39
1 Political Framing Over Time: The Effect of Individuals’ Need-To-Evaluate By Jonathan Webber Advisor: Jamie Druckman

Political Framing Over Time: The Effect of Individuals ... · score. The need-to-evaluate indicator and affiliated memory types will be explained in ... (Druckman 2001). The rally

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 1

    Political Framing Over Time: The Effect of Individuals’ Need-To-Evaluate

    By Jonathan Webber Advisor: Jamie Druckman

  • 2

    I. Abstract

    One of the main purposes of political science is to study the interaction of

    political elites and the public. Understanding how elites affect individual decision

    making through a combination of providing “cues” in the form of endorsements to

    individuals and “framing” issues presented to the public is vital in understanding our

    political system. Within the field of political framing, there has been much research on

    the immediate effects of frames and cues separately, but little done on their interaction,

    and even less done on how this interaction gains or loses influence over time.

    Individuals are constantly being exposed to varying frames and cues, so looking

    at the effect of time on how these stimuli affect an individual’s political opinion-making

    is an important aspect of the political framing field that has so far been under-examined.

    Using the results from an experiment I completed with a team at Northwestern

    University during Winter 2008, I examine the effect of time on the influence of frames

    and cues. Our experiment includes data at multiple points in time, allowing a time

    analysis to be completed. I look specifically at which individual opinion-making

    attributes influence whether or not frames and cues will have long-standing effects on

    opinion-formation, and I find that opinion-making attributes can significantly influence

    the impact of frames and cues interacting over time.

  • 3

    II. Introduction

    Democracy involves the public formulating opinions and translating them to

    government officials who attempt to respond to public opinions. Therefore,

    understanding the nature of public opinion is critical. Examining how the media and

    politicians affect public opinion, and then how public opinion in turn affects political

    decisions, is essential in understanding how our representative democracy operates.

    Scholars have spent much time studying many different aspects of these issues.

    Within the field working to examine what influences individual decision making and

    opinion formation, much of the focus has been on one of two things: A) how political

    elites provide ‘cues’ to shape opinion formation, or B) how political elites ‘frame’ issues,

    campaigns, news items, etc. Much of the focus for both areas has been in a snap-shot

    view fashion, meaning examining the effect at an individual point in time instead of over

    a period of time. What I will examine in this paper is how cues and frames interact, and

    how their interaction, along with individual decision-making characteristics, affects

    opinion formation over time. There has not been sufficient research done in either the

    interaction of cues and frames, nor in their effect over a period of time.

    Examining the effect of time is an important and essential aspect of developing a

    comprehensive theory of the effect of media framing and campaign cues on political

    opinion formation in voters. It is not possible to analyze the true effect of a frame or a

    cue in a snapshot of time – for the realities of campaigns are not like that at all. In real-

    life campaigns, all the candidates as well as their staff, their surrogates, local and

    sometimes national media, political action committees and other organizations, and

    voters themselves communicate in a back-and-forth over many weeks or months. It is

  • 4

    certainly important to understand the immediate effect of a stimulus frame or cue at a

    particular time, but also important to understand the effect of its placement both on the

    timeline of a campaign and also in relation to other competing frames on that timeline.

    The true effect of an individual frame or cue in a campaign is very difficult to

    identify, which is why most researchers and studies trying to understand their effect

    default to looking at opinion changes in respondents immediately after exposure. This

    approach, however, misses many of the realities and important determining factors of

    campaigns. For one, the effectiveness of a frame may not be constant across time. Some

    arguments, types of arguments, or loudness of arguments may be much more successful

    at the start of a campaign as compared with during a get-out-the-vote attempt. Some may

    require repetition over time, or only be effective when packaged with a variety of other

    frames or cues presented at other times – as part of a pattern of argumentation or

    information. Some may have an effect right away but then diminish in importance; while

    for others the effect may not emerge until some unit of time has passed. All in all, a

    snapshot cannot capture comprehensively the impact of a media or political elite frame or

    cue.

    Because of the difficulty in studying the complex, detailed realities of opinion

    formation based on mass information consumption over a large amount of time, it is

    suggested in the literature that a good place to start is thinking about the ability of

    individuals to learn and retain information over a period of time (Druckman and Chong,

    n.d.). Looking at how individuals learn and remember information during a campaign

    will be helpful in determining the long-term effect of an instance of exposure to a

    political frame or cue. My work focuses on this question, and specifically whether

  • 5

    individual personality differences in forming evaluative opinions of candidates affect the

    long-term retention of information. Through use of a ‘need-to-evaluate’ indicator of

    individual opinion formation, I evaluate the results of t a lab experiment to learn how

    individuals making on-line evaluations learn and retain information about a campaign, as

    compared to those who use memory-based processing and have a lower need-to-evaluate

    score. The need-to-evaluate indicator and affiliated memory types will be explained in

    depth later. Our lab experiment and my subsequent research are designed to help begin

    to answer the following questions:

    1. How do individuals process cues and frames over time?

    2. What individual differences moderate this processing?

  • 6

    III. Literature Review Frames and Cues A political attitude is an evaluation of an object, such as a candidate or political

    issue. In political science the two main influencers of political attitudes and attitude

    formation are frames and cues, and I will start by discussing each in turn. Frames refer to

    emphasis of a particular argument about an issue; cues refer to a political elite’s

    endorsement of one side on an issue.

    Political scientists have long acknowledged that individuals often do not know

    much about politics (e.g. Delli-Carpini and Keeter, 1996). To compensate for this,

    individuals often rely on simple cues from political elites such as endorsements (e.g.

    Downs 1957, Popkin 1991, Lupia and McCubbins 1998, Lau and Redlawsk 2006).

    When deciding a political opinion, individuals may have a thorough understanding of an

    issue or set of candidates and thus look to political elites for help in the form of listening

    for endorsement cues. It is has been clearly shown that individual voters often rely on

    these cues to fill information gaps when creating or changing political opinions, although

    influence of endorsement cues certainly changes due to attributes like loudness,

    repetition, and trustworthiness of the source.

    Frames have also been shown to affect political opinions. Frames differ from

    cues because they tell people what dimensions are salient, or in other words how to think

    about a particular issue. Dennis Chong (1993) describes framing as the “essence of

    public opinion formation.” Framing is the primary way of elites influencing an

    individual’s political opinion formation for a particular issue. According to Druckman

    and Nelson (2003), a “framing effect occurs when in the course of describing an issue or

  • 7

    event, a speaker’s emphasis on a subset of potentially relevant considerations causes

    individuals to focus on these considerations when constructing their opinions.” The slant

    in which they present an issue or event is the framing event, which they hope will be

    salient for those consuming the information.

    The most commonly used example in the political framing literature is about how

    a hate-rally can be viewed. The light in which a speaker, newspaper, group or individual

    presents the rally can have significant effects on how others create their opinion on

    whether it should be allowed or not (Druckman 2001). The rally could be framed as a

    free speech issue, leading citizens to focus on speech considerations, thus being more

    likely to support allowance of the rally to take place. Or, the rally could be framed as a

    public disturbance/safety issue, leading citizens to be more likely to oppose such a rally

    based on possible safety implications. Multiple studies have found that contrasting these

    frames in an asymmetric study show statistically significant effects of the different

    approaches to framing the hate-rally issue (e.g., Nelson, Clawson, and Oxley 1997).

    There are numerous other examples of the importance of framing – practically

    every political or policy issue provides a solid example. Advocates of abortion rights are

    ‘pro-choice’ whereas opponents consider themselves ‘pro-life.’ Each group frames itself

    in a positive light, and the opposite in a negative light. Opponents of abortion, for

    example, label abortion rights advocates as ‘pro-abortion,’ which carries a strong

    negative connotation. Another example is that advocates of laws allowing employees at

    unionized workplaces to always be allowed to opt-out of union dues call the issue ‘right

    to work,’ while opponents who believe the efforts are intended to diminish the power of

    unions to collectively bargain for higher wages and better work conditions refer to the

  • 8

    laws as ‘right to work for less.’ Social security, the war in Iraq, and affirmative action

    are other volatile political issues in which argument framing is pivotal and has huge

    opinion implications.

    Framing studies have been done in a number of different areas (e.g. Li 1998,

    Levin et al 1998, Camerer 1995, Zaller 1992, Tversky and Kahneman 1981, Cobb and

    Kuklinski 1997). Studies have been done on the interaction between frames and

    individuals’ values, on the effect of frame repetition, on the strength of frames and their

    corresponding change in effect on opinions, and many other areas. Framing affects

    campaigns in many ways, such as attempting to push voters toward focusing on issues or

    frames in a campaign. Some candidates are advantaged when individuals focus on

    specific policy issues, whereas others are advantaged by individuals looking at candidate

    personality and stylistic differences (Druckman and Holmes 2004).

    How frames function when competing is another important area of study. The

    study of frames competing with each other in a variety of ways has been the subject of

    increasing research in the last several years (Sniderman and Theriault 2004, Druckman

    and Chong 2007). In 2004, Sniderman and Theriault wrote “framing studies . . . have

    neglected the fact that frames are themselves contestable. They have instead restricted

    attention to situations in which citizens are artificially sequestered, restricted to hearing

    only one way of thinking about a political issue.” Several recent studies have focused on

    the impact of time on frame effects, which is also an area of focus for this thesis.

    What is clear from past research is that individuals’ opinions and process of

    opinion formulation can certainly be affected by political elites and media

    communications. The common result of many snap-shot, single frame exposure

  • 9

    experimental studies has been that respondents’ opinions are likely to reflect the frame to

    which they have been exposed. These studies are the first step towards researchers’

    development of a comprehensive framing theory, but they ignore many important issues

    such as frame persistence, interaction with cues, placement among other frames in a

    competitive environment, individual personality differences, strength and loudness of

    frames, and others.

    Over-Time Effects

    In the same way that Sniderman and Theriault (2004) argued that studies

    overlooking frame competition do not adequately explain the effect of frames, studies

    that fail to include an aspect of time may not be able to adequately gauge the true

    effectiveness of particular frames and cues. There have not been any published studies in

    contemporary political science looking at the affect of cues over-time, although there is

    some older work in psychology (Hovland and Weiss 1951-52). There have been three

    main studies looking at framing over time (Druckman and Nelson 2003, Tewksbury et al

    2000, de Vreese 2004), and all three suggest a strong effect of time on the influence of

    frames. Several of these studies have suggested relatively short-lived frames, meaning

    over time the influence of the frames have decreased. No study has looked at frames vs.

    cues over time, nor how individual decision-making attributes affect these frame/cue

    interactions over time, which is the area that I focus on in this thesis.

    It is important to put the issue of time in context. A particular media frame or

    elite cue is presented to the public, or a particular respondent, at a time t. That time is

    after t=0 which is the beginning of the campaign (and at which time preconceptions must

    be taken into account), and before the time of the election. At any given time t, there

  • 10

    may be 0, 1, or multiple frames or cues interacting. And the effect of any frame or cue

    presented at time point t is determined partially by what comes before and after t in the

    campaign, in addition to the strength, loudness, and repetition of that frame.

  • 11

    IV. Need-to-Evaluate Moderator The area I will focus most on is on individuals’ “need-to-evaluate” (Bizer et al

    2004, Bizer et al 2005,Tormala and Petty 2001, Frederico and Schneider 2007).

    Specifically, I will look at the effect that need-to-evaluate (NE) has both on short and

    long term effects of framing and cues. According to Bizer et all (2004), need-to-evaluate

    is “an individual difference variable that measures the extent to which people

    spontaneously evaluate objects or experiences as either good or bad.” Put simply, those

    with a high need-to-evaluate will quickly evaluate or create attitudes towards things, and

    are more likely to have opinions on a wide variety of issues. Individuals with a lower NE

    are less likely to evaluate, and will have fewer opinions overall than those with high NE.

    Individuals with high NE develop opinions and evaluations more spontaneously than

    those with low NE. Individuals with a high NE are said to form attitudes on-line, so

    when asked to make a judgment on something from the past, they will think back to what

    their attitude or opinion was then, rather than coming up with a new opinion on the spot

    after thinking back on memories. Individuals with low NE are said to be memory-based

    processors, basing decisions and evaluations on remembering situations.

    In 2004, Bizer et al wrote a paper looking at the need-to-evaluate moderator’s

    effect on an individual political decision-making process. To do so, they analyzed data

    from the 1998 and 2000 National Election Survey. They found that the effects of the

    need-to-evaluate moderator went far beyond solely holding more opinions. It affected

    both how opinions were made as well as correlated with a number of other individual

    characteristics. They found high NE individuals more likely to use party ID or issues

    stances in forming political opinions, and that NE in individuals was connected with the

  • 12

    likelihood that they participate in political activism and with their news media

    consumption habits. Bizer et al suggest that there are cognitive, behavioral, and affective

    effects of the NE moderator that affect political activity and opinion formation for

    individuals. This leads to the general expectation that high NE individuals will maintain

    opinions formed from one time to the next, while low NE will not, and instead will base

    their future opinions on what information they can remember.

  • 13

    V. Description of Experiment

    I use data from an experiment completed at Northwestern University by a team

    including Professor Jamie Druckman and myself. We conducted the experiment in

    Winter 2008, and it was done in two parts. The first part consisted of a 45-minute in-

    person study, and the second part consisted of a 10-minute online follow-up conducted

    approximately two weeks later. I assisted with Part 1 of the experiment, and fully

    conducted Part 2. We had 416 individuals participate in the first portion of the study, and

    359 of them also complete the follow-up several weeks after.

    Part 1

    The beginning of Part 1 of the experiment consisted of gathering background data

    from the respondents. After that, respondents were given some basic information about a

    Republican primary for a US House seat from Massachusetts in a 2007 special election.

    The two candidates in the Republican primary were Tom Tierney and Jim Ogonowski.

    Photos of the candidates were distributed to help respondents from confusing the two.

    Next, respondents were exposed to an article said to be from The Boston Globe.

    There were nine (9) different versions of this article, varying in two dimensions – cues

    and frames. On the cue dimension – the article either stated endorsements for Tierney,

    endorsements for Ogonowski, or had no mention of any endorsement. For the conditions

    which mentioned endorsements of Tierney, the newspaper article stated that, “The

    campaign has recently attracted substantial attention because, somewhat surprisingly,

    Meehan, the departing representative, came out in support of Tierney, even over the

    candidates from his own Democratic party. Several other prominent Democrats, as well

  • 14

    as the state Republican Party, have also endorsed Tierney.” The articles that discussed

    endorsements of Ogonowski read the same except with Ogonowski’s name.

    For the frame dimension, the news articles either included an “issue frame,” an

    “image frame,” or no frame. The issue frame for articles was a paragraph that discussed

    the main issues analysts expected the campaign to focus-on, and quoted a political

    observer saying, “This election is about the issues such as the war and healthcare – the

    voters need to determine who will put their preferences into action.” The image frame

    for articles was a paragraph discussing the importance of personal differences between

    the candidates, and quoted the same political observer saying, “The candidates share

    similar outlooks on the issues, but they substantially differ in terms of their backgrounds

    and capabilities. This election is about the candidates’ personal strengths and

    weaknesses.” The ‘no frame’ articles do not comment on what the election should or

    would focus on. In short, articles with the issue frame encouraged individuals to base

    their opinions on issues like the war and healthcare, while articles with the image frame

    encouraged individuals to base their opinions on personal candidate characteristics like

    experience and capabilities. The articles with no frames did not encourage individuals to

    base their opinion in any way.

    The nine conditions came from a 3 x 3 matrix of all combinations of the three cue

    possibilities and the three frame possibilities.

    Table 1 – Condition matrix for round-one Ogonowski Cue Tierney Cue No Cue

    Issue Frame 1 2 3 Image Frame 4 5 6 No Frame 7 8 9

  • 15

    Following the provided Boston Globe article, respondents watched a 20-minute

    clip of a debate between Tierney and Ogonowski, and then answered another set of

    questions. The questions included asking respondents who they believe won the debate,

    who they would have voted for, how the candidates compared on issues such as honesty

    and knowledge, how the candidates as well as the respondent felt on a variety of political

    issues such as healthcare or the Iraq war, and a variety of questions to determine how

    much the respondent remembered factually from the news articles and video clips.

    The first set of questions at the very beginning of the experiment helped identify

    important personality, demographic, and interest qualities of the respondent. The

    questionnaire asked three questions to measure individuals’ need-to-evaluate (NE). In

    the original political science research, the need-to-evaluate measure consisted of 16

    questions, but due to the prohibitive length, recent experiments have used three questions

    to measure need-to-evaluate using response scales (Bizer et al, 2004). For our measure of

    NE, we used the same three questions as those used by Bizer et al in their 2004 studies

    involving the 1998 and 2000 National Election Survey (NES) Pilot Study. We also

    scored individuals’ NE levels the same way Bizer et all (2004) did. Those three

    questions are below, with coded scores in parenthesis after each answer option:

    Some people have opinions about almost everything; other people have opinions about just some things; and still other people have very few opinions. What about you? Would you say you have opinions about almost everything, many things, some things, or very few things? Answers: Almost everything (1.0), many things (.66), some things (.33), very few things (0). Compared to the average person, do you have far fewer opinions about whether things are good or bad, somewhat fewer opinions, about the same number of opinions, somewhat more opinions, or far more opinions? Answers: Far fewer opinions (0), somewhat fewer opinions (.25), about the same (.5), somewhat more opinions (.75), far more opinions (1.0).

  • 16

    Some people say it is important to have firm opinions about lots of things, while other people think it is better to remain neutral on most issues. What about you? Do you think it is better to have firm opinions about lots of things or to remain neutral on most issues? Answers: Firm opinions (1.0), remain neutral (0).

    An index of respondents’ NE was developed by averaging the three scores, so 1 is

    the maximum of the index and 0 the minimum. This moderator will be explained further

    in the analysis portion later.

    Part 2

    Part 2 of the experiment consisted of contacting respondents two weeks after the

    initial experiment and asking them to take part in a 5-10 minute follow-up. We

    conducted the follow-up online, and it consisted of having the respondents fill out a

    questionnaire via e-mail and send it back to us. After briefly reminding individuals of the

    candidates, we ask them to respond to 26 questions – most of which were exactly the

    same as questions asked during part 1 of the experiment. We started by asking if the

    respondents have accessed any information about the election since Part 1 of the

    experiment, and almost all responded that they had not, thus ensuring that our data was

    not effected by frames/cues or other information not presented in Part 1 of the study. We

    also asked if their media consumption habits had been normal in the past several weeks.

