64
Debbie Hebert, OT Reg. Ont. BSc(OT), MSc (Kin) [email protected]

Debbie Hebert, OT Reg. Ont. BSc(OT), MSc (Kin) debbie ...westgtastroke.ca/wp-content/uploads/2016/10/Apraxia...Ideational Apraxia sensory stim. Protocol + 20 mins Verbal and Visual

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • Debbie Hebert, OT Reg. Ont.BSc(OT), MSc (Kin)

    [email protected]

  • Participants will:

    Appreciate the current understanding of apraxia

    Be able to discuss current top-down and bottom-up intervention approaches to treatment

    Apply learned knowledge to a case study

  • 1. Incidence, Impact and Definitions

    2. Selected Model

    3. Assessment

    4. Intervention approaches and possible strategies to sensory, conceptual and production issues

    5. Occupation based approaches

    6. Case study

  • Incidence: 28 – 57% - left hemisphere damage0 – 34% - right hemisphere damage

    (Donkervoort, Dekker, Van den Ende, Stehmann-Saris, & Deelman, 2000)

    - problem persists in 20% diagnosed (Basso, Capitani, Sala, Liacona, & Spinnler, 1987)

    Impact: ◦ direct impact on functional performance (Foundas, 1995 (eating),

    Hanna-Pladdy et al., 2003(toiletting and bathing)

    ◦ increased caregiver burden (Sundet et al., 1988)

  • “a disorder of skilled movement not caused by weakness, akinesia, deafferentation, abnormal tone or posture, movement disorders such as tremors or chorea, intellectual deterioration, poor comprehension, or uncooperativeness.” (Heilman KM, Rothi LJG. Clinical neuropsychology. Apraxia. 3rd ed. 1993, p141-163)

  • “A deficit of learned skilled purposive movements that specifically effects the efficiency and accuracy of the production of a movement and cannot be accounted for by elemental sensorimotor, cognitive or attentional deficits.” Rothi & Heilman, 1997, p 294.

    gestures are inaccurate (clumsy) due to abnormal spatial (posture, orientation of hand to objects and direction of movement) and/or temporal (timing) aspects of movement. (Smania et al., 2006)

    person knows what to do with the object but cannot call upon the nervous system to execute the command

    seen in conditions of verbal command (pantomime) and/or imitation and/or object use (Staminova et al., 2012)

  • Has been used to describe impairments in actual tool use (De Renzi, 1985) oran inability to perform a sequence of acts using tools and objects to achieve an intended goal (Poeck, 1983; Poeck & Lehmkuhl, 1980).

    Ochipa, Rothi and Heilman (1992) suggest ideational apraxia is a problem in sequencing movements and conceptual apraxia is based on errors due to loss of object knowledge.

    Typical errors involve sequencing or object use errors

    Classically, errors are seen, more in Pantomime and Object Use (Roy, 2001) but not in imitation

  • Ideational Ideomotor

    Tool/Object Related ErrorsInappropriate tool selectionPerformance of a related but incorrect action to the target object*Performance of an unrelated action to the target object*Use of hand to perform action rather than imagining

    target tool*

    Spatial ErrorsSpatial orientation of hands incorrect to imagined tool (internal configuration)*Finger arm and hand in inappropriate position with respect to imagined tool and object (external configuration)*Movement in the wrong plane +use of wrong muscle groups (movement)*Body part as tool * +Note: Tools are the instruments performing the action. Objects are the recipients of the action.

    Sequencing ErrorsPerseveration of part of the series of the action+*Problems in Terminating movements +Sequence of action out of order +Blending of two actions +Omission of action part +Repetitions +substitution of movements unrelated to the movement +

    sequencing of individual movements in an action +

    Temporal ErrorsTiming of movement irregular*Occurance or increase in repetitions required for the action*Unsustained actionClumsiness

    Sequencing of pantomime *

    OtherNo response

    Perplexity +

    OtherPerseveration of sub part of the action+No responseamorphous movementsgestural or verbal augmentation when performing the movement

    *From Rothi, Raymer, Heilman (1997)+From Roy (1998)Unmarked Other items From Tate and MacDonald (1995)

  • Liepmann’s Model (1905 -1920)

