17
TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH AUTISM BY REFINING THE TACTIC OF USING STEREOTYPY AS REINFORCEMENTJACQUELINE N. POTTER NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN AND WESTERN NEW ENGLAND UNIVERSITY GREGORY P. HANLEY WESTERN NEW ENGLAND UNIVERSITY MATOTOPA AUGUSTINE NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN AND WESTERN NEW ENGLAND UNIVERSITY AND CASEY J. CLAY AND MEREDITH C. PHELPS NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN Use of automatically reinforced stereotypy as reinforcement has been shown to be successful for increasing socially desirable behaviors in persons with intellectual disabilities (Charlop, Kurtz, & Casey, 1990; Hanley, Iwata, Thompson, & Lindberg, 2000; Hung, 1978). A component analysis of this treatment was conducted with 3 adolescents who had been diagnosed with autism, and then extended by (a) progressively increasing the quantitative and qualitative aspects of the response requirement to earn access to stereotypy, (b) arranging objective measures of client preference for contingent access to stereotypy compared to other relevant treatments for their automatically reinforced stereotypy, and (c) assessing the social validity of this treatment with other relevant stakeholders. Implications for addressing stereotypy and increasing the leisure skills of adolescents with autism are discussed. Key words: autism, automatic reinforcement, differential reinforcement, engagement, leisure skills, play, social validity, stereotypy Persons who have been diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often engage in repetitive acts that appear to serve no useful function (Goldman et al., 2008). These repetitive acts are collectively referred to as stereotypy due to the formal similarity and the periodicity with which they are emitted. Reducing the level of stereotypy can be difficult (Singer, 2009), espe- cially when the responses are maintained by automatic reinforcers (i.e., reinforcers that are a direct result of the behavior; Vollmer, 1994). Although punishment is sometimes necessary during intervention for socially mediated prob- lem behavior (Fisher et al., 1993; Hagopian, Fisher, Sullivan, Acquisto, & LeBlanc, 1998), there has been a noticeable trend towards the use of reinforcement-based interventions since the advent of functional analysis (Pelios, Morren, This manuscript was submitted as a doctoral dissertation by the first author to Western New England University. The first author would like to acknowledge the feedback provided by William Ahearn, Rachel Thompson, and Dennis Kolodziejski on earlier versions of this manuscript. Thanks also to Lydie Bierdron, Zachary Bird, and Patrick Malloy for their assistance in data collection. Casey J. Clay is now at Utah State University. Meredith C. Phelps is now at ACES Village School. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jacqueline N. Potter, New England Center for Children, 33 Turnpike Road, Southborough, Massachu- setts 01772 (e-mail: [email protected]). doi: 10.1002/jaba.52 JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2013, 46, 407423 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2013) 407

TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITHAUTISM BY REFINING THE TACTIC OF “USING STEREOTYPY

AS REINFORCEMENT”

JACQUELINE N. POTTER

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN AND WESTERN NEW ENGLAND UNIVERSITY

GREGORY P. HANLEY

WESTERN NEW ENGLAND UNIVERSITY

MATOTOPA AUGUSTINE

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN AND WESTERN NEW ENGLAND UNIVERSITY

AND

CASEY J. CLAY AND MEREDITH C. PHELPS

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN

Use of automatically reinforced stereotypy as reinforcement has been shown to be successful forincreasing socially desirable behaviors in persons with intellectual disabilities (Charlop, Kurtz, &Casey, 1990; Hanley, Iwata, Thompson, & Lindberg, 2000; Hung, 1978). A component analysis ofthis treatment was conducted with 3 adolescents who had been diagnosed with autism, and thenextended by (a) progressively increasing the quantitative and qualitative aspects of the responserequirement to earn access to stereotypy, (b) arranging objective measures of client preference forcontingent access to stereotypy compared to other relevant treatments for their automaticallyreinforced stereotypy, and (c) assessing the social validity of this treatment with other relevantstakeholders. Implications for addressing stereotypy and increasing the leisure skills of adolescentswith autism are discussed.Key words: autism, automatic reinforcement, differential reinforcement, engagement, leisure

skills, play, social validity, stereotypy

Persons who have been diagnosed with anautism spectrum disorder (ASD) often engage inrepetitive acts that appear to serve no useful

function (Goldman et al., 2008). These repetitiveacts are collectively referred to as stereotypy due tothe formal similarity and the periodicity withwhich they are emitted. Reducing the level ofstereotypy can be difficult (Singer, 2009), espe-cially when the responses are maintained byautomatic reinforcers (i.e., reinforcers that are adirect result of the behavior; Vollmer, 1994).

Although punishment is sometimes necessaryduring intervention for socially mediated prob-lem behavior (Fisher et al., 1993; Hagopian,Fisher, Sullivan, Acquisto, & LeBlanc, 1998),there has been a noticeable trend towards the useof reinforcement-based interventions since theadvent of functional analysis (Pelios, Morren,

This manuscript was submitted as a doctoral dissertationby the first author to Western New England University. Thefirst author would like to acknowledge the feedbackprovided by William Ahearn, Rachel Thompson, andDennis Kolodziejski on earlier versions of this manuscript.Thanks also to Lydie Bierdron, Zachary Bird, and PatrickMalloy for their assistance in data collection.

Casey J. Clay is now at Utah State University.Meredith C. Phelps is now at ACES Village School.Correspondence concerning this article should be

addressed to Jacqueline N. Potter, New England Centerfor Children, 33 Turnpike Road, Southborough, Massachu-setts 01772 (e-mail: [email protected]).

doi: 10.1002/jaba.52

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2013, 46, 407–423 NUMBER 2 (SUMMER 2013)

407

Page 2: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

Tesch, & Axelrod, 1999). Applications ofinterventions based on differential reinforcement(DR) are particularly common; with thesetreatments, the reinforcer used is that whichwas found to maintain the problem behavior(E. G. Carr & Durand, 1985; Tiger, Hanley, &Bruzek, 2008). A similar trend is not evident inthe literature on the treatment of automaticallyreinforced problem behavior in general or forstereotypy in particular. Several practical andconceptual barriers have prohibited the adoptionof function-based DR procedures in the treat-ment of automatically reinforced stereotypy.Barriers that most likely hinder the adoption of

function-based treatments for automatically re-inforced problem behavior include the difficultyin disrupting the response–reinforcer relation andthe difficulty in controlling the delivery of oraccess to the automatic reinforcer for moresocially acceptable behavior. Despite these bar-riers, efficacious outcomes of function-basedinterventions have been reported (e.g., Hanley,Iwata, Thompson, & Lindberg, 2000; Lindberg,Iwata, & Kahng, 1999). Functionally equivalentalternative responding may be generated by usinga participant’s own automatically reinforced stereo-typy to strengthen a more desirable responsealternative (Charlop, Kurtz,&Casey, 1990;Hanleyet al., 2000; Hung, 1978; Wolery, Kirk, &Gast, 1985). For instance, Hung (1978) increasedappropriate speech in two children with an ASD byproviding tokens for appropriate speech that couldbe traded in for 2 min of access to their own motorstereotypy. Charlop et al. (1990) found thatproviding access to children’s own problembehaviorwas amore effective reinforcer than providing edibleitems when attempting to increase correct respond-ing on academic tasks.Because blocking or restricting access to

stereotypy and its direct reinforcers is a necessaryprerequisite to using stereotypy as reinforcement,Hanley et al. (2000) conducted a componentanalysis of the procedure to determine the effectsof blocking independent of the reinforcementcontingency for three individuals who engaged in

high levels of stereotypy. These authors firstprovided access to leisure activities, addedprompting to contact the leisure activities, thenrestricted access to stereotypy via responseblocking. They added DR of activity interactionwith brief access to the participants’ ownstereotypy only when treatment goals were notachieved with other components. Two of thethree participants did not require the DRcomponent to achieve low levels of stereotypyand high levels of item interaction, suggestingthat restricted access to stereotypy was the keycomponent of this treatment (these results aresimilar to those of Koegel, Firestone, Kramme, &Dunlap, 1974). However, the DR componentwas necessary for similar results to be realizedwith the third participant.The Hanley et al. (2000) study requires

replication for several reasons. First, the treatmentshows promise because it appears to be the onlyfunction-based treatment for automatically re-inforced stereotypy in which the maintainingreinforcer is also provided. Second, additionalreplications are necessary to determine thegeneral probability with which each of the fourcomponents of this intervention is necessary.Prior research has demonstrated the effects ofeach of the four components on stereotypy (e.g.,free access to activities, Roscoe, Iwata, &Goh, 1998; prompting leisure activity, Lindberget al., 1999; blocking stereotypy, Thompson,Iwata, Conners, & Roscoe, 1999; and DR,Charlop et al., 1990), but no studies haveevaluated the combinations of those interventionsor the importance of individual components in atreatment package. Third, the long-term effects ofthis intervention have not yet been evaluated;therefore, it is important to evaluate its longerterm impact. Hanley et al. did show thatstereotypy neither increased in baseline contextsfollowing intervention (indicative of a reboundeffect of treating stereotypy) nor decreased duringbaseline (indicative of desirable generalization).Nevertheless, both possible outcomes needfurther examination. Fourth, it is important to

408 JACQUELINE N. POTTER et al.

Page 3: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

evaluate a longer term application of thecontingency while the reinforcement require-ment is increased to determine if more complexleisure skills will develop.Themain purpose of this study was to replicate

the component analysis conducted by Hanleyet al. (2000) to determine if all components (e.g.,activities, prompting, blocking, and DR) of thetreatment package are necessary to increasefunctional leisure skills in adolescents with autism.The present study extends Hanley et al. by (a)generating more complex use of leisure items byprogressively increasing the quantitative andqualitative aspects of the response requirement toearn access to stereotypy, (b) using proceduresdescribed by Hanley (2010) to assess participants’preferences for the treatment package compared toother treatment components that have been usedfor reducing automatically reinforced stereotypy,and (c) assessing the social validity of the treatmentgoals, procedures, and effects with other relevantstakeholders.

GENERAL METHOD

Participants, Settings, and PreassessmentsThree boys who had been diagnosed with ASD

participated. Caregiver report and informalobservations suggested that none of themexhibited any independent leisure skills andthat each engaged in disruptive levels of motorstereotypy throughout the day across a variety ofactivities and settings despite the availability ofpreferred leisure items. Jon was 14 years old,Patrick was 16 years old, and Edward was 14 yearsold at the start of the study. Previouslyunsuccessful treatments included responserestriction and redirection (Ahearn, Clark,MacDonald,&Chung, 2007), response blockingalone, free access to stimuli (e.g., toys, music),and loss of stimuli (i.e., time-out) contingent onan occurrence of motor stereotypy. Because theirstereotypy occurred almost continuously andtherefore interfered with learning academicconcepts and daily life skills and precludedmeaningful social relationships, finding a treat-

ment that would decrease stereotypy and increasesome adaptive behavior was a priority.All sessions occurred in the participants’ school

classroom or vocational room. The work area wasapproximately 2 m by 2 m and was equippedwith a table and chair. Activity materials includedin the study were unavailable to the participantsoutside the session. All sessions were videotaped.Treatment component analysis sessions lasted10 min for Jon, and all other sessions across allparticipants and assessments lasted 5 min.Terminal-link duration during the treatmentpreference assessment was 2 min. Sessions wereconducted one to eight times per day, 3 to 5 daysper week with each participant.Informal interviews with staff and informal

direct observations suggested that the partic-ipants’ stereotypy produced its own reinforce-ment. A functional analysis was conducted basedon the procedures described by Iwata, Dorsey,Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982/1994). Theresults showed high rates of stereotypy during thealone conditions for all participants, suggestingthat stereotypy was maintained by automaticreinforcement and was not sensitive to socialevents as reinforcers. We also conducted acompeting items assessments (Derby et al.,1995; Piazza, Fisher, Hanley, Hilker, &Derby, 1996) to determine if effective leisureitems could be identified (i.e., those that wouldprovide a type of automatic reinforcement thatwould compete effectively with that which theparticipants derived from engaging in stereotypy).Both engagement with the items and stereotypyoccurred at consistently high levels across assesseditems for all participants. We selected three orfour items for subsequent analyses based on theirpotential to develop into more complex anddevelopmentally appropriate leisure skills.

