Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minority Learners

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    1/32

    Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages, Inc. TESOL)

    Word Meanings Matter: Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minority LearnersAuthor(s): JEANNETTE MANCILLA-MARTINEZSource: TESOL Quarterly, Vol. 44, No. 4 (December 2010), pp. 669-699Published by: Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages, Inc. (TESOL)Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/27896759 .Accessed: 12/12/2014 18:58

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

    .JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    .

    Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages, Inc. (TESOL) is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,preserve and extend access to TESOL Quarterly.

    http://www.jstor.org

    This content downloaded from 82 .33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=tesolhttp://www.jstor.org/stable/27896759?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/27896759?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=tesol
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    2/32

    Word Meanings Matter: CultivatingEnglish Vocabulary Knowledge in FifthGrade Spanish-Speaking Language

    Minority Learners

    JEANNETTEMANCILLA-MARTINEZUniversity of llinois at ChicagoChicago, Illinois, United States

    This pilot study investigated the effects of a 20-week quasiexperimentalvocabulary intervention aimed at improving Spanish-speaking language

    minority students' English vocabulary and writing outcomes.

    Participants were twomatched samples of fifth graders (N= 49) in a

    predominantly Latino, low-income urban school. Pre- and posttest

    analyses revealed that the treatment group gained knowledge of a

    larger number of target words than did the contrast group and that thetreatment group students were generally better at determining their

    own word knowledge. Further, individual growth modeling revealedthe treatment students' overall writing quality improved over the courseof the 20-week intervention, even though writing instruction was not

    part of the intervention, and improvements in students' writing qualitywere larger during the last 10weeks of the intervention. The need for

    purposeful activities that provide students with authentic contexts tolearn and productively use newly taught words is discussed.dot: 10.5054/tq.2010.213782

    There

    areapproximately

    98 million ethnicminority group

    members(or 33% of the country's population) in the United States, with

    Latinos comprising the largest and fastest growing segment of this

    population (U.S. Census Bureau, 2005). Over the past 30 years thenumber of school-age children who spoke a language other than Englishat home, known as language minority (LM) learners, nearly tripled(3.8 million to 10.6 million; U.S. Department of Education, National

    Center for Education Statistics, NCES, 2007a), with Spanish being themost common home language. These statistics have clear and immediate

    implications for U.S. schools, as the number of Latino children is

    expected to continue to increase in the coming decades (Harwood,Leyendecker, Carlson, Asencio, & Miller, 2002). Of concern is that this

    rapidly growing population of Spanish-speaking LM learners accounts

    TESOL QUARTERLY Vol. 44, No. 4, December 2010 669

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    3/32

    for a disproportionate percentage of struggling comprehenders (August& Shanahan, 2006; NCES, 2007b). Given that Latino families continueto be

    overrepresented amongAmerica's

    poor (Suarez-Orozco8c

    Paez,2002) and that low-income status is a well-known risk factor associatedwith poor academic outcomes (Hart 8c Risley, 1995), a considerable

    proportion of Spanish-speaking Latino students is doubly at-risk forschool failure. It is beyond the scope of this article to attempt to

    disentangle the effect of LM status from low socioeconomic status onstudent academic outcomes, but it is clear that these variables tend to beconfounded, creating compounding obstacles to successful academicoutcomes for the growing population of low-income LM students. Manyfactors are associated with LM students' reading comprehensiondifficulties, but low levels of vocabulary knowledge continue to emergeas key impediments to successful comprehension (Garcia, 1991; Nagy,1997; Stahl 8c Nagy, 2006; U.S. Department of Health and HumanServices, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Child Healthand Human Development, NICHD, 2000; Verhoeven, 1990). Despite the

    strong and well-established link between vocabulary and comprehension(Anderson 8c Freebody, 1981; Anderson & Nagy, 1991; U.S. Department

    of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement, RAND

    Reading Study Group, 2002), over the past 20 years relatively fewresearchers have conducted

    experimentalstudies on the effectiveness of

    vocabulary interventions with LM students (August 8c Shanahan, 2006;Calder?n, August, Slavin, Duran, Madden, 8c Cheung, 2005). The

    present study strengthens this research domain by assessing theeffectiveness of a pilot vocabulary intervention designed to improvefifth-grade (ages 10-11 years) LM students' literacy outcomes.

    LITERATURE REVIEW

    Notwithstanding the poor academic outcomes of many LM learners at

    all grade levels, reading research has tended to focus on the readingdevelopment of young monolingual English speakers (e.g., NICHD,2000; Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998), and that which has been conducted

    with LM learners has similarly focused on young children (for a review,see Lesaux, Koda, Siegel, & Shanahan, 2006). Thus research and

    practice have focused more on word reading skills than vocabulary and

    comprehension skills. However, converging evidence finds that LMlearners tend to develop relatively strong word reading skills, but often

    without the necessary language skills to support comprehension (e.g.,

    Droop 8c Verhoeven, 2003; Hoover & Gough, 1990; Hutchinson,Whiteley, Smith, & Connors, 2003; Proctor, Carlo, August, & Snow,2005; Proctor, August, Carlo, & Snow, 2006; Verhoeven, 1990, 2000).

    670 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    4/32

    Thus a distinction between word reading and knowledge of the

    meanings of the words being read is especially important in under

    standing LM learners' literacy development. It is estimated that studentsneed to know (not simply recognize and/or decode) at least 95% of thewords they encounter in text for successful comprehension (Calder?net al., 2005; Lipson & Wixson, 2003). Estimates of words learned duringa typical school year range from 1,000 (Goulden, Nation, & Read, 1990)to 3,000 (Nagy & Herman, 1987); although clearly most of these wordsare learned incidentally, explicit vocabulary instruction has also beenfound to contribute to word learning (e.g., Biemiller, 2003).Considering the early vocabulary knowledge disadvantage that manyLM learners face, a sole reliance on incidental vocabulary learning for

    this group of learners is both impractical and negligent. However, onlyfive vocabulary intervention studies have focused on LM learners(Calder?n et al., 2005; Carlo et al., 2004; Perez, 1981; Ramirez, 1986;

    Vaughn-Shavuo, 1990), and only one (Carlo et al., 2004) has targetedupper-elementary students.

    Vaughn-Shavuo's (1990) doctoral dissertation investigated the effectof vocabulary instruction by randomly assigning two groups of first-gradeSpanish-dominant children to two groups. Over the course of 3 weeks,31 words were presented to both groups. Group one (the experimentalgroup) received vocabulary instruction focused on elaborated meanings.Specifically, they learned the target words in meaningful paragraphs(i.e., the sentences containing the target words formed narratives), they

    were provided with picture cards of the target words that illustrated their

    meanings, and they dictated their own sentences using the target words.In contrast, the other group (the control group) received instructionfocused on individual sentence contexts (i.e., the sentences containingthe target words were unconnected). Results showed that the experi

    mental group learned more words than the control group. In another

    study, Perez's (1981) work with third graders revealed that the

    experimental group receiving20 min of

    dailyoral

    languageinstruction

    on word meanings, over the course of about 3 months, showed

    significant improvements on word learning over the group receivingregular instruction (i.e., reading text and answering questions). Thethird vocabulary study (Ramirez, 1986) also focused on third-gradeSpanish speakers. In this study, amethod called suggestopedia (a languagelearning method that uses music to create an atmosphere conducive to

    learning) was used. Ten words were taught per day in 40-min lessonsover the course of 4 days, and the experimental groups performedsignificantly better than the control. Calder?n and colleagues (2005)

    likewise investigated Spanish-speaking third graders' word learning, butthe major goal of their 22- to 25-week intervention (an adaptation of theSuccess for All reading program) was on facilitating students' Spanish-to

    WORDMEANINGS MATTER 671

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    5/32

    English transition from a Spanish reading program. However, anadditional goal of the intervention was on building English vocabulary.

    Thus,in the 90-min

    lessons, vocabularywas a

    major focus,as 30 min were

    devoted to oral language activities revolving around grade-levelchildren's literature. Their results revealed modest positive effects onstudents' English vocabulary. Finally, and most recently, Carlo and

    colleagues (2004) conducted a 15-week vocabulary intervention with

    fifth-grade Spanish-speaking LM learners. They found that, while the

    impact of their 30- to 45-min a day intervention was greater on

    vocabulary than on reading comprehension, there were significantimprovements in both domains.

    The scope of vocabulary intervention work with LM learners is sparse,

    most notably beyond the primary grades, but findings to date point tothe promising role of vocabulary instruction to improve LM learners'

    vocabulary knowledge. Further, the strong and significant correlationbetween vocabulary and reading comprehension among LM learners

    (Droop & Verhoeven, 2003; Hoover & Gough, 1990; Hutchinson et al.,2003; Proctor et al., 2005, 2006; Verhoeven, 1990, 2000) suggests thatLM learners can benefit from targeted vocabulary instruction. Further,even though vocabulary might be expected to also impact writing, which

    plays an increasingly prominent role in evaluating students' academic

    performance after the primary grades, a key limitation in the field is thatnone of the vocabulary intervention studies conducted to date haveexamined writing outcomes.

    Some work with third- and fourth-grade monolingual English speakers suggests that limited vocabulary knowledge contributes to dependence on repetitive uses of the same words and thus to underelaborationof thoughts and ideas in writing (Moats, Foorman, 8c Taylor, 2006).Additionally, Saddler and Graham (2007) note that writers' familiaritywith the writing topic is related to writing performance, suggesting that

    background knowledge plays a central role in students' writing quality.Because LM learners tend to have more limited vocabularies and

    background knowledge (two highly interrelated areas) than their

    monolingual English-speaking counterparts, we can expect LM students'

    writing to be greatly impeded. Indeed, the 2002 National Assessment ofEducation Progress writing assessment revealed that only about one

    quarter of 4th, 8th, and 12th graders performed at or above the proficientlevel in writing (NCES, 2004), but even more troubling is the fact thatsubstantial differences emerged when examining the data by ethnicity:

    At all grade levels, on average, Whites and Asians scored above the 50%percentile, while Blacks and Latinos scored near the 25% percen tile.

    It seems reasonable to postulate that vocabulary instruction mightresult in improved writing outcomes, but there is a surprisingly limitedresearch base on the effect of vocabulary instruction on students'

    672 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    6/32

    writing, even amongst native-English-speaking populations (Duin 8cGraves, 1987; Graham 8c Perin, 2007). It is thus difficult to draw even

    tentative conclusions about the impact of vocabulary instruction onstudents' writing and particularly difficult to ascertain the nature of this

    relationship for Spanish-speaking LM learners. To my knowledge, onlyfour studies have directly investigated the writing development of

    Spanish-speaking LM learners (Bermudez & Prater, 1994; Davis, Carlisle,& Beeman, 1999; Ferris & Politzer, 1981; Lanauze & Snow, 1989).However, none of these studies examined the potential relationshipbetween vocabulary and writing development. Students need vocabularyto write, but the only evidence available on the role of vocabulary insecond language writing comes from English-as-a-foreign-language(EFL) college students. The educational context of LM learners differsin substantive ways from that of older EFL students, greatly limiting the

    generalizations that can be drawn from EFL work to the LM learner

    school-age population. Notwithstanding, EFL studies with collegestudents indeed find that a key determinant in nonnative Englishspeakers' overall writing quality is vocabulary (e.g., Leki & Carson, 1994;

    Raimes, 1985; Walters & Wolf, 1996). The lack of empirical research

    investigating this potential relationship amongst the large and growingpopulation of school-age LM learners is amajor limitation in the literacyresearch field.

    Because receptive vocabulary knowledge generally precedes productive vocabulary knowledge (Laufer, 1998; Meara, 1996; Nation, 1990;

    Pearson, Hiebert, & Kamil, 2007), it can be expected that students, andin particular LM learners, will require ample opportunities to actively

    use newly taught vocabulary in oral language before they are able to usethe new words in their writing. To date, very few studies have examinedstudents' use of newly taught vocabulary in writing (Bravo 8c Tilson,2006; Lee, 2003; Lee & Muncie, 2006). As part of a larger studyexamining the effects of an integrated science-literacy curriculum, Bravoand Tilson

    (2006) analyzedsecond- and

    third-gradestudents' use of

    science vocabulary in writing, finding that students spontaneously used

    newly taught science words in their writing. The authors suggest that useof newly taught vocabulary in writing represents growth in science

    knowledge and also indicates that students have productive control overscience vocabulary. However, an analysis of whether students' overall

    writing quality showed improvements was not conducted, and the

    majority of students in this study were White native English speakers,limiting the generalizations that can be made to LM learners.

    The other two studies investigated productive vocabulary use in

    writing among secondary school multinative language intermediateEnglish-as-a-second-language students in Canada. Specifically, Lee(2003) investigated correct usage of target vocabulary in the writing of

    WORDMEANINGSMATTER 673

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    7/32

    65 students, and Lee and Muncie (2006) investigated learners' (N= 48)

    use of target vocabulary and how their target vocabulary use influencedtheir lexical

    frequency profile (seeLaufer 8c

    Nation, 1995,for

    details).Like Bravo and Tilson (2006), Lee (2003) did not investigate the effectsof vocabulary encountered in reading instruction on students' overall

    writing quality. Lee and Muncie considered the relationship between

    vocabulary encountered in reading and learners' use of the vocabularyin writing, including improvements in writing quality, finding thatteacher elicitation, explicit explanation, discussion and negotiation, and

    exposure to target vocabulary increase students' productive vocabularyuse in writing. However, like the other two studies, student income levelswere not reported, effectively limiting our understanding of whether

    these findings can be expected to be replicated with low-incomepopulations.

    Despite the limited number and limited scope of studies exploringthe relationship between vocabulary instruction and writing, there isreason to believe that vocabulary instruction may indeed be a step in the

    right direction to improve the writing skills of LM learners. Drawing onresearch to date, two reasonable hypotheses are that (1) LM learners willuse newly taught words in their writing z/given opportunities to do so ona consistent basis, and (2) vocabulary instruction will strengthen LMlearners' overall

    writing qualityover time. To

    adequately explorethe

    effects of vocabulary instruction on students' literacy outcomes,research-based vocabulary instruction components and strategies must

    be attended to.

    Components of Effective Vocabulary Instruction

    The NICHD (2000) concluded that there is no single best researchbased method for vocabulary instruction, noting that a variety ofmethods are needed, including incidental and structured instruction.

    Although indirect instruction is vital to any program aiming to developstudents' vocabulary, direct, carefully designed instruction is also an

    integral part of the puzzle. The goal of explicit instruction is for studentsto learn the meanings of words across various contexts to ultimatelyimprove their literacy outcomes. Researchers agree that the specific

    words to be taught should be guided by their potential to aid students'

    understanding of text and/or concepts and that words students are

    likely to encounter relatively frequently should be targeted (e.g., Beck,McKeown, & Kucan, 2002; Biemiller & Slonim, 2001; Nation, 2001). In

    other words, wordsto

    be taught should be functional, cross-disciplinary,and developmentally appropriate (see Beck et al., 2002, for one widelyused system for selecting words).

    674 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    8/32

    Considering the more limited vocabulary levels of many LM learners,vocabulary instruction for LM learners must target students' language

    skills not only more intensively, but also more broadly. Because exposureto academic language is largely confined to the regular school day forLM learners, LM learners arguably need more opportunities, bothincidental and structured, to hear and use academic language thannative English speakers. Additionally, effective vocabulary programs forLM learners should specifically target the development of wordconsciousness and of word learning strategies to help LM learners catch

    up with monolingual English speakers. Further, the target vocabulary forinstruction should be presented in meaningful, engaging contexts thatare not only relevant to students' interests (e.g., Carlo et al., 2004) but

    that also serve to bolster their overall background knowledge. Finally, anarea that is seldom attended to is ensuring that students are provided

    with opportunities to actively use newly taught vocabulary, both orallyand in writing. Writing becomes a major form of evaluation after the

    primary grades for all students and, on average, over one-quarter ofLatino students write at the below basiclevel (NCES, 2003). Itmay be that,by giving LM learners ample opportunities to write on a consistent basisas part of a vocabulary program, their understanding of the words theyare being taught will be strengthened. That is, the sheer use of newly

    taughtwords in

    writing mayfoster

    learningof the words.