    Roy’s Model (1996), Stamenova, Black and Roy (2013)

    Cubelli et al. (2000)

    Rothi and Heilman (1997)

    Buxbaum and Kalenine Model (2010)

    Rouinis and Humphreys (2015)

  • System Purpose of System

    Sensory-perceptual system

    Conceptual

    Production System

    Analysis of visual-gestural and auditory-verbal information and visual-tool

    Tool function and action storage

    Motor execution and motor control

  • Sensory/Perceptual System

    Conceptual System

    Production System

    Visual/Gestural Information

    Auditory/Verbal Information

    Visual Tool/Object Information

    Knowledge of Action

    Knowledge of Tool/Object Function

    Image Generation

    Response Selection

    Working Memory

    Response Organization/Control

    Delayed Imitation

    Concurrent Imitation

    Pantomime

    Stamenova, V., Black, S. E. and Roy, E. A, An update on the Conceptual–Production Systems model of apraxia: Evidence from stroke. Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 53–63.

  • Pattern Gesture - Tool System ProblemRecognition

    P+ / DI- / CI- – Sensory Perceptual Problem

    P- / DI+ / CI+ – Conceptual System (Ideational)

    P- / DI+ / CI+ + Production System (e.g. Response Selection)

    P+ /DI- / CI+ + Production System (working memory)

    P- / DI- / CI+ + Production System (preserved analysis)

    P+ / DI- / CI- + Production System (Response Organization and Control)

    P- / DI- / CI- + Production System Impairment (Ideomotor)

    P- / DI- / CI- – All System Impairment

  • Testing complicated by lack of agreed upon terminology (Tate and MacDonald, 1995)

    Comparability of research tests of apraxia questionable (Butler, 2002)

    No Gold Standard

    Clinically non-directive

  • Observational

    Screening - Identification of the problem and it’s severity

    Assessment Batteries - identify the place where the praxis process breaks down

  • A-One - Arnadottir (1990)

    AMPS – Fisher (1990)

    Experimental Rating scales: The ADL observations - Van Heugten et al. (1999, 2000)

  • Comprehensive TULIAItems◦Imitation – non-symbolic◦Imitation – intransitive◦Imitation – transitive◦Pantomime – non-symbolic◦Pantomime – intransitive◦Pantomime – transitive

    48 items-6 point error based scoring method (0–5)

    Also TULIA (AST) 12 item version (Vanbenlingen et al., 2011)Maximum score =12 with cut off

  • Aphasia Screen, Visual Screen, Perceptual Test Traditional Apraxia Tests (Transitive

    Representational and Non Representational Gestures) Error scoring is desirable.

    Tool and Action Identification Tests Card Sequencing Multiple Objects Tests Mechanical Problem Solving (new learning)

    (See Florida Assessment Battery (Rothi et al. 1997); Florida Apraxia Battery–Extended and Revised Sydney (FABERS), (Power et al. 2010) and Component Model Assessments )

  • http://www.google.ca/url?sa=i&rct=j&q=&esrc=s&source=images&cd=&cad=rja&uact=8&ved=0ahUKEwjO_-CltbTSAhUJzoMKHYQ7A2kQjRwIBw&url=http://tiny-kylo-ren.tumblr.com/post/148254659481/so-you-want-to-sew-a-button&psig=AFQjCNGv7BNHhdi6J0KOBD2JUuFZKo_O7g&ust=1488427132316634http://www.google.ca/url?sa=i&rct=j&q=&esrc=s&source=images&cd=&cad=rja&uact=8&ved=0ahUKEwjO_-CltbTSAhUJzoMKHYQ7A2kQjRwIBw&url=http://tiny-kylo-ren.tumblr.com/post/148254659481/so-you-want-to-sew-a-button&psig=AFQjCNGv7BNHhdi6J0KOBD2JUuFZKo_O7g&ust=1488427132316634

  • Current Context:

    decreased time for intervention and length of stay

    lack of research based on models of praxis assessment time minimal high complexity and variability in presentation of

    apraxia and co-morbidities – few people fall into experimental population eg. Aphasia and apraxia are often co-morbid (Kobayashi & Ugawa, 2013)

    natural environments best for praxis intervention – not often the setting for clients

  • Limited RCTs

    West et al. (2009) Cochrane systematic review found 3 trials and concluded not enough evidence to support of refute effectiveness.