Response Measurement and ReliabilityWe used a handheld computer to measure

motor stereotypy, simple engagement (SE),generic functional engagement (GFE), specificfunctional engagement (SFE), and selections

STEREOTYPY AS REINFORCEMENT 409

Page 4: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

made by the participant during preferenceassessments. We used duration recording for SEand report these data as a percentage. We usedfrequency recording for stereotypy, all forms offunctional engagement, prompts, and number ofaccess periods earned, and report these data asresponses or events per minute. We measuredstereotypy throughout each session, but collectedand reported separately on stereotypy thatoccurred during the session when the participantwas at the table and that which occurred duringperiods of access. We measured selections madeduring preference assessments and report thesedata as a number per session.All participants’ stereotypic topographies in-

cluded hand flapping or waving, finger tapping orflicking, and body rocking. Jon and Patrick alsoengaged in ear holding or twisting, spit play,jumping, body or object pressing, mouthing, andspinning objects in a nonfunctional manner.Patrick also positioned or contorted his hands infront of his face. We measured these behaviorsacross all analyses. We also defined and scoredeach behavior individually; a behavior ended anda new instance was scored if the behavior ceasedfor a minimum of 1 s or there was a minimum ofa 1-in. separation from the participant’s body forbehaviors such as mouthing, ear holding, and soon.After the introduction of the blocking proce-

dure, we scored and reported all blockedstereotypy attempts as stereotypy. We definedblocking as an interruption of a stereotyped act ora 1-s hands-down procedure contingent on theparticipant’s attempt to engage in stereotypy. Aninterruption consisted of the therapist movingher hands to disengage the participant’s stereoty-py (e.g., for mouthing or ear twisting) or placingher hands on the participant’s shoulders tointerrupt stereotypy (e.g., for body rocking orjumping). Hands down was defined as thetherapist physically guiding the participant’shands to lie flat on the table or immobilizingthe participant’s hands for approximately 1 s(e.g., for hand flapping or finger tapping). It was

possible that stereotypy could occur with onehand while the other was blocked, in that manytopographies of stereotypy occurred quickly. Forthis reason, we did not deduct blocking time fromoverall session time.Simple engagementwas defined as touching any

of the materials present during the session. If theparticipant engaged in stereotypy with or whileholding a toy, both stereotypy and SE wererecorded. Generic functional engagement wasdefined as manipulation of an activity as intendedor designed (with one or both hands; defineddistinctly for each set of materials). Specificfunctional engagement was defined as emission ofa particular response germane to each activity,which was determined prior to the analysis.During the analysis of procedures for generatingmore complex leisure engagement, prompted SFEwas defined as emission of a particular responseimmediately after or with any form of therapistassistance. Prompted SFE included the therapistproviding hand-over-hand assistance, light guid-ance, a point cue, verbal instruction, or any com-bination of the aforementioned prompts. (SeeSupporting Information online on Wiley OnlineLibrary for definitions of engagement manipu-lations for each activity included in the study.)Prompts were defined as (a) the therapist

manually guiding the participant to contact oneof the available activities or (b) a model, pointcue, or verbal prompt (e.g., “that one!”). Wedefined access as being removed at least 30 cmfrom the table for 30 s of no prompting and noblocking of stereotypy. All topographies ofstereotypy were allowed to occur during anaccess period. The therapist physically pulled orturned the participant’s chair away from the tableto make the access period clearly different fromthe rest of the session.A second observer independently collected

data using a handheld computer. We calculatedinterobserver agreement by dividing each sessioninto 10-s bins and comparing the two observers’scores for each bin, dividing the number ofagreements by the total number of agreements

410 JACQUELINE N. POTTER et al.

Page 5: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

plus disagreements, and then converting theresult to a percentage. A second observer collectedinterobserver agreement data for stereotypy, allforms of functional engagement, prompts, thenumber of access periods earned per session,stereotypy during access, selections, item posi-tioning during all preference assessments, andselection of the videos that were included in thesocial validity assessment with relevant stake-holders. Interobserver agreement data werecollected for a minimum of 20% of sessions ineach condition across all analyses for allparticipants; mean agreement was always above80%.

OverviewAfter functional analyses and competing items

assessments, a treatment component analysis wasconducted to determine intervention compo-nents necessary to increase functional playbehavior and to decrease stereotypy. The subse-quent analysis of procedures for generating morecomplex play focused on increasing the amountand complexity of specific forms of functionalengagement. We then conducted an assessmentto determine the clients’ preference for varioustreatments commonly used to treat stereotypy,and ended with a social validity assessment withother relevant stakeholders to assess the socialacceptability of the treatment goals, implementedprocedures, and the effects of treatment.

TREATMENT COMPONENT ANALYSIS

ProcedureThe treatment component analysis systemati-

cally added treatment components and used amultielement design in which sessions with thetreatment package were rapidly alternated withsessions in which there were no programmedcontingencies. Patrick experienced activities-onlysessions followed by sessions in which allintervention components were present. Theactivities-only condition was rapidly alternatedwith the treatment package sessions to determine

the effects of the additional components beyondaccess to the activities themselves.Activities only. This condition served as the

primary baseline. All activities identified via thecompeting items assessment (i.e., stamps and astamp pad, Interstars, linking rings, and stringingbeads for Jon; stringing beads, Megabloks, and ashape sorter for Patrick and Edward) were presenton the table and available to the participant eachsession. The therapist stood approximately 1 maway from the participant but provided noprogrammed consequences for any behavior.Activities plus prompting. The therapist

prompted (via manual guidance, a model, pointcue, or verbal cue) the participant to engage inGFE every 30 s. If the participant was alreadyengaging in GFE, the therapist waited until thenext 30-s interval.Activities plus prompting plus blocking. Proce-

dures were identical to those in the previouscondition, except the therapist stood behind theparticipant and interrupted stereotypy or deliv-ered a 1-s hands down, as described above in theresponse measurement section, contingent onattempts to engage in stereotypy.Activities plus prompting plus blocking plus DR

of GFE. In addition to activities, prompting, andblocking as described above, the therapistintroduced differential reinforcement of alterna-tive behavior (DRA) and differential reinforce-ment of other behavior (DRO). The therapistprovided 30-s periods of access to stereotypycontingent on the completion of a GFE responserequirement in the absence of stereotypy. Theinitial fixed-ratio (FR) schedule of reinforcementwas 1 and was increased to an FR 2 by the end ofthe assessment for Jon and Edward and to an FR10 for Patrick.

Results and DiscussionJon. Stereotypy persisted to the exclusion

of GFE during the activities-only baseline(Figure 1). The addition of prompts did notproduce a sufficient increase in GFE ordecrease in stereotypy; therefore, blocking was

STEREOTYPY AS REINFORCEMENT 411

Holly
Highlight
Holly
Highlight
Page 6: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

added as a treatment component. GFE initiallyoccurred on an increasing trend with blockingbut then decreased. Stereotypy was on a slightincreasing trend throughout the blocking condi-

tion (i.e., no reduction in stereotypy wasobserved).The persistent high level of stereotypy

observed after blocking was introduced could

Figure 1. The top panel depicts stereotypy, the second panel depicts simple engagement, the third panel depictsindependent and prompted generic functional engagement, and the bottom panel depicts prompts across activities-only andtreatment sessions of the treatment component analysis for Jon. The bracket denotes the increasing fixed-ratio (FR)requirement.

412 JACQUELINE N. POTTER et al.

Page 7: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

have been an effect of extinction (i.e., anextinction burst) and potentially would havedecreased over time. It also is possible thattherapists did not block all instances of stereoty-py, resulting in an intermittent schedule ofautomatic reinforcement and, thus, an increase instereotypy. As a final consideration, the persistentlevel of stereotypy could have been due to aparticularly strong motivation to obtain rein-forcement. As shown in the previous conditions,the reinforcement derived from engaging in theleisure materials did not effectively compete withthe reinforcement derived from engaging instereotypy. When we introduced blocking, Jonhad no opportunity to earn or obtain thestereotypy as reinforcement, which may havestrengthened the establishing operation for thatparticular event.This last explanation appears to be the

most plausible because stereotypy immediatelydecreased and remained low after the therapistintroduced the DR component for GFE. Inaddition, GFE steadily increased when we addedDR and thinned the schedule of reinforcement toFR 2. Prompts also decreased as GFE increased.This result shows that improvements in GFEwere not an exclusive function of prompting andalso that the DR contingency played a major rolein increasing GFE and decreasing stereotypy. Jonearned an average of 10 30-s access periods persession (range, 1 to 15), and stereotypy duringaccess averaged 7 responses per minute (range,0 to 14). The number of access periods earned persession gradually increased across the condition.Jon appropriately engaged with the materials inthe absence of stereotypy for about half of thetotal session duration, and the other half ofthe session was spent engaging in high rates ofstereotypy during the access periods. It could behypothesized that the opportunity to engage instereotypy during the access periods abolished, tosome extent, the automatic reinforcers gained viastereotypy.Increases in GFE and decreases in stereotypy

occurred only when all treatment components

were present. The reintroduction of the activities-only sessions verified that the DR contingenciesaccounted for the changes in responding becausestereotypy returned to baseline levels and GFEtypically remained at near zero when noprogrammed consequences were in place. Theactivities-only sessions conducted during theblocking and DR phases also showed that neithera rebound effect (i.e., subsequent increase instereotypy after successful treatment) nor desir-able generalization (low levels of stereotypy andhigh levels of GFE observed without therapistinvolvement) was observed, both of which areconsistent with results of Hanley et al. (2000).Patrick. During the activities-only condition,

Patrick engaged in moderate levels of GFE, butalso engaged in moderate and variable levels ofstereotypy (see Figure 2). The treatment packagewas associated with an initial decrease in GFE,which quickly recovered and continued on asteady overall increasing trend throughout thecondition. Patrick eventually emitted 10 GFEresponses to earn a single 30-s access period. Heearned an average of 5 access periods per session(range, 3 to 7), and stereotypy during accessaveraged 10 responses per minute (range, 3 to18). Similar to the pattern observed with Jon,these data highlight that Patrick independentlyengaged with the leisure materials withoutstereotypy for about half of the session andengaged in high rates of stereotypy during theearned access periods for the other half of thesession. Alternation of the activities-only sessionswith the treatment condition further showed thatthe treatment was necessary to maintain lowlevels of stereotypy while increasing GFE.Edward. During the activities-only condition,

stereotypy occurred between 0.2 and 3.0responses per minute and GFE occurred onlyduring the first two sessions (Figure 3). With theintroduction of prompting, the level of stereotypydid not change significantly, but GFE didincrease slightly. When blocking was introduced,stereotypy increased and GFE decreased. Theincreasing trend in stereotypy was similar to that

STEREOTYPY AS REINFORCEMENT 413

Page 8: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

observed for Jon. Once again, it appeared thatrestricting access to stereotypy through responseblocking increased Edward’s motivation toobtain the reinforcement produced by engagingin stereotypy. With the addition of the DRcomponent, stereotypy immediately decreased tonear zero and remained low throughout thecondition. (Edward also engaged in stereotypyduring the majority of access periods earned,although the video footage was lost for thetreatment component analysis sessions. Evidence

of his rates of stereotypy during access will beavailable in the subsequent analysis.) GFEincreased to a level much higher than in previousconditions. The FR requirement to earn an accessperiod was increased from FR 1 to FR 2 by theend of the condition. The reintroduction ofactivities-only sessions further confirmed that theDR component was necessary to achieve lowlevels of stereotypy and increases in GFE.The results of the treatment analyses with all

three participants support the findings of priorstudies. First, numerous studies have demon-strated that the mere presence of toys is usuallynot an effective treatment for automaticallyreinforced behavior and that additional treatmentcomponents are necessary to decrease problembehavior and increase more desirable responses(e.g., J. E. Carr, Dozier, Patel, Adams, &Martin, 2002; Koegel et al., 1974; Lindberget al., 1999; Tarbox, Wallace, & Tarbox,2002). Provision of free access to activitieswas not an effective treatment for any of theparticipants.If the presence of leisure materials does not

result in decreases in problem behavior andincreases in functional engagement, prior re-search has suggested that the addition of promptsto engage with the toys may facilitate behaviorchange (e.g., Britton, Carr, Landaburu, &Romick, 2002; Lindberg et al., 1999). However,the addition of prompts was insufficient fordesirable behavior changes for any participants,a finding similar to that of Hanley et al.(2000).Response blocking decreased problem behav-

ior and increased alternative behavior for twoof the three participants in Hanley et al. (2000).J. E. Carr et al. (2002) also observed thatnoncontingent reinforcement and responseblocking alone were both ineffective in decreasingautomatically reinforced object mouthing, butwhen combined, acceptable levels were quicklyachieved (see also Tarbox et al., 2002). Theaddition of response blocking in the presentanalysis did not, however, decrease stereotypy. In

Figure 2. The top panel depicts stereotypy, the secondpanel depicts simple engagement, the third panel depictsindependent and prompted generic functional engagement,and the bottom panel depicts prompts across activities-onlyand treatment sessions of the treatment component analysisfor Patrick. The bracket denotes the increasing fixed-ratio(FR) requirement.

414 JACQUELINE N. POTTER et al.

Page 9: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

fact, this component actually resulted in increasesfor two participants. Although inconsistent withthe results of J. E. Carr et al. and Tarbox et al.,other studies also have demonstrated unsatisfac-tory effects of response blocking (e.g., Hanley

et al.; see DeLeon et al., 2011, for furtherdiscussion). It is important to note, however, thatalthough response blocking was not effective onits own, it was a necessary component of thecurrent treatment package because access to the

Figure 3. The top panel depicts stereotypy, the second panel depicts simple engagement, the third panel depictsindependent and prompted generic functional engagement, and the bottom panel depicts prompts across activities-only andtreatment sessions of the treatment component analysis for Edward. The bracket denotes the increasing fixed-ratio (FR)requirement.

STEREOTYPY AS REINFORCEMENT 415

Page 10: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

automatic reinforcer had to be restricted viablocking before it could function as a reinforcerfor engagement.Lang et al. (2010) provided additional support

for restricting access to stereotypy and providingit at certain points during treatment. Duringtheir treatment sessions, play was prompted andinstances of stereotypy were redirected to play.Prior to some sessions, participants were allowedto engage in stereotypy for a brief period. Resultsshowed less stereotypy during treatment sessionsthat followed the period of brief access tostereotypy. The authors hypothesized that accessto stereotypy before sessions had an abolishingeffect and thus resulted in lower levels ofstereotypy during the subsequent treatmentsessions. Perhaps in the present analysis, restric-tion of access to stereotypy during the fulltreatment sessions (through response blocking)created a sufficient state of deprivation, increasingthe value of the automatic reinforcer and thestereotypy that produced it (e.g., Lang et al.). It isplausible that intermittent access to automaticreinforcers during the full treatment reduced thevalue of the automatic reinforcers during theteaching portion of the sessions and would helpexplain why stereotypy abated and responseblocking was more effective under theseconditions.The present results highlight the importance of

manipulating antecedent and consequent eventsfor both the stereotypy and the alternativebehavior. The antecedent manipulation forstereotypy was the presence of activities, andthe consequent manipulation was responseblocking. The antecedent manipulation for thealternative behavior (play) was prompting by thetherapist, and the consequent manipulation wasDR using access to stereotypy as the reinforcer.Decreases in problem behavior and increases inalternative behavior were observed only when allevent manipulations were present. The efficacy ofthis treatment package also illustrates that it ispossible and often necessary to implement afunction-based and reinforcement-based treat-

ment for automatically reinforced problembehavior.

GENERATING MORE COMPLEX LEISURE SKILLS

ProcedureBefore this analysis, Jon and Edward had

experienced analyses of different contingencyarrangements and reinforcement schedule thin-ning. The procedures that resulted in slightlybetter outcomes with Jon included a DRA plusDRO schedule of reinforcement, access periodswithout materials, and response marking (i.e., a“thumbs up” paired with a monotone “good” or“yes” for each instance of SFE). For Edward,DRA only, access periods withmaterials available,and no response marking resulted in optimaleffects. We then incorporated these most effectivecontingency arrangements into the shapingprocedure for more complex SFE. We conductedbaseline sessions with activity materials present todetermine whether SFE would eventually occurin the absence of the treatment, but we did notincorporate any programmed prompting orconsequences. The treatment for Jon includedmaterials from the Interstars activity, promptingof SFE, blocking of stereotypy, response markingfor each SFE response until he earned access, anda DRA plus DRO schedule of reinforcement. Inthe DR component, Jon had to meet the SFEresponse requirement with no occurrence ofstereotypy. The treatment for Edward includedaccess to Megabloks, prompting SFE, blockingstereotypy, no response marking, and a DRA thatinvolved access periods with materials present.Sessions were conducted in blocks of four, withthree treatment sessions followed by a baselinesession.Jon. Jon’s terminal goal included building two

cubes with the Interstars activity. Materialsincluded eight i-bars and four t-bars, and theterminal goal required 10 responses. The SFErequirement began with Jon connecting one i-barvertically on any corner of a t-bar to earn access.The GFE responses no longer resulted in access

416 JACQUELINE N. POTTER et al.

Page 11: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

and were ignored. Both prompted and indepen-dent responses could fulfill the response require-ment for the schedule.Prompt fading occurred in each session

dependent on the levels of SFE and stereotypy.The fading procedure was similar to a most-to-least model in which the most intrusive prompts(e.g., hand-over-hand guidance) were providedinitially and were faded to less intrusive prompts(e.g., point cue). If Jon initiated a response, thetherapist allowed 3 to 5 s for independentresponding to occur. The criteria to increasethe response requirement consisted of fournonconsecutive sessions with stereotypy lowerthan 1.6 responses per minute, SFE above 3.0responses per minute, and a maximum of oneprompt per session. The planned steps inthinning the schedule were an increase by oneresponse through FR 5 (i.e., one cube con-structed), and then an FR 10 (i.e., two cubesconstructed) was arranged.Edward. The SFE targeted with Edward

included building two Megabloks houses com-prised of six responses each for a total of 12responses. The FR requirement began at an FR 1with a planned increase by one response throughFR 6 (i.e., one house constructed), then an FR 12(i.e., two houses) was arranged. We implementedmost-to-least prompting and faded in the sessionas described for Jon. The criteria to increase theFR requirement by one response included fournonconsecutive sessions with stereotypy below1.0 responses per minute, SFE above 1.3responses per minute, and a maximum of oneprompt per session.

