8
Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and Aitude Aect Perceptions of a Robot’s Social alities? Thomas Arnold Human-Robot Interaction Laboratory Tufts University Medford, MA [email protected] Matthias Scheutz Human-Robot Interaction Laboratory Tufts University Medford, MA [email protected] ABSTRACT The complex role of touch is an increasingly appreciated horizon for HRI research. The explicit and implicit registers of touch, both human-to-robot and robot-to-human, have opened up pressing questions in design and HRI ethics about embodiment, communi- cation, care, and human aection. In this paper we present results of an MTurk survey about robot-initiated touch in a social context. We examine how a positive or negative attitude from the robot, as well as whether the robot touches an interactant, aects how a robot is judged as a worker and teammate. Our ndings conrm previous empirical support for the idea of touch as enhancing social appraisals of a robot, though the extent of that positive tactile role was complicated and tempered by the survey responses’ gender eects. KEYWORDS robot touch; gender eects; observation study ACM Reference format: Thomas Arnold and Matthias Scheutz. 2018. Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and Attitude Aect Perceptions of a Robot’s Social Qualities?. In Proceedings of 2018 ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Chicago, IL, USA, March 5–8, 2018 (HRI ’18), 8 pages. https://doi.org/10.1145/3171221.3171263 1 INTRODUCTION The complex role of touch is an increasingly appreciated horizon for HRI research. The explicit and implicit registers of touch, both human-to-robot and robot-to-human, have opened up pressing questions in design and HRI ethics about embodiment, communi- cation, care, and human aection. As products like Paro nd their way into therapeutic use, researchers are nding positive eects of tactile human-robot interaction on people’s emotional state and performance. Yet there are charged debates around human-robot intimacy, especially around sexuality, that are becoming more cir- cumspect about what such touch might bring with it. It is a critical Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for prot or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the rst page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specic permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]. HRI ’18, March 5–8, 2018, Chicago, IL, USA © 2018 Association for Computing Machinery. ACM ISBN 978-1-4503-4953-6/18/03. . . $15.00 https://doi.org/10.1145/3171221.3171263 project within HRI to anticipate and prepare for the risks and oppor- tunities that tactile interaction will present, as social interaction on the whole gets more sophisticated and varied. HRI as a eld must oer its distinctive insights in a technological environment where haptic developments in AR and VR, for instance, are complicating notions of what touch in “real-life” is and could be. The last decade has seen a number of interesting and engaging eorts to tackle the varied dimensions of touch. From providing anxiolytic comfort to lending encouragement and motivation, these approaches have shown that a robot’s vocal and facial presentation is just part of the story of how human-robot interaction can meet human needs and promote human abilities. As areas of practical robotic applications grow, it is all the more apparent that the social context and complexity in which touch might feature will need to be studied as constitutive of how touch is interpreted, experienced, and reshaped. In addition, these applications will need to account for how dierent social locations and roles may condition how touch should best be employed. In this paper, we attempt to build on this trajectory of research in tactile HRI by presenting a concrete example of touch and sur- veying how observers judge a robot touching a person. We present results of an MTurk survey testing dierent conditions for this touch, situated in an oce-like task environment. We are inter- ested in how the evaluation of a robot unfolds in cases where the robot’s touch is accompanied by positive or negative attitude via verbal response, as well as what dierence it may make when the robot touches a female or male actor in the scenario. Our ndings conrm previous empirical support for the idea of touch as enhanc- ing social appraisals of a robot, though gauging that positive role of touch was complicated and tempered by gender eects in the survey responses. The degree to which the robot’s performance was appraised positively had intriguing dependencies on whether a male or female was being touched and with what attitude, and the results suggest that gender roles and workplace expectations can shape how social robots’ touch bears on evaluating the robot itself. 2 BACKGROUND AND MOTIVATION It is not a new observation that HRI work needs to incorporate richer research into touch as a facet of interaction [13]. The last decade has seen a steady increase of inquiries into human touch of robots, mutual touch, and robot-initiated touch of people [2] – re- cently given a thorough mapping by Homann [18]. The emergent attention of “implicit interaction” [20] has only drawn further atten- tion to how shared space and various shades of contact can connect physiological reactions with social aspects of robotic function [23]. More explicit, hot-button issues such as sex robots bring touch to

Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and ...examined how communication cues like gaze and style of touch can aect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and ...examined how communication cues like gaze and style of touch can aect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One

Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch andA�itude A�ect Perceptions of a Robot’s Social�alities?

Thomas ArnoldHuman-Robot Interaction Laboratory

Tufts UniversityMedford, MA

[email protected]

Matthias ScheutzHuman-Robot Interaction Laboratory

Tufts UniversityMedford, MA

[email protected]

ABSTRACTThe complex role of touch is an increasingly appreciated horizonfor HRI research. The explicit and implicit registers of touch, bothhuman-to-robot and robot-to-human, have opened up pressingquestions in design and HRI ethics about embodiment, communi-cation, care, and human a�ection. In this paper we present resultsof an MTurk survey about robot-initiated touch in a social context.We examine how a positive or negative attitude from the robot,as well as whether the robot touches an interactant, a�ects how arobot is judged as a worker and teammate. Our �ndings con�rmprevious empirical support for the idea of touch as enhancing socialappraisals of a robot, though the extent of that positive tactile rolewas complicated and tempered by the survey responses’ gendere�ects.

KEYWORDSrobot touch; gender e�ects; observation study

ACM Reference format:Thomas Arnold and Matthias Scheutz. 2018. Observing Robot Touch inContext: How Does Touch and Attitude A�ect Perceptions of a Robot’sSocial Qualities?. In Proceedings of 2018 ACM/IEEE International Conferenceon Human-Robot Interaction, Chicago, IL, USA, March 5–8, 2018 (HRI ’18),8 pages.https://doi.org/10.1145/3171221.3171263

1 INTRODUCTIONThe complex role of touch is an increasingly appreciated horizonfor HRI research. The explicit and implicit registers of touch, bothhuman-to-robot and robot-to-human, have opened up pressingquestions in design and HRI ethics about embodiment, communi-cation, care, and human a�ection. As products like Paro �nd theirway into therapeutic use, researchers are �nding positive e�ectsof tactile human-robot interaction on people’s emotional state andperformance. Yet there are charged debates around human-robotintimacy, especially around sexuality, that are becoming more cir-cumspect about what such touch might bring with it. It is a critical

Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal orclassroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributedfor pro�t or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citationon the �rst page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACMmust be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish,to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior speci�c permission and/or afee. Request permissions from [email protected] ’18, March 5–8, 2018, Chicago, IL, USA© 2018 Association for Computing Machinery.ACM ISBN 978-1-4503-4953-6/18/03. . . $15.00https://doi.org/10.1145/3171221.3171263

project within HRI to anticipate and prepare for the risks and oppor-tunities that tactile interaction will present, as social interaction onthe whole gets more sophisticated and varied. HRI as a �eld musto�er its distinctive insights in a technological environment wherehaptic developments in AR and VR, for instance, are complicatingnotions of what touch in “real-life” is and could be.

The last decade has seen a number of interesting and engaginge�orts to tackle the varied dimensions of touch. From providinganxiolytic comfort to lending encouragement and motivation, theseapproaches have shown that a robot’s vocal and facial presentationis just part of the story of how human-robot interaction can meethuman needs and promote human abilities. As areas of practicalrobotic applications grow, it is all the more apparent that the socialcontext and complexity in which touch might feature will need tobe studied as constitutive of how touch is interpreted, experienced,and reshaped. In addition, these applications will need to accountfor how di�erent social locations and roles may condition howtouch should best be employed.

In this paper, we attempt to build on this trajectory of researchin tactile HRI by presenting a concrete example of touch and sur-veying how observers judge a robot touching a person. We presentresults of an MTurk survey testing di�erent conditions for thistouch, situated in an o�ce-like task environment. We are inter-ested in how the evaluation of a robot unfolds in cases where therobot’s touch is accompanied by positive or negative attitude viaverbal response, as well as what di�erence it may make when therobot touches a female or male actor in the scenario. Our �ndingscon�rm previous empirical support for the idea of touch as enhanc-ing social appraisals of a robot, though gauging that positive roleof touch was complicated and tempered by gender e�ects in thesurvey responses. The degree to which the robot’s performancewas appraised positively had intriguing dependencies on whether amale or female was being touched and with what attitude, and theresults suggest that gender roles and workplace expectations canshape how social robots’ touch bears on evaluating the robot itself.

2 BACKGROUND AND MOTIVATIONIt is not a new observation that HRI work needs to incorporatericher research into touch as a facet of interaction [13]. The lastdecade has seen a steady increase of inquiries into human touch ofrobots, mutual touch, and robot-initiated touch of people [2] – re-cently given a thorough mapping by Ho�mann [18]. The emergentattention of “implicit interaction” [20] has only drawn further atten-tion to how shared space and various shades of contact can connectphysiological reactions with social aspects of robotic function [23].More explicit, hot-button issues such as sex robots bring touch to

Page 2: Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and ...examined how communication cues like gaze and style of touch can aect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One

the fore as a potential threat to human intimacy and indeed a pos-sible model for abuse [16, 28]. The broader ethical implications ofdesigning robots for tactile interaction are receiving closer scrutinyand clearer articulation [4, 38].

Understandably, research in HRI has explored the many ques-tions that robot touch raises with a corresponding range of methods.The prospect of robots whose purpose is chie�y tactile has led re-searchers to test whether a robot’s touch, whether active or passive,is as e�ective as a human being’s touch, or at least qualitativelysimilar: for example, for a head massage [40] or other stimuli [37].Beyond how users report about how the robot feels, tests of thera-peutic products like Paro have examined its physiological e�ects onthose who touch it [39]. The possibility of humanoid robots beingable to convey a�ection through touch, or enact a cultural practice,continues to garner empirical evidence [1, 12], and human perfor-mance itself on a task-oriented activity has recently been measuredto gauge how a robot touch might provide encouragement [32].

While pleasant or tactile feedback characterizes many robotsmeant for human touch, robot-initiated touch has also, by andlarge, been found to improve receptions of social robots by theirinteractants [13, 18, 25]. This holds not just for pure a�ection orfriendly touch, but more functional touch as well (e.g. where thehuman interactant sees a direct purpose for it, such as taking pulse).Even warmth [26] can yield positive e�ects for how a human beingevaluates being touched by a robot.

The context in which a robot touches a human being can framehow it is being evaluated and measured for e�ects. A nursing homeimplementation [11] will have di�erent expectations and prioritiesthan those in domestic or school settings [42]. Recent work hasexamined how communication cues like gaze and style of touchcan a�ect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One of theresearch challenges moving forward, then, is to yoke tests of touchto plausible, practical, and robust settings where robots seem likelyto be present, operational, and reasonably appropriate. This is allthe more challenging given the mechanical and logistical subtletyof touch in a live setting, whether lab or “in the wild.” Consistentlyadministering or receiving touch is crucial for having a legitimatecomparison across participants for what they are experiencing.

Helpfully, the literature on interpersonal touch suggests howreactions to robotic touch might still be tested without live, in-lab touch. The “social meaning model” of touch [9] registers howobservers can interpret touch in a manner quite similar to theperson and person being touched. Based on such a model, baredescriptions in language of touch scenarios, or pictures representingsuch touch, have been used to elicit reactions and judgements fromstudy participants [18].

Given the many complex and increasingly nuanced social is-sues that HRI presents, and the public debates about robots andAI in which those issues have featured, what HRI touch researchcan expand upon is more than just positive or negative ratings ofa robot, feelings of a�ection, or experiential similarity to humantouch. Robot-initiated touch in particular may be a key leadingedge to how robotic autonomy and/or agency is interpreted inshared spaces. Thus, evaluating the robot on the basis of tactileaction will need to involve a broader set of attributions than typ-ically employed [5], opening onto notions of social competence,responsibility, companionability, and dependability.

The present study attempts to advance HRI touch researchalong these lines, situating robot-initiated touch in a plausible task-oriented context and then examining how third-party observersevaluate the robot as a social agent after seeing the tactile inter-action. Embedding touch into a more robust context allows theinterpretation of touch to incorporate the appearance and attitudefrom the robot, with more detail visible in terms of the robot’s func-tion and performance. How does touch a�ect one’s attributionstoward a robot if the robot is giving instructions while reactingcritically or encouragingly? To capture further some of the real-lifespontaneity with which robot-initiated touch may occur, the studyseeks to represent a touch that is not announced or prepared forby the robot. Because of how gender roles can a�ect how robotsare interpreted [30], including the gender-typicality of those withwhom they interact, the study also sought to represent the robottouching a male and female subject in respective conditions [10, 22].One of the questions being investigated is how gender might a�ecthow touch is interpreted by an observer, especially given di�erent"social meanings" for touch between genders in settings where sta-tus is asserted [7, 15]. Accordingly, we are interested in paying closeattention to how male and female participants respectively ascribesocial qualities to the robot after observing its overall interaction.

An in-lab setting for such a design would, again, yield directexperience for a participant of the robot’s touch. Facing di�cultiesof consent, previous in-lab worked has often prepared participantsfor being touched by having it announced beforehand, or by havingthe participant touch the robot �rst. Such measures, however, notonly make it harder to know how the touch itself is interpreted (asopposed to the bonding of touching the robot �rst), as admittedin [25], but also lose some of spontaneity that would more closelyresemble interpersonal touch.

Given the “social meaning” point mentioned above, then, a legit-imate research step forward can be taken by having an observerinterpret a simulated scenario that shows a real touch in social con-text. To give more detail than a verbal description or photograph,this study created a video for participants to observe before takinga survey about the robot, described below.

From the previous HRI research our primary hypothesis is thatrobot touch, whether with negative and positive attitudes, willimprove ratings of the robot’s social qualities and performance.Though our review of the literature on touch, gender expectations,and interpersonal attraction cues us to look for gender e�ects fromparticipant rating, neither past HRI work nor studies of interper-sonal touch provide a conclusive suggestion about what the signi�-cant e�ects would be [34, 36, 41]; accordingly we did not hypoth-esize signi�cant gender e�ects in terms of attributions, whetherbetween male and female participants or between the gender of theperson observed being touched.

3 METHODSIn order to test how people appraise a social robot whose interactionmight include touch, we set up a survey that would be based ona videorecording (with audio) of a human-robot-interaction. Toextend the usual metrics for assessing robots’ sociality, this surveysought to elicit judgments about the robot as a worker, teammate,and overall agent, not just as a touching instrument. We decided

Page 3: Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and ...examined how communication cues like gaze and style of touch can aect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One

on an Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT) in part because we wouldhave a chance at reach a broader demographic than the collegestudents typically signing up in a university setting. Nor, when itcame to robot touch in an ordinary (simulated) task environment,did we think the college demographic would be as usefully variedin terms of the o�ce environment presented. We �nd that AMTstudies have been suitably defended as an appropriate method [14],though we also recognize recent cautions about ways in whichAMT’s might have unanticipated limits to the subjects recruitedthat end up participating [33]. By way of avoiding one of the pitfallsmentioned by Stewart et al., we recruited participants at both dayand evening times.

3.1 MaterialsScenario. The scenario (used as video template) was that of a

person entering a room and being greeted by a PR2 robot standingnext to a desk, on which sits a computer keyboard and monitor. Therobot �rst asks the person to put their cellphone in a bin next to thecomputer, and it then invites the person to sit down at the computerand enter the information requested on the screen. It gestures withone arm toward the computer screen and with the other arm pointsto the chair. In each condition, the computer goes black after theperson inputs a few items of information. At that point the robotlooks toward the participant then the screen and back and either 1)o�ers the verbal feedback, “That’s OK, I know how to �x this” whilegently touching the person on the back (above the chair back), 2)o�ers negative feedback of “What did you do?” while touching theperson in the same fashion, 3) o�ers the positive feedback withouttouching, 4) o�ers the negative feedback without touching. Afterthis is done, the robot tells the person to “Hit the Enter key twice”at which point the screen returns to normal and the person �nishestheir input of information. The robot then says, “Thank you” to theparticipant and the video ends.

Video recordings. We made a separate video for each of the fourresponse scenarios above. Because we were interested in the e�ectof gender on how touch is regarded, these four were recordedfor a male actor and female actor both, making a total of eightdi�erent videos from which the AMT would choose (randomly) fora participant to observe before taking the survey. Fig. 1 shows thefour snapshots from the moment in the video where the male orfemale actor were either touched or not touched by the robot. ThePR2 robot used for the recording was fully remotely controlled.

Survey. The post-experiment survey consisted of 12 questions,plus a validating question at the end to make sure the participanthad seen correctly whether or not the robot had touched the personin the video. For each question participants were asked to rate theiragreement on the following scale: 1 = strongly disagree 2 = disagree3 = neither disagree nor agree 4 = agree 5 = strongly agree. Thequestions were as follows:

I felt that the robot was very capable of performing its jobI had con�dence in the skills of the robotI believe that the robot was well quali�edThe robot has specialized capabilities that can increase our perfor-manceI believe the robot really looked out for what was important to thepersonThe person’s needs and desires were very important to the robotI think the robot went out of its way to help the personI believe that the robot tried to be fair in dealings with the personThe robot has a strong sense of justiceI liked the values of the robotSound principles seem to guide the robot’s behaviorTo what extent would you want this the robot as your teammate?

G T A F Mf + + 21 22f + - 20 28f - + 12 28f - - 21 24m + + 16 25m + - 12 25m - + 12 25m - - 21 20

Figure 2: Number offemale (F) and male(M) subject in eachcondition (G=actor gen-der, T=touch/no-touch,A=positive/negative).

Participants. We recruited400 US subjects from AMT:68 were eliminated eitherbecause they did not �nish theexperiment or because theydid not pass the “probe”(whichtested whether they saw therobot touch the person), leaving332 subjects for the study(135 female/197 male) withan average age of 44.43 years(see Fig. 2 for a breakdownaccording to each experimentalcondition).

Procedure. Participants wereinformed that the purpose of thestudy was to collect data on howhumans perceive human-robotinteractions in daily life scenar-ios. They were told that, upon giving informed consent and com-pleting a brief demographic questionnaire, they would be presentedwith a brief video showing the scenario and then be asked to answerquestions about the scenario.

4 RESULTSWe performed an analysis to determine the optimal number offactors to retain in an exploratory factor analysis using di�erentmethods (such as the Kaiser rule or optimal coordinates) and ob-tained that two factors (for three out of four methods) and onefactor (for one out of three methods) were optimal. From a Max-imum Likelihood Factor Analysis with varimax rotation we ob-tained loadings for all twelve questions, with questions 1 through4 loading on one factor, which was roughly about robot skills andcapabilities, and questions 5 through 12 loading on the other fac-tor, which was about roughly about the robot’s moral attitude (to-gether both factors explained about 70% of the variance). We thenperformed 2x2x2x2 ANOVAs with subject gender (male/female),agent gender (male/female), attitude (positive/negative) and touch-ing (touch/no touch) as independent variables and each of thetwo factors as dependent variable, respectively. For the �rst fac-tor on robot skills and capabilities we found signi�cant main ef-fects for subject gender (F (1, 316) = 8.67, p = .003, �2p = .027)

Page 4: Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and ...examined how communication cues like gaze and style of touch can aect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One

Figure 1: Snapshot from the video conditions. Top row: the female and male subjects as they are touched by the robot. Bottomrow: the female and the male subjects without being touch by the robot.

and attitude (F (1, 316) = 13.30, p < .001, �2p = .040), as well assigni�cant two-way interactions between touch and actor gen-der (F (1, 316) = 10.75, p = .001, �2p = .033) and subject genderand attitude (F (1, 316) = 8.45, p = .004, �2p = .026), and a sig-ni�cant three-way interaction between touch, subject gender andactor gender (F (1, 316) = 5.26, p = .022, �2p = .016). For the sec-ond factor on robot moral attitudes we found signi�cant maine�ects for touch (F (1, 316) = 16.17, p < .001, �2p = .049), ac-tor gender (F (1, 316) = 4.77, p = .030, �2p = .015), and attitude(F (1, 316) = 11.13, p < .001, �2p = .034), but no signi�cant interac-tions. Based on these results, we performed more detailed analyseson the individual questions to examine the signi�cant di�erencesfound for both factors.

Answers to “Capable of performing its job”We performed a 2x2x2x2 ANOVAwith subject gender (male/female),agent gender (male/female), attititue (positive/negative) and touch-ing (touch/no touch) as independent variables and the answer tothe “I felt that the robot was very capable of performing its job.”as dependent variable. We found signi�cant main e�ects for sub-ject gender (F (1, 316) = 8.45, p = .004, �2p = .026) and attitude(F (1, 316) = 24.99, p < .001, �2p = .073) which were eclipsed by asigni�cant two-way interactions for subject gender and attitude

(F (1, 316) = 8.45, p = 0.004, �2p = .033) showing that males agreedsigni�cantly less that the robot was performing its job when itsattitude was negative than when its attitude was positive while forfemales the agreement the negative attitude made no real di�er-ence. We also found a signi�cant two-way interactions betweenagent gender and touch F (1, 316) = 6.80, p = 0.009, �2p = .021)which was eclipsed by a signi�cant three-way interactions betweensubject gender, agent gender, and touch F (1, 316) = 5.11, p = 0.024,�2p = .016) showing that male subjects only gave the male no-touchcondition a lower rating (M = 3.47, SD = 1.06), the other condi-tions were about equal around 3.8, while female subjects rated themale touching highest (M = 4.35, SD = .78), followed by femaleno-touch (M = 4.21, SD = .78), female touch (M = 3.98, SD = 1.11)and male no touch (M = 3.64, SD = .90).

Answers to "Con�dence in the skills of therobot"We performed a 2x2x2x2 ANOVAwith subject gender (male/female),agent gender (male/female), attitude (positive/negative) and touching(touch/no touch) as independent variables and the answer to the “Ihad con�dence in the skills of the robot.” as dependent variable. Wefound signi�cant main e�ects for subject gender (F (1, 316) = 4.2,p = .041, �2p = .013), attitude (F (1, 316) = 11.31, p < .001, �2p =

Page 5: Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and ...examined how communication cues like gaze and style of touch can aect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One

.035), and touch (F (1, 316) = 4.1, p = .044, �2p = .013) with femalesubjects having higher con�dence in the robot’s skills (M = 3.81,SD = 1.09 vs M = 3.58, SD = 1.08 for males), subjects preferringthe positive attitude (M = 3.86, SD = .98 vs. M = 3.49, SD = 1.16for the negative attitude) and touching (M = 3.79, SD = 1.06vs. M = 3.55, SD = 1.11 for no touch). Moreover, we found astrong two-way interaction between agent gender and touching(F (1, 316) = 12.26, p < .001, �2p = .034) showing that subjectsrated the robot touching the male subject signi�cantly higher incon�dence in skills (M3.92, SD = .93)) than in the no touchingcondition (M = 3.28, SD = 1.13), while the con�dence in therobot’s skills was rated numerically lower (M = 3.69, SD = 1.15)for touching the female than no touch (M = 3.79, SD = 1.04).

Answers to "Well-quali�ed"We performed a 2x2x2x2 ANOVAwith subject gender (male/female),agent gender (male/female), attitude (positive/negative) and touching(touch/no touch) as independent variables and the answer to the“I believe that the robot was well quali�ed.” as dependent variable.We found a signi�cant main e�ects for subject gender (F (1, 316) =10.72, p = .001, �2p = .033) and for attitude (F (1, 316) = 20.50,p < .001, �2p = .059) which was eclipsed by a signi�cant two-wayinteraction between subject gender and attitude (F (1, 316) = 5.92,p � .021, �2p = .016) showing that the robot’s positive vs. negativeattitude did not make as much of a di�erence for female subjects(M = 3.98, SD = .97 vs.M = 3.78, SD = 1.15) as for male subjectsregarding its quali�cation (M = 3.83, SD = .95 vs. M = 3.13,SD = 1.13).