    Lindsten-McQueen et al. (2014) ◦ Systematic review using FAME approach (Murphy et al, 2009)

    ◦ examined 8 studies (4 RCTs) and found Level A, B effectiveness for Errorless Learning (Goldenberg & Hagmann, 1998) Gesture Training (Smania et al., 2000, 2006) and Strategy Training (van Heugten, 1998,2000; Donkervoort et al. 2001, Guesgens et al. 2006, 2007)

  • Apraxic clients have an impaired ability to learn & retain information (Heilman et al, 1975 (rotary pursuit); Kimura, 1977 (manual sequence box); Poole,1991 (tying shoes)

    Learning is specific to targeted interventions and generalization must be taught? (For exception see Smania et al.,2000, Geusgens et al. 2007)

    Interventions may not improve apraxia dramatically but may improve occupational performance.

  • Sensory-Perceptual Interventions1. Maximizing sensory reception (Mary Warren, 1990)

    Vision: Use of proper corrective lenses Good lighting Good contrast between objects and background Allow ample time to study environmental cues Improve scanning accuracy and speed Augment features of items important for movement by

    using color or arrows Decrease environmental complexity (decrease patterns on

    items and numbers of items) Remove unnecessary items for task.

  • Audition

    Use hearing aids

    Ensure reception of command by asking for repetition of commands

    Allow ample processing time

    Simplify statements – less information

    Decrease steps

  • Tactile:

    Ensure ample processing time for exploration of items

    Augment sensory awareness through texture

    Re-educate re important features of objects

    Kinesthetic: Assist client to move slowly enough to ensure appreciation of movement

  • Assumption: Apraxia key problem is inadequate sensory integration or an inabiltiy to select the most important sensory information – could be addressed through sensory input or enhancement (Ayres, 1985; Croce, 1993; Goodgold-Edwards & Cermak, 1990)

    Sensory Stimulation (Butler, 1997, 2001)

  • Sensory Stimulation (Butler, 1997, 2001)

    e.g. 2001 Case Study Male with Ideomotor and Ideational Apraxia

    ◦ sensory stim. Protocol + 20 mins Verbal and Visual Mediation of motor activities (touching targets, imitation, bilateral imitation, verbalizing hand position) which continued from baseline

    ◦ Outcomes – Improvements on motor tests, tests of ideational apraxia and imitating gestures and less inconsistency of movements.

  • Which works best as a request for movement?

    visual object, visual picture, visual sequence of pictures, concurrent model imitation, delayed model imitation, tactile object presentation, combination tactile/visual object presentation, kinesthetic imitation.

    Utilize the strongest method or modality when requesting movement and correcting movement during a task

    When re-teaching a gesture (as in learning gestures for communication) – use multiple cues and fade out stronger cues first (Maher et al.,1991; Code and Gaunt, 1986)

    Cuing may assist client to remember conceptual representations that are inaccessible. (Jefferies et al, 2007)

  • Structure and Function Relearning Based on the notion that persons with

    conceptual apraxia have poor mechanical knowledge and problem solving with respect to objects (Goldenberg 2001).

    Re- educate patients on the relationship between the structure of objects and their function. E.g. What features of objects go with what functions. (Mechanical Problem Solving)

  • Cubelli et al.1991: Single case study: Client’s acquisition of communicative gestures improved with Structure/Function training coupled with modeling gestures that represent the use of the object.

    Goldenberg & Hagmann, 1998, 2001 Studies found that this “exploratory intervention” was not successful in changing function in contrast to direct Errorless ADL interventionExploration training involved: explanation, copying, touching, comparison between objects, comparison of objects with photographs

  • Limit objects for a task to those needed to decrease confusion caused by objects that might afford similar actions.

    Highlighting objects with a visual symbol of the action may cue the action associated with the object. (Miller, 1986)

    Pictures demonstrating object function for common objects (in context)

    Bypass the conceptual system and request tasks through imitation

    Teach client to self cue by humming/verbalizing rhythmically through a task – up/down/up/down

    Utilize errors such as perseveration to start and complete an action – e.g. feeding

  • Why address production errors?

