17
This article was downloaded by: [York University Libraries] On: 21 November 2014, At: 21:03 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Anxiety, Stress, & Coping: An International Journal Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gasc20 Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words Hanjoo Lee a , Jennifer E. Turkel a , Stuart P. Cotter a , Jennifer M. Milliken a , Jesse Cougle b , Amy R. Goetz a & Alexandra M. Lesnick a a Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, USA b Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA Accepted author version posted online: 19 Jul 2012.Published online: 13 Aug 2012. To cite this article: Hanjoo Lee, Jennifer E. Turkel, Stuart P. Cotter, Jennifer M. Milliken, Jesse Cougle, Amy R. Goetz & Alexandra M. Lesnick (2013) Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words, Anxiety, Stress, & Coping: An International Journal, 26:5, 493-507, DOI: 10.1080/10615806.2012.713474 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2012.713474 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &

Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

This article was downloaded by: [York University Libraries]On: 21 November 2014, At: 21:03Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping: AnInternational JournalPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gasc20

Attentional bias toward personallyrelevant health-threat wordsHanjoo Leea, Jennifer E. Turkela, Stuart P. Cottera, Jennifer M.Millikena, Jesse Cougleb, Amy R. Goetza & Alexandra M. Lesnicka

a Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee,Milwaukee, WI, USAb Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee,FL, USAAccepted author version posted online: 19 Jul 2012.Publishedonline: 13 Aug 2012.

To cite this article: Hanjoo Lee, Jennifer E. Turkel, Stuart P. Cotter, Jennifer M. Milliken, JesseCougle, Amy R. Goetz & Alexandra M. Lesnick (2013) Attentional bias toward personally relevanthealth-threat words, Anxiety, Stress, & Coping: An International Journal, 26:5, 493-507, DOI:10.1080/10615806.2012.713474

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2012.713474

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever orhowsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arisingout of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &

Page 2: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 3: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

Hanjoo Leea*, Jennifer E. Turkela, Stuart P. Cottera, Jennifer M. Millikena,

Jesse Cougleb, Amy R. Goetza and Alexandra M. Lesnicka

aDepartment of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, USA;bDepartment of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA

(Received 20 December 2011; final version received 16 July 2012)

Conflicting findings have emerged regarding the presence of attentional biases(ABs) in health anxiety, probably due to methodological limitations in the stimuliused in cognitive tasks and the assessment of health anxiety-relevant factors. Thecurrent study sought to examine ABs toward health-related threats usingidiographically chosen health-threat words in a non-clinical sample. A modifieddot-probe task using idiographically selected health-threat words was adminis-tered to an undergraduate sample. Self-report measures were administered toassess somatic, cognitive, and behavioral aspects of health anxiety, in addition toassessing negative affect, anxiety sensitivity, and experience of actual medicalconditions. Results showed that behavioral and somatic aspects of health anxietywere significantly associated with AB toward personally relevant threat words,even after controlling for negative affect, anxiety sensitivity, and experience ofactual medical conditions. Additional analyses revealed that these biases reflecteddifficulty disengaging attention from threat rather than a facilitated detection ofthreat. In contrast, illness-related cognitions were found to be unrelated to ABs.These findings suggest an association between threat-related ABs and excessivehealth-care seeking efforts.

Keywords: health anxiety; hypochondriasis; attentional bias; attentional disen-gagement; dot-probe task; health-care seeking

Introduction

The core feature of hypochondriasis (HC) is an intense preoccupation with the

irrational belief that one has, or is in danger of developing, a serious medical illness

(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Individuals with HC experience excessive

worries or concerns about their suspected physical illness and engage in compulsive

self-assessment of health, reassurance seeking, and excessive utilization of medical

resources, although such strenuous efforts are highly unlikely to be successful

(Deacon, Lickel, & Abramowitz, 2008). Currently, HC is classified as a somatoform

disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000); However, several authors

(Deacon & Abramowitz, 2008; Noyes, 2001; Olatunji et al., 2009) have proposed

that HC may be better conceptualized as severe health anxiety due to a number of

shared clinical features and elevated comorbidity between HC and anxiety disorders,

particularly panic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. One recent study

found that patients with HC display elevated cognitive biases such as attentional

*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 2013

Vol. 26, No. 5, 493�507, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2012.713474

# 2013 Taylor & Francis

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 4: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

vigilance to bodily symptoms, which is central to other anxiety disorders (Deacon &

Abramowitz, 2008).

Similar to cognitive models of anxiety disorders (e.g., Clark, 1986; Salkovskis,

1985), current cognitive-behavioral conceptualizations of HC (Abramowitz,

Schwartz, & Whiteside, 2002; Warwick & Salkovskis, 1990) propose a debilitating

self-sustaining cycle of pathological health anxiety. Individuals with heightenedhealth anxiety are thought to hold dysfunctional beliefs about their bodily symptoms

and possible illnesses. Such beliefs can result in hypervigilance toward (benign)

bodily symptoms and amplified somatosensory perceptions, which may increase

illness-related catastrophic cognitions and strenuous efforts to use safety-seeking

behaviors. The current model posits that cognitive biases (e.g., distorted perceptions

of bodily sensations, vigilant attention toward physical cues, enhanced memory of

illness-related information) are at the center of various mechanisms that maintain

pathological health anxiety. Particularly, attentional vigilance toward potential

health threats appears to be a maintaining factor that may lead an individual to

selectively process negative aspects of health-related information and develop

irrational illness-related beliefs. Attentional biases (ABs) toward threatening

information have been well-documented in emotional disorders (MacLeod, Math-

ews, & Tata, 1986; Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996), and health anxiety may

not be an exception.

Only a handful of experimental studies have examined AB associated with healthanxiety, and existing data are mixed (see Marcus, Gurley, Marchi, & Bauer, 2007

for a review). Individuals with heightened health anxiety were found to show

greater interference effects toward illness-related stimuli on modified Stroop tasks

(Karademas, Christopoulou, Dimostheni, & Pavlu, 2008; Owens, Asmundson,

Hadjistavropoulos, & Owens, 2004), which suggests that these individuals are less

efficient in preventing attentional interference by task-irrelevant health-threat

information. Another study found that during a bogus pain-delivery task, individuals

with health anxiety showed greater attention toward bodily symptoms, whereas no

such AB was observed among those low in health anxiety (Pauli, Schwenzer, Brody,

& Rau, 1993). However, other studies have been unsuccessful in providing evidence

for AB in health anxiety. For example, Lees, Mogg, and Bradley (2003) found that

those high vs. low in health anxiety did not differ in performance on a visual probe

task, although the magnitude of AB was shown to be greater among individuals with

elevated anxiety sensitivity. Similarly, Lecci and Cohen (2002) failed to find

significant emotional Stroop interference effects unless participants’ illness concerns

were activated (i.e., participants were led to believe that their blood pressure was

‘‘dangerously high’’). Taken together, AB in health anxiety has not been demon-strated consistently across studies, although attentional vigilance is one of the core

components of the current cognitive model of health anxiety (Abramowitz et al.,

2002; Warwick & Salkovskis, 1990). Therefore, AB research is an important step

toward elucidating its underlying cognitive features and guiding the development of

effective cognitive interventions for health anxiety.

There are a few methodological issues that are important to consider to

improving the design of AB research in health anxiety. First, given the vast range

of potential health concerns across individuals, cognitive tasks that present generic

health threats may not be sufficiently sensitive to detect biased attentional processing

(e.g., an individual with concerns over having a heart attack may not display

494 H. Lee et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 5: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

attentional vigilance toward threatening information related to cancer). It is

important to assess AB toward idiographically chosen health-threat words/pictures

that are relevant to one’s own health concerns. Second, most existing studies (e.g.,

Karademas et al., 2008; Lees et al., 2003; Owens et al., 2004) assess the severity of

health anxiety using measures that are largely focused on cognitive aspects of health

anxiety (e.g., Illness Attitude Scale, Kellner, 1986; Health Anxiety Inventory,

Salkovskis, Rimes, Warwick, & Clark, 2002) rather than instruments that assess

other dimensions of the condition including behavioral (e.g., efforts to seek health-

care resources or illness-related information) and somatic symptoms (e.g., com-

plaints of physical symptoms including palpitations, shortness of breath and chest

pain). Including measures that address behavioral, somatic, and cognitive dimen-

sions of health anxiety would allow investigators to examine how various aspects of

health anxiety are linked to attentional vigilance toward threatening health

information.

The current study sought to examine the association between health anxiety and

attentional vigilance, using a well-established cognitive experimental paradigm (i.e.,

dot-probe task) that has been highly successful in revealing ABs in emotional

disorders (MacLeod et al., 1986). To this end, we generated an individually tailored

dot-probe task using personally relevant health-threat words for each participant, and

assessed how such cognitive biases would be associated with various factors related

to health anxiety. We hypothesized that the severity of health anxiety assessed in

somatic (i.e., current physical symptoms), cognitive (i.e., illness-related negative

cognitions), and behavioral (i.e., efforts toward health-care utilization and informa-

tion-seeking) aspects would be significantly associated with the magnitude of

attentional vigilance toward idiographically chosen health-threats. In addition, we

sought to explore the associations between the three domains of health anxiety and

AB to threatening health information.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from undergraduate psychology classes at a large mid-

western university. We sought to obtain a study sample characterized by a range of

health anxiety scores on the Short Health Anxiety Inventory (SHAI; Salkovskis

et al., 2002). To achieve this recruitment goal, 326 students were screened and 40

students were randomly selected from each of the four quartiles on total scores of the

SHAI and invited to participate in the current study. Of the 160 individuals invited to

the study, a total of 56 responded and participated in return for partial course credit.

Survey responders were evenly distributed across the four quartiles. A one-sample

Kolmogorov-Smirnov test confirmed that SHAI Total scores were normally

distributed in the current sample (Kolomogorov-Smirnov z�.77, p�.59;

Mean�37.82, SD�10.31, Range�19�65). Our sample included 20 males and 36

females (64.3%). Their mean age was 21.16 (SD�2.95) and there were no significant

gender differences. They reported the following racial/ethnic characteristics (with

multiple selections allowed): Caucasian (80.4%), Asian (12.5%), African-American/

Black (10.7%), Hispanic (5.4%), and Pacific Islander (1.8%).

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 495

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 6: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

Measures

Short Health Anxiety Inventory

The SHAI is a widely-used self-report measure of health anxiety, consisting of 18

items that assess negative cognitions associated with health anxiety such as perceived

likelihood of illness and negative consequences of illness (Abramowitz, Deacon, &

Valentiner, 2007; Salkovskis et al., 2002). Each item presents four statements that

vary in severity (e.g., 0: I do not worry about my health � 3: I spend most of my time

worrying about my health), and respondents are asked to select one of the four

statements that best describes their feelings over the past week. The SHAI has

adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s a�.86) and test�retest reliability

(Abramowitz et al., 2007; Salkovskis et al., 2002). The measure is an abbreviated

version of the 64-item Health Anxiety Inventory (HAI; Salkovskis et al., 2002). The

HAI was administered to participants to examine the relevancy of the author-

constructed Health Care Seeking and Utilization Scale (see below).

Patient Health Questionnaire � Somatoform Module (PHQ)

The PHQ is a self-report instrument designed to be used in primary care settings to

diagnose common DSM-IV disorders (Somatoform; Spitzer, Kroenke, & Williams,

1999). The PHQ screens for five groups of disorders frequently present in primary care

settings: Depression, Anxiety, Alcohol Use, Somatoform, and Eating Disorders. The

PHQ showed acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s a�.79) among patients who

display moderate levels of somatization (Interian, Allen, Gara, Escobar, & Diaz-

Martinez, 2006). The PHQ-Somatoform section assesses 15 physical symptoms in the

past month on a 3-point scale (0�not bothered at all, 1�bothered a little, 2�bothered

a lot). We used the PHQ-Somatoform module to examine the severity of various

physical complaints experienced over the previous month.

In Section 2 of the PHQ, we administered a list of physical illnesses to examine

the presence of actual medical conditions participants have experienced in the past or

currently (i.e., last four weeks). The list included seven serious illnesses (i.e., heart

disease, respiratory disease, active ulcer, kidney disease, epilepsy, stroke, and head

injury). Additionally, participants were allowed to indicate up to three medical

conditions that were not included in the list, which resulted in reports of fiveadditional medical conditions (i.e., liver disease, cancer, sickle cell disease, spine

problem, and anemia). Overall, 28.5% of the current sample indicated that they have

experienced at least one of these medical conditions. Given the chronic nature of

these conditions, we produced a lifetime prevalence-based index that indicates the

number of actual medical conditions they have experienced (i.e., PHQ-Medical

Conditions).

Health Care Seeking and Utilization Scale (HCSUS)

This is an author-constructed 7-item self-report measure designed to measure

behavioral aspects of health anxiety. The seven items assessed the level of efforts

made to seek and utilize health-care resources over the past month including: (1) the

number of times the individual made a phone call to health-care professionals

(e.g., physician, nurse, counselor), (2) the number of times the individual visited

496 H. Lee et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 7: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

non-emergent health care settings (e.g., clinic, hospital, student health center), (3) the

number of times the individual visited an emergency room, (4) the number of

physicians the individual has seen, (5) the number of prescriptions the individual has

received, (6) the number of days the individual has received inpatient treatment, and

(7) the number of hours per week the individual spent checking and collecting

information on his/her potential or diagnosed illness on the Internet. Given the

unstandardized format of the response scales across the seven items, we generated a

summary index by averaging z-scores generated from each item. In the current

sample, internal consistency (Cronbach’s a) for the seven items was .77.

To demonstrate the utility of the HCSUS as a measure of behavioral health

anxiety symptoms, we examined its association with (1) the 64-item HAI and its

relevant subscales (i.e., Reassurance-seeking and Avoidance), and (2) the PHQ-

Medical Conditions index. We found that the HCSUS z-scores were significantly

associated with the HAI-Reassurance subscale (r�.39, pB.005) and HAI-Total

score (r�.39, pB.005), but were not associated with the HAI-Avoidance subscale

(r�.02, p�.91). More importantly, the HCSUS z-scores were not significantly

associated with the PHQ-Medical Conditions Index (r�.22, p�.10). These data

indicate that elevated scores on the HCSUS reflect health anxiety-driven reassurance

seeking and preoccupation, rather than proper health-care seeking efforts propor-

tionate to actual medical conditions. Compared to other measures of health-related

behaviors (e.g., Illness Attitudes Scale � Kellner, 1986; Medical Utilization

Questionnaire � Abramowitz et al., 2007), which mix different facets of health

anxiety symptoms (e.g., various safety behaviors, somatic complaints, concerns

about pain, and clinical impairment), the HCSUS is expected to provide a more pure

assessment of behavioral domains of health anxiety (i.e., health-care seeking

motivated for reassurance).

Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS)

The DASS assesses three negative emotional states including depression, anxiety, and

stress on a 4-point Likert-type scale (0�Did not apply to me at all � 3�Applied to

me very much or most of the time). Acceptable reliability has been found for the

Depression, Anxiety, and Stress scales (Cronbach’s a�.91, .84, and .90, respectively)

in a non-clinical sample (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). The total and subscale scores

have also demonstrated excellent construct validity (Henry & Crawford, 2005). The

depression and anxiety subscales were included in the current study to index severity

of negative affect.

Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3)

The ASI-3 is an 18-item self-report measure assessing the fear of anxiety-related

arousal and sensations based on a 5-point Likert-type scale (0�very little � 4�very

much). The scale contains three subscales: Physical, Social, and Cognitive Concerns.

The ASI-3 has shown good psychometric properties, including adequate internal

consistency (Cronbach’s a; Physical�.76, Social�.78, Cognitive�.86) and con-

struct validity in a nonclinical sample (Taylor et al., 2007).

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 497

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 8: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

The Health Threat Word Rating Form

We constructed a rating form containing 106 potentially threatening health-related

words (e.g., ache, blood, cancer, coronary, vomit) adapted from existing studies

(Ferguson, Moghaddam, & Bibby, 2007; Owens et al., 2004; Roberts, Hart, &

Eastwood, 2010) and the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW; Bradley &

Lang, 1999). Participants were asked to indicate how threatening they found the

meaning of each word based on a 7-point scale (0�Not at all � 6�Extremely). Thisrating form was developed to individualize the dot-probe task by identifying

personally relevant health-threat words for each participant. In the current sample,

internal consistency coefficient for this measure was .98.

Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview 5.0 � Hypochondriasis Module

(MINI-HC)

The MINI is a widely used clinician-administered brief structured diagnostic

interview for DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and ICD-10 (World

Health Organization, 1992). Its validity has been established against existing well-validated structured diagnostic interviews such as the Structured Clinical Interview

for DSM-III-R Patient Version and the Composite International Diagnostic

Interview for ICD-10 (Sheehan et al., 1998). We administered the HC module to

examine whether the severity range of health anxiety in the current study would also

encompass the clinical threshold level that warrants the diagnosis of HC based on

DSM-IV criteria. The MINI interviewers were thoroughly trained by completing a

workshop offered by one of the original developers of the MINI, and received

20 hours of training by the first author (HJL). Although inter-rater reliability wasnot formally examined, each interview case was reviewed at weekly supervision

meetings, and there was 100% agreement on the diagnosis of HC (n�6) in the

current sample.

Dot-probe task

We generated a dot-probe task tailored for each participant, using the 20 most

threatening words endorsed on the Health Threat Word Rating Form. Each of these

words was matched with a neutral word of the same length in order to form 20 pairsof health-threat versus neutral words. Neutral words were selected and adapted from

the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW; Bradley & Lang, 1999). Thus, the

health-threat words and neutral words included in the dot-probe task varied across

participants. Each dot-probe trial proceeded in the following sequence: First, a small

fixation cross was presented at the center of a 17-inch LCD monitor for 500 ms.

Second, a pair of words was presented for 500 ms immediately following the fixation

cross disappearance. One word appeared 1.1 cm above, and the other word appeared

1.1 cm below the position of the central fixation cross. Following the pair of words, aletter probe (‘‘E’’ or ‘‘F’’) appeared in one of the two word locations, and stayed on-

screen until the participant pressed the response key (either ‘‘E’’ or ‘‘F’’) to identify

the letter. The inter-trial interval was 1500 ms.

The task began with eight practice trials, which presented neutral�neutral pairs

of words. A total of 160 dot-probe trials were then administered over two blocks of

498 H. Lee et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 9: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

80 trials each. Of 160 trials, 80% (�128 trials) presented an idiographically chosen

health-threat word paired with a neutral word. The other 20% (�32) of trials

presented neutral�neutral word pairs, which enables us to clarify whether attentional

vigilance is characterized by facilitated detection of threatening information ordifficulty in disengaging attention from the threatening information (Koster,

Crombez, Verschuere, & De Houwer, 2004). The thematic category of words (i.e.,

health-threat vs. neutral) and the type of letter probes (‘‘E’’ vs. ‘‘F’’) were balanced

between the two stimulus positions in the current dot-probe task.

From the dot-probe task, we generated the AB index to evaluate the nature and

magnitude of ABs in response to health-threat words as compared with neutral

words, using the following formula:

Attentional Bias ðABÞ Index ¼ Average reaction time ðRTÞcomputed from the trials presenting the letter probe on the neutral

word position� Average RT computed from the trials presenting

the letter probe on the health� threat word position:

If the individual’s attentional allocation is biased toward health-threat words, RTs

should be shorter when the probe appears on the threat word position rather than

the neutral word position. Alternatively, if the individual diverts attention away

from health-threat words, RTs should be longer when the probe appears on the

health-threat word position. Thus, the above AB index is proportionate to thedegree of the individual’s AB toward health-threat words. In contrast, negative

values of the AB index reflect attentional avoidance from health-threat words.

Furthermore, the nature of ABs (e.g., facilitated detection of health-threat cues

vs. difficulty in disengaging attention from them) can be further examined by

comparison with reference RTs produced from the trials displaying neutral�neutral word pairs. For example, if RTs toward neutral words (paired with

health-threat words) are longer than the reference RTs, the positive AB index

suggests difficulty in disengaging attention from health-threat words. In contrast,if RTs toward health-threat words (paired with neutral words) are shorter than

the reference RT, the positive AB index indicates facilitated detection of heath-

threat words.

Procedures

After providing informed consent, each participant was interviewed to determine

diagnostic status based on the MINI-HC. Next, a computerized battery of

instruments was administered with the Health Threat Word Rating Form as the

first measure. Upon the completion of the rating form, the experimenter produced an

individually tailored dot-probe task using a pre-programmed task generationtemplate (which allowed the task to be built quickly incorporating the chosen

health-threat words and matched neutral words), while the participant filled out the

remaining measures in the battery. After a 10-minute break, the participant

completed the dot-probe task as the final step of the single-session assessment

procedure.

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 499

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 10: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

Data analytic strategy

We conducted hierarchical regression analyses to examine potential predictors of AB

toward health-threat words. In Step 1, DASS-Depression and Anxiety subscales were

entered to control for the influence of negative affect. This was designed to test

whether the AB index would be uniquely explained by health anxiety-relevant factors

beyond the contribution of negative affect. Although negative affect may be an

inseparable component of health anxiety, this analytic approach is important giventhe wealth of research that has demonstrated ABs linked to depression and anxiety

(see Cisler & Koster, 2010; Koster, De Raedt, Goeleven, Franck, & Crombez, 2005).

