SCIreviewA&D08

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    1/11

    The premild cognitive impairment, subjective cognitive impairmentstage of Alzheimers disease

    Barry Reisberga,*, Leslie Prichepb, Lisa Mosconic, E. Roy Johnb, Lidia Glodzik-Sobanskac,Istvan Boksaya, Isabel Monteiroa, Carol Torossiana, Alok Vedvyasa, Nauman Ashrafa,

    Imran A. Jamila, Mony J. de Leonc

    aSilberstein Aging and Dementia Research Center, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USAbBrain Research Laboratories, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA

    cCenter for Brain Health, Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA

    Abstract Background: Subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) has been a common, but poorly understoodcondition, frequently occurring in older persons.

    Methods: The past and the emerging literature on SCI and synonymously named conditions is reviewed.

    Results: Findings include: (1) There is support from at least one longitudinal study for a long-standing

    concept of SCI as a premild cognitive impairment (MCI) condition lasting 15years. (2) There are

    complex relationships between SCI and depression and anxiety. (3) Differences in SCI subjects from

    age-matched non-SCI persons are being published in terms of cognitive tests, hippocampal gray matter

    density, hippocampal volumes, cerebral metabolism, and urinary cortisol levels. Psychometric and

    dementia test score differences between SCI and MCI subjects have long been evident. (4) Predictive

    electrophysiologic features of subsequent decline in SCI subjects are being published.

    Conclusions: Studies of therapeutic agents in SCI treatment and resultant Alzheimers disease

    prevention appear to be feasible. These trials are also necessary from a public health perspective.

    2008 The Alzheimers Association. All rights reserved.

    Keywords: Subjective cognitive impairment; Cognitive complaints; Brain aging; Mild cognitive impairment; Dementia;

    Alzheimers disease

    1. Background

    Studies of the course of Alzheimers disease (AD) from

    its earliest antecedents are presently producing new insights

    in terms of the earliest behavioral manifestations of AD and

    in terms of the basic etiopathogenesis of AD. This brief

    perspective will focus on the most salient and important

    recent observations from our perspective and the therapeutic

    opportunities and challenges associated with these findings.

    2. Overview of the behavioral course of incipient and

    progressive AD

    A schema of our current understanding of the typical be-

    havioral course of AD in terms of major staging and mental

    status methodologies is shown in Figure 1. The legend for

    Figure 1 refers to some of the most important references

    supporting this characteristic course of AD [18].

    This perspective will focus on the earliest manifest stage

    of this characteristic course of AD, which, while requiring

    continuing elucidation, is also potentially a very fertile area

    for therapeutic intervention.

    3. Subjective cognitive impairment, the incipient stage of

    AD (before mild cognitive impairment) when the

    patient knows, but [presently] the doctor doesnt know

    As illustrated inFigure 1, the Global Deterioration Scale

    (GDS) [7,8] and the related Functional Assessment Staging

    (FAST) procedure [9,10] each contain 7 major stages. The

    FAST staging procedure also contains a total of 11 sub-

    stages in the FAST stage 6 to 7, advanced dementia, range.

    Hence, FAST staging identifies a total of 16 stages and*Corresponding author. Tel.: 212-263-8550; Fax: 212-263-6991.

    E-mail address:[email protected]

    Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108

    1552-5260/08/$ see front matter 2008 The Alzheimers Association. All rights reserved.

    doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2007.11.017

    http://mail%20to:[email protected]/http://mail%20to:[email protected]/http://mail%20to:[email protected]/
  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    2/11

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    3/11

    been suggested [16], was hypothesized more than 20 years

    ago to be a stage in the evolution of AD [1]. More specif-

    ically, it was estimated in 1986 that this SCI stage lasts a

    mean of approximately 15 years before the subsequent MCI

    stage in the evolution of AD pathology [1]. If this estimate

    of a 15-year duration for SCI were true, and if a surveyed

    population of persons with SCI is evenly distributed acrossthe SCI spectrum, then it would be anticipated that approx-

    imately 1/15 of persons with SCI would decline to an MCI

    or a more severe diagnosis per year. Hence, a longitudinal

    study that included healthy persons with SCI, who are

    normally distributed across the severity spectrum, would

    show approximately a 6.67% rate of decline to MCI or

    dementia per annum. However, SCI symptoms can also be

    associated with conditions other than the progression of AD,

    notably anxiety, depression, and non-AD neurologic pathol-

    ogy such as head trauma, strokes, etc. Therefore, a study

    that seeks to examine this hypothesis of a 15-year duration

    of the SCI stage in ultimately manifest MCI and dementiaassociated with AD would have to exclude persons with

    significant non-ADrelated pathology at baseline and would

    have to be conducted prospectively for a sufficient period of

    time. Such studies are beginning to be published [6,17].

    3.1.2. Observed duration of the SCI stage of incipient AD

    Prichep et al [6] enrolled normal older, otherwise

    healthy, community-residