30
ON THE MENTAL LEXICON HEINZ VATER 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe the mental lexicon (ML) on the basis of the representations by EMMOREY / FROMKIN (1988), LEVELT (1989), SCHWARZ (1992; 1996 2 ) and HANDKE (1995) and then show that some of their claims concerning the organization of the ML and the structure of lexical entries have to be modified due to more recent research. In the beginning, I would like to describe the place of psycholinguistics within the realm of linguistics. Linguistics covers a vast area of topics and is divided into many subdisciplines including the nuclear areas (phonology, morphology, syntax and semantics), and interdisciplinary areas as phonetics, computational ling., mathematical linguistics, philosophical linguistics, textlinguistics, neuro-. and psycholinguistics.

1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

ON THE MENTAL LEXICON

HEINZ VATER

1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks

After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

the mental lexicon (ML) on the basis of the representations by

EMMOREY / FROMKIN (1988), LEVELT (1989), SCHWARZ (1992;

19962) and HANDKE (1995) and then show that some of their claims

concerning the organization of the ML and the structure of lexical

entries have to be modified due to more recent research. In the

beginning, I would like to describe the place of psycholinguistics

within the realm of linguistics. Linguistics covers a vast area of topics

and is divided into many subdisciplines including the nuclear areas

(phonology, morphology, syntax and semantics), and interdisciplinary

areas as phonetics, computational ling., mathematical linguistics,

philosophical linguistics, textlinguistics, neuro-. and

psycholinguistics.

Page 2: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

176

Fig. 1 subdisciplines of linguistics

Neurolinguistics

1.1 Areas of psycholinguistics

BIERWISCH (1987: 646) formulates three questions

psycholinguistics has to answer:

[1] What is knowledge of language? [2] How is knowledge of language acquired? [3] How is knowledge of language put to use?

[1] concerns the static ("representational") aspect of

psycholinguistics, i.e. primarly the question how language is stored in

Phonology

Syntax

Phonetics

Semantics Morphology

Computational linguistics

Psycho-linguistics

Language philosophy

Mathematical lingustics

Pragmatics

Sociolinguistics

Text linguistics

Page 3: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

177

the human memory, whereas [2] and [3] concern the dynamic

("procedural") aspect. [2] characterizes the area of language

acquisition, and [3] the area of language processing (speech

production and speech comprehension). CHOMSKY (1986) mentions

language loss (e.g. in aphasia) as a fourth area of psycholinguistics;

this is also a section of neurolinguistics. Further areas concern:

– the relationship between language and perception (cf. MILLER / JOHNSON-LAIRD 1976);

– the relationship between language and thought (WYGOTSKI 1964, HÖRMANN 1967, ENGELKAMP 1974);

– onomatopoesis and sound imitaion (cf. HÖRMANN 1967, XII); – the neuronal base of language functions (cf. NAUTA / FEIRTAG 19889

and GESCHWIND 19889).

Cognitive Linguistics (CL) has been defined (a) as a

subdiscipline of psycholinguistics concerning cognitive structures and

processes (complementary to affective processes; cf. the schema by

GRIMM / ENGELKAMP 1981: 18), (b) as linguistics under cognitive

aspect (cf. BIERWISCH 1987 and SCHWARZ 19962). I prefer viewing

CL as an interdisciplinary area (a), where linguistics meets cognitive

science, since I hold that there is no need for a term characterizing

linguistics as a cognitive discipline. "Cognition" can be defined,

according to SCHWARZ (19962: 36) as "the set of all mental structures

and processes comprising the total of human activities having to do

with knowledge“. Perception, language and thought are cognitive

systems belonging to the mental equipment of man. Fig. 2 gives a

survey of the cognitive areas.

Page 4: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

178

Fig. 2 cognitive systems and processes related to them Mind

Perception Language Thought

vis. acst. tact. olfct. gust. lang.re- lang.pro- lang.loss syst. syst. syst. syst. syst. present. cessing vis. acst. tact. olfct. gust. Mental speech speech proc. proc. proc. proc. proc. Lexicon production recept. other systems language Memory acquisition SIS STM LTM Abbreviations: acst. = acoustic syst. = system gust. = gustatory tact. = tactile lang. = language vis. = visual olfct. = olfactory LTM = long term memory proc. = process SIS = sensory information storage recept. = reception STM = short term memory

Concerning the memory, ATKINSON / SHIFFRIN (1968) dis-

tinguish three stages: a sensory information storage (SIS), where a

stimulus is stored only for a fragment of a second, a short term

memory, where elements are processed from ca. 250 msec up to a

couple of minutes, and a long term memory (LTM), being a

permanent store (cf. 1.3 for greater detail).

Page 5: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

179

There are specific processes associated with every system; e.g.

speech production and reception processes are associated with the

linguistic system. The systematic part of thought comprises memory

(as a store of thoughts or thought structures), the mental lexicon

forming a part of it. A person’s memory is built up gradually, and it

can also be destroyed gradually. For FODOR (1983), perception and

language are input systems, whereas thought (incl. argumentation,

learning and problem solving) is considered to be a central system.

FODOR (1983: 2ff) gives the following description of input systems:

− Input systems are modality and domain specific. Domains are the areas of physical stimuli that are operated on in the specific modalities, e.g. light waves in the visual modality, acoustic waves in the auditorial modality. Central systems are neutral in relation to modalities and domains.

− Input systems function quickly and automatically, whereas central systems are slower and less automatic; thought processes can be exerted in a conscious way.

− Input systems are "encapsulated" according to FODOR (1983:2), whereas central systems have unrestricted access to data from memory as well as from input systems.

All this means that input systems are modular, central systems

are holistic (CARSTON 1988: 43). As for myself, I do not think that

language is automatic and reflex-like.1 Some phenomena speak

against it:

− Usually we have a choice as to how express a message. Thus, all sentences in (1-01) express the same state of affairs, but they do it in different ways as to the emotional attitude expressed in relation to it:

1 "Fodor (1985) talks of natural language parsing and visual processing as

reflex-like; they are reflex-like in their automaticity and encapsulation. However, this analogy breaks down with regard to the other main property of reflexes, that is their direct unmediated response to the appropriate stimulus". (CARSTON 1988:42).

Page 6: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

180

(01) a The president passed away. b The president deceased. c The president died. d The president kicked the bucket. e The president croaked. (02) a Would you please leave me alone? b Please, go! c Get lost! − Slips of the tongue betray that the complex planning of an utterance can

get mixed up on different steps; thus, (03)a concerns lexical planning, (03)b phonological realization:

(03) a Das Haus verkommelt. (verkommt / vergammelt) (WIESE 1987: 50) "The house is wearing down" (i.e. tearing down / wearing out) b ... in the fast pew weeks (in the past few weeks) (FROMKIN 1973: 219)

1.2 Psycholinguistic tests as evidence of the Mental Lexicon

Psychology is based on observations (cf. ARNOLD / EYSENCK /

MEILI 19918: 1369): Since precise single observations are not

sufficient, at least two observations of the same phenomenon have to

be compared. For the mental lexicon, tests from all areas of

psycholinguistics and neurolinguistics have been applied:

− Tests checking language production, − tests checking language comprehension, − tests checking language acquisition, − tests checking the performance of people suffering from aphasia &

Williams syndrome, − analysis of slips of the tongue and TOT phenomena, − positron-emission tomography (PET) and other methods applied in

neuro-anatomy.

In all these cases, on-line as well as off-line tests have been

applied. The analysis of slips of the tongue and of tip-of-the-tongue

(TOT) phenomena constitute indirect methods primarily used to

investigate speech production but can also tell us something about the

organization and the functioning of the mental lexicon (which is

Page 7: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

181

neutral as to speech production and comprehension). In online tests,

the test persons themselves can be involved, e.g. choosing a certain

type of utterance by pushing a button (cf. SCHWARZ 19962). In (04),

the testees have to choose one of the two options as an adequate

continuation:

(04) a If you are a pilot, landing planes ... is /are dangerous. b If you are close to an airport, landing planes ... is/are dangerous.

There are several ways to carry through offline tests, e.g. by

having the testees repeat some utterances or judge their grammat-

icality. As examples for slips of the tongue, (03)a concerns lexical

planning, indicating that there must be a semantic as well as a

morphological organization of the entries in the ML; the two entries

that got mixed up (vergammeln and verkommen) are closely related in

semantic respect, the first one expressing a deterioration of the outer

appearance, the second one a deterioration of outer appearance or of

inherent quality. At the same time, the two verbs are morphologically

related by being derivates with the same prefix ver-. (03)b, showing a

confusion in the phonological realization, is a case of metathesis.

1.3 The memory

The memory is, according to SCHWARZ (19962: 35) "the storing

of ontogenetically acquired information". The ability of the human

organism to store experience in a permanent way is the precondition

for all complex behavioral activities (including thought processes).

The structure of the memory has been investigated especially by

ATKINSON / SHIFFRIN (1968). Their model contains three components:

Page 8: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

182

Fig. 3 the memory

memorizing

sensory informa- short-term long-term tion storage (SIS) memory (STM) memory (LTM)

forgetting forgetting

The sensory information storage is an iconic storage storing

visual or acoustic stimuli for a fraction of a second. WETTLER (1980:

16) describes an experiment, where visul stimuli, e.g. letters, were

offered to the testees in a chessboard pattern for 50 milliseconds.

Immediately after it, the same stiuli were shown in the same positions

as the stimuli presented before. The testees have to tell whether the

second stimulus is identical to the first one in the same position. The

testees were able to do this. However, if the task is modified slightly,

e.g. by replacing a small letter by a capital letter, the performance of

the testee does not exceed the level of accidental hit or miss.

According to MILLER (1956), seven elements (plus minus two)

can be stored in the STM simultaneously. They remain there as long

as you pay them conscious attention (cf. WETTLER 1990: 17). The

information stored in the LTM is not just a copy of the memory trace

in the STM. Thus, the representation of Schimmel does, in STM, not

distinguish between "white horse" ("weißes Pferd" and "fungus"

(Schimmelpilz"). In the LTM, for each of these homonyms a special

trace is constructed; on the other hand, Schimmel1 and weißes Pferd

Page 9: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

183

("white horse") have the same or similar representations: the LTM is

considered to be a semantic memory. According to ATKINSON/

SHIFFRIN (1968) stimuli are transferred from the SIS into the STM,

from there into LTM. They neglect the fact that the representation of a

word in the STM (our working memory) obviously depends on

knowledge (also concerning the social acceptability) stored in the

LTM, which can be exemplified by "refusal of perception": The time

span for the correct recognition of a word in brief visual presentation

is higher for words that are socially tabooed than for neutral words

(e.g. Arsch "ass" vs. Barsch "sole"). Inspite of this shortcoming, the

model by ATKINSON/ SHIFFRIN (1968) was successfully used for the

interpretation of many experiments (WETTLER 1980: 18).

The main differences between STM and LTM are the following:

– The STM has a limited capacity, the LTM has an unlimited one.

– The STM encodes primarily physical and phonological characteristics, the LTM semantic ones (cf. Schimmel).

– Whereas the synthesis of certain protein enzymes plays an important role for the functioning of the LTM (as shown by experiments with rats that had to remember ways through a labyrinth), it does not play a role in the functioning of the STM.

According to SCHWARZ 19962: 87), concepts are the minimal

entries of storage in memory, storing knowledge of the world.2

2 According to SCHWARZ (19962: 80), clinical observations show that the

Hippocampus is responsable for long term storing of information. But on the whole, there is no distinct relation between local damages of the brain and the occurrence of disturbances of memory.

Page 10: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

184

2. The mental lexicon (ML) 2.0 General remarks

The mental lexicon constitutes a central area of language

representation. SCHWARZ (19962: 35) holds that "linguistic

competence is a system of knowledge, encoded in memorial traces".

The human lexicon is, according to HANDKE (1995: 50) the central

module of a language processing system, interacting with the other

components of the language processor and containing detailed

information concerning the words to be produced or comprehended.

The mechanical lexica contain a section of the total vocabulary of a

language (e.g. the weather-forecast lexica), whereas the mental

lexicon (unless it has not been damaged by a lesion of the brain)

contains the total vocabulary. CUTLER (1994: 81) emphasizes that a

lexicon is acquired:

"The contents of a lexicon are so patently language-specific that it goes without saying that a lexicon cannot be inborn – it must be acquired, on the basis of linguistic experience."

As to the entries of the lexicon, there is agreement on the fact

"that the contents of a lexicon consist of sound-to-meaning mappings

in discrete chunks" CUTLER (1994: 82). But what is the size of these

"sound-to-meaning mappings"? Most linguists agree on words being

the main type of entries stored in the ML. The word as a linguistic unit

is not easy to define.

A difference has to be made between inflectional words like

child vs. children and lexical words like CHILD. There is more or less

Page 11: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

185

agreement on lexical words (lexemes) rather than inflectional words

being considered to be entries of the ML (cf. HANDKE 1995: 52). On

the other hand, it is not easy to tell what has to be considered to be a

lexeme.3 Homonyms are usually counted as two different lexemes.

There are unproblematic cases like Eng. hole "hollow space" and

whole "entire(ty)", which are differentiated in spelling or Ger. Bank1

(Eng. "bank") and Bank2 ("Eng. "bench") that are differentiated in

their inflection (Banken vs. Bänke).

Inflectional words as stateN and stateV have to be associated to

two different lexemes for the same reason. The question how to

differentiate between different lexemes has not been answered in a

satisfactory way. For this reason, the number of words (lexemes) an

average person has stored in the ML is a matter of disagreement.4

There are probably big differences between different speakers of the

same language; the temporal factor plays a role as well, since every

person learns new words or lexical expressions continually. Thus, we

should assume that a person’s ML has changed more or less after

reading a scholarly article or listening to a lecture.

3 A lexical word (lexeme), being an abstract unit can only be made audible (or

visible) by using one of the inflectional words as its representative (e.g. the nom. sing. with nouns or the infinitive with verbs).

4 AITCHISON (1987:6ff) assumes that a speaker of English knows between 50.000 and 250.000 words. What does it mean to know a word? "Knowing" is to be interpreted as comprehending; it concerns the passive vocabulary. There are no estimates concerning the active vocabulary – except that it forms a subset of the passive vocabulary.

Page 12: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

186

Also morphemes (at least as constituents of regular words; cf.

2.2) have to be taken into account as storage entries, as well as idioms

or "phraseolexemes" (cf. TOPCZEWSKA 2004).5

The structure of the ML is a matter of discussion too. Most

models postulate modular lexical structures as well as (phonological,

morphological etc.) subcomponents of the ML. Disturbances concern-

ing specific areas of the lexicon are taken as evidence (cf. 1.3). Jean

AITCHISON (1987) − cf. also AITCHISON (19984) − gives a declarative

representation of the lexicon. In describing the "discovery trips" ling-

uists have to carry out, she quotes a claim made by Sherlock Holmes:

"'From a drop of water', said the writer, 'a logician could infer the possibility of an Atlantic or a Niagara without having seen or heard of one or the other. So all life is a great chain, the nature of which is known whenever we are shown a single link of it.'" (Arthur Conan Doyle, A Study in Scarlet)

According to AITCHISON (1987), evidence relevant for the study

of the ML consists of four types:

– Word-retrieval problems of normal speakers, – word-retrieval problems of speakers with language deficits, – psycholinguistic experiments (association tests etc.), – results of theoretiscal linguistics.

EMMOREY / FROMKIN (1988: 145f) propose a model of the ML:

– The mental lexicon contains independent sublexica that specify different categories of lexical information – e.g. phonological, semantic and (with literate people) orthographic information. They are connected in a network (as shown by priming effects); in dyslexia, these connections are cut off.

5 FROMKIN / RODMAN (19935: 124) call the lexicon "a mental storehouse of

information about words and morphemes".

Page 13: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

187

– The phonological units contain abstract phonemic representations. In speech production, the phonological chain must be transformed into a sequence of commands to the motor system.

– Orthographic representations take regular and irregular spellings into account.

− Morphologically related words are listed in one entry or in a cluster (cf. 2.2).

– Lexical and grammatical morphemes are marked as such, as has been shown by differences in the ways they are processed.

– In the lexical-semantic subcomponent words are grouped according to semantic features.

Detailed information concerning the structure of the ML is

found in LEVELT (1989), in connection with language production.

HANDKE (1995: 69) adopts Levelt's structure of lexical entries:

Fig. 4 ACCESS UNIT (according to HANDKE 1995: 69)

Morphological Phonological Specification Specification Form

lexical specification Lexical Pointer Syntactic Conceptual Specification Specification Lemma

Unfortunately, most linguists dealing with the ML do not

answer the question whether linguistic concepts are stored separately

from cognitive concepts or together or as part of them (i.e. linguistic

concepts forming a subset of general concepts).

Page 14: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

188

2.1 Phonological structure of lexical entries

According to EMMOREY / FROMKIN (1988:145f), the

phonological subcomponent contains lists that are organized

according to onset segments and syllables (cf. the cohort approach).

But there are also other organizations, e.g. according to mono- and

polysyllabic words, frequencies of words, etc.

LEVELT (1989) claims that the phonological structures furnished

by Autosegmental Phonology consisting of a skeletal tier and a

segmental tier provide adequate patterns for the phonological

structures of lexical units:

σ

R

O N K

(05) C C V C

b l I t s

(06) ω

σ σ

R R

O N O N

C V C V

h I m l

One of the reasons for the fact hat AP has been widely accepted

among phonologists is the circumstance that there is no 1:1

correspondance between the two tiers: the affricate /ts/ has to be

analysed as a sequence of a plosive and a fricative, because there is a

difference in the features ([−continuous] with /t/, [+continuous] with

Page 15: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

189

/s/, all other feature specifications being the same), whereas /ts/

behaves as a unitary entry in phonotactic respect (e.g. by having the

same combinatory potential as /t/ or /p/). It has been shown that in

slips of the tongue and the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon, the

phonotactic structure (i.e. the skeletal tier of AP) is maintained,

whereas a replacement, omission or addition took place on the

segmental tier. Thus e.g., when I could not find the name of the

famous tenor Luciano Pavarotti, Ruggiero came to my mind as a first

try, a name that has the same syllable structure and stress pattern as

Luciano.

Facts from early child language indicate that phonological

representations stored in the ML are not necessarily identical to

"spelled out" articulated sound sequences. WODE (1993, 1994)

mentions cases where small children said [tat] instead of [ʃtat] but

rejected the pronunciation [tat] offered by grown up people insisting

on that they said [tat] rather than [tat]! This shows that their

articulatory organs were not ready for the cluster [ʃtat] yet, although

they had stored it in the ML.6

2.2 Morphological structure of lexical entries

According to LEVELT (1989), the morphological structures of

simple or complex words are stored in the ML as shown in (07) −

6 JAKOBSON (1969: 23) reports a similar case, where a French speaking girl used

torchon for garcon as well as for cochon, but protested when grown ups did not make the difference, using cochon for a boy or garcon for a pig.

Page 16: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

190

(09).7 In this representation, roots of words are considered to be cyclic

entities, which I doubt. I think that we need the traditional (cyclic) unit

"stem" to count for the structure of derivates like (09).

(07) WORD (08) WORD

ROOT ROOT

house ROOT AFFIX

ROOT AFFIX

nation al ity

(09) WORD

WORD WORD

WORD WORD WORD WORD

WORD JOINT ROOTN ROOTV JOINT ROOTN

Straß en bahn halt e stelle

For EMMOREY / FROMKIN (1988: 145f), it has not been decided

yet whether morphologically complex words are listed as stem +

affixes or as individual morphemes to be combined by rules. Recent

morphological research seems to give an answer to their question:

− CLAHSEN et al. (1991) found evidence for the so-called "dual route" model, according to which regular inflectional words like leb-t-e "lived" can be said to be combinations of (separately stored) morphemes (roots and affixes), whereas irregular forms like war "was", tat "did" or wurde "became" are not decomposed but rather stored as whole words.

− PENKE / KRAUSE (2000) elicitated 20 irregular noun plurals and 49 participles (23 regular ones, 26 irreg. ones) from 2 German subjects who were diagnosed with William’s syndrome (WS). Eight unimpaired children served as controls. Subjects had to transform (i) a given 1.pers.sg. present form into a participle or (ii) a singular noun into its

7 WUNDERLICH/ FABRI (1995) offer different ways of morphological structuring.

Page 17: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

191

plural form. The authors found that with regard to the default –s-plural, WS children show lower error scores compared to the controls (17% vs. 30%); in contrast, the distribution of errors for the irregular –er- and –n-plurals show the reverse pattern (32% WS, 17% controls). Regular participle formation turned out to be intact (correctness scores being the same as those for the controls). In contrast to the English data by CLAHSEN / SONNENSTUHL (1999), both WS and control children achieve relatively low correctness scores for irregular participles (62% WS, 66% control). A clear difference, however, emerges when frequency is taken into consideration: 78% of the control’s errors occur with infrequent irregular participles; in WS children, 45% of errors result in overregularizations of frequent irregular participles. 8

− KOSTIKAS-TSELEPIS (2000) reports on a research project in which 22 Aromunian/Greek bilinguals were tested. Presented with Aromunian simple words, complex and ill-formed (prefixed and suffixed) nonwords, the testees accepted 99% of the well-formed novel words prefixed with palju- ("old/bad") as compared to only 36.66% of the novel words suffixed with the ornative suffix –osu. This means that "a decomposition process for affixed words is … operant, however to different degrees."

− In their study of the mental representation of German compounds, ISEL / GUNTER / FRIEDERICI (2000) offered their 24 native German testees 160 compounds (40 transparent-transparent, 40 opaque, 40 transp.-opaque and 40 opaque-transp.). The testees responded by pressing one of two buttons of a response box. Left constituents were activated only for transp.-transp. and opaque-transp. compounds. The authors conclude that left constituents are not activated on-line and not processed before the processing of the head has been completed. The findings propose a hierarchical model with the transparency of the head being the crucial factor in determining the mode of lexical access. Decomposition only takes place when the head is transparent (cf. FRIEDERICI et al. 2000).

− Neuro-anatomic procedures like the positron-emission tomography (PET) have been applied successfully to the functioning of the ML, e.g. in the study of verb morphology in English by JAEGER et al. (1996). The authors found that subjects produced the past tense forms of regular verbs significantly faster than irregular and that error rates for irregular

8 The papers by KOSTIKAS-TSELEPIS and ISEL/GUNTER/FRIEDERICI, are research

reports printed in the documentation Second Intern. Conference on the Mental Lexicon. Montréal, 2000 (cf. references).

Page 18: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

192

verbs were considerably higher than for regular or nonce past tense forms. Past-tense forms of regular verbs were computed on-line: Activation of left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which had been shown in other studies to be involved in on-line behavior of intentional novel behavior, occurred only in the regular and nonce conditions, which can be interpreted such that this cortical area is involved in the computation of the regular rule. The hypothesis is that irregular past tense forms are computed by activating some aspect of lexical memory; the activation of the left middle temporal gyrus (involved with auditory memory traces) occurred only in the irregular task.