    The questions repeated from Part 1 included questions about who the respondents

    believed to have won the debate, who they would have voted for, where the candidates’

    positions fell on a range of issues, a thermometer rating for the respondent’s feeling

    toward the candidates, and whether the respondent remembered if either of them had

    been endorsed by republicans and democrats.

  • 17

    The questions were designed to provide us insight into how much the respondent

    remembered from Part 1 of the study – and specifically which areas they remembered

    most about. Questions were also asked about the respondent’s own political leanings to

    track their opinions over time. Out of the 416 participants in Part 1, we were able to

    gather data from 359 of them for the follow-up portion of the experiment. Data was then

    matched using individual ID numbers from Part 1 to Part 2, to enable time-series

    regression analysis to be completed.

  • 18

    VI. Hypotheses of Results

    Based on previous literature about need-to-evaluate as well as that on framing and

    cues, there are several anticipated results from our study:

    Hypothesis #1: There is no difference in effectiveness of frames and cues for low vs.

    high NE after the initial exposure.

    I predict that individuals with a low NE as compared with a high NE will not be

    significantly different in how they are affected by the frames and cues in round-one. That

    is, directly after initial exposure, the effect of individuals making a quick evaluation as

    opposed to individuals thinking back on the very recent stimuli will not yield

    significantly different effects from frames or cues.

    Hypothesis #2: Individuals with high NE are more likely to be consistent on candidate

    preference over time.

    I expect that the time effect will be more exaggerated for memory-based

    processors than on-line processors. Individuals with a low need-to-evaluate and who

    process in a memory-based manner will be less likely to be consistent on opinion

    formation than high need-to-evaluate on-line processors. One explanation for this is that

    individuals with low NE will rely on their debate information recall in making their

    round-two decision, whereas individuals with a high NE will directly recall their round-

    one decision.

  • 19

    Hypothesis #3: Cues will more significantly impact round-two decisions for low NE

    individuals than will frames.

    I predict that cues will play a more significant role in round-two decisions (after

    controlling for round-one decisions) because cues are more easily and directly

    remembered after a two-week lag than are frames, which point individuals towards

    candidates less directly.

  • 20

    VII. Results Results Overview

    Before discussion of the results in comparison to the hypotheses, it is important to

    present general results from rounds one and two of our study. All portions of the study

    were completed in Winter 2008. As stated above, we had 416 individuals complete the

    first round of the study. There were nine different study conditions, each completed by

    approximately the same number of individuals (~46). Two weeks after the initial study,

    participants were asked to complete in a ten-minute questionnaire, which we refer to as

    ‘round-two’ of the study. 359 of the 415 initial respondents completed round-two, and

    almost all reported that they did not access additional information about the campaign in

    the approximately two weeks between study rounds.

    Voter choice is the best indicator of preference, and correlates highly with the

    thermometer scores give to candidates as well as the determination of who won the

    debate, so that is the indicator that we focus on for most of this analysis. This is rated on

    a 1-7 scale, with the higher end being those more likely to vote for Tierney and the lower

    end being those more likely to vote for Ognowski. A response of 1 means the respondent

    strongly prefers Ognowski, a 2 means medium preference towards Ognowski, and a 3

    means weak preference for Ognowski. A response of 4 means the respondent is

    completed undecided, and a 5, 6, or 7 shows preference towards Tierney. Because the

    variable we use to show voter preference is not dichotomous, we utilize OLS multiple

    variable regressions and not loglinear regressions for our analysis throughout the paper.

    Results are robust to using OLS rather than an ordered probit model. Below is the

    distribution of responses for round-one and round-two candidate preference.

  • 21

    Table 2 – Distribution of vote-preference in round-one Ranking Score Frequency Percent

    Strong Ognowski 1 61 14.7% 2 64 15.4% 3 59 14.2%

    44.3%

    Tie 4 62 14.9% 5 64 15.4% 6 49 11.8%

    Strong Tierney 7 56 13.5% 40.7%

    Table 3 – Distribution of vote-preference in round-two Ranking Score Frequency Percent

    Strong Ognowski 1 51 14.2% 2 66 18.3% 3 48 13.4%

    46.0%

    Tie 4 45 12.5% 5 51 14.2% 6 58 16.2%

    Strong Tierney 7 40 11.1% 41.5%

    The means and standard deviations of each vote-preference distribution are

    below, and it is also important to note that the correlation between first and second time

    vote choice opinion is .53, which is significant. The means and standard deviations show

    that, overall, the distributions are quite similar.

    Table 4 – Comparison of vote-choice distributions for rounds Study Round Mean Std. Dev.

    Round-one 3.90 1.98 Round-two 3.87 1.98

    Overall Analysis

    We will look at a variety of independent variables to analyze the main dependent

    variable, vote choice. Within the differences in conditions, we will look at the effect of

    individuals receiving image versus issue frames, as well as the effect of those receiving

    varying cue information. The different cues, and the text of each of the two frames, were

    presented earlier. To refresh, in the news articles respondents read, some individuals’

  • 22

    articles included an endorsement for Tierney (Tierney cue), an endorsement for

    Ognowski (Ognowski cue), or no endorsement (no cue). Additionally, articles either

    stated that the election was most about issues like Iraq and healthcare (issue frame), that

    it was most about personality differences like experience (image frame), or gave no

    statement on what the race was most about (no frame).

    Through first looking at the responses to initial questions asked to respondents, it

    appears that Tierney is advantaged on issue evaluations and Ogonowski is advantaged on

    image evaluations. These results are found when looking at all respondents (which

    differing conditions are hoped to all even out), as well as when looking specifically at

    condition 1 – which are respondents receiving no cue and no frame information – the

    main control group.

    Looking specifically at cues we found no significant cue effect on vote choice,

    although as anticipated, endorsement cues for candidates did not hurt them – the cues

    pushed the average opinions in the expected directions, but not significantly. Although

    there was some effect of the cue, cues did not trump all of the other factors in decision-

    making. In other words, individuals did not simply follow the cues over all the other

    information and frames presented. When looking more specifically at whether cues

    influenced respondents’ opinions on issues or images of the candidates, there are several

    interesting observations.

