23
_______________________________________________________________ _______________________________________________________________ Report Information from ProQuest December 03 2014 09:53 _______________________________________________________________ 03 December 2014 ProQuest

The Role of Mixed Emotion

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

The Role of Mixed Emotions in Consumer Behaviour

Citation preview

Page 1: The Role of Mixed Emotion

_______________________________________________________________

_______________________________________________________________ Report Information from ProQuestDecember 03 2014 09:53_______________________________________________________________

Page 2: The Role of Mixed Emotion

Document 1 of 1 The role of mixed emotions in consumer behaviour Author: Penz, Elfriede; Hogg, Margaret K ProQuest document link Abstract: Mixed emotions (i.e. consumer ambivalence) play a central role in approach-avoidance conflicts inretailing. In order to assess how consumer ambivalence impacts shopping behaviour, this paper seeks toconceptualize and investigate the multi-dimensional antecedents of approach-avoidance conflicts, experiencedby shoppers in changing retail environments, and the importance of approach-avoidance conflicts forconsumers' decision to stay and complete their purchase in that particular shopping channel. Using a cross-country study, which compared online and offline consumers, the paper tested the influence of the situation,product, and reference group on shoppers' intentions; and identified how consumers' mixed emotions influencedapproach-avoidance conflicts in different retail settings. Whereas some distinctions could be drawn betweenonline and offline contexts when examining the impact of market-related, product-related and social factors onconsumers' decision to shop (H1, H2, H3 and H4), no clear distinction could be drawn between online andoffline channels in terms of mediating effects of mixed emotions (H5, H6 and H7). Mixed emotions(ambivalence) did mediate the impact of certain product-related, market-related and personal factors onconsumers' intention to purchase. Retailers need to reduce the impact of consumers' emotional responses tothe retail setting where mixed emotions are likely to lead to consumers leaving the stores. For online shops,those retailers are successful who are able to induce behavioural reactions that make consumers return andexplore the web site and not use it for search only. Responding to calls for further research on mixed emotionsand their consequences, the paper captures the complex impact of consumers' mixed emotions on approach-avoidance conflicts, and thereby extends earlier work on consumer ambivalence. Full text: Introduction Emotions are central to consumption ([3] Bagozzi et al. , 1999; [42] Holbrook and Hirschman, 1982; [48] Leoneet al. , 2005; [94] Watson and Spence, 2007) and particularly to understanding consumer behaviour in serviceenvironments such as retailing ([35] Foxall and Greenley, 1999). Mixed emotions (i.e. consumer ambivalence)play a key role in consumer behaviour. However, with a few exceptions (e.g. [64] Otnes et al. , 1997), currentresearch has largely failed to capture the complex impact of consumers' mixed emotions on approach-avoidance conflicts. In response to the specific call for further research into the "ramifications of mixedemotions" ([94] Watson and Spence, 2007, p. 506), and in order to contribute to earlier work on consumerambivalence ([63] Olsen et al. , 2005; [64] Otnes et al. , 1997; [77] Ruth et al. , 2002), we conceptualize andinvestigate the inter-relationship between consumer ambivalence (i.e. mixed emotions) and the multi-dimensional antecedents of approach-avoidance conflicts in different retail settings. Retail channels represent a rich empirical context for investigating approach-avoidance conflicts in consumerbehaviour ([35] Foxall and Greenley, 1999; [36] Foxall and Yani-de-Soriano, 2005). While there is increasingresearch on e-channels as new distribution modes ([95] Wikström, 2005) and also on the emotional aspects ofconsumer choices in online environments ([29] Eroglu et al. , 2003; [31], [32] Fiore et al. , 2000, 2005; [56]Menon and Kahn, 2002), very little is known about the different motivational conflicts that occur in onlinecompared with offline shopping environments. Our aim is to investigate how online shopping and traditionalbrick and mortar shopping environments differ with respect to the inter-relationship between consumerambivalence and approach-avoidance conflicts. Our objective is to examine the antecedents that causeconsumers to use one retail channel as a way of escaping from the other channel in response to the negative ormixed emotional states provoked by their patronage of the original channel. This will contribute to understandingwhy consumers abandon their purchases in a brick and mortar store in favour of an online channel; or,

Page 3: The Role of Mixed Emotion

alternatively, quit online transactions and turn to offline channels. The findings will contribute to the developmentof managerial strategies, which could help reduce these lost sales. We begin by reviewing current research on mixed emotions, ambivalence and approach-avoidance conflicts.We then consider the impact of antecedents (linked to product-relevant, market-relevant, [66] Pan and Zinkhan,2006) and social factors such as the situation, the product or service, and social influences on purchaseintentions in online and traditional brick and mortar shopping environments. From this literature review wederive a conceptualization, which models ambivalence in relation to approach-avoidance conflicts. Hypothesesare formulated and tested using a large-scale international survey. We present and interpret our findings; andconclude with a more general discussion first, about how the impact of various antecedents of approach-avoidance conflicts differs across retailing channels; and second about the mediating effect of ambivalence (i.e.mixed emotions) on consumers' experiences of approach-avoidance conflicts, and their intentions to purchase. Conceptual background Consumer ambivalence and approach-avoidance conflicts in online and traditional brick and mortar shoppingenvironments A consumer's intention to buy a product does not necessarily result in its purchase. Consumers may have astrong desire to buy a product. However they can feel constrained by various factors from completing apurchase, and can sometimes even entirely abandon their initial intention to purchase. Purchase decisions maybe abandoned because of a "competition between incompatible responses" within an individual ([57] Miller,1944, p. 431). The simultaneous appearance of positive and negative tendencies leads to an inner conflict andresults in approach or avoidance behaviour. The differences between approach and avoidance motivation havebeen linked to valence: "in approach motivation, behavior is instigated or directed by a positive/desirable eventor possibility, whereas in avoidance motivation, behavior is instigated or directed by a negative/undesirableevent or possibility" ([27] Elliot and Thrash, 2002, p. 804). Approach-avoidance conflict theory therefore helpsus understand how consumers think about and manage the potential negative aspects or consequences(avoidance factors) involved in making a purchase decision (for more on approach factors, see also [35] Foxalland Greenley, 1999; [36] Foxall and Yani-de-Soriano, 2005). Consumers are motivated to manage the mixed emotions they experience in the marketplace that either drivethem towards or inhibit them from making purchases either online or offline. Overload is one of the keyantecedents of consumer ambivalence where consumers feel "overwhelmed or ill-prepared during thepurchasing process and the sheer volume of purchasing decisions to be made" ([64] Otnes et al. , 1997, p. 87).The freedom to choose turns into a form of hyperchoice, i.e. consumers are confronted with (and are oftenoverwhelmed by) the paralyzing nature of choice represented by the increasing number of outlets to choosefrom; by having to decide what to choose from an ever-expanding range of products and services; and alsodeciding when to make their choice. Greater choice is thus accompanied by fears of not being able to controlone's behaviour; the risk of information overload; and cognitive dissonance evoked by feelings of regret whichare derived, in part, from the products which have not been selected ([38] Harrison et al. , 2006). Individuals are generally motivated to minimize their experiences of arousal generated via such tensions,inconsistencies, or mixed emotions. However a certain level of arousal has been shown to be indispensable forevoking emotions such as pleasure, joy or happiness, and also their negative opposites (e.g. [74] Priester andPetty, 1996; [78] Schachter and Singer, 1962). On experiencing arousal, individuals are motivated to search forpossible causes for their state of arousal in the environment ([78] Schachter and Singer, 1962). This allowsindividuals to label the arousal and the positive emotions they experience, such as joy or pleasure. Unexplainedarousal, in contrast, is perceived as unpleasant and usually leads to the assignment of a negative label, e.g.fear or apprehension ([41] Hogg and Vaughan, 2005). In early work on choice, [37] Hansen (1976) modelled aninverted u-shaped conflict curve that resulted from the stimulation of the environment, and captured thedivergent influence of arousal on individual experience. The conflict between arousal and the environment is