    PRESENT STUDY

    Given that LM students tend to have both less breadth and less depthof vocabulary knowledge and knowing that vocabulary is strongly relatedto students' overall school success, LM students with limited vocabulariesare very disadvantaged academically. The vocabulary program piloted inthis study draws from the research base on effective vocabularyinstruction and rests on the premise that vocabulary instruction can

    reasonably be expected to improve fifth-grade (ages 10-11 years) LMlearners' literacy outcomes. Fifth grade is an optimal school year tointervene, before students enter the more academically demanding

    middle school grades when struggling comprehenders rapidly fallfurther behind in all content areas. The study addressed the followingresearch questions:

    (1) Following the 20-week vocabulary intervention, do the treatment orcontrast group students gain knowledge of a greater number of

    targeted words?

    (2) In the treatment group's weekly student essays, what is the extent oftarget vocabulary word use over the course of the 20-weekintervention? Specifically, do students use the target vocabulary

    WORD MEANINGSMATTER 675

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    9/32

    words cumulatively, or is their use confined to words taught in thecurrent week? Further, is there any change in the quality (e.g.,coherence and academic

    language use)of the

    essaysover time?

    METHOD

    Research Context

    The data for this study were taken from one school in theNortheastern United States participating in an on-going study designedto improve students' literacy outcomes. This school, known by the

    pseudonym the Mystic School, is a Strategic Education Research

    Partnership (SERP) site. SERP is an organization with the overarchinggoal of improving student outcomes by acknowledging the wisdom of

    practice and bringing well-established research knowledge to bear.Within this SERP partnership, upper-elementary and middle-school

    teachers from the participating schools attend cross-university researchseminars, and researchers collaborate with the teachers on research

    projects. The Mystic School is an urban K-8 school serving a 91% Latino,91% low-income population. Seventy-nine percent of the student body is

    LM (Spanish), with 46% designated limited English proficient and 8%designated special education. The primary concern of the Mysticteachers centered on students' low levels of vocabulary knowledge and

    poor reading comprehension outcomes.

    Design and Participants

    This quasiexperimental pilot study employed a matched-control

    design. As a pilot study exploring the preliminary effects of a vocabularyintervention, piloting of the intervention was limited to one grade level(fifth grade) in the same predominantly Latino, low-income, urban K-8school in the Northeast to carefully monitor the implementation of theintervention. There were only three fifth-grade classrooms in this school;two were mainstream English classrooms, and the other was a selfcontained classroom for recent immigrants and thus did not participate.

    One classroom served as the treatment group and the other as thecontrast group.

    All 53 students in the two mainstream classrooms participated, buttwo students from the treatment and two students from the contrast

    classroom transferred to other schools during the intervention; thus thefinal sample for statistical analyses consisted of 24 students from thetreatment and 25 from the contrast classroom. Except for the number of

    676 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    10/32

    students formerly identified as limited English proficient, with more inthe treatment classroom,1 there were no significant differences in

    gender, race, and first language characteristicsacross

    the classrooms(see Table 1). The treatment group received the 20-week intervention,starting in January 2007 and ending in May 2007, while the contrast

    group continued with the regular, district-wide literacy instruction.

    Regular literacy instruction in this school district centered on at least80 min of the reading and writing workshop model in which a

    balanced approach to literacy instruction is followed.

    Intervention Overview and Implementation

    Word Generation (WG), developed through a SERP collaborativeeffort under the leadership of Dr. Catherine Snow, is a research-based20-week vocabulary intervention designed to build students' academic

    vocabulary across the content areas. Academic vocabulary refers to wordsthat students are likely to encounter in textbooks and on tests (e.g., inferand element), but not in spoken language. Without explicit instruction onthese types of words, students, and especially LM learners, are likely to

    experience difficulty with comprehension. The goal ofWG is to increasestudents' academic vocabulary, in an effort to improve literacy outcomes.

    Thefollowing components

    areemphasized: (1) building vocabularyknowledge through repeated exposure to frequently occurring academic

    words in various contexts, (2) cultivating general word and world

    TABLE 1Background Characteristics of Treatment and Contrast Group Students (N

    = 49)

    Treatment (n = 24) Contrast {n= 25) p value

    GenderFemale 17

    Male 7Race

    Latino 23Black 0

    White 0Asian 1

    First languageSpanish 23

    English/other 1

    Formerly limited English proficientYes 17No 7

    1411

    21130

    223

    916

    1.16

    5.07

    1.00

    5.97

    0.28

    0.17

    0.32

    0.01

    1Students at the Mystic School are randomly assigned to classrooms and thus the greaternumber of limited-English-proficient students in the treatment group compared with thecontrast group happened by chance.

    WORD MEANINGSMATTER 677

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    11/32

    knowledge, as well as word study strategies, and (3) engaging students in

    weekly persuasive writing. The core program centers on the weekly

    presentationof five

    high-utility targetwords to be learned in the context

    of brief passages outlining controversies currently under debate in theUnited States (ranging from the abilities of women inmath and scienceto global warming).2 The 100 target words are therefore relevant to a

    range of settings and subject areas and were selected from the AcademicWord List (Coxhead, 2000), which was originally developed as a support

    for instruction to college-level nonnative English speakers and does notinclude words that are in the most frequent 2,000 words of English. The

    main criteria in selecting the 100 target words were that they be highutility, high-functional, and cross-disciplinary (see Appendix A for the

    full list of WG words). The cross-content focus on a small number ofwords each week aims to enable students to understand the variety of

    ways in which words are related, and the multiple exposures to words indifferent contexts (e.g., math and history) seek to provide students with

    ample opportunities for deeper understanding of the words. The WGmaterials include a teacher's guide that explains the structure of the

    vocabulary program and rationale behind it; a set of 20 engagingparagraphs written about current topics in journalistic style, whichconnect to real word issues and to students' lives; brief instructionalactivities associated with

    weekly topicsand

    targetwords; and references

    to support teachers in implementing WG activities (see http://www.wordgeneration.org/index.html for more detailed information about

    WG, including access to the Teacher's Guide and a sample weeklylesson).

    Another central component of every WG lesson to build students'academic vocabulary is classroom talk. Aside from improving students'

    vocabulary knowledge, the promotion of classroom discussion and talkalso aims to support the development of students' reasoning and their

    ability to express their reasoning. Thus the following are key features of

    the WG intervention: revoicing by the teacher (i.e., repeating a student'sutterance with the purpose of checking back with them for clearer

    interpretation of their statement or position), student repetition (i.e.,having other students repeat or paraphrase another student's position inorder to check on their interpretation of the statement), asking studentsto debate (i.e., giving students opportunities to agree and disagree and

    having them state and make clear their reasoning), and partner talk

    (i.e., giving students who are less inclined to join whole groupdiscussions the opportunity to talk with a partner to ensure that allstudents are on the same page). Finally, the end-of-week writing activity

    2The WG paragraphs are written at a 6th grade readability level, because the WG materialswere specifically developed for use with middle school students (grades 6-8).

    678 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    12/32

    is essential toWG, because the goal is to make writing an integral part ofthe vocabulary program, such that students have the opportunity to

    composea

    short piece basedon

    the controversial topic they havediscussed all week. This provides yet another means of allowing studentsto express their thoughts and opinions. For LM learners, in particular,

    writing serves as a nonthreatening way to express their views.

    Additionally, the incorporation of a weekly writing component aims to

    provide students the opportunity to explore the use of newly taughtvocabulary in their writing, which is critical to cultivate deeper learningof the words.

