48
University of Richmond UR Scholarship Repository Master's eses Student Research 1969 e use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate reading problems among retarded children James Over Rust Follow this and additional works at: hp://scholarship.richmond.edu/masters-theses Part of the Psychology Commons is esis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Research at UR Scholarship Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's eses by an authorized administrator of UR Scholarship Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Rust, James Over, "e use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate reading problems among retarded children" (1969). Master's eses. Paper 918.

The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

University of RichmondUR Scholarship Repository

Master's Theses Student Research

1969

The use of the Slingerland screening tests todelineate reading problems among retardedchildrenJames Over Rust

Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarship.richmond.edu/masters-theses

Part of the Psychology Commons

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Research at UR Scholarship Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion inMaster's Theses by an authorized administrator of UR Scholarship Repository. For more information, please [email protected].

Recommended CitationRust, James Over, "The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate reading problems among retarded children" (1969). Master'sTheses. Paper 918.

Page 2: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

THE USE OF THE SLINGERLAND SCREENII'«J TESTS

TO DELINEATE READING PROBLEMS AMONG

RETARDED CHILDREN

~y

JAMES. OVER RUST

A THESIS

SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL

OF THE UNIVERSITY OF RICHMOND

IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE

DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS

AUGUST, 1969

Page 3: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

THE USE OF THE SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS

TO DELINEATE READING PROBLEMS AMONG

RETARDED CHILDREN

Approved:

Examining Committee

/ii~~

~~~

Page 4: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would particularly like to thank Mrs. Jean N. Dickinson,

my Committee Chairman, for her enthusiastic support and assis­

tance throughout the research. I would also like to thank

Dr. William H. Leftwich and Dr. Kenneth A. Blick for serving

on my Committee and for recommendations concerning statistics

and experimental design. Mr. Peter Bahler (Director of Com­

puter Center, University of Richmond) was most helpful in the

analysis of the data. Finally, I thank Miss Nancy Thomas

(Supervisor of Special Education, Chesterfield, Virginia) for

making the testing possible by arranging for subjects.

Page 5: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

TABLE OF CONTENTS

I INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1

I I METHOD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10

I II RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15

IV DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28

V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . 33

REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35

APPENDIX I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40

VITA . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41

Page 6: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

LIST OF TABLES

TABLE

I CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READI­

NESS TESTS AND SCORES FROM SLINGERLAND SCREENING

TESTS . • . . . • . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16

II CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READI-

NESS TESTS AND WISC I.Q. SCORES . • • • • 1 7

III MULTIPLE CORRELATION CDEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLI­

TAN READINESS TESTS AND SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS

AND WISC TESTS •.

IV SUMMARY TABLE FOR TESTING SIGNIFICANCES OF PARTIAL

• 18

REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS . • • • • 20

V CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READI­

NESS TESTS AND SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS WHEN Ss

ARE DIVIDED INTO LOWER AND HIGHER I.Q. GROUPS •..• 22

VI CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READI­

NESS TESTS AND SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS WHEN Ss

ARE DIVIDED BY RACE. . . .

VII CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READI­

NESS TESTS AND SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS \\/HEN Ss

23

ARE DlVIDED BY AGE. . • . . . . . . . • . . . • . . . 25

VI II CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BET\\IEEN METROPOLITAN READJ­

NE&<.) TESTS AND SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS \\/HEN S~

ARE DIVIDED BY SEX ..••...•..•...•

Page 7: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

LIST OF FIGURES

-I AVERAGE CORRELATIONS {r) BETWEEN METROPOLITAN AND

SLINGERLAND TESTS WHEN THE DATA HAS BEEN DIVIDED IN

TERMS OF SEX, RACE, I.Q., AND AGE .•••..•. • • 27

Page 8: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

CHAPTER I

Introduction

Perception of lan9ua9c stimuli throu9h thQ auditory­

visual-kincsthctic sensory channels is the first step in the

acquisition of lan9ua9c (Slin90rland, 1968). The ir:iportnnce

of perception in learning to use lan9ua9c has tx.>cn considered

in a variety of ways. Jt has tx?cn considered in terns of

visual discrimination (~nton, 1962), auditory-visual intc9ration

(Birch and 8'?1nont, 1965), pc.?rceptual span (Vernon, 1957), nnd

constitutionally dctcr=ined dcvclopr::icntal deviations (Orton,

1937; Clcmncns, 1964). These invcsti9ators a9rco that 1,•arn-

i n9 is cmoplcx (or any child, and when the word for=s Arc

obscured by perceptual deficits the proble=• nr~ t'lA9ni!ied.

No unique theoretical explanation i~ necessary to relate per­

ception and rcadin9 in a retarded population brcau~~ in a

qualitative sense the retarded and nora.al child dC'v~lop sia::ii -

larly (BruecknC"r and Bond, lQSS).

Experi~cntcrs who atteept to qu..\ntify thr sprcific rela­

tionship between perc'-•ption and rC?adin9 hnvv difficult~· n~achinc;

a9recl!lent. Lovell, "1'litc, and Wlitcl)' (l<J<>5) natch<'d !i>ixty

pairs of retarded subjects on a9e, sex, intclli9cnc~, •ocial

status, and school. One of each pair was a poor reader whil~

the other was a better reader. Readin9 ability was a•se&~ed

usin9 the Vernon 9rade!d •r1rd r~3din9 h.•i.l. The tx•\ h.•r rC!actc·r~

Page 9: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

2

scored significantly higher on the author's map following tests

of visual perception, spatial orientation, and sentence copy­

ing. Coleman (1968) found that 49.5 percent of 87 disabled

readers in grades one to six had severe visual-perceptual

problems. His research tested for visual acuity, hand-eye-foot

dominance, refractive error, writing ability, number sequences,

visual memory, spatial orientation, and balance. Stroud (1945)

correlated Chapman-Cook rate of reading scores and rate of

visual perception using 570 fourth, fifth, and sixth graders.

His tests of visual perception included six parts: word selec­

tion, digit selection, letter selection, paired drawing, paired

digits, and paired letters. He reported significant reading­

perception correlations.

On the other hand, Birch and Belmont {1965) used 220 five

to twelve year olds and could only identify a significant

perceptual-reading relationship for first and second grade

children. Their tools included the Metropolitan Readiness Tests

for the first grade and the Stanford Achievement Tests for the

older children. These scores were correlated with scores on a

test of visual-auditory integration. The test consisted of the

experimenter tapping out rhythms on a desk and the Ss selecting

which of three dot sequences best represented the rhythm.

Belmont and Birch (1966) used intelligence scores to match

nine and ten year old weak readers with adequate readers.

Page 10: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

3

Reading ability was determined by three reading tests and a

word discrimination test. The groups were then compared in

terms of WISC subtest scores. The performance/verbal ratios

from the WISC were significantly higher for the weak readers

than for the adequate readers. This difference led the authors

to conclude that reading differences were due to language skills

and not to perceptual or manipulative skills. In drawing this

conclusion the authors assume that perceptual skill does not

participate in the development of language skills. They also

put the WISC in the dubious position of being able to differen­

tiate language and perceptual abilities.

Kendall (1948} found no relationship between visual motor

integration and reading retardation. He used 118 children ages

six to sixteen. He measured perceptual integration with the

Memory for Design Test and used the Jenkins Oral Reading Test

to determine reading ability.

No two experimenters used the same tests; therefore, there

is no real contradiction in the studies presented. However,

in interpreting the results in terms of the role of perception

in the learning of reading, a controversy does exist. This con­

troversy is partially resolved when consideration is given to

the critical period of perceptual development, the ages four to

seven (Frostig and Maslow, 1968}. The tendency to see letters

and words reversed is a particularly common perceptual weakness

Page 11: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

4

for four to seven year old children (Davidson, 1967; Gilkey and

Parr, 1944; Ilg and Aines, 1950). The fact that in normal readers

this problem often appears and then clears up naturally, reflects

the developmental course of perception (Benton, 1962). It fol­

lows that in experiments where subjects' average age is in the

crucial developmental period, one would expect to find a more

profound perceptual-reading relationship than in experiments

with older Ss. The greater relationship is expected because

more perceptual confusion naturally exists with the younger

Ss. The fact that Birch and Belmont (1965) were able to find

a perceptual-reading relationship for their younger Ss but not

for their older Ss is an example of this point.

The retarded youngster will develop reading skill in much

the same manner as his normal peer. The difference is that

the retarded child develops much slower (Hutt and Gibby, 1965;

Johnson, 1963; Kephart, 1960). Therefore, what was a temporary

maturational lag in the normal child appears to last longer

and is more pronounced in the slow child.

In order to remediate language weakness it is essential

that consideration be given to perceptual abilities (Slinger­

land, 1968). When the emphasis is on improving perception,

the program should include attention to language development

(Frostig and Maslow, 1968). A number of remedial techniques

Page 12: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

have been developed to teach perceptually deficient readers.

Although these were constructed for populations of normal

intelligence, there is no reason to believe that they could

not be adopted for retarded children (Mazurkiewicz, 1967).

In addressing this point Johnson (1963) notes that a highly

organized reading system is beneficial to slow learners.

Johnson (1967) describes tracing and kinesthetic tech­

niques which bring visual-auditory and kinesthetic-tactile

stimulation into the reading experience. The process begins

through mastery of the student's language, words, and ideas.

Words are learned as the child expresses a need for them.

Words are examined in syllable parts and as a whole. They are

spoken, traced, and written, but never copied.

The programs, Words in Color (Gattegno, 1963) and Color

Phonics (Bannatyne, 1967), share the common aim of reducing

linquistic confusion by adding a color dimension. Consistent

phoneme-color relationships are established so that an indivi­

dual can figure out how a word sounds from how it looks.

The Initial Teaching Alphabet (i/t/a) was developed to

provide the beginning reader with a system of one symbol-one

sound relationships (Mazurkiewicz, 1967). Forty-four distinct

symbols are used. Each represents a unique sound. Reading

becomes purely a decoding process while writing is purely

encoding.

5

Page 13: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

Frostig and Horne (1964) designed a program for the

development of visual perception. The program includes a wide

range of exercises from knee bends to imagining spacial rela­

tionships. This system has also been reported as a ~upplement

to a reading program based upon the Illinois Test of Psycho­

linguistic Ability (Frostig and Maslow, 1968). When reading

instruction begins, Frostig (1965) recommends the use of

phonics to aid in the association between the sound and the

word.

The Orton (1964) and Gillingham-Stillman {1960) tech­

niques insist that the student be kept constantly aware of how

a letter or word looks, sounds, and feels. Letters and then

blendings are taught with emphasis on phonetic word building

{Orton, 1967).

Kirk (1940) states that remedial help is essential for

retarded children with reading weakness, but he points out

that these children should not be burdened with numerous pho­

netic rules. Brueckner and Bond (1955) list procedures for

teaching slow learning children to read. Their ideas are to

slow down and control presentation of material, to begin

instruction later, and to provide maximum auditory and visual

word experience. Ebersole, Kephart, and Ebersole (1968) detail

readiness exercises which they deem necessary for beginning

reading. Their efforts are directed toward the student gaining

6

Page 14: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

a consistent idea of perceptual body image, laterality, direc­

tionality, and temporal projection. Van Witsen {1967) has

written a manual to be used in training retarded children in

perception. She includes exercises for each of the senses

and a section on phonics. On the other hand, Money (1967)

states that none of the gross perceptual or visual motor coor­

dination training techniques have been found to have any

direct effect on learning to read.

In the area of language disability there is currently a

lag between the development of remedial techniques and appro­

priate diagnostic tests. The Frostig Test of Visual Perception

{1961) shows moderate correlations with reading scores (Maslow,

Frostig, and Lefever, 1964). Perhaps this correlation would

have been stronger if consideration for auditory and kines­

thetic factors had been included. The Illinois Test of

Psycholinguistic Ability (McCarthy, 1961) is well validated

(McCarthy, 1965) and is intended to diagnose a variety of per­

ceptual problems. However, it does not test for auditory dis-

crimination {Frostig and Maslow, 1968), and it must be

individually administered.

Slingerland recognized the need for an appropriate screen­

ing test for language disability and developed her Screening

Tests for Identifying Children with Specific Language Disa­

bility (1962). The purpose of the tests is to identify

7

Page 15: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

particular language difficulties as early as grade one in

average and above average I.Q. chlldren. The tests evaluate

various combinations of the auditory-visual-kinesthetic

perceptual functions.

Purpose

Retarded children are currently being placed into Special

Education Classes where they are often treated as if they are

a homogenous group. Hutt and Gibby (1965} have pointed out

that such grouping is inaccurate because retarded youths have

as many intragroup differences as normal children. If between­

student distinctions are to be found in the area of language