Results and DiscussionJon. Stereotypy occurred more often than SFE

during the first four sessions (Figure 4), but itquickly decreased while independent SFE in-creased and remained high throughout theanalysis. Prompted SFE remained low through-out the analysis, and any increases typicallycoincided with increases in stereotypy. Jon earneda total of 402 access periods across 97 treatment

sessions, averaging four access periods per session(range, 0 to 9) while stereotypy averaged 6responses per minute (range, 0 to 15) and theterminal response requirement of FR 12 was met.The baseline sessions showed much higher andvariable levels of stereotypy and zero instances ofSFE throughout the analysis. These baselinesessions further validate that the treatmentpackage was necessary to decrease Jon’s stereotypyand increase more complex and specific forms offunctional engagement.Edward. The introduction of treatment was

associated with an increasing trend in SFE(Figure 5); responding then decreased andstabilized as the schedule was thinned. Stereotypyremained low and steady throughout the analysis,occurring in a little more than half of thetreatment sessions. Edward earned a total of 267access periods across 64 treatment sessions, withan average of four access periods per session(range, 0 to 8). The rate of stereotypy duringaccess periods averaged 3 responses per minute(range, 0 to 10). Edward met the terminalresponse goal of building two Megabloks houses.During baseline sessions, stereotypy occurredbetween 0.2 and 5.6 responses per minute, withthe majority of sessions around 3 responses perminute. Unlike Jon, Edward engaged in SFEduring some of the baseline sessions, suggestive ofgeneralization of leisure skills.For these two participants, any automatic

reinforcement produced by engaging in theactivity was not more valuable than thatproduced by engagement in stereotypy. Thelack of consistent SFE during ongoing baselinesessions (i.e., activities available but noprogrammed contingencies) suggests that thetreatment package was necessary to decreasestereotypy and increase functional engagement,but the reinforcers for the more complexalternative behavior did not effectively substitutefor or compete with those provided via stereotypy.Although the activities were not age appropriatefor either participant, the leisure repertoires weremore complex by the end of the analyses.

STEREOTYPY AS REINFORCEMENT 417

Page 12: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

SOCIAL VALIDITY ASSESSMENT

Procedure

Social validation of the treatment procedureswas assessed via a treatment preference assess-ment with the three participants using aconcurrent-chains procedure (Hanley, 2010). Italso was assessed with other stakeholders byarranging for them to watch brief videos ofbaseline and treatment and then respond to aquestionnaire.During the treatment preference assessment, a

long table was divided into three equal sectionsand marked with blue tape (with the exception ofPatrick; see description below). We placed a chairin the middle of each section with a colored cardon the back of each chair to represent the initiallink. We selected three colors for each participant

based on the results of a paired-stimulus colorpreference assessment (Luczynski &Hanley, 2009). The colors selected for inclusionwere neither highest nor least preferred. Thetherapist presented the color cards, said “touchthe color card of the table you’d like to sit at,” andallowed the participant to select one of thecolored cards, sit down at the table, andexperience the associated terminal link.No differential consequences. Before assigning

each color a differential consequence in the formof different treatments, we conducted sessions inwhich all selections resulted in delivery of thesame small edible item. We did this to determineif a color bias was evident.Differential consequences. After undifferentiat-

ed responding occurred, we randomly assignedeach color to a treatment. The first treatment

Figure 4. Stereotypy, independent SFE, and prompted SFE during treatment (top) and baseline sessions (bottom) forJon. The gray bars, plotted on the secondary y axis (top), depict the increasing response requirement for SFE (specificfunctional engagement).

418 JACQUELINE N. POTTER et al.

Page 13: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

included enriching the environment with activi-ties. The second included physically blockinginstances of stereotypy with no activities present.The third included activities, prompting func-tional engagement every 30 s if the participantwas not already engaged, blocking stereotypy, andproviding 30-s access to the participant’s ownstereotypy for appropriately engaging with theactivities (i.e., the full treatment). We includedthese three treatments because they are common-ly used for treating motor stereotypy and becausethe participants had previous experience witheach of them.We conducted six trials per session and

randomly assigned and rotated the position ofthe colored cards across sessions but not trials.The colored cards on the backs of the chairs alsohad photos representing the treatment that wouldbe experienced during the terminal link. Weconducted Patrick’s sessions in three similarlysized research rooms at his school. We placed thecolored cards with photos on the frame of each

doorway. When Patrick made a selection (i.e.,touched the picture), he entered the room, sat atthe table, and experienced the terminal-linktreatment associated with that color.Before each session, the therapist briefly

described the experience for each terminal linkwhile she pointed to the corresponding chair. Weused both concurrent-chains and reversal designsto evaluate the control of initial-link respondingby the contexts arranged in the terminal links.A second social validity assessment was

conducted with stakeholders relevant to Jonand Edward. We invited two teachers whoworked with the participants on a daily basis,clinical case supervisors, and executive clinicalsupervisors to participate. After gaining writtenconsent, we invited the stakeholders to watch a2- to 3-min video that showed a clip duringbaseline in which no contingencies were presentand a clip during treatment with DR. Werandomly selected the video clips using MicrosoftExcel. After viewing the video, the assessor filled

Figure 5. Stereotypy, independent SFE, and prompted SFE during treatment (top) and baseline sessions (bottom) forEdward. The gray bars, plotted on the secondary y axis (top), depict the increasing response requirement for SFE (specificfunctional engagement).