    The teacher was asked to implement WG for at least 15 min daily, 5

    days a week. Though the Tuesday toThursday activities varied from week

    to week (these were the days that the target vocabulary was discussed inthe content areas of math, science, and social studies), the structure of

    WG is such that the first day of the week (typically Monday) begins withthe introduction of the passage and ends (typically Friday) with the

    open-ended essay-writing activity.On Monday, the teacher began the weekly WG lesson by introducing

    the controversial topic of the week. For example, the teacher would tellthe class that they would be discussing their thoughts on whether rap

    music has a negative impact on kids, and she would explicitly tell herstudents that they would be learning five words, which appeared in bold

    type in the passage. Before reading the weekly passage with the class,the teacher showed the five weekly target words on large index cards tothe class, one at a time, asking if they knew what the words meant; this

    was a way for the teacher to gauge her students' knowledge of the five

    target words. Most of the time, the students were asked to complete aword chart for homework, during which they were asked to do one ormore of the following: provide the part of speech and meanings of thewords, provide inflectional forms of the words, identify prefixes andsuffixes, and list any related words. On Tuesday, Wednesday, and

    Thursday,the teacher selected from an

    arrayof

    math, science,and

    social studies related activities. For math, there was typically a problemof the day, in which at least two of the five target words were

    incorporated into a word problem that resembled the type of word

    problem students would likely encounter on state standardized tests.The inclusion of the math problem of the day was not on teachingmath skills per se. Rather, the focus was on exposing students to the

    weekly target vocabulary in a math context to the extent possible. Thescience activities tended to consist of cloze paragraphs, in which manyof the five target words needed to be filled in to complete the sentences

    dealing with a science topic. Finally, the most typical social studiesactivity was the weekly whole class debate. During the debate, students

    were asked to take a stance on the controversial topic they had been

    WORDMEANINGSMATTER 679

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    13/32

    learning about and to present their view to the group holding an

    opposing view. When time permitted, students were allowed to rebutthe

    opposing group's argument. Finally,on

    Friday,students were

    required to take a stand by writing a short persuasive essay about thecontroversial topic they had been discussing. Students were encouragedto use the five weekly target WG words, as well as previously learned

    target words, in their essays. However, this was not required. On

    average, students tended to finish writing their persuasive essay inabout 10 min. Importantly, the teacher did not provide any writinginstruction related to the WG intervention or give students feedback onthe weekly essays they produced.

    The treatment group teacher completed a weekly feedback form to

    report on the implementation of the intervention (see Appendix B). Shecompleted the form each of the 20 weeks. Her responses indicated thatshe had implemented WG every week. Additionally, 38 observations inthe treatment classroom and 12 in the contrast classroom were

    conducted, to ensure that the treatment teacher was implementingWG as expected and that the contrast teacher was continuing with the

    regular instruction and not implementing WG or some other instructional program targeting academic language specifically. The directclassroom observations revealed that, on average, the teacher imple

    mented WG for about 20 min aday.

    In accordance with the teacher

    reports on the weekly form, the direct observations confirmed that theintervention was being implemented faithfully, even during the periodsof district-mandated testing.

    Measures

    Students in the treatment and contrast groups were administered

    pretests in the fall of 2006 and posttests in the spring of 2007. Pretestmeasures included researcher-developed and standardized (for match

    ing purposes) group and individually administered tests of languagecomprehension, word reading, reading comprehension, and writing.Posttest measures included the researcher-developed tests only. Allmeasures were administered during the school day.

    Researcher-Developed Measures

    Two measures of students' knowledge of the target vocabulary wordswere administered. The multiple-choice (MC) test and the vocabularyself-check (VSC) were both group administered. The MC test was

    comprised of a randomly selected sample of 30 of the 100 target WGwords. Following standard format, students silently read a phrase orshort sentence in which the target word was printed in bold type, and

    680 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    14/32

    they selected the appropriate meaning of the target word from a list offour choices. For example, students read the following sentence: The

    building collapsed after the earthquake. After reading the sentence, studentswere required to choose the word or group of words that mean the sameor almost the same as the underlined word. In this example, theirchoices were: (a) exploded, (b) fell apart, (c) stayed standing, and (d)collated. Based on work by Dale (1965) and Wesche and Paribakht(1996), the VSC assesses students' familiarity with the same 30 words3

    and 10 pseudowords. Students rated their own knowledge of the words

    using the following scale: (1) I do not know it, (2) I have heard it, (3) Iknow something about it, and (4) I know it well and can use it.

    Responses on the VSC were averaged for analysis, such that an overall

    average score close to four indicated students reported knowing thewords well enough to use them, whereas an average score close to oneindicated that students reported knowing few word meanings. On thefall pretest, only 4 of the 30WG words were reportedly known by 80% or

    more of all students, indicating' that the WG target words merited

    explicit instruction. Further, the 10 pseudowords were reported asunknown by 49 (88%) of all students, suggesting that students were

    generally able to assess their own word knowledge. Both the MC andVSC pretests significantly correlated with the standardized literacymeasures (see Appendix C).

    Additionally, students in the treatment group wrote persuasive essayson a weekly basis. All essays were transcribed by three trained researchassistants and scored using a researcher-developed rubric that consistedof three sections: ideas, overall cohesion/structure, and academic

    language (see Appendix D). Interrater agreement was .81 (Kendall'sCoefficient of Concordance), and raters were blind to the time (i.e.,week) in which the essays were produced. Analyses of the essaysprovided information on WG target word use, as well as on overall

    writing quality.

    Standardized Measures

    Vocabulary, listening comprehension, and reading comprehensionwere assessed using the Group Reading Assessment and DiagnosticEvaluation (GRADE) (Williams, 2002). The GRADE is an untimedgroup-administered reading test that includes vocabulary, listeningcomprehension, sentence comprehension, and passage comprehensionsub tests. The Reading Vocabulary sub test measures students' vocabularyknowledge without the benefit of contextual clues. Students silently read

    3There was oneexception

    to this set of 30randomly

    selected WGtarget

    words for the

    pretest: reluctant appears on the self-check and not on the multiple-choice test; culture

    appears on the multiple-choice test and not on the self-check. This was corrected for the

    posttest.

    WORD MEANINGSMATTER 681

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    15/32

    a phrase or short sentence in which one of the words is printed in bold

    type, and they select the appropriate meaning of the word from a list offour or five choices. The

    Listening Comprehensionsubtest measures

    students' linguistic comprehension without printed cues. Students listento a sentence or pair of sentences that are read aloud by the testadministrator, and they then select one of four pictures that best

    matches what was read aloud to them. Finally, the Sentence and PassageComprehension subtests yield the comprehension composite.Sentence Comprehension measures students' comprehension of asentence as a whole thought or unit. Students silently read shortsentences in which one of the words ismissing (as indicated by a blank),and they then select the appropriate word from a list of choices. PassageComprehension measures students' comprehension skills with anextended passage. After silently reading a passage with one or more

    paragraphs, students answer multiple-choice questions about the

    passage. Level 5, Form A was used at pretest. Internal consistencyreliability was reported as .95.

    Word reading accuracy and fluency were assessed individually usingthe Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE; Torgesen, Wagner, &

    Rashotte, 1999). The Sight Word Efficiency subtest assesses the numberof real words a student can read in 45 s; the Phonemic Decoding

    Efficiencysubtest assesses the number of

    pronounceablenonsense

    words a student can read in 45 s. The TOWRE subtests were combinedfor analysis (r

    = .8219, p= < 0.0001). Internal consistency reliability was

    reported as .95 and scorer reliability as .99.

    RESULTSResearch question 1: Following the 20-week vocabulary intervention,

    do the treatment or contrast group students gain knowledge of a greaternumber of targeted words?

    Analytic Approach

    First, the treatment and contrast groups were compared on allmeasures (researcher-developed and standardized) prior to the

    beginning of the intervention. Next, posttest performance on the

    researcher-developed measures was examined by conducting Bonferoni?-tests on the gain scores (change from pretest to posttest). Finally,standardized effect sizes (Cohen's d; Cohen, 1988) were computedusing differences inmean performance divided by the pooled standarddeviation.