    The critical regression shows round-one vote on exposure to the Tierney cue,

    Ognowski cue, issue frame, or image frame. The regression is shown below, and it

    shows the impact of the different stimuli across conditions. The results reveal that the

    cues do not have significant effect on round-one opinion making, while both the image

  • 23

    and issue frames do. And, as expected, the issue frame is significant in pushing votes

    towards Tierney, and the image frame has a significant negative effect, pushing votes

    towards Ognowski.

    Regression #1 – Round-one opinion for all respondents Regress Round_1_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue .28 .24 1.18 0.24 Ognowski Cue -.28 .23 -1.18 0.24 Issue Frame .42 .23 1.79 0.07* Image Frame -.44 .23 -1.88 0.06* Constant 3.91 .21 18.47 0.00 N = 415 R2 = .045 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

    We now do a similar regression for round-two opinion of all respondents on the

    four different stimuli. Because the vast majority of respondents did not access additional

    information about the Tierney – Ognowski election after round-one, we can begin to see

    the effect of frames and cues two weeks after initial exposure to respondents.

    Interestingly, in this regression the two cues are significant (both at a 95% level), and

    neither the issue or image frame is significant – although both are close to being

    significant at the 90% level. The cues are significant in the expected direction, with the

    Tierney cue in the positive (Tierney) direction, and the Ognowski cue in the negative

    (Ognowski) direction. And, although only marginally significant, the issue and image

    frames also seem to work in the anticipated direction – with the issue frame exposure

    pushing respondents towards Tierney, and the image frame exposure pushing respondents

    down towards Ognowski.

  • 24

    Regression #2 – Round-two opinion for all respondents Regress Round_2_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue .96 .24 4.03 0.00*** Ognowski Cue -.71 .24 -2.92 0.00*** Issue Frame .35 .23 1.51 0.13 Image Frame -.39 .24 -1.65 0.10 Constant 3.77 .22 17.29 0.00 N = 359 R2 = .148 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

    While regression #2 shows an effect of all four stimuli, and the cues in particular,

    on voting in round-two, it immediately raises the question of what role individuals’

    round-one vote played in their round-two decision. In other words, are individuals

    making their decision in round-two based on what they decided in round-one? Are they

    going through the same decision-making thinking as they did in round-one? Or are the

    frames and/or cues having some additional and different effect on respondents in the

    follow up? To test the effect, we run an additional regression on respondents’ round-two

    opinion, with the same independent variables as before, except this time additionally

    regressing on the respondents round-one opinion score.

    Not surprisingly, the round-one vote is highly significant, indicating that how

    respondents voted in round-one strongly affected their round-two decision. Also

    interesting, however, is the effect that inclusion of the round-one variable has on the

    significance of frames and cues. Inclusion of the round-one variable renders the frames

    much more insignificant as compared to regression #2 above. This shows that the

    significance of the issue and image frames on round-two opinion is working almost

    entirely through their initial impact on round-one decision making. However, the results

    show that the cues are still both highly significant – both at the 99% level – indicating

    that the cues seem to have re-emerged during round-two to have an independent and

  • 25

    additional impact. At the end of the next section, we will analyze how the regression

    functions when broken up into the two need-to-evaluate groupings.

    Regression #3 – Round-two opinion for all respondents Regress Rd_2_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Rd.1 Vote Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue .79 .21 3.85 0.00*** Ognowski Cue -.62 .21 -3.00 0.00*** Issue Frame .17 .20 0.88 0.38 Image Frame -.16 .21 -0.76 0.45 Rd. 1 Vote 0.49 .04 11.27 0.00*** Constant 1.87 .25 7.42 0.00 N = 359 R2 = .374 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

    Through analyzing the results of all respondents, there are several interesting

    patterns that emerge. The first is the strong impact of frames during round-one combined

    with a weak showing by cues in round-one, followed by a reversal of roles in round-two

    for frames and cues. It appears that cues do not have a significant affect on voting

    behavior when there is significant other information (the news article and the debate

    video) being shown immediately prior to voting. However, they have a much stronger

    effect after several weeks pass by and the details of the election scenario no doubt

    diminish substantially in the minds of respondents.

    Second, as anticipated, the effect of the frames on round-two decision making

    decreases substantially when including round-one decision as an independent variable in

    regression #3. The frame works much more subtly as a cue, so it is predictable that after

    a two-week lag period how a person ended up deciding might be more of an indicator on

    their next decision than the frame. Both of these observations will be discussed more

    thoroughly after an analysis of the results using the need-to-evaluate moderator.

  • 26

    “Need-to-Evaluate” Groupings

    As discussed previously, the need-to-evaluate indicator will be used frequently in

    our analysis, and was computed in the same way Bizer et al. (2004) did. It is created by

    combining three questions asked in round-one about an individual’s decision making

    habits. Once individual NE scores were calculated and scored on a 0 to 1 weighted scale,

    we completed several tests to show our data is similar in terms of NE results to that of

    Bizer et al.’s. They reported a .23 correlation between NE score and general political

    knowledge (calculated by creating a weighted variable combining the many questions

    asked in round-one about political knowledge and consumption habits), and our similar

    correlation is .21. We also found that the three on-line items combined into one score

    have an alpha value of .65, which is sufficiently high to combine into one construct. For

    analysis purposes, respondents will be split into ‘high NE’ and ‘low NE’ groups based on

    which side of the median their NE score falls on. There were 212 individuals classified

    as low NE, and 199 classified as high NE.

    Before presenting results and conclusions, it is also necessary to look at how the

    two groupings of NE scores compare on vote choice. We find that, overall, low and high

    NE individuals do not significantly differ on vote choice at either round-one or round-

    two. Means and standard deviations for the rounds are shown below. Completed t-tests

    (included in full as Appendix #1) show that we can fail to reject the null hypothesis that

    the means of the two groups are different for both round-one and round-two. This

    observation is essential, because if we rejected the hypothesis that vote choice was the

    same, we would not be able to compare the significance of variables across different

    regression groups based on the NE group.

  • 27

    Round One Round Two

    Group Mean Standard Dev. Mean Standard Dev.

