80
University of Calgary PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository Graduate Studies The Vault: Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2019-08-19 Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic Cigarette Fitzpatrick, Chelsea Louise Fitzpatrick, C. L. (2019). Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic Cigarette (Unpublished master's thesis). University of Calgary, Calgary, AB. http://hdl.handle.net/1880/110757 master thesis University of Calgary graduate students retain copyright ownership and moral rights for their thesis. You may use this material in any way that is permitted by the Copyright Act or through licensing that has been assigned to the document. For uses that are not allowable under copyright legislation or licensing, you are required to seek permission. Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca

Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

University of Calgary

PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository

Graduate Studies The Vault: Electronic Theses and Dissertations

2019-08-19

Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an

Electronic Cigarette

Fitzpatrick, Chelsea Louise

Fitzpatrick, C. L. (2019). Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic Cigarette

(Unpublished master's thesis). University of Calgary, Calgary, AB.

http://hdl.handle.net/1880/110757

master thesis

University of Calgary graduate students retain copyright ownership and moral rights for their

thesis. You may use this material in any way that is permitted by the Copyright Act or through

licensing that has been assigned to the document. For uses that are not allowable under

copyright legislation or licensing, you are required to seek permission.

Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca

Page 2: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY

Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic Cigarette

by

Chelsea Louise Fitzpatrick

A THESIS

SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES

IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE

DEGREE MASTER OF SCIENCE

GRADUATE PROGRAM IN PSYCHOLOGY

CALGARY, ALBERTA

AUGUST, 2019

© Chelsea Louise Fitzpatrick 2019

Page 3: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

2

Table of Contents

ABSTRACT 5

Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic Cigarette 6

Electronic Cigarettes 7

Potential Harms of E-Cigarette Use 9

Patterns of E-Cigarette Use 11

The Link Between E-Cigarette and Later Use of Combustible Tobacco Cigarettes 12

Transitioning from E-Cigarettes to Traditional Cigarettes 13

The Gateway Theory 13

The Catalyst Model 14

Cognitive Processes and Attentional Biases 15

Methods for Measuring Attentional Biases 17

Attentional Biases in Tobacco Smoking 18

Attentional Biases in E-Cigarette Users 19

Rationale 20

Method 22

Participants 22

Measures 23

Demographics. 23

E-Cigarette Usage. 23

E-Cigarette Dependence. 23

E-Cigarette Craving. 23

Psychopathology. 24

Substance Use. 24

Impulsivity. 25

Apparatus (Eye-Tracking) 26

Stimuli 26

Ethics Approval 27

Procedure 27

Statistical Analyses 28

Results 29

Preliminary Analyses 29

E-Cigarette Use 29

Psychopathology 29

Substance Use 29

Page 4: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

3

Impulsivity 30

Attentional Processing 30

Vaping vs Smoking Trials. 31

Smoking vs Neutral Trials. 31

Multiple Regressions 32

Discussion 32

Potential Implications 37

Future Directions 38

Limitations and Strengths 38

Conclusion 40

References 41

Appendix 64

Page 5: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

4

List of Tables

Table 1 .......................................................................................................................................... 55 Table 2 .......................................................................................................................................... 57 Table 3 .......................................................................................................................................... 59 Table 4 .......................................................................................................................................... 60 Table 5 .......................................................................................................................................... 61 Table 6 .......................................................................................................................................... 62 Table 7 .......................................................................................................................................... 63

Page 6: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

5

ABSTRACT

Electronic cigarettes (e-cigarettes) have been gaining popularity in recent years and are now

being used in large numbers by youth who smoke tobacco cigarettes as well as among youths

who do not smoke. Previous research has indicated that substance users can develop attentional

biases towards stimuli related to their use. Yet, the existence of attentional biases among users of

e-cigarettes remains largely unexplored. Using a sample of non-smokers, the present study

examined whether e-cigarette users develop an attentional bias toward e-cigarette stimuli and

whether this can be transferred to traditional cigarette stimuli. Using eye-gaze tracking to

identify attentional biases, it was found that e-cigarette users significantly differed in comparison

to controls in terms of the average amount of time that they attended to e-cigarette stimuli.

However, e-cigarette users did not preferentially attend to e-cigarette stimuli over neutral stimuli.

Furthermore, e-cigarette users did not attend more to smoking stimuli over neutral stimuli.

Rather, e-cigarette users appeared to avoid smoking cues. The results of this study indicate that

e-cigarette users’ attention towards e-cigarettes is increased in comparison to non-users, which

may have implications as to how they react to e-cigarette cues in real-world settings.

Keywords: Electronic cigarettes, vaping, attentional biases, non-smokers

Page 7: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

6

Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic Cigarette

Rates of electronic cigarette (e-cigarette or vaping) use have been continually on the rise

since their introduction to the North American marketplace in the early 2000’s (King, Alam,

Promoff, Arrazola, & Dube, 2013; McMillen, Gottlieb, Shaefer, Winickoff, & Klein, 2018; US

Department of Health and Human Services, 2016). A potential reason for the popularity in the use

of e-cigarettes is the perception that they are a safer alternative to traditional combustible cigarettes

(Choi & Forster, 2013). Additionally, e-cigarettes have been commonly viewed as a cessation tool

for combustible cigarette smoking (Siegel, Tanwar, & Wood, 2011). Recent research, however,

suggests that e-cigarettes may be more harmful than once thought (Soneji, Sung, Primack, Pierce,

& Sargent, 2018). Furthermore, there is evidence to indicate that e-cigarettes may also contribute

to the migration towards the use of traditional cigarettes among non-smoking e-cigarette users

(Spindle et al., 2017).

Despite the widespread use of e-cigarettes, the underlying mechanisms behind e-cigarette

use remains poorly understood. Furthermore, cognitive processes which may influence the

migration from vaping to smoking have yet to be adequately investigated. One component of

cognition which may contribute to this transition are attentional biases, which occur when an

individual displays heightened awareness and processing of cues related to their addiction (Field

& Cox, 2008). The present research investigated whether e-cigarette users can also develop an

attentional bias towards e-cigarettes in much the same way that occurs with other substances (e.g.,

tobacco (Bradley, Mogg, Wright, & Field, 2003), and alcohol (Townshend & Duka, 2001)).

Furthermore, this study also assessed whether transference of attentional biases from e-cigarettes

to traditional tobacco cigarettes can also occur among non-smoking e-cigarette users, which may

Page 8: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

7

be a potential mechanism to help understand the migration from e-cigarettes to traditional

cigarettes.

Electronic Cigarettes

An e-cigarette is a device which delivers nicotine or non-nicotine containing e-juice

through inhaled vapor. The device delivers this vapor through the process of heating a solution

containing propylene glycol or glycerol (glycerin), nicotine (in many cases but not always), and

the option of flavoring (Grana, Benowitz, & Glantz, 2014). With an e-cigarette, individuals can

vary the nicotine concentration, volume of solution in the product, carrier compound,

additives/flavors and battery voltage. Furthermore, users are also able to modify the products, and

can use vaping devices to administer other drugs including tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), the

primary psychoactive constituent in marijuana (Morean, Kong, Camenga, Cavallo, & Krishnan-

Sarin, 2015). There are also a number of different types of e-cigarettes, including disposable,

rechargeable, pen-style and tank-style (Grana et al., 2014). When motives for use have been

assessed, individuals report using e-cigarettes for a multitude of reasons including; fewer location

restrictions, can use nicotine more, smoking cessation, thinking that there are fewer harms to health

(compared to traditional cigarettes), better and more diverse flavors (i.e., don’t have the same taste

as tobacco cigarettes), and are not known to damage teeth and skin as combustible cigarettes do,

to name a few (Dawkins, Turner, Roberts, & Soar, 2013; Goniewicz, Lingas, & Hajek, 2013). In

regard to nicotine absorption rates, they vary by both the product and the person (based on number

and length of puffs), and as such, can be comparable to the nicotine absorption rates seen in

conventional cigarettes (Dawkins & Corcoran, 2014; Vansickel & Eissenberg, 2013).

E-cigarette usage has been steadily increasing year-over-year (King, Alam, Promoff,

Arrazola, & Dube, 2013; McMillen, Gottlieb, Shaefer, Winickoff, & Klein, 2018). For instance,

Page 9: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

8

epidemiological evidence from the United States revealed that ever e-cigarette usage among young

adults increased from 6.9% in 2011 to 14.3% in 2014 (US Department of Health and Human

Services, 2016). Moreover, e-cigarettes are particularly popular with adolescents and young adults.

Among Canadian high school students surveyed between 2014-2015, ever use for e-cigarettes was

estimated to be 17.7%, and current (past 30 days) use at 5.7% (Montreuil et al., 2017). Data from

the United States National Survey in 2013-2014, estimated ever use at 35.8% and current use at

13.6% among young adults aged 18 to 24 (US Department of Health and Human Services, 2016).

An additional prevalence report in 2014 found that 12.6% of adults had tried an e-cigarette. When

broken down by age range, young adults (18 to 24 year olds) had the highest prevalence with

21.6%, followed by 25-44 year olds with 16.6%, 45 to 64 year olds with 10.2% and lastly 65+ with

3.7% (Schoenborn & Gindi, 2015). From these studies, it can be observed that e-cigarette use is

most prevalent among young adults, with both ever use and current use on the rise (Schoenborn &

Gindi, 2015; US Department of Health and Human Services, 2016).

Along with this rise in prevalence there has been a coinciding increase in e-cigarette

advertisements (Kim, Arnold, & Makarenko, 2014). To date, regulations restricting e-cigarette

advertising have been sparse. Most concerning, e-cigarette advertisements often minimize the risks

while focusing on the benefits of use (Klein et al., 2016). In 2012, 18.3 million dollars was spent

on ads in the United States; representing a three-fold increase over the 6.4 million spent in 2011,

with the majority of focus placed on national markets through television ads (Kim et al., 2014).

Concomitant with this increase in overall advertising, youth exposure to e-cigarette ads on

television increased by 256% from 2011 to 2013 (Duke et al., 2014). Not surprisingly, exposure

to e-cigarette advertisements among youth was significantly associated with both current and ever

e-cigarette usage, while adjusting for sex, grade, ethnicity and past 30 day tobacco use, odds ratios

Page 10: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

9

ranged from 1.20 to 1.68 dependent on the type of media (internet, print, retail and

television/movies) (Mantey, Cooper, Clendennen, Pasch, & Perry, 2016). Furthermore, exposure

to e-cigarette advertisements increased the odds of past 30 day use by 1.22 and being in high school

(opposed to middle school) was also associated with an odds ratio of 2.37 (Mantey et al., 2016).

This suggests that increased exposure to e-cigarette advertisements may partially account for the

findings that more students are currently using e-cigarettes. Among those exposed to e-cigarette

advertisements, higher receptivity was associated with perceptions that e-cigarettes are less

harmful, which in turn, was associated with higher e-cigarette usage (Pokhrel, Fagan, Kehl, &

Herzog, 2015). Lastly, another large market for exposure to e-cigarettes advertising is through

online videos. The majority of these videos are sponsored by marketers for e-cigarettes and videos

tend to highlight economic and social benefits as well as health benefits which have been

proscribed by the FDA (Paek, Kim, Hove, & Huh, 2014).

Potential Harms of E-Cigarette Use

As research on the health of e-cigarettes has increased, evidence has emerged to indicate

that e-cigarettes may actually be doing more harm than good. While research on the dangers of

using traditional tobacco cigarettes has been extensive, analogous studies focusing on the potential

harms of e-cigarettes are still in their infancy. Yet, there is some indication of physical harm

associated with e-cigarettes. Firstly, there is the potential for nicotine dependence associated with

using e-cigarettes containing nicotine (Perkins, Karelitz, & Michael, 2015). Nicotine presents other

concerns beyond dependence and being a toxic substance such as health concerns including cancer,

cardiovascular diseases, and effects on reproductive health (Lee & Fariss, 2017). Conversely, little

is known about e-cigarettes which do not contain nicotine (Bell & Keane, 2012), other than the

Page 11: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

10

fact that e-juice without nicotine appears to be less addictive than those containing nicotine (Etter

& Eissenberg, 2015).

While it does appear that e-cigarettes are less addictive than combustible cigarettes, with

74% of former cigarette smokers reporting that their e-cigarette dependence was weaker than their

previous cigarette dependence (Etter & Eissenberg, 2015), the majority of e-cigarette users do

believe that e-cigarettes are addictive. For instance, one study revealed that 93% of current e-

cigarette users believed that e-cigarettes can be addictive, and additionally, approximately half

believed they were currently dependent on e-cigarettes (Goniewicz et al., 2013).

While developing dependence for vaping is clearly concerning, recent research also

suggest that some e-cigarette devices may also be responsible for other health concerns. For

instance, it has been found that the chemical processes involved in the heating of e-cigarette e-

juice can result in the release of formaldehyde into the vapor. Formaldehyde is a chemical which

is also a suspected carcinogen (Kosmider et al., 2014). The release of formaldehyde vapor is said

to resemble that of a combustible cigarette when using a propylene glycol solution and a high

voltage (4.8 V) e-cigarette (Kosmider et al., 2014). Additionally, when looking at the specific use

of an e-cigarette, approximately 10 puffs using a high voltage (5.0 V) e-cigarette, releases roughly

twice the amount of formaldehyde when compared to one traditional cigarette (Jensen, Luo,

Pankow, Strongin, & Peyton, 2015).

The harms of e-cigarettes extend beyond physical and addictive harms. Past research with

combustible tobacco has found that cigarette smoking is associated with higher levels of

psychological distress (Jamal et al., 2016). Similarly, a recent study has found that e-cigarette

usage is also associated with psychological distress; with dual users (e-cigarettes and combustible

cigarettes) experiencing the highest level of distress (Park, Lee, Shearston, & Weitzman, 2017).

Page 12: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

11

Patterns of E-Cigarette Use

Dual use of tobacco and vaping is a phenomenon which occurs in both adults and youth;

with dual use reported in 2% to 12% of youth US e-cigarette users (Action on Smoking and Health,

2014). In adults, 32% reported currently using one or more tobacco products, with 28% of those

individuals reporting using e-cigarettes in addition to tobacco cigarettes (Lee, Hebert,

Nonnemaker, & Kim, 2014). Dual users tend to use the two products quite differently. E-cigarettes

tend to be used for smoking cessation and for the reduction of health risks as dual users perceive

e-cigarettes as being safer and less addictive (Maglia, Caponnetto, Di Piazza, La Torre, & Polosa,

2017). In contrast, tobacco cigarettes are used more in hedonistic situations, locations where there

are no restrictions on smoking, and when they are more stressed and/or anxious (Maglia et al.,

2017).

In a study done by Choi & Forster (2013), over 2,000 adults from the Midwestern United

States were asked about their awareness, usage and perceptions of e-cigarettes. It was found that

70% of the participants were aware of e-cigarettes, with 7% having ever-used and 1% currently

using. Of those currently using, they were 1.52 times more likely to be between 20-24 years old,

and 1.98 times more likely to be male. Additionally, they were 10.07 times more likely to be a

current smoker, and 3.51 times more likely to be a former smoker over being a never smoker.

Among current and former smokers, they were more likely to believe that e-cigarettes are less

addictive than combustible cigarettes, while only current smokers were more likely to believe that

e-cigarettes are less harmful and that vaping can help them to quit smoking. With the multiple

relationships between e-cigarettes and tobacco cigarettes, including dual use, there is also the

possibility that one begins with using an e-cigarette and later transitions to tobacco cigarette

smoking.

Page 13: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

12

The Link Between E-Cigarette and Later Use of Combustible Tobacco Cigarettes

Although the potential usefulness of vaping for smoking cessation is promising, there also

appears to be the potential hazard associated with migration from e-cigarette use to cigarette

smoking among some individuals. For example, it has been estimated that anywhere from 16.3%

to 33.3% of e-cigarette users are actually current nonsmokers (i.e., never or former smokers)

(Sharapova, Singh, Agaku, Kennedy, & King, 2018; McMillen et al., 2018). This estimate from

McMillen’s study in 2018 has risen from 2012, when it was estimated that 17.2% and 7.7% of e-

cigarette users were former and never smokers respectively (McMillen, Maduka, & Winickoff,

2012). Moreover, recent research suggests that e-cigarettes could contribute to nicotine addiction

as well as the renormalization of tobacco use (McMillen et al., 2018).

In recent years, there has been an increased focus toward exploring the potential of non-

smoking e-cigarette users later transitioning to tobacco use, especially among youth and

adolescents. In one study conducted with 11th and 12th graders in Southern California, it was found

that those currently using e-cigarettes were twice as more likely to be susceptible to cigarette use

(i.e., no commitment to not smoke cigarettes) as compared to those who had never used e-cigarettes

(Barrington-Trimis, et al., 2016). Additionally, a study involving adolescents in grades 6 through

12 found that the intention to smoke cigarettes was 1.7 times higher in ever e-cigarette users as

compared to never users (Bunnell et al., 2015). When predicting future use of tobacco cigarettes,

the usage of e-cigarettes (ever use or current use) was related to a 3.37 to 3.41 times higher chance

of having tried a traditional cigarette at time two (Spindle et al., 2017). Ever use of e-cigarettes

was also related to a 3.30 times higher chance of being a current smoker at time two (Spindle et

al., 2017). When evaluating the relationship between e-cigarette use and subsequent smoking

among adolescents who became of age to legally purchase tobacco products, e-cigarette users were

Page 14: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

13

over six times more likely to have initiated cigarette smoking when compared to never users of e-

cigarettes (Barrington-Trimis, et al., 2016). When specifically looking at the transition period

when adolescents are able to legally purchase tobacco, the odds of initiating cigarette smoking is

considerably higher than when not evaluating during this transition period.

The above studies suggest that there appears to be possible transitions in both directions.

That is, it appears that some current cigarette smokers have initiated e-cigarette use and vice versa.

Unfortunately, there is a paucity of research which has attempted to weigh the two transitions in

terms of harms and benefits. At a population level, the balance of harm to benefit leans more

toward the harm side with there being possible negative impacts to both never smokers and former

smokers (Levy et al., 2017). In a study conducted by Soneji, Sung, Primack, Pierce, & Sargent

(2018), using a Monte Carlo stochastic simulation model, it was estimated that e-cigarette usage

in 2014 alone would amount to 1,510,000 years lost when taking into account both years gained

from former smokers quitting by using e-cigarettes as well as e-cigarettes facilitating smoking

cigarettes in never smokers. At this time, there has been a limited number of studies which have

researched the cost vs benefit. As this is a growing area, future studies will be needed to determine

concrete cost-benefit analyses of e-cigarettes as their use continues to evolve.

Transitioning from E-Cigarettes to Traditional Cigarettes

Importantly for the present research, there is a paucity of research examining potential

mechanisms that lead to the migration to traditional cigarettes from e-cigarettes. There are

several theories which could help to explain the process of transitioning from e-cigarettes to

combustible tobacco cigarettes. A few of such theories are discussed below.

The Gateway Theory. According to the Gateway Theory, use of one drug may increase

susceptibly to later trying other drugs; for example someone using cannabis may later transition to

Page 15: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

14

using methamphetamines (Kandel, 1975; Kandel, Yamaguchi, & Klein, 2006). In terms of the

present research, vaping can result in nicotine use/dependence, feelings and gestures associated

with inhalation, and contact with smokers. Through these characteristics, individuals may begin to

smoke (Etter, 2017). Exposure to these factors may ultimately serve as a ‘gateway’ to other

methods for administering nicotine including cigarette smoking. In one of the few studies which

has investigated this relationship, it was found that e-cigarette users were more likely to turn to

traditional cigarettes at time 2 if they had affiliations with friends who smoked, had more positive

smoking expectancies, and had a higher marijuana score during the baseline assessment (Wills,

Gibbons, Sargent, & Schweitzer, 2016). However, more recent critiques argue that the gateway

theory is flawed in terms of its’ relationship to e-cigarettes and the uptake of traditional cigarettes.

For instance, the gateway theory does not explain how the uptake of e-cigarettes first occurs

(Schneider & Diehl, 2016).

The Catalyst Model. Another potential explanation is the Catalyst Model. This model

goes further than the Gateway theory in that it first explains why an individual would begin using

an e-cigarette in the first place. In other words, what are the factors that influences an individual

from not using an e-cigarette to using an e-cigarette. Thereafter, the Catalyst Model moves on to

propose hypotheses which would lead one from vaping to smoking (Schneider & Diehl, 2016).

Thus, whereas the Gateway Theory is a one-step process (from e-cigarettes to smoking), the

Catalyst Model can be seen as two-step process (from no use to e-cigarette and then from e-

cigarette use to smoking). The model proposes numerous reasons as to why one would engage in

e-cigarette use, including flavor, price, role model, concealment and acceptance (more socially

acceptable), as well as why one would transition to tobacco cigarettes including the addiction

(nicotine addiction), accessibility (where e-cigarettes are sold, often tobacco cigarettes are too),

Page 16: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

15

and experience hypotheses (experience with an e-cigarettes trains one to smoke a traditional

cigarette). Lastly, they also propose two hypotheses which may explain more than one pathway

within the model. The liability hypothesis proposes that use of either an e-cigarette or traditional

cigarette are unrelated to one another, and that individuals engage in these activities due to

personality traits and socialization. And the second is the renormalization hypothesis, which

proposes that e-cigarettes may lead to the renormalization of tobacco products, and as such

individuals will begin to believe they are less dangerous than once thought. The renormalization

hypothesis also speaks to social learning theory, with users of any kind of cigarette act as a social

role model and may influence this uptake of cigarettes.

Many of the Catalyst Model’s hypotheses regarding why an adolescent would begin using

an e-cigarette have been supported with studies that have researched reasons that an individual

vapes. For instance, in a National Youth Tobacco Survey conducted in the United States in 2016,

some of the top reasons amongst youth to use an e-cigarette coincide with the above hypotheses.

The results indicated that some of the most popular reasons were that a friend or family member

used an e-cigarette (39%), the available flavours (31%), and that they are less harmful than

traditional cigarettes (17.1%) (Tsai et al., 2018). An additional study from 2014 which evaluated

11th and 12th graders found that susceptibility for cigarette use was increased if the individuals’

friends used or had positive attitudes towards e-cigarettes. This study further concludes that e-

cigarettes may be contributing to social normalization of smoking behaviours and nicotine, and

thus affecting future cigarette smoking (Barrington-Trimis, Berhane, et al., 2016).

Cognitive Processes and Attentional Biases

Attentional biases may also help to understand the migration from e-cigarettes to cigarettes.

Number of theories have been proposed as to why an attentional bias may develop in response to

Page 17: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

16

exposure to external stimuli associated with addictive behaviours. One of the earliest theories

describing the development of an attentional bias relates to the influence of classical conditioning.

According to learning theories, the repeated experience of pairing a substance of abuse with

environmental stimuli (often unrelated to the addictive behaviour itself) will eventually lead to an

increase in attention directed towards these cues during future exposures (Field & Cox, 2008). In

their influential theory, Robinson and Berridge (1993) further describe why some individuals

compulsively use drugs as a result of Incentive Sensitization, a theory which posits that there is a

dopaminergic response which occurs following repeated administrations of the substance,

resulting in increased sensitization toward the drug. In other words, the substance gains incentive

salience; or “drug wanting” and these motivational properties lead to an increased likelihood of

additional drug administrations (Robinson & Berridge, 2000). Related to this compulsive drug

wanting, individuals develop an underlying bias in their attentional processing towards the

substance as well as substance-related stimuli (Robinson & Berridge, 2008). This increased

attentional processing can also be considered an attentional bias.

Transference is described as the ability of selective attention towards one addiction to be

transferred to another, similar substance. In other words, it is when an individual who preferentially

attends to one substance can also experience increased attention directed toward a second

substance which elicits a similar response. An example of this includes a recent study done with

cigarette smokers, where it was found that these individuals had also developed an attentional bias

towards e-cigarettes as well as traditional cigarettes (Lochbuehler et al., 2018). Accordingly, it is

feasible that the reverse may also be true; whereby e-cigarette users may also preferentially attend

to cigarette cues over neutral cues, which may help in understanding the migration from e-

cigarettes to traditional cigarettes.

Page 18: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

17

Methods for Measuring Attentional Biases

An attentional bias can be measured using differing methodologies including both direct

and indirect measures of attention. Indirect measurements include tasks such as the addiction

Stroop task, dual-task procedures and flicker-induced change blindness. Whereas more direct

measures include the visual probe task, attentional cueing tasks, and eye-gaze tracking (Field &

Cox, 2008). Indirect measures tend to be less reliable over direct measures as there is an inference

associated with indirect measures, such that we assume that slower reaction time to name a color

in the addiction Stroop task is due to selective attention towards substance-related words.

However, there may be alternative explanations for this delay such as attempts to avoid higher

level processing of substance related words (Klein, 2007), or that the individual is experiencing

subjective craving due to the substance-related words, given craving is known to utilize cognitive

resources which would slow down reaction time for the substance-related words (Algom, Chajut,

& Lev, 2004). These lapses of attention may indicate an attentional bias, or alternatively, may

simply indicate that the individual is avoiding the substance-related words. It is also feasible that

the individual is experiencing acute craving for their drug of use, which can be related to an

attentional bias, but may not be directly reflective of one.

Importantly for the present study, research has found that the direct measures (i.e., eye-

gaze tracking) provide a more sensitive measure of attentional biases when compared against

indirect measures. Furthermore, eye-gaze tracing often displays superior reliability and construct

validity (Field & Cox, 2008; Field, Marhe, & Franken, 2014). Eye movements are believed to be

tightly coupled with attention (Wright & Ward, 2008), whereas an indirect measure involves a

two-step process whereby the bias in attention would be assumed from some other measure (e.g.,

for the addiction Stroop task an individual must indicate the colour of the word shown, a slower

Page 19: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

18

reaction indicating an attentional bias as they are said to be processing the word over the colour).

Specifically, for measuring an attentional bias via eye-gaze tracking, an attentional bias can be

identified when the group of interest significantly differs from the control group, in terms of their

attention allocated towards the addiction-related stimuli (Field & Cox, 2008).

Attentional Biases in Tobacco Smoking

The existence of attentional biases has been established across differing addictive

behaviours (Field & Cox, 2008). This includes gambling (Honsi, Mentzoni, Molde, & Pallesen,

2012), tobacco (Bradley et al., 2003), alcohol (Townshend & Duka, 2001), as well as other illicit

drugs (Leeman, Robinson, Waters, & Sofuoglu, 2014). Moreover, the associated strength of an

attentional bias has been shown to be positively related to the subjective craving for the substance

(Field, Munafò, & Franken, 2009). Field and colleagues (2009) also found that this association

differed based on the substance in question, and how the attentional bias was measured; with direct

measures resulting in larger associations.

A sizeable body of evidence has identified the existence of attentional biases among

tobacco users for cigarette cues. Using a modified Stroop task, Munafo, Mogg, Roberts, Bradley,

and Murphy (2003) found that current cigarette smokers had a processing bias for smoking-related

cues over that of neutral cues. Additional studies have aimed to determine what differences are

present that may heighten an attentional bias. In a study by Bradley and colleagues (2003), it was

found that when a shorter duration of time (500ms) was used for the visual probe task, an

attentional bias was only observed for smokers who have had numerous failed attempts to quit.

However, when a longer exposure duration was used (i.e., 2000ms), the bias was observed across

all smokers regardless of previous attempts to quit. These results suggest that those with numerous

attempts to quit smoking display a higher vigilance for smoking cues, and these individuals’

Page 20: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

19

inability to quit smoking may be related to their attentional bias towards smoking stimuli. Waters

and Feyerabend (2000) examined the role of tobacco abstinence in attentional biases toward

smoking stimuli. Participants were required to abstain from smoking for 24 hours prior to testing

and then completed an emotional Stroop task where they were presented with smoking and neutral

words. They found that those who were abstinent showed a greater bias toward smoking cues

compared to smokers who were not abstinent. In terms of the relationship between strength of

attentional biases and the ability to quit smoking, one study found that those who with a greater

attentional bias towards smoking-related cues were significantly more likely to relapse within one

week of quitting (Waters et al., 2003). Past research using a visual probe task has found that

smokers in comparison to non-smokers will attend to smoking cues for longer than neutral cues,

and that this diversion of attention is related to both increased craving as well as increased affective

and motivational valance associated with smoking stimuli (Mogg, Bradley, Field, & De Houwer,

2003). Given the presence of attentional biases in tobacco users, there is strong reasoning for e-

cigarette users to also display attentional biases, which unfortunately has rarely been examined.

Attentional Biases in E-Cigarette Users

Compared to the much more established literature on tobacco smoking, there has been a

paucity of research exploring many of these same concepts in e-cigarette users. In a study with

current smokers, participants were asked to first observe a confederate drink water (control

condition) and then smoke a cigarette or use an e-cigarette. Passive exposure to either e-cigarettes

or cigarettes significantly increased desire and urge to smoke combustible cigarettes. Furthermore,

passive e-cigarette exposure elicited also increased e-cigarette craving (King, Smith, McNamara,

Matthews, & Fridberg, 2015). However, a major limitation of this study was that the sample was

Page 21: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

20

not comprised of primarily e-cigarette users. Indeed, only half of the sample had past e-cigarette

use and a mere 23% reported use within the past month (i.e., current use).

King and colleagues (2016) investigated whether viewing a video depicting e-cigarette use

would elicit subjective urges to smoke a tobacco cigarette or use an e-cigarette. The videos

consisted of advertisements for e-cigarettes or bottled water (control stimuli). This study found

that e-cigarette cues in people who smoke tobacco cigarettes were able to produce increased

craving ratings for both cigarette smoking as well as for e-cigarettes. However, similar to the

previous study conducted by this group, a potential limitation was that the majority of participants

were not regular users of an e-cigarette. In the second study, past year use ranged from 70-76%;

however, past month use ranged from only 25-29%.

The most recent study on this topic examined whether smokers have developed an

attentional bias for e-cigarette cues. The study utilized eye-gaze tracking to determine whether

both current and former smokers display increased attention towards e-cigarette cues in

comparison to neutral images. It was found that current smokers, more so than non-smokers or

former smokers, displayed a bias towards e-cigarette images over neutral stimuli (Lochbuehler et

al., 2018). Similar to the past two studies, this study was more focused on traditional cigarette

smokers as opposed to e-cigarette users. Thus, the extent to which attentional biases toward e-

cigarette imagery occurs among individuals who only vape (and don’t smoke) remains unknown.

Rationale

Currently there is a paucity of research on the cognitive processes involved in e-cigarette

usage. As with many addictive substances, it is possible that e-cigarette stimuli may also become

sensitized resulting in attention being prioritized towards e-cigarette cues over other neutral

environmental stimuli. In turn, an attentional bias towards e-cigarette cues may also lead to further

Page 22: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

21

craving and increased use as has been witnessed in attentional bias research on tobacco smokers.

Additionally, there is the added potential for the transference of attentional biases for e-cigarette

cues toward traditional combustible cigarettes, which if it occurs, may provide one potential

mechanism for why e-cigarette use may contribute to the initiation of using traditional cigarettes

among never smokers. Given that nicotine is a highly addictive substance, the potential for stimuli

frequently paired with this form of nicotine administration to induce attentional biases is a topic

requiring further exploration.

To my knowledge, this study was the first to directly measure attention in e-cigarette users

towards both e-cigarette visual stimuli as well as tobacco cigarette stimuli. There were two primary

aims in this study. The first was to determine if, much like other substance users, e-cigarette users

develop an attentional bias towards e-cigarettes. If an attentional bias to e-cigarettes exists, it is

theoretically conceivable that users may also develop cravings e-cigarettes following exposure,

which is ultimately a concern for triggering relapse among abstainers. The second aim of this study

was to examine if e-cigarette users who have never smoked also experience attentional biases

toward tobacco cigarettes (i.e., transference). If this is indeed the case, consistent with the Gateway

Theory, this process may be a potential explanatory factor for the enhanced risk of migration to

later tobacco smoking among non-smoking e-cigarette users.

Based on the previous literature on attentional biases in traditional tobacco users, it was

hypothesized that (H1) compared to a sample of non-e-cigarette users, individuals who currently

vape would display an attentional bias toward e-cigarette cues over neutral stimuli; (H2) e-cigarette

users would also preferentially attend to e-cigarette cues over tobacco-related cues; and (H3)

consistent with possible transference, e-cigarette users would also preferentially to tobacco

cigarette cues over neutral stimuli.

Page 23: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

22

Method

Participants

A sample primarily composed of undergraduate students was recruited through the online

research participation system (SONA) hosted by the Department of Psychology at the University

of Calgary, as well as via posters placed around campus. Participants were comprised of current

e-cigarette users (n = 67) and non-users (n = 71) who were matched on demographic characteristics

where possible (e.g., age, gender). Ten non-users excluded from the analyses (as there was no eye-

gaze tracking data for eight participants and two due to exceptionally low average dwell times)

and eight e-cigarette users were ultimately excluded (as there was no eye-gaze tracking data

available for three participants, three who were identified to be current smokers of traditional

cigarettes on test day, one as they were a past cigarette smoker, and one who was not a current e-

cigarette user). With these exclusions, the final sample included for analyses consisted of 59 e-

cigarette users and 61 non-users (controls). This sample size is consistent with similar two group

design eye-tracking studies previously conducted by our team (e.g., McGrath, Meitner, & Sears,

2018; Newman & Sears, 2015). Individuals were compensated with course credit for their

participation (for students), or a $20 e-gift card (for community members and non-psychology

students).

Prospective participants first filled out an online questionnaire, or a phone screen to

determine if they met the eligibility criteria. To be included in the e-cigarette group, individuals

had to report currently using e-cigarettes, defined as use within the past 30 days (Liu, Wasserman,

Kong, & Foulds, 2017), and had to be never smokers (defined as smoking fewer than 100 cigarettes

in their lifetime). Individuals in the non-user group had to report not currently using an e-cigarette,

not having used one in the past 30 days, and had to be never smokers of traditional cigarettes.

Page 24: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

23

Furthermore, all participants had to report normal to corrected vision and no colorblindness and

be 18 years of age or older. If eligible, individuals signed up for a time slot to come into the

Substance Use and Gambling Laboratory at the University of Calgary to complete the eye-tracking

experiment and questionnaires.

Measures

Demographics. Demographic information (e.g., age, sex, ethnicity, education) was

collected using a self-report questionnaire which has been used in prior studies by our lab (e.g.,

McGrath, Meitner, & Sears, 2018).

E-Cigarette Usage. Participants also completed an author-compiled questionnaire which

included questions related to e-cigarette usage and other tobacco use. Items included age at which

participants first tried vaping, the type of e-cigarette products they use, primary reasons for vaping,

and any negative reactions to using an e-cigarette.

E-Cigarette Dependence. Level of e-cigarette dependence was evaluated using the Penn

State Electronic Cigarette Dependence Index (Foulds et al., 2015). The PS-ECDI was developed

from the Penn State Cigarette Index within a population of former smokers. It is a ten item self-

report questionnaire which measures e-cigarette dependence by looking at usage, behavior and

craving. Sample items include: ‘Do you sometimes awaken at night to use your e-

cigarette/vaporizer?’ and ‘Do you ever have strong craving to use an e-cigarette/vaporizer?’.

Total scores from 0–3 indicate not dependent, from 4–8 are low dependence, 9–12 are medium

dependence, and 13+ indicate high dependence. The internal consistency of the Penn State E-

Cigarette Index in the present study was = .60.

E-Cigarette Craving. E-cigarette craving was evaluated using the Questionnaire of

Vaping Craving (QVC; Dowd, Motschman, & Tiffany, 2018). The QVC is a ten item self-report

Page 25: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

24

questionnaire which measures an individual’s craving level for vaping by looking at desire,

intention, and positive outcome. Each item is scored from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly

agree). Scores are calculated as a mean of the ten items, with higher total scores indicating higher

craving levels. The internal consistency of the QVC in the present study was = .94.

Psychopathology. Individuals’ mental health was examined with the Depression, Anxiety

and Stress Scale (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995), a 21-item self-report questionnaire

which measures emotional states of depression, anxiety and stress. Each item is scored from 0

(never) to 3 (almost always). The DASS-21 compiles three separate subscales for psychological

distress (depression, anxiety and stress), each with 7 items. Scores on each subscale are compiled

separately, with ranges being indicative of normal to extremely severe. Example items include; ‘I

felt that I have nothing to look forward to, I felt I was close to panic, and I found it hard to wind

down’. The DASS-21 has been found to validly measure all three subsets; depression, anxiety and

stress (Antony, Bieling, Cox, Enns, & Swinson, 1998; Henry & Crawford, 2005). Internal

consistency was = .91 for depression, = .81 for anxiety and = .87 for the stress subscale in

the current study.

Substance Use. Usage of both alcohol and cannabis was measured. The Alcohol Use

Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT; (Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Fuente, & Grant, 1993)

was used to determine potential alcohol use problems. The AUDIT is a ten item self-report

measure, where each item is scored from 0 to 4. Total scores range from 0 to 40; where a score of

8 or higher would indicate problematic alcohol use. The AUDIT covers questions regarding

alcohol consumption, drinking behaviours as well as other alcohol-related problems. Items include

‘How often do you have a drink containing alcohol?’, ‘How often during the last year have you

failed to do what was normally expected from you because of drinking?’, and ‘Have you or

Page 26: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

25

someone else been injured as result of your drinking?’. The AUDIT has been found to show

reliable and valid results when looking at alcohol related problems (Reinert & Allen, 2002).

Cronbach’s alpha for the AUDIT was = .78 for the present study. The Cannabis Use Disorders

Identification Test- Revised (CUDIT-R; Adamson et al., 2010) was used to determine potential

problems with cannabis. The CUDIT-R is an eight item self-report measure, with each item scored

from 0 to 4. Total scores range from 0 to 32; where a score of 13 or higher is thought to indicate

problematic cannabis use. Items include ‘How often do you use cannabis’, and ‘How often during

the past 6 months did you fail to do what was normally expected from you because of using

cannabis?’. Internal consistency of the CUDIT in this study was = .83.

Impulsivity. The UPPS-P (UPPS-P; (Lynam, Smith, Whiteside, & Cyders, 2006) was used

to assess facets of impulsivity. Specifically, the short version of the UPPS-P was utilized, the

SUPPS-P (Lynam, 2013), which consists of 20 items. Each item is scored from 1 (disagree

strongly) to 4 (agree strongly) with total scores indicating greater levels of impulsivity. The UPPS-

P measures five domains of impulsivity; negative urgency, positive urgency, lack of premeditation,

lack of perseverance, and sensation seeking. Negative and positive urgency are defined as acting

rashly in response to extreme negative and positive emotions respectively, with items including

‘When I am upset I often act without thinking and I tend to lose control when I am in a great mood’.

Lack of premeditation is when one acts without thinking, with one of the items being ‘I like to stop

and think things over before I do them’ (reversed). Lack of perseverance is the tendency to not

finish tasks, with one item being ‘I generally like to see things through to the end’ (reversed). And

sensation seeking is when one enjoys experiencing new and exciting activities or sensations, with

one item being ‘I quite enjoy taking risks’. The SUPPS-P has been shown to be a reliable and valid

measure of impulsivity within adult populations (Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey, & Karyadi, 2014).

Page 27: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

26

The internal consistencies of the subscales for this study were as follows; negative urgency ( =

.79), positive urgency ( = .79), lack of premeditation ( = .61), lack of perseverance ( = .78),

and sensation seeking ( = .71).

Apparatus (Eye-Tracking)

An Eyelink 1000 eye-gaze tracking system (SR Research Ltd., Ottawa, Ontario) was

utilized for collecting eye-gaze movements. The system uses an infrared camera to track eye

movements with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz which allows for a temporal resolution of 2 ms and

an average gaze error of less than 0.5 degrees of visual angle. Stimuli were presented on a 24-inch

LCD monitor approximately 60 cm away from the participant.

Stimuli

There was a total of 180 images presented to participants, with each image presented once.

Images were presented two at a time (as matched pairs) and were comprised of three separate

categories; e-cigarette, cigarette and neutral stimuli. The high-resolution images were selected

from stock photography websites. Images were matched in terms of the scenes presented as closely

as possible to the other image simultaneously presented on the screen in order to avoid biases

regarding the features, color etc. (McGrath et al., 2018). For example, one image may depict an

individual holding an e-cigarette and the matched image of someone holding a pen. Cigarette

images were chosen in order to determine if the incentive salience applied to e-cigarettes may be

transferred onto cigarettes as well, resulting in an attentional bias toward cigarettes over that of

neutral stimuli. The neutral images were matched to the e-cigarette or cigarette image in terms of

colours and shape. The neutral images consisted of a pen (or a hand holding a pen) in a similar

scene. E-cigarette images were presented in only 40 of the 90 trials (44%) in order to reduce

potential demand characteristics. That is, given that the recruitment procedure focused on ‘e-

Page 28: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

27

cigarette users’, it is possible that participants would anticipate that the experimenters are assessing

attention to vaping images, and as such, may alter their viewing behavior to conform to this

expectation.

Ethics Approval

The study obtained ethics approval from the Conjoint Faculties Research Ethics Board at

the University of Calgary prior to the start of the study (REB 18-1165).

Procedure

Upon arrival, eligible participants were greeted by the experimenter and were first asked

to complete the informed consent form. Next participants were led into a room containing the eye-

tracking apparatus. Participants were provided with both written and verbal instructions for

completing the eye-tracking task. Individuals were told that their eye-movements will be tracked

during the slideshow, during which they are free to look at whatever image they desire. Prior to

the eye-tracking task, calibration was first completed to ensure accurate eye-gaze data. Next, two

practice eye-tracking trials were completed which allowed participants to orient themselves to the

procedure. Following the practice trials, 90 test trials of images were presented, each trial lasting

4 seconds (Isaacowitz, Toner, Goren, & Wilson, 2008) with approximately 2 seconds between the

end of one trial and the beginning of the next. Each trial began with a black dot in the center of a

white screen to ensure the direction of their gaze was focused in the center of the screen. Presented

in each trial was two images; the images consisted of e-cigarette, cigarette and neutral stimuli. In

total, there were four sets of images; smoking vs. neutral (n = 20), e-cigarettes vs. neutral (n = 20),

smoking vs. e-cigarettes (n = 20) and neutral vs. neutral (n = 30). The order in which the trials

were presented was randomized for each participant, with each participant being shown all 90

trials. The 30 trials consisting solely of neutral images acted as fillers, which is in line with

Page 29: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

28

previous attentional bias research (e.g., Bradley et al., 2003; Mogg et al., 2003; Mogg, Field, &

Bradley, 2005). Data collection of participant’s eye-gaze movements was tracked during each trial

and the entire procedure on average, lasted 10 minutes. Upon finishing the eye-tracking task,

individuals returned to the testing room and completed the questionnaires. Lastly participants were

debriefed regarding the purpose of the study and were then compensated with one RPS credit, or

a $20 e-gift card. On average, the total time to complete all aspects of the study was ~40 minutes.

Statistical Analyses

Prior to analyses, eye-gaze data was screened for any eye-blinks, missing data and

recording artifacts using the EyeLink Data Viewer analysis software (SR Research) with the

default settings. The filler trials (neutral vs. neutral) were removed from the data as these trials

were not relevant to the analyses. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS (v25). Dependent

variables of interest were: first run time, and total mean dwell time, both measured in milliseconds.

First run time is the time which an individual spends attending to an image once first fixating

towards it, such that a greater first run time would indicate greater interest towards this image.

Total dwell time is the total amount of time spent fixating on a type of image (i.e., smoking, e-

cigarettes or neutral) during the 4-second presentation of the trial. These variables were first

created for each person for each trial, and then averaged across the trials for that type (i.e., vaping

vs neutral trials). The data was analyzed using separate ANOVAs for each trial type (vaping

smoking vs smoking neutral vs vaping neutral) for each of the dependent variables.

Page 30: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

29

Results

Preliminary Analyses

The sample was largely Caucasian (55.8%), with an almost equal sample of males and

females (51.7% female) who ranged in age from 18 – 35 years old (M = 19.99, SD = 2.27). A full

demographic profile of each group can be seen in Table 1.

E-Cigarette Use

Among the e-cigarette users, there were 27 daily users and 32 occasional users. The

majority of users were not dependent on e-cigarettes or reported low dependence (64%) as

measured by Penn State Electronic Cigarette Dependence Index. The top reasons for e-cigarette

use included enjoying the nicotine buzz (n = 42), they come in appealing flavours (n = 28), and

they seem less harmful than cigarettes (n = 27). In terms of nicotine usage; 47 participants (87.0%)

used nicotine in their e-cigarette. Full results are shown in Table 2.

Psychopathology

Among all participants, the majority fell within normal range for depression (61.7%),

anxiety (49.2%) and stress (70.0%) as measured with the DASS-21. There were no differences

between e-cigarette users and controls in terms of their DASS-21 scores (mean total score) for all

the subscales. Full results for the DASS-21 can be seen in Table 3.

Substance Use

The average AUDIT score was 7.78 (SD = 4.61) for e-cigarette users and 3.38 (SD = 3.96)

for controls. There was a difference between the groups (t (118) = -5.62, p < .001, d = 1.02), such

that e-cigarette users had a higher AUDIT score in comparison to controls. There were 37

individuals who met criteria for problematic alcohol use; 8 controls, and 29 e-cigarette users.

Page 31: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

30

The average CUDIT score was 2.47 (SD = 4.46) for e-cigarette users and 1.02 (SD = 2.28)

for controls. The homogeneity of variances assumption was violated, and therefore, equal

variances was not assumed. There was a significant difference between the groups (t (85.80) = -

5.32, p < .001, d = 0.41), such that e-cigarette users had a higher CUDIT score in comparison to

controls. There were 3 individuals who met criteria for problematic cannabis use, all of which were

e-cigarette users.

Impulsivity

E-cigarette users and controls did not differ on any level of impulsivity, using the SUPPS-

P total scores, after adjusting for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni corrections. More

comprehensive results for the SUPPS-P can be found in Table 4.

Attentional Processing

The primary dependent variables of interest for this study were recorded by the eye-tracker.

First, average dwell time for the entire trial, across trials (how much of the four seconds was spent

looking at the interest areas) was analyzed to determine whether there were individuals who had

not paid attention during the task, and outliers were excluded as their total dwell time also affects

the possible amount of time looking at each image during a trial (interest area dwell time). In terms

of differences between e-cigarette users and non-users, there were no differences for total dwell

time (t = -.89, p = .802). The following analyses assessed differences between non e-cigarette

users, occasional, and daily e-cigarette users.

Vaping vs Neutral Trials. A one-way ANOVA was conducted using total dwell time

difference scores as the dependent variable. The difference score was calculated by taking the

vaping total dwell time and subtracting the neutral image total dwell time. ANOVA assumptions

were run prior to the analysis and were met. The omnibus test was significant (F (2,117) = 5.73,

Page 32: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

31

p = .004, partial 2 = .09). Post-hoc tests were conducted to determine where the differences lied

using Tukey’s LSD. These analyses indicated that the only significant difference was between

controls and daily users (p = .001), such that daily users spent longer looking at vaping images in

comparison to the non-users. See table 6 for all total dwell time ANOVAs.

Another one-way ANOVA was conducted using first run dwell time as the dependent

variable. All assumptions of an ANOVA were met prior to analysis. There were no differences in

terms of first run time for the vaping vs neutral images (F (2,117) = 1.25, p = .289, partial 2 =

.02). See table 7 for all first run time ANOVAs.

Vaping vs Smoking Trials. A one-way ANOVA was conducted using total dwell time

difference scores as the dependent variable. The difference score was calculated by taking the

vaping total dwell time and subtracting the smoking image total dwell time. ANOVA assumptions

were run prior to the analysis and were met. The omnibus test was significant (F (2,117) = 5.66, p

= .005, partial 2 = .09). Post-hoc analyses indicated that controls differed from both occasional

users (p = .006) and daily users (p = .008). Both occasional and daily e-cigarette users viewed the

vaping images for longer in comparison to the controls. Another one-way ANOVA was conducted

using first run dwell time as the dependent variable. ANOVA assumptions were run prior to

analyses. There were no differences between the groups in terms of first run time for the vaping

and smoking images (F (2,117) = 2.42, p = .093, partial 2 = .04).

Smoking vs Neutral Trials. A one-way ANOVA was conducted using total dwell time

difference scores as the dependent variable. The difference score was calculated by taking the

smoking total dwell time and subtracting the neutral image total dwell time. ANOVA assumptions

were run prior to the analysis and were met. The omnibus test was not significant (F (2,117) =

0.12, p = .886, partial 2 = .00).

Page 33: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

32

Another one-way ANOVA was conducted using first run dwell time as the dependent

variable. ANOVA assumptions were met. There were no differences between groups in terms of

first run time of smoking vs neutral images (F (2,117) = 0.68, p = .506, partial 2 = .01).

Multiple Regressions. Three separate multiple regressions were run within the e-cigarette

users for various usage scores, including the Penn State total score, QVC total score, and nicotine

concentration used, using total dwell time as the dependent variable. These regressions were

performed to determine if there were specific e-cigarette characteristics which were able to predict

a higher attentional bias for e-cigarette cues or smoking cues. No assumptions for conducting

multiple regression were violated (independence of observations, linearity, homoscedasticity,

multicollinearity, normality and unusual points; such as outliers, leverage and influential points).

The multiple regression model for the smoking vs vaping IA dwell time was not significantly

predicted by the e-cigarette usage variables, F (3, 37) = 1.30, p = .289, adjusted R2 = .02. Smoking

vs neutral IA dwell time was also not significantly predicted by predictor variables, F (3, 37) =

1.77, p = .169, adjusted R2 = .06. Lastly, vaping vs neutral IA dwell time difference was not

significantly predicted by the predictor variables, F (3,37) = 0.56, p = .646, adjusted R2 = -.03.

Table 5 provides more specific details of the multiple regressions.

Discussion

Given the increasing prevalence of e-cigarette use, especially among never-smokers, and

the association between e-cigarette use and tobacco, the need for research regarding e-cigarette

use and the migration from vaping to tobacco smoking is becoming increasingly warranted. The

purpose of the current study was to address a gap in attentional biases literature relating to e-

cigarettes. Currently, the few studies that have focused on e-cigarette attentional biases have only

recruited smokers who also use e-cigarettes. In other words, no study has examined attentional

Page 34: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

33

bias among e-cigarette users who do not smoke, despite the increasing rise in prevalence in this

group (McMillen et al., 2018; Sharapova, Singh, Agaku, Kennedy, & King, 2018). Attentional

biases are an important contributing factor in addictive behaviors which relate to craving as well

as subsequent relapse after attempts to quit (Field & Cox, 2008). As such, the aim of this research

was to determine: (i) if individuals using e-cigarettes have developed an attentional bias towards

e-cigarettes and (ii) to further assess whether this attentional bias has been transferred to traditional

cigarettes using an eye-gaze tracking paradigm. The present study measured attentional biases

towards e-cigarettes using a free eye-gaze tracking paradigm to determine whether e-cigarette

users preferentially attend to e-cigarette stimuli over neutral stimuli and whether the attention

patterns of e-cigarette users differ from that of non-users. Furthermore, the study assessed whether

non-smoking e-cigarettes users also preferentially attend toward another nicotine product;

specifically, tobacco cigarettes.

For the psychopathology and personality characteristics, e-cigarette users and non-users

did not differ in terms of their depression, anxiety, or stress as well as impulsivity facets. These

results are contrary to many other substance users when compared to controls. For example, one

study which had examined mental health and impulsivity among university e-cigarette users had

founds that e-cigarette users were more likely to have higher anxiety and greater levels of

impulsivity. However, and similar to the current study, e-cigarette use was not associated with

significantly higher depressive symptoms (Grant, Lust, Fridberg, King, & Chamberlain, 2019).

Another study which compared impulsivity among e-cigarette users and non-users that also used

the SUPPS-P found that the groups did not differ in terms of sensation seeking, lack of

premeditation and perseverance, which is also the case in the current study. However, they did

find that users differed from non-users in terms of negative and positive urgency, such that e-

Page 35: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

34

cigarette users scored higher on both facets (Hershberger, Um, & Cyders, 2017). E-cigarette users

in the current study not showing higher levels of impulsivity may be partly due to the fact that they

did not display a high severity, or dependence for e-cigarettes.

In contrast to above, e-cigarette users and non-users did differ when compared for alcohol

and cannabis use, such that e-cigarette users scored higher on measures of both alcohol and

cannabis use. The findings that e-cigarette users are more likely to be using other substances has

been found in numerous other studies. In the study by Grant et al. (2019) discussed above, the

authors also found that e-cigarette use was associated with the use of other drugs including alcohol

and cannabis. This is a typical relationship among substance users and has been found in tobacco

users (Schauer, Berg, Kegler, Donovan, & Windle, 2015) and alcohol users (Weinberger et al.,

2019) previously. In other words, people who use one substance are likely to use other substances.

In terms of the total dwell time ANOVA, the results suggest that both controls and

occasional e-cigarette users preferentially attended to neutral stimuli, while daily e-cigarette users

spent slightly more time attending to the vaping images. Daily e-cigarette users significantly

differed from the non-users in that, daily users attended to e-cigarette stimuli longer than the non-

users did. The results suggest that those who frequently vape have developed an attentional bias

towards e-cigarettes compared to neutral images. For the first run time dwell ANOVA, no

significant findings were found, indicating that the groups did not differ in their initial attention

towards either e-cigarettes or neutral images.

These results somewhat differ from those of previous studies which examined attentional

bias for addictive substances such as tobacco (Bradley et al., 2003), alcohol (Townshend & Duka,

2001), and illicit substances (Leeman et al., 2014). Specifically, unlike the previous research, the

present study found that the neutral stimuli had a strong pull for both occasional e-cigarette users

Page 36: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

35

and controls. That said, the daily and occasional e-cigarette group did attend longer toward e-

cigarette images than non-users, indicating at least some preference for this image category. There

are several possible explanations as to why occasional e-cigarette users did not more strongly

prefer the e-cigarette stimuli over neutral images. One plausible explanation would be that the e-

cigarette users were not highly dependent on e-cigarettes according to the Penn State E-Cigarette

Dependence Index. In other words, the majority of e-cigarette users in the sample were not

dependent on e-cigarettes. Indeed, most studies on attentional biases have found that current

dependence to nicotine is a strong predictor of attentional bias toward smoking cues (Munafò et

al., 2003). It could have been the case that low levels of nicotine dependence, and the related lack

of craving associated with nicotine withdrawal, blunted group responses toward e-cigarette

imagery. Another potential explanation for these findings could be related to the novelty of the

neutral imagery. That is, it may be possible that novelty effects associated with the neutral stimuli

were responsible for enhanced attention toward that category. For instance, the neutral images

consisted of a hand holding a pen, camera lenses, or a phone and while not novel images, offered

something different for users to attend to rather than simply looking at another e-cigarette image.

Finally, it is conceivable that e-cigarette users simply do not display strong attentional biases

toward visual cues. The act of vaping itself involves multiple sensory domains (taste, olfactory,

visual), and simply viewing images may not strongly illicit attentional responses. Future studies

involving in vivo cue exposure may be required to more fully elucidate these effects.

Second, attentional preference for smoking vs. neutral trials was assessed. The results for

the total dwell time analysis revealed that e-cigarette users and controls both avoid smoking stimuli

when given the option to look at smoking or neutral images. The three groups did not differ in

terms of their time spent looking at the smoking vs neutral stimuli. The first run dwell time

Page 37: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

36

difference for the ANOVA also suggests that e-cigarette users and controls spend less time first

attending to smoking images in comparison to the neutral images, and that the groups did not differ

significantly. In comparison to the smoking images, all groups spent 300ms – 400ms longer when

first looking at neutral images. Overall, these results suggest that e-cigarette users and controls do

not differ when it comes to smoking vs neutral stimuli, and that both groups tend to avoid smoking

stimuli.

These findings are contrary to original hypothesis, that there would be transference of the

incentive motivation properties of e-cigarettes to traditional cigarettes. Given the demographic that

many studies have focused on when looking at the transition between e-cigarettes and tobacco

cigarettes; typically, high school students (Barrington-Trimis, Urman, et al., 2016; Wills et al.,

2016), it is theoretically feasible that the age group in the present study is past a critical age where

the transition is most likely to occur. Another possible reason that e-cigarette users do not attend

to smoking stimuli over neutral, could be that they find cigarettes to be aversive, given previous

findings that many e-cigarette users use an e-cigarette over smoking partially because they believe

it to be safer and healthier than tobacco cigarettes (Dawkins et al., 2013). In addition to believing

that vaping is healthier than smoking, this is an age group which was raised at the height of the

anti-tobacco ads, and this has likely played a large role in their perceptions (Weiss et al., 2006).

In terms of the vaping vs smoking trials, similarly to what was shown when smoking

images were presented with neutral images, both e-cigarette users and controls tend to avoid

smoking images and focus on vaping images. Within the total dwell time ANOVA, e-cigarette

users (occasional and daily users) differed significantly from controls, such that e-cigarette users

spent more time on e-cigarette images than the controls. However, both controls and e-cigarette

users spent more time on e-cigarettes over the smoking images. The first run time ANOVA was

Page 38: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

37

not significant, indicating that e-cigarette users and non-users did not differ in terms of their initial

attention towards vaping and smoking images.

This finding that both groups attend to vaping over smoking images may be partially due

to the aversiveness of the smoking images, in this demographic. Having grown up at the height of

the anti-tobacco ads, and then being exposed to e-cigarettes as healthier alternatives to tobacco

cigarettes, may have instilled the notion that smoking is dangerous and that e-cigarettes are less

dangerous.

Potential Implications

As the first study to measure attentional bias to e-cigarettes in e-cigarette users, the results

may have policy related implications as e-cigarette users differ from non-users in their attention

towards e-cigarette cues. Given this distinction, e-cigarette related cues in the real-world may have

a negative impact on current e-cigarette users in general, and especially those attempting to cut

down or quit use, as it may increase their e-cigarette craving and subsequent use (Field et al.,

2009). However, future studies would be needed to examine whether e-cigarette cues may have

adverse clinical effects (e.g., relapse) among e-cigarette users who are trying to reduce or quit their

use. Additionally, while occasional and daily users did not significantly differ, there was a

significant trend such that daily users demonstrated greater attentional bias. Future studies should

further explore the attentional bias of occasional and daily users with greater sample sizes to

examine whether frequency of use is associated with greater attentional bias among e-cigarette

users.

Given that the e-cigarette users did not display a bias towards the cigarette images, another

possible implication is that attentional biases may not be indicative of whether an individual will

transition from e-cigarettes to cigarettes. It is possible that the transition occurs more as a result of

Page 39: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

38

situational factors (i.e., an individual at a party wanting to use an e-cigarette and is offered a

tobacco cigarette instead). Future research examining when e-cigarette users transition to

cigarettes and what factors play a role in this transition would be highly informative.

Future Directions

There are numerous future directions that this research should explore. One of which our

research group plans to undertake is looking at dual-users of both e-cigarettes and traditional

cigarettes to determine how they differ from non-smoking e-cigarette users. This may provide

insights into the attention of dual users, specifically if they have a higher bias towards tobacco

cigarettes or e-cigarettes. An additional direction would be to deprive individuals of nicotine/e-

cigarette use prior to their testing session in order observe if their bias is larger when in deprivation.

By recruiting individuals who have and have not abstained from nicotine, a more comprehensive

evaluation can be made of how e-cigarette images affect users. Lastly, as research suggests people

are likely to vape in various social situations, future studies could investigate how a persons’

environment affects their attention towards e-cigarettes. Results from such a study may give more

indication as to which specific situation initiates one to use an e-cigarette, and furthermore, which

cues to avoid if an individual is attempting to cut down. Another important future direction would

be to develop a reliable scale which specifically measures e-cigarette dependence in never

smokers, former smokers and dual users of traditional cigarettes and e-cigarettes rather than

adapting measures from the tobacco literature, given the low internal consistency found in the

present study.

Limitations and Strengths

The study had some methodological limitations. Firstly, measures were self-report

(including the AUDIT, CUDIT, DASS-21, SUPPS-P, Penn State E-cigarette Dependence Index

Page 40: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

39

and QVC). Instead, semi-structured interviews would provide a better insight into several of the

constructs measured such as alcohol and cannabis use and psychopathology. This is a limitation

as self-report measures may be susceptible to biases. Another possible limitation is the use of a

student population. Having said that, the sample of university students can also be seen as a

strength as undergraduate students are the age range in which e-cigarette use is most prevalent

(Schoenborn & Gindi, 2015). An additional limitation is the poor internal consistency that was

present in the Penn State E-Cigarette Dependence Index. While a limitation, this is also an

interesting finding as it may point to the fact that never smoker, e-cigarette users are fundamentally

different than dual users, and former smoker, e-cigarette users. As well, traditional measures of

nicotine dependence may not fully translate to e-cigarette dependence. Using static images during

an eye-tracking task to measure an attentional bias also limits the results. It is possible that this

visual stimulus is not salient enough, and that their actual bias is towards vaping in the real world

which is not being peaked in this static design. A stronger study may use dynamic stimuli or use

an in-vivo approach, as well having a measure of physiological changes during the eye-tracking

paradigm. The physiological component may be important as it may be able to provide additional

insight over that of an individual’s attention. For example, it may be better able to determine if an

individual was avoiding a certain stimulus by examining their heartrate or skin conductance.

Along with these limitations, the study also had many strengths to the design. Firstly, eye-

gaze tracking was used to measure attentional biases, which as, a direct measure of attention has

higher internal validity over indirect measures (Ataya et al., 2012; Christiansen, Mansfield,

Duckworth, Field, & Jones, 2015; Field & Cox, 2008). An additional strength of the study was

that participants were matched between groups on age and gender to ensure age and gender did

not confound the results. Another strength regarding the sample was that they were all never

Page 41: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

40

smokers, given that much of the past literature has focused on smokers, who may or may not be

dual users. The use of strictly never smokers ensures that the biases shown toward e-cigarettes was

due to e-cigarette use and not transference from tobacco cigarettes as has been found in the

literature (Lochbuehler et al., 2018).

Conclusion

In conclusion, this was one of the first studies that measured attentional biases towards e-

cigarettes with e-cigarette users. The study utilized an eye-gaze tracking design to measure

attentional biases towards i) e-cigarettes and ii) tobacco cigarettes. In order to compare biases

scores, both e-cigarette users and non-users were recruited, both of which were never smokers of

traditional cigarettes. Overall, e-cigarette users do not display an attentional bias towards e-

cigarettes, or traditional cigarettes. However, e-cigarette users do significantly differ from non-

users in that they attend to e-cigarette stimuli for longer than non-users do, in the presence of either

neutral or traditional cigarette stimuli. The results of this study may be able to provide future

avenues for research such as examining how dual users of both e-cigarette and tobacco cigarettes

balance their attention towards these stimuli. As well, future studies may want to research whether

e-cigarette users with a higher bias are more likely to transition from e-cigarette use to traditional

cigarette use.

Page 42: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

41

References

Action on Smoking and Health. (2014). Use of Electronic Cigarettes in Great Britain.

Adamson, S. J., Kay-lambkin, F. J., Baker, A. L., Lewin, T. J., Thornton, L., Kelly, B. J., &

Sellman, J. D. (2010). An improved brief measure of cannabis misuse : The Cannabis Use

Disorders Identification Test-Revised ( CUDIT-R ) ଝ. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,

110(1–2), 137–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.02.017

Algom, D., Chajut, E., & Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at the emotional stroop phenomenon: A

generic slowdown, not a stroop effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,

Page 43: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

42

133(3), 323–338. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.323

Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). Psychometric

properties of the 42-item versions of the depression anxiety stress scales in clinical groups

and a xommunity sample. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 176–181.

https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.176

Ataya, A. F., Adams, S., Mullings, E., Cooper, R. M., Attwood, A. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2012).

Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias. Drug and Alcohol

Dependence, 121(1–2), 148–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.08.023

Barrington-Trimis, J. L., Berhane, K., Unger, J. B., Cruz, T. B., Urman, R., Chou, C. P., …

McConnell, R. (2016). The E-cigarette Social Environment, E-cigarette Use, and

Susceptibility to Cigarette Smoking. Journal of Adolescent Health, 59(1), 75–80.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.03.019

Barrington-Trimis, J. L., Urman, R., Berhane, K., Unger, J. B., Cruz, T. B., Samet, J. M., …

McConnell, R. (2016). E-Cigarettes and Future Cigarette Use. Pediatrics, 138(1), 1–8.

https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-0379

Bell, K., & Keane, H. (2012). Nicotine control: E-cigarettes, smoking and addiction.

International Journal of Drug Policy, 23(3), 242–247.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugpo.2012.01.006

Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Wright, T., & Field, M. (2003). Attentional bias in drug dependence:

Vigilance for cigarette-related cues in smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17(1),

66–72. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.17.1.66

Bunnell, R. E., Agaku, I. T., Arrazola, R. A., Apelberg, B. J., Caraballo, R. S., Corey, C. G., …

King, B. A. (2015). Intentions to smoke cigarettes among never-smoking US middle and

Page 44: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

43

high school electronic cigarette users: National youth tobacco survey, 2011-2013. Nicotine

and Tobacco Research, 17(2), 228–235. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntu166

Choi, K., & Forster, J. (2013). Characteristics associated with awareness, perceptions, and use of

electronic nicotine delivery systems among young US Midwestern adults. American

Journal of Public Health, 103(3), 556–561. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2012.300947

Christiansen, P., Mansfield, R., Duckworth, J., Field, M., & Jones, A. (2015). Internal reliability

of the alcohol-related visual probe task is increased by utilising personalised stimuli and

eye-tracking. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 155, 170–174.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.07.672

Cyders, M. A., Littlefield, A. K., Coffey, S., & Karyadi, K. A. (2014). Examination of a short

English version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale. Addictive Behaviors, 39(9),

1372–1376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.02.013

Dawkins, L., & Corcoran, O. (2014). Acute electronic cigarette use: Nicotine delivery and

subjective effects in regular users. Psychopharmacology, 231(2), 401–407.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-013-3249-8

Dawkins, L., Turner, J., Roberts, A., & Soar, K. (2013). “Vaping” profiles and preferences: An

online survey of electronic cigarette users. Addiction, 108(6), 1115–1125.

https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12150

Dowd, A. N., Motschman, C. A., & Tiffany, S. T. (2018). Development and Validation of the

Questionnaire of Vaping Craving. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, (April), 1–8.

https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/nty046

Duke, J. C., Lee, Y. O., Kim, A. E., Watson, K. A., Arnold, K. Y., Nonnemaker, J. M., & Porter,

L. (2014). Exposure to Electronic Cigarette Television Advertisements Among Youth and

Page 45: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

44

Young Adults. Pediatrics, 134(1), e29–e36. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-0269

Etter, J. F. (2017). Gateway effects and electronic cigarettes. Addiction, 113(10), 1776–1783.

https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13924

Etter, J. F., & Eissenberg, T. (2015). Dependence levels in users of electronic cigarettes, nicotine

gums and tobacco cigarettes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 147, 68–75.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.12.007

Field, M., & Cox, W. M. (2008). Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its

development, causes, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 97(1–2), 1–20.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.030

Field, M., Marhe, R., & Franken, I. H. A. (2014). The clinical relevance of attentional bias in

substance use disorders. CNS Spectrums, 19(3), 225–230.

https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852913000321

Field, M., Munafò, M. R., & Franken, I. H. A. (2009). A Meta-Analytic Investigation of the

Relationship Between Attentional Bias and Subjective Craving in Substance Abuse.

Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 589–607. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015843.A

Foulds, J., Veldheer, S., Yingst, J., Hrabovsky, S., Wilson, S. J., Nichols, T. T., & Eissenberg, T.

(2015). Development of a questionnaire for assessing dependence on electronic cigarettes

among a large sample of ex-smoking e-cigarette users. Nicotine and Tobacco Research,

17(2), 186–192. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntu204

Goniewicz, M. L., Lingas, E. O., & Hajek, P. (2013). Patterns of electronic cigarette use and user

beliefs about their safety and benefits: An Internet survey. Drug and Alcohol Review, 32(2),

133–140. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2012.00512.x

Grana, R., Benowitz, N., & Glantz, S. A. (2014). E-cigarettes: A scientific review. Circulation,

Page 46: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

45

129(19), 1972–1986. https://doi.org/10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.114.007667

Grant, J. E., Lust, K., Fridberg, D. J., King, A. C., & Chamberlain, S. R. (2019). E-cigarette use

(vaping) is associated with illicit drug use, mental health problems, and impulsivity in

university students. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry : Official Journal of the American

Academy of Clinical Psychiatrists, 31(1), 27–35. Retrieved from

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30699215

Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short-form version of the Depression anxiety stress

scales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample.

British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(2), 227–239.

https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X29657

Hershberger, A. R., Um, M., & Cyders, M. A. (2017). The relationship between the UPPS-P

impulsive personality traits and substance use psychotherapy outcomes: a meta-analysis.

Drug and Alcohol Dependence. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.05.032

Honsi, A., Mentzoni, R. A., Molde, H., & Pallesen, S. (2012). Attentional Bias in Problem

Gambling: A Systematic Review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29(123), 359–375.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-012-9315-z

Isaacowitz, D. M., Toner, K., Goren, D., & Wilson, H. R. (2008). Looking while unhappy:

Mood-congruent gaze in young adults, positive gaze in older adults. Psychological Science,

19(9), 848–853.

Jamal, A., King, B. A., Neff, L. J., Whitmill, J., Babb, S. D., & Graffunder, C. M. (2016).

Current Cigarette Smoking Among Adults — United States , 2005 – 2015. Morbidity and

Page 47: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

46

Mortality Weekly Report, 65(44), 1205–1211.

Jensen, R. P., Luo, W., Pankow, J. F., Strongin, R. M., & Peyton, D. H. (2015). Hidden

Formaldehyde in E-Cigarette Aerosols. The New England Journal of Medicine, 372(4),

389–392. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMc1414731

Kandel, D. (1975). Stages in Adolescent Involvement in Drug Use. Science, 190, 912–914.

Kandel, D. B., Yamaguchi, K., & Klein, L. C. (2006). Testing the gateway hypothesis.

Addiction, 101(4), 470–472. Retrieved from

http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&PAGE=reference&D=emed7&NEWS=N&AN=

2006120795

Kim, A. E., Arnold, K. Y., & Makarenko, O. (2014). E-cigarette advertising expenditures in the

U.S., 2011-2012. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 46(4), 409–412.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2013.11.003

King, A. C., Smith, L. J., Fridberg, D. J., Matthews, A. K., McNamara, P. J., & Cao, D. (2016).

Exposure to Electronic Nicotine Delivery Device (ENDS) Visual Imagery Increases

Smoking Urge and Desire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30(1), 106–112.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.08.003.Personal

King, A. C., Smith, L. J., McNamara, P. J., Matthews, A. K., & Fridberg, D. J. (2015). Passive

exposure to electronic cigarette (E-cigarette) use increases desire for combustible and e-

cigarettes in young adult smokers. Tobacco Control, 24(5), 501–504.

https://doi.org/10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-051563

Page 48: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

47

King, B., Alam, S., Promoff, G. P., Arrazola, R., & Dube, S. R. (2013). Awareness and Ever-Use

of Electronic Cigarettes Among US Adults, 2010-2011. Nicotine and Tobacco Research,

15(9), 1623–1627. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntt013

Klein, A. A. (2007). Suppression-induced hyperaccessibility of thoughts in abstinent alcoholics:

A preliminary investigation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(1), 169–177.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.012

Klein, E. G., Berman, M., Hemmerich, N., Carlson, C., Htut, S., & Slater, M. (2016). Online E-

cigarette Marketing Claims: A Systematic Content and Legal Analysis. Tobacco Regulatory

Science, 2(3), 252–262. https://doi.org/10.18001/TRS.2.3.5

Kosmider, L., Sobczak, A., Fik, M., Knysak, J., Zaciera, M., Kurek, J., & Goniewicz, M. L.

(2014). Carbonyl compounds in electronic cigarette vapors: Effects of nicotine solvent and

battery output voltage. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 16(10), 1319–1326.

https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntu078

Lee, P. N., & Fariss, M. W. (2017). A systematic review of possible serious adverse health

effects of nicotine replacement therapy. Archives of Toxicology, 91(4), 1565–1594.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00204-016-1856-y

Lee, Y. O., Hebert, C. J., Nonnemaker, J. M., & Kim, A. E. (2014). Multiple tobacco product use

among adults in the United States: Cigarettes, cigars, electronic cigarettes, hookah,

smokeless tobacco, and snus. Preventive Medicine, 62, 14–19.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.01.014

Leeman, R. F., Robinson, C. D., Waters, A. J., & Sofuoglu, M. (2014). A Critical Review of the

Page 49: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

48

Literature on Attentional Bias in Cocaine Use Disorder and Suggestions for Future

Research. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 22(6), 469–483.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037806

Levy, D. T., Cummings, K. M., Villanti, A. C., Niaura, R., Abrams, D. B., Fong, G. T., &

Borland, R. (2017). A framework for evaluating the public health impact of e-cigarettes and

other vaporized nicotine products. Addiction, 112(1), 8–17.

https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13394

Liu, G., Wasserman, E., Kong, L., & Foulds, J. (2017). A comparison of nicotine dependence

among exclusive E-cigarette and cigarette users in the PATH study. Preventive Medicine,

104, 86–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.04.001

Lochbuehler, K., Wileyto, E. P., Tang, K. Z., Mercincavage, M., Cappella, J. N., & Strasser, A.

A. (2018). Do current and former cigarette smokers have an attentional bias for e-cigarette

cues? Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(3), 316–323.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881117728418

Lovibond, P., & Lovibond, S. (1995). The Structure of Negative Emotional States: Scales

(DASS) With the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U

Lynam, D. R. (2013). Development of a short form of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale.

Unpublished Technical Report.

Lynam, D. R., Smith, G. T., Whiteside, S. P., & Cyders, M. A. (2006). The UPPS-P: Assessing

five personality pathways to impulsive behavior. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University.

Maglia, M., Caponnetto, P., Di Piazza, J., La Torre, D., & Polosa, R. (2017). Dual use of

Page 50: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

49

electronic cigarettes and classic cigarettes: a systematic review. Addiction Research &

Theory, 0(0), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2017.1388372

Mantey, D. S., Cooper, M. R., Clendennen, S. L., Pasch, K. E., & Perry, C. L. (2016). E-

Cigarette Marketing Exposure Is Associated with E-Cigarette Use among US Youth.

Journal of Adolescent Health, 58(6), 686–690.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.03.003

McGrath, D. S., Meitner, A., & Sears, C. R. (2018). The specificity of attentional biases by type

of gambling: An eye-tracking study. PLoS ONE, 13(1), 1–16.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190614

Mcmillen, R. C., Gottlieb, M. A., Shaefer, R. M. W., Winickoff, J. P., & Klein, J. D. (2018).

Trends in Electronic Cigarette Use Among U . S . Adults : Use is Increasing in Both

Smokers and Nonsmokers. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 17(10), 1195–1202.

https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntu213

McMillen, R., Maduka, J., & Winickoff, J. (2012). Use of emerging tobacco products in the

United States. Journal of Environmental and Public Health, 2012.

https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/989474

Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Field, M., & De Houwer, J. (2003). Eye movements to smoking-

related pictures in smokers: Relationship between attentional biases and implicit and

explicit measures of stimulus valence. Addiction, 98(6), 825–836.

https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00392.x

Mogg, K., Field, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional and approach biases for smoking cues

Page 51: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

50

in smokers: An investigation of competing theoretical views of addiction.

Psychopharmacology, 180(2), 333–341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-2158-x

Montreuil, A., MacDonald, M., Asbridge, M., Wild, T. C., Hammond, D., Manske, S., &

Rutherford, E. (2017). Prevalence and correlates of electronic cigarette use among Canadian

students: cross-sectional findings from the 2014/15 Canadian Student Tobacco, Alcohol and

Drugs Survey. CMAJ Open, 5(2), E460–E467. https://doi.org/10.9778/cmajo.20160167

Morean, M. E., Kong, G., Camenga, D. R., Cavallo, D. A., & Krishnan-Sarin, S. (2015). High

School Students’ Use of Electronic Cigarettes to Vaporize Cannabis. Pediatrics, 136(4),

611–616. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-1727

Munafò, M., Mogg, K., Roberts, S., Bradley, B. P., & Murphy, M. (2003). Selective processing

of smoking-related cues in current smokers, ex-smokers and never-smokers on the modified

Stroop task. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17(3), 310–316.

https://doi.org/10.1177/02698811030173013

Newman, K. R., & Sears, C. R. (2015). Eye Gaze Tracking Reveals Different Effects of a Sad

Mood Induction on the Attention of Previously Depressed and Never Depressed Women.

Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(3), 292–306. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-

9669-x

Paek, H. J., Kim, S., Hove, T., & Huh, J. Y. (2014). Reduced harm or another gateway to

smoking Source, message, and information characteristics of E-cigarette videos on

YouTube. Journal of Health Communication, 19(5), 545–560.

https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2013.821560

Page 52: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

51

Park, S. H., Lee, L., Shearston, J. A., & Weitzman, M. (2017). Patterns of electronic cigarette use

and level of psychological distress. PLoS ONE, 12(3), 1–13.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173625

Perkins, K. A., Karelitz, J. L., & Michael, V. C. (2015). Reinforcement enhancing effects of

acute nicotine via electronic cigarettes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 153, 104–108.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.05.041

Pokhrel, P., Fagan, P., Kehl, L., & Herzog, T. A. (2015). Receptivity to e-cigarette marketing,

harm perceptions, and e-cigarette use. Am. J. Health Behav., 39(1), 121–131.

https://doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.39.1.13

Reinert, D. F., & Allen, J. P. (2002). The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): A

Review of Recent Research. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 26(2), 272–

279. https://doi.org/10.1177/088541220201700103

Robinson, T., & Berridge, K. (2008). The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some

current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,

363(1507), 3137–3146. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0093

Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-

sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research, 18(3), 247–291.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P

Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2000). The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: an

incentive–sensitization view. Addiction, 95(8), 91–117.

https://doi.org/10.1080/09652140050111681

Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993).

Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO

Page 53: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

52

Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption-II.

Addiction, 88(6), 791–804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x

Schauer, G. L., Berg, C. J., Kegler, M. C., Donovan, D. M., & Windle, M. (2015). Assessing the

overlap between tobacco and marijuana: Trends in patterns of co-use of tobacco and

marijuana in adults from 2003-2012. Addictive Behaviors, 49, 26–32.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.05.012

Schneider, S., & Diehl, K. (2016). Vaping as a Catalyst for Smoking ? An Initial Model on the

Initiation of Electronic Cigarette Use and the Transition to Tobacco Smoking Among

Adolescents. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 18(5), 647–653.

https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntv193

Schoenborn, C., & Gindi, R. (2015). Electronic cigarette use among adults: United States, 2014.

National Center for Health Statistics, (217), 1–7. Retrieved from

http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/databriefs/db217.pdf

Sharapova, S. R., Singh, T., Agaku, I. T., Kennedy, S. M., & King, B. A. (2018). Patterns of E-

cigarette Use Frequency—National Adult Tobacco Survey, 2012–2014. American Journal

of Preventive Medicine, 54(2), 284–288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2017.09.015

Siegel, M. B., Tanwar, K. L., & Wood, K. S. (2011). Electronic cigarettes as a smoking-

cessation tool: Results from an online survey. American Journal of Preventive Medicine,

40(4), 472–475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2010.12.006

Soneji, S. S., Sung, H.-Y., Primack, B. A., Pierce, J. P., & Sargent, J. D. (2018). Quantifying

population-level health benefits and harms of e-cigarette use in the United States. Plos One,

Page 54: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

53

13(3), e0193328. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193328

Spindle, T. R., Hiler, M. M., Cooke, M. E., Eissenberg, T., Kendler, K. S., & Dick, D. M.

(2017). Electronic cigarette use and uptake of cigarette smoking: A longitudinal

examination of U.S. college students. Addictive Behaviors, 67, 66–72.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.12.009

Townshend, J. M., & Duka, T. (2001). Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: Differences

between heavy and occasional social drinkers. Psychopharmacology, 157(1), 67–74.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s002130100764

Tsai, J., Walton, K., Coleman, B. N., Sharapova, S. R., Johnson, S. E., Kennedy, S. M., &

Caraballo, R. S. (2018). Reasons for Electronic Cigarette Use Among Middle and High

School Students — National Youth Tobacco Survey, United States, 2016. MMWR.

Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 67(6), 196–200.

https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm6706a5

US Department of Health and Human Services. (2016). E-cigarette use among youth and young

adults: a report of the Surgeon General. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/tobacco

Vansickel, A. R., & Eissenberg, T. (2013). Electronic cigarettes: Effective nicotine delivery after

acute administration. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 15(1), 267–270.

https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntr316

Waters, A. J., & Feyerabend, C. (2000). Determinants and effects of attentional bias in smokers.

Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 14(2), 111–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-

164X.14.2.111

Page 55: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

54

Waters, Andrew J., Shiffman, S., Sayette, M. A., Paty, J. A., Gwaltney, C. J., & Balabanis, M. H.

(2003). Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation. Health Psychology, 22(4),

378–387. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.4.378

Weinberger, A. H., Pacek, L. R., Giovenco, D., Galea, S., Zvolensky, M. J., Gbedemah, M., &

Goodwin, R. D. (2019). Cigarette Use Among Individuals with Alcohol Use Disorders in

the United States, 2002 to 2016: Trends Overall and by Race/Ethnicity. Alcoholism:

Clinical and Experimental Research, 43(1), 79–90. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13922

Weiss, J. W., Cen, S., Schuster, D. V., Unger, J. B., Johnson, C. A., Mouttapa, M., … Cruz, T. B.

(2006). Longitudinal effects of pro-tobacco and anti-tobacco messages on adolescent

smoking susceptibility. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 8(3), 455–465.

https://doi.org/10.1080/14622200600670454

Wills, T. A., Gibbons, F. X., Sargent, J. D., & Schweitzer, R. J. (2016). How is the effect of

adolescent e-cigarette use on smoking onset mediated: A longitudinal analysis. Psychology

of Addictive Behaviors, 30(8), 876–886. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000213

Wright, R. D., & Ward, L. M. (2008). Orienting of attention. Oxford University Press.

Page 56: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

Tables

Table 1

Demographic Characteristics of E-Cigarette Users and Non-Users

E-Cig Users (n = 59) Non-Users (n = 61)

Characteristics Males (n = 30)

M (SD)/freq (n)

Females (n = 29)

M (SD)/freq (n)

Males (n = 28)

M (SD)/freq (n)

Females (n = 33)

M (SD)/freq (n)

Age 19.40 (1.22) 19.21 (1.78) 20.18 (1.83) 21.06 (3.17)

Ethnicity

White 73.3% (22) 69.0% (20) 21.4% (6) 57.6% (19)

Chinese 3.3% (1) 6.9% (2) 25.0% (7) 6.1% (2)

South Asian 6.7% (2) 10.3% (3) 14.3% (4) 9.1% (3)

Black 6.1% (2)

Filipino/Pacific

Islander

6.7% (2) 3.4% (1) 17.9% (5) 3.0% (1)

Latin American 10.0% (3) 6.9% (2) 3.6% (1) 6.1% (2)

Southeast Asian 7.1% (2) 3.0% (1)

Arab 3.3% (1) 3.4% (1) 10.7% (3) 9.1% (3)

West Asian 3.4% (1) 6.1% (2)

Korean 3.3% (1) 3.6% (1)

Other 6.7% (2) 3.4% (1) 3.6% (1)

Annual income

$0 - $25,000 13.3% (4) 20.7% (6) 21.4% (6) 27.3% (9)

$26,000 - $50,000 20.0% (6) 20.7% (6) 17.9% (5) 21.2% (7)

$51,000 - $100,000 33.3% (10) 17.2% (5) 35.7% (10) 33.3% (11)

Over $100,000 33.3% (10) 41.4% (12) 25.0% (7) 18.2% (6)

Page 57: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

56

Highest level of

education

No degree,

certificate or

diploma

0 3.4% (1) 0 6.1% (2)

High school

diploma

100.0% (30) 82.8% (24) 78.6% (22) 78.8% (26)

Trades or

apprenticeship

certificate

0 3.4% (1) 3.6% (1) 6.1% (2)

College, CEGEP or

other non-university

certificate

0 3.4% (1) 0 0

University degree,

certificate or

diploma

0 6.9% (2) 10.7% (3) 6.1% (2)

Bachelor’s degree 0 0 7.1% (2) 6.1% (2)

Master’s degree 0 0 0 3.0% (1)

Marital Status

Single 96.7% (29) 93.1% (27) 96.4% (27) 87.9% (29)

Common-law 3.3% (1) 3.4% (1) 0 9.1% (3)

Married 0 3.4% (1) 3.6% (1) 3.0% (1)

Note: M = mean, SD = standard deviation, E-Cig = electronic cigarette. Participants were able to select more than one ethnicity,

resulting in frequency counts higher than the sample size.

Page 58: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

57

Table 2

Comparison of Male and Female E-Cigarette Users on E-Cigarette Characteristics

E-Cig Users (n = 59)

Characteristic Males (n = 30)

M (SD)/freq (n)

Females (n = 29)

M (SD)/freq (n)

Test Statistic p Effect Size

(Cohen’s d)

E-Cigarette Use

Occasional 43.3% (13) 63.3% (19)

Daily 56.7% (17) 33.3% (10)

Nicotine Concentration 22.71mg (19.35) 20.00mg (18.37) t (52) = 0.51 .614 0.14

Penn State Total Score 4.2 (3.03) 3.45 (4.39) t (42) = 0.68 .504 0.20

Not dependent (0-3) 20.8% (10) 65.0% (14)

Low dependence (4-8) 66.7% (11) 10.3% (3)

Medium dependence (9-12) 12.5% (3) 13.7% (4)

High dependence (13+) 0 0

QVC Mean Score 2.2 (1.10) 1.9 (1.20) t (57) = 1.02 .313 0.27

Reasons for use

Appealing flavours 53.3% (16) 41.4% (12)

Experimenting with flavours 26.7% (8) 34.5% (10)

Most acceptable to non-smokers 10.0% (3) 20.7% (6)

Socializing with others 36.7% (11) 37.9% (11)

People who are important to me use them 26.7% (8) 55.2% (16)

People in the media/public figures use them 0 0

Help people quit smoking cigarettes 3.3% (1) 0

Page 59: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

58

Might be less harmful than cigarettes 43.3% (13) 48.3% (14)

Quit smoking using them 3.3% (1) 0

Use when I cannot smoke 0 0

Feel like regular cigarettes 0 3.4% (1)

They don’t smell 36.7% (11) 27.6% (8)

Cost less than other forms of tobacco 30.0% (9) 3.4% (1)

I think they are cool and/or intriguing 30.0% (9) 31.0% (9)

I like the nicotine buzz but not cigarettes 76.7% (23) 65.5% (19)

Use them to manage my weight 6.7% (2) 3.4% (1)

Other 16.7% (5) 10.3% (3)

Health effects

Respiratory problems 46.7% (14) 34.5% (10)

Headaches 33.3% (10) 48.3% (14)

Chest pains 10.0% (3) 17.2% (5)

Heart issues 0 0

Note: M = mean, SD = standard deviation, E-Cig = electronic cigarette, QVC = Questionnaire of Vaping Craving

Page 60: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

59

Table 3

Comparison of DASS-21 Scores Between E-Cigarette Users and Non-Users

Scale E-Cig Users (n = 59)

M (SD)

Non-Users (n = 61)

M (SD)

t Statistic p Effect Size

(Cohen’s d)

DASS-21

Depression 7.73 (7.01) 10.79 (10.64) 1.86a .065 0.34

Anxiety 8.61 (6.24) 9.64 (9.05) 0.73a .469 0.13

Stress 12.17 (8.02) 14.10 (9.94) 1.17 .245 0.21

Note: M = mean, SD = standard deviation, E-Cig = electronic cigarette, a indicates that equal variance was not assumed in the analysis,

DASS-21 = depression, anxiety and stress scale

Page 61: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

60

Table 4

Comparison of SUPPS-P Scores Between E-Cigarette Users and Non-Users

Scale E-Cig Users (n = 59)

M (SD)

Non-Users (n = 61)

M (SD)

t Statistic p Effect Size

(Cohen’s d)

SUPPS-P

Negative Urgency 2.30 (.77) 2.16 (.75) 1.02 .311 0.18

Positive Urgency 1.88 (.66) 1.71 (.64) 1.38 .170 0.25

Perseverance 1.77 (.43) 1.61 (.41) 2.02 .046 0.38

Premeditation 1.72 (.49) 1.57 (.48) 1.81 .074 0.33

Sensation Seeking 2.64 (.72) 2.56 (.68) 0.65 .515 0.11

Note: M = mean, SD = standard deviation, E-Cig = electronic cigarette, SUPPS-P = short UPPS-P impulsivity scale

Page 62: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

61

Table 5

Multiple Regression using E-cigarette Usage and Eye-Tracking Scores Among E-Cigarette Users

Variable B SEB p

Smoking vs vaping

Intercept 592.87 140.09

Nicotine concentration 7.64 3.90 .34 .058

QVC score -7.75 70.52 -.02 .913

Penn State total score -21.52 23.12 -.18 .358

Smoking vs neutral

Intercept -773.66 198.14

Nicotine concentration -11.62 5.52 -.36 .042

QVC score 77.31 99.74 .14 .443

Penn State total score 32.93 32.70 .19 .320

Vaping vs neutral

Intercept -175.55 186.96

Nicotine concentration -2.39 5.21 -.08 .649

QVC score -33.89 94.12 -.07 .721

Penn State total score 39.12 30.85 .26 .213

Note: B = unstandardized regression coefficient; SEB = standard error of the coefficient; = standardized coefficient. Smoking vs

vaping, DV was interest area dwell time difference score for vaping, smoking vs neutral, DV was interest area dwell time difference

score for smoking, and vaping vs neutral, DV was interest area dwell time difference score for vaping.

Page 63: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

62

Table 6

Comparison of Total Dwell Time Difference Scores Among Non, Occasional and Daily Users.

Total Dwell Time

Difference Score

Non-Users

n = 61

Occasional Users

n = 32

Daily Users

n = 27 F p

Partial

2

Observed

Power

Vaping vs Neutral

Trials -420.62 (478.86)a -184.40 (710.34) 3.48 (542.97) 5.73 .004 .09

.86

Vaping vs Smoking

Trials 351.45 (430.03)a,b 649.76 (485.49) 656.76 (612.31) 5.66 .005 .088

.85

Smoking vs Neutral

Trials -669.19 (613.02) -735.55 (656.69) -690.18 (452.20) 0.12 .886 .00

.07

Note: Post hoc differences based on Tukey’s LSD. a indicates a significant difference from daily users, and b indicates a significant

difference from occasional users

Page 64: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

63

Table 7

Comparison of First Run Dwell Time Difference Scores Among Non, Occasional and Daily Users.

First Run Dwell Time

Difference Score

Non-Users

n = 61

Occasional Users

n = 32

Daily Users

n = 27 F p

Partial

2

Observed

Power

Vaping vs Neutral

Trials -195.62 (332.68) -122.94 (469.98) -68.45 (269.47) 1.25 .289 .02

.27

Vaping vs Smoking

Trials 174.68 (247.22) 296.88 (257.32) 240.76 (286.99) 2.42 .093 .04

.48

Smoking vs Neutral

Trials -301.64 (396.06) -393.41 (433.82) -368.90 (286.40) 0.69 .506 .01

.16

Page 65: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

64

Appendix

Questionnaire package that participants filled out via Qualtrics.

Demographics, E-Cigarette Usage and Psychopathology

Below you will find some questions about yourself. Although you are not required to answer any questions

that make you uncomfortable, it is important to remember that all of your answers are completely

confidential.