    682 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    16/32

    Pretest Performance

    Exceptfor the

    TOWRE,on which the treatment

    groupscored

    slightlyhigher, the treatment and contrast groups were very well matched at

    pretest (see Table 2).

    Posttest Performance

    Table 3 presents the results of posttest performance, including means,standard deviations, gain scores, significance tests, and effect sizes on the

    researcher-developed measures.

    Word Generation Multiple-Choice TestThe treatment group students gained knowledge of approximately six

    target WG vocabulary words, whereas the contrast group students gainedknowledge of about one. Only 30 of the 100 taught WG vocabulary wordswere randomly included in the MC test, and thus a gain of 6 target wordson the MC test translates into gained knowledge of approximately 20

    target words and a gain ratio of 1 into approximately 3. Put differently,students in the treatment group went from knowing about 65% of the

    target words at pretest to knowing about 83% at posttest, whereasstudents in the contrast

    groupwent from

    knowing65% of the

    targetwords at pretest to knowing about 68% at posttest. The difference in the

    gain was statistically significant, with an impressive effect size (d=

    1.24).

    TABLE 2Fall Pretest Scores on All Literacy Measures for the Treatment and Contrast Group Students (N= 49)

    Treatment Contrast Bonferroni Effect(n= 24) (w 25) Rvalues size

    WG multiple-choice testa Mean 19.71 19.96 1.00.06SD 4.16 4.63

    WG real words self-checkb Mean 2.97 2.97 1.00.00SD 0.37 0.55

    WG nonsense words self- Mean 1.28 1.44 0.46.36checkc SD 0.32 0.55GRADE vocabulary compre- Mean 15.08 13.84 1.00.28hensiond SD 4.35 4.63GRADE listening compre- Mean 13.50 13.36 1.00.07hensione SD 1.77 2.18TOWRE word reading flu- Mean 56.06 48.50 0.04 .86

    ency composite SD 8.23 9.39GRADE comprehension Mean 28.67 26.32 1.00 .25

    compositef SD 8.83 9.91

    Note. SD = standard deviation. aMaximum = 30. bMaximum = 4. 'Maximum = 4 (a highaverage is not desirable, as it indicates that students reported knowing nonsense words well

    enough to use them). dMaximum= 35. ' Maximum = 17.

    fMaximum = 39.

    WORD MEANINGSMATTER 683

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    17/32

    TABLE 3Spring Posttest and Gain Scores on Researcher-Developed Measures for the Treatment andContrast Group Students (n

    =49)

    Spring scores Gain scores

    WG multiplechoice testa

    WG real wordsself-Checkb

    WG nonsense

    Treatment Contrast Treatment Contrast Bonferroni Effect

    (n= 24) {n= 25) (n= 24) (n= 25) Rvalues sizeMean

    SDMean

    SDMean

    words self-checkc SD

    25.293.143.640.291.290.30

    21.124.223.410.471.770.60

    5.583.720.670.350.0030.27

    1.163.400.440.440.330.55

    0.0005 1.24

    0.1200 0.58

    0.0400 0.80

    Note. SD = standard deviation. aMaximum = 30. bMaximum = 4 (a higher average is desirable,

    as it indicates that students reported knowing real words well enough to use them). cMaximum= 4 (a higher average is not desirable, as it indicates that students reported knowing nonsensewords well enough to use them).

    Word Generation Vocabulary Self-Check

    Although there were no significant differences between the two

    groups on the VSC for real words, the direction of the effects suggestthat, on average, students in the treatment group reported knowing

    more of the target words from pretest to posttest. On the VSC fornonsense words, the treatment group reported knowing a significantlylower number of known nonsense words than the contrast group. Infact, except for two nonsense words reported as known well enough to

    use by one student in the treatment group, none of the students in thetreatment group reported knowing the nonsense words well enough to

    use them. However, each of the 10 nonsense words were consistentlyreported as known well enough to use them by students in the contrast

    group.Research question 2: In the treatment group's weekly student essays,

    what is the extent of target vocabulary word use over the course of the20-week intervention?

    Specifically,do students use the

    target vocabularywords cumulatively or is their use confined to words taught in thecurrent week? Further, is there any change in the quality (e.g.,coherence and academic language use) of the essays over time?

    Analytic Approach

    All available essays were transcribed in the codes for the human

    analysis of transcripts (CHAT) format of the child language data

    exchangesystem

    (MacWhinney,1995;

    MacWhinney& Snow, 1985,

    1990), and the computerized language analysis (CLAN) program(MacWhinney, 2000) was used for analysis. The focus of analysis was

    684 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    18/32

    on whether students used the newly taught WG vocabulary words in their

    writing. Thus all essays were corrected prior to being scored with the

    researcher-developed writing rubric (see Appendix E foran

    example).Frequency counts of the weekly target words, as well as of past targetwords, were run in CLAN. To determine whether students' overall

    writing quality improved across the 20-week intervention, individual

    growth modeling (IGM) using the multilevel model for change (Singer& Willett, 2003) was used. The analyses were conducted in a personperiod dataset that contained the longitudinal data on all sampledchildren, using SAS PROC MIXED4 with full maximum likelihoodestimation. The use of IGM allows for robust estimates of growth, even

    with occasional missing or incomplete data points for individual

    children. As suggested by Singer and Willett (2003), the likelihoodratio test was used as the primary criterion for evaluating model fit, andthe Akaike and Bayesian information criteria were also provided asadditional indicators of goodness of fit.

    Target Word Use

    Over the course of the 20-week intervention, students used an averageof two of the five weekly target words in their weekly essay (SD

    =1.21).

    Further, on average, 10 previously taught words were used in the weeklyessays from week 2 to week 20; the use of previously taught words was

    greater toward the end of the intervention: Students used an average of8 past target words during weeks 2-10 compared with an average of 12

    during weeks 11-20. At the individual level, there was substantial

    variability in the total number of past target words individual studentsused across the 20-week intervention; 21% of the students used morethan 12 past target words in their essays, 42% used 6-11 past target

    words, and 33% used 5 or fewer past target words.

    Writing Quality

    The final area of investigation involved examining whetherstudents' overall writing quality improved over the course of the 20

    week intervention. Individual Growth Modeling results indicate thatthe average writing quality score at the beginning of the intervention

    was 4.53 (SD= 1.05; scale of 0-9 points), with an average growth of

    .04 points per week (see Table 4). This corresponds to an averagewriting quality growth of .71 points (nearly 1 full point) over the

    4SAS PROC MIXED is a generalization of the general linear model (GLM) ; it fits the widerclass of mixed linear models, incorporating random effects, allowing for the specificationof covariance structures, and providing a better mechanism for handling missing values.

    WORDMEANINGSMATTER 685

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    19/32

    TABLE 4Individual Growth Modeling Results for Students' Writing Quality Growth (N

    = 24)

    Model 1 Model 2Fixed effectsInitial status

    Weeks 1-20Weeks 1-10Weeks 11-20

    4.5308***0.0353***

    4.5931***

    0.02910.0406*

    Variance componentsLevel 1Level 2

    Wi thin-personBetween-person

    0.6988***1.0636***

    0.6988***1.0633***

    Deviance (?2 LL)AICBIC

    1,393.40

    1,401.401,406.20

    1,393.30

    1,403.301,409.20

    Note. Model 1 overall average writing quality growth across the 20-week vocabulary intervention;Model 2 = writing quality growth across weeks 1-10 versus weeks 10-20. AIC

    = Akaikeinformation criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; LL

    = lower limit. < 0.001.

    course of the 20-week intervention, translating into a substantial effectsize (d

    =.67).

    The growth trajectories were broken up into separate linear

    components to investigate the possibility of differential growth between

    the early and the later period of the intervention (Raudenbush & Bryk,2002). Writing quality was significant only during the last 10weeks of theintervention (see Figure 1): 0.03 points per week during the first10 weeks (or .29 points over the course of the first 10 weeks) did notreach significance, but .04 points per week during the last 10 weeks ofthe intervention was statistically significant. These differential results,though subtle, are substantively important, as they suggest that theeffects on students' writing would have gone undetected had the

    vocabulary intervention lasted only 10 weeks.

