59
FACETS OF MEMORY DISTORTION: EVIDENCE FROM AMNESIA, CONFABULATION AND FRONTAL-LOBE DYSFUNCTION Brenda Me10 A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master's of Arts Graduate Department of Psychology University of Toronto Q Copyright by Brenda Melo, 1998

FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

FACETS OF MEMORY DISTORTION: EVIDENCE FROM

AMNESIA, CONFABULATION AND FRONTAL-LOBE DYSFUNCTION

Brenda Me10

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements

for the degree of Master's of Arts

Graduate Department of Psychology

University of Toronto

Q Copyright by Brenda Melo, 1998

Page 2: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

National Libmry Bibliothèque nationale du Canada

Acquisitions and Acquisitions et Bibliograp hic Services services bibliographiques

395 Wellington Street 395, nie Wellington OnawaON K 1 A W Ottawa ON KIA ON4 canada Canada

The author has granted a non- exclusive licence allowing the National Lhrary of Canada to reproduce, loan, distriiute or seU copies of this thesis in microform, paper or electronic formats.

The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantial extracts fkom it may be printed or otherwise reproduced without the author's permission.

L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive permettant ai la Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou vendre des copies de cette thèse sous la forme de rnicxofiche/£üm, de reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique.

L'auteur conserve la propriété du droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés ou autrement reproduits sans son autorisation.

Page 3: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Melo, Brenda

Department of Psychology

University of Toronto

Facets of rnernory distortion: Evidence from amnesia, confabulation and fhntal-lobe

dyshction.

Master's of Arts Degree, 1998.

False recail and false recognition of nonstudied words that are preceded by a list of strong

associates was examined in amnesic patients with medial temporal lobe or ventromedial

frontal40 be damage and in nonamnesic patients with fiontai-10 be damage. We hypothesized

that susceptibility to fdse recall and false recognition is determïned by several factors,

narnely havùig some memory for the studied words, the ability to extract the semantic and

associative information about the studied list, and a deficit in strategic monitoring.

Nonamnesic frontal-lobe patients consistently produced high levels of fdse recall and fdse

recognition. Since these patients had relatively good memory they extracted the semantic

information about the list. However, due to their strategic monitoring deficit they failed to

reject nonstudied related words. The false recollection pattern of amnesic patients was more

variable. Those who extracted the semantic/associative nature of the list and remembered it,

produced hi& levels of fdse recall.

Page 4: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Aclmow Iedgements

1 would like to thank my thesis advisors, Dr. Gordon Winocur and Dr. Moms Moscovitch,

for their expertise, encouragement and support throughout the preparation of this thesis. 1

would like to thauk the Psychology and Behavioural Neurology departments at Baycrest

Centre for Genatnc Care, and in particular, Bria. Richards and Dr. Don Stuss for the referral

of patients to this study. 1 also owe a debt of gratitude to Marilyue Ziegler for her technical

expertise, and would like to acknowiedge the collective support of everyone at the Memory

Lab. Many thanks to my fellow graduate students, Maria Arrnilio and Reza Habib, for their

positive comments, assistance and support.

iii

Page 5: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Table of Contents

Page

Introduction .......................................................................................... 1

Method ................................................................................................ 18

. . Participants ........................................................................................ 19

Materials ........................................................................................... 22

Design and Procedure ............................................................................ 22

Results ................................................................................................. 24

Discussion ............................................................................................. 36

Page 6: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Day to day Life would be impossible if the human brain was not equipped with

mechanisms which allowed memories to be formed and retrieved. Lndeed, the survival of

the human species is dependent on its ability to access accurate information about the past.

Despite its obvious efficiency, our memory system is not perfect and memories themselves

do not preserve a Iiteral representation of the world. Consequently, memones are not always

accurate, and under some conditions, may be extremely distorted. Beginning with the

pioneering studies of Sir Fredenc Bartlett (1932), psychologists have been aware that

memory is a fundamentally constructive process that is sometimes prone to mors,

distortions, and illusions. Clearly, understanding memory distortion has many important

implications for everyday life. Recently, cognitive psychologists and neuropsychologists

have been especially concerned with constructive aspects of memory, in part as a result of

real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy

of memories recovered in psychotherapy. These debates raise issues that are relevant to

cognitive neuroscience. For example, how accurate is memory and under what conditions is

it subject to distortion? Ln recent yean, investigators have begun to explore the phenornena

that illuminate constructive aspects of remembering, such as source amnesia, false

recognition, fdse recall, and confabulation.

HISTORICAL BACKGROüND:

The first controlled studies that provided evidence of memory distortion were carried

out by European investigators who were interested in the reliability and suggestibility of

eyewitness testimony, particularly children's testimony. Binet (1 900), for instance, exposed

children to various objects and then tested their memories in the presence or absence of

misleading questions. Binet found that misleading questions produced systematic distortions

Page 7: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

in children's recollections. Stem (1 9 10) desmbed experiments in which the investigator

staged an event in front of a group or a class, and later asked them questions about what

happened and who âid it. Like Binet, Stem reported that misleading questions could induce

distorted memories, and he also observed that young children were apt to confuse real and

imagined events. Evidence concerning the unreliability of eyewitness memory was

summarized in a well-known monograph by psychologist Hugo Munsterbug (1 908).

In addition to these empiricai demonstrations of distorted memory, the late 19th- and

early 20th- centuries witnessed a number of relevant theoreticai developments. in

experimental psychology, the predominant account of memory distortion held that stored

memories fade, change, and alter spontaneously over time, although it was never specified

exactly how such alterations took place (e.g. Kennedy, 1 898). An alternative view was

advanced by the Geman biologist Richard Semon (Semon, lgO4/l92 1; l909/1923).

According to Semon, every act of encoding and storing new Uiformation (engraphy) involves

some retrievd of thoughts, images, and memories that are activated by the curent situation

(ecphory). T'us, a newly created memory trace or engram is not a literai replica of reality,

but is always an interpretation that includes retrieved information. If the input to the memory

system is not an accurate reflection of reality, then the output will necessarily be distorted.

Semon also argued that memory distortion arises because every act of ecphory constitutes an

act of engraphy. That is, when we remember a past experience, it is encoded anew hto the

memory system. However, we may focus on or tW about only certain aspects of the

retrieval experience, thereby changing its subsequent memory representation. Semon

dernonstrated great foresight in proposing these ideas, though they were generally ignored in

his day.

Page 8: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Freud developed a rather different and influential approach to memory distortion.

Freud's best known ideas about distortion are related to his still controversial notion of

repression, which holds that painful memories are defensively excluded nom consciousness.

Although Freud dehed the concept of repression differenty at different times, he

consistently argued that recollection of past experience could be distorted by dynamic and

defensive forces. Freud's discussions of the effctts of early sexual trauma on adult

psychopathology are also related to issues of rnernory distortion. In his early writings, Freud

(1 896) argued that repressed memories of sexual abuse and trauma cause persisting

pathological symptoms, and contended that the task of psychoanaiysis is to allow patients to

retrieve lost traumatic memories.

As is now well known, Freud soon abandoned this b'seduction theory" of early trauma

in favor of the view that the "mernories" that his patients retrieved were in fact fantasies or

confabulations. Whatever the reasonç for his change in view, it is clear that many aspects of

Freud's later psychoanalytic thought were built on the notion that early memories are highly

likely to be distortions that are produced by fantasy-based confabulations. This theme was

evident in Freud's (1 899) idea of "screen mernories", which holds that recollections of early

childhood experiences are often distortions that protect people nom a more unpleasant

reality that is hidden behind the superficial "screen" image that is presented to consciousness.

Clinical observations of rnernory distortion in brain-damaged patients were also made

during the turn-of-the-century period. ShortIy f ier Korsako ff s (1 889) initial description of

the amnesic syndrome that now bears his name, Bonhoeffer (1904) pointed out that such

patients often produce extensive and sometimes bizarre confabulations conceming events that

Page 9: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

never occurred. These memory distortions contmsted sharply with the patients' inability to

remember accurately even the simplest episodes îtom their day-to-day lives.

During the 1920s and 1 WOs, a number of experiments were conducted in

psychological laboratories that examined the hypothesis that memories change autonomously

over time in a manner dictated by Gestalt laws of organization. That is, memones change

spontaneously toward becoming "good Gestaltstf. For example, Wulf (1 922) reported that

memories of visual foms become more symmetrical with increasing time f i e r initial

leaming. KoBa (1 935) cited these autonomous transformation effects as support for his

Gestalt theory of memory. Others, however, argued that such effects are Iargely

experimental artifacts (e.g. Zangwill, 1937). Modem researchers have tended to agree with

this latter view, and have thus dismissed the Gestalt account of memory distortion (e.g.

Baddeley, 1976).

The single most important development concerning memory distortion during this

period was the publication of Bartlett's (1932) classic monograph Remembering. Bartlett

exposed his subjects to an engaging story, an old Indian legend entitled The Wur of the

Ghosts. After hearing the story subjects were asked to retell it on several occasions. Bartlett

found that people rarely recalled dl of the events in the story accurately; they often

remembered occurrences that made generd sense, or fit their expectations of what should

have happened, even though they were not in fact part of the original story. Bartlett also

observed that the recollectious of his participants changea somethnes substantially, across

multiple retelhgs of the story. Curiously, Bartlett's onginal results have not been replicated

by others and may have been attributable to the specific test instructions that he gave to his

subjecîs.

Page 10: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Nonetheless, on the basis of his observations Bartlett concluded that mernories are

imaginative reconstructions of past events that are heavily influenced by the rememberer's

preexisting knowledge structures or schemas. Bartlett argued that schemas influence what is

extracted fiom an expenence and determine how it is reconstructed. He argued M e r that

remembering is a hdamentally social activity that is inevitably distorted by the attitudes and

needs of the rememberer. Bartlett's (1932) contribution was an enduring one because he

distinguished between reproductive and recomtructive memory. Reproductive memory

refers to accurate rote production of material firom memory, whereas reconstructive memory

emphasizes the active process of f i lhg in missing elernents while remembering, with errors

fiequently occurring.

In the tradition of Bartlett's early ideas, it has generally been assumed that the act of

remembering materials rich in meaning gives rise to reconstructive processes (and therefore

emors), whereas the act of remembering simplified materials (e.g. nonsense syllables, word

lists) gives nse to reproductive (and thus accurate) memory. For this reason, the investigatoa

of fdse mernories have mostly followed Bartlett's (1932) lead. Most evidence has been

collected in paradigms that use sentences (Bransford & Franks, 1971; Brewer, 1977)- prose

passages (Sulin & Dooling, 1974), slide sequences (Loflus Miller, & Burns, 1978) or

videotapes (Loftus & Palmer, 1974). In al1 these paradigrns. evidence of false mernories has

been obtained, although the magnitude of the effect depends on the method of testing

(McCloskey & Zaragoza, 1985; Payne. Toglia, & Anastasi, 1994). The predominance of

materials that tell a story (or can be represented by a script or schema) can probably be

attributed to the belief that only such materials will cause fdse memones to occur.

Page 11: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

There were two very significant deviations from this general trend in research on fdse

mernories. Both used a List-leaming paradigm. Deese (1959), for instance, had subjects

study a list of words (e-g. bed, pillows, rest, sheets, etc.) that are strong associates of a

nonstudied critical item (e-g. sleep), and then asked for recall of the List words. He reported

that subjects often produced the nonstudied critical items on the recall test. That is, they

exhibited false memones for the nonstudied but strongiy associated items. Recent work

indicates that Deese's procedure is indeed a powerfd method for inducing subjectively

compeliing false recollections (McDermott, 1996; Roediger & McDermott, 1 995).

Underwood (1 965) reported a similar phenornenon in the domain of recognition

memory. He gave subjects a continuou recognition task in which they decided if each

presented word had been given previously in the list. Words appearing later in the list bore

various relations to previously shidied words. Underwood showed that words associatively

related to previously presented words were fdsely recognized. Underwood argued that these

false recognitions arise fiom "implicit associative responses7' that occur during initial study

of the Iist items.

In the neurological and neuropsychological literatures, scattered clinicd observations

of confabulation in Korsakoff s patients continued to appear, together with a variety of new

theoretical accounts of the phenornena (e.g. Talland, 196 1). For instance, Talland (1 96 1)

disfinguished between two major foms of memory distortion in Konakoff amnesics:

confabulation, where patients misremember the time and other contextual aspects of actual

events; and fabrication, where patients concoct improbable and bizarre scenarios that could

not have in fact occurred. In addition, Whitty and Lewin (1 957) reported that patients who

had undergone surgical removal of the anterior cingulate for relief of intractable obsessional

Page 12: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

neurosis exhibited spontaneous confabulations even though they did not exhibit amnesia,

thereby suggesting that the presence of severe mernory disorder is not a necessary condition

for the occurrence of confabulation. Other non-Korsakoff amnesic patients were described,

such as the famous case of H.M. (Scoville & Milner, 1957), who displayed a profound loss

of memory for recent events after bilateral resection of the media1 temporal lobes. H.M. did

not exhibit extensive confabulation, thus Uidicating that severe memory disorder is also not a

sufXcient condition for confabulation (McGlynn & Schacter, 1989; Moscovitch, 1995).

These kinds of observations suggest that confabulation is based on brain structures that are

dinerent fiom the structures that are damaged in cases of severe amnesia.

The 1970s witnessed a surge of interest in "cognitive constructivisrn". This was .

stirnulated in part by Neisser's (1967) constructive account of memory which rekindled

interest in Bartlett's ideas about schemas and reconstnictive memory. By his view, al1

mernories are constructions because they include generd knowledge that was not part of a

specific event, but is necessary to reconstruct it. It is this inherently reconstnictive nahire of

memory in turn that makes it susceptible to various kinds of distortions and inaccuracies.

As a result of this rediscovery of Bartlett's ideas, cognitive psychologists focused

intensively on investigating various kinds of memory distortions. For example, Sulin &

Dooling (1 974) examined e m n attributable to schema-based inferences. They gave subj ects

brief passages to read about a wild and unruiy girl; some were told that the passage was about

Helen Keller, and others were told that the passage concemed Carol Harris. Mer a week's

delay, the former group of subjects were much more likely than the latter to claim that they

had read the sentence "She was deaf, dumb and blind", even though the sentence had not

been presented in the initial passage.

Page 13: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

At around the same tixne, Loftus and her colleagues published a series of studies that

demonstrated that leadhg questions cm systematically alter rnemory reports (Loftus &

Palmer, 1974), and that post-event misinformation can alter memory for an original event

(e.g. Loftus, Miller & Bums, 1 978). In neuropsychology, new evidence pointed toward a

link between frontal-lobe dysfûuction and confabulation (Stuss, Alexander, Lieberman, &

Levine, 1978), and the theoretical implications of confabulation for theories of rnernory

began to be appreciated (Mercer, Wapner, Gardner, & Benson, 1977).

In the 1980s and l99Os, significant inroads were made in the shidy of source memory,

retrieval environment, and implicit memory, with important implications for reconstructive

memory. For example, source memory, also referred to as source monitoring (Johnson,

Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993) refers to processes that allow people to remember when, where

and how a memory was acquired. Interest in this issue was sparked by several developments.

Johnson and colleagues (198 1) reported an important series of experïments and ideas

conceming the processes involved in distinguishing between memones of actuai events and

memories of prior imaginings and fantasies ("reality monitoring"). Subsequent studies

delineated conditions under which recollections of extemal events and intemal imaginings

can be confused, thereby producing distorted memones (Johnson & Suengas, 1989). This

line of research also began to yield evidence that young children have difficulties

distinguishing between memories of real and imagined events (Johnson & Foley, 1984), a

finding that bas been confïrmed and extended in various recent studies (Ceci, 1995).

Neuropsychological research revealed that brain-damaged patients sometimes exhibit

a drarnatic phenornenon known as source amnesia Schacter, Harbluk, and McLachlan

(1984) reported that amnesic patients could occasionally leam new fictitious facts such as

Page 14: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

"Bob Hope's father was a fireman". However, even when they could recall a fact, the

patients were ofien unable to remember that the experimenter had just told it to them

minutes earlier. Mead, they fkequently invented a plausible source, claiming that they had

read the fact in a newspaper or heard about it on the radio. Importantly, amnesics who

showed signs of fiontal-lobe damage were more vulnerable to source amnesia than amnesics

who did not exhibit fkontal signs (Schacter et al., 1984; Shimamuta & Squire, 1987).

Subsequent research indicated that source amnesia could be observed in patients with

restricted fiontai-lobe lesions (Janowsky, Shimamura., & Squire, 1989). Source memory

deficits were also documented in elderly adults, particularly those with neuropsychologicai

signs of frontal impairment (Craik, Morris, Morrs, & Loewen, 1990; McIntyre & Craik,

1 987; Schacter, Kaszniak, Kihlstrom, & valdisem, 1 99 1 ), and in young c hildren (Lindsay,

Johnson, & Kwon, 199 1; Schacter, Kagan, & Leichtman, 1995).

Source amnesia became directly linked to memory distortion in several difTerent

ways. Neuropsychological studies suggested that failures of source memory and reality

monitoring are implicated in the confabulations of brain-damaged patients, and also provided

increasing evidence that fiontal-lobe pathology is often associated with confabulation

(DeLuca & Cicerone, 1991 ; Johnson, 199 1; Moscovitch, 1995; Moscovitch & Melo, 1997).

Cognitive research revealed that failures of source memory play a key role in the kind of

post-event misinfoxmation effects that were initially studied by Loftus and colleagues (Belli,

Lindsay, Gales, & McCarthy, 1994; Lindsay, 1990; Zaragoza & Lane, 1994). These

experiments indicated that when people witness a particular event, and are later given

misleadhg information about it, they often fail to remember whether the critical information

was part of the original event or was ody suggested to them later. In light of other

Page 15: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

experiments showing that post-event information does not necessarily elirninate the original

memory (McCloskey & Zaragoza, 1 WS), it is now clear that failures of source memory are a

major contributor to memory distortions that are produced by post-event misinformation.

Ceci (1 995) describes recent developmentai evidence indicating that distorting influences of

post-event suggestion in young children are closely Linked with deficient source memory.

Another important line of research revealed that when people forget the source of

their knowledge, they become susceptible to various 0 t h kinds of memory distortions and

illusions. For instance, Jacoby, Kelley, Brown & Jasechko (1989) found that people who had

been exposed to a nonfamous name would later cal1 that name "famous" if they were tested

after a long delay and no longer recollected that they had seen the name in a previous

experimental session. The name may have seemed like a familiar one when it was exposed a

second tirne, but because people failed to recollect the source of their knowledge they

mistakenly attributed the name's familiarity to the "fame" of the nonfamous person. This

resuit highlights that people often make Ùiferences and attributions concerning the source of

retrieved knowledge, and that these source attributions are quite prone to error (Kelley &

Jacoby, 1990). This observation provides a link to the various phenomena of implicit

memory that were investigated extensively during the 1980s, such as priming and ski11

learning (Roediger & McDemott, 1993; Schacter, 1987; Schacter, Chiu, & Ochsner, 1993).

Irnplicit memory phenomena, by definition, involve loss of conscious access to source

information. Isolated images and feelings that may manifest as priming effects or related

phenomena could provide a basis for inaccurate reconstruction of past events when people

attempt to make attributions about the source of unexplained images and feelings (Kelley &

Jacoby, 1990; Squire, 1995; Schacter, 1996).

Page 16: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

There has also been a growing body of laboratory research on memory for

emotionally-arousing events, which indicates that emotional arousal typically enhances the

accuracy of memory for the central aspects of an event and impairs memory for more

peripheral details (Heuer & Risberg, 1992). Some emotionally cornpelling "flashbulb

memones" can even be subject to outright distortion (Neisser & Harsch, 1992). In addition,

research on mood and memory during the 1980s began to reveal that a person's mood can

exer& biasing effects on memory, such that information that is incongruent with a current

mood tends to be well remembered, and information that is incongruent with a current mood

tends to be more poorly remembered (Bower, 1992). These effects have been observed in

college students and also in patients suffering from depression and other affective disorders

(Mineka & Nugent, 1995).