Answers to "Tried to be fair"We performed a 2x2x2x2 ANOVAwith subject gender (male/female),agent gender (male/female), attitude (positive/negative) and touching(touch/no touch) as independent variables and the answer to the “believe that the robot tried to be fair in dealing with the person.” asdependent variable. We found signi�cant main e�ect for attitude(F (1, 316) = 10.96, p = .001, �2p = .035) and touching (F (1, 316) =9.2, p = .003, �2p = .028) which were eclipsed by a signi�cant three-way interactions between subject gender, attitude, and touching(F (1, 316) = 6.08, p = .014, �2p = .019) showing that for malesubjects a negative attitude without touching yields the lowestfairness rating (M = 2.86, SD = 1.21) compared to negative withtouching (M = 3.26, SD = 1.21), or positive without (M = 3.36,SD = 1.27) and with touch (M = 3.53, SD = 1.21). For femalesubjects the ratings, the overall range is wider: as with male subjectscase, negative with no touching has the lowest rating (M = 2.67,SD = 1.20), then come equally positive without touch (M = 3.54,SD = 1.38) or negative with touch (M = 3.56, SD = 1.41), thenpositive with touch as the highest (M = 3.76, SD = 1.3).

Answers to the other questionsThe questions “I believe the robot looked out for what was impor-tant to the person.”, “I think the robot went out of its way to helpthe person.”, “Sound principles seem to guide robots behavior.”, “Towhat extent would you want this robot as your teammate?” and “Iliked the values of the robot.” had highly signi�cant main e�ects

for attitude and touch: positive attitude leads to higher ratings, andtouching leads to higher ratings. “I liked the values of the robot”and “The robot has a strong sense of justice.” led to higher ratingsonly with touch, while “The person’s needs and desires were veryimportant to the robot.” only led to higher ratings with positiveattitude.

5 DISCUSSIONThe general results across all twelve questions showed that condi-tions in which the robot touched the person in the video producedhigher agreement that the robot possessed the positive qualitiesposed by the survey. The study’s results therefore support previous�ndings in HRI, and our hypothesis, that touch improves people’sevaluation of a robot’s social performance. Survey respondentsrated the robot who touched the person higher on being skilled,looking out for what was important, being attentive to needs anddesires of person, and being a desirable teammate. Touch also bol-stered respondents’ ratings of the robot being fair, having theirvalues, and going “out of its way” for people. While the survey’sattributions did not increase uniformly with touch (the e�ect on theattribute of being “capable of performing its job” being marginal,not signi�cant for “well-quali�ed”), they certainly con�rm that theobserved gesture of touch enhanced the overall social appraisal ofthe robot.

The positive attitude of the robot via verbal response, as onewould expect, yielded more positive ratings for the robot overall(it had only a marginal e�ect for the robot possessing a “sense ofjustice,” which may show that such a notion may be too weighty toinvolve positive attitude for the video’s simulated task performance).More notably, negative attitude did not render the robot’s touchcounter-productive or anti-social – touchwas generally preferred asan accompaniment to the negative feedback as well. This suggeststhat the character of the observed touch (gentleness, motion, partof the body, timing) has some independent force relative to visualor verbal channels of interaction, rather than just being alignedwith other modalities of behavior shown to the observer.

So far as those general points go, the empirical results squarewell with what previous research would have led us to expect. Aswe zoom in on some of the gender e�ects and interactions fromthe survey, however, there are some very interesting, distinctivepatterns to consider for tactile HRI investigations. There are signi�-cant di�erences both in whether a male or female actor is observedbeing touched in the video and in howmale and female respondentsevaluated the robot’s touch.

To begin with, attribution to the robot of being “capable” showan interesting discrepancy in the e�ect of touch. Male and femaleparticipants collectively found the robot more “capable” when therobot was observed touching the male actor rather than not touch-ing him (M = 4, SD = .94 vs.M = 3.53, SD = 0.99), but femaleparticipants found the robot more capable when not touching thefemale actor than when touching her (M = 4.21, SD = .78 vs.M = 3.9, SD = 1.11). Male participants, for their part, found therobot slightly more capable in touching the female actor than not(M = 3.82, SD = 1.02 vs.M = 3.71, SD = 1), but showed a widergap between touching and not touching the male actor (M = 3.8,

Page 6: Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and ...examined how communication cues like gaze and style of touch can aect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One

SD = .97 vs. M = 3.47, SD = 1.06). In other words, male partici-pants seemed to take a male not being touched as more negative forrobot appraisal than do female participants, and female participantsare more positive than male ones toward a robot that does not toucha female actor.

The same asymmetry holds for “con�dence in the robot’s skills”and the robot being “well-quali�ed” – both male and female partic-ipants agreed most about the robot’s having skills when observingthe touching of the male actor (with female con�dence atM = 4.25,SD = .80 and male atM = 3.74, SD = .96), but female respondentswere comparatively more con�dent in the robot’s skills when it didnot touch the female actor than when it did (M = 4.03, SD = .92vs. M = 3.75, SD = 1.18). As for the robot observed not touchingthe male actor, both males and female participants rated the robotlower in skills than any other conditions (M = 3.27, SD = 1.12 andM = 3.3, SD = 1.19). For whether the robot was “well-quali�ed,”female participants rated the robot highest when the male actorwas touched (M = 4.14, SD = 1.01) and when the female actor wasnot (M = 4.06, SD = .93), with the female being touched slightlylower (M = 3.85, SD = 1.2) and not touching the male actor ratedlowest (M = 3.48, SD = 1.03 ). Male participants rated the robottouching the male actor and female actor equally well-quali�ed(M = 3.58, SD = 1.01 and M = 3.58, SD = 1.18), while rating nottouching a female actor higher (M = 3.44, SD = 1.07) than nottouching the male actor (M = 3.33, SD = 1.15).

In terms of interaction between the gender of the person touchedand the robot’s appraisal, then, we have 1) a higher appreciationon the part of female participants for a female subject not beingtouched, and 2) a shared sense that the male not being touched re-sults in the lowest rating for robot’s capability, skills, quali�cations,etc. We will return to these points below.

Why is there a lower rating of a robot not touching a male amongthese conditions?Whenwe examine the role of positive vs. negativeattitude via the verbal response (“It’s OK” vs. “What did you do?!”),we see a related discrepancy when it comes, again, to capability andcon�dence in the robot’s skills. In conditions where the robot hada negative attitude, male participants showed much lower ratingsthan than they gave for the robot with a positive attitude – bothfor capability (M = 4.08, SD = .85 vs. M = 3.32, SD = 1.03)and skills (M = 3.84, SD = .07 vs. M = 3.3, SD = 1.13). Femaleparticipants’ ratings, on the other hand, do not seem as a�ectedby negative vs. positive attitude, whether for capability (M = 4.08,SD = .86 vs. M = 3.95, SD = 1.02) or skills (M = 3.90, SD = 1.01vs. M = 3.74, SD = 1.16). This striking result could be couchedas women being more accepting, or at least more accustomed tofacing negative attitudes (especially, perhaps, in a technologicalcontext); alternatively, one could surmise male participants aremore sensitive and reactive to a negative attitude from the robot.At the same time, one could ask if participants, especially femaleparticipants, observed the interaction as a setting of cross-gendertouch (due to the male voice and particular form of the PR2) thatwas less appropriate in a workplace than same-gender touch [7].