Page 4: The Role of Mixed Emotion

seen to be at its highest at a moderate level of arousal. This is noteworthy since [23] Donovan and Rossiter(1982) argue that arousal has positive effects only in pleasant environments while an unpleasant store, forexample, might even lead to negative emotions. This is true only for positive (pleasant) or negative (unpleasant)situations. In a neutral situation (neither pleasant nor unpleasant) moderate arousal leads to greater approach,while very high or very low arousal leads to avoidance behaviours. Figure 1 [Figure omitted. See Article Image.]illustrates a possible link between ambivalence (mixed emotions) and approach-avoidance conflicts based onthe combination of pleasure and arousal. Emotional reactions to any environment include pleasure-displeasure (PL), arousal-nonarousal (AR) anddominance-submissiveness (DO) (known as PAD dimensions) and are said to fully account for emotionalresponses to any environment ([23] Donovan and Rossiter, 1982; [24] Donovan et al. , 1994). PL describesfeelings of joy and happiness while AR refers to excitement, alertness and stimulation ([92] Van Kenhove andDesrumaux, 1997, p. 353). Emotions can be viewed as the outcome of explaining arousal, and are positive,negative or mixed (ambivalence) ([39] Harrist, 2006; [41] Hogg and Vaughan, 2005; [78] Schachter and Singer,1962). [5] Baker et al. (1992) argued that "affective states produced by the store environment do influenceconsumers' willingness to buy" (p. 457). Affective states determine whether or not consumers spend time in astore and will return again ([23] Donovan and Rossiter, 1982). The conflicting nature of reactions, shapes theperception of ambivalence, and consumers' consequent experiences of approach, and avoidance tendencies. Within the framework of human-computer interaction, affect and emotion have been identified as importantinfluences (see [69] Peter and Beale, 2008). With regard to online shopping, the emotional states of pleasureand arousal are important for their potential impact on consumers' future behaviours, particularly browsing ([56]Menon and Kahn, 2002). Pleasure experienced during shopping online leads to positive attitudes towards futureapproach behaviour and a preference for stimulation. Stimulation, in turn, affects future approach behaviourbecause too much arousal due to over-stimulating environments can lead to lower browsing behaviour. Fromhere, we propose the following hypotheses regarding the emotional states and behavioral aspects of approach-avoidance conflicts H1. Emotional states (pleasure, H1a ; arousal, H1b ; dominance, H1c ), behavioural aspects of approach-avoidance conflicts (enjoyment, H1d ; return and explore, H1e ) as well as ambivalence (H1f ) differ betweenoffline and online stores. In investigating ambivalence, most studies of approach-avoidance behaviour have concentrated on consumers'experiences within the retail environments, rather than examining how shoppers are influenced in their choicesby the variety of conflicts that they potentially experience in relation to other market-related, product-related([66] Pan and Zinkhan, 2006) and social factors. These factors can include the situation (e.g. the rate ofinformation in the store environment); the product or service to be purchased (e.g. perceived product risk; levelof involvement); and the social influences such as reference group. These all influence intentions to patronize orpurchase from a store; and often generate approach-avoidance conflicts linked to well-recognized emotionalstates such as arousal, pleasure, enjoyment and the desire to explore the store in the retail environment. Thenext section discusses possible sources of approach-avoidance conflict. Antecedents to approach-avoidance conflicts Environmental psychology is drawn on in order to gain a thorough understanding of the consumers' internalprocesses and behavioural responses to the two different retail store formats. It informs our understanding ofthe: Impact of environmental stimuli on consumers, as well as of the influences from the social environment. The influence of the merchandise itself. Physical environment - environmental stimuli Retail atmospherics are important for both offline and online channels because they comprise variables thataffect shopping behaviour ([23] Donovan and Rossiter, 1982, p. 34). Within retailing management, the influence

Page 5: The Role of Mixed Emotion

of the store atmosphere on consumer responses is conceptualized in earlier studies (e.g. [23] Donovan andRossiter, 1982; [24] Donovan et al. , 1994), drawing heavily on the Mehrabian-Russell environmentalpsychology model (see [35] Foxall and Greenley, 1999, for a detailed review of earlier studies, which haveemployed this model; [55] Mehrabian and Russell, 1974). Store atmosphere is seen as a better predictor ofconsumers' patronage of a store because it overcomes previous limitations of using unidimensionality andsingular variables when exploring the concept of store image ([23] Donovan and Rossiter, 1982; [24] Donovanet al. , 1994). In addition, the behavioural perspective model ([34] Foxall, 1997; [35] Foxall and Greenley, 1999;[36] Foxall and Yani-de-Soriano, 2005) outlines the influence of the intersection between the consumerbehaviour setting and learning history on consumer behaviour. Among store atmosphere variables, the information rate is the main construct that accounts for the influence ofenvironmental stimuli on consumer responses ([55] Mehrabian and Russell, 1974). Information rate representsthe degree of information inputs in a channel during a certain time frame ([84] Spies et al. , 1997), andcomprises novelty, complexity, density and size ([24] Donovan et al. , 1994; [86] Tai and Fung, 1997). Whilenovelty refers to unexpected, surprising and new elements, complexity refers to the number of elements andchanges in the environment. Density describes those elements in the environment that either made a personfeel that they have sufficient privacy within which to shop; or make a person feel crowded, threatened andrestricted in terms of movement. Finally, size refers to the scale of the store, the interior decoration, or spatialelements such as how well the merchandise is organized and displayed ([86] Tai and Fung, 1997). The physical environment can be classified into two sets of factors: ambient (smell, temperature, music) anddesign (location of items, size of store) ([4] Baker, 1986; [19] d'Astous, 2000). These factors can be the sourceof shopping irritants ([19] d'Astous, 2000) inducing negative emotions and behaviour in the retail channel. (Notethat irritants can also stem from social factors, such as crowding or the behaviour of the sales personnel; andalso from characteristics of the product itself, e.g. unfavourable information; [61] Mizerski (1982)). Ambientfactors are not noticed unless they reach an unpleasant level, e.g. when the lighting is too bright or the music istoo loud. Design factors, however, are actively evaluated by shoppers and include aesthetic elements such asarchitecture, colour and style, which can increase a consumer's sense of pleasure when entering a store.Functional design elements, such as layout or comfort, can also contribute to a shopper's sense of well being.Atmospheric effects have been shown to influence online purchase intentions, for example music ([1] Allan,2008; [96] Wu et al. , 2008), and colours ([96] Wu et al. , 2008). Just as consumers are influenced by the rate and amount of information in store (e.g. its novelty, complexity,density and size, [84] Spies et al. , 1997), which can stimulate or overwhelm customers, there is similarevidence for consumers' experiences of feeling stimulated or overwhelmed by web sites when shopping online([31] Fiore et al. , 2005). For online stores, web site design and navigation are vital, and research has confirmedthe importance of clear interfaces during all stages of the decision making process ([65] Palmer, 2002; [82]Sinkovics and Penz, 2005). The design of the web site is central to determining the atmosphere experienced byconsumers online ([54] McCarthy and Aronson, 2000; [93] Wang and Tang, 2003). In a model of consumers'web navigational behaviour a series of influencing variables were identified, which included flow experience,need for cognition or level of preferred stimulation, as well as situational factors such as site involvement ([76]Richard and Chandra, 2005). The retail atmospherics experienced in an online store can lead to positiveemotions, and thus to favourable attitudes towards online-shopping, the presented goods, and consequentlyhigher satisfaction with a purchase ([28] Eroglu et al. , 2001; [29] Eroglu et al. , 2003). Online shopping usuallywins in terms of price advantage and convenience when compared to traditional retail formats, but the onlinechannel is less attractive with regard to enjoyment and sociability. The social aspects of shopping have been along-standing source of consumer pleasure in offline retail environments [see the following] (e.g. [22] Dennis etal. , 2002; [31] Fiore et al. , 2005; [90] Tractinsky and Rao, 2001). We propose that the store's physicalattributes, i.e. the information rate, differ between offline and online shopping environments. In more detail we

Page 6: The Role of Mixed Emotion

hypothesize that: H2. Online stores are perceived as more complex (H2a ), more novel (H2b ) and denser (H2c ) respectivelythan offline stores. Physical environment - social stimuli and reference group influence The physical environment also includes social factors ([4] Baker, 1986), which refer to the presence of anaudience, fellow customers and/or service personnel who are responsible for generating the atmosphere (whichcan be positive, e.g. a lively bar; or negative, e.g. busy checkout queues). The attractive appearance and thepleasant behaviour of service personnel, as well as the presence of other customers, can also be reassuringcomponents in a retail environment. Other social factors include reference groups, which are "social groups thatare important to a consumer and against which he or she compares himself or herself" ([30] Escalas andBettman, 2003, p. 341). The store environment, such as crowding and stimulation, as well as sales staffassistance, can increase impulse purchases ([52] Mattila and Wirtz, 2008). Reference group influence dependson the type of product (e.g. luxury or necessity) and whether the products are consumed publicly or privately([15] Childers and Rao, 1992). Reference groups exert two main types of social influence: informational andnormative (the latter can be either value-expressive or utilitarian) ([7] Bearden and Etzel, 1982; [15] Childersand Rao, 1992; [50] Mangleburg et al. , 2004). Word of mouth in this shopping context can be linked tomavenism, retail opinion sharing, and susceptibility to interpersonal influence. Susceptibility relates either toshoppers' needs to receive rewards or avoid being punished (i.e. the exertion of utilitarian influence), or toshoppers' needs for "psychological affiliation" (i.e. value-expressive influence) ([15] Childers and Rao, 1992, p.199). While reference groups play a very important role in traditional brick and mortar stores, very little is known aboutthe effects of this type of social influence on consumers when shopping online. Except for participating in virtualcommunities or chat groups, online shoppers act in a "lonely crowd" ([10] Burton, 2002, p. 793). Someconsumers might want to use online shopping on purpose to avoid consumer culture, choosing to deliberatelyavoid the sociability associated with traditional retail outlets. This provides evidence for what might be seen asthe anti-social nature of some of these forms of online shopping ([10] Burton, 2002). We propose that thereference group's influence differs between online and offline shopping environments. Thus, the following ishypothesized: H3. Normative influence (H3a ), informational influence (H3b ), interpersonal influence (H3c ) and opinionsharing on decision-making (H3d ) respectively are stronger in an offline than in an online store. Product influence Goods or services involve different levels of perceived risk and involvement. The concept of risk encompassesboth potentially positive and negative outcomes ([59] Mitchell, 1999), with a focus mainly on potentially negativeoutcomes. Perceived risk comprises "subjective expectations of loss" ([85] Stone and Gronhaug, 1993, p. 42)linked to uncertainty and consequences ([16] Conchar et al. , 2004; [17] Cox, 1967; [26] Dowling and Staelin,1994; [37] Hansen, 1976; [43] Jacoby and Kaplan, 1972; [44] Kaplan et al. , 1974; [58] Mitchell and Hogg,1997). Uncertainty involves confidence, reliability, dependability, trust likelihood and probability, whereasconsequences involve trust, danger, relevance and seriousness ([58] Mitchell and Hogg, 1997). Perceived riskencompasses several dimensions such as product performance, financial, psychological, time, social, andphysical risk ([44] Kaplan et al. , 1974). Risk perception increases rather than decreases in different phases ofthe consumer decision-making process ([60] Mitchell and Boustani, 1994): pre-purchase search and purchaseevaluation can be very challenging phases. Involvement, on the other hand, is the importance which a shopper attaches to a particular product "based ontheir inherent needs, values and interests" ([97] Zaichkowsky, 1985, p. 342). High involvement products areregarded as more risky than low involvement products. With regard to product involvement, research shows thatconsumers seeking excitement use the internet for surfing, downloading software or communicating ([79]