Anxiety sensitivity (as assessed by the ASI-3) was also entered into Step 1 to examine

whether health anxiety-related AB would merely be a function of varying levels of

anxiety sensitivity. In Step 2, the PHQ-Medical Conditions index was entered to

examine whether observed ABs would be the result of the presence of actual medical

conditions rather than health anxiety variables. In Step 3, health anxiety-relevant

factors were entered, including (1) the severity of illness-related cognitions (SHAI-Total score), (2) the severity of various physical complaints (PHQ-Somatoform), and

(3) the efforts to seek and utilize health-care resources (HCSUS � z score). This

analytic strategy allowed us to test the pattern of association between various health

anxiety factors and AB toward health-threat words after controlling for other

relevant variables.

Results

Preparation of RT data

The current sample’s overall correct response rate on the dot-probe task was 96.9%.

In generating the RT data from the dot-probe task, RTs from incorrect trials were

removed. Additionally, RTs falling outside the range of mean92 SD (1.0%) were

excluded to reduce the influence of outliers. Thus, the AB index was generated using

95.9% of the trials.

Basic clinical characteristics of the current sample

Table 1 presents basic clinical characteristics of the current sample, as measured bythe SHAI, ASI-3, DASS, PHQ, and HCSUS. Kolmogorov-Smirnov test revealed

that all subscale and total scores of the study measures and the AB index were

normally distributed. The MINI-HC module showed that six individuals met DSM-

IV diagnostic criteria for HC, and these individuals scored significantly higher on the

SHAI than those without HC: means (SDs)�51.83 (10.32) vs. 36.14 (9.03),

t(54)�3.97, pB.001. Zero-order correlations among the study measures and their

correlations with the AB index are presented in Table 1.

Predicting the AB index with health anxiety-relevant variables

We conducted hierarchical regression analyses to examine the contribution of

various aspects of health anxiety symptoms in predicting AB toward health-threat

words paired with neutral words. None of the variables entered in the first two

steps significantly predicted attention bias (R2DB.03, FsB1.4). In Step 3, SHAI,

500 H. Lee et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 11: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

PHQ-Somatoform, and HCSUS scores explained an additional 28.6% of the

variance (R2D�.29, F(3,46)�6.65, pB.001). In the model, the HCSUS emerged

as the strongest predictor of AB toward health-threat words (b�.46, t�3.24,

pB.005). PHQ-Somatoform was also predictive of the AB index (b�.32, t�2.04,

pB.05). In contrast, SHAI-Total was not a significant predictor of the AB index

(b�.12, t�.73, p�.47).1 Overall, the model explained 34.0% of the variance in AB

toward health-threat words. Multicollinearity was not an issue for this regression

model, including the three health anxiety factors entered in Step 3 (their VIF values

ranged between 1.42 and 1.86).

Specific pattern of AB toward threatening health words: facilitated detection or delayedattentional disengagement?

To specify the nature of attentional vigilance associated with health-care seeking

efforts, we examined the top and bottom 30% scorers on the HCSUS (see Figure 1).

A t-test revealed that the high health-care seekers displayed a significantly greater AB

index score (� vigilance toward the health-threat words), relative to the low health-

care seekers (see Table 2): t(33)�2.29, p�.029, Cohen’s d�.80. Among the top 30%

scorers, paired t-tests revealed that the average RT to neutral words (paired with

health-threat words) was significantly longer than the reference RT: means

(SDs)�505 ms (54) vs. 495 ms (59), t(16)�2.59, pB.05; whereas the average RT

to health-threat words was not different from the reference RT: t(16)�.75, p�.46. In

contrast, low health-care seekers (bottom 30% scorers) showed a longer RT to

health-threat words (paired with neutral words), relative to the reference RT: 492 ms

(37) vs. 483 ms (37): t(17)�2.74, pB.05. The average RT to neutral words (paired

with health-threat words) did not differ from the reference RT: t(17)�1.35, p�.20.

Taken together, these data suggest that attentional vigilance observed among high

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlational analyses of self-report measures and attention

bias index (N�56).

M (SD)

Attention Bias

Index 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. DASS-

Depression

12.75 (9.68) .03 �

2. DASS-Anxiety 8.82 (7.67) .08 .60*** �3. ASI-Total 20.82 (13.61) �.03 .46*** .77*** �4. SHAI-Total 37.82 (10.31) .24 .51*** .52*** .50*** �5. PHQ-

Somatoform

6.88 (4.57) .26 .51*** .61*** .53*** .44** �

6. HCSUS 5.04 (4.20) .48*** .35** .20 .01 .43** .23 �7. PHQ-Medical

Conditions

.36 (.62) .14 .38** .16 .18 .35** .29* .22

Note: DASS, Depression Anxiety Stress Scale; ASI, Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3; SHAI, Short HealthAnxiety Inventory; PHQ-Somatoform, Patient Health Questionnaire-Somatoform Module; HCSUS,Health Care Seeking and Utilization Scale; PHQ-Medical Conditions, Experience of actual medicalconditions.*pB.05, **pB.01, ***pB.001

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 501

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 12: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

health-care seekers is characterized by difficulty in disengaging attention from

health-threat words rather than facilitated attentional shifts.

Discussion

Considerable evidence has accumulated over the past few decades that anxious

individuals show attentional vigilance toward emotionally threatening information

(Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van Ijzendoorn, 2007;

Cisler & Koster, 2010; MacLeod et al., 1986; Mogg & Bradley, 1998; Williams et al.,

1996). The significant role of AB in anxiety disorders is also indicated by a recently

emerging line of research that has demonstrated promising therapeutic effects of

Figure 1. Reaction times across the different types of dot-probe trials among high versus low

health-care seekers.