STEREOTYPY AS REINFORCEMENT 419

Page 14: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

out a survey that included questions with a7-point scale about the acceptability of thetreatment followed by an open-ended question.

Results and DiscussionWe observed undifferentiated responding for

all participants when no differential consequen-ces occurred (Figure 6), indicating no strong colorbiases. When differential consequences wereprovided (i.e., different treatments) for selections,Jon selected the activities-only treatment during45% of opportunities and the treatment with DRon 47% of opportunities, showing that hepreferred these treatments similarly over theblocking-only treatment. Patrick and Edwardshowed preference for the treatment with DR(selected 58% and 75%, respectively) over boththe activities-only and the blocking-only treat-ments. Selections were undifferentiated duringthe reversal to no differential consequences,providing additional evidence that selections inthe previous condition (i.e., differential conse-quences) were influenced by the treatments.All participants responded away from the

blocking-only treatment, and two of the threepreferred the treatment with DR. They preferreda context with a requirement to behave in acertain manner to earn access to their stereotypyover a context in which stereotypy was freelyavailable. Although it is not possible to specify theamount of reinforcement obtained by eachparticipant, it seems reasonable to infer thatmore automatic reinforcers were available duringthe activities-only sessions because stereotypy washigher during the activities-only sessions com-pared to stereotypy during access periods of thetreatment sessions. Therefore, amount of rein-forcement earned probably did not drive theobserved selections.An alternative explanation for the observed

preferences is that the intermittent responseblocking during treatment created a state ofdeprivation for the automatic reinforcers associ-ated with stereotypy. Therefore, the initial linkassociated with the treatment may have devel-

oped properties of a conditioned establishingoperation (McGill, 1999). In this explanation, itcould be assumed that the automatic reinforce-ment derived from engaging in stereotypy wouldbe more reinforcing in the preferred treatmentthan when stereotypy was available for free.Another explanation relates to the differencebetween the two treatments with regard to thepresence or absence of a programmed contingency.The present results may provide more evidence ofpreference for contingent over noncontingentreinforcement. Numerous studies have shownthat individuals prefer contingent reinforcementover noncontingent reinforcement when thereinforcement is tangible or socially mediated(Hanley, 2010; Luczynski & Hanley, 2009;Singh & Query, 1971). The present results areunique in that the apparent preference forcontingency involved both mediated and auto-matic reinforcement.Another important finding was that no one

preferred the blocking-only treatment. Blockingonly was the least effective arrangement forreducing stereotypy, so perhaps the participantspreferred the context in which treatment wasmost effective and avoided the least effectivetreatment. Regardless of the controlling variables,these results highlight the importance of includ-ing a reinforcement component (e.g., DR) whenconsidering the use of a likely punishmentcomponent such as blocking.Results of the social validity assessment with

stakeholders showed that the stakeholders agreedthat the treatment goals, procedures, and amountof behavior change were acceptable, appropriate,and important for both participants (see Table 1).

GENERAL DISCUSSION

The current results demonstrated that thestereotypy of three participants could be used as areinforcer for a more desirable alternativebehavior. We replicated results of Hanley et al.(2000) in that a combination of activities,prompting engagement, restriction of the

420 JACQUELINE N. POTTER et al.

Holly
Highlight
Holly
Highlight
Page 15: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

Figure 6. Cumulative number of selections for treatment conditions when differential consequences and no differentialconsequences were provided for selections for Jon, Patrick, and Edward.

STEREOTYPY AS REINFORCEMENT 421

Page 16: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

automatic reinforcer during teaching periods, andcontingent access to that reinforcer was aneffective treatment for automatically reinforcedstereotypy. In addition, we extended the resultswith two participants by demonstrating that theamount and complexity of an alternative behaviorcould be shaped in the context of this treatment.Two participants also preferred the treatmentpackage over commonly used components ofthe treatment, and stakeholders found the goals,procedures, and results highly acceptable despitethe procedural complexity. In essence, wedemonstrated the effectiveness of and preferencefor a function- and reinforcement-based treat-ment in decreasing stereotypy and increasing theindependent leisure skills of three adolescentswith an ASD.Three important logistical considerations for

this treatment include the necessity of carefulplanning before implementation, the laboriousnature of the procedures, and the extensive timeperiod necessary to observe changes in behavior.Hundreds of treatment sessions were required todevelop the level of complexity of leisure skillsobserved at the end of the evaluation, and theseskills were still not at a level or with items that areultimately suitable for adolescents. These consid-erations highlight that an extensive amount ofwork and time is required to change a repertoireof automatically reinforced stereotypy. Theyalso highlight the importance of targeting the

development of automatically and sociallyreinforced leisure skills early, when an individualis young, so that age- and developmentallyappropriate play are less disparate at the start oftreatment and intractable levels of stereotypy canbe prevented.

REFERENCES

Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., MacDonald, R. P. F., &Chung, B. I. (2007). Assessing and treating vocalstereotypy in children with autism. Journal of AppliedBehavior Analysis, 40, 263–275. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2007.30-06

Britton, L. N., Carr, J. E., Landaburu, H. J., & Romick, K.S. (2002). The efficacy of noncontingent reinforcementas treatment for automatically reinforced stereotypy.Behavioral Interventions, 17, 93–103. doi: 10.1002/bin.110

Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behaviorproblems through functional communication training.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126. doi:10.1901/jaba.1985.18-111

Carr, J. E., Dozier, C. L., Patel, M. R., Adams, A. N., &Martin, N. (2002). Treatment of automaticallyreinforced object mouthing with noncontingent rein-forcement and response blocking: Experimental analy-sis and social validation. Research in DevelopmentalDisabilities, 23, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/S0891-4222(01)00090-7

Charlop, M. H., Kurtz, P. F., & Casey, F. G. (1990). Usingaberrant behaviors as reinforcers for autistic children.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 163–181. doi:10.1901/jaba.1990.23-163

DeLeon, I. G., Allman, M. J., Richman, D., Triggs, M.,Frank, M., Carreau, A., & Vartoomian, S. (2011).Functional analysis of behavioral disturbance related to

Table 1Questions and Results of the Social Validity Questionnaire

Questions

Responses

M (range)

Jon Edward

1. Do you think that the treatment that involved prompting engagement or playbehavior, blocking stereotypy, and differentially reinforcing engagement with 30-saccess to the participant’s own stereotypy was acceptable?