    Low NE 3.85 1.94 3.80 1.95

    High NE 3.94 2.02 3.90 2.01

    Overall 3.90 1.98 3.85 1.97

    NE Analysis

    In the “Overall Analysis” section above, we regressed round-one vote on

    exposure to the four different stimuli (the two cues and two frames), and found that

    overall the cues did not seem to have a significant effect on round-one opinion, but the

    two frames did – the issue frame effect being positive (pushing respondents toward

    Tierney) and the image frame effect being negative (pushing respondents toward

    Ognowski). We now look at the same regression for round-one only, but this time

    separated for the low NE and high NE groups. As can be seen in the results, as with the

    first overall regression, neither group is impacted significantly by the cues in round-one.

    Additionally, we can observe that only the image frame affected the low on-line group

    significantly, and only the issue frame affected the high on-line group significantly.

    Regression #4 – Low NE group for Round-one opinion Regress Round_1_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue .32 .34 0.94 0.35 Ognowski Cue -.32 .31 -1.01 0.31 Issue Frame .13 .32 0.41 0.68 Image Frame -.72 .32 -2.27 0.02** Constant 4.09 .27 13.81 0.00 N = 212 R2 = .054 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

  • 28

    Regression #5 – High NE Group for Round-one opinion Regress Round_1_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue .24 .34 0.71 0.48 Ognowski Cue -.24 .36 -0.65 0.51 Issue Frame .68 .35 1.95 0.05* Image Frame -.20 .35 -0.59 0.55 Constant 3.77 .31 12.23 0.00 N = 199 R2 = .045 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

    My first hypothesis, looking at the difference in frame and cue effects for low vs.

    high NE individuals after the initial exposure, appears to neither be confirmed nor

    rejected. Regression #1 showed a significant effect of both of the frames and neither of

    the cues when looking at all respondents. Regressions #4 and #5, however, show that for

    low NE individuals the image frame was significant, and for high NE individuals the

    issue frame was significant – and the cues were significant for neither. Why this

    difference in frame effect appears is unclear, although it is shown through regressions #4

    and #5 that the effect of cues for low and high NE individuals seem to be consistent for

    round-one (in both cases all the cues are insignificant). One possibility is that high NE

    individuals may be more motivated to focus on issues than are low NE individuals.

    An equivalent regression can be estimated for low NE and high NE on round-two

    opinion, and is shown below in regression #6 and #7. The low NE group shows

    significance for both cues on round-two opinion, but neither frame shows significance on

    round-two decision making for low NE individuals. The high NE group shows

    significance for both frames, but not for either of the cues. These results indicate strong

    evidence that the two groups are forming opinions differently in round-two. It is

  • 29

    particularly interesting to see the emergence of cues as so significant for the low on-line

    NE group. The reasoning, and true significance, of this is discussed further below.

    Regression #6 – Low NE Group for Round-two opinion Regress Round_2_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue 1.56 .30 5.21 0.00*** Ognowski Cue -1.22 .29 -4.23 0.00*** Issue Frame .10 .29 0.34 0.73 Image Frame -.23 .29 -0.80 0.43 Constant 3.81 .27 14.36 0.00 N = 183 R2 = .351 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

    Regression #7 – High NE Group for Round-two opinion Regress Round_2_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue .39 .35 1.11 0.27 Ognowski Cue .06 .38 .15 0.88 Issue Frame .69 .36 1.96 0.05* Image Frame -.71 .36 -1.94 0.05* Constant 3.70 .32 11.30 0.00 N = 172 R2 = .090 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

    As done in the General Analysis section above, next we complete a regression by

    adding in the round-two vote as an independent variable, in an attempt to isolate the true

    affect of the frames and cues on respondents’ round-two decision – taking out the affect

    the stimuli already had on the individuals’ round-one opinion. To recount the result of

    regression #3 above, adding in round-one vote rendered the frames insignificant, with the

    cues still being significant. This pattern is continued in the following two regressions,

    which do the equivalent regression except split by NE group. Regression #8 shows that

    round-one vote is highly significant for the low NE group, as are both of the cues.

    Regression #9, however, shows the only significant variable is round-one vote (although

    the image frame is very marginally significant).

  • 30

    Regression #8 – Low NE Group for Round-two opinion Regress Rd_2_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Rd.1 Vote Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue 1.48 .28 5.27 0.00*** Ognowski Cue -1.14 .27 -4.23 0.00*** Issue Frame .07 .27 0.27 0.78 Image Frame -.07 .27 -0.26 0.80 Rd. 1 Vote 0.29 .06 5.02 0.00*** Constant 2.63 .34 7.65 0.00 N = 183 R2 = .433 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

    Regression #9 – High NE Group for Round-two opinion Regress Rd_2_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Rd.1 Vote Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue .09 .26 0.37 0.71 Ognowski Cue .10 .27 0.36 0.72 Issue Frame .29 .26 1.11 0.27 Image Frame -.37 .27 -1.38 0.17 Rd. 1 Vote 0.68 .05 12.45 0.00*** Constant 1.16 .31 3.70 0.00 N = 172 R2 = .530 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

    Regressions #8 and #9 indicate that low NE individuals during round-two are

    trying to reconstruct their opinions, which is consistent since low NE individuals

    presumably do not have running evaluations from which to draw. They will have some

    memory of their round-one decision, but will supplement that by recalling the

    endorsement cues, which they were presented with during the initial study. The cues

    probably have more of an impact during round-two than they did during round-one

    because they are much simpler to remember as compared with frames. Which candidate

    was endorsed is much more direct than remembering a frame, which suggests that

    individuals decide based on a particular quality, and then the qualities of the candidates

    as related to the frame. Most interesting, however, is that instead of fading in importance

    and impact, the cue stimuli seem to increase their impact over time. Regression #8

  • 31

    supported hypothesis #3, that cues more significantly impact round-two decisions for low

    NE individuals than do frames.

    In comparison, members of the high NE group were able to pull up their running

    opinion already generated (i.e. their round-one vote) and base their decision almost solely

    on this as opposed to thinking back to the frames, cues, and information presented in

    round-one of the study.

    The last regression presented, regression #10, adds in a dummy variable for NE

    along with interaction terms of the NE dummy and other independent variables (each of

    the four stimuli as well as round-one decision). This regression is constructed to check

    the statistical significance of the dynamic discussed above that low on-line individuals

    (low NE) were more significantly impacted by cues in round-two than their high NE

    counterparts. In the regression, high NE individuals are scored as 1 for the dummy

    variable, with low NE respondents scored 0.