Page 7: The Role of Mixed Emotion

Schiffman et al. , 2003). Consumer purchase behaviour when shopping online is fundamentally different fromtraditional or offline-shopping ([22] Dennis et al. , 2002), indicating that standardized or search goods areusually more suitable for online-shopping (e.g. books, music) when compared with experience goods (e. g.personal care products, clothes, [14] Chiang and Dholakia, 2003; [46] Kwak et al. , 2002; [62] Monsuwe et al. ,2004; [81] Shim et al. , 2001); and supporting the argument that the more tangible a product, the more suitableit is for online-shopping (see also [45] Koernig, 2003, on services). Perceived risk, in its turn, was found to function as a motivational barrier to purchasing online ([51] Mariani andZappalà, 2006; [68] Penz and Kirchler, 2006) and to have a negative impact on the evaluation of the experienceof online-purchasing. This helps to explain barriers to online-shopping ([33] Forsythe and Shi, 2003; [88] Teoand Yeong, 2003). In order to reduce risk, strategies such as using reference group appeal, marketer'sreputation or brand image are proposed ([87] Tan, 1999). Therefore, we propose the following: H4. Product risk (social, H4a ; financial, H4b ; performance, H4c ) is higher in an online than in an offline store,but product involvement (need for product, H4d ; exciting product, H4e ) is higher in an offline than in an onlinestore. Emotional states and behavioral aspects of approach-avoidance conflict that intervene to affect the predictabilityof purchase intention So far, we have emphasized the value of including emotional states in explaining consumer responses (H1 )and have discussed the importance of three factors (situation, product, reference group) in two retail formats (H2 , H3 and H4 ). Next, therefore, we develop a conceptual model (see Figure 2 [Figure omitted. See ArticleImage.]) that deals with the mediating effect of emotional states and behavioural aspects of approach-avoidance conflicts on the relationship between the conflict antecedents and purchase intentions. Based on the conceptualization as illustrated in Figure 2 [Figure omitted. See Article Image.], the followinghypotheses were formulated: H5. The impact of the store's physical attributes (complexity, H5a ; novelty, H5b ; density, H5c ) on futureshopping intentions is mediated by emotional states, behavioural responses and ambivalence. H6. The reference group influence (normative influence, H6a ; informational influence, H6b ; interpersonalinfluence, H6c ; decision making, H6d ) on future shopping intentions is mediated by emotional states,behavioural responses and ambivalence. H7. The influence of product characteristics (social risk, H7a ; financial risk, H7b ; performance risk, H7c ; needfor product, H7d ; exciting product, H7e ) on future shopping intentions is mediated by emotional states,behavioural responses and ambivalence. See Table I [Figure omitted. See Article Image.] for a summary of the concepts and hypotheses developed forempirical testing. Research design The purpose of this research was to extend understanding of consumer ambivalence by identifying andinvestigating the impact of mixed emotions on approach-avoidance conflicts in consumer behaviour using acomparative study of online and offline settings. From an initial literature review and framework, a conceptualmodel (see Figure 2 [Figure omitted. See Article Image.]) was developed of approach-avoidance behaviour inconflicts, and operationalized via a series of related hypotheses, which were tested. An instrument was developed that captured first, the possible sources of conflicts; second, approach-avoidanceemotional states; and third, behavioural responses in conflicts. For the questionnaire context, before answeringthe questions the respondents had to think of a recent shopping situation where they had bought something(either for themselves or someone else) that had led to an approach-avoidance conflict (this was fully explainedat the beginning of the questionnaire). Scales were taken from existing literature and were adapted for our research purposes. To capture possiblesources of conflicts which derive from:

Page 8: The Role of Mixed Emotion

- the purchase/shopping situation, we used [86] Tai and Fung's (1997) measure of information rate. In additionwe measured the effect of store atmosphere (offline only) and web site effectiveness (online only, [82] Sinkovicsand Penz, 2005). In order to elaborate the causes related to - products/services, we measured different levels of risk perception ([21] DelVecchio and Smith, 2005; [25]Doolin et al. 2005), and product involvement ([98] Zaichkowsky, 1994). Another set of items measured reference group influence on shopping related aspects. In particular, scales thatcaptured interpersonal influence susceptibility ([8], [9] Bearden et al. , 1989, 1990) and retail opinion sharing([67] Paridon, 2004) were employed. The approach-avoidance conflict was operationalized using [55]Mehrabian and Russell's (1974) measure of emotional states and the behavioural aspect of intention. Convenience sampling was used for this international study (Austria, Greece, the UK). These countries werechosen first, because consumers in all three countries use both traditional and online modes of shopping; andsecond because Austria and Greece represent northern and southern Europe respectively, while the UKrepresents "off shore Europe" with strong links to the USA and its rather different patterns of consumption andshopping channel behaviour. Sampling in international marketing research is critical. In our case we wereprimarily interested in developing a conceptual model of mixed emotions and the impact on purchase intentionsbased on an international sample (contextual research) and not on a comparative study that identifiedsimilarities and differences across countries ([75] Reynolds et al. , 2003). Following [49] Malhotra et al. (1996)we focused on behaviour settings as units for the present study rather than the nation-state information.According to [49] Malhotra et al. (1996), "a behaviour setting represents all the forces acting on individualmembers of a setting to enter and participate in its operation in particular ways [...] The selection of cultures(cultunits or behaviour settings) to be investigated should be based on the theoretical or applied objectives ofthe study (p. 25)". Based on extensive exploratory research (which included the collection of stories fromrespondents in UK and Austria, interviews, secondary data and literature review) in all three countries, weselected shopping (online and brick and mortar) as behaviour settings. Participants were approached either inperson and invited to complete a hard copy questionnaire; or participants were approached via e-mail with an e-questionnaire that was available on a web site. The condition (offline or online shopping) was randomlyassigned to both sets of respondents. In total, there were 335 usable questionnaires (Austria n =127, Greece n =111, the UK n =117). A total of 171answers referred to traditional shopping contexts (Austria n =61, Greece n =53, the UK n =57), 184 answerswere for online shopping (Austria n =66, Greece n =58, the UK n =60). About half of the respondents were agedbetween 18 and 25 years old. The remaining half was between 26 and 30+. A total of 54.1 per cent were femaleand 45.9 per cent were male. The samples can be characterized as being representative for the youngersegments in the respective countries. In the online shopping condition, tickets were the product ( f =76) mostoften chosen to discuss, followed by electronics (f =46). In traditional shopping contexts, respondents mainlyreported their experiences of purchasing clothing (f =63) and electronics (f =52). See Table II [Figure omitted.See Article Image.] for summary statistics of different patterns of online product purchase by consumers inAustria, Greece and the UK compared with consumers overall in the EU 27. Findings In order to test the hypotheses, factor and reliability analyses were first conducted to derive a manageablenumber of dimensions. The procedure involved running exploratory factor analyses, eliminating those items withdouble-loadings and/or low loadings and calculating reliability measures (Cronbach's alpha, [18] Cronbach,1951) for the remaining items. The explained variance was above 50 per cent for all scales and Cronbach'salpha ranged from 0.42 to 0.92 (see Table III [Figure omitted. See Article Image.]). The extracted dimensionswere the basis for a series of correlation and t-tests and multiple mediated regression analyses. In order to come up with a numerical value for ambivalence, an index was calculated based on subjectiveambivalence considerations ([47] Lavine, 2004; [74] Priester and Petty, 1996; [89] Thompson and Zanna, 1995).