    A Note on Essay Length

    A long-standing finding in the writing field is that essay lengthcorrelates with overall writing quality (e.g., Hiller, Marcotte, & Martin,1969). This may raise the question of whether students in the presentstudy were merely writing more and thus producing more high-qualityessays over time. As expected, essay length was correlated withstudents' overall writing quality (r

    = .45, p < .05). However, essaylength remained stable throughout the duration of the intervention

    (mean=

    90, SD=

    18), indicating that students were not writing moreover the course of the intervention. Indeed, controlling for essaylength, the correlation between target word use and writing quality

    686 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    20/32

    5.5

    Average growth= .71points over the 20-weeks (

  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    21/32

    knowledge. This word awareness is critical for comprehension, withresearch underscoring the need to encourage students to ask when theydo not know the

    meaningsof words

    (Biemiller, 2003;Graves &

    Watts,2002; Nagy & Scott, 2000). Taking these findings together, and given the

    stability of low vocabulary among LM students at all grade levels, theeffects of explicit vocabulary instruction (even if for just a few minutes a

    day) should not be underestimated.In addition to examining effects on vocabulary, this study was

    designed to explore the extent to which newly taught words would

    appear in students' writing. The results of the current study begin toshed light on this relationship amongst LM learners in the UnitedStates, with analyses revealing that some transfer occurs relatively

    quickly. In fact, use of newly taught words in writing suggests that thewords are at least partially in students' lexicons. Further, it was

    encouraging to find that students used target words from previousweeks in their essays. Not surprisingly, a greater number of past targetwords were used during the final 10 weeks of the intervention. A keyimplication?highlighting the fact that students need opportunities to

    use newly taught words?is that it will take time for students tointernalize the newly taught words before they are willing or able to

    productively use them in writing. For example, research shows that ittakes 5-16 exposures to new words for receptive word knowledge(Nation, 1990). Because productive word knowledge ismore difficult,

    presumably, it should take even longer for students to use words intheir writing. The key takeaway point is thus that finding multiple waysof exposing students to the newly taught words is critical.

    The final area of investigation involved examining the effects of theintervention on writing quality. The dearth of research exploring this

    relationship amongst school-age LM learners effectively limits our

    understanding of whether increased vocabulary knowledge results inbetter writing quality, whether opportunities to write results in better

    vocabulary,or whether there is a

    reciprocal relationship. Thoughthe

    current study does not provide any definitive answers about the natureof these relationships, results of this study do suggest that thecombination of vocabulary instruction (including other aspects of theintervention, such as the weekly debates that fostered increased

    language use) and having students write on a weekly basis likelycontributes to students' overall writing quality gains. Importantly,during the course of the 20-week intervention, feedback on the essayswas not provided by the teacher, and the teacher did not provideinstruction focused on persuasive or argumentative writing. Further,

    the writing quality gains cannot be attributed to increased essay length,as students did not produce longer essays over time. A possibility to befurther explored, then, is that students' writing quality improved

    688 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    22/32

    because they were practicing and because they had accumulated a storeof new words that could make their writing more precise and effective.

    Importantly, improvements in students' writing quality were smallerduring the first 10 weeks of the intervention, underscoring the need forsustained vocabulary instruction combined with ample opportunitiesfor students to write. A clear implication from this study is thus thatsustained vocabulary instruction is needed to detect improvements instudents' writing.

    Implications for Research

    In thispilot study, only researcher-developed

    measures wereadministered at posttest, because effects on standardized measures

    were not expected. As Stahl and Fairbanks (1986) noted more than twodecades ago, a relatively high number of explicitly taught words mustbe present in standardized measures for them to be sensitive to theeffects of vocabulary instruction; only 2 of the 30 words on the GRADEstandardized vocabulary test were target WG words. Regarding readingcomprehension outcomes, WG exposed students to a wide range of

    topics, from global warming to the death penalty, and therefore helpedbroaden students' general background knowledge. Background knowl

    edge is a strong predictor of students' reading comprehensionperformance (Anderson & Freebody, 1981; Anderson & Pearson,1984; Droop & Verhoeven, 1998), and it is likely that the value of theintervention for reading comprehension outcomes will be evident overtime, as students encounter passages about (or related to) the topicsthey have been exposed to through WG. Thus the benefits of WG onstudents' reading comprehension can be expected to be cumulative,rather than immediately following the 20-week intervention. The

    pervasive challenge, however, is that interventions are commonlyimplemented for short periods of time with the expectation of large,immediate gains. Accordingly, many interventions with high potentialare erroneously deemed ineffective and consequently terminated onthe basis of weak results. An implication of this research is that

    vocabulary instruction can be effective for LM learners and shouldbecome part of students' every day curriculum, sustained throughoutthe school years. Refining the elements of effective vocabularyinstruction to meet the differentiated needs of students will be acritical next step, but it should be clear that lasting gains can only be

    expected with well-designed and, equally importantly, sustained

    vocabulary instruction. Further, the effects of vocabulary instructionshould be evaluated more robustly (e.g., examining effects on writing).For the growing population of LM learners who enter school with low

    WORD MEANINGSMATTER 689

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    23/32

    levels of vocabulary, vocabulary instruction must be a part of theirschool day.

    Limitations and Future Research

    First, the demonstrated literacy gains found in this quasiexperimentalpilot study cannot be causally related to the intervention; a randomized

    study is a necessary next step. Second, as was the case in this study,multiple-choice tests are widely used to index students' vocabularyknowledge, but future work should incorporate measures of vocabularythat attend to depth of word knowledge. Third, the relatively small samplesize limited the feasibility of investigating whether, as some research hasfound, the effect of vocabulary instruction varies by ability levels, withlower performing students exhibiting greater benefits (e.g., Nelson &

    Stage, 2007). On a related note, a monolingual-English-speakingcomparison group would help disentangle whether vocabulary instructionhas differential effects for LM versus non-LM students, and the extent ofthose differential effects on various literacy measures. Finally, longitudinalstudies designed to track students' literacy progress, including writing,over time are needed to robustly evaluate the components of effective

    vocabulary instruction for different types of learners. In the present study,

    an open question iswhether the demonstrated gains will be lasting onesand whether gains on comprehension can be expected. Notwithstandingthese limitations, students in the study are representative of a growingpopulation of learners in the United States?children from Spanishspeaking homes enrolled in urban, generally low-income, schools?andthe present pilot vocabulary intervention study thus extends previous

    work and strengthens our understanding of the effects of vocabularyinstruction on upper-elementary LM students.

    CONCLUSION

    Because vocabulary knowledge is cumulative, greater instructionalattention to vocabulary is needed starting in and continuing well beyondthe primary grades. This point must be underscored for LM learners, whotend to have fewer English language models and thus more limitedvocabularies compared to native English speakers. Further, aside from

    increasing students' vocabulary knowledge, it appears that explicit vocabularyinstruction has the potential for increasing students' overall writing quality,even without explicit writing instruction. This pilot study suggests that

    sustained vocabulary instruction, not short-term interventions, are neededand that purposeful activities that provide students with authentic contexts tolearn and productively use newly taught words are integral components of

    690 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    24/32

    effective vocabulary instruction. Until vocabulary becomes an integral, dailyaspect of the K-12 curriculum, all students, and in particular LM learners,

    may continue to learn to read (i.e., decode), but the development ofvocabulary and writing skills is simultaneously essential for academic success.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    I would like to thank Catherine E. Snow, Nonie K. Lesaux, and Terrence Tivnan fortheir helpful comments on this manuscript. This study was made possible by the

    Strategic Education Research Partnership (SERP), funded by the CarnegieCorporation, the William and Flora Hewlett Foundation, and The SpencerFoundation.

