnostik bei

Aphasie, Dyslexie und Dysgraphie (München: Elsevier, Urban und Fischer).

28

HILLIS, A. E. (ed.), 2015, The handbook of adult language disorders. Second edition (New York: Psychology Press).

HUBER, W., POECK, K., WENIGER, D. AND WILLMES, K., 1983, Der Aachener Aphasie Test (Göttingen: Hogrefe).

HUBER, W., SPRINGER, L., and WILLMES, K., 1999, Approaches to aphasia therapy in Aachen. Neurona, Reprint Vol. 16, 36-49.

KAWALLA, M., 2011, Die Evaluation der sprachpragmatischen Fähigkeiten mit dem ANELT - Eine empirische Studie zur Wirksamkeit der

Therapie auf der Aachener Aphasiestation. [Evaluation of pragmatic abilities with the ANELT - An empirical study of the efficacy

of therapy on the Aachen Aphasia Ward]: Rheinisch-Westfälische Technische Hochschule Aachen.

KELLY, H., BRADY, M. C. AND ENDERBY, P., 2011, Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke. The Cochrane database

of systematic reviews, 5, CD000425.

LIEPOLD, M., ZIEGLER, W. AND BRENDEL, B., 2003, Hierarchische Wortlisten. Ein Nachsprechtest für die Sprechapraxiediagnostik

(Dortmund: Verlag modernes Lernen Borgmann).

NOBIS-BOSCH, R., RADERMACHER, I. AND SPRINGER, L., 2006, Das elektronische Hilfsmittel B.A.Bar in der Aphasietherapie: Eine

Einzelfallstudie zum supervidierten Heimtraining. [Supervised home training in aphasia by means of B.A.Bar – a single case study]

(German Paper). Forum Logopädie, 2, 14–19.

NOBIS-BOSCH, R., SPRINGER, L., RADERMACHER, I. AND HUBER, W., 2011, Supervised home training of dialogue skills in chronic

aphasia: a randomized parallel group study. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 54, 1118–1136.

ROSE, M. L., ATTARD, M. C., MOK, Z., LANYON, L. E. AND FOSTER, A. M., 2013, Multi-modality aphasia therapy is as efficacious as

a constraint-induced aphasia therapy for chronic aphasia: A phase 1 study. Aphasiology, 27, 938–971.

SPRINGER, L.,2008, Therapeutic Approaches in Aphasia Rehabilitation. In B. STEMMER andWHITAKER, H. A. (eds.), Handbook of the

neuroscience of language (London, England: Elsevier).

STACHOWIAK, F. J.,1993, Computer-based aphasia therapy with the Lingware/STACH system. In F. J. STACHOWIAK, BLESER, R. D.,

DELOCHE, G., KASCHEL, R., KREMIN, H. andNORTH, P. (eds.), Developments in the assessment and rehabilitation of brain-

damaged patients (Tübingen: Gunther Narr Verlag).

STARK, B. C. AND WARBURTON, E. A., 2016, Improved language in chronic aphasia after self-delivered iPad speech therapy.

Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 1–14.

29

VAN DE SANDT-KOENDERMAN, W. M. E., 2011, Aphasia rehabilitation and the role of computer technology: Can we keep up with

modern times? International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 21–27.

VAN DER MEULEN, I., VAN DE SANDT-KOENDERMAN, W. M., DUIVENVOORDEN, H. J. AND RIBBERS, G. M., 2010, Measuring

verbal and non-verbal communication in aphasia: reliability, validity, and sensitivity to change of the Scenario Test. International

Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45, 424-35.

VAN DER MEULEN, I., VAN GELDER-HOUTHUIZEN, J., WIEGERS, J., WIELAERT, S. AND VAN DE SANDT -KOENDERMAN, M.

W. E., 2008, Scenario Test (Houten (NL): Bohn Stafleu van Loghum).

VISCH‐BRINK, E., VAN DE SANDT-KOENDERMAN, W. M. E. AND EL HACHIOUI, H., 2010, ScreeLing (Nederland: Bohn).

WENIGER, D. AND SPRINGER, L.,2006, Therapie von Aphasien. In W. HARTJE and POECK, K. (eds.), Klinische Neuropsychologie, 6.

ed. (Stuttgart: Thieme).

WILLMES, K., 2010, The methodogical and statistical foundations of neuropsychological assessment. In J. M. GURD, U. KISCHKA, & J.

C. MARSHALL (eds.), Handbook of clinical neuropsychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

WORRALL, L., SHERRATT, S., ROGERS, P., HOWE, T., HERSH, D., FERGUSON, A. AND DAVIDSON, B., 2011, What people with

aphasia want: Their goals according to the ICF. Aphasiology, 25, 309–322.

ZHENG, C., LYNCH, L. AND TAYLOR, N., 2016, Effect of computer therapy in aphasia: a systematic review. Aphasiology, 30, 211–244.

30