    Important in relearning communicative gestures

    Important in increasing efficiency of movement

    Two types of problems:Correction of production errors, spatial and temporal

    Recognition/awareness of making errors

  • Sample Model (Based on Maher et al., 1991)

    1. Select gestures for common items used in tasks important to the client (number could be determined or could select communicative gestures)

    2. Request task using multiple cues (actual tool, picture of tool, verbal, imitation). Fade cues until you get correct production of gesture to visual target tool.

    3. Provide feedback immediately on correctness of response

    4. Correct error using physical manipulation or imitation

    5. Set criterion of correct execution 3 times before moving on.

  • Targeting approach of limb to task, positioning of hand and activating the correct joint using verbal guidance and repetition, (Ochipa et al, 1995)

  • 2000 RCT: Training was graded for each type of gesture (transitive, intransitive symbolic and non-symbolic gestures.

    E.g. Transitive Gestures: Tool Use – Picture of someone doing a gesture – Picture of tool

    Participants cued manually, verbally, visually

    50 mins, 3 Xs per week, 35 sessions produce improved performance on untrained IA and IMA tasks. No long term follow-up.

    2006 RCT: Participants treated for apraxia 50 mins, 3 Xs per week, 30 sessions improved in praxis

    measures for ideational and ideomotor apraxia and ADL (caregiver burden) – effects sustained 2 months post stroke

  • If client can detect error but does not, slow down the activity

    If client cannot detect error utilize:

    Stop action

    Videotape

    awareness training

  • Most logical focus

    Most articles address this method of rehabilitation

  • Theoretical roots in the Conductive Education technique (Cotton and Sutton, 1986)

    Aimed at reorganization of action through using speech to regulate movement

    Utilized in studies and reports by Wilson (1986) and Pilgrim and Humphreys (1994), Butler

  • break the desired movement/action down into sub-movements.

    client executes each sub-movement while the instructor (& client) verbalize the intent of each sub movement & indicates timing of movement & perhaps sequencing

    assistance to complete each sub movement is given as required

    assistance is faded and then the verbal mediation is faded (in original form of conductive education, the client is instructed to internalize this mediated speech or say the instructions to his/herself)

  • not been investigated in isolation in apraxia intervention literature but has been used for procedural memory types of training (Wilson, 1987)

    objective is to reestablish the ordered connection of the subtasks by increasing demands on the client to perform additional links of the chain together

  • Curran, M. C., Hussain, Z., Park, N. W. (presented at 109th meeting APA, San Francico [email protected]

    Goal: To teach novel naturalistic skills (e.g. wheelchair transfers, preparing food)

    Clients who were given a demonstration with verbal descriptions accompanying each step (verbal scaffolding) performed in a superior fashion to those who had a demonstration only.

    mailto:[email protected]

  • Picture cards with steps of action

    Checklists

    Lay out objects in order of action

    Use of recorders

  • van Heugten, 1998,200; Donkervoort et al. 2001, Guesgens et al. 2006, 2007

    Intervention geared at phases of activity with problems

    1. Initiation (selection of action and objects). Instructions were the focus of intervention

    2. Performance (executing the activity)

    Assistance

    3. Correcting and Controlling Activity

    Feedback Strategies

  • van Heugten et al. 1998 ◦ pre/post no control group, ◦ 12 weeks of (3 – 5 xs/wk, 30 mins) training ◦ 33 subjects improved on motor function (Motricity

    Index), apraxia tests (object use and imitation) and to a greater extent on ADL observation and Barthel

    van Heugten et al. 2000 ◦ used previous treatment cohort and added control

    cohort of non-apraxic participants to compare co-morbidities

    ◦ demonstrated improvements could occur with cognitive co-morbidity

    ◦ most severe apraxic participants (praxis and motor control) initially improved the most

  • Donkervoort et al. 2001 ◦ strategy training experimental and usual-treatment control

    group – used previous tests◦ tested 8 weeks and 5 months – treatment time average 25

    sessions and 15 hrs (strategy group) and 27 sessions and 19hrs (control)