7 6.6 (6–7)

2. Do you think that the amount of behavior change (i.e., the effects of treatment) wasacceptable or sufficient?

6.6 (6–7) 6.8 (6–7)

3. Do you feel that the overall goals of this treatment were acceptable, appropriate, andimportant for the individual?

6.6 (5–7) 7

4. Would you recommend this treatment package to other therapists or providers whoare attempting to decrease motor stereotypy and increase age-appropriate play skills?

6.8 (6–7) 6.8 (6–7)

422 JACQUELINE N. POTTER et al.

Page 17: TREATING STEREOTYPY IN ADOLESCENTS DIAGNOSED WITH … · 23/04/2012  · in the study were unavailable to the participants outside the session. All sessions were videotaped. Treatment

autistic symptomatology. In E. Mayville & J. Mulick(Eds.), Behavioral foundations of autism intervention(pp. 111–135). New York, NY: Sloan.

Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Andelman, M., Berg, W.,Drew, J., Prouty, A., & Laffey, P. (1995). Two measuresof preference during forced-choice assessments. Journalof Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 345–346. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1995.28-345

Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., Jefferson,G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional communicationtraining with and without extinction and punishment.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 23–36. doi:10.1901/jaba.1993.26-23

Goldman, S., Cuiling, W., Salgado, M. W., Greene, P. E.,Kim, M., & Rapin, I. (2008). Motor stereotypies inchildren with autism and other developmental disor-ders. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 51,30–38. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03178.x

Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W.W., Sullivan, M. T., Acquisto, J.,& LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness of functionalcommunication training with and without extinctionand punishment: A summary of 21 inpatient cases.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 211–235. doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.31-211

Hanley, G. P. (2010). Toward effective and preferredprogramming: A case for objective measurement ofsocial validity with recipients of behavior-changeprograms. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 3, 13–21.

Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., Thompson, R. H., & Lindberg,J. S. (2000). A component analysis of “stereotypyas reinforcement” for alternative behavior. Journal ofApplied Behavior Analysis, 33, 285–297. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2000.33-285

Hung, D. W. (1978). Using self-stimulation as reinforce-ment for autistic children. Journal of Autism andChildhood Schizophrenia, 8, 355–366. doi: 10.1007/BF01539637

Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., &Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis ofself-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27,197–209. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1994.27-197 (Reprintedfrom Analysis and Intervention in DevelopmentalDisabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982)

Koegel, R. L., Firestone, P. B., Kramme, K. W., & Dunlap,G. (1974). Increasing spontaneous play by suppressingself-stimulation in autistic children. Journal of AppliedBehavior Analysis, 7, 521–528. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1974.7-521

Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Machalicek, W., Rispoli,M., Lancioni, G., & Fragale, C. (2010). The effects ofan abolishing operation intervention component onplay skills, challenging behavior, and stereotypy.Behavior Modification, 34, 267–289. doi: 10.1177/0145445510370713

Lindberg, J. S., Iwata, B. A., & Kahng, S. (1999). On therelation between object manipulation and stereotypicself-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied BehaviorAnalysis, 32, 51–62. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1999.32-51

Luczynski, K. C., & Hanley, G. P. (2009). Do childrenprefer contingencies? An evaluation of the efficacy ofand preference for contingent versus noncontingentsocial reinforcement during play. Journal of AppliedBehavior Analysis, 42, 511–525. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-511

McGill, P. (1999). Establishing operations: Implications forthe assessment, treatment, and prevention of problembehavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32,393–418. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1999.32-393

Pelios, L., Morren, J., Tesch, D., & Axelrod, S. (1999). Theimpact of functional analysis methodology on treat-ment choice for self-injurious and aggressive behavior.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 185–195. doi:10.1901/jaba.1999.32-185

Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., Hilker, K., &Derby, K. M. (1996). A preliminary procedure forpredicting the positive and negative effects of reinforce-ment-based procedures. Journal of Applied BehaviorAnalysis, 29, 136–146. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1996.29-137

Roscoe, E. M., Iwata, B. A., & Goh, H. L. (1998). Acomparison of noncontingent reinforcement andsensory extinction as treatments for self-injuriousbehavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31,635–646. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1998.31-635

Singer, H. S. (2009). Motor stereotypies. Seminars inPediatric Neurology, 16, 77–81. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2009.03.008

Singh, D., & Query, W. T. (1971). Preference for work over“freeloading” in children. Psychonomic Science, 24,77–79.

Tarbox, J., Wallace, M. D., & Tarbox, R. S. F. (2002).Successful generalized parent training and failedschedule thinning of response blocking for automati-cally maintained object mouthing. Behavioral Inter-ventions, 17, 169–178. doi: 10.1002/bin.116

Thompson, R. H., Iwata, B. A., Conners, J., & Roscoe, E.M. (1999). Effects of reinforcement for alternativebehavior during punishment of self-injury. Journal ofApplied Behavior Analysis, 32, 317–328. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1999.32-317

Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functionalcommunication training: A review and practical guide.Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 16–23.

Vollmer, T. R. (1994). The concept of automatic reinforce-ment: Implications for behavioral research in develop-mental disabilities. Research in DevelopmentalDisabilities, 15, 187–207. doi: 10.1016/0891-4222(94)90011-6

Wolery, M., Kirk, K., & Gast, D. L. (1985). Stereotypicbehavior as a reinforcer: Effects and side-effects. Journalof Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15, 149–161.doi: 10.1007/BF01531601

Received April 23, 2012Final acceptance November 4, 2012Action Editor, Linda LeBlanc

STEREOTYPY AS REINFORCEMENT 423