Page 9: The Role of Mixed Emotion

Our index is based on the assumption that emotional states and behavioural responses could co-exist, either aspositive, as negative or as a mixture of positive and negative experiences. The index ranges between -2.5 and2.5. The higher the index the more the ambivalence is felt: Equation 1 [Figure omitted. See Article Image.] Firstof all, we tested H1 , H2 , H3 and H4 and found significant differences between the retail formats for the effectsof various influences (see Table IV [Figure omitted. See Article Image.]). Regarding the emotional states,pleasure (H1a ), arousal (H1b ) and dominance/control (H1c ) were higher in the online store, as well as thedesire to return and explore the store (H1e ). The felt ambivalence, however, was higher in the traditional store (H1f ). The situation when shopping online is perceived as more complex (confirms H2a ). Product-specificinfluences were found in the performance risk (H4c ), which is perceived to be lower in the traditional store. Bothforms of product involvement were higher in the traditional store (H4d and H4e ). Finally, no differences in termsof reference group influence, was found (rejecting H3a , H3b , H3c and H3d ). In order to find out how the emotional states relate to each other correlation analyses were run separately forthe two conditions (online versus offline). In the online condition, arousal was significantly correlated with theother emotional states, i.e. pleasure, dominance, enjoyment of the online store and intention to return andexplore the store. Pleasure is also positively related to enjoyment and intention to return and explore the store.Dominance is positively correlated with pleasure and intention to return and explore the store. Thus, in theonline store, high arousal produces positive emotional and behavioural responses. In the traditional store, arousal is not correlated to these emotional states (see Table V [Figure omitted. SeeArticle Image.]). However, enjoyment correlates significantly with pleasure and the willingness to return andexplore the store, which is also correlated with pleasure. Finally, pleasure and dominance is also significantlycorrelated. To sum up, the emotional states are correlated. While in the online store arousal is linked to theother emotional states, this is not the case in the traditional store. Next, in order to test the mediating effects of emotional states, either as mixed or non-mixed states, a multiplemediation design was used[1] . Mediation exists when a predictor affects a dependent variable indirectlythrough a mediator, which is an intervening variable. A mediator accounts for a relation between predictor andcriterion. It explains how external events take on psychological significance. Mediators specify when certaineffects will hold ([6] Baron and Kenny, 1986; [71] Preacher and Hayes, 2004; [73] Preacher et al. , 2007). Theanalytical procedure follows Preacher et al. ([71], [72] Preacher and Hayes, 2004, 2008; [73] Preacher et al. ,2007) and makes use of bootstrapping as a method to test for the indirect effect of the mediators on purchaseintentions (see Figure 2 [Figure omitted. See Article Image.]). In the following the mediating effects of emotionalstates (pleasure, arousal, dominance), behavioural responses (return and explore, enjoyment) and ambivalenceare discussed. Additionally, the context (online versus offline) and gender were included as control variables inthe model (see Table VI [Figure omitted. See Article Image.]). Emotional states mediate the influence of environmental stimuli on purchase intentions Greater complexity significantly leads to greater emotional states (return and explore, enjoyment). In turn,pleasure, return and explore and enjoyment lead to higher purchase intentions. Testing for the mediation effect,the analysis reveals that the total (c) and direct (c') effects of complexity on purchase intention are 0.42, p <0.00and 0.27, p <0.00, respectively. The difference between the total and the direct effects is 0.15 and a 95 per centBCa (Bias corrected and accelerated) bootstrap CI (confidence interval) of 0.10 to 0.21 (≠0). Since the CI of themediators, "return and explore" and "enjoyment" are different from zero they are thus significant mediators (seeTable VII [Figure omitted. See Article Image.]). Greater novelty significantly leads to greater emotional states (pleasure, return and explore, enjoyment). In turn,return and explore and enjoyment lead to higher, ambivalence to lower, purchase intentions. The total anddirect effects of novelty on purchase intentions are 0.25, p <0.00 and 0.12, p <0.01, respectively. The differencebetween the total and the direct effects is 0.12 and BCa CI

95% is 0.06 to 0.19 (≠0). Again, CI of the mediators

"return and explore" and "enjoyment" are different from zero and thus represent significant mediators.

Page 10: The Role of Mixed Emotion

Greater density significantly leads to greater dominance. In turn, return and explore and enjoyment lead tohigher, ambivalence to lower, purchase intentions. The total and direct effects of density on purchase intentionsare -0.04, p <0.39 and -0.05, p <0.24, respectively, which means they are not significant. The differencebetween the total and the direct effects is 0.01, and BCa CI

95% contains zero (-0.06 to 0.07), thus no mediators

contribute to the indirect effect. To sum up, we found evidence for H5a and H5b in that return and explore and enjoyment are aspects ofemotional states that significantly mediate the influence of complexity and novelty on purchase intentions. Theycan reduce the effect of complexity and therefore are important variables to consider. While complexity andnovelty lead to stronger return and explore and enjoyment, density impacts the level of felt dominance; inaddition, the emotional states have impacts on purchase intentions: return and explore and enjoyment lead tohigher, ambivalence to lower, purchase intentions. In terms of recommendations, those stores that make consumers feel they should come back and explore thestore further and that lead to feelings of enjoyment are predictors for purchase intentions. In particular, complexand novel environments are responsible for these feelings. Dense stores (packed products, shelf size, etc.)make consumers feel the environment to be controlling and influential. In all models, the difference betweenonline and offline situations was not significant. Emotional states mediate the influence of the reference group on purchase intentions Greater normative influence significantly leads to lower return and explore (p <0.00) and higher enjoyment andambivalence (p <0.05). In turn, pleasure, return and explore and enjoyment lead to higher purchase intention.The total and direct effects of normative influence on purchase intentions are -0.07, p <0.18 and -0.06, p <0.18,i.e. no significant effect of normative influence on purchase intention. The difference between the total and thedirect effects is 0.01 and BCa CI

95% is not different from zero (-0.08 to 0.07). CI of enjoyment is 0.01 to 0.10 and

thus mediates IV and DV significantly (support for H6a ). Greater informational influence significantly leads to lower dominance (p <0.03) and higher return and explore (p <0.05). In turn, return and explore and enjoyment, lead to higher, ambivalence to lower, purchase intention.The total and direct effects of informational influence on purchase intentions are 0.19, p <0.00 and 0.12, p<0.01. The difference between the total and the direct effects is 0.06 and BCa CI

95% is not different from zero (-

0.01 to 0.12). CI of the mediator "return and explore" is different from zero (0.01 to 0.06) and thus mediates IVand DV significantly (support for H6b ). Greater interpersonal influence significantly leads to higher enjoyment (p <0.05). In turn, return and explore andenjoyment, lead to higher purchase intention. The total and direct effects of normative influence on purchaseintentions are 0.13, p <0.01 and 0.09, p <0.04. The difference between the total and the direct effects is 0.0430and BCa CI

95% is not different from zero (-0.02 to 0.11). No mediating effects can be found.

Greater influence on decision making significantly leads to higher return and explore (p <0.02) and lowerambivalence (p <0.03). In turn, return and explore and enjoyment lead to higher purchase intention. The totaland direct effects of influence on purchase intentions are 0.28, p <0.00 and 0.25, p <0.00. The differencebetween the total and the direct effects is 0.04 and BCa CI

95% is not different from zero (-0.03 to 0.09). CI of the

mediator "return and explore" is different from zero (0.01 to 0.06) and thus mediates IV and DV significantly(support for H6d ). Summarizing the potential mediating effects of the impact of reference group on purchase intention, enjoymentis responsible for the weaker impact of normative influence on purchase intention, although normative influenceis not significant. However, the tendency is that the more consumers enjoy the store, the less they areinfluenced by others' behaviour. The willingness to return and explore the store as an emotional reaction to the store lessens the informationalinfluence from others, as well as their impact on the decision-making on purchase intentions. Thus, others areless influential if consumers themselves have the feeling that they should return to the store and explore it

Page 11: The Role of Mixed Emotion

further. Emotional states mediate the influence of product aspects on purchase intentions Greater social risk significantly leads to higher enjoyment (p <0.00). In turn, return and explore and enjoymentlead to higher, ambivalence to lower, purchase intentions. The total and direct effects of social risk on purchaseintentions are 0.12, p <0.01 and 0.04, p <0.41, respectively, which means that only the simple effect issignificant. The difference between the total and the direct effects is 0.01 and BCa CI

95% is different from zero

(0.02 to 0.15). CI of "enjoyment" is different from zero (0.02 to 0.11) and thus proves to be a significant mediator(support for H7a ). Greater financial risk significantly leads to higher pleasure and arousal (p <0.05). In turn, return and explore andenjoyment, lead to higher purchase intentions. The total and direct effects of financial risk on purchaseintentions are -0.08, p <0.14 and -0.09, p <0.05, i.e. no significant effect of financial risk on purchase intention.The difference between the total and the direct effects is 0.01 and BCa CI

95% is not different from zero (-0.06 to

0.07). CI of the mediator "dominance" is different from zero (0.01 to 0.04) and thus proves to be a significantmediator. Although financial risk does not significantly influence dominance, there is a difference between totaland direct effect. Greater performance risk significantly leads to lower return and explore (p <0.00). In turn, pleasure, return andexplore, and enjoyment lead to higher, ambivalence to lower, purchase intentions. The total and direct effects ofperformance risk on purchase intentions are not significant -0.08, p <0.12 and -0.06, p <0.15), as is thedifference between the total and the direct effects (-0.02, BCa CI

95% -0.09 to 0.05). No CI of mediators is

different from zero. Some mediational effects were found with regard to products' influence on purchase intentions. Whileenjoyment significantly mediates the influence of social risk, dominance mediates the influence of financial riskon purchase intentions. In other words, in cases where consumers enjoy the situation, the fact that a productsignals one's position to others is less important for the intention to purchase. The influence of financial risk, onthe other hand, is mediated by dominance: if the situation is perceived as controlling, the financial risk is lesslikely to weaken purchase intention. Greater need for product significantly leads to higher pleasure and arousal (p <0.00). In turn, return and explore,and enjoyment lead to higher, ambivalence to lower, purchase intentions. The total and direct effects of need forproduct on purchase intentions are 0.17, p <0.00 and 0.10, p <0.03, respectively. The difference between thetotal and the direct effects is 0.07 and BCa CI

95% is different from zero (0.01 to 0.15). No CI of mediators is

different from zero, thus no mediating effects can be found. Greater excitement for product significantly leads to higher emotional states (p <0.00). In turn, return andexplore, and enjoyment lead to higher, ambivalence to lower, purchase intentions. The total and direct effects ofexcitement for product on purchase intentions are 0.22, p <0.00 and 0.04, p <0.49, respectively. The differencebetween the total and the direct effects is 0.19 and BCa CI