When one considers the general lower ratings for the robot thatdoes not touch the male actor, one could well view touch as helpingto mitigate or soften a perceived insult or reprimand. One mightalso theorize about female interactants’ comparative strength in�ltering out negative attitude in their appraisal of skill, capability,

etc. It adds an interesting question to the higher rating femaleparticipants gave to the robot not touching the female actor – doestouch lack the mitigating function that it might play generally formale interactants? Or, again, is this a case where cross-gender touchis considered less appropriate?

The results for the robots having “sound principles” tend tosupport the idea that some kind of cultural norm or phenomenonconcerning male vs. female treatment is feeding into robot appraisal(albeit a norm held to di�erent degrees in terms of male vs. femaleparticipants). The robot was seen to be most principled when givingpositive feedback and touching both the male and female actor,but much less principled when giving negative feedback and nottouching themale actor thanwhen giving negative feedback and nottouching the female actor. This perhaps suggests that touch directedtoward a man is thought of more positively and straightforwardlypro-social, with not touching a man carrying more negative cost tothe rating.

As one further wrinkle to how gendered dimensions to robotictouch, attitude, and role might play into larger societal judgmentsand values, we can look more closely at the question of whetherthe robot was seen as trying to be “fair.” Looking at the interactionsbetween attitude and the gender of the person touched (or nottouched), the positive attitude combined with touch was ratedhigher for both a female and male actor being touched (M = 3.7,SD = 1.15 and M = 3.56, SD = 1.36). And, not unexpectedly, apositive attitude combined with not touching the female actor wasequally high as a negative attitude with touch (M = 3.17, SD = 1.43and M = 3.17, SD = 1.34). A negative attitude with no touch forher was lowest ranked for fairness (M = 2.87, SD = 1.14). With themale actor, on the other hand, the condition of negative attitudeand not being touched received decidedly lower rank (M = 2.66,SD = 1.28) than the other three combination (positive attitude withtouch, M = 3.56, SD = 1.36; positive with no touch, M = 3.67,SD = 1.11; and negative with touch M = 3.65, SD = 1.18). Again,there seems to be a drop-o� that comes with a negative attitudeand no mitigating touch when it comes to a male interactant in thisrobotic work context.

When it comes to the interpretation of touch in a social envi-ronment, our results bear out a general point that understandingthe signaling and function of touch cannot be isolated from othermodes of sociality in HRI analyses. Those observing them beingtouched will be robust interpreters of, and thus possibly dynamicinteractants with, the robot’s performance, not excluding expecta-tions of gender and workplace roles. Given the gender e�ects wehave found, there is more �ne-grained work to be done to test thepresuppositions behind less approval for robot-initiated touch ofwomen than men. One direction in which to build is to connecttactile HRI with work on technology and gender, for example theidea of “male self-assurance and female hesitation” [8]. In paral-lel, the results point to further examination of di�erent workplaceexpectations, to see more clearly how cross-gender touch is seenand how perceived robot gender instantiates them. In what waysis not being touched related to one’s attitude toward the observedor perceived technology? For now, this study at least establishesevidence that male observers approve of robot touch on di�erentterms than female observers. The results around capability andskills invite further re�ection about how gender stereotypes, roles,

Page 7: Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and ...examined how communication cues like gaze and style of touch can aect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One

and expectations may play into how touch works with a robot’spresumed competence, authority, and power in a given context.It will be crucial in future e�orts to discern more precisely howcultural and societal norms are �nding their way into appraisals ofrobot touch and intensify debates around robot uses [31]. CertainlyHRI research will bene�t from staying connected to interpersonalresearch and gender research in psychology [19, 27], to better dis-cern where human-robot interaction poses distinctive challengesand o�ers distinctive details to how interactions operate in context[10, 21]. This will require paying attention to context-based expec-tations of touch, for example the appropriateness of cross-gendertouch within subordinate-superior workplace relationships, as wellas how female interactants may respond to forms of politeness morepositively than male interactants [29, 35]. From these particularresults there seems to be an important set of HRI discussions todevelop around gender dynamics amid personal con�ict, settingsof instruction and/or correction, and emotional regulation.

Given the size of the PR2, and that in the video its voice soundsmore typically male, another clear direction for further work willbe looking at variation in form, voice (and perhaps even cues like agiven name, to see if that frames expectations). It will also be im-portant to probe expectations and assumptions about how gender-typical certain roles might be, to see how much those inform howcapable, skilled, value-oriented, etc. a social robot is deemed. In-labresearch may adjust the con�guration of shared space, includingdi�erent interfaces (e.g. text vs. voice), to see on what terms tactileexperience enhances a robot’s perceive competence and trust. Thatsaid, the lab environment itself may not be the best indication ofhow participants would react in real-world settings [18].

Again, taken all together, these results con�rm touch’s potentialfor relationship improvement between human and robot, but theycarry important cautionary markers for how context and societalexpectations may contour that overall positive e�ect for di�erentpeople. Certainly the study underscores that designing human-robot interactions needs to account for how expected roles, rep-resentation of authority, and ascriptions of ability help conditionnotions of “teamwork,” “rapport,” and "compliance" [6], as well asHRI ethics writ large [3]. This will be even more important for themapping and study of robotic application domains, to which anygeneral theoretical commitment about robot touch should be putto the test. Anticipating how the sta� and residents of a nursinghome, say, will present robotic technology’s skill and authority,versus how children will interact in a domicile, may lead to bet-ter tactile accompaniment and more humane safeguards for socialinteractions.