*Error bars�standard error of the mean.

Table 2. Descriptive data and attention bias scores from the dot-probe task.

All participants

(n�56)

Low health care

seekers (n�18)

High health care

seekers (n�17)

Errora RT (ms) Error RT (ms) Error

RT (ms)

M (SD)

Mean

(SD)

Mean

(SD)

Mean

(SD)

Mean

(SD)

Mean

(SD)

Probe on the health-

threat word position

486 (51) 2.11 (1.98) 492 (37) 1.72 (1.32) 495 (59) 2.29 (1.86)

Probe on the neutral

word position

488 (47) 1.78 (2.06) 488 (33) 1.22 (1.51) 505 (54) 1.76 (2.36)

Neutral�neutral pair

condition

482 (48) 1.09 (1.50) 483 (37) .83 (.86) 490 (56) 1.24 (1.64)

Attention bias scoresb 2.29 (16.33) �3.54 (12.04) 9.97 (21.80)

aError values indicate the sum of errors made in each section of the task.bAttention bias scores�RT on trials with the probe on neutral word position � RT on trials with the probeon health-threat word position.

502 H. Lee et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 13: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

attention-modification procedures on reducing anxiety symptoms (e.g., Amir, Beard,

Burns, & Bomyea, 2009; Amir, Weber, Beard, Bomyea, & Taylor, 2008; Schmidt,

Richey, Buckner, & Timpano, 2009). Several authors have proposed that HC may be

better conceptualized as an excessive health anxiety condition that shares many

phenomenological features with other anxiety disorders (Deacon & Abramowitz,

2008; Noyes, 2001; Olatunji et al., 2009), however, our current knowledge regarding

attentional processing underlying health anxiety is limited.The current study sought to examine the nature of attentional processing

associated with health anxiety, with two important methodological strengths

including: (1) employing a dot-probe task using idiographically selected health-

threat words, and (2) examining AB in light of various health anxiety-relevant

factors (i.e., cognitive, somatic, and behavioral features) while controlling for other

relevant variables, including anxiety sensitivity, negative affect, and experience of

actual medical conditions. Overall, our data show that behavioral (i.e., efforts to seek

and utilize health-care resources) and somatic (i.e., complaints of various physical

symptoms) aspects of health anxiety were significantly associated with AB toward

personally relevant health-threat words even after controlling for the aforementioned

variables.

Among the three domains of health anxiety, health-care seeking efforts emerged

as the strongest predictor of attentional vigilance toward health-threat cues.

Specifically, those who reported elevated efforts to seek health-care resourcesdisplayed greater difficulty in disengaging attention from threatening health

information, whereas those low in health-care seeking efforts tended to divert

attention away from threatening health information. Excessive reassurance seeking

through health-care resources, in the absence of medical evidence for a physical

illness, is a common behavioral feature of HC (e.g., ‘‘doctor-shopping’’ or strenuous

efforts to find a health care professional who can (dis)confirm one’s fears about a

physical condition), and is considered a counter-productive behavior that may

reinforce health anxiety (Abramowitz & Deacon, 2004). Consequently, prolonged

health care seeking and continuous search for the absence (or presence) of somatic

signs of abnormalities may amplify the individual’s emotional sensitivity to potential

health threats and AB toward them. Alternatively, it may be that difficulty in

attentional disengagement renders the individual’s attentional resources fixated

toward threatening health information, and thereby sustaining concerns about the

suspected illness and increasing subsequent efforts for health care and reassurance

seeking. The cross-sectional nature of our data preclude the conclusion as to whether

attentional vigilance to potential health threats is a cause or consequence of

prolonged health-care seeking efforts. Further research is needed to clarify the role ofAB and excessive health care seeking in the maintenance of health anxiety. It is

noteworthy that individuals displaying low health-care seeking efforts showed an

opposite pattern of attentional processing (i.e., avoidance from potential health-

threat cues). Further research is needed for the possibility that low health-care

seekers may exhibit a protective/regulatory mode of attentional processing that

minimizes cognitive processing of threatening health information (without regard to

the experience of actual medical conditions).

It should also be noted that the health-care seeking efforts, assessed by the

HCSUS, were significantly associated with reassurance seeking efforts and overall

health concerns, but not with the experience of chronic bona fide medical conditions

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 503

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 14: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

or general anxiety. The pattern of associations between the HCSUS and other study

measures provide support for its utility as a relevant measure of behavioral aspects of

health anxiety. This also indicates that the current findings on the association

between AB and health-care seeking efforts (assessed by the HCSUS) are unlikely to

be explained by the individual’s personal experience of actual medical conditions. In

future research, it will be important to assess the validity of the HCSUS against other

similar measures (e.g., Medical Utilization Questionnaire � Abramowitz et al., 2007)to establish its validity as a self-report index that reflects hypochondriacal behavior

as opposed to legitimate health-care seeking and utilization.

Cognitive symptoms in health anxiety (as assessed by the SHAI) did not

significantly predict AB toward health-threat words, even though stimuli were

chosen idiographically to increase the personal relevance of the dot-probe task.

Similarly, Lees and colleagues (2003) did not find ABs as a function of health anxiety

during a visual probe task. Lecci and Cohen (2002) also found that slowed Stroop RTs

toward health-threat words were associated with the somatic aspect (i.e., elevated

sensitivity to bodily sensations), but not with the cognitive aspect (i.e., illness

preoccupation and worry) of hypochondriacal tendencies. Unlike behavioral or

somatic features of health anxiety, negative illness-related cognitions might reflect

entrenched beliefs that are relatively unaffected by specific patterns of attentional

allocation at earlier stages of information processing. However, given the central role

of AB in current cognitive models of health anxiety (Abramowitz et al., 2002;

Warwick & Salkovskis, 1990), further research is warranted on the associationbetween AB toward potential health threats and illness-related beliefs.