Fuially, research conducted during the 1980s and 1990s has supported the idea that

memory consists of a variety of dissociable systems and subsystems (Cohen & Eichenbaurn,

1993; Eichenbaum, 1997; Gabrielli, 1995, Moscovitch 1994; Schacter & Tulving, 1 994;

Squire, 1992; Vargha-Khadern, 1997). This perspective has encouraged the view that

memones are constructed on the basis of stored hgments of experience that are distributed

throughout a variety of cortical systems that work cooperatively with cortical storage areas

during both encoding and reirieval (Damasio, 1 989; McClelland, McNaughton, & O ' Reill y,

1995; Schacter, 1996; Squire, 1992). From this perspective, memory distortions are a nahual

by-product of constructive activities that draw on multiple components and subsystems

(McClelland, 1995; Squire, 1995).

In many areas of memory research, neuropsychological study of amnesic patients has

had a large impact on theorking about nomial memory. Nevertheless, despite a centwy's

Page 17: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

worth of psyc hological research conceming memory distortions and illusions (Schacter,

1995a) and scattered observations of confabdations and reIated false memories in brain-

damaged patients (Johnson, 199 1 ; Johnson, O'Connor, & Cantor, 1997; Moscovitch, 1995;

Moscovitch & Melo, 1997) there has been Little progress in our theoretical understanding of

memory illusions. Widening the scope of research on memory illusions to include extensive

neuropsychological study of various patient populations would provide usefhl information

about the neural and cognitive processes underlying illusory memories.

One of the few attempts at examining memory distortion in amnesic patients is found

in a study of fdse recognition reported by Cermak, Butters, and Gerrein(1973). Cermak et

al. used the false recognition paradigm developeci by Underwood (1965). They found that

when Korsakoff amnesics and controls encountered the four different types of lures:

unrelated, associates, synonyms, and homophones, amnesic patients produced significantly

more false alarms to associates and homophones than did control subjects, and they also

showed a slight, nonsignificant trend for false alarms to synonyms and unrelated words.

These data suggest that amnesic patients are sometimes more prone to fdse d a m s than are

nonamnesic controls. Other studies of recognition memory have shown that amnesics

sometimes exhibit a higher false alarm rate than control subjects even to nonstudied words

that have no particular relation to studied words (Knowlton & Squire, 1995; Verfaellie &

Treadwell, 1993). Together with their reduced hit rates, the elevated false alann rates of

amnesic patients in the latter studies may reflect an inability to discriminate between studied

and nonstudied items resulting in haphazard guessing.

The available evidence suggesting that amnaic patients sometimes make more false

positive responses than control subjects raises the possibility that they rnight be unusually

Page 18: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

susceptible to memory illusions that are expressed by fdse alarms to noastudied items.

Reinitz, Verfaellie, and Milberg (1996) report that arnnesic patients are more prone than

controls to fdse dams based on illusory rnemory conjunctions, where subjects claim to have

seen a new stimulus when in fact they have only seen its component features (Reinitz,

Lammers, & Cochran, 1992). Kroll, Knight, Metcalfe, WoK, and TuIWig (1996) report

similar hdings in patients with left, right, or bilateral hippocampal damage. These effects

may reflect irnpaired consolidation processes in arnnesic patients, processes that ordinarily

serve to bind together distinct attributes of an event into a unified engram.

Yet, the apparent susceptibility of amnesic patients to produce more false memones

cannot be applied to al1 experimentd situations. For example, Schacter, Verfaellie &

Pradere found that amnesic subjects were far less susceptible to false recognition of critical

lures than were controls, whereas they made more fdse alarms to unrelated lures than control

subjects did. They used a version of the Deese (1959) paradigm described earlier, recently

revived and modified by Roediger & McDermott (1995). Roediger & McDermott (1 995)

replicated the false recall effect initially reported by Deese, and extended it to recognition

memory. Mer studying associates of nonpresented target words such as sweet, participants

fiequently claimed with high confidence that sweet had appeared previously on the study list.

Moreover, when asked to make rememberhow judgments (Tulving, 1985) indicating

whether they rnaintained a specific recollection of having studied a word during list

presentation ("remember"), or thought that a word was on the List because it seemed farniliar

('laiow"), people claimed to "remembef' nonpresented associates nearly as often as they

claimed to "remember" words that had actually appeared on the List (Roediger & McDermott,

1995).

Page 19: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Using this paradigm, Schacter, Verfaellie, and Pradere (1996) reported that both

Korsakoff and non-Korsakoff (Le. medial temporal lobe) amnesic patients showed reduced

levels of vendical recognition memory compared to a matched control group. Amnesics

attained fewer hits than controls to previously presented words and made more false alarms

than controls to nonpresented words that bore no associative relationship to previously

presented words. More importantiy, amnesic patients made fewer false alarms than controls

to nonpresented associates such as nueet.

This latter finding suggests that false recognition of nonpresented associates depends

on retention of associative or semantic information that also supports veridical recognition of

presented words - idormation that Schacter and colleagues suggest is not available to

amnesic patients. More specifically, they contend that control subjects bind together shidied

items and generated associates, thereby fomiing and retaining a well-organized, focused

representation of the semantic gist of the study list. Related test distractors that match this

semantic gist representation, such as sweet, are likely to be falsely recognized; unrelated

distractors that do not match it are likely to be correctly rejected. They suggest M e r that

amnesic patients do not form andor retain a focused semantic representation of gist. On the

one hand, this idea accounts for the finding that amnesic patients showed reduced false

recognition of related distractors: there is Little opportunity for a related test item to match a

semantic gist representation. On the other hand, this suggestion can also account for

amnesics' infiated false recognition of unrelated distractors: whereas control subjects can use

their semantic gist representation to reject unrelated distractors that do not match it, m e s i c

patients are less able to do so. Encoding andor retrieval of semantic gist information

presumably depends on medial temporal lobe structures that are damaged in amnesia.

Page 20: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Schacter and colleagues (1996) suggest that when numerous associates of a fdse target are

presented, as ui the Roediger and McDamott/Deese paradigm, nonamnesic controls establish

a well-organized gist representation that enhances the sense of familiarity or recollection

associated with a matching false target, and thus increases the magnitude of the false

recognition effect. Amnesic patients, by contrast, encode or retain less gist information and

hence show reduced levels of fdse recognition.

However, when only a single related item precedes a fdse target, as in the

Underwood pmdigm, nonamnesic controls establish a less robust gist representation and can

use their intact explicit memory abilities to counteract or oppose (Jacoby, 1991) the sense of

familiarity or recollection they may expenence when encountering a false target. Thus, for

example, a nonamnesic individual who encounters the false target table, and can recollect

having previously shidied the associate chair, cm use this information to avoid making a

fdse recognition response (Brainerd et al., 1995). Amnesics, however, are less able to use

recollection to oppose the sense of familiarity engendered by a fdse target, and thus exhibit

increased levels of false recognition compared to nonamnesic controls.

The studies of false memory in the neuropsychological patients just described have

focused on those with damage to medid temporal lobe structures including the hippocampus.

The difference in production of false memories in the two types of paradigms described

would suggest that these structures are critical for the processes underlying false memory in

the DeeseRoediger and McDermott (1959/1995) task, namely remembering semantic or

associative information,

In a PET study with normal control subject, Schacter et al. (1996) made an important

observation. They found that the medial temporal lobe was activated as much during

Page 21: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

recognition of fdse targets as true targets, suggesting that "faulty" output h m the mediai

temporal lobe is a conûibuting factor to fdse memory. This suggests that because output

from the medid temporal lobe is not perfect even in normal people, memory distortions or

false memories are necessarily a cornmon feature of normal memory. However, it appears

that other false mernory phenornena may not be dependent on these particular structures.

A number of neuropsycholgists have noted that damage to the ventromedial aspect of

the fiontal-lobes and basal forebrain are O ften associated with the most flagrant fonn of

memory distortion, namely confabulation (Dalla Barba, 1993; Johnson, 199 1, Johnson et al.,

1997; Moscovitch, 1995, Moscovitch & Melo, 1997). Moscovitch and his colleagues have

proposed that the prefiontal cortex is necessary for strategic search, monitoring, venfication

and organization of the automatic output nom medial temporal lobe structures.

Confabulation, therefore, arises as a result of impairment to strategic retrievai and monitoring

processes that depend on prefrontal regions (Moscovitch, 1995; Moscovitch & Melo, 1997).

Recently, the frontal-lobes have been implicated in other false memory phenomena

such as fdse recognition. Schacter and colleagues(l996) have recently studied a 65-year-old

man, BG, who suffered an infarction restricted to the right fiontal-lobe (Schacter, Curran,

Galluccio, Milberg, & Bates, 1996). From their description of this case study, it was clear

that BG does not spontaneously generate extensive confabulations, shows no signs of

amnesia, and is generally alert, attentive, and cooperative. He does, however, exhibit a

striking pattern of fdse recognitions that provides usefûl clues concerning the role of

prefiontai regions in illusory memories. Parkin and colleagues (1 996) have reported a patient

with left eontal-lobe darnage who makes excessive numbers of false alarms and resembles

patient BG in many respects. Schacter et al. (1997) also found that normal old people, whose

Page 22: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

medial temporal lobe and prehntal structures tend to be compromise& exhibited an

exaggerated false memory effect. Taken together, these £indings support the idea that

memory distortion is exaggerated when output from the mediai temporal lobes is faulty and

monitoring by the prefrontal cortex is irnpaired, as is likely in the case of the elderly. When

few, if any, of the associates ( m e targets) are remembered, as in the case of arnnesia, there

effectively is no rnedid temporal lobe output that can be prone to distortion.

It appears then that a variety of distinct neural and psychological processes underlie

different kinds of memory illusions and these are not yet fùlly understood. The broader

question that emerges from these fuidings concenis the monitoring huiction of the prefiontal

cortex in producing memory distortions. Because the DeeseMcDermott and Roediger

(195911995) paradigm induces faulty output fkom the medial temporal lobe even in normal

people, it might be the case that poor monitoring has no role in producing fdse memories in

this laboratory procedure. On the other hand, if poor monitoring contributes to false

memories, prefiontal cortex patients should show increased levels of fdse memories in this

paradigm. This prediction is appropriate because an exaggerated production of false recall

and false recognition in the DeeselRoediger and McDermoa (1 959/1994) paradigm appears

to be the product of three cognitive processes: 1) having some memory for the shidy lists, 2)

being able to extract the semantic/associative gist of the lists, and 3) having a deficit in

monitoring that makes decisions about specific items in a given Iist prone to error. The tint

two processes are subserved by medial temporal lobe structures and the third is dependent on

the prefiontal cortex. These criteria are met by patients with damage restricted to the frontal-

lobes so they should show elevated levels of false memory. The purpose of the present study

is to elucidate the relationship between prefiontal cortex function and memory distortion.