    ◦ strategy training group showed superior performance on ADL observation test

    ◦ no difference 5 months follow-up

    Geuegens et al. 2006◦ examined trained and untrained ADL observation tasks in

    study above. ◦ change scores in strategy training group were larger for

    non-trained tasks than in usual treatment group

  • van Heugten et al. 1998 ◦ pre/post no control group, ◦ 12 weeks of (3 – 5 xs/wk, 30 mins) training ◦ 33 subjects improved on motor function (Motricity

    Index), apraxia tests (object use and imitation) and to a greater extent on ADL observation and Barthel

    van Heugten et al. 2000 ◦ used previous treatment cohort and added control

    cohort of non-apraxic participants to compare co-morbidities

    ◦ demonstrated improvements could occur with cognitive co-morbidity

    ◦ most severe apraxic participants (praxis and motor control) initially improved the most

  • Geusgens et al, 2007

    29 participants with apraxia

    8 weeks strategy training on 4 out of 6 tasks selected from AMPS tasks with same level of difficulty. Two were untrained.

    Tested pre-treatment, 8 weeks, 8 weeks at home and 5 months

    Replicated earlier studies. Strategy training Improved ADL scores on trained and untrained tasks at hospital, home and 5 months later and was maintained over time.

    Apraxia scores also improved

  • Direct Treatment: Focused on errorless completion of ADL; Support withdrawn when client could do on own; used hand over hand - “parallel” concurrent imitation – demonstration

    ◦ 15 subjects

    ◦ 1of 3 activities trained per week followed by a an ADL test. Repeated until subjects could perform with 0 or 1 fatal error

    ◦ no improvement on untrained tasks

    ◦ 6 months later 7 were retested. Improvements were maintained in only those who practiced at home.

  • ◦ Compared direct treatment and exploratory treatment.

    ◦ Direct therapy done in intervals of 6 sessions – 1 hour each. 4 activities trained. One pair were trained with exploratory and then direct. Next pair reversed. 1 week post tested with a variant of objects used. 3 months tested using original objects

    ◦ Exploratory not effective. Direct Treatment post ADL assessment showed reduction in errors (reparable and fatal) & need for assistance

    ◦ Treatment effects were maintained on follow-up but error rates were increased with different tools.

    ◦ Concluded training should be specific to individual

  • Capitalizing on Mirror Neuron System – Action Observation, Motor Imagery and Imitation (Garrison et al, 2010)

    Wu et al., 2011: Case study combining mental and physical practice with a 44 yr. old male with hemiparesis and ideomotor apraxia◦ Tasks practiced: Reach and grasp for cup and turning pages◦ Person first physically practiced tasks for the day for 30

    mins then did 30 mins of audio guided mental practice for the task. 3xs/week, 6 weeks

    ◦ Post intervention demonstrated improved function on Arm Mobility Scale and reported improved performance and satisfaction untrained tasks (COPM).

  • Alex Milhailidis, Geoff Fernie & colleagues (http://www.iatsl.org)

  • Client’s hands, the soap and the towel tracked through the video feed from overhead camera

    Using artificial intelligence, the system can play a verbal audio prompt if (and only if) the client misses or repeats a step in the activity

    Prompt specificity automatically adjusts to suit the client’s abilities

    3rd set of clinical trials are underway, which also investigate the use of video cues

    No images are recorded and the system runs autonomously

  • Mr. P. is a 65 yr. old male with a left hemisphere bleed affecting his frontal and parietal areas. He has a mild receptive aphasia. He has difficulty producing movements in all aspects of apraxia

    testing – pantomime, imitation and object use and multiple object tests. He can recognize objects.

    During everyday tasks he misuses objects and often picks up any object to start a task.

    Currently he needs help with dressing and grooming. He can eat on his own.

    He has a 6 week length of stay. He will return to live with his wife and children but will be alone during the lunch hour. He wants to prepare lunch for himself at home.

    What is your approach?

  • 51 yr. old person with stroke affecting left parietal area

    On testing, she can pantomime to verbal command and can match objects and gestures but makes spatial and timing errors during execution of tasks. This suggests she has preserved conceptual abilities. Imitation of postures demonstrates the same types of awkward posturing. In everyday tasks such as brushing her teeth and putting sugar in her tea, she grasps the correct tool but does so incorrectly. She keeps on attempting these tasks though she is failing.