Limitations. While this study uses video to provide a standard-ized stimulus for observing participants, one must bear in mindthat ultimately the study relies on an observation of touch, not afull participation in touching or being touched. To the degree thata controlled setting will be able to provide a consistent and directtactile stimulus, future study could yield richer detail into how a ro-bot’s touch operates socially and physically. Even within the frameof observing (e.g., a confederate being touched), actual physicalpresence near the observed touch (rather than video) may changethe terms by which the touch is judged. Whether as observed or di-rectly experienced, it is clear this study’s recorded interaction could

have included additional, important variations, especially given thegender e�ects it gathered. As mentioned above, it is notable that therobot’s voice had a typical male register; for that reason, having amore female-identi�ed robot voice might shed crucial light on howmuch a male identi�cation might have informed the various ratingsof the robot. Likewise, the norms of cross-gender touching andworkplace expectations will a great deal of further experimentalattention.

Another limitation of the study concerns the type of survey ad-ministered. Because we sought attributions to the robot itself, thequestions were directed to personal qualities, not the nature of thetouch and behavior itself. Going forward with research into HRItouch will require ways to track attributions without cuing partici-pants that touch is the behavior being examined, while building onprevious interpersonal measures of social and task attraction [24].

6 CONCLUSIONThrough this study we have con�rmed the general trajectory of re-sults from previous HRI work around robot-initiated touch, namelythat it can lead to more positive evaluations of the touching robot.We have expanded upon those �ndings to show that this positivee�ect extends to an array of agent-like qualities attributed to therobot, both work-oriented and relationship-oriented. In addition,we have found that a touch gesture situated in a simulated workcontext elicits gender e�ects from the survey responses, both interms of female respondents generally giving more positive ratingto a lack of robotic touch (toward female interactants) and in termsof di�erent reactions to a male not being touched with negativeattitude. We can conclude that robot-initiated touch may well chan-nel and re�ect broader gender-oriented evaluations about whenand for what touch is appropriate in a work context. Further workwill need to keep testing the rough ground of socially embeddedrobot-human touch, with more �ne-tuned variance among tasks,relationships, observers, and interactants. The way that touch maymitigate or dampen con�ict with a male user, for instance, should beconsidered in light of the wider context where that interaction takesplace and the expectations of those observing touch. More progressin testing promises to come from settings of emerging implementa-tions, where the line between “functional” and simply “a�ective”will often blur (care, therapy, companionship, guidance in publicspace). Given the qualities and attributions investigated in thisstudy, these more subtle tests may hold particular import for workon multi-agent systems and human-robot teams. Though some-times neglected in analyses of social interaction, touch promises tokeep revealing itself as inextricable from humanity’s most funda-mental social practices and deepest ideals for how to live with oneanother.

7 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTSThanks to Evan Krause, Brad Oosterveld, Romy Aboudarham, andAvi Block for their assistance with the video and survey. This workwas in part funded by grant N00014-16-1-2278 from the US O�ce ofNaval Research and grant IIS 1316809 from the US National ScienceFoundation.

Page 8: Observing Robot Touch in Context: How Does Touch and ...examined how communication cues like gaze and style of touch can aect the perception of the robot’s touch itself [17]. One

REFERENCES[1] Mehdi Ammi, Virginie Demulier, Sylvain Caillou, Yoren Ga�ary, Yacine Tsalamlal,

Jean-Claude Martin, and Adriana Tapus. 2015. Haptic human-robot a�ectiveinteraction in a handshaking social protocol. In Proceedings of the Tenth AnnualACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction. ACM, 263–270.

[2] Brenna D Argall and Aude G Billard. 2010. A survey of tactile human–robotinteractions. Robotics and autonomous systems 58, 10 (2010), 1159–1176.

[3] Thomas Arnold and Matthias Scheutz. 2017. Beyond Moral Dilemmas: Exploringthe Ethical Landscape in HRI.. In HRI. 445–452.

[4] Thomas Arnold and Matthias Scheutz. 2017. The Tactile Ethics of Soft Robotics:Designing Wisely for Human–Robot Interaction. Soft Robotics 4, 2 (2017), 81–87.

[5] Christoph Bartneck, Dana Kulić, Elizabeth Croft, and Susana Zoghbi. 2009. Mea-surement instruments for the anthropomorphism, animacy, likeability, perceivedintelligence, and perceived safety of robots. International journal of social robotics1, 1 (2009), 71–81.

[6] Aude Billard. 2017. On the mechanical, cognitive and sociable facets of humancompliance and their robotic counterparts. Robotics and Autonomous Systems(2017), 157–164.

[7] Silvia Bonaccio, Jane O?Reilly, Sharon L O?Sullivan, and François Chiocchio.2016. Nonverbal behavior and communication in the workplace: A review andan agenda for research. Journal of Management 42, 5 (2016), 1044–1074.

[8] Agnetha Broos. 2005. Gender and information and communication technologies(ICT) anxiety: Male self-assurance and female hesitation. CyberPsychology &Behavior 8, 1 (2005), 21–31.

[9] Judee K Burgoon and Deborah A Newton. 1991. Applying a social meaning modelto relational message interpretations of conversational involvement: Comparingobserver and participant perspectives. Southern Journal of Communication 56, 2(1991), 96–113.

[10] Julie Carpenter, Joan M Davis, Norah Erwin-Stewart, Ti�any R Lee, John DBransford, and Nancy Vye. 2009. Gender representation and humanoid robotsdesigned for domestic use. International Journal of Social Robotics 1, 3 (2009),261–265.

[11] Ti�any L Chen, Chih-Hung Aaron King, Andrea L Thomaz, and Charles C Kemp.2014. An investigation of responses to robot-initiated touch in a nursing context.International Journal of Social Robotics 6, 1 (2014), 141–161.

[12] Martin D Cooney, Shuichi Nishio, and Hiroshi Ishiguro. 2015. Importance oftouch for conveying a�ection in a multimodal interaction with a small humanoidrobot. International Journal of Humanoid Robotics 12, 01 (2015), 1550002.

[13] Henriette Cramer, Nicander Kemper, Alia Amin, and Vanessa Evers. 2009. Thee�ects of robot touch and proactive behaviour on perceptions of human-robotinteractions. In Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), 2009 4th ACM/IEEE InternationalConference on. IEEE, 275–276.

[14] Matthew JC Crump, John V McDonnell, and Todd M Gureckis. 2013. EvaluatingAmazon’s Mechanical Turk as a tool for experimental behavioral research. PloSone 8, 3 (2013), e57410.