Page 23: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

To address this question we administered the false rnemory paradigm, as described by

Schacter, Verfaellie & Pradere (1996) to three groups of patients, namely non-confabulating

amnesics with damage to the medial temporal lobe or diencephalic structures, confabulating

amnesics who had medial temporal lobe damage but also had additionai darnage to the

ventromedial prebntal regions, and non-confabulating, non-amnesic patients with damage

restricted to the prefkontal cortex which did not include the ventrornedial prefiontal regions.

It was predicted that if the above three criteria are necessary for increased production of fdse

recall and false recognition than it should be possible to replicate Schacter et ai.'s (1996)

finding that amnesic patients, with damage restricted to mediai temporal lobe structures,

should produce significantly less false recall and recognition than normal controls and that

non-amnesic &ontal-lobe patients should produce more fdse recall and false recognition than

normal controls. We have included this other, very interesting subgroup of amnesics, those

who have additional damage to the ventromedial aspect of the prefiontal cortex because on

the one hand, they have a deficit in monitoring which probably leads to their propensity to

confabulate, but on the other hand they have very severe memory deficits which will impair

their ability to remember the Lists and extract the semantic gist. Thus, we would predict that,

in the Deese/Roediger & McDermott paradigm, any propensity they may have towards

memory distortion (i.e. false recall and false recognition) will be counteracted by their severe

amnesia.

METEOD

Participants.

Page 24: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

The participants in this shidy were 12 people with brain damage (5 fernales, 7 males).

Seven of these people were m e s i c patients with various etiologies. Three amnesics were

classified as confabulators and 4 as non-confabulators, depending on their behaviour in real

life and on their performance on a test that elicited confabulations (see Moscovitch & Melo,

1997). Lesion localization was not as precise as one would have iiked for this group of

patients. Not al1 patients had focal lesions, in some cases the lesion was diffise. The darnage

in al1 3 of the patients who were classified as confabulators was caused by some form of

cerebral vascular accident. Al1 3 of these patients had confïrmed lesions in the ventromedial

aspect of the prefiontal cortex. This darnage is consistent with that reported by other

investigators to be associateci with confabulation (De Luca & Diamond, 1995; Fisher,

Alexander, Dy Esposito, & Otto, 1995; Moscovitch, 1995; Moscovitch & Melo, 1997). These

patients also had evidence of additional damage to diencephalic or medial temporal-lobe

structures. Because the non-confabulating amnesics ail had darnage to diencephalic or medial

temporal-lobe structures (but not to the ventromedial fiontal cortex), it is unlikely that

damage to these structures leads to confabulation, though it does produce severe memory loss

which may be a prerequisite for confabulation (Moscovitch & Melo, 1997). The average age

of the confabulating amnesics was 60, and their mean education level was 11.7 years. The

nonconfabulating amnesics were slightly younger and more educated than the confabulating

amnesics; they had a mean age of 50.5, and an average of 1 6.5 years of education. The

intellechial functioning of the entire group of amnesics was in the average range, as indicated

by a mean Full Scale IQ of 92.3 on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised. In

contrast, they consistently exhibited severe deficits on a variew of explicit memory tasks

Page 25: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

fkom the Wechsler Memory Scale - Revised. They obtained a mean General Memory Index

of 69 and a mean Delayed Memory Index of 53.

A second group of participants consisted of 5 people who had focal lesions restricted

to the fiontal-lobes but which did not include the ventromedid region. Their memory was

not severety impaired and they were not classified as confabulators. The average age of

these patients was 57.4, and their education level was on average 15.2 years. The overall

intellectual functioning of these patients was in the average range. They did not exhibit

severe memory impairment.

Table 1 shows, for each subject, the etiology, probable or confïrmed site of brain

damage as detemiined by MRI, CAT or SPECT scans, and demographic data

Control subjects consisted of eight individuals (4 males, 4 fernales) with an average

age of 54.5 and a mean education level of 13 years.

Page 26: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

fable 1: Demographic data on Patients

I.D. Sex Age du. ~tiology Lesion Location

Nonlconfabulating Amnesics DA male 45 17 Herpes encephalitis bilat. MTL KC male 45 15 Traumatic brain injury bilat. MTL, R-fronto-par, occ CC male 60 22 Astrocytorna and hydroceph, 3rd vent., diencephalon PT female 52 14 R-Temporal artery R-TL

malformation Confabulating Amnesics FE female 60 15 ACommA aneurysm clipped L-orbitofrontal and dorsolateral frontal RC male 61 10 CVA bilat. orbitofronbl incl. VMF region GE male 59 10 CVA bilat. frontal including bilat VMF region

Frontal Patients CB fernale 29 12 EpilepsyResection Right dorsolateral frontal DW male 59 13 Brain Tumour Right frontal PK female 35 16 Traumatic brain injury Right frontal ED female 65 7 ACornrnA aneurysm clipped Bilateral frontal DS male 60 16 ACommA aneurysm clipped R-lnferior medial frontal

Page 27: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Materials

The materiais consisted of 24 Lists of 16 words, identical to those used by Roediger

and McDermott (1995). Each list contains 15 words to be presented for study and a target

word (critical lue) that is not presented for study. The study words are dl highly associated

to the critical lure and are ordered such that the strongest associates occur fint in the list. In

order to counterbalance the List order, the 24 lists were subdivided into three sets of 8 lists.

The lists were presented auditorily via a desktop computer equipped with Soundblaster

software.

Design and procedure

Because one purpose of this study was to replicate Schacter, Verfaellie & Pradere's

(1996) hdings with amnesic patients, ail participants were tested in the two conditions used

in that experiment (Le. Study + R e d 1 condition and a Study + Arithmetic condition,

admuiistered in two sessions separated by at least one week). Half of the subjects received

the Study + Recall condition during the h t session and the Study + Arithmetic condition

during the second session. For the other half of the participants, this order was revened.

In each condition a set of eight lists was used. The remaining eight tists were not

studied. Four of these appeared on the recognition test that accornpanied the Study + Recall

condition, whereas the other four appeared on the recognition test that accompand the Study

+ Arithmetic condition. Lists were counterbalanced so that they were used equally often in

the Study + Recall condition, in the Study + Arithmetic condition, and as nonstudied lists.

Nonstudied lists were counterbalanced across the two recognition tests. Before presentation

of each study kt, the subjects were told that they would hear several lists of words Ma

speakers attached to a PC and that they should try and remember the words. The words were

Page 28: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

recorded in a fernale voice and were presented at a rate of one word every 1.5 seconds.

Immediately following presentation of a study list, participants were asked to Say out Ioud as

many of the words as they couid remember (Study + Recall condition) or to perform simple

addition and multiplication problems (Study + Anthmetic condition). Approximately 1

minute was given for the completion of either of these tasks, d e r which the next study list

was presented. Eight shidy lists were presented duMg a single session. Presentation of al1

eight study lists took approximately 20 minutes.

The recognition test was adrninistered approximately 2 minutes d e r completion of

the recail or arithrnetic task that followed the final study list. The recognition test contained

48 words, 24 studied words and 24 nonstudied words. The studied words were obtained by

selectùig for each of the eight study lists the items in serial positions 1,8, and 10. The

nonstudied words consisted of the cntical lues corresponding to each of the eight studied

lists, the critical lures corresponding to four nonstudied lists, and the items in serial positions

1,8, and 10 of these nonstudied lists. The words were presented al1 at once in columns on a

cornputer screen.

Subjects were asked to indicate for each word whether they had heard it earlier in any

of the Lists. They were instxucted to move down the columns using the cursor key and to

select @y pressing a different key- the space bar) only those words they had heard

previously. For each of the words they selected, they were then asked to indicate whether

they remembered or knew the word by typing an "R" (remember) or a 'X" (know) beside the

word. Instructions for the remember/lmow judgment were simila. to those used by Roediger

and McDermott (1995) and Schacter, Verfaellie & Pradere (1996). Participants were told

that a remember judgment should be made if they can specificaiiy recollect hearing a word

Page 29: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

via the speakers- that is, they cm actuaily travel back in their min& to the precise moment of

hearing and studying the word and remember something about that event. It was explained

that such recollection might include remembering something about the speaker's voice or

about the thoughts they had when they heard the word. They were told that a know judgment

should be made if they feel or know that a word was presented earlier on the tape recorder,

but if they cannot recollect anything specific about the word or its occurrence.

RESULTS:

Free RecaII. The mean proportion of study list words and critical lues produced by the three

patient groups and control subjects on the free recall test averaged across the eight target lists.

is presented in Table 2. Due to the relatively small nurnber of subjects in each of the patient

groups, nonparametric analyses were conducteci. As expected both groups of amnesic

patients (nonconfàbulators and confabulators) recalled on average a much smaller proportion

of shidy list words (-29 and .2 1 respectively) than did controls (.49), Z = 2.3 18, pc.05

(nonconfabulators); 2 = 2.3 1 1, pc.05 (confabulaton). The difference between the proportion

of shidy list words recalled by nonconfabulating amnesics was not significantly different

h m that recaüed by confabulatïng amnesics, Z = 1.427, pc.5. Although the proportion of

study list words recalled by control subjects was higher than the proportion of shidy list

words recalled by bntal patients (.38) this difference was not significant, Z = 1.8, p<l .

There was also no significant difference between the proportion of study list words recalled

by hnta l patients and that recalled by nonconfabuating amnesics, Z = 1.246, p<S. The

difference between the proportion of snidy lia words recalled by frontal patients and that

Rcalled by confâbulatiug amnesic patients approached signïfîcance, Z = 1.938, FM.