    Of note in the results are three significant variables: the interaction variables of

    round-one vote, Tierney cue, and Ognowski cue. The positive significant effect of the

    interaction round-one vote variable indicates that a respondent’s round-one vote was

    much more significant for high NE individuals as compared with low NE individuals.

    The negative and significant Tierney cue interaction, as well as the positive and

    significant Ognowski interaction variable, indicate that the impact of cues on round-two

    decision making is much stronger for low NE respondents. This is shown because the

    interaction cue variables push in the opposite direction to how the variables normally

    work (i.e. the Tierney cue pushes individuals towards Ognowski, and the Ognowski cue

    pushes individuals towards Tierney). This result is essentially making up for the even

  • 32

    stronger effect the normal two cues variables have in the regression – meaning it is

    minimizing the cue-effect for the high NE respondents. Thus, our observation is

    confirmed through statistical significance in this last regression.

    Regression #10 – Both NE Groups for Round-two opinion Regress Rd_2_Vote on Tierney_Cue Ognowski_Cue Issue_Frame Image_Frame Rd.1 Vote NE_Dummy NE_Rd.1_Vote NE_Issue NE_Image NE_Tierney_Cue NE_Ognowski_Cue Variables Coef. Std. Error T P>|t| Tierney Cue 1.47 .28 5.31 0.00*** Ognowski Cue -1.15 .27 -4.36 0.00*** Issue Frame .08 .26 0.31 0.76 Image Frame -.02 .27 -0.08 0.94 Rd. 1 Vote .36 .05 6.78 0.00*** NE Dummy -1.03 .46 -2.31 .02** NE Rd. 1 Vote .27 .07 3.81 .00*** NE Issue .25 .38 0.68 .50 NE Image -.35 .39 -0.89 .37 NE Tierney Cue -1.32 .38 -3.42 .00*** NE Ognowski Cue 1.29 .39 3.28 .00*** Constant 2.36 .32 7.29 .00 N = 355 R2 = .469 * = sig. at 90%, ** = sig, at 95%, *** = sig. at 99%

    Regression #10 provides support for hypothesis #2, namely that individuals with

    high NE are more likely to be consistent on candidate preference over time. The

    hypothesis is supported because the interaction variable of NE score and round-one

    opinion is significant. That is, the fact that the interaction variable is positively significant

    shows evidence that there is an additional effect of the round-one opinion for high NE

    respondents as compared with low NE respondents.

    Finally, we present several graphical depictions of the impact of cue effects and

    frame effects over time for the two different NE groups. The charts and corresponding

    graphs look at the change in average score for exposure to either the Tierney cue or the

    Ognowski cue. In other words, we ignore the control group for these charts, and take the

    % difference from the 1-7 scale between those individuals exposed to the Tierney cue

  • 33

    against those exposed to the Ognowski cue. The first chart shows that for the low NE

    group (the line with the square on the end), cues seem to have an enormous impact over

    time, whereas not nearly as much for high NE individuals.

    CUE EFFECT Time 1 Time 2 All 9.17% 28.00% Low NE 11.00% 46.33% High NE 8.00% 5.33%

    The frame effect chart and graph below do an equivalent analysis, except this time

    comparing percentage difference over time for individuals having an issue frame against

    those with an image frame in round-one (again ignoring control groups). The results

    show much smaller changes, but particularly of note is that for high NE individuals, the

    frames maintain their effect over-time, whereas the effect fades for the low NE line.

  • 34

    Frame EFFECT Time 1 Time 2 All 14.50% 12.50% Low NE 14.00% 5.17% High NE 15.00% 20.83%

  • 35

    IX. Conclusion

    The results of our study help advance the field of political framing and the

    political science understanding of the effect of time on the influence of both cues and

    frames. Prior to our study, it had not been analyzed what difference of effect cues would

    have over time for individuals varying in on-line processing characteristics. Most

    interesting in our results is effect of the cues observed – instead of the impact of the cues

    decreasing over time, their importance appears to increase over time for individuals with

    a low NE. This goes against the idea that frames and cues are short-lived, that is that

    their importance and influence decreases over time.

    A possible explanation for this phenomenon is that when presented with large

    amounts of information individuals are able to synthesize and create opinion mostly on

    their own, but later on when not being able to remember the intricacies of a situation or

    campaign individuals resort to remembering who a political elite endorsed. Thus, while

    endorsements (political cues) are not necessarily the most influential indicator in opinion

    formation immediately after expanded exposure to an issue or campaign, their importance

    grows as one’s familiarity with the issue or campaign decreases over time. Our study

    advanced the field of political framing by completing a study over time which looked at

    both cues and frames, and how they interact, instead of solely cues or solely frames.

    There are several areas in which we would like to extend our study and work. Of

    particular interest to me would be to complete a similar study with more time points. So,

    instead of only having one follow-up questionnaire two weeks after the initial exposure to

    the information, cues and frames, we would have two or more follow-up questionnaires.

    This would make it possible to do time-series regressions to observe how the effects of

  • 36

    the different cues and frames change over particular amounts of times, rather than how

    they change over “time” in a general sense. In other words, it would be interesting to

    observe if the effects of “time” took an hour, day, or week to begin to take effect and be

    observable.

    It would also be interesting to complete a study of frames and cues by presenting

    stimuli to respondents at different points of time. For our study, we solely presented

    stimuli in round-one – round-two was only completion of a questionnaire – but it would

    be interesting to see what effect additional exposure to stimuli would have on the impact

    of frames and cues from previous time periods. In a real-life competitive campaign

    situation, there would be multiple exposures to frames and cues over time, not solely one

    exposure followed by a waiting time prior to the election day or decision making

    moment. Thus, our results look at what the effect of the initial frames and cues would be

    if there were no other contacts made – something unrealistic for a real-life situation. Our

    study does, however, provide a launching point to these two more advanced study ideas.

    Overall, our study more closely replicates the complexities and intricacies of real-

    life competitive situations, while maintaining the simplicity to be able to analyze results

    in a significant manner. Our results indicate that individual candidates and political

    parties should focus on providing argumentation leading to high NE individuals quickly

    making evaluative decisions early in a campaign, as well as on cues and arguments that

    are easily remembered for low NE individuals to remember and process on or near the

    election or decision making time.