2.3 Syntactic structure of lexical units

In speech production, an important step (after conceptualization

and lexical insertion) is the construction of a syntactic structure, into

which lexical entries have to be inserted (cf. LEVELT 1989). The

syntactic constructions have to take the syntactic properties of the

lexical items into consideration. Since TESNIÈRE (1959), “valency”

has been known to be the most relevant of these syntactic properties.

It concerns the number and quality of linguistic elements a certain

lexical entry can be combined with; valency is associated especially

with verbs; but nouns and adjectives can have valency too. According

to ÁGEL (1995), we have to distinguish between a valency potential

associated with a lexical entry and valency realization depending on

the valency potential of a lexical item and on the syntactic

construction that has been chosen (cf. also VATER 2003).

The verb to paint requires two complements, one of them

designating the agent, the other one the theme. In an active

construction, both complements are obligatory, the agent being

realized as the subject of the sentence, the theme as its object: John

Page 19: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

193

painted the wall. In a passive construction, only the theme is

obligatory (as the subject of the sentence), whereas the agent can be

realized by a PP (as a prepositional object) or be omitted: The wall

was painted (by John). A verb can have several variants, each having

its own valency, often also a specific meaning, like the German verb

gehen “to go”. Since each of the variants can be inserted into different

syntactic frames, a considerable amount of syntactic realizations

arises:

(10) a Paul geht zum Bahnhof. 'P. is walking to the station' b Paul geht nach Amerika. 'P. is going (emigrating") to A.' c Paul geht zum Theater. 'P. is going to the theatre (i.e. becoming an actor)' d Paul geht (zum 31.12.). 'P. is leaving (by the 31.12.)' e Das Paket geht nach München. 'The parcel is sent to Munich' f Die Uhr geht. 'The watch works' g Der Roman von Walser geht ausgezeichnet (wie warme Semmeln). 'The novel by W. sells extremely well' h Wie geht es? – Es geht. 'How is it going?' − 'Not (too) bad'

Each of the examples illustrates a different variant (with

decreasing semantic similarity): Whereas gehen in (10)a means 'to

walk' (i.e. 'on foot'), in b and c it still means moving but not 'by means

of one’s feet'; variant a allows dropping of the directional adverbial,

which is not possible with b or c; a − d express intentional movement

as opposed to e (a parcel is not being moved by its own intention).

In f , g and h no movement is involved: in f, geht means 'funct-

ions', in g, 'is sold'. In h, gehen occurs in impersonal constructions: the

first sentence means 'how is it going?' and the second one 'more or less

well' (Fr. comme ci, comme ça; Pol. tak sobie).

Page 20: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

194

The only psycholinguistic study concerning syntactic inform-

ation in lexical entries of the ML I could find is BROWN TESOLIN / DE

ALMEIDA (2000).9 The task for the 25 participants was to provide a

noun that would make two sentences serving as a frame conceptually

plausible (as e.g. door in The person closed the ____ and The ____

closed as well as nouns that were impossible in that frame (as e.g.

moon). Data from this experiment showed equal priming effects for

both plausible and implausible conditions. The authors do not offer an

interpretation of their results (maybe it is offered in ALMEIDA 2004,

which was not accessible for me).

2.4 Semantic structure of lexical units

According to SCHWARZ (19962: 35), experiments show that the

LM is subdivided semantically into subsystems equivalent to semantic

fields as shown by the loss of sections of the vocabulary (like loss of

the field of plant names) with people suffering from certain types of

aphasia. What does the semantic structure of lexical entries look like?

Basing herself on the types of evidence mentioned in 2.0, AITCHISON

(1987: 72) holds that words are not composed of semantic primitives,

but are rather connected "in a gigantic multi-dimensional cobweb".10

9 Paper printed in Second Intern. Conference on the Mental Lexicon, Montréal. 10 A holistic hypothesis is also forwarded by LÖBACH (2000). Other

psycholinguists analyze words semantically in (necessary and sufficient) features or in prototypes, like SCHWARZ (19962:88), dividing the concept of DOG into prototype features (including categorical information like IS AN ANIMAL), HAS FOUR LEGS, HAS A TAIL and CAN BARK. According to SCHWARZ (19962:85) it is not clear yet, "ob das mentale Lexikon nur sprachliches ... Wissen oder auch allgemeines Weltwissen beinhaltet" ("whether the mental lexicon contains only ling. knowledge or also general knowledge").

Page 21: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

195

Concepts are not stored in isolation but in narrow relations with each

other (cf. SCHWARZ 19962: 67). This is also true of linguistic concepts

as entries of the ML. The cobweb is only partially similar to a

semantic field as described by BAUMGÄRTNER (1967), where lexical

entries are connected with each other by superordinates or

subordinated semantic features (i.e. by hyponomy relations) or on the

basis of their similarity (as co-hyponymes). AITCHISON (1987: 74f)

assumes four different types of semantic relations:

(11) Relations among entries of the Mental Lexicon

– SUPERORDINATION: orange − citrus fruit − fruit – COORDINATION: orange − mandarine − lemon – COLLOCATION: orange − to peel – SYNONYMY: oculist − eye doctor

Association tests as well as slips of the tongue and tip of the

tongue (TOT) cases show that the concept ORANGE is in a narrow

relationship with superordinate nouns (like citrus fruit and fruit)11 as

well as with co-hyponyms like mandarine and lemon, synononyms

(like Ger. Apfelsine and Orange) and frequently co-occuring

predicational concepts like KNIFE, TO PEEL, HAS A PEEL, IS JUICY etc.

Unfortunately SCHWARZ (19962) employs (like PIAGET 1928) the term "concept" for entities of thought as well as for entities of language (stored in the memory), whereas BIERWISCH (1983) postulates a difference (but does not give any details as to the different types of storage).

11 STEINITZ (1968) shows, however, that not all kinds of superordinate nouns are equally natural, e.g. as anaphoric replacements: Auf der Straße lag ein Pudel. Der Hund / *das Säugetier / das Tier bellte mich an (On the street, a poodle was lying. The dog / *the mammal / the animal barked at me).

Page 22: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

196

A fifth relationship should be added: ANTONYMY (cf. beginning

– end; warm – cold; rise – fall).12

LÖBACH (2000) shows, in a very interesting study that the

assumption of "innate ideas" as suggested by Fodor and other

representatives of Generative Grammar has to be rejected. Children

form their first concepts on the basis of percepts before deriving other

concepts out of these basic concepts. The author thinks that these

concepts are holistic as to their semantics rather than composed of

features or prototypes.

Klaudia GROTE (2000) investigated whether the language

modality (visual-gestural vs. auditory-vocal) affects the architecture

and nature of the semantic lexicon and whether structural diversities

of the lexicon influence memory processes.13 Signs and spoken words

are produced and perceived in a completely different modality. Sign

languages take full advantage of the parallel capacity of the visual

system expressing information both linearly and simultaneously. The

first study involved examining whether deaf and hearing subjects

show different reaction times in a verification task where they had to

judge the presence or absence of a semantic relation between two

signs and two spoken words, respectively. The second verification

task investigated whether the structure of the semantic lexicon was

projected into the visual semantic system: Subjects had to judge line- 12 I noticed that antonymy is involved in slips of the tongue (produced by myself

as well as by other persons) and in TOT phenomena. AITCHISON (1987) mentions antonymy but does not include it into her scheme.

13 Paper printed in Second intern. conference on the mental lexicon. Montréal.

Page 23: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

197

drawings instead of signs which were (1) not related, (2) associatively

related to studied target, (3) associatively related + using a classifier

which referred to target item. False recognition rate the best for (1),

followed by (2), the worst for (3). A group of 20 native DGS-signers

and 20 native DLS-speakers was compared. The author's hypothesis −

strength of syntagmatic and paradigmatic relations within lexical

organization of DGS and DLS vary with different language modality

− was supported. Temporal and structural differences between signed

and spoken languages have an effect on implicit or explicit activation

of semantically related words and lead to different memory errors.

The question whether we have to assume two lexicons in our

mind, a mental lexicon containing linguistic units, and another lexicon

containing non-linguistic concepts, has not been investigated

thoroughly, although two different positions concerning the

relationship between semantric and conceptual structure have been

advocated: JACKENDOFF (1983) holds that "semantic structure is

conceptual structure", whereas BIERWISCH (1983) thinks that both are

related but basically independent of each other. In any case, there is

evidence of the fact that, in language comprehension, the recipient

needs linguistic knowledge as well as knowledge of the world both

being in close interaction with each other. EMMOREY / FROMKIN

(1988: 146) assume that the mental lexicon does not contain

knowledge of the world:

Page 24: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

198

"The fact that cases of agnosia show a dissociation between lexical knowledge and nonlinguistic knowledge suggests that lexical knowledge should be represented separately from nonlinguistic knowledge (Damasio 1985, Newcombe 1985)." 14

This would confirm the approach advocated by BIERWISCH

(1983). SCHWARZ (1992) holds that there is an "amodal" level in our

mental representation where concepts are stored without being

specified as perceptional, linguistic or purely notional. But this is a

completely theoretical assumption, not based on empirical evidence.

Also in this area, further research is necessary.

3. Conclusions

The overview of problems and results of studies concerning the

ML given by EMMOREY / FROMKIN (1988: 144ff) has to be

complemented (and partially modified) in several respects if we take

results of more recent research into account:

– The mental lexicon of a language contains information about words, morphemes and/or idioms.

– The information concerns the phonological form of a lexical entry (including phonological constraints), its morphological structure, its meaning (semantic representation), its combinatory potential (syntax), its orthographic representation (which I disregarded in this article).

14 According to ARNOLD / EYSENCK / MEILI (1991: 37) agnosia is a malfunction

of recognition in spite of intact functions of the perceptual organs, intelligence and consciousness. A perceived object is not recognized, i.e. is not related to remembrance.

Page 25: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

199

– Based on these kinds of information, the speaker decides whether a phonic or graphic chain contains words really occurring in his language or potential words.15

− Recent research projects concerning morphology in language processing, language acquisition or language loss support the dual storage hypothesis, according to which decomposition takes place in transparent regular words, whereas irregular and/or non-transparent words seem to be stored as wholes (cf. CLAHSEN et al. 1991 and PENKE / KRAUSE 2000, where also problems concerning the dual storage are mentioned).

− Concerning the syntax of lexical entries, the differentiation into valency potential (in the ML) and valency realization seems to be fruitful (cf. AGEL 1995, VATER 2003). In this area, there has not been done very much research; thus, a lot of work is still to do.

− Concerning the semantics of lexical entries, there is agreement that entries are stored not in isolation but in close relations which each other. There is disagreement as to the nature of these relationships: whereas some linguists stick to semantic features (ordered within a network), others (like SCHWARZ 19962) prefer prototypes or "cobwebs" relating entries by relations of coordination, subordination, co-occurrence etc. (AITCHISON 1987 and 19984); still others like LÖBACH (2000) consider lexical entries to be holistic in semantic respect.

Heinz Vater

Universität zu Köln [email protected]

15 EMMOREY / FROMKIN (1988:144f) speak about "non-occurring nonsense

forms". This is too general. Potential words like the derivate stealer ("one who steals") or the compound clearhead can be predicted because of their morphological regularities. Nonsense words may be potential words (formed according to morphological rules) like Ringelnatz' Tintenschwein "inkpig", or perversions ('Verballhornungen') of consisting words like Robert GERN-HARDT’s Schleistgoß (from Schloßgeist "castle ghost") or "half legal" words like MORGENSTERN’s Gingganz as opposed to monomorphematic forms like MORGENSTERN’s Greule which are not likely to be used as words.