    THE AUTHOR

    Jeannette Mancilla-Mart?nez is an Assistant Professor in Literacy, Language, andCulture at the Universityof Illinois at Chicago. Her primary research interest is the

    language and literacy development of at-risk populations, including students who

    struggle with reading, language minority learners, and immigrant students.

    REFERENCES

    Anderson, R. C, &Freebody,

    P.(1981). Vocabulary knowledge. Inj.

    T. Guthrie

    (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77-117). Newark, DE:International Reading Association.

    Anderson, R. C, 8cNagy, W. E. (1991). Word meanings. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamii, P.Mosenthal 8c P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. II pp. 231

    256). White Plains, NY: Longman.Anderson, R. C, 8c Pearson, P. D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in

    reading comprehension. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamii, & P. Mosen thai

    (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 255-291). New York, NY: Longman.August, D., & Shanahan, T. (Eds.) (2006). Developing literacy in second-language

    learners: Report of the ational Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth.

    Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2002). B?nging words to life: Robustvocabulary instruction. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Bermudez, A. B., & Prater, D. L. (1994). Examining the effects of gender and second

    language proficiency on Hispanic writers' persuasive discourse. Bilingual Research

    fournal, 18, 47-62.

    Biemiller, A. (2003). Vocabulary: Needed if more children are to read well. ReadingPsychology, 4, 323-335. doi: 10.1080/02702710390227297.

    Biemiller, A., 8c Slonim, N. (2001). Estimating root word vocabulary growth innormative and advantaged populations: Evidence for a common sequence of

    vocabulary acquisition, fournal of Educational Psychology, 93, 498-520. doi:

    10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.498.

    Bravo,.A., 8c Tilson, J. (2006 April), Assessment magazines: Gauging students' depth of

    reading comprehension and science understanding. Talk presented at the AmericanEducation Research Association Annual Meeting, San Francisco, CA.

    WORD MEANINGSMATTER 691

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    25/32

    Calder?n, M., August, D., Slavin, R., Duran, D., Madden, .,& Cheung, A. (2005).

    Bringing words to life in classrooms with English-language learners. In E. H.Hiebert 8c M. L. Kamil (Eds.), Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bnnging research to

    practice (pp. 115-136). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Carlo, M., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C, Dressler, C, Lipman, D., White, C.

    (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English LanguageLearners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 39,88-215. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.39.2.3.

    Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,

    NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Coxhead, A. (2000). A new academic word list. TESOL Quarterly, 34, 213-238. doi:

    10.2307/3587951.Dale, E. (1965). Vocabulary measurement: Techniques and major findings.

    Elementary English, 42, 895-901.

    Davis,L.

    H., Carlisle, J. F.,8c

    Beeman,.M.

    (1999). Hispanicchildren's

    writingin

    English and Spanish when English is the language of instruction. In T. Shanahan8c F. Rodriquez-Brown (Eds.), National reading conference yearbook (pp. 238-249).

    Chicago, IL: National Reading Conference.

    Droop, M., 8cVerhoeven, L. (1998). Background knowledge, linguistic complexity,and second-language reading comprehension. Journal of Literacy Research, 30,253-271. doi: 10.1080/10862969809547998.

    Droop, M., & Verhoeven, L. T. (2003). Language proficiency and reading ability infirst- and second-language learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 78-103. doi:

    10.1598/RRQ.38.1.4.Duin, A. H., & Graves, M. F. (1987). Intensive vocabulary instruction as a prewriting

    technique. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 311-330. doi: 10.2307/747971.Ferris, M. R., 8c Politzer, R. L. (1981). Effects of early and delayed second language

    acquisition: English composition skills of Spanish-speaking junior high schoolstudents. TESOL Quarterly, 15, 263-274. doi: 10.2307/3586752.

    Garcia, G. E. (1991). Factors influencing the English reading test performance of

    Spanish-speaking Hispanic children. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 371-392. doi:

    10.2307/747894.Goulden, R., Nation, P., 8cRead, J. (1990). How large can a receptive vocabulary be?

    Applied Linguistics, 11, 341-363. doi: 10.1093/applin/11.4.341.Graham, S., 8c Perin, D. (2007). Writing next: Effective strategies to improve uniting of

    adolescents in middle and high schools?A report to Carnegie Corporation ofNew York.

    Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent Education.

    Graves, M. F., & Watts, S. M. (2002) The place of word consciousness in a researchbased vocabulary program. In S. J. Samuels, 8cA. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What researchhas to say about reading instruction (3rd ed., pp. 140-165). Newark, DE:International Reading Association.

    Hart, B., 8cRisley, T. (1995).Meaningful differences n the everyday xperience f youngAmerican children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Harwood, R. L., Leyendecker, B., Carlson V.J., Asencio, M., 8cMiller, A. M. (2002).

    Parenting among Latino families in the U.S. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), The handbook

    of parenting (2nd ed., Vol. 4, pp. 21-46). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Hiller, J. H., Marcotte, D. R., & Martin, T. (1969). Opinionation, vagueness, and

    specificity-distinctions: Essay traits measured by computer. American EducationalResearch

    Journal, 6,271-286.

    Hoover, W. A, & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and

    Writing: An Interdisciplinary ournal 2, 127-160. doi: 10.1007/BF00401799.

    692 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    26/32

    Hutchinson, J. M., Whiteley, . E., Smith, C. D., 8c Connors, L. (2003). The

    developmental progression of comprehension-related skills in children learningEAL. Journal of Research in Reading, 26, 19-32. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.261003.

    Lanauze, M., 8c Snow, C. E. (1989). The relation between first- and second-languagewriting skills: Evidence from Puerto Rican elementary school children in

    bilingual programs. Linguistics and Education, 1, 323-339. doi: 10.1016/S0898

    5898(89)80005-1.Laufer, . (1998). The development of passive and active vocabulary in second

    language: Same or different? Applied Linguistics, 19, 255-271. doi: 10.1093/applin/19.2.255.

    Laufer, ., 8c Nation, P. (1995). Vocabulary size and use: Lexical richness in L2written production. Applied Linguistics, 16, 307-322. doi: 10.1093/applin/16.3.307.

    Lee, S. H. (2003). ESL learners' vocabulary use in writing and the effects of explicit

    vocabularyinstruction.

    System, 31,537-561. doi:

    10.1016/j.system.2003.02.004.Lee, S. H., 8c Muncie, J. (2006). From receptive to productive: Improving ESL

    learners' use of vocabulary in a postreading composition task. TESOL Quarterly,40, 295-320. doi: 10.2307/40264524.

    Leki, I., 8c Carson, J. G. (1994). Students' perceptions of EAP writing instruction and

    writing needs across the disciplines. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 81-101. doi: 10.2307/3587199.

    Lesaux, N., Koda, K., Siegel, L., 8c Shanahan, T. (2006). Development of literacy. InD. August 8c T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners:

    Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Mino?ty Children and Youth

    (pp. 75-122). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Lipson, M. Y., 8cWixson,.K. (2003). Assessment and instruction of reading and uniting

    difficulty. oston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.MacWhinney, B. (1995). The CHILDES Project: Took for analyzing talk (2nd ed.).

    Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Toob for analyzing talk (3rd ed.).Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    MacWhinney, B., 8c Snow, C. E. (1985). The child language data exchange system.Journal of Child Language, 12, 271-296. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900006449.

    MacWhinney, B., 8c Snow, C. E. (1990). The child language data exchange system:An update. Journal of Child Language, 17, 457-472. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900013866.

    Meara, P. (1996). The dimensions of lexical competence. In G. Brown, K.

    Malmkjaer, 8c J. Williams (Eds.), Performance and competence in second languageacquisition (pp. 35-53). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Moats, L., Foorman, B., 8c Taylor, P. (2006). How quality of writing instruction

    impacts high-risk fourth graders' writing. Reading and Writing: An Interdis?plinary

    Journal, 19, 363-391. doi: 10.1007/sl 1145-005-4944-6.Nagy, W. (1997). On the role of the context in first- and second-language vocabulary

    learning. In Schmitt,. 8cMcCarthy, M., (Eds.), Vocabulary: description, acquisition

    and pedagogy (pp. 64-73). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Nagy, W. E., 8c Herman, P. A. (1987). Breadth and depth of vocabulary knowledge:

    Implications for acquisition and instruction. In M. G. McKeown 8c.E. Curtis

    (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary cquisition (pp. 19-35). Hillsdale, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum.