  • 37

    Appendix #1 – T-Tests for Low NE vs High NE Vote-Choice

    Test #1 – for Round-one Two-sample t test with equal variances ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Group | Obs Mean Std. Err. Std. Dev. [95% Conf. Interval] ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- Low NE | 212 3.853774 .133604 1.945303 3.590404 4.117143 High NE | 199 3.949749 .143864 2.029452 3.666046 4.233451 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- combined | 411 3.900243 .0978952 1.984642 3.707804 4.092682 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- diff | -.0959752 .1960703 -.4814064 .2894561 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ diff = mean(0) - mean(1) t = -0.4895 Ho: diff = 0 degrees of freedom = 409 Ha: diff < 0 Ha: diff != 0 Ha: diff > 0 Pr(T < t) = 0.3124 Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.6248 Pr(T > t) = 0.6876 **For all three hypotheses, the null hypothesis that the means are equal can not be rejected. Test #2 – for Round-two Two-sample t test with equal variances ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Group | Obs Mean Std. Err. Std. Dev. [95% Conf. Interval] ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- Low NE | 183 3.803279 .144446 1.954029 3.518275 4.088283 High NE | 172 3.901163 .1533124 2.010673 3.598534 4.203791 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- combined | 355 3.850704 .1050597 1.979476 3.644085 4.057324 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- diff | -.0978841 .2104534 -.5117843 .3160161 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ diff = mean(0) - mean(1) t = -0.4651 Ho: diff = 0 degrees of freedom = 353 Ha: diff < 0 Ha: diff != 0 Ha: diff > 0 Pr(T < t) = 0.3211 Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.6421 Pr(T > t) = 0.6789

    **For all three hypotheses, the null hypothesis that the means are equal can not be rejected.

  • 38

    Bibliography

    Bizer, George, Jon Krosnick, Allyson Holbrook, Christian Wheeler, Derek Rucker, and Richard Petty. 2004. “The Impact of Personality on Cognitive, Behavioral, and Affective Political Processes: The Effects of Need to Evaluate.” Journal of Personality 72 (5): 995-1028. Bizer, George Y., Z. L. Tormala, D. D. Rucker, and R.E. Petty. 2006. Memory-based Versus On-line Processing: Implications for Attitude Strength. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 42, 646-653. Camerer, C. 1995. “Individual Decision Making.” In J. H. Kagel, and A. E. Roth (Eds.), The Handbook of Experimental Economics. Princeton University Press. Chong, Dennis. 1993. “How People Think, Reason, and Feel about Rights and Liberties.” American Journal of Political Science 37(3): 867-899. Cobb, MD, and J Kuklinski. 1997. “Changing Minds: Political Arguments and Political Persuasion.” American Journal of Political Science 41: 88-121. Delli-Carpini, Michael, and Scott Keeter. 1996. What Americans Know About Politics and Why it Matters” Yale University Press. de Vreese, Claes. 2004. “The Effects of Strategic News on Political Cynicism, Issue Evaluations, and Policy Support: A Two-Wave Experiment.” Mass Communication and Society 7(2): 191-214. Downs, Anthony. 1957. An Economic Theory of Democracy. Harper and Row. Druckman, Jamie. 2001. “The Implications of Framing Effects for Citizen Competence.” Political Behavior 23(3): 225-256. Druckman, Jamie, and Dennis Chong. 2007. “Framing Public Opinion in Competitive Democracies.” American Political Science Review 101: 637-655. Druckman, Jamie, and Justin Holmes. 2004. “Does Presidential Rhetoric Matter?: Priming and Presidential Approval.” Presidential Studies Quarterly 34: 755-778. Druckman, Jamie, and Kjersten R. Nelson. 2003. “Framing and Deliberation: How Citizens’ Conversations Limit Elite Influence.” American Journal of Political Science 47(4): 729-745. Druckman, Jamie, and Dennis Chong. No date. “Framing Effects Over Time.” Unpublished manuscript.

  • 39

    Frederico, C.M., and M.C. Schneider. 2007. “Political Expertise and the Use of Ideology: Moderating Effects of Evaluative Motivation.” Public Opinion Quarterly 71(2): 221-252. Hovland, C., and W. Weiss. 1951-52. “The Influence of Source Credibility on Communication Effectiveness.” Public Opinion Quarterly 15: 635-650. Lau, Richard, and David Redlawsk. 2006. How Voters Decide: Information Processing in Election Campaigns. Cambridge University Press. Levin, I. P., S.L. Schneider, and G.J. Gaeth. 1998. “All Frames are Not Created Equal: A Typology and Critical Analysis of Framing Effects.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Making Processes 76: 149-188. Li, S. 1998. “Can the Conditions Governing the Framing Effect be Determined?” Journal of Economic Psychology 19: 133-153. Lupia, Arthur, and Mathew McCubbins. 1998. The Democratic Dilemma: Can Citizens Learn What they Need to Know? Cambridge University Press. Nelson, Thomas E., Rosalee A. Clawson, and Zoe M. Oxley. 1997. “Media Framing of a Civil Liberties Conflict and Its Effect on Tolerance.” American Political Science Review 91: 567-583. Popkin, Samuel. 1991. The Reasoning Voter: Communication and Persuasion in Presidential Campaigns. University of Chicago Press. Sniderman, Paul M., and Sean M. Theriault. 2004. “The Structure of Political Argument and the Logic of Issue Framing.” Willem Saris and Paul Sniderman (eds), Studies in Public Opinion. Princeton University Press. Tewksbury, David, Jennifer Jones, Matthew Peske, Ashlea Raymond, and William Vig. 2000. “The Interaction of News and Advocate Frames: Manipulating Audience Perceptions of a Local Public Policy Issue.” Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly 77(4): 804-829. Tormala, Zakary, and Richard Petty. 2001. “On-Line Versus Memory-Based Processing: The Role of “Need to Evaluate” in Person Perception.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27(12): 1599-1612. Tversky, A., and D. Kahneman. 1987. “Rational Choice and the Framing of Decisions.” In R. M. Hogarth, and M. W. Reder (Eds.), Rational Choice: the Contrast Between Economics and Psychology. University of Chicago Press. Zaller, J. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Communication. Cambridge University Press.