1. How old are you? _______

2. Please indicate your gender:

Female

Male

Other (please specify): ___________________

3. Please indicate your marital status:

Single (never legally married)

Common-Law

Married

Separated

Divorced

Widowed

4. Please indicate your ethnicity?

Caucasian

Chinese

South Asian (e.g. East Indian, Pakistani, Sri Lankan)

Black (e.g. African, Haitian, Jamaican, Somali, etc.)

Filipino/ Pacific Islander

Latin American

Southeast Asian (e.g. Cambodian, Indonesian, Laotian, Vietnamese)

Arab

West Asian (e.g. Afghan, Iranian)

Japanese

Korean

Aboriginal (e.g. First Nation, Métis, Inuit)

Other (please specify)____________________________________

5. Area of residence:

Urban

Rural

6. Are you currently enrolled in a post-secondary institution?

Yes

No

7. Please indicate your highest level of education completed:

No degree, certificate or diploma

Page 66: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

65

High school diploma or equivalent

Trades or apprenticeship certificate or diploma

College, CEGEP (general and vocational college in QC), or other non-university certificate or diploma

University degree, certificate, or diploma below bachelor level

Bachelor’s degree

Master’s degree or diploma/certificate above bachelor level

Doctorate or other professional degree (please specify) _______________________

8. Which of the following best describes your present job status (please check all that apply)?

Employed full-time (30 hours per week or more)

Employed part-time (less than 30 hours per week)

Unemployed

Student

Retired

Other (please specify): ______________________

9. Household income:

$0 - $25,000

$26,000 - $50,000

$51,000 - $100,000

Over $100,000

10. What colour would you identify your eyes to be (according to your driver’s license)?

Hazel

Brown

Black

Grey

Green

Blue

Other (please specify): ______________________

11. How important is religion in your life?

Very important

Somewhat important

Not very important

Not important at all

12. How important is spirituality in your life?

Very important

Somewhat important

Not very important

Not important at all

13. What religious group are you most affiliated with?

Catholic

Muslim

Protestant

Jewish

Buddhist

Page 67: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

66

Hindu

Sikh

No religion (e.g., agnostic, atheist, etc.)

Other Christian (e.g., Anglican, United, Presbyterian, etc.)

Other (please specify): ________________________

E-Cigarette/Vaping Use

1. Have you ever smoked tobacco cigarettes?

Yes

No

2. Do you currently smoke cigarettes?

Daily

Occasionally

Not at all

3. Have you smoked 100 cigarettes in your lifetime?

Yes

No

4. Have you ever used any of the following tobacco products? If yes, please indicate how many times

you have used that product

Hookah; times

Small Cigars; times

Chewing Tobacco; times

Smokeless tobacco products; times

5. Have you ever vaped or used electronic (e-cigarettes)?

Yes

No

6. Do you currently vape or use e-cigarettes?

Daily

Occasionally

Not at all

7. Are you currently trying to quit using e-cigarettes or vaping?

Yes

No

Eventually

Never

8. At what age did you first try vaping or an e-cigarette? years

9. Please describe your e-cigarette or vaping device:

a. Is your device: ◻ Rechargeable ◻ Disposable

◻ Other, please specify:

Page 68: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

67

b. Does it contains a tank system?

◻ Yes

◻ No

c. Please select the usual level of nicotine in your e-liquid

◻ 0 mg

◻ 6 mg

◻ 12 mg

◻ 18 mg

◻ 26 mg

◻ Other: mg

◻ Don’t Know

d. Do you use any other substances with your vaporizer/e-cigarette (e.g., marijuana)?

◻ No

◻ Yes, please list ___________________________

10. Please describe your typical e-cigarette/vaporizing use:

a. Approximately how many puffs per session? puffs

b. What is your average length of session (in minutes)? minutes

Page 69: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

68

11. What reasons do you use e-cigarettes/vaporizer? Select all that apply

They come in appealing flavors

I like experimenting with various flavors

They are most acceptable to non-smokers

I like socializing with other users

People who are important to me use them

People in the media or other public figures use them

They help people quit smoking cigarettes

They might be less harmful than cigarettes

I quit smoking regular cigarettes using them

I use them when I cannot smoke regular cigarettes

Using them feels like smoking regular cigarettes

They don’t smell

They cost less than other forms of tobacco

I use them because I think they are cool and/or intriguing

I like the buzz from nicotine but I don’t like regular cigarettes

I use them to manage my weight

12. How soon after you wake in the morning do you use your e-cigarette/vaporizer?

Within 5 minutes

6-30 minutes

31-60 minutes

After 60 minutes

I do not use every day

13. Have you ever had any of the following adverse or negative reactions to e-cigarettes/vaporizer?

Respiratory problems (cough, bronchitis, etc.)

Headaches

Chest pains

Heart issues

Page 70: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

Penn State Index

1. How many times per day do you usually use your electronic cigarette or vaporizer?

Assume that one “time” consist of around 15 puffs or lasts around 10 minutes

0-4

5-9

10-14

15-19

20-29

30+

2. On days that you can use your e-cigarette/vaporizer freely, how soon after you wake up

do you first use your electronic cigarette

0-5 minutes

6-15 minutes

16-30 minutes

31-60 minutes

60-120 minutes

120+

3. Do you sometimes awaken at night to use your e-cigarette/vaporizer

Yes

No

4. If yes, how many nights per week do you typically awaken to use your e-

cigarette/vaporizer?

0-1 nights

2-3 nights

4+ nights

5. Do you use an e-cigarette/vaporizer now because it is really hard to quit?

Yes

No

6. Do you ever have strong cravings to use an e-cigarette/vaporizer?

Yes

No

7. Over the past week, how strong have the urges to use e-cigarette/vaporizer been?

None/Slightly strong

Moderate/Strong

Page 71: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

70

Very Strong/Extremely Strong

8. Is it hard to keep from using an e-cigarette/vaporizer in places where you are not

supposed to?

Yes

No

9. Did you feel more irritable because you couldn’t use an e-cigarette/vaporizer?

Yes

No

10. Did you feel nervous, restless or anxious because you couldn’t use an e-

cigarette/vaporizer?

Yes

No

Page 72: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

71

QVC - Questionnaire of Vaping Craving

Using the follow scale below, please indicate your level of agreement with the following

statements:

I have a strong desire for an e-cigarette right now.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

I have an urge for an e-cigarette.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

All I want right now is an e-cigarette.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

I am missing vaping right now.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

I am craving an e-cigarette right now.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

I need to vape now.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

Page 73: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

72

I am going to vape as soon as possible.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

I will vape as soon as I get the chance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

Nothing would be better than vaping right now.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

Vaping would make me happier now.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Strongly

Disagree

Neither

Agree nor

Disagree

Strongly

Agree

Page 74: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

73

DASS-21

Please read each statement below and circle a number 0, 1, 2 or 3, which indicates

how much the statement applied to you over the past week. There are no right or

wrong answers. Do not spend too much time on any statement.

The rating scale is as follows:

0 Does not apply to me at all - NEVER

1 Applies to me to some degree, or some of the time – SOMETIMES

2 Applies to me to a considerable degree, or a good part of time - OFTEN

3 Applies to me very much, or most of the time - ALMOST ALWAYS

Page 75: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

74

1. I found it hard to wind down

0

1

2

3

2. I was aware of dryness of my mouth

0

1

2

3

3. I couldn’t seem to experience any positive feeling at all

0

1

2

3

4. I experienced breathing difficulties (eg, excessively rapid

breathing, breathlessness in the absence of physical exertion)

0

1

2

3

5. I found it difficult to work up the initiative to do things

0

1

2

3

6. I tended to over-react to situations

0

1

2

3

7. I experienced trembling (eg, in the hands)

0

1

2

3

8. I felt that I am using a lot of nervous energy

0

1

2

3

9. I was worried about situations in which I might panic and make a fool

of myself

0

1

2

3

10. I felt that I have nothing to look forward to

0

1

2

3

11. I found myself getting agitated

0

1

2

3

12. I found it difficult to relax

0

1

2

3

13. I felt down-hearted and blue

0

1

2

3

14. I was intolerant of anything that kept me from getting on with what I

was doing

0

1

2

3

15. I felt I was close to panic

0

1

2

3

16. I was unable to become enthusiastic about anything

0

1

2

3

17. I felt I was not worth much as a person

0

1

2

3

18. I felt that I was rather touchy

0

1

2

3

19. I was aware of the action of my heart in the absence of physical exertion

(eg, sense of heart rate increase, heart missing a beat)

0

1

2

3

20. I felt scared without any good reason

0

1

2

3

21. I felt that life was meaningless

0

1

2

3

Page 76: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

75

AUDIT

Please circle the answer that is correct for you:

1. How often do you have a drink containing alcohol?

0 1 2 3 4

Never Monthly or less Two to four

times a month

Two to three

times a week

Four or more

times a week

2. How many drinks containing alcohol do you have on a typical day when you are

drinking?

0 1 2 3 4

1 or 2 3 or 4 5 or 6 7 to 9 10 or more

3. How often do you have six or more drinks on one occasion?

0 1 2 3 4

Never Less than

monthly

Monthly Weekly Daily or

almost daily

4. Have often during the last year have you found that you were not able to stop drinking

once you had started?

0 1 2 3 4

Never Less than

monthly

Monthly Weekly Daily or

almost daily

5. How often during the last year have you failed to do what was normally expected from

you because of drinking?

0 1 2 3 4

Never Less than

monthly

Monthly Weekly Daily or

almost daily

Page 77: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

76

6. How often during the last year have you needed a first drink in the morning to get

yourself going after a heavy drinking session?

0 1 2 3 4

Never Less than

monthly

Monthly Weekly Daily or

almost daily

7. How often during the last year have you had a feeling of guilt or remorse after drinking?

0 1 2 3 4

Never Less than

monthly

Monthly Weekly Daily or

almost daily

8. How often during the last year have you been unable to remember what happened the

night before because you had been drinking?

0 1 2 3 4

Never Less than

monthly

Monthly Weekly Daily or

almost daily

9. Have you or someone else been injured as result of your drinking?

0 1 2 3 4

No Yes, but not in

the last year

Yes, during

the last year

10. Has a relative or friend, or a doctor or other health worker been concerned about your

drinking or suggested you cut down?

0 1 2 3 4

No Yes, but not in

the last year

Yes, during

the last year

Page 78: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

77

CUDIT-R

1. How often do you use cannabis?

• Never

• Monthly or less

• 2-4 times a month

• 2-3 times a week

• 4 or more times a week

2. How many hours were you “stoned” on a typical day when you had been using cannabis? (less

than 1 thru to 7 or more)?

• Less than 1

• 1 or 2

• 3 or 4

• 5 or 6

• 7 or more

3. How often during the past 6 months did you find that you were not able to stop using cannabis

once you had started?

• Never

• Less than monthly

• Monthly

• Weekly

• Daily or almost daily

4. How often during the past 6 months did you fail to do what was normally expected from you

because of using cannabis?

• Never

• Less than monthly

• Monthly

• Weekly

• Daily or almost daily

Page 79: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

78

5. How often in the past 6 months have you devoted a great deal of your time to getting, using, or

recovering from cannabis?

• Never

• Less than monthly

• Monthly

• Weekly

• Daily or almost daily

6. How often in the past 6 months have you had a problem with your memory or concentration

after using cannabis?

• Never

• Less than monthly

• Monthly

• Weekly

• Daily or almost daily

7. How often do you use cannabis in situations that could be physically hazardous, such as

driving, operating machinery, or caring for children?

• Never

• Less than monthly

• Monthly

• Weekly

• Daily or almost daily

8. Have you ever thought about cutting down, or stopping, your use of cannabis?

• Never

• Yes, but not in the past 6 months

• Yes, during the past 6 months

Page 80: Attentional Bias in Non-Smokers Who Use an Electronic

ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN E-CIG USERS

79

SUPPS-P

Below are a number of statements that describe ways in which people act and think. For each

statement please indicate how much you agree or disagree with it.

Disagree

Strongly

Disagree

Somewhat

Agree

Somewhat

Agree

Strongly

I generally like to see things through to the end. o o o o

My thinking is usually careful and purposeful. o o o o

When I am in a great mood, I tend to get into

situations that could cause me problems. o o o o

Unfinished tasks really bother me. o o o o

I like to stop and think things over before I do

them.

o o o o

When I feel bad, I will often do things I later

regret in order to make myself feel better now. o o o o

Once I get going on something, I hate to stop. o o o o

Sometimes when I feel bad, I can’t seem to stop

what I am doing even though it is making me feel

worse.

o o o o

I quite enjoy taking risks. o o o o

I tend to lose control when I am in a great mood. o o o o

I finish what I start. o o o o

I tend to value and follow a rational, “sensible”,

approach to things. o o o o

When I am upset I often act without thinking. o o o o

I welcome new and exciting experiences and

sensations, even if they are a little frightening

and unconventional.

o o o o

When I feel rejected, I will often say things that

I later regret. o o o o

I would like to learn to fly an airplane. o o o o

Others are shocked or worried about the things I

do when I am feeling excited. o o o o

I would enjoy the sensation of skiing very fast

down a high mountain slope. o o o o

I usually think carefully before doing anything. o o o o

I tend to act without thinking when I am really

excited. o o o o