    Nagy, W. E., & Scott, J. A. (2000). Vocabulary processes. In M. L. Kamil, P. B.

    Mosenthal, D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3,

    pp. 269-284). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    WORDMEANINGSMATTER 693

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    27/32

    Nation, I. S. P. (1990). Teaching and learning vocabulary. Boston, MA: Heinle 8c HeinlePublishers.

    Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge, England:

    Cambridge University Press.Nelson, J. R., & Stage, S. A. (2007). Fostering the development of vocabulary

    knowledge and reading comprehension through contextually-based multiplemeaning vocabulary instruction. Education and Intervention of Children, 30, 1-22.

    Pearson, P. D., Hiebert, E. H., 8c Kamil, M. L. (2007). Vocabulary assessment: Whatwe know and what we need to learn. Reading Research Quarterly, 42, 282-296. doi:

    10.1598/RRQ.42.2.4.Perez, E. (1981). Oral language competence improves reading skills of Mexican

    American third graders. Reading Teacher, 35, 24-27.

    Proctor, C. P., Carlo, M., August, D., 8c Snow, C. (2005). Native Spanish-speakingchildren reading in English: Toward a model of comprehension. Journal of

    EducationalPsychology, 97,

    246-256. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.246.Proctor, C. P., August, D., Carlo, M., 8c Snow, C. E. (2006). The intriguing role of

    Spanish language vocabulary knowledge in predicting English reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 159-169. doi: 10.1037/00220663.98.1.159.

    Raimes, A. (1985). What unskilled ESL students do as they write: A classroom studyof composing. TESOL Quarterly, 19, 229-258. doi: 10.2307/3586828.

    Ramirez, S. Z. (1986). The effects of suggestopedia in teaching English vocabulary to

    Spanish-dominant Chicano third graders. Ekmentary School fournal, 86, 325-333.doi: 10.1086/461453.

    Raudenbush, S. W., 8c Bryk, A S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications anddata analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Saddler, B., 8c Graham, S. (2007). The relationship between writing knowledge and

    writing performance among more and less skilled writers. Reading & WritingQuarterly, 3, 231-247. doi: 10.1080/10573560701277575.

    Singer, J. D., 8cWillett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal ata analysis:Modeling changeand event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Slavin, R. E., 8cMadden, N. A. (2000). Research on achievement outcomes of Successfor All: A summary and response to critics. Phi Delta Kappan, 82, 38-40, 59-66.

    Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., 8c Griffin, P. (Eds.) (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in

    young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Stahl, S. A., 8c Fairbanks, M. M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: Amodel-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56, 72-110.

    Stahl,S.

    A.,8c

    Nagy,W. E.

    (2006). Teachingword

    meanings. Mahwah, NJ:Lawrence

    Erlbaum.

    Suarez-Orozco, M. M., & Paez, M. M. (2002). Latinos: Remaking America. Berkeley, CA:

    University of California Press.

    Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., 8c Rashotte, C. A (1999). Test of word reading efficiency(TOWRE). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    U.S. Census Bureau. (2005). U.S. Census Bureau, Population Estimates Program,April 1, 2000, and July 1, 2004. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/population/www/ pop-profile/files/dynamic/RACEHO.pdf

    U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2003). Thenation's report card: Writing 2002, trial urban district assessment (Report No. NCES

    2003-530). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2004). The

    condition of education 2004 (Report No. NCES 2004-007). Washington, DC:Government Printing Office.

    694 TESOLQUARTERLY

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    28/32

    U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2007a).The condition of education 2006 (Report No. NCES 2007-064). Washington, DC:Government Printing Office.

    U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, National Assessmentof Education Progress. (2007b). The nations report card: Reading 2007 (Report No. NCES

    2007-496). Washington, DC: United States Government Printing Office.U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Research Improvement, RAND

    Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understanding: oward an R&D programin reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.

    U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health,National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Teachingchildren to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on

    reading and its implications or reading instruction NIH Publication No. 00-4769).Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.

    Vaughn-Shavuo, F. (1990). Using story grammar and language experience for improvingrecall and comprehension in the teaching of ESL to Spanish-dominant first-graders(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Hosfra University, New York, NY.

    Verhoeven, L. (1990). Acquisition of reading in a second language. Reading Research

    Quarterly, 5, 90-114. doi: 10.2307/747596.Verhoeven, L. T. (2000). Components in early second language reading and

    spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4. 313-330. doi: 10.1207/S1532799XSSR0404_4.

    Walters, J., 8c Wolf, Y (1996). Language awareness in non-native writers:

    Metalinguistic judgments of need for revision. Language Awareness, 5, 3-25. doi:

    10.1080/09658416.1996.9959888.Wesche, M., & Paribakht, T. S. (1996). Assessing second language vocabulary

    knowledge: Depth vs. breadth. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 53,13-40.

    Williams, .T. (2002). Group Reading Assessment and Diagnostic Evaluation (GRADE).Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.

    Appendix A

    100Word Generation Target Vocabulary Words

    Week 11.Analyze2. Factor3. Function4. Interpret5. Structure

    Week 521. Considerable22. Contribute23. Demonstrate24. Sufficient25. Valid

    Week 26. Context7. Indicate8. Variable9. Create10. Benefit

    Week 626. Rely27. React28. Alternative29.

    Justify30. Proportion

    Week 311. Complexity12. Culture13. Element14. Resourceful15. Tradition

    Week 731. Access32. Civil33. Despite34.

    Integrate35. Promote

    Week 416. Design17. Features18. Impact19. Potential20. Transfer

    Week 836. Attribute37. Cycle38. Hypothesis39.

    Project40. Statistics

    WORD MEANINGS MATTER 695

    This content downloaded from 8 2.33.77.134 on Fri, 12 Dec 201 4 18:58:01 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Word Meanings Matter Cultivating English Vocabulary Knowledge in Fifth-Grade Spanish-Speaking Language Minori

    29/32

    Week 941. Compounds42. Conflict

    43. Fundamental44. Substitution45. Alter

    Week 1361. Advocate62. Contrary63. Reverse64. Release65. Prohibited

    Week 1781. Decades82. Violation83. Temporary84. Unified85. Incompatible

    Week 1046. Modified47. Monitor

    48. Adjustment49. Transition50. Exposure

    Week 1466. Phenomenon67. Priority68. Transmission69. Intervention70. Suspended

    Week 1886. Bulk87. Accommodate88. Unethical89. Route90. Confine

    Week 1151. Acknowledge52. Incidence

    53. Incorporate54. Initiatives55. Transport

    Week 1571. Abandon72. Biased73. Contemporary74. Dramatic75. Exploit

    Week 1991. Collapse92. Conceive93. Incline94. Intrinsically95. Nonetheless

    Week 1256. Diversity57. Enhance

    58. Migration59. Presumed60. Reveal

    Week 1676. Accumulation77. Contradict78. Exhibit79. Inevitable80. Manipulate

    Week 2096. Convince97 Enormous98. Integrity99. Persistent100. Reluctant

    Appendix

    Word GenerationWeekly

    Teacher Form

    Grade_Subject Area_Day of Week_Please fill in this anonymous questionnaire for WEEK_of Word Generation (WG).

    1.What activity did you do inWG this week?

    2.Word Generation went well this week.Yes_No_Because

    of(check all that

    apply):_Student enthusiasm

    _Design of Materials

    _Support from teachers/coach/administrator_My own preparation_Other_

    3. I had enough time for WG this week.Yes_No_

    Because of (check all that apply):_Students' actions/interest level

    _Support (or lack of support) from others

    _My own preparation_Nature of preexisting curriculum

    _Nature of WG ma