The current findings have some important clinical implications. First, given

growing evidence for the critical role of AB in the maintenance and treatment of other

anxiety disorders (e.g., Amir et al., 2009; Schmidt et al., 2009), cognitive interventions

that directly target reducing attentional vigilance toward potential health threats may

yield a beneficial therapeutic effect. Specifically, our data hint the possibility that

reducing attentional vigilance to health-threat cues is likely to be associated with

therapeutic changes in a behavioral domain of health anxiety such as maladaptive

health-care seeking efforts. Second, our data suggest that excessive health care seeking

is an important variable that is linked to underlying cognitive biases, and also serves

the purpose of gaining reassurance that may contribute to sustaining health anxiety.

The intervention for health anxiety needs to aim at decreasing excessive health-care

seeking behavior as one of the primary treatment targets.

Some limitations of the study and considerations for future research should be

noted. This study used a non-clinical student sample displaying a wide range of healthanxiety symptoms, assuming a linear association between levels of AB and health

anxiety on a continuum. This limits the generalizability of the findings as individuals

with severely impairing HC may display a different pattern of AB and its cognitive and

behavioral correlates. Other limitations concern the use of the dot-probe task, which

consisted of only word stimuli. A different mode of stimuli (e.g., pictorial health-threat

cues) may trigger a different pattern of attentional processing. For example, Lees et al.

(2003) found an AB that was associated with anxiety sensitivity on a dot-probe task

using pictorial cues, but not on a task based on illness-related words. Relatedly, the

current dot-probe task did not include general emotional threats (e.g., dysphoric

emotional cues) because we aimed to test AB associated with personally relevant health-

threat words. Inclusion of general threat words would have made it difficult to interpret

504 H. Lee et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 15: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

whether the observed AB resulted from specific threat content (i.e., health threat) or

from personal relevance. Nonetheless, it is important to further investigate whether

individuals with heightened health anxiety would display ABs toward general

emotional threat. Additionally, the current study did not take into consideration a

potential threat-induced slow-down effect, which might have contributed to the

difficulty in attentional disengagement from health-threat words (observed in high

health-care seekers). Mogg, Holmes, Garner, and Bradley (2008) demonstrated that

differences in RT between threat and neutral cues cannot be assumed to provide pure

measures of shift or disengagement components of AB because of the threat-induced

response slowing. Future investigation may include trials that can index threat-induced

response slowing effects by removing the need for attentional shift (e.g., a central cue

task; see Mogg et al., 2008), which would produce a more pure index of attentional

shift vs. disengagement related to health anxiety.

In sum, the current data suggest that excessive health-care seeking efforts and

somatic complaints are significantly associated with attentional vigilance toward

personally relevant health-threat information. Particularly, individuals displaying

elevated efforts for health-care resource seeking showed difficulty in disengaging

attention from health-threat cues. It is imperative to further investigate the nature of

cognitive processing underlying health anxiety in order to gain better knowledge of

its maintenance and develop effective cognitive interventions, including attention-

modification procedures, for the reduction of symptoms.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Research Growth Initiative Grant from the University ofWisconsin-Milwaukee awarded to Hanjoo Lee.

Note

1. Additionally, we examined whether the binary diagnostic status of HC was significantlyassociated with the AB index, although only six individuals met DSM-IV criteria for thiscondition. Results showed that, similar to the overall level of health anxiety reported on theSHAI, the diagnostic status of HC did not significantly predict the magnitude of the ABindex.

References

Abramowitz, J.S., & Deacon, B. (2004). Severe health anxiety: Why it persists and how to treatit. Comprehensive Therapy, 30, 44�49. doi: 10.1007/s12019-004-0023-1

Abramowitz, J.S., Deacon, B., & Valentiner, D. (2007). The Short Health Anxiety Inventory:Psychometric properties and construct validity in a non-clinical sample. Cognitive Therapyand Research, 31, 871�883. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9058-1

Abramowitz, J.S., Schwartz, S.A., & Whiteside, S.P. (2002). A contemporary conceptual modelof hypochondriasis. Mayo Clinic Preecedings, 77, 1323�1330. doi: 10.1016/S0025-6196(11)62432-4

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders(4th ed., text rev). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Amir, N., Beard, C., Burns, M., & Bomyea, J. (2009). Attention modification program inindividuals with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 28�33.doi: 10.1037/a0012589

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 505

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 16: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

Amir, N., Weber, G., Beard, C., Bomyea, J., & Taylor, C.T. (2008). The effect of a single-sessionattention modification program on response to a public-speaking challenge in sociallyanxious individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 860�868. doi: 10.1037/a0013445

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van Ijzendoorn,M.H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals:A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1�24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

Bradley, M.M., & Lang, P.J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Stimuli,instruction manual and affective ratings (pp. 4�45, Technical report C-1). Gainesville, FL:The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

Cisler, J.M., & Koster, E.H.W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat inanxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 203�216. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003

Clark, D.M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 461�470. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90011-2

Deacon, B., & Abramowitz, J.S. (2008). Is hypochondriasis related to obsessive compulsive-disorder, panic disorder, or both? An empirical evaluation. Journal of CognitivePsychotherapy, 22, 115�127. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.22.2.115