    What type of apraxia?

    What approach would you choose?

  • Interventions choices are still limited in apraxia intervention. The team are left to their own creativity to develop or co-develop strategies with the client to achieve daily living activities

    Occupation based interventions seem the most practical in current climate

    Inspired strategies for treating people with apraxia that stem from the understanding of praxis literature should be shared and evaluated.

    Bring on the clinical case reports and studies!

  • Bienkiewicz, M., Brandi, M., Goldenberg, G., et al. (2014). The tool in the brain: apraxia in ADL behavioural and neurological correlates of apraxia in daily living. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 1-13.

    Butler,(2000) J. Rehabilitation in severe ideomotor apraxia using sensory stimulation strategies: a single case design. BJOT,63, 319 –328.

    Butler, J. (1997) Intervention effectiveness: Evidence from a case study of ideomotor and ideational apraxia. BJOT, 60, 491 – 497.

    Butler, JA. (2002) How comparable are tests of apraxia? Clinical Rehabilitation, 16, 389-398.

    Buxbaum, L.J. (2001). Ideomotor Apraxia: a Call to Action. Neurocase, 7(6), 445-458.

    Buxbaum, L.J., Haaland, K.Y., Hallett, M., et al. (2008). Treatment of Limb Apraxia: Moving Forward to Improved Action. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 87(2), 149-161.

    Cantagallo, A., Maini, M. & Rumiati, R. (2012). The cognitive rehabilitation of limb apraxia in patients with stroke. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 22(3), 473-488.

    Code C, Gaunt C. Treating severe speech and limb apraxia in a case of aphasia. British Journal of Disorders of Communication 1986;21:11-20

    Cubelli R, Trentini P, Montagna CG (1991) Re-education of gestural communication in a case of chronic global aphasia and limb apraxia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 1991;8:369 –380

    Daumuller, M. (2010). Therapy to improve gestural expression in aphasia: a controlled clinical trial. Clinical rehabilitation, 24, 55-65.

    De Renzi, E. Methods of limb apraxia examination and their bearing on interpretation of the disorder.(1985) In Roy, E.A. ed. Neruopsychological Studies of Apraxia and Related Disorders. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers, 45 – 64.

    De Renzi, E. & Luchelli, F. (1988). Ideomotor apraxia. Brain, 111, 1173 – 1185.

    Donkervoort, M., J. Dekker, et al. (2001). "Efficacy of strategy training in left hemisphere stroke patients with apraxia: A randomised clinical trial." Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 11(5): 549-566.

    Donkervoort M, Dekker J, van den Ende E, Stehmann-Saris JC, Deelman BG. Prevalence of apraxia among patients with a first left hemisphere stroke in rehabilitation centres and nursing homes. Clinical Rehabilitation 2000;14:130 – 136

    Foundas A, Macauley BL, Raymer AM, Mayer LM, Heilman KM, Rothi LJG. Ecological implications of limb apraxia: evidence from mealtime behaviour. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 1995;1:62 – 66.

    Geusgens, C.A., Van Heugten, C.M., Cooijmans, J.P., et al. (2007). Transfer effects of a cognitive strategy training for stroke patients with apraxia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(8), 831-841.

    Geusgens, C., van Heugten, C., Donkervoort, M., et al. (2006). Transfer of training effects in stroke patients with apraxia: an exploratory study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 16(2), 213-229.

  • Goldenberg, G., Daumuller, M. & Hagmann, S. (2010). Assessment and therapy of complex activities of daily living in apraxia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 11(2), 147-169.

    Goldenberg, G & Hagmann, S. (1998). Therapy of activities of daily living in patients with apraxia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8. 123 – 141.

    Goldenberg. G., Daumuller, M. & Hagmann, S. (2001). Asessment and therapy of complex activities in daily living in apraxia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 11, 147- 169.

    Heilman KM, Rothi, LJG. Limb apraxia: A look back. In: Rothi LJ, Heilman KM, editors. Apraxia: The neuropsychology of action. Hove, Psychology Press, 1997: 7-18

    Heilman KM, Rothi J, Valenstein E. Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology 1982;32: 342 –346

    Heugten, C. M. v., J. Dekker, et al. (1998). "Outcome of strategy training in stroke patients with apraxia: a phase II study." Clinical Rehabilitation 12: 294-303.