development and reading, then a diagnostic test must be

developed. The test must be easily administered, its direc-

tions must be easily understood, and it must delineate probable

perceptual language weaknesses. The question considered here

was whether the Slingerland tests would apply to slow learners

and therefore meet this need.

Reliability and validity studies on ttiese screening tests

have not been reported. However, Slingerland (1962) states

that in an average grade seventy percent of the children show

no indications of language disabilities. Whether or not any

similar kind of percentage exists in a retarded population has

not been investigated. The test's author (Slingerland; Renton

School System, Washington, personal communication) has reported

8

Page 16: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

that she has no reason to doubt the test's applicability to

mentally handicapped youths.

If this study finds significant correlations between the

Slingerland and a readiness test, and if the tests point out

areas of perceptual weakness, then they may be interpreted as

appropriate for a retarded population. If, on the other hand,

the correlations with the readiness test are modest, then the

tests will be considered inappropriate. In this case the

development of a screening test for low I.Q. children would be

necessary.

The hypotheses to be tested are that there are not sig­

nificant relationships (1) between the Slingerland Tests and

Metropolitan Readiness·Tests, (2) between the Slingerland

Tests and WISC I.Q. Test, and (3) between the Metropolitan

Tests and WISC Test.

Further hypotheses are that there are no significant

multiple relationships between criterion scores on the Metro­

politan and predictor scores on the Slingerland and WISC.

The final hypotheses are that no significant differences

exist between Metropolitan-Slingerland correlations when the

data is divided in terms of age, race, sex, and I.Q.

9

Page 17: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

Subjects

CHAPTER I I

Method

Thirty-six Special Education students in Chester!icld

County, Virginia, served as Ss. The sample represents edu­

cable retarded youths age seven to fourteen. They came !rom

classes whose total enrollment was 54. Three o! the classes

were integrated while one was segregated white. Two o! the

integrated classes were predominantly white and one was pre­

dominantly Negro. O! the students who were dropped from the

experiment, ten were absent !or at least one o( the tests and

could not be scheduled !or retesting, five did not have WISC

scores available, and three were untestable due to severe

retardation. Permission to do the testing was obtained from

the Assistant Superintendent !or Instruction at Chesterfield.

The range of the WISC scores was 45-H7, the oean was

64.61, and the median was 63.5. Twenty-si~ of the Ss were

white and ten were Negro. Twenty-three were aale; thirteen

were female. The ~s ranged in age from seven to fourteen.

The average age was 10.89.

Materials

10

Scores on the He tropoli tan Rradiness Tests Form A (Hildreth,

Griffiths, McG&uvran, 1965) were used as the criterion mc:?a­

sure. These tests are in wide US<.! throughout the United

Page 18: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

States. Their purpose is to measure achievements of school

beginners. They determine whether an individual's intellec­

tual development is sufficient to enable him to succeed in

reading instruction (Harcourt, Brace, and World, 1969). The

Metropolitan Tests were used here to provide an index of

achievement. There are six subtests: word meaning, listen­

ing, matching, alphabet, numbers, and copying. The scores on

these are additive. The tests were given in three sittings,

taking a total of about one hour.

Using a normal I.Q. population the Metropolitan is known

to correlate approximately .60 with the Stanford Achievement

Test and aooroximately .80 with the New York State Readiness

Tests. It has also been shown to correlate around .60 with

the Stanford Binet and Otis-Lennon I.Q. tests (Harcourt, Brace,

and World, 1969). Similar information was not available for

a retarded sample. However, no special difficulties were

anticipated provided that the children to be tested mani­

fested approximately the same intellectual development as a

riormal first 9rader (Mitchell; Harcourt, Brace, and World,

personal communication).

Scores on the Grade I-II Screening Tests for Identifying

Children with Specific Language Disability (Slingerland, 1962)

were used as a predictor measure. This is a relatively new

instrument and has yet to gain wide circulation. Its purpose

is to. select out of a normal and above normal I .Q. group those

11

Page 19: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

youngsters whose real or potential difficulties with language

go far beyond any problems anticipated by considering the rest

of their intellectual functioning. This isolated problem with

language is called "specific language disability". The eight

subtests measure visual, auditory, and kinesthetic perception

as they are needed in language development. These subtests

divide into three groups. The first consists of two copying

tests which require visual perception near and far, and a

kinesthetic response. The second group consists of three sub­

.tests and requires visual perception, recall, and kinesthetic

association. The final three subtests require auditory per­

ception and recall with both visual and kinesthetic associa­

tions (Slingerland, 1962).

The tests were given in about an hour and a quarter and

the children were given two short breaks.

WISC scores were also used as a predictor. These were

obtained from the Ss files from school. They had been adminis­

tered by a variety of psychologists on a number of dates. Over

two-thirds of the tests had been given within the last two

years. Only one was over four years old, and it was given six

years ago to a child who is now fourteen.

Procedure

The Metropolitan and Slingerland tests were administered

to four groups of not more than 12 Ss. The tests were given

12

Page 20: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

to the whole class in their normal classroom setting. The

regular teacher remained in the classroom to help with proctor­

ing. The manuals of both tests provide directions :for adminis­

tration procedure. The Metropolitan's directions proved

explicit and ample. The instructions :for the Slingerland were

by design less precise and needed to be expanded upon in

response to the Ss questions.

The Metropolitan was presented :first. The Slingerland

:followed not less than a week nor more than three weeks later.

All testing was don~ as soon a:fter the children arrived at

school as possible, generally around 9 a.m. The Ss were

scattered around the room to prevent any copying. They were

also watched closely. Crayons and pencils were provided by E

:for the Metropolitan and Slingerland respectively.

Analysis o:f Data

The tests were scored by g_ in accordance with the manuals.

This data was then analyzed by an I.B.M. 1620 computer. Pro­

gram number 6.0.148, a "Single and Multiple Linear Regression

Analysis Program" :from the General Program Library was used.

Numerous correlations between the various combinations of tests

and subtests were found.

The Slingerland was considered in :five ways: total score,

copy phase (subtests 1 and 2), visual-kinesthetic phase (sub­

tests 3, 4, and 5), auditory-visual-kinesthetic phase (subtests

6, 7, and 8), and subtests 3, 4, S, 6, 7, and 8 toge.ther. The

13

Page 21: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

Metropolitan Score was considered undivided. I.Q. scores from

the WISC verbal, performance, and total were evaluated

separately.

Multiple correlations between the Metropolitan and each

of the possible fifteen pairs of Slingerland and WISC scores

were obtained. Twenty-three single correlations were investi­

gated to show the relationships between each Slingerland and

each WISC I.Q. and the Metropolitan.

Finally, the data was split among four criteria: I.Q.,

·age, sex, and race. Separate analyses were run for each. The

Slingerland scores of Ss with I.Q.s over the median, 63.5,

were correla.ted with their Metropolitan Scores. The same was

done with Ss whose I.Q.s were under 63.5. The two correlations

were then compared using z values to find if they differed sig­

nificantly (Downie and Heath, 1965). A similar technique was

used to compare ..§_s along criteria of age, sex, and race.

These correlations were included to determine whether any of

the four factors considered had an undue influence upon the

reading-perception relationship.

14

Page 22: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

CHAPTER III

Results

The range of errors on the Slingerland was 2 to 92. The

average number of errors was 33 which was 30% of the 110 items.

The Ss had the most difficulty with the final three subtests

(the auditory phase). An average of 40% of that part was

missed. The Ss made 23% errors on the copying subtests and

27% errors on the visual subtests. On the Metropolitan Tests

the students were incorrect 32% of the time.

Table I presents the correlations (r) between the scores

from the Slingerland Screening Tests and the Metropolitan.

Each of the correlations differed significantly from zero at

the .01 level; each correlation was positive. This evidence

15

rejects the hypotheses that the correlation between the Slinger­

land and Metropolitan is zero.

Correlations between WISC and Metropolitan scores are

listed in Table II. Again, every correlation measured reached

positive significance at the .01 confidence level. Again, the

hypotheses of no relationship were rejected.

Table III shows multiple correlations (Rn) between each

possible pair of Slingerland, WISC predictor scores, and the

Metropolitan criterion. In each case the correlation was

found to be significant. Therefore, the hypotheses that the

correlations R1 , R2 , ... R15 = O were rejected, and it has been

Page 23: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

16

TABLE I

CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READINESS TESTS

AND SCORES FROM SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS

CRITERION PREDICTOR CORRELATION (Y) ( X) (r)

Met. Sling. Copy .69**

Met. Sling. Vis. .63**

Met. Sling. Aud. .75**

Met. Sling. 3 to 8 .77**

Met. Sling Total .81**

** P-<:.01

Page 24: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

17

TABLE II

CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN

READINESS TESTS AND WISC I.Q. SCORES

CRITERION PREDICTOR CORRELATION (Y) (X) ( r)

Met. I.Q. Verbal . 52**

Met. I.Q. Perf. .59**

Met. I. Q. Full .62**

** p < .01

Page 25: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

TABLE III

MULTIPLE CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN

METROPOLITAN READINESS TESTS AND SLINGERLAND

SCREENING TEST AND WISC TESTS

CRITERION PREDICTORS CORRELATIONS CORRELATION ( y) (X1,X2) (r) BETWEEN x1 ,x2

Met. Sling. Copy, I.Q. Verbal .84** .05 Met. Sling. Copy, I.Q. Perform. .80** .30 Met. Sling. Copy, I.Q. Full .84** .21

Met. Sling. Vis., I.Q. Verbal .73** .26 Met. Sling. Vis. , I.Q. Perform. .75** .32 Met. Sling. Vis., I.Q. Full .77** .32

Met. Sling. Aud. , I.Q. Verbal . 82** .27 i\let. Sling. Aud., I.Q. Perform. .86** .24 Met. Sling. Aud. , I.Q. Full .86** .29

Met. Sling. 3-8, I. Q. Verbal .83** .29 !\let. Sling. 3-8, I. Q. Perform. .86** .29 Met. Sling. 3-8, I. Q. Full .87** • 32

Met. Sling. Total, I. Q. Verbal .88** .22 Met. Sling. Total, I. Q. Perform. .88** • 32 Met. Sling. Total, I • Q. Full .90** .31

~ + p < '01 ..... CXl

Page 26: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

established that a positive relationship between the predictors

and the criterion does exist.

Table III also shows the simple correlation between all

possible pairs 0£ Slingerland and WISC scores. None of these

~reached significance. The hypothesis that r=O was not

rejected. No significant relationship between Wechsler I.Q.

scores and Slingerland scores was obtained.

Table IV lists the partial regression coefficients for

the multiple correlations from Table III.

cant at the .Ol level.

Each was signifi-

Tables V, VI, VII, VIII, and Figure I consider the

Metropolitan-Slingerland data in terms of an added dimension.

This was done by repeatedly splitting the Ss into two groups

according to I.Q., race, age, and sex. Table V shows that

with 18 Ss, Slingerland-Metropolitan correlations were still

positive and significant. "t" tests were done on the differ-

ences between the correlations for lower and higher I.Q.

groups. The data pairs had been normalized using a Fisher z

transformation (Downie and Heath, 1965). No significant dif­

ferences w~re found.

In Table VI the Ss were divided by race. All correla­

tions for both races were positive and were significant at .01.

The "t" tests on the differences between correlations on the

white-Negro dimension found no significant differences.

19

Page 27: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

TABLE IV

SUMMARY TABLE FOR TESTING SIGNIFICANCES OF

PREDICTORS

Slin9. Copy

i.g. Full

Slin9. Copy

r.g. Verbal

Sling.. Copy

I.Q. Per!ornance

Slin9. Visual

r.g. Full

Sling. Visual

I.g. Verbal

Sling. Visual

I.Q. Per f ort:i."\1lC e

Sling. Auditory

I.Q. Full

Sling. Auditory

1.g. Verbal

Slin9. Auditory

I.Q. Perforr-'lnce

Slin9. 3-8

I.Q. Full

•• p <.01

PARTIAL REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS

PARTIAL. REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS

1.10

.65

1.24

.62

1.04

• 50

1.33

.60

1.48

.48

1. 37

. 51

.A9

• 57

.95

.43

.93

• S2

.65

. 53

STD. ERROR OF' TllE PARTIAL.

REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS

• 16

.12

.17

.12

.21

• 13

• 32

• 1 s

• 34

• 1 (>

.33

.14

• 13

.12

.15

.13

.1 J

• 11

.10

.11

20

t(JJ)

6.11 ..

S.42••

7.29••

S.17••

4.95**

3.BS••

4 .16* •

4.oo••

4.35**

3,oo••

4. 15* •

3.64**

6.Hs••

4.75**

(>.33••

1.31••

1. 15• •

4. 12•.

<>.so••

4. H2 • •

Page 28: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

PREDICTORS

Sling. 3-8

I.Q. Verbal

Sling. 3-8

I.Q. Per:f ormance

Sling. Total

I .Q. Full

Sling. Total

I ~Q. Verbal

Sling. Total

I.Q. Per:f ormance

** P(.01

TABLE IV (con't)

PARTIAL REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS

.69

.40

.67

.47

.49

.53

• 52

.45

. 50

.44

STD. ERROR OF THE PARTIAL

REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS

.10

.13

.10

.11

.06

.10

.06

.11

.06

.10

21

t(33)

6.90**

3.08**

6.70**

4.27**

8.17**

5.30**

8.67**

4.09**

8.33**

4.40**

Page 29: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

22

TABLE V

CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READINESS TESTS

AND SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS WHEN Ss ARE DIVIDED INTO LOWER

AND HIGHER I.Q. GROUPS

I .Q. CRITERION PREDICTOR CORRELATION. {Y) {X) (r)

Low (18Ss) Met. Sling.Copy .81**

Hi (18Ss) Met. Sling.Copy .79**

Low Met. Sling.Vis. .74**

Hi Met. Sling.Vis. .66**

Low Met. Sling.Aud. .74**

Hi Met. Sling.Aud. .86**

** P < .Ol

Page 30: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

23

TABLE VI

CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READINESS TESTS

AND SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS WHEN Ss ARE DIVIDED BY RACE

RACE CRITERION PREDICTOR CORRELATION (Y} (X} (r}

Negro ( 10.§J Met. Sling.Copy .79**

White (26S} Met. Sling.Copy .56**

Negro Met. Sling.Vis. .71**

White Met. Sling.Vis. .64**

Negro Met. Sling.Aud. .84**

White Met. Sling.Aud. .66**

** p < .01

Page 31: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

Age was used to group ~s in Table VII. The correlation

failed to reach significance when older .§.s Metropolitan­

Slingerland Copy scores were compared. The other five corre­

lations were significant. The "t" tests on the difference

between correlations for the younger-older groups failed to

find a significant difference.

Table VIII divides Ss by sex. Metropolitan-Slingerland

Auditory correlations for females were not found to be signifi­

cant. The other correlations were all significant. "t" tests

on the Ss divided by sex were not significant.

24

Page 32: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

25

TABLE VII

CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READINESS

TESTS AND SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS WHEN Ss ARE DIVIDED

BY AGE

AGE CRITERION PREDICTOR CORRELATION {Y) (X) (r)

Young (11-) (21.§.s) Met. Sling.Copy .74**

Old (12+) (15Ss) Met. Sling.Copy .47

Young Met. Sling.Vis. .61**

Old Met. Sling.Vis. .80**

Young Met. Sling.Aud. .78**

Old Met. Sling.Aud. .68**

** p (.01

Page 33: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

26

TABLE VIII

CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN METROPOLITAN READINESS TESTS

AND SLINGERLAND SCREENING TESTS WHEN Ss ARE DIVIDED BY SEX

SEX CRITERION PREDICTOR CORRELATION {Y) {X) (r)

Male (23Ss) Met. Sling.Copy .77**

Female {13Ss) Met. Sling.Copy .67**

Male Met. Sling.Vis. .65**

Female Met. Sling.Vis. .60*

Male Met. Sling.Aud. .84**

Female Met. Sling.Aud. .49

* p (.05

** p ~ .01

Page 34: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

~

10 ..

-

50 ..

40 ..

FIGURE I

AVERAGE CORRELATIONS (r) BEIWEEN METROPOLITAN

AND SLINGERLAND TESTS WHEN THE DATA HAS BEEN

DIVIDED IN TERMS OF SEX, RACE, I.Q., AND AGE

.78 .ii :is :7<;.

.71

.~S'

.~2

.5q

MALE FEMALE •EHO W1tl TE LOW Klitl 12 AMO II AlD OLDER TOU"6£1t 1.q. l.Q.

27

Page 35: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

CHAPTER IV

Discussion

The goal of this research was to determine whether the

Slingerland Screening Tests were applicable to mentally

retarded youngsters. The Slingerland Tests had been designed

to pick out specific language disability in average I.Q. chil­

dren by examining combinations of visual, auditory, and kines­

thetic perceptual skills. The tests were not intended to be

used on children who in addition to being deficient in lan9ua<Je

skills were also generally intellectually slow. However, the

author of the test expressed interest in this new application

of her work and saw no reason why it would not prove appro­

priate (Slingerland; Renton School System, Washington, personal

communication).

The high predictor-criterion correlations which were

found were essential in determining the applicability o{ the

Slingerland. Each perceptual channel was found to relate to

language perfornancc. Such results indicate why one-channel

diagnostic tests fail to correlate highly with readin9, a

multisensory skill.

The chi ldrcn tested were found to achieve a "·ide ran<Jl' o(

scores on the Slin<;crland. They did not bunch at one end of

the scale or the other. Thi~ indicates that the tests wprc of

about the right degree of difficulty for the sanplc• and is an

inportant consideration in test construction.

28

Page 36: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

The multiple and simple correlations used in the data

analysis express relationships between shared variance but are

not intended to say anything about cause and effect. While

the results do say that perceptual problems and reading prob­

lems are related, they say nothing about perceptual problems

causing reading difficulty.

Column 4, Table III, lists the WISC-Slingerland correla-

tions. They are small and none reach significance. Slinger-

land (1962) observed that with a normal I.Q. population, her

tests did not correlate highly with I.Q. It is interesting to

note that while Metropolitan-WISC scores correlate highly

(Table II), and Metropolitan-Slingerland scores correlate

highly (Table I), the WISC-Slingerland scores only correlate

modestly (Table III). To interpret such a relationship the

Metropolitan must be seen as measuring two unrelated abilities.

In other words, reading development was related to I.Q. and to

perception, but I.Q. and perception were not related.

29

Figure I and Tables V, VI, VII, and VIII show Metropolitan­

Slingerland correlations when the data was divided by I.Q.,

race, age, and sex. Although some directional differences do

exist, none were significant. It can be said that no nestin<J

of high correlations for high or low I.Q., Negro or white,

younger or older, or male or female was found. Specifically,

the results on Table VII fail to support the proposed rela­

tionship between the reading-perceµtion correlation and a<JP.

Page 37: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

30

The proposal was that the younger children's correlations ought