Page 26: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

200

References Ágel, Vilmos (1995). Valenzrealisierung, Grammatik und Valenz. Zeitschr.

f. germanist. Linguistik 234:2−32. Ágel, Vilmos (2000). Valenztheorie. Tübingen: Narr (= narr studienbücher). Aitchison, Jean (19984). The Articulate Mammal. An Introduction to

Psycholinguistics. London: Routledge. Aitchison, Jean (1987). Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental

Lexicon. Oxford: Blackwell. Almeida, Roberto de (2000). Accessing arguments and conceptual fillers of

verbs. Poster presented at the Second International Conference on the Mental Lexicon, Montreal, Canada.

Almeida, Roberto de (2004). The effect of context on the processing of type-shifting verbs. Brain and Language 90: 249−261.

Anderson, J.R. (1983). The Architecture of Cognition. Cambr./MA: Harvard University Press.

Arnold, Wilhelm / Eysenck, Hans Jürgen / Meili, Richard (19918): Lexikon der Psychologie. Freiburg, Basel, Wien.

Atkinson, Richard C. / Shiffrin, Richard M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In: Spence, K.W./ Spence, J.T. (eds.) (1968). Advances in the Psychology of Learning and Motivation Research and Theory, Vol. II. New York: Academic Press, 89-195.

Baumgärtner, Klaus (1967). Die Struktur des Bedeutungsfeldes. In: Moser, Hugo (ed.) (1967). Satz und Wort im heutigen Deutsch. Düsseldorf: Schwann (= Sprache der Gegenwart 1), 165-167.

Bierwisch, Manfred (1983). Semantische und konzeptuelle Repräsentation lexikalischer Einheiten. In: Růžička, R./ Motsch, W. (eds.) (1983). Untersuchungen zur Semantik. Berlin: Akademie Vl. (= studia gramm. XXII), 61-99.

Bierwisch, Manfred, (1987). Linguistik als kognitive Wissenschaft. Zeitschrift für Germanistik 8, 645-667.

Blumstein, Sheila E. (1988). Neurolinguistics. An Overview of Language-Brain Relations in Aphasia. In: Newmeyer 1988, III:210–236.

Bosshardt, Hans-Georg (ed.) (1986). Perspektiven auf Sprache. Interdiszipinäre Beiträge z. Gedenken an Hans Hörmann. Berlin: de Gruyter.

Bühler, Karl (1934, 19652). Sprachtheorie. Stuttgart: Fischer. Caplan, David (1988). The biological basis for language. In: Newmeyer

1988, III: 237–255. Carston, Robyn (1988). Language and Cognition. In: Newmeyer (1988), III:

38-68.

Page 27: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

201

Chomsky, Noam (1986). Knowledge of Language. Its Nature, Origin, and Use. New York: Praeger.

Chomsky, Noam (1988). Language and Problems of Knowledge. The Managua Lectures. Cambridge, Mass: MIT.

Clahsen, Harald / Eisenbeiß, Sonja / Vainikka, A. (1991). The Seeds of Structure. A Syntactic Analysis of the Acquisition of Case Marking. (= Arbeiten des Sonderforschungsbereichs 282), Nr.2.

Clahsen, Harald / Sonnenstuhl, Ingrid (1999). Lexical entries and the rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 991−1060.

Cutler, Anne (1994). Segmentation problems, rhythmic solutions. In: Gleitman, L. / Landau, B. (eds.), 81–104.

Damasio, Antonio R. (1985): Prosopagnosia. In: Trends in Neuroscience 8:132–135.

Emmorey, Karen D. / Fromkin, Victoria A. (1988). The mental lexicon. In: Newmeyer, F. (1988), III: 124–149.

Engelkamp, Johannes (1974). Psycholinguistik. München: Fink (= UTB 297).

Fanselow, Gisbert / Felix, Sascha, (1987, 19933). Sprachtheorie. 2 Bde. Tübingen: Francke (= UTB 1441/1442).

Flores d'Arcais, Giovanni (1986). Konzeptuelle Strukturen und das mentale Lexikon. In: Bosshardt (1986):130-148.

Fodor, Jerry A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge/MA: MIT Press.

Friederici, Angela (1984). Neuropsychologie der Sprache. Stuttgart, Berlin, Köln: Kohlhammer.

Friederici, Angela / Mecklinger, Axel / Schröger, Erich (2000). Working on working memory. Leipzig: Leipziger Universitäts-Verlag.

Fromkin, Victoria (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language 47: 27–52. Reprinted in: Fromkin, Victoria (ed.) (1973): 215–242.

Fromkin, Victoria / Rodman, Robert (1974, 19935). An Introduction to Language. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Fromkin, Victoria (ed.) (1973). Speech Errors as Linguistic Evidence. The Hague: Mouton.

Garman, M (1990). Psycholinguistics. Cambridge: Cambr. Univ. Press. Garnham, Alan (1985). Psycholinguistics. Central Topics. London:

Methuen. Geschwind, Norman (19889). Die Großhirnrinde. In: Gehirn und

Nervensystem, 112-120.

Page 28: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

202

Gleitman, Lila / Landau, Barbara (eds.) (1994). The Acquisition of the Lexicon. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Görz, Günther (ed.) (1993). Einführung in die künstliche Intelligenz. Bonn: Addison-Wesley.

Grimm, Hannelore / Engelkamp, Johannes (1981). Sprachpsychologie. Handbuch und Lexikon der Psycholinguistik. Berlin: Schmidt.

Handke, Jürgen (1995). The Structure of the Lexicon. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

Hörmann, Hans (1967). Psychologie der Sprache. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer (verbesserter Nachdruck 1970).

Jackendoff, Ray (1983). Semantics and Cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

Jaeger, Jeri J. / Lockwood, Alan / Kemmerer, David / Van Valin, Robert / Murphy, Brian / Khalak, Hanif (1996). A positron emission tomographic study of regular and irregular verb morphology in English. Language 72: 451−497.

Jakobson, Roman (1969). Kindersprache, Aphasie und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp (= edition suhrkamp 330).

Kleiber, George (1993). Prototypensemantik: eine Einführung. Tübingen: Narr.

Lang, Ewald (1994). Semantische vs. konzeptuelle Struktur: Unterscheidung und Überschneidung. In: Schwarz, Monika (ed.) (1994). Kognitive Semantik / Cognitive Semantics. Tübingen: Narr (= TBL 395), 25–40.

Lenneberg, Eric (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Leuninger, Helen (1989). Neurolinguistik. Probleme, Paradigmen, Perspektiven. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.

Levelt, Willem J.M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Löbach, Brigitte (2000). Semantikerwerb. Ein Beitrag zu einer empiristisch-naturalistischen Bedeutungstheorie. Tübingen: Niemeyer (= LA 423).

Meibauer, Jörg (1995). Neugebildete er-Derivate im Spracherwerb. Ergebnisse einer Langzeitstudie. Sprache und Kognition 14: 138-160.

Meibauer, Jörg / Rothweiler, Monika (eds.) (1999). Das Lexikon im Spracherwerb. Tübingen: Francke (= UTB Mittl. Reihe 2039).

Miller, George A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 63:81-97.

Miller, George / Johnson-Laird, Philip (1976). Language and Perception. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard Univ. Press.

Page 29: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

203

Nauta, Walle J. H. / Feirtag, Michael (19889). Die Architektur des Gehirns. In: Gehirn und Nervensystem, 19889: 88-98.

Newcombe, Nora (1985). Methods for the study of spatial representation. In: Cohen, R. (ed.). The development of spatial cognition. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 277−300.

Newmeyer, Frederick J. (ed.) (1988). Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey. Vol. III: Psychological and Biological Aspects. Cambridge, Mass. et al.: CUP.

Penke, Martina / Krause, Marion (2000). German noun plurals − a challenge to the Dual-Mechanism model. Brain and Language 81: 303−311.

Pinker, Steven (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: Morrow. Ramers, Karl Heinz / Vater, Heinz / Wode, Henning (eds.) (1994).

Universale phonologische Strukturen und Prozesse. Tübingen: Niemeyer (= LA 310).

Rickheit, Gert / Strohner, Hans (1993). Grundlagen der kognitiven Sprachverarbeitung. Tbg.: Francke (UTB 1735).

Rosch, Eleanor (1977). Human Categorization. In: Warren, N. (ed.) (1977). Studies in Cross-Cultural Psychology, Vol.1. London: Academic Press, 1-49.

Schnelle, Helmut (ed.) (1981, 19902). Sprache und Gehirn. Frankfurt a.M.: Suhrkamp (= stw 343).

Schwarz, Monika (1992). Kognitive Semantiktheorie und neuropsychologische Realität. Repräsentationale und prozedurale Aspekte der semantischen Kompetenz. Tübingen: Niemeyer (= LA 273).

Schwarz, Monika (19962). Einführung in die Kognitive Linguistik. Tübingen: Francke (= UTB 1636).

Second international conference on the mental lexicon. Montréal, Canada, Oct. 18–20, 2000.

Sonnenstuhl, Ingrid / Eisenbeiss, Sonja / Clahsen, Harald (1999). Morphological priming in the German mental lexicon. Cognition 72 (3): 203−326.

Steinitz, Renate (1968). Nominale Proformen. In: Kallmeyer, Werner et al. (eds.) (1974). Lektürekolleg zur Textlinguistik, Bd. 2: Reader. Frankfurt/M.: Athenäum Fischer, 246-265.

Stephany, Ursula (1994). Phonologische Gesetzmäßigkeiten der Kindersprache aus synchroner und diachroner Sicht. In: Ramers, K. H. / Vater, H. / Wode, H. (eds.), 205-222.

Tesnière, Lucien (1959). Eléments de syntaxe structurale. Paris: Klincksieck.

Page 30: 1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics1. The subject and goals of psycholinguistics 1.0 Preliminary remarks After a brief introduction to psycholinguistics, I will describe

Studi Linguistici e Filologici Online 4.1 Dipartimento di Linguistica – Università di Pisa

www.humnet.unipi.it/slifo

204

Topczewska, Urszula (2004). Phraseolexeme in Paulusbriefen und ihre Wiedergabe im Deutschen und im Polnischen anhand ausgewählter Bibelübersetzungen. Trier: WVT (= Fokus 28).

Vater, Heinz (1994). Linguistik – Natur- oder Geisteswissenschaft? In: Roggausch, W. (ed.). Germanistentreffen Bundesrepublik Deutschland – Polen. Tagungsbeiträge. Bonn: DAAD, 219-232.

Vater, Heinz (1994, 20024). Einführung in die Sprachwissenschaft. München: Fink. (= UTB 1799).

Vater, Heinz (2003). Valency and diathesis. In: Cuyckens, Hubert / Berg, Thomas / Dirven, René / Panther, Klaus-Uwe (eds). Motivation in Languages: Studies in Honor of Günter Radden. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins, 99–122.

Vater, Heinz (2005). Referenz-Linguistik. München: Fink (= UTB 2685). Wettler, Manfred (1980). Sprache, Gedächtnis, Verstehen. Berlin/New

York: de Gruyter. Wiese, Richard (1983). Psycholinguistische Aspekte der Sprachproduktion.

Hamburg: Buske. Wiese, Richard (1987). Versprecher als Fenster zur Sprachstruktur. Studium

Linguistik 21:45-55. Williams, Edwin (1994). Remarks on lexical knowledge. In: Gleitman, L. /

Landau, B. (eds.) (1994):7–34. Wode, Henning (1993). Psycholinguistik. Eine Einführung in die Lehr- und

Lernbarkeit von Sprachen. München: Hueber. Wode, Henning (1994). Perzeption, Produktion und die Lernbarkeit von

Sprachen. In: Ramers, K. H. / Vater, H. / Wode, H. (eds.), 169-187. Wunderlich, Dieter / Fabri, Roy (1995). Minimalist morphology: An

approach to inflection. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 14: 236−294. Wygotski, L. S. (1964). Denken und Sprechen. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.