95% is different from zero (0.11 to 0.27). CI of return

&explore, enjoyment and ambivalence are different from zero (0.03 to 0.09; 0.06 to 0.16; .01 to 0.04), and thusprove to be significant mediators (support for H7e ). With regard to product involvement, the excitement for the product is mediated by emotional states: return andexplore, enjoyment and ambivalence, weaken the purchase intention. In other words, if the excitement for theproduct is mixed up with other emotional feelings that refer either to the store (return and explore, enjoyment) orinvolve different feelings at the same time, the effect of the product's influence on purchase intentions isweakened. Discussion This study provided more detailed results first, about the impact of a variety of different antecedents onapproach-avoidance conflicts, and second about the mediating effect of ambivalence (i.e. mixed emotions) onconsumers' experiences of approach-avoidance conflicts, and their intentions to purchase. Whereas some

Page 12: The Role of Mixed Emotion

distinctions could be drawn between online and offline contexts when examining the impact of market-related,product-related ([66] Pan and Zinkhan, 2006) and social factors on consumers' approach-avoidance conflicts (H1 , H2 , H3 and H4 ); no clear distinction could be drawn between online and offline channels in terms ofmediating effects of emotions on shoppers' intention to purchase (H6 , H7 , H8 and H9 ). However mixedemotions (ambivalence) did mediate the impact of certain product-related, market-related and personal factorson consumers' intention to purchase across both channels. For approach-avoidance conflicts, these results indicate much more clearly than earlier research the moderatingrole-played by different antecedents. The influences of market-related, product-related ([66] Pan and Zinkhan,2006) and social factors were elicited on consumers' intention to purchase. Examining the characteristics of theenvironment, the product, and the reference group, meant that this study extended earlier work, which hadconcentrated only on the environment as an antecedent factor of approach-avoidance conflicts. [24] Donovanet al. (1994, p. 284) only showed how the [55] Mehrabian and Russell (1974) model related "features of theenvironment (S) to approach-avoidance behaviors (R) within the environment, mediated by the individual'semotional states (O) aroused by the environment". This study, therefore, builds on [35] Foxall and Greenley's(1999) and [36] Foxall and Yani-de-Soriano's (2005) earlier research which identified the important role ofdifferent contexts for applying the [55] Mehrabian and Russell (1974) model; and this study provides a morenuanced picture of a wider range of antecedents. The emotional and behavioural aspects of consumers' approach-avoidance conflicts differed between onlineand offline channels. The level of arousal is important in relation to the emotions which consumers experienceand shapes subsequent consumers behavior. Overload is one of the key antecedents of consumer ambivalencewhere consumers feel "overwhelmed or ill-prepared during the purchasing process and the sheer volume ofpurchasing decisions to be made" ([64] Otnes et al. , 1997, p. 87). Traditional retail stores need strategies,which enhance customers' experiences in-store so that shoppers experience positive emotions of pleasure,arousal and dominance. Bricks and mortar retailers have to monitor and manage customers' feelings ofpleasure, arousal and dominance very carefully. Retail atmospherics can play a very important role in ensuringthat offline environments offer just the right amount of stimulation to arouse consumers' positive emotions, whileavoiding either stimulating negative emotions or provoking no arousal at all (see Figure 1 [Figure omitted. SeeArticle Image.]), thus confirming earlier research ([35] Foxall and Greenley, 1999; [36] Foxall and Yani-de-Soriano, 2005). Retailers need to reduce the impact of consumers' emotional responses to the retail settingwhere mixed emotions are likely to lead to consumers leaving the store without making a purchase. [23]Donovan and Rossiter (1982, p. 50) demonstrated that arousal was a "key mediator of intentions to spend timein the store" as well as a "positive relationship between arousal and affiliation. The implication of the arousal-affiliation relationship is that more aroused shoppers will be more likely to interact with other people in the store"and that in-store stimuli such as bright lights and rock music may increase arousal. Thus retail managers needto ensure their physical environment facilitates affiliation (either with staff or fellow shoppers or shoppingpartners (e.g. mothers, wives, sisters)). In contrast for online sites, consumers need to be enticed to return,explore and complete the purchase. Online channels are highly regarded during the search stage of theconsumption process. The challenge is to ensure that potential customers return and make a purchase on asubsequent occasion online, rather than defecting after the successful completion of the search stage to theoffline channel for the purchase completion stage. Online retailers need to provide positive experiences, whichreinforce shoppers' desire to return and explore the web site. This would clearly provide a potentially importantway of countering the competitive advantage of bricks and mortar retailers' strategies for stimulating shoppers'intention to patronize traditional retail stores. The impact of the complexity and novelty of the environmental situation on shopping behavior confirmed andextended earlier work ([24] Donovan et al. , 1994; [35] Foxall and Greenley, 1999; [36] Foxall and Yani-de-Soriano, 2005) by showing that within approach-avoidance conflicts, online stores are perceived as more

Page 13: The Role of Mixed Emotion

complex than offline stores (H2 ). This increases the challenge for web site designers and online retailers toensure that all aspects of the shopping experience online is not perceived as too complicated, difficult, andoverwhelming to cope with (e.g. clear signage and navigation buttons). Careful design of web sites, particularlyuser interfaces, will ensure that consumers find the online shopping experience to be relaxing rather thanthreatening. In online shopping channels, the sheer volume of decisions required while navigating the web siteillustrates the impact of complexity on online shopping experiences. This suggests online retailers need tomonitor carefully the complexity of their web sites so as not to deter shoppers. We extended earlier work on perceived risk (e.g. [13] Chaudhuri, 1997) by showing that the perceived riskattached to product performance has a greater impact on approach-avoidance conflicts in online channels;whereas levels of product involvement (i.e. need for product; and exciting product) have a greater impact onapproach-avoidance conflicts in offline stores ( H4 ). For online retailers this means that they need to continueto invest in building trust into the customer-supplier relationship by a variety of mechanisms, concentratingparticularly on the main sources of shopper concern, e.g. security in terms of the payment and delivery systemsin order to reduce consumers' approach-avoidance conflicts. Conversely, for offline channels it is product(rather than channel) performance which is at the heart of consumers' approach-avoidance conflicts, and sostrategies are needed which reassure customers about product performance in order to alleviate the effect ofperceived risk on approach-avoidance conflicts in offline channels. Our findings support the emphasis placed by earlier researchers on the importance of understanding theemotional drivers of behaviors in approach-avoidance conflicts ([23] Donovan and Rossiter, 1982; [35] Foxalland Greenley, 1999; [36] Foxall and Yani-de-Soriano, 2005). We also extend earlier work on consumerambivalence and the effect of mixed emotions on consumers' experiences which often resulted "directly frominteractions in, or structural features of, the marketplace" ([64] Otnes et al. , 1997, p. 80). We saw this with theimpact of the complex online environment on shopping intentions. Supplementing recognized models of theconsumption process with ambivalence promises to be a fruitful way of extending our understanding ofconsumer behaviour in both online and offline settings. Ambivalence derives from consumers' feelings of mixedemotions, which can be linked to approach and avoidance motivational behaviours, identified in earlier studies(e.g. [23] Donovan and Rossiter, 1982); and can be experienced either simultaneously or sequentially. Earlierresearch has established that ambivalence (i.e. mixed emotions) facilitate or frustrate consumers' motivation toshop to varying degrees. These findings allow us to start to interpret further the role that mixed emotions play inconsumer decision-making in retail environments, particularly when trying to compare approach-avoidancemotivational conflicts across offline and online channels. As mentioned previously, no clear distinctions could be drawn between online and offline channels in terms ofthe mediating effects of consumers' emotions on intention to purchase. This meant, for instance, that the impactof the store's physical attributes (complexity and novelty) on intention to shop is mediated by consumers'emotional states both offline and online, but the impact of the density of the store (e.g. if consumers find theenvironment is crowded or dominating) does not seem to be mediated by emotions ( H5 ). The impact of thenormative and informational influence of reference groups on consumers' intention to purchase was mediatedby consumers' emotions (H6 ), both offline and online, but the impact of interpersonal influences on intentions toshop were not mediated by consumers' emotional states. Consumers' emotions also mediated the impact ofcertain product characteristics (e.g. product's perceived social risk; and whether or not it is an exciting product)on their intentions to shop either offline or online, but other product related characteristics (e.g. financial orperformance risk, or need for the product) did not seem to be mediated by consumers' emotions ( H7 ). An important challenge for retailers in both channels is to manage the potential flip-flopping between online andoffline channels by consumers who clearly exploit the various strengths of each channel. Consumers have theability to search on line but then make the purchase offline in order to manage perceived performance riskderived from negative feelings about the reliability and security of online channels, especially payment and