Page 30: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Contrary to our expectations, nonconfabulating arnnesic patients intmded a larger

proportion of criticai lues (S0) than did controls (.38), although this difference did not attain

significance, Z = .63 1, pc 1. In Schacter et ai.'s study, amnesics produced about the same

proportion of study list targets and critical lures (.27 vs .29), whereas control subjects

produced significantly more study list targets than critical lures (.52 vs .33). Although the

control subjects produced significantly more study list targets than critical lures (-49 vs .38),

the proportion of study list wordç recailed by the nonconfabulating amnesic patients was

considerably, though nonsignificantly, lower than the proportion of critical lures intruded

(.29 vs -50). In contrast, confabulating amnesics intruded a nonsignificantly smaller

proportion of critical Iures (-21) than did controls (.39), Z = 1.713, pc .S. Also the

proportion of critical lues intruded by confabulating amnesics was not significantly different

fkom that produced by nonconfabulating amnesics. Interestingly, the pattern of performance

for the confabuiating amnesics was similar to the performance of the amnesic patients in

Schacter et al.'s (1996) study. That is, the confabulating amnesics produced the same

proportion of study list targets and cntical lures (.2 1 vs .2 1). The fkontal patients showed a

sirnilar pattern of performance as the nonconfabulating amnesics. They produced a larger

proportion of cntical lures (.45) than did controls (.3 9), although this difference was

nonsignificant, Z = .279, pc 1. They also produced a smaller proportion of study list words

than critical lures (.38 vs .45). The proportion of critical lures intnided by fiontal patients

was not significantly different from nonconfabulating amnesics. However, it differed

significantly fkom confabuiating amnesics, Z = 2.037, pC.05. Although they are inconclusive

the preceding nonparametric analyses provide some evidence for a difference in the relation

between critical lure intrusions and recail of target items in the patients and controls.

Page 31: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

However, this difference, at least with respect to the nonconfabulating amnesic patients is not

in the predicted direction based on Schacter et al.3 (1996) findings. Noaconfabulating

amnesic patients in this study did not produce a significantly smaiIer proportion of critical

lures than either control subjects or fiontal patients. Figure 1 depicts the relation of critical

lure intrusions and recall of target items for each group

Following Schacter et al. (1996), the intrusions of critical lures were compared to the

mean proportion of words recalied fiom the nonrecency and nonprirnacy regions of the serial

position curve (Le. positions 4-1 1). These data are presented in Table 3. For control

subjects, the only group in the previous analyses whose overall recall rate was higher than the

proportion of cntical lures intruded, the proportion of studied words recalled fkom these

positions (.37) was now about equal to the proportion of lues intruded (.38). A similar

pattern, though more pronounced, was found in al1 three patient groups. For

nonconfabulating and confabulating amnesics, the proportion of words recalled fkom the

middle positions of the senal position curve (. 18 and .10 respectively) was nonsignificantly

smaller than the proportion of cntical lures uitruded (S0, .2 1 respectively), Z = 1.46 1, p=. 14

(nonconfabulating amnesics); Z = 1.069. p =.29 (confabulating amnesics). These fïndings do

not support the notion that the relation between recall of studied words and intrusion of

critical lues differs in amnesic patients and control subjects. The proportion of study List

words recaiied fiom serial position 4-1 1 (.25) was significantly lower than the proportion of

critical lures intruded (.45) by fiontal patients, Z = 2.023, pc.05. This suggests that frontal

patients are more Likely than control subjects to produce fdse recall of critical lures.

Page 32: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Table 2: Mean proportion of study list words and critical Iures prodnced by patients and control subjects.

Group Mean Proportion of Stady Meao Proportion of Critical List Words Produced Lures Intruded

Nonconfabuiating Amndcs 0.29 0.50

Confabulating Amnesics

Frontal Patients

Control Subjects 0.49 028

Page 33: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Figure 1: Mean proportion of stndy üst words compared to critical lures intruded by patients and controls

nanamfab. canfab. fron ta1 contrds

arnnesics amnesics patients subjects

group

i ustudied items 1

; critical lures

Page 34: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Table 3: Mean proportion of study Iist words reeded fiom the nonrecency and nonprimacy regions of the senal position curve (positions 4-1 1) and cnticai Iures produced by patients

and controI subjects.

Group Mean proportion of studied words Mean proportion of critical from serial positions 4-1 1 lures inbudeci

Nonconfabulating Amnesics 0.18 050

Frontal Patients

Control Subjects 0.37 038

Page 35: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

The fïndings by Schacter et ai. (1996) suggested that amnesics malie on average many

more noncritical lure intrusions than control subjects. Furthemore, the nature of these

intrusions is somewhat different for the two groups. Specifically, while the two groups were

equally likely to produce unrelated intrusions, amnesic patients were less likely than controls

to produce intrusions related to the just studied List, and correspondingly more Iikely to

produce perseverations to previously studied lists. This pattern of intrusions did not hold for

the patients in the present shidy. Aside from one nonconfabuiating amnesic patient who

produced a tremendous number of noncritical lure intrusions (38 in total), amnesic patients in

general produced very few noncritical lure intrusions. In fact when the one

nonconfabulating amnesic patient who produced such a large number of intrusions was

removed, the nonconfabulating amnesics produced an average of -292 noncritical intrusions

per list and confabulators produced an average of -125 noncritical intrusions per list. This is

in cornparison to control subjects who produced .472 noncritical intrusions per list.

Furthemore, with respect to the nature of these intrusions, there were no apparent differences

between controls and amnesics in tems of the type of intrusions made. Frontal patients also

made relatively few noncritical intrusions (.275/List) and the nature of these intrusions did not

differ fiom the other groups.

If cntical lure intrusions arise f?om associative processes operating during recall itself

than one might expect more critical lure intrusions when subjects recall many targets from a

list than when they recall few targets. Schacter et al. (1996) addressed this issue which was

originally raiseci by Roediger & McDmott (1995). They found that amnesic patients

recalied more target items from lists for which they produced the critical lure than fkom lists

for which they did not produce the critical lure. Control subjects in their expriment showed

Page 36: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

comparable levels of target recall whether they produced the critical lure or not. In the

present experirnent control subjects, showed comparable levels of target recall whether they

produced the critical lure (mean 7.1) or not (mean = 7.8). However, it was not the case that

either nonconfabulating amnesics or confabulating arnnesics produced more target items

from lists for which they produced the critical lure (mean=4.0 for nonconfabulators,

mean=3.0 for confabulators) than f?om lists for which they did not produce the critical lure

(mean=4.0 for nonconfabulaton, mean=3.2 for confabulators). Rather, like contro ls subj ects,

amnesic patients, regardless of whether they were confabulators or not produced comparable

levels of target recail regardless of whether they produced the critical lure or not. This

pattern was also true of the fiontal patients whose mean recall was 5.2 when they produced

the critical lure and 5.7 when they did not. This analysis, although not conclusive due to the

mal1 number of patients in each group, does not appear to support the suggestion that

associative processes during the recall test play a more prominent role in the cntical lure

intrusions of amnesic patients than control subjects or fiontal patients.

To examine this issue fùrther, we followed Schacter et al. ( 1 996) once again and

examined the output position of the critical lure. It was reasoned that a relatively late output

position for the critical lure would tend to indicate a role for associatively related items

produced previously during the recali test. For nonconfabulating amnesics, the average

output position for critical lures was 4.46 (of 4-02 words produced for lists in which there

was a critical lure intrusion). For nonconfabulating amnesics, the average output position for

critical lues was 3.5 (of 3 words produced for lists in which there was a criticai lure

intrusion). For fiontal patients, the mean output position was 5 and the average number of

items produced for these lists was 5.2. For controls, the average output position for criticai

Page 37: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

lures was 5.45, but the average number of target items produced for these lis& was 6.8 1.

Thus, in relative terms, and in support of a the finding by Schacter et al., al1 three patient

groups produced the critical lure later than did the control subjects. This Gnding appears to

support the idea that in control subjects the intrusion of critical lures is less dependent on

associative processes during recall than it is in amnesic and fiontal patients.

Recognition. Table 4 presents the proportion of old responses to studied words, critical

Lures, and their corresponding distractors for the three patient groups and control subjects. As

already mentioned, subjects were tested on two separate occasions. On one occasion they

were asked to engage in fiee recall prior to the recognition test and on the other they were

asked to do simple arithmetic questions prior to the recognition test. As in Schacter et ale's

(1996) study it was found that type of task (fkee recall vs. ax-ithmetic) did not yield any main

effects or interactions, so all analyses are collapsed across the fiee recall and anthmetic

conditions.

Analyses of studied words and corresponding distractors revealed a significantly

higher hit rate in control subjects (-70) than in nonconfabulating amnesic patients (.47), Z =

2.224, pC.05. Frontal patients also produced a significantly higher hit rate (.75) than

nonconfabulating amnesics, Z = 2.183, pc.05. Although control subjects and frontal patients

produced higher hit rates than confabulating arnnesics (.66), these differences did not attain

significance. There was a significantly higher false alarm rate in the nonconfabulating

amnesics (-54) than in control subjects (. 13), Z = 3.154, p=.O 1. Similarly, confabulating

amnesics showed significantly higher levels of false alarms (-43) than the control subjects

(.13), Z = 3.059, pC.01. Frontal patients produced significantly lower Ievels of false alarms

(. 1 8) than either nonconfabulating amnesics (2 = 2.6 12, pc.0 1) or confabulating amnesics (2

Page 38: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

= 2.735, pC.0 1); but there was no significant ciifference between the false aiarrn rate of the

frontal patients and the control subjects.

In correcting for standard high-threshold, the false alarm rate was subtracted fiom the

hit rate. This analysis revealed that recognition accuracy in nonconfabulating amnesic

patients (-20) was significantly lower than in control subjects (.65), (Z = 3.719, pc.0001) or

fiontal patients (.67), (2 = 3.339, pC.005). Once again control subjects and fiontal patients

had proportiondly higher recognition accuracy than confabulating amnesics (.45), however,

these differences were not significant .

False alarms rates to critical lures (nonpresented words preceded by a list of high

associates) were compared to fdse alam rates for their correspondhg distractors (words

drawn from the same pool of critical lues that were not preceded by a list of high associates).