to be higher than the older children's due to the existence of

more natural perceptual confusion in younger children. However,

saying that no relationship was found is not tantamount to say­

ing that no relationship exists, for it is quite possible that

significance was lost when the data was arbitrarily divided in

two at eleven year olds. Dividing the data also had the effect

of reducing the size of N for the correlations in Tables V-VIII.

When N was made smaller, the impact of a few highly irregular

scores was increased and the overall correlation suffered.

This study supports the thesis that perception (as mea­

sured by the Slingerland) and language ability (as measured by

the Metropolitan) are related in retarded children. The rela-

tionship between visual perception, orientation in space and

reading ability has been reported for a retarded population

(Lovell et al., 1965). The present study is more to the point,

however, because it employs a published and easily obtained

diagnostic test to measure perception. It also tests for the

perception of academi~ally relevant symbols.

Researchers have demonstrated that difficulty at the auto­

matic sequential (or nonmeaning1ul} level of the perceptual

process causes reading problems among children with learning

retardation (Bateman and Wetherall, 1965). It is errors in

retention of symbols and symbol sequences that make the use l>f

Page 38: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

31

language particularly difficult. The representational (meaning­

ful) level does not present the same degree of difficulty for

the low I.Q. child. It is the fact that the Slingerland tests

for perceptual weakness through letter and number sequences

that makes it apply to the automatic sequential level.

Bateman and Wetherell (1965) also state that their study

with the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Ability reveals a

stronger development of the visual channel as compared w.ith the

auditory channel for mentally retarded youths. Similar results

were found in this study. The Ss made the highest percentage

of errors (40%) on the final three subtests. These subtests

require a variety of responses to auditory stimuli. The Ss

made only an average of 25% errors on the visually perceived

tests. This finding implies that it is particularly difficult

for the low I.Q. child to effectively handle auditory symbols.

Spelling would be an extremely difficult task for such a child.

Knowing that the Slingerland Screening Tests delineate

reading disabilities among low I.Q. children can have important

implications. Children who are shown to have severe weaknesses

in visual perception should be taught to read with the emphasis

on "sounding out" words, i.e., using phonics. Children who

are extremely weak in auditory perception will be confused

with phonics and should be taught by the "look and say" method.

Children with weakness in both visual and auditory µerception

can be taught through their tactile and kin0sthetic senses.

Page 39: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

Using the tools which have been developed for normal children

with perceptual-reading problems to teach language to mentally

retarded children stands to improve their linquistic

development.

Suggestions for Further Research

There is a need for an adaptation of the Slingerland

Screening Test for slow children. This test should have more

explicit directions, and it should be shorter than the current

Slingerland. The degree of question difficulty should remain

about the same as that on the present test for the first and

second grades. Training teachers to use new techniques is one

of the most salient problems in education (Slingerland, 1968).

Part of further work in this area must be teaching teachers

what has already been learned.

32

Continued examination of the perceptual-reading

relationship is important. A large sample of retarded children

with particular language disability should be used to discover

appropriate teaching techniques for particular problems. It is

important to pinpoint the crucial periods of perceptual develop­

ment and to determine whether that development is enhanced

through an enriched environment or through a specific instruc­

tional method. It would be interesting to discover how long

such a crucial perceptual period lasts for the retarded child

as compared to the normal child.

Page 40: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

CHAPTER V

Summary and Conclusions

This research was carried out to investigate the Slinger­

land Screening Tests' ability to delineate the relationship

between perceptual abilities and reading readiness among edu-

cable mentally retarded children. Previous researchers

addressing the perception-reading relationship have reported

mixed results. They usually have not used published tests to

measure perceptual skill, and thus their studies have not been

replicated.

The Metropolitan was used as the criterion measure 0£

reading readiness. WISC scores were obtained from the Ss

files to measure I.Q. Thirty-six mentally retarded Ss were

tested. Their Slingerland scores £ell across the entire possi-

ble range. Numerous correlations were performed on the test's

scores. The results were consistent and generally positive.

Every Slingerland score correlated significantly with reading

readiness. Every WISC score correlated significantly with

reading readiness.

was insignificant.

And every Slingerland-WISC correlation

These relationships indicate that the

Metropolitan Readiness Tests measure two distinct unrelated

abilities, perception and I.Q. The Ss had more difficulty

with the subtest designed to measurL' auditory pc..> rcept ion than

with those designed to measure visual perception.

33

Page 41: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

34

When the data was divided in two in terms of age, sex, I.Q.,

and race, the results were less profound. No significant dif­

ferences between perception-reading correlations along any of

these dimensions were found.

Conclusions

The Slingerland Screening Test is sensitive to perceptual­

reading relationships among mentally retarded children.

Since children with perceptual problems require special

teaching techniques to learn reading, and since such techniques

now exist for normal I.Q. children, these methods ought

to be adapted and used to teach perceptually weak Special

Education children.

Page 42: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

35

REFERENCES

Bannatyne, A. D. The color phonics system. In: J. Money (ed.),

The disabled reader, education of the dyslexic child.

Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1967.

Bateman, B. and Wetherell, J. Psycholinguistic aspects of

mental retardation. Mental Retardation, 1965, 3, 8-13.

Belmont, L. and Birch, H. G. The intellectual profile of