Page 14: The Role of Mixed Emotion

delivery systems. This contrasts with consumers' ability in brick and mortar stores to evaluate products, andthen move to take advantage of the pricing strategies of online channels. Brick and mortar could either try andcompete on price, which could be difficult in view of all the additional overheads carried by offline stores; or theycould try and exploit the service advantage offered by many offline retailers, e.g. interpersonal relationships withstore staff to tie customers into relationships, and thus ensure that shoppers remain loyal customers throughoutthe consumption process and complete the final purchase with the bricks and mortar retailer. For onlinechannels, exploiting competitive advantage involves offering some form(s) of reassurance to customers so thatthey can manage the security risks associated with channel performance (e.g. payment and delivery systems)which consumers perceive as associated with purchasing online. Both bricks and mortar and online retailers areequally effective during the early stages of the consumption process (i.e. searching for information, evaluatingalternatives). The challenge is to build up consumer loyalty in these preliminary and preparatory stages so thatconsumers remain for the all important end game, i.e. making the purchase in that channel, rather thanswitching at the point of purchase from one channel to the other - as both the opportunity and real costs forretailers are, otherwise, to provide the services to support the early stages of consumer decision-making (i.e.search, choice) without reaping the benefits of winning the final purchase. Conclusion This exploratory and contextual international study confirmed that examining approach-avoidance conflictspromises to be a fruitful avenue for understanding both global and incremental aspects of ambivalence. This isimportant because of the effect of mixed emotions (ambivalence) on consumer behaviour in both offline andonline shopping channels. We briefly review the theoretical and empirical conclusions from our study; thelimitations; and potential directions for future research. Theoretically, we have followed [2] Armitage and Conner (2000) in pursuing a global view of ambivalence.However, [12] Chaiken et al. (1995) propose "three forms of evaluative inconsistency: cognitive-affective;affective-evaluative, and evaluative-cognitive" ([2] Armitage and Conner, 2000, p. 1430). Adopting a moreincremental view of ambivalence would allow more refined measures to be used to capture different types ofevaluative inconsistency ([12] Chaiken et al. , 1995) in consumer decision-making, in both offline and onlinesettings. By eliciting three separate facets, it might be possible to obtain a sharper focus on the "differentialeffects of levels of ambivalence" ([2] Armitage and Conner, 2000, p. 1430) on consumer behaviour in retailingchannels. Earlier research on ambivalence has also indicated that attitudes are based on separate positive and negativecomponents and has problematized the conceptualization of attitudes and emotions along a bipolar continuum([74] Priester and Petty, 1996). [70] Petty et al. (1997, p. 613)) have argued that the assumption that "positiveand negative evaluative reactions are reciprocally activated" is not necessarily tenable but rather "positive andnegative responses should be viewed as a bivariate evaluative plane" ([11] Cacioppo and Berntson, 1994).Similarly, [100] Babin et al. (1998, cited in [53] Maxwell and Kover, 2003, p. 554) argue "that positive andnegative affects are often but not always unipolar rather than bipolar dimensions ... one cannot, consequentlyconsider negative affect as simply the opposite of positive affect". [11] Cacioppo and Berntson (1994) also posit"the inability of traditional bipolar attitude scales to fully differentiate among these possibilities [i.e. positive andnegative responses] and would suggest the value for future research to use separate measures of the positiveand negative bases of attitudes" ([70] Petty et al. , 1997, p. 613). In designing our questionnaire we adopted theconventional assumption about bipolar scales for measuring attitudes. However, for future research we wouldsuggest drawing on all these viewpoints and endeavouring to separate positive and negative aspects ofattitudes into unipolar measurements for further studies of ambivalence and mixed emotions. For other future research, it is important to assess the feeling states that are brought to the store ([20] Dawsonet al. , 1990) as well as the feeling states induced by the store, as explored by earlier studies which examinedpurchase behaviors ([23] Donovan and Rossiter, 1982). This is because ambivalence (mixed emotions) is just

Page 15: The Role of Mixed Emotion

as susceptible to the influence of recall and pre-existing experiences as it is to new experiences in the retailchannel. [91] Updegraff et al. (2004) also argued that past negative emotional experiences may be "chronicallysalient sources" for subsequent experiences, indicating that future studies of approach-avoidance conflictsshould elicit the all-important feelings and memories that consumers bring to their experiences of shopping inboth offline and online channels as these pre-existing feelings contribute to the priming of consumers for theirnext set of shopping experiences. Ambivalence needs to be understood therefore, not just in both global andincremental terms, but also within the longitudinal setting of consumers' histories and memories of their earliershopping experiences across both channels. For this empirical study we used convenience sampling, which limits the generalizability of the results; and alsothe capacity to perform cross-country analyses to bring out any potential variations among European countries.Future research could usefully seek more representative country samples to allow cross-country comparisons,and also generalization from the findings. In terms of cross-cultural validation, future studies should test whetherthe proposed model is culturally invariant, given similar behaviour settings are selected. The main focus in thecurrent study was to use an international sample to test the conceptual model (see [75] Reynolds et al. , 2003),and not to compare the respective countries regarding their purchase intentions, which imply certain limitations. Within the context of the cultures used for this study (Austria, Greece and the UK) a larger scale study might beable to draw out differences in shopping propensities for online and offline retailing environments betweenAustrian, Greek and UK consumers; and link these differences to the role of mixed emotions (i.e. ambivalence)in approach-avoidance conflicts in retailing. Drawing on earlier cross-cultural research (e.g. [40] Hofstede, 2001;[80] Schwartz and Zanna, 1992) it would be interesting to examine how far the five dimensions (power distanceindex (PDI); individualism (IDV); masculinity (MAS); uncertainty avoidance (UAI); and long term orientation(LTO)) as well as values vary across national contexts between the specific retailing contexts of online or offlineshopping. In terms of masculinity for instance, do cultures which score low on masculinity (and thus high onfemininity) demonstrate similar sets of behaviours when shopping offline and online, or do the sociable aspectsof shopping mean that a distinction can be drawn so that shopping offline is identified as preferred to shoppingonline, whereas cultures high on masculinity scores prefer online to offline shopping environments? In terms ofuncertainty avoidance, do consumers' perceptions of risk related to purchasing lead to greater aversion toshopping on line and a greater preference for offline shopping environments in cultural contexts which scorehigh on uncertainty avoidance (e.g. Greece). Expanding beyond the immediate cultural contexts for thisempirical study (Austria, Greece and the U.K.) would allow selection of participant countries on the basis ofnational scores on these dimensions for further study. Different countries might potentially provide morenuanced insights into how cultural contexts influence shopping propensities for online and offline retailingenvironments; and highlight differences in the role of mixed emotions (i.e. ambivalence) in approach-avoidanceconflicts in culturally different retailing contexts. Another important direction for future study would be to incorporate more detailed and explicit psychologicalelements into the model building. As this was an exploratory study, we concentrated on developing and testingthree selected influences on purchase intention: situation, product and reference group in this initial stage oftheory building. This allowed us to focus on three specific topic areas and measure the causal relations amongand mediating effects of approach-avoidance on these constructs. However individual determinants play animportant and complex role within retailing contexts particularly in predicting purchase intentions, and sopsychological factors will impact the operation of mixed emotions and ambivalence. Thus includingpsychological factors in the next stage of model development and theory testing could potentially provide amore nuanced understanding of the impact of consumers' mixed emotions on approach-avoidance conflicts. The authors would like to thank Christina Wastlbauer and Christina-Isidora Kyritsi who undertook the datacollection for this study.The authors would also like to thank the Editors and the three reviewers for their helpfuland insightful comments on earlier drafts of this paper.

Page 16: The Role of Mixed Emotion

Footnote 1. In our model we explore how independent variables (situation, product, reference group) have an impact onpurchase intentions via emotional states, i.e. emotional states intervene (mediation effect). We are notassuming that emotional states influence the relation of the independent variables (situation, product, referencegroup) and the intention to purchase, which would mean they cause the intention ([6] Baron and Kenny, 1986). References 1. Allan, D. (2008), "Sound retailing: a review of experimental evidence on the effects of music on shoppingbehavior", in Lowrey, T.M. (Ed.), Brick &Mortar Shopping in the Twenty-first Century, Lawrence Erlbaum, NewYork, NY, pp. 33-52. 2. Armitage, C.J. and Conner, M. (2000), "Attitudinal ambivalence: a test of three key hypotheses", Personalityand Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 26 No. 11, pp. 1421-32. 3. Bagozzi, R.P., Gopinath, M. and Nyer, P.U. (1999), "The role of emotions in marketing", Journal of theAcademy of Marketing Science, Vol. 27 No. 2, pp. 184-206. 4. Baker, J. (1986), "The role of the environment in marketing services: the consumer perspective", in Czepeil,J.A., Congram, C.A. and Shanahan, J. (Eds), The Services Challenge: Integrating for Competitive Advantage,American Marketing Association, Chicago, IL, pp. 79-84. 5. Baker, J., Levy, M. and Grewal, D. (1992), "An experimental approach to making retail store environmentaldecisions", Journal of Retailing, Vol. 68 No. 4, p. 445. 6. Baron, R.M. and Kenny, D.A. (1986), "The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychologicalresearch: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,Vol. 51 No. 6, pp. 1173-82. 7. Bearden, W.O. and Etzel, M.J. (1982), "Reference group influence on product and brand purchasedecisions", Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 183-94. 8. Bearden, W.O., Netemeyer, R.G. and Teel, J.E. (1989), "Measurement of consumer susceptibility tointerpersonal influence", Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 15, March, pp. 473-81. 9. Bearden, W.O., Netemeyer, R.G. and Teel, J.E. (1990), "Further validation of the consumer susceptibility tointerpersonal influence scale", Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 17, pp. 770-6. 10. Burton, D. (2002), "Postmodernism, social relations and remote shopping", European Journal of Marketing,Vol. 36 Nos 7/8, pp. 792-955. 11. Cacioppo, J.T. and Berntson, G.G. (1994), "Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: a criticalreview, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates", Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 115No. 3, pp. 401-23. 12. Chaiken, S., Pomerantz, E.M. and Giner-Sorolla, R. (1995), "Structural consistency and attitude strength", inPetty, R.E. and Krosnick, J.A. (Eds), Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences, Lawrence Erlbaum,Mahwah, NJ. 13. Chaudhuri, A. (1997), "Consumption emotion and perceived risk: a macro-analytic approach", Journal ofBusiness Research, Vol. 39 No. 2, pp. 81-92. 14. Chiang, K.-P. and Dholakia, R.R. (2003), "Factors driving consumer intention to shop online: an empiricalinvestigation", Journal of Consumer Psychology, Vol. 13 Nos 1-2, pp. 177-83. 15. Childers, T.L. and Rao, A.R. (1992), "The influence of familial and peer-based reference groups onconsumer decisions", Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 19 No. 2, pp. 198-211. 16. Conchar, M.P., Zinkhan, C.M., Peters, C. and Olavarrieta, S. (2004), "An integrated framework for theconceptualization of consumers' perceived-risk processing", Academy of Marketing Science Journal, Vol. 32No. 4, pp. 418-36. 17. Cox, D.F. (1967), Risk Taking and Information Handling in Consumer Behavior, Division of ResearchGraduate School of Business Administration, Harvard University, Boston, MA.