[15] Norah E Dunbar and Judee K Burgoon. 2005. Perceptions of power and interac-tional dominance in interpersonal relationships. Journal of Social and PersonalRelationships 22, 2 (2005), 207–233.

[16] Sinziana Gutiu. 2012. Sex robots and roboticization of consent. In We Robot 2012conference, Coral Gables, Florida. Retrieved April, Vol. 15. 2013.

[17] Takahiro Hirano, Masahiro Shiomi, Takamasa Iio, Mitsuhiko Kimoto, Ivan Tanev,Katsunori Shimohara, and Norihiro Hagita. 2017. How Do Communication CuesChange Impressions of Human–Robot Touch Interaction? International Journalof Social Robotics (2017), 1–11.

[18] Laura Ho�mann. 2017. That Robot Touch that Means so Much: On the PsychologicalE�ects of Human-Robot Touch. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Duisburg-Essen,Germany.

[19] Robert A Josephs, Hazel R Markus, and Romin W Tafarodi. 1992. Gender andself-esteem. Journal of personality and social psychology 63, 3 (1992), 391.

[20] Wendy Ju. 2015. The design of implicit interactions. Synthesis Lectures on Human-Centered Informatics 8, 2 (2015), 1–93.

[21] Dieta Kuchenbrandt, Markus Häring, Jessica Eichberg, Friederike Eyssel, andElisabeth André. 2014. Keep an eye on the task! How gender typicality of tasksin�uence human–robot interactions. International Journal of Social Robotics 6, 3

(2014), 417–427.[22] Dingjun Li, PL Patrick Rau, and Ye Li. 2010. A cross-cultural study: E�ect of

robot appearance and task. International Journal of Social Robotics 2, 2 (2010),175–186.

[23] Jamy Li, Wendy Ju, and Byron Reeves. 2016. Touching a mechanical body: tactilecontact with intimate parts of a humanoid robot is physiologically arousing. In66th Annual Conference of the International Communication Association. Fukuoka,Japan.

[24] James C McCroskey and Thomas A McCain. 1974. The measurement of interper-sonal attraction. (1974).

[25] Kayako Nakagawa, Masahiro Shiomi, Kazuhiko Shinozawa, Reo Matsumura,Hiroshi Ishiguro, and Norihiro Hagita. 2011. E�ect of robot’s active touch onpeople’s motivation. In Proceedings of the 6th international conference on Human-robot interaction. ACM, 465–472.

[26] Jiaqi Nie, Michelle Park, Angie Lorena Marin, and S Shyam Sundar. 2012. Can youhold my hand? Physical warmth in human-robot interaction. In Human-RobotInteraction (HRI), 2012 7th ACM/IEEE International Conference on. IEEE, 201–202.

[27] KV Petrides and Adrian Furnham. 2000. Gender di�erences in measured andself-estimated trait emotional intelligence. Sex roles 42, 5 (2000), 449–461.

[28] Kathleen Richardson. 2016. The asymmetrical’relationship’: parallels betweenprostitution and the development of sex robots. ACM SIGCAS Computers andSociety 45, 3 (2016), 290–293.

[29] Virginia P Richmond and James C McCroskey. 2000. The impact of supervisorand subordinate immediacy on relational and organizational outcomes. Commu-nications Monographs 67, 1 (2000), 85–95.

[30] Paul Schermerhorn, Matthias Scheutz, and Charles R Crowell. 2008. Robotsocial presence and gender: Do females view robots di�erently than males?.In Proceedings of the 3rd ACM/IEEE international conference on Human robotinteraction. ACM, 263–270.

[31] Matthias Scheutz and Thomas Arnold. 2016. Are we ready for sex robots?. In TheEleventh ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human Robot Interaction. IEEEPress, 351–358.

[32] Masahiro Shiomi, Kayako Nakagawa, Kazuhiko Shinozawa, Reo Matsumura,Hiroshi Ishiguro, and Norihiro Hagita. 2017. Does A Robot?s Touch EncourageHuman E�ort? International Journal of Social Robotics 9, 1 (2017), 5–15.

[33] Neil Stewart, Jesse Chandler, and Gabriele Paolacci. 2017. Crowdsourcing Samplesin Cognitive Science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences (2017).

[34] Deborah S Stier and Judith AHall. 1984. Gender di�erences in touch: An empiricaland theoretical review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47, 2 (1984),440.

[35] Megan Strait, Priscilla Briggs, and Matthias Scheutz. 2015. Gender, more sothan Age, Modulates Positive Perceptions of Language-Based Human-RobotInteractions. In 4th international symposium on new frontiers in human robotinteraction.

[36] Juulia T Suvilehto, Enrico Glerean, Robin IM Dunbar, Riitta Hari, and LauriNummenmaa. 2015. Topography of social touching depends on emotional bondsbetween humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112, 45 (2015),13811–13816.

[37] Chantal Triscoli, Håkan Olausson, Uta Sailer, Hanna Ignell, and Ilona Croy. 2013.CT-optimized skin stroking delivered by hand or robot is comparable. Frontiersin behavioral neuroscience 7 (2013).

[38] Jan BF Van Erp and Alexander Toet. 2013. How to touch humans: Guidelines forsocial agents and robots that can touch. In A�ective Computing and IntelligentInteraction (ACII), 2013 Humaine Association Conference on. IEEE, 780–785.

[39] Kazuyoshi Wada and Takanori Shibata. 2007. Living with seal robots?its so-ciopsychological and physiological in�uences on the elderly at a care house. IEEETransactions on Robotics 23, 5 (2007), 972–980.

[40] Ryan Walker and Christoph Bartneck. 2013. The pleasure of receiving a headmassage from a robot. In RO-MAN, 2013 IEEE. IEEE, 807–813.

[41] Andrea Webb and Joann Peck. 2015. Individual di�erences in interpersonal touch:On the development, validation, and use of the ?comfort with interpersonaltouch?(CIT) scale. Journal of consumer psychology 25, 1 (2015), 60–77.

[42] Steve Yohanan and Karon E MacLean. 2012. The role of a�ective touch in human-robot interaction: Human intent and expectations in touching the haptic creature.International Journal of Social Robotics 4, 2 (2012), 163–180.