Deacon, B., Lickel, J., & Abramowitz, J.S. (2008). Medical utilization across the anxietydisorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 344�350. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.03.004

Ferguson, E., Moghaddam, N.G., & Bibby, P.A. (2007). Memory bias in health anxiety isrelated to the emotional valence of health-related words. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,62, 263�274. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.01.015

Henry, J.D., & Crawford, J.R. (2005). The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety StressScales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample.British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 227�239. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29657

Interian, A., Allen, L.A., Gara, M.A., Escobar, J.I., & Diaz-Martinez, A.M. (2006). Somaticcomplaints in primary care: Further examining the validity of the Patient HealthQuestionnaire (PHQ-15). Psychosomatics, 47, 392�398. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.47.5.392

Karademas, E.C., Christopoulou, S., Dimostheni, A., & Pavlu, F. (2008). Health anxiety andcognitive interference: Evidence from the application of a modified Stroop task in twostudies. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1138�1150. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.007

Kellner, R. (1986). Somatization and Hypochondriasis. New York, NY: Praeger.Koster, E.H.W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention to

threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1183�1192. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001

Koster, E.H.W., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional Bias in Dysphoria: Maintained attention to and impaired disengage-ment from negative information. Emotion, 5, 446�455. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.446

Lecci, L., & Cohen, D.J. (2002). Perceptual consequences of an illness-concern induction andits relation to hypochondriacal tendencies. Health Psychology, 21, 147�156. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.21.2.147

Lees, A., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B.P. (2003). Health anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, and attentionalbiases for pictorial and linguistic health-threat cues. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 453�462.doi: 10.1080/02699930441000184

Lovibond, P.F., & Lovibond, S.H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states:Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depressionand Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 335�343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U

MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journalof Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15�20. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15

Marcus, D.K., Gurley, J.R., Marchi, M.M., & Bauer, C. (2007). Cognitive and perceptualvariables in hypochondriasis and health anxiety: A systematic review. Clinical PsychologyReview, 27, 127�139. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.003

Mogg, K., & Bradley, B.P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. BehaviourResearch and Therapy, 36, 809�848. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00063-1

506 H. Lee et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 17: Attentional bias toward personally relevant health-threat words

Mogg, K., Holmes, A., Garner, M., & Bradley, B. (2008). Effects of threat cues on attentionalshifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals. Behaviour Researchand Therapy, 46(5), 656�667. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.011

Noyes, R., Jr. (2001). Epidemiology of hypochondriasis. In V. Starcevic & D.R. Lipsitt (Eds.),Hypochondriasis: Modern perspectives on an ancient malady (pp. 127�154). New York, NY:Oxford University Press.

Olatunji, B.O., Wolitzky-Taylor, K.B., Elwood, L., Connolly, K., Gonzales, B., & Armstrong,T. (2009). Anxiety sensitivity and health anxiety in a nonclinical sample: Specificity andprospective relations with clinical stress. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33, 416�424. doi:10.1007/s10608-008-9188-8

Owens, K.M.B., Asmundson, G.J.G., Hadjistavropoulos, T., & Owens, T.J. (2004). Attentionalbias toward illness threat in individuals with elevated health anxiety. Cognitive Therapy andResearch, 28, 57�66. doi: 10.1023/B:COTR.0000016930.85884.29

Pauli, P., Schwenzer, M., Brody, S., & Rau, H. (1993). Hypochondriacal attitudes, painsensitivity, and attentional bias. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 37, 745�752. doi:10.1016/0022-3999(93)90103-M

Roberts, K.E., Hart, T.A., & Eastwood, J.D. (2010). Attentional biases to social and healththreat words in individuals with and without high social anxiety or depression. CognitiveTherapy and Research, 34, 388�399. doi: 10.1007/s10608-009-9245-y

Salkovskis, P.M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 571�583. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-6

Salkovskis, P.M., Rimes, K.A., Warwick, H.M.C., & Clark, D.M. (2002). The Health AnxietyInventory: Development and validation of scales for the measurement of health anxiety andhypochondriasis. Psychological Medicine: A Journal of Research in Psychiatry and the AlliedSciences, 32, 843�853. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702005822

Schmidt, N.B., Richey, J.A., Buckner, J.D., & Timpano, K.R. (2009). Attention training forgeneralized social anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 5�14. doi:10.1037/a0013643

Sheehan, D.V., Lecrubier, I., Sheehan, K.H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., . . . Dunbar,M.D. (1998). The MINI-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): The develop-ment and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV andICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 22�33.

Spitzer, R.L., Kroenke, K., & Williams, J.B.W. (1999). Validation and utility of a self-reportversion of PRIME-MD: The PHQ Primary Care Study. JAMA: Journal of the AmericanMedical Association, 282, 1737�1744. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.18.1737

Taylor, S., Zvolensky, M.J., Cox, B.J., Deacon, B., Heimberg, R.G., Ledley, D.R., . . .Cardenas, S.J. (2007). Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: Development and initialvalidation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychological Assessment, 19, 176�188. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.176

Warwick, H.M., & Salkovskis, P.M. (1990). Hypochondriasis. Behaviour Research andTherapy, 28, 105�117. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90023-C

Williams, J.M.G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task andpsychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3�24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3

World Health Organization (1992). Tenth revision of the international classification of diseasesand related health problems (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 507

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Yor

k U

nive

rsity

Lib

rari

es]

at 2

1:03

21

Nov

embe

r 20

14