Page 17: The Role of Mixed Emotion

18. Cronbach, L.J. (1951), "Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of test", Psychometrika, Vol. 16,September, pp. 297-334. 19. d'Astous, A. (2000), "Irritating aspects of the shopping environment", Journal of Business Research, Vol. 49No. 2, pp. 149-56. 20. Dawson, S., Bloch, P.H. and Ridgway, N.M. (1990), "Shopping motives, emotional states, and retailoutcomes", Journal of Retailing, Vol. 66 No. 4, pp. 408-27. 21. DelVecchio, D. and Smith, D.C. (2005), "Brand-extension price premiums: the effects of perceived fit andextension product category risk", Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 33 No. 2, pp. 184-96. 22. Dennis, C., Harris, L. and Sandhu, B. (2002), "From bricks to clicks: understanding the e-consumer",Qualitative Market Research, Vol. 5 No. 4, pp. 281-90. 23. Donovan, R.J. and Rossiter, J.R. (1982), "Store atmosphere: an environmental psychology approach",Journal of Retailing, Vol. 58 No. 1, pp. 34-57. 24. Donovan, R.J., Rossiter, J.R., Marcoolyn, G. and Nesdale, A. (1994), "Store atmosphere and purchasingbehavior", Journal of Retailing, Vol. 70 No. 3, pp. 283-94. 25. Doolin, B., Dillon, S., Thompson, F. and Corner, J.L. (2005), "Perceived risk, the internet shoppingexperience and online purchasing behavior: a New Zealand perspective", Journal of Global InformationManagement, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 66-88. 26. Dowling, G.R. and Staelin, R. (1994), "A model of perceived risk and intended risk-handling activity", Journalof Consumer Research, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 119-54. 27. Elliot, A.J. and Thrash, T.M. (2002), "Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: approach andavoidance temperaments and goals", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 82 No. 5, pp. 804-18. 28. Eroglu, S.A., Machleit, K.A. and Davis, L.M. (2001), "Atmospheric qualities of online retailing: a conceptualmodel and implications", Journal of Business Research, Vol. 54 No. 2, pp. 177-84. 29. Eroglu, S.A., Machleit, K.A. and Davis, L.M. (2003), "Empirical testing of a model of online storeatmospherics and shopper responses", Psychology &Marketing, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 139-50. 30. Escalas, J.E. and Bettman, J.R. (2003), "You are what they eat: the influence of reference groups onconsumers' connections to brands", Journal of Consumer Psychology, Vol. 13 No. 3, pp. 339-48. 31. Fiore, A.M., Jin, H.-J. and Kim, J. (2005), "For fun and profit: hedonic value from image interactivity andresponses toward an online store", Psychology &Marketing, Vol. 22 No. 8, pp. 669-94. 32. Fiore, A.M., Yah, X. and Yoh, E. (2000), "Effects of a product display and environmental fragrancing onapproach responses and pleasurable experiences", Psychology &Marketing, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 27-54. 33. Forsythe, S.M. and Shi, B. (2003), "Consumer patronage and risk perceptions in internet shopping", Journalof Business Research, Vol. 56 No. 11, pp. 867-75. 34. Foxall, G.R. (1997), "The emotional texture of consumer environments: a systematic approach toatmospherics", Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 18 No. 5, pp. 505-23. 35. Foxall, G.R. and Greenley, G.E. (1999), "Consumers' emotional responses to service environments",Journal of Business Research, Vol. 46 No. 2, pp. 149-58. 36. Foxall, G.R. and Yani-de-Soriano, M.M. (2005), "Situational influences on consumers' attitudes andbehavior", Journal of Business Research, Vol. 58 No. 4, pp. 518-25. 37. Hansen, F. (1976), "Psychological theories of consumer choice", Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 3 No.3, pp. 117-42. 38. Harrison, T., Waite, K. and Hunter, G.L. (2006), "The internet, information and empowerment", EuropeanJournal of Marketing, Vol. 40 Nos 9/10, pp. 972-93. 39. Harrist, S. (2006), "A phenomenological investigation of the experience of ambivalence", Journal ofPhenomenological Psychology, Vol. 37 No. 1, pp. 85-114. 40. Hofstede, G. (2001), Culture's Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions and Organizations

Page 18: The Role of Mixed Emotion

across Nations, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA. 41. Hogg, M.A. and Vaughan, G.M. (2005), Social Psychology, Prentice-Hall, Harlow. 42. Holbrook, M.B. and Hirschman, E.C. (1982), "The experiential aspects of consumption: consumer fantasies,feelings, and fun", Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 132-40. 43. Jacoby, J. and Kaplan, L.B. (1972), "The components of perceived risk", Advances in Consumer Research,Vol. 1 No. 1, pp. 382-93. 44. Kaplan, L.B., Szybillo, G.J. and Jacoby, J. (1974), "Components of perceived risk in product purchase: across-validation", Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 59 No. 3, pp. 287-91. 45. Koernig, S.K. (2003), "E-scapes: the electronic physical environment and service tangibility", Psychology&Marketing, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 151-67. 46. Kwak, H., Fox, R.J. and Zinkhan, G.M. (2002), "What products can be successfully promoted and sold viathe internet", Journal of Advertising Research, January/February, pp. 23-38. 47. Lavine, H. (2004), "Attitude ambivalence in the realm of politics", in Haddock, G. and Maio, G.R. (Eds),Contemporary Perspectives on the Psychology of Attitudes, Psychology Press, Hove, pp. 93-119. 48. Leone, L., Perugini, M. and Bagozzi, R.P. (2005), "Emotions and decision making: regulatory focusmoderates the influence of anticipated emotions on action evaluations", Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 19 No. 8,pp. 1175-98. 49. Malhotra, N.K., Agarwal, J. and Peterson, M. (1996), "Methodological issues in cross-cultural marketingresearch: a state-of-the-art review", International Marketing Review, Vol. 13 No. 5, pp. 7-43. 50. Mangleburg, T.F., Doney, P.M. and Bristol, T. (2004), "Shopping with friends and teens' susceptibility topeer influence", Journal of Retailing, Vol. 80 No. 2, pp. 101-16. 51. Mariani, M.G. and Zappalà, S. (2006), "Risk perception in online shopping", in Zappalà, S. and Gray, C.(Eds), Impact of E-Commerce on Consumers and Small Firms, Ashgate, Aldershot, pp. 207-21. 52. Mattila, A.S. and Wirtz, J. (2008), "The role of store environmental stimulation and social factors on impulsepurchasing", Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 22 No. 7, pp. 562-7. 53. Maxwell, S. and Kover, A. (2003), "Negative affect: the dark side of retailing", Journal of Business Research,Vol. 56, pp. 553-9. 54. McCarthy, R. and Aronson, J.E. (2000), "Activating consumer response: a model for web site designstrategy", The Journal of Computer Information Systems, Vol. 41 No. 2, pp. 2-8. 55. Mehrabian, A. and Russell, J.A. (1974), An Approach to Environmental Psychology, MIT Press, Cambridge,MA. 56. Menon, S. and Kahn, B. (2002), "Cross-category effects of induced arousal and pleasure on the internetshopping experience", Journal of Retailing, Vol. 78 No. 1, pp. 31-40. 57. Miller, N.E. (1944), "Experimental studies of conflict", in Hunt, J. McV. (Ed.), Personality and the BehaviorDisorders, pp. 431-65. 58. Mitchell, V.W. and Hogg, M.K. (1997), "Perceived risk - issues of identification and measurement forconsumer research", working paper, Manchester School of Management, Manchester. 59. Mitchell, V.-W. (1999), "Consumer perceived risk: conceptualisations and models", European Journal ofMarketing, Vol. 33 Nos 1/2, pp. 163-95. 60. Mitchell, V.-W. and Boustani, P. (1994), "A preliminary investigation into pre- and post-purchase riskperception and reduction", European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 1, pp. 56-71. 61. Mizerski, R.W. (1982), "An attribution explanation of the disproportionate influence of unfavorableinformation", Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 9 No. 3, pp. 301-10. 62. Monsuwe, T.P., Dellaert, B.G.C. and Ruyter, K. (2004), "What drives consumers to shop online? A literaturereview", International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 15 No. 1, pp. 102-21. 63. Olsen, S.O., Wilcox, J. and Olsson, U. (2005), "Consequences of ambivalence on satisfaction and loyalty",