retarded readers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1966, 22,

787-816.

Benton, A. L. Dyslexia in relation to form perception and

directional sense. In: J. Money (ed.), Reading disability:

progress and research needs in dyslexia. Baltimore: Johns

Hopkins Press, 1962.

Birch, H. G. and Belmont, L. Auditory visual integration,

intelligence, and reading ability in school children.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 20, 295-305.

Brueckner, L. T. and B9nd, G. L. Diagnosis and treatment of

learning difficulties. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,

1955.

Clemmens, R. I. Obscure causes of school failure, a pediatric

viewpoint. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 1964, 14, 32-38.

Coleman, H. M. Visual perception and reading dysfunction.

Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1968, 1 (2), 116-123.

Page 43: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

Davidson, H. P. A study of the confusing letters, b, d, p,

and q. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1935, 47, 458-468.

Downie, N. M. and Heath, R. W. Basic statistical methods.

New York: Harper and Row, 1965.

Ebersole, M., Kephart, N. c., and Ebersole, J. B. Steps to

achievement for the slow learner. Columbus, Ohio:

Charles E. Merrill, 1968.

Frostig, M. Marianne Frostig developmental test of visual

perception, third edition. Palo Alto, California:

Consulting Psychologist Press, Inc., 1964.

36

Frostig, M. Corrective reading in the classroom. The Reading

Teacher, 1965, 18 (7), 573-578.

Frostig, M. and Horne, D. The Frostig program for development

of visual perception. Chicago: Follett Publishing Company,

1964.

Frostig, M. and Maslow, P. Language training: a form of abil­

ity training. Journal of Language Disabilities, 1968,

1 ( 2) ' 15-25.

Gattegno, c. Words in color. Chicago: Encyclopedia

Britannica Press Inc., 1963.

Gilkey, B. G. and Parr, F. w. An analysis of the reversal

tendency of fifty selected elementary school pupils.

Journal of .Educational Psychology, 1944, 35, 284-292.

Page 44: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

Gillingham, A. and Stillman, B. W. Remedial training for

children with specific disability in reading, spelling,

and penmanship, sixth edition. Cambridge, Massachusetts:

Educators Publishing Service, 1960.

Harcourt, Brace, and World (pub.) Supplementary information

on the Metropolitan readiness tests. New York: 1969.

Hildreth, G. H., Griffiths, N. L. and McGauvran, M. E.

Metropolitan readiness tests. New York: Harcourt, Brace,

and World, Inc., 1965.

Hutt, M. L. and Gibby, R. G. The mentally retarded child,

development, education, and treatment, second edition.

Boston: Allyn and Bacon, Inc., 1965.

37

Ilg, F. L. and Ames, L. B. Developmental trends in reading

behavior. Journal of Genetic Psycholo9J:'._, 1950, 76, 291-312.

Johnson, G. o. Education for slow learners. Englewood Cliffs,

New Jersey: Prentice Hall, 1963.

Johnson, M. J. Tracing and kinesthetic techniques. In:

J. Money (ed.), The disabled reader, education of the

dyslexic child. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1967.

Kendall, B. S. A note on the relation of retardation in reading

to performance on a memory-for-designs test. Journal of

Educational Psychol~, 1948, 39, 370-373.

Kephart, N. C. The slow learner in the classroom. Columbus,

Ohio: Charles E. Merrill, Inc., 1960.

Page 45: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

Kirk, S. A. Teaching reading to slow learning children.

Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1940.

Lovell, K., White, C., and Whiteley, R. Studying backward

readers. Special Education, 1965, 54 (3), 9-13.

McCarthy, J. J. Notes on the validity of the I.T.P.A.

Mental Retardation, 1965, 3 (2), 25-26.

McCarthy, J. J. and Kirk, s. A. Illinois test of psycho­

linguistic abilities. Urbana, Illinois: Institute for

Research on Exceptional Children, University of Illinois,

1961.

38

Maslow, P., Frostig, M., Lefever, D. W., and Whittlesey, J. R. B.

The Marianne Frostig developmental test of visual perception,

1963 standardization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1964,

19, 463-499.

Mazurkiewicz, A. J. The initial teaching alphabet (i/t/a).

In J. Money (ed.), The disabled reader, education of the

dyslexic child. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1967.

Money, J. (ed.) The disabled reader, education of the

dyslexic child. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1967.

Orton, J. L. A guide to teaching phonics. Winston-Salem,

North Carolina: Orton Reading Center, 1964.

Orton, J. L. The Orton-Gillingham approach. In: J. Money (ed.),

The disabled reader, education of the dyslexic child.

Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1967.

Page 46: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

39

Orton, s. T. Reading, writing, and speech problems in children.

New York: W.W. Norton and Company, Inc., 1937.

Slingerland, B. H. Screening tests for identifying children

with specific language disability. Cambridge, Massachusetts:

Educators Publishing Service, 1962.

Slingerland, B. H. Specific language disability from the

educators point of view. Paper presented at Washington

State Medical Association Annual Meeting, Seattle: 1968.

Stroud, J. B. Rate of visual perception as a factor in rate

of reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1945,

36, 487-498.

Van Witsen, B. Perceptual training activities handbook.

New York: Teachers College, Columbia University Press, 1967.

Vernon, M. D. Backwardness in reading. Cambridge, England:

Cambridge University Press, 1957.

Wechsler, D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children.

New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1949.

Page 47: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

APPENDIX I

STANDARD ERRORS OF CDRRELATION

Metropolitan .••.

Slingerland Copy •.•••••

. . . . . . .

Slingerland Visual.

Slingerland Auditory.

Slingerland 3 to 8.

Slingerland Total • • •

. . . . . . . .

. . . . . . .

16.96

12.51

13.33

11.39

10.98

10.19

WISC I.Q. Verbal. ••..••.••• • • • • • 14.74.

WISC I.Q. Performance •

WISC I.Q. Full Scale. . . . . 13.88

13.46

40

Page 48: The use of the Slingerland screening tests to delineate

41

VITA

James Over Rust was born on March 24, 1945 in Pittsburgh,

Pennsylvania. He received his entire primary and secondary

education at Shady Side Academy and graduated in 1963. That

same year he entered Lehigh University from which he graduated

with a B. A. in psychology in 1967. After completing a year of

graduate school at the University of Richmond he was elected

into Psi Chi. He spent the 1968-1969 school year teaching

special education in Chesterfield, Virginia. Upon receiving

his M. A. in psychology in August, 1969 he will continue

teaching and coaching in Chesterfield.