    Heugten, C. M. v., J. Dekker, et al. (2000). "Rehabilitation of stroke patients with apraxia: the role of additional cognitive and motor impairments." Disability and Rehabilitation 22(12): 547-554.

    Kobayashi, S. and Y. Ugawa (2013). "Relationships between Aphasia and Apraxia." Journal of Neurology and Translational Neuroscience 2 (1)(1028): 1-5.

    Lindsten-Mcqueen, K., Weiner, N.W., Wang, H., Josman, N. & Connor, L.T. (2014). Systematic review of apraxia treatments to improve occupational performance outcomes. OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Heath, 34(4), 183-192.

    Maher, L. M. & Ochipa, C. (1997). Management and treatment of limb apraxia. In. Rothi, L.J.G. & Heilman, K. M. eds. Apraxia: The Neuropsychology of Action. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press, 75 – 91.

    Maher, L. M., Reed, E., Schwartz, M. F., Montgomery, M., & Palmer, C. (1990) Buttering a hot cup of coffee. In. Tupper, D. E & Cicerone, K. D. eds. The neuropsychology of everyday life. Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers. 259 – 285.

    Maher L M, Rothi L, JG Greenwald, M L. Treatment of gesture impairment: a single case. American Speech and Hearing Association 1991;33:195

    Pool, J. L.(1998) Effects of apraxia on the ability to learn one-handed shoe tying. The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 18, 99 – 388.

    Rothi, L.J.G & Heilmann, K. M. (1997). Apraxia: The neuropsychology of action. Hove,UK: Psychology Press

    Rothi, L.J.G., Raymer, A. M. & Heilman, K. M. (1997). Limb Praxis Assessment. In. Rothi, L.J.G. & Heilman, K. M. eds. Apraxia: The Neuropsychology of Action. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press, 61- 73.

    Rothi, L.J.G., Ochipa, C. & Heilman, K. M. (1997). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis and apraxia. In. Rothi, L.J.G. & Heilman, K. M. eds. Apraxia: The Neuropsychology of Action. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press, 29 - 49.

    Roy, E. Hand preference, manual assymentries and limb apraxia. In Elliott, D., & Roy, E. A. Manual Assymetries in Motor Performance. London: CRC Press, 215 – 236.

  • Roy, E.A. & Square, P. A. Common considerations in the study of limb, verbal and oral apraxia. In Roy, E.A. ed. NeruopsychologicalStudies of Apraxia and Related Disorders. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers, 111 – 162.

    Smania, N., Aglioti, S.M., Girardi, F., et al. (2006). Rehabilitation of limb apraxia improves daily life activities in patients with stroke. Neurology, 67(11), 2050-2052.

    Smania N, Girardi F, Domenicali C, Lora E., Aglioti S. The rehabilitation of limb apraxia: A study in left-brain-damaged patients. Archives of Physical Medicine Rehabilitation 2000;81:379 – 388

    Stamenova, V., Black, S. E., & Roy, E. A. (2012). An update on the conceptual-production systems

    model of apraxia: Evidence from stroke. Brain and Cognition, 80,53–63. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.

    2012.03.009

    Van Heugten, C. M., Dekker, J., Deelman, B. G., Stehmann-Saris, J. C., & Kinebanian, A. (2000) Rehabilitation of stroke patients with apraxia: the role of additonal cognitive and motor impairments. Disability and Rehabilitation, 22, 547 – 554.

    Van Heugten, C. M., Dekker, Deelman, B. G., Stehmann-Saris, J. C., & Kinebanian, A. (1999) Assessment of disabilities in stroke patients with apraxia: internal consistency and inter-observer reliability. The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 19, 55 –73.

    Vanbellington, T., Kersten, B., Van Hemelrijk, B., et al. (2010). European Journal of Neurology, 17(1), 59-66.

    West, C., Bowen, A., Hesketh, A. & Vail, A. (2008). Interventions for motor apraxia following stroke. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews, 1, CD004132.