ûverall, nonconfabulating amnesic patients made fewer fake alarms to critical lues (.66)

than did control subjects (.83) but this difference did not achieve significance. However,

nonconfabulating patients made significantiy fewer false alarms to critical lures than did

fiontal patients (.93), 2=2.099, pC.01. The false alarm rate of confabulating amnesics to

critical lues (.65) was proportionally lower than those of control subjects and fiontal

patients, however this difference was not significant. Nonconfabulating amnesics made more

false d a m s to the distracton for critical lures (S0) than did control subjects (. 13), Z = 2.797,

p<0.01; and &ontal patients (.25) although the difference between these amnesics and &ontal

patients only approached significance (Z = 1.729, pc.09). Sirnilarly, confabulating amnesics

made more fdse alamis to the distractors for critical lures (.70) than did controi subjects, Z =

Page 39: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Table 4: Recognition data (mean proportions) for studied and target words and studied and nonstiidied critical lures in patients and controls

Group Studied Criticaï Distractors for Distractors for items Iures studied items criticai lures

Nonconfab. 0.47 0.66 0.54 0.50 Amnesics

Confabuiating 0.66 Amnesics

Frontal Patients

Control Subjects

Page 40: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

3.55 1, pc.000 1 ; and fiontal patients Z = 2.874, p<.0 1. The difference in proportions of fdse

alarms to the distracton for critical lures for control subjects and fiontal patients was not

significant

These analyses provide evidence for the fact that amnesic patients and control

subjects respond in a qualitatively different mamer to the critical lures and their distractors.

It also shows that amnesic patients and fiontal patients differ in their response to critical lures

and their distractors.

Although both groups of amnesic patients made more fdse alarxns to distractors than

did control subjects or fiontal patients, they made fewer false alarms to d i c a l lures than did

control subjects or fiontal patients. When the proportion of old responses to distractors for

critical lures was subtracted fiom the proportion of old responses to critical lures, the

corrected proportion was significantly smaller in both nonconfabulating (.40) and

confabulating amnesics (-29) than in either control subjects (.75), Z = 2.573,

p=.Ol (nonconfabulators), Z = 2.56 1, p=.Ol (confabulators); or fkontal patients (.87), Z =

2.368, pC.01 (nonconfabulators), Z = 2.367, pc.0 1 (confabulators). Schacter et al. (1996)

proposed that if subjects make significantly more old responses to critical lures than to

distractors, this may suggest that the presentation of associatively related items reliably

influenced their recognition performance. In the present study it was certainly the case that

control subjects produced significantly more old responses to critical lures than to distracton,

Z = 2.120, p<.0001; as did &ontal patients, Z = 2.820, pc.005. Nonconfabulating amnesics

also showed this efféct, Z = 2.120, pc.05; whereas confabulating amnesics did not produce

signincantly more old responses to critical lures than to distractors.

Page 41: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

A cornpaison of studied items and critical Iures revealed that nonconfabulating

amnesics, fiontal patients, and control subjects made more old responses to critical lues than

to studied items (.66 vs .47 for nonconfabulating amnesics, .93 vs -75 for fiontal patients and

-83 vs .70 for control subjects). This was a hding also made by Roediger & McDermott,

1995 and Schacter et al. (1996).

Remember vs h o w responses. We subdivided the proportion of old responses made by

amnesic patients and controls into remember and know responses, respectively

Roediger and McDermott (1995) found that subjects claimed to remember critical Iures as

often as they claimed to remember studied words, and our data show a sirnilar pattern for

both control subjects andpatients. Overall, control subjects provided remember responses to

-60 of the studied words and .65 of the critical lures. Similarly, fkontal patients provided

remember responses for .65 of the studied words and .66 of the critical lures.

Nonconfabulating amnesic patients provided remember responses to .47 of the studied words

and .40 of the critical lures; the corresponding proportions for confabulating amnaics was

.55 and -58. These fïndings are in h e with Roediger & McDermottYs suggestions.

However, as Schacter et al. (1996) found in their study, these affects are difficult to interpret

in light of the fact that none of the groups used 'laiow" responses more often for studied

words than for their corresponding distractors. Similarly, none of the groups used know

responses signi ficantly more O fien for critical lures than for their corresponding distrac tors.

These analyses imply that all subject groups tended to use the know response when they were

just guessing that a word had appeared on the list, and used the remember response whenever

they felt a degree of certainty that a word had been studied previously.

Page 42: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

DISCUSSION:

The primary results of this experiment can be summarized as foliows: At recall,

when their overdl intrusion rate was taken into account, confabdating amnesic patients

showed a Iowa proportion of critical lure intrusions thau did control subjects. However,

nonconfabulating amnesics and frontal-lobe patients demonstrated a higher proportion of

critical lure intrusions than control subjects. Furthemore, control subjects recalled more

targets than critical lures, whereas nonconfabulating amnesic patients and frontal- lo be

patients "'recalled" more critical lues than targets. At recognition, both nonconfabulating

and coafabulating amnesics recognized a smaller proportion of studied words and intruded a

srnalier propomon of critical lures than did either controls or frontal lobe patients. The

qualitative differences between these fou. groups are intriguing. At the outset of the shidy, it

was hypothesized that individuds who met three specific criteria, would be most likely to

show the highest levels of fdse recognition and recall would be hi&. Those conditions

included having some memory for the word lists, being able to extract the

semantic/associative gist of the Lists and having a deficit in strategic monitoring processes.

We, therefore, proposed that fiontal-lobe patients would show the highest levels of false

recall and false recognition because they fulfill al1 three criteria While the data fiom this

experiment support this prediction, they also provide evidence to suggest that high levels of

false recall can occur if even one or two of these criteria are met.

The main fïnding in a study by Schacter, Verfaellie & Pradere (1996) was that

amnesic patients, with primarily medial temporal lobe damage, were less likely than normal

control subjects to produce fdse recaii or false recognition (i.e. they were less likely to

produce the unpresented cnticai lures for studied words). It was thus surprishg to find that

Page 43: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

nonconfabuiating amnesic patients in our study actually produced a higher proportion of

cnticai lures than controls at recali. It is even more surprising that amnesics produced at least

as much false recail as fiontal-lobe patients. In fact, it was predicted that if any of the

amnesics were going to produce high levels of fdse memones, it would be the confabulators

since they have fiontal-lobe damage and they already demonstrate a propensity for memory

distortion in their everyday life. Why is it then that nonconfabulating amnesics perfoxm so

much like &ontal-lobe patients? Although one would not want to h p l y that exactly the same

cognitive processes are occurring in both fiontal-lobe patients and nonconfabulating

amnesics, it appears that the end result is similar, namely these two groups of patients

produced the highest levels of false r a d . Nonconfabulating amnesics presumably have

faulty medial temporal lobe output. Unlike fiontal-lobe patients, nonconfabulating amnesics

do not have a deficit in monitoring. Having suggested that the production of false memones

in this paradigm is dependent on memory for the general theme of the study list and the

individual words in it, both processes necessitating intact medial temporal lobe functioning,

how c a . the high levels of fdse recall show by these amnesic patients be explained?

Underwood's (1 965) classic "implicit associative response" account, can still account

for this effect. According to this perspective, at the t h e of study, target words activate

associates that are later confused with the target itself. One version of this idea holds that

associates are tnily activated implicitly, in the sense that the subject does not become

consciously aware of the associate during study; it is generated by spreading activation

through an associative network. Roediger and McDermott (1995) note a problem with this

view. They propose that people claim to remember the cntical lures, whereas responses

based on implicit spreading activation would not be expected to generate such recollective

Page 44: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

experiences. The data in Schacter et al.'s (1996) study, and our own hdings speak to this

issue - if responses to critical lures were based solely on nonconscious activation processes,

we might expect amnesic patients to be influenced to the same degree as control subjects

even at recognition since explicit memory processes would not be involved.

Another version of Underwood's account implies that subjects consciously thllik of

the word at the tirne of study and are later subject to source memory confusion (Johnson,

Hashtroudi & Lindsay, 1993), consequentiy, they can no longer remember whether they

actuaily heard it or only thought of it themselves. This hypothesis is important to

understanding fdse recognition of critical Iures because it highhghts the point at which

distortion of memory occurs (i.e. when subjects mistake their prior thoughts for pnor

perceptions) (Schacter, VerfaelLie, & Pradere, 1996). It has been found that frontal patients

and amnesics tend to have more difficulty remembering source information than do controls

(Schacter, Harbluk, & McLachlan, 1984; Shimamura & Squire, 1987). However, problems

in source monitoring alone cannot be responsible for false recognition of cntical lures in this

experiment. That is, if amnesic patients (whether they are confabulaton or nonconfabulators)

are as likely as control subjects to generate the cntical l u e at the t h e of study (there is no

empirical basis for suggesting that they can not, see Schacter, Verfaellie & Pradere, 1 996),

one would expect them al1 to make more false alaxms to cntical l u e s at recall and

recognition.

Brainerd and Reyna's "fuzzy trace" (Brainerd, Reyna & Kneer, 1995; Brainerd,

Reyna & Brandes, 1995, Reyna & Brainerd, 1995) theory provides a useful k e w o r k for

interpreting the data £iom this experiment. They account for the phenornena of false recall

and recognition by proposing that memory can be based either on a "gist trace" that

Page 45: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

preserves the general meanhgs and interpretations engendered by studied items or an a

''verbath trace" that preserves specific information about the exact identity of each item. It

appears that although nonconfabulatuig amnesics are cleariy impaired at recalling specific

studied words (verbatim trace), they are nevertheless able to extract and retain the semantic

gist of the Iist and so they produce the critical lure at recall. Certaialy? this is a useful and

generally effective strategy for amnesic patients to employ in everyday iife. Since they are so

impaired at retaining the specific details about a situation, they do well to focus their efforts

on extracthg the main theme. Indeed, for many situations in everyday Iife, we must d l

employ this strategy so as not to be overwhelmed by the sheer amount of information we are

bombarded with.

The performance of these patients at recognition also lends support to Brainerd et al's

theoretical stance. Because they are severely memory impaired, even the "gist trace"

becomes degraded over time so that at recognition they no longer produce higher levels of

fdse memory than controls or fiontal patients; in fact they pedorm in much the same fashion

as the confabuiatuig amnesics. h cornparison to fkontai lobe patients and control subjects,

both groups of amnesics produce significantly lower levels of false recognition.

Apparently though, not al1 amnesics use this strategy-as was evident from the

performance of confabulating amnesics who produced low levels of false recall in our study.

Why is there such a diffrence in the performance of confabulating and nonconfabulating

amnesic patients at recaiI? Both groups have medial temporal lobe damage which causes

their severe amnesia. However, the confabuiating amnesics also have frontal-lobe damage.

This additional damage to critical frontal regions may af5ect their ability to employ cognitive

strategies effectively. Indeed, as Moscovitch and colleagues have proposed (Moscovitch,

Page 46: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

1995; Moscovitch & Melo, 1997) confabulating amnesics have impaireci strategic retrieval

processes resulting nom damage to the region of the ventromediai prefiontal cortex. These

strategic retrieval processes not only help monitor output £iom the medial temporal lobe

system but they also help initiate and guide search in episodic and semantic memory and they

help organize the output ffom those systems as well. (Moscovitch & Melo, 1997). The fact

that the confabulating patients do not use the strategy of extracthg the gist of the study list

may implicate the ventromedid prefiontal region in processes necessary for flexibility in

implernenting such a strategy. It is interesthg that the very same pattern of damage leads to

flagrant mernory distortion (Le. confabulation) on the one han& but does not contribute to

false recall and fdse recognition in the DeeselRoediger & McDermott paradigm on the other.

Thus, while confabulating arnnesics seem neurologically like good candidates for fdse recall

and false recognition, due to their history of confabulation and their poor monitoring; as was

mentioned earlier, a deficit in monitoring does not matter much when the output fkom the

media1 tempord lobe is almost nonexistent. Furthmore, their may be other deficits arisuig

from their ventromedial frontal-lobe damage that do not allow them to employ strategies at

encoding (such as extracthg the semantic/associative gist of the lists).

The nonamnesic frontal-lobe patients are similar to the control subjects in several

ways. They have a reasonably good memory of studied words and they are also able to

remember the main theme of the Lists. However, they differ kom controls in that they also

have a deficit in their ability to monitor their memories effectively. If poor monitoring

contributes to the production of false recall and recognition in this paradigrn, the fiontal-lobe

patients should produce more false recall and recognition than control subjects. Although the

results of this study are consistent with this hypothesis, the statistical significance of our

Page 47: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

hdings is limited by the small number of subjects in each group. At both recall and

recognition, the mean proportion of critical lures intnided was higher for frontal patients.

It is noteworthy that al1 of the frontal patients we tested had nght frontal-lobe

involvement. Schacter and colleagues (1996) have recently documented a case of a man who

suffered an infarction restricted to the right fiontal lobe. He exhibits a striking pattern of

fdse recognitions that provides useful clues conceming the role of the prefrontal regions in

illusory mernories. The investigators suggest that BG relies excessively on idormation about

the general correspondence between a test item and previously shidied words when making a

recognition decision. Control subjects in this study typically clairned to "remembe?' that a

word or picture had appeared on a shidy list only when they retreived specific information

about a particular word or picture. BG by contrast, relied inappropriately on a match between

a test item and general characteristics of the study episode when making his recognition

decisions. In addition, Schacter et al. (1996) analyzed what he claims to recall when he

makes a bbremember" false alarm. They found that he tends to provide associations to other

words or to events in his life, specific information from an inappropriate context. Given

previous evidence implicating the fkontal lobes in memory for source or contextual

information (Schacter, Harbluck & McLachlan, 1984), these authors suggest that it seems

likely that a source memory deficit contributes to the character of BG's false recollection.

According to this view, the right prefkontd cortex may be involved in setting up contextual

representations that guide reûieval by allowing one to focus in on a target episode and filter

out irrelevant material. Norman and Schacter (1996) have argued that BG's deficits are

related to problems generating a specific, focused representation of a study episode and

filtering out nonessential information. These observations would also hold for the fiontal-

Page 48: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

lobe patients who participated in this experiment and are very closely related to the issue of

defective monitoring processes in hntal-lobe patients. Just Wre BG, the fiontal-lobe

patients in our study may be capable of generating only a vague or docused representation

of characteristic feahires of the target words agallist which test items are matched. Items, in

this case the critical lures, that are generally similar to previously studied ones are accepted

as old because they match the general contextuai representation and are not filtered out.

The findings fiom this study illustrate the fact that the relationship between various

kinds of rnemory distortions are complicated. Memory distortions are in no way a unitary

process. By comparïng and contrasting different kinds of memory illusions, it should be

possible to delineate the component processes involved in each of them.

Page 49: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

References

Anisfeld, M., 8r Knapp, M. (1 968). Association, synonymity and directionality in false

recognition. Journal of Experimenîal Psychology, 77,17 1 - 179.

Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. (1988). Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndrome.

Brain and Cognition. 7,2 12-230.

Baddeley, A. D. (1976). The Psychologv of Mernos>. New York: Basic Books.

Bartlett, F. C. (1 93 2). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Belli, R. F., Lindsay, D. S., Gales, M. S., & McCarthy, T. T. (1994). Memory impairment

and source misattribution in postevent misinformation expenments with short

retention intervals. Mernory & Cognition, L 5,657-668.

Berlyne, N. (1 972). Confabulation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 120,3 1-39.

Binet, A. (1 900). La suggestibilite. Paris: Schleicher Freres.

Bonhoeffer, K. (1 904). Die Korsakowsche Symptomenkomplex in seinen Bezeiehungen ni

den verschiedenen Krankheitsformen. Allg. 2. Psychiat.. 61, 744.

Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Brandes, E. (1995). Are children's false memories more

persistant than their true memories? Psychologicnl Science. 6,3 59-3 64.

Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Kneer, R. (1995). False-recognition reversal: When

similarity is distinctive. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 1 57- 1 85.

Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1996). Confabulation and the control of recollection.

Memory, 4(4), 3 59-4 1 1.

Ceci. S. J. (1 995). False beliefs: Some developmentai and clinical considerations. In D.L.

Schacter. J. T. Coyle, G. D. Fishbach, M. M. Mesulam, & L.E. Sullivan (Eds.),

Page 50: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Memory Distortion, Cambridge. MA: Harvard University Press, pp.9 1 - 128.

Cennak, L. S., Butters, N., & Gerrein, J. (1973). The extent of the verbal encoding ability

of Korsakoff patients. Neuropsychologia, 1 1,85-94.

Cohen, N. J., & Eichenbaum, H. (1993). Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampd system.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Craik, F. 1. M., Moms, L. W., Moms, R. G., & Loewen, E. R. (1990). Relations between

source amnesia and frontal-lobe hctioning in O lder adults. Psychology and Agi%

5, 148-151.

Curran, T., Schacter, D. L., Norman, K. A., & Gailucio, L. ( ? False recognition afler a

right fiontal-lobe infarction: Memory for general and specific information

Neuropsychologia

Dalla Barba, G. (1 993). Confabulation: Know ledge and recollective experience. Cognitive

Neuropsychology. 10(1), 1-20.

Deese, J. (1 959). On the prediction of occurrence of particula. verbal intrusions in

immediate recall. Journal of &perimental Psychology, 58, 1 7-22.

Delbecq-Derouesne, J., Beauvois, M. F., & Shallice, T. (1990). Preserved recail venus

impaired recognition. Brain, 1 13, 1045- 1074, 189-2 1 1.

De Luca, J., & Cicerone, KD. (199 1). Confabulation following aneurysm of the anterior

communicating artery. Cortex, 27,417-423.

Dywau, J. & Jacoby, L. L. (1990). Effect of aging and source monitoring: Differences in

susceptibility to false fame. Psychology and Aging. 3,379-387.

Eichenbaum, H. (1 997). How does the brain organize mernories? Science, 2 77, 330-332.

Freud, S. (1899). Screen Mernories. In J. Strachey (Ed.), ï7ze Standard Edition of the

Page 51: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Cornpiete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud (yol. 3). London: Hogarth Press.

Freyd, J. J., Gleaves, D. H. (1996). 'Xemembering7' words not presented in Lists:

Relevance to the current recovered/false memory controversy. Journal of

Qoen'mentd Psycho[ogy: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 22(3), 8 1 1-8 13.

Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. 1. (1 993). Recognising and remembering. In A.F. Collins, S. E.

Gathercole, M. A. Conway, & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of Memory, Hove, LTK:

Erlbaum, pp. 163-188.

Gillund, G., & Shi&, R. M. (1984). A retrieval mode1 for both recognition and recall.

Psychological Review, 9 1, 1 -67.

Hatakeyama, T. (1 997). Adults and children with high imagery show more pronounced

perceptual percephlal priming effect. Perceptual Motor Skills. 84(3), 1 3 1 5- 1 329.

Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C. M., & Dywan, J. (1 989). Memory aîtrïbutions. in H. L.

Roediger III & F. 1. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness:

Essays in honour of Endel Tulving, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 39 1-422.

Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C. M., Brown, J., & Jasechko, J. (1989). Becorning famous overnight:

Limits on the ability to avoid unconscious influences of the past. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 56,3 26-33 8.

Janows& J. S., Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1989). Source memory impairment in

patients with fiontal-lobe lesions, Neuropsychologia. 2 7, 1 043- 1 056.

Johnson, M. K. (1991). Reaiity monitoring: Evidence fkom confabulation in organic brain

diseasepatients. In G. P. Prigatana & D. L. Schacter (Eds.), Awareness of defcit

afier brain injury: Clinicd and theoretical h i e s , New York: Oxford University

Press, pp. 176-197.

Page 52: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1 993). Source monitoring. Psychological

BuIletin, 1 14, 3-28.

Johnson, M. K., O'Connor, M., & Cantor, 1. (1997). Confabulation, Memory Deficits,

and Frontal Dysfunction. Brain and Cognition, 34, 189-206.

Johnson, M. K., & Sumgas, A. G. (1 989). Reality monitoring judgements of other people's

rnemories. Bulletin of the Psychonomir Society, 2 7, 1 0% 1 1 0.

Knowlton, B. I., & Squire, L. R (1995). Remembering and knowing: two different

expressions of declarative memory. Journal of Experimentaf Psychofogy:

Learning. Memory and Cognition. 21. 699-7 10.

Korsakoff, S. S. (1889). Etude medico-psychologique sur une form des maladies de la

memoire (Medical-psychological study of a form of diseases of memory). Revue

Philosophique, 28,50 1-530.

Kroll, N. E. A., Knight, R. T., Metcalfe, J. , Wolf, E. S., & Tulving, E. (1996). Cohesion

failure as a source of memory illusions. Journal ofMemory and Language. 35,

176- 196.

Lindsay, D. S. (1 990). Misleading suggestions can impair eyewitnesses ' ability to remember

event details. Journal of Expenmental Psychology: Learning. Memory and

Cognition, 16, 1077- 1 083.

Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal

information into a visual memory. Journal of m e ~ m e n t a l Psychology: Human

Learning and Memory. 4,19-3 1.

Page 53: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobilie destruction: An

example of the interaction between language and mernory. Journal of Verbal

Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585-589.

Mather, M., Henkel, L. A., Johnson, M. K. (1 997). Evaluating characteristics of fdse

memories: Remembedknow judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire

compared. Memory and Cognition, 25(6), 826-83 7 .

McCleliand, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O'Reilly, R C. (1995). Why there are

complementary learning systerns in the hippocarnpus and neocortex: [nsights fiom

the successes and failures of connectionist models of leaming and memory.

Psychological Review, 1 02, 4 1 9-457.

McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. (1985). Misleading postevent information and mernory for

events: Arguments and evidence against memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of

Ehperimental Psychology: General. 11 4, 1 - 16.

McDermott, K.B. (1996). The persistence of fdse memories in list recall. Journal of

Memory and Language, 35,2 12-230.

McGlynn, S. M., & Schacter, D. L. (1989). Unawareness of deficits in neuropsychological

syndromes. Journal of Chical and Experimental Neuropsychoiogy, 1 1 , 143-205.

McIntyre, 5. S., & Craik, F. 1. M. (1987). Age differences in memoiy for item and source

information. Canadian Journal of Psychologv, 41, 175- 192.

Memer, B., Wapner, W., Gardner, H., & Benson, D. F. (1977). A study of confabulation.

Archives of Neurology, 34,429-43 3.

Moscovitch, M. (1995). Confabulation. In D. L. Schacter, J. T. Coyle, G. D. Fischbach, M.

M. Mesuiam & L. E. Sullivan (Eds.), Memory Distortion: How Min& Braîns. and

Page 54: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Societies Reconstruct the Past, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 226-

254.

Moscovitch, M. (1 989). Confabulation and the fkontal systerns : Strategic versus associated

retrieval in neuropsychological theories of memory. In H. L. Roediger III & F. 1. M.

Craik, (Eds.), Yarieties of Memory und Consciousness: Essqys in Honour of Endel

Tulving, Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaurn Associates, pp. 1 33- 160.

Moscovitch, M. & Melo, B. (1997). Strategic retrievai and the fiontal-lobes: Evidence

form confabulation and amnesia. Neuropsychologia. 35(7), 1 0 1 7- 1 034.

Noman, K. A., & Schacter, D. L. (1 997). False recognition in younger and older adults:

exploring the characteristics of illusory memones. Memov & Cognition, 25(6), 838-

848.

Parkin, A. J., Bindschaedler, C., Harsent, L., & Metzler, C. Pathologicd fdse alam rates

followuig darnage to the lefi fiontal cortex. Brain & Cognition. 32(1), 14-27.

Payne, D. G., Elie, C. I., Blackwell, J. M., & Neuschatz, J. S. (1996). Memory illusions:

Recalling, recognizing, and recollecting events that never occurred. Journal of

Memory and Language. 3&26 1 -285.

Reinitz, M. T., Lammers, W. J., & Cochran, B. P. (1992). Memory conjunction errors:

Miscombination of stored stimulus features cm produce illusions of mernory.

Memory and Cognition, 20, 1 - 1 1.

Reinitz, M. T., Verfaellie, M., & Milberg, W. (1996). Memory conjunction erron in normal

and amnesic subjects. Journal of Memor-y and Language, 35,286-299.

Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning

and hdividual Dzfferences, 7 , 1-25.

Page 55: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Roediger, K. L. ItI, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating fdse mernories: Rernembering

words not presented in lists. J o m a i of EjLperimental Psychology: Leaming,

Memory and Cognition. 21(4), 803-8 14.

Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (1996). False perceptions of fdse memones.

Journal of Experimental Psychologyr Learning, Memory and Cognition. 22(3),

814- 816.

Sc hacter, D. L. ( 1 996). Illusory mernories: A cognitive neuroscience anaiysis. Proceedings

of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 93(24), 13527-

33.

Schacter, D. L. (1995). Memory Distortion: History and curent status. in D. L. Schacter,

J. T. Coyle, G. D. Fischbach, M. M. Mesulam, & L. E. Sullivan (Eds.). Memory

K. Distortion: How rninds. brains and societies recortstruct the paît, Cambridge,

MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 1-43.

Schacter, D. L., Buckner, R. L., Koutstaal, W., Dale, A. M., & Rosen, B. R. (1997).

Late onset of anterior pref?ontal activity during true and false recognition: An event-

related fMRI study. Naroimage, 6(4), 259-269.

Schacter, D. L., C m , T., Galluccio, L., Milberg, W. P., & Bates, I. F. (1996). False

recognition and the nght &ontal-lobe: A case study . Neur~psycho~o~a, 34(8),

793-808.

Schacter, D. L., Koutstaal, W., Johnson, M. K., Gross, M. S., Angell, K. E. (1997) False

recollection induced via photographs: A cornparison of older and younger adults.

Psychology and Aging, 22(2), 203-2 15.

Schacter, D.L., Reiman, E., Curran, T., Sheng Yun, L., Bandy, D., McDermott, K. B., &

Page 56: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Roediger, H. L. III. (1996). Nemanatomical correlates of veridical and illusory

recognition memory: Evidence 60m positron emission tomography. Neuron. 1 7,

267- 274.

Schacter, D. L., Savage, C. R., Alpert, N. M., Rauch, S. L., & Albert, M. (1996). The

role of the hippocarnpus and fiontal cortex in age-related memory changes: A PET

study. NeuroReport, 7, 1 165- 1 169.

Schacter, D. L., Verfaille, M., & Anes, M. D. (1997). Illusory memones in amnesic

patients: concephial and perceptual false recognition. Neuroprychoiogy, 11(3),

33 1-342.

Schacter, D. L., Verfaille, M., & Pradere, D. (1 996). The neuropsychology of memory

illusions: False recall and recognition in amnesic patients. Journal of Memory and

Lunguage. 35,3 19-334.

Schnider, A., von Daniken, C., & Gutbrod, K. (1996). The mechanisms of spontaneous and

provoked confabulations. Brain. 119, 1365- 1 375.

Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocarnpal

lesions. Journal of hreurology. Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 20, 1 1 -2 1.

Semon, R. (1 9OM92I). The mnerne. London: George Allen & Unwin.

Semon, R. (1 909/1923). Mnemic psychology. London: George Allen & Unwin.

Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1 987). A neuropsychological study of fact memory and

source amnesia. Journal of Equerimental Psychology: Leurning, Memory and

Cognition. 21,267-287.

Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis ftom hdings with rats,

monkeys, and humans. PsychologicuI Revrevrewt 99, 195-23 1.

Page 57: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Stem, W. (1910). Abstracts of Lectures on the psychology of testirnony and on the shidy of

individuality. Amerkm Journal of PsychoZogy, 21,270-282.

Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Lieberman, A., & Levine, H. (1978). An extraordinary

form of confabulation. Narr010g-y. 28. 1 166- 1 172.

Stuss, D. T., Eskes, G. A., & Foster, J. A. (1 994). Experimental neuropsychological stuclies

of fiontal-lobe functions. In F. Boller & J. Grafinan, (Eds.), Handbook of

Neuropsychology, Vol. 9, Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1 49- 1 84.

Stuss, D. T., Gubexman, A., Nelson, R., & Larochelle, S. (1 988). The neuropsychology

of paramedian thalamic infarction. Brain and Cognition. 8, 348-378.

Sulin, R. A., & Dooling, D. J. (1 974). Intrusion of a thematic idea in retention of prose.

Journal of merimentai Psychology, 103,255-262.

TaIIand, G. A. (1961). Confabulation in the Wemike-Korsakoff Syndrome. J. Nerv. Ment.

Dis. 132,361.

Tulving, E. (1985). Varieties of consciousness and levels of awareness in mernory. In A.

Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: Selection. awareness and control: A

hibute to Donald Broadbent. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Undenvood, B. J. (1965). False recognition produced by implicit verbal responses.

Journal of Experimental Psychologv. 70, 122- 129.

Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K .E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, W., &

Mishkin, M. (1 997). Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on

episodic and semantic memory. Science, 2 77,3 76-3 80.

Verfaellie, M., & Treadwell, J. (1 993). Status of recognition memory in amnesia

Neuropsychologia, 7,s - 1 3.

Page 58: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic rnemory:

The &ontal-lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin.

22 Z(3).

Whitty, C. W. M., & Lewin, W. (1957). Vivid day-dreaming: An unusual form of confusion

following anterior cingulectomy. Brain. 80,72.

Wulf, F. (1 922). Uber die Verandemg von Vontellungen. Psychologisch Forschung. I ,

333-373.

Zangwill, O. L. (1937). An investigation of the reiationship between the process of

reproducing and recognizing simple figures with special reference to Koffka's trace

theory. British Journal of Psychology, 27,250-276.

Zaragoza, M. S., & Lane, S. M. (1994). Source misattributions and the suggestibility o f

eyewihiess memory. Journal oflihperimenfaf Psyrhologv: Learning, Memoty, and

Cognition, 2 4 934-945.

Page 59: FACETS OF MEMORY - University of Toronto T-Space€¦ · real-world controversies conceming the suggestibility of children's memory and the accuracy of memories recovered ... out

IMAGE NALUATION TEST TARGET (QA-3)

APPLIED IMAGE. lnc - - - 1653 East Main Street - -* - - Rochester. NY 14609 USA -- -- - - Phone: 71 6 / 4 8 ~ - O W ~ -- -- - - Fax: 716t200-5989

O t 993. Applied Image. Inc. AM RighB Resenrrtd