Page 19: The Role of Mixed Emotion

Psychology &Marketing, Vol. 22 No. 3, pp. 247-69. 64. Otnes, C., Lowrey, T.M. and Shrum, L.J. (1997), "Toward an understanding of consumer ambivalence",Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 24 No. 1, pp. 80-93. 65. Palmer, J.W. (2002), "Web site usability, design, and performance metrics", Information Systems Research,Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 151-67. 66. Pan, Y. and Zinkhan, G.M. (2006), "Determinants of retail patronage: a meta-analytical perspective", Journalof Retailing, Vol. 82 No. 3, pp. 229-43. 67. Paridon, T.J. (2004), "Retail opinion sharing: conceptualization and measurement", Journal of Retailing andConsumer Services, Vol. 11, pp. 87-93. 68. Penz, E. and Kirchler, E. (2006), "Affective states, purchase intentions and perceived risk in onlineshopping", in Zappalà, S. and Gray, C. (Eds), Impact of E-Commerce on Consumers and Small Firms, Ashgate,Aldershot, pp. 191-205. 69. Peter, C. and Beale, R. (2008), Affect and Emotion in Human-Computer Interaction: From Theory toApplications, Springer, Berlin. 70. Petty, R.E., Wegener, D.T. and Fabrigar, L.E. (1997), "Attitudes and attitude formation", Annual Review ofPsychology, Vol. 48, pp. 609-47. 71. Preacher, K.J. and Hayes, A.F. (2004), "SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simplemediation models", Behavior Research Methods, Vol. 36 No. 4, pp. 717-31. 72. Preacher, K.J. and Hayes, A.F. (2008), "Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparingindirect effects in multiple mediator models", Behavior Research Methods, Vol. 40 No. 3, pp. 879-91. 73. Preacher, K.J., Rucker, D.D. and Hayes, A.F. (2007), "Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: theory,methods, and prescriptions", Multivariate Behavioral Research, Vol. 42 No. 1, pp. 185-227. 74. Priester, J.R. and Petty, R.E. (1996), "The gradual threshold model of ambivalence: relating the positive andnegative bases of attitudes to subjective ambivalence", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 71No. 3, p. 431. 75. Reynolds, N.L., Simintiras, A.C. and Diamantopoulos, A. (2003), "Theoretical justification of samplingchoices in international marketing research: key issues and guidelines for researchers", Journal of InternationalBusiness Studies, Vol. 34 No. 1, pp. 80-9. 76. Richard, M.-O. and Chandra, R. (2005), "A model of consumer web navigational behavior: conceptualdevelopment and application", Journal of Business Research, Vol. 58 No. 8, pp. 1019-29. 77. Ruth, J.A., Brunel, F.F. and Otnes, C.E. (2002), "Linking thoughts to feelings: investigating cognitiveappraisals and consumption emotions in a mixed-emotions context", Journal of the Academy of MarketingScience, Vol. 30 No. 1, pp. 44-58. 78. Schachter, S. and Singer, J. (1962), "Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state",Psychological Review, Vol. 69, pp. 379-99. 79. Schiffman, L.G., Sherman, E. and Long, M.M. (2003), "Toward a better understanding of the interplay ofpersonal values and the internet", Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 169-86. 80. Schwartz, S.H. and Zanna, M.P. (1992), "Universals in the content and structure of values: theoreticaladvances and empirical tests in 20 countries", Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 25, pp. 1-65. 81. Shim, S., Eastlick, M.A., Lotz, S.L. and Warrington, P. (2001), "An online prepurchase intentions model: therole of intention to search", Journal of Retailing, Vol. 77, pp. 397-416. 82. Sinkovics, R.R. and Penz, E. (2005), "Empowerment of SME web sites: development of a web-empowerment scale and preliminary evidence", Journal of International Entrepreneurship, Vol. 3 No. 4, pp. 303-15. 84. Spies, K., Hesse, F. and Loesch, K. (1997), "Store atmosphere, mood and purchasing behavior",International Journal of Research in Marketing, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 1-17.

Page 20: The Role of Mixed Emotion

85. Stone, R.N. and Gronhaug, K. (1993), "Perceived risk: further considerations for the marketing discipline",European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 27 No. 3, pp. 39-50. 86. Tai, S.H.C. and Fung, A.M.C. (1997), "Application of environmental psychology model to in-store buyingbehaviour", The International Review of Retail, Distribution and Consumer Research, Vol. 7 No. 4, pp. 311-37. 87. Tan, S.J. (1999), "Strategies for reducing consumers' risk aversion in internet shopping", The Journal ofConsumer Marketing, Vol. 16 No. 2, pp. 163-80. 88. Teo, T.S.H. and Yeong, Y.D. (2003), "Assessing the customer decision process in the digital marketplace",Omega, Vol. 31 No. 5, pp. 349-63. 89. Thompson, M.M. and Zanna, M.P. (1995), "The conflict individual: personality-based and domain specificantecedents of ambivalent social attitudes", Journal of Personality, Vol. 63, pp. 259-88. 90. Tractinsky, N. and Rao, V.S. (2001), "Incorporating social dimensions in web-store design", HumanSystems Management, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 105-21. 91. Updegraff, J.A., Gable, S.L. and Taylor, S.E. (2004), "What makes experiences satisfying? The interactionof approach-avoidance motivations and emotions in wellbeing", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,Vol. 86 No. 3, pp. 496-504. 92. Van Kenhove, P. and Desrumaux, P. (1997), "The relationship between emotional states and approach oravoidance responses in a retail environment", The International Review of Retail, Distribution and ConsumerResearch, Vol. 7 No. 4, pp. 351-68. 93. Wang, Y.-S. and Tang, T.-I. (2003), "Assessing customer perceptions of web site service quality in digitalmarketing environments", Journal of End User Computing, Vol. 15 No. 3, pp. 14-31. 94. Watson, L. and Spence, M.T. (2007), "Causes and consequences of emotions on consumer behaviour; areview and integrative cognitive appraisal theory", European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 41 Nos 5/6, pp. 487-511. 95. Wikström, S. (2005), "From e-channel to channel mix and channel integration", Journal of MarketingManagement, Vol. 21 Nos 7/8, pp. 725-53. 96. Wu, C.-S., Cheng, F.-F. and Yen, D.C. (2008), "The atmospheric factors of online storefront environmentdesign: an empirical experiment in Taiwan", Information &Management, Vol. 45 No. 7, pp. 493-8. 97. Zaichkowsky, J.L. (1985), "Measuring the involvement construct", Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 12No. 3, pp. 341-52. 98. Zaichkowsky, J.L. (1994), "The personal involvement inventory: reduction, revision, and application toadvertising", Journal of Advertising, Vol. 23 No. 4, pp. 59-70. 100. Babin, B.J., Darden, W.R. and Babin, L.A. (1998), "Negative emotions in marketing research: affect orartifact?", Journal of Business Research, Vol. 42 No. 3, pp. 271-85. Further Reading 1. Sparks, P. (2000), "Subjective expected utility-based attitude-behavior models: the utility of self-identity", inTerry, D.J. and Hogg, M.A. (Eds), Attitudes, Behavior, and Social Context. The Role of Norms and GroupMembership, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 31-46. Appendix Corresponding author Elfriede Penz can be contacted at: [email protected] AuthorAffiliation Elfriede Penz, Wirtschaftsuniversität (WU), Institute for International Marketing and Management, Vienna,Austria Margaret K. Hogg, Department of Marketing, Management School, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK Illustration Equation 1

Page 21: The Role of Mixed Emotion

Figure 1: Possible linkage between ambivalence and approach-avoidance behaviour in conflicts Figure 2: Conceptual model of approach-avoidance behaviour in conflicts and related hypotheses Table I: Overview of concepts and developed hypotheses Table II: Percentage of Austrian, Greek and UK individuals (compared to EU 27) purchasing via the internet inthe last 12 months (2008) Table III: Factor and reliability analyses for constructs Table IV: Differences between the two different retail formats (online shopping situations vs. traditional brick andmortar "offline" contexts) in terms of the effects of various influences (t -tests, independent samples) Table V: Correlation matrix for approach-avoidance variables (online context) Table VI: Correlation matrix for approach-avoidance variables (offline context) Table VII: Results for multiple mediated regression analyses Subject: Studies; Consumer behavior; Electronic commerce; Retailing; Marketing; Classification: 9130: Experimental/theoretical; 7100: Market research; 5250: Telecommunications systems &Internet communications; 8390: Retailing industry; 7000: Marketing Publication title: European Journal of Marketing Volume: 45 Issue: 1/2 Pages: 104-132 Publication year: 2011 Publication date: 2011 Year: 2011 Publisher: Emerald Group Publishing, Limited Place of publication: Bradford Country of publication: United Kingdom Publication subject: Business And Economics--Marketing And Purchasing ISSN: 03090566 Source type: Scholarly Journals Language of publication: English Document type: Feature Document feature: Diagrams Tables References DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/03090561111095612 ProQuest document ID: 848013930 Document URL: http://search.proquest.com/docview/848013930?accountid=61315 Copyright: Copyright Emerald Group Publishing Limited 2011 Last updated: 2011-06-23 Database: ProQuest Psychology Journals

Page 22: The Role of Mixed Emotion

BibliographyCitation style: Harvard - British Standard

PENZ, E. and MARGARET, K.H., 2011. The role of mixed emotions in consumer behaviour. European Journalof Marketing, 45(1), pp. 104-132.

_______________________________________________________________ Contact ProQuest Copyright 2014 ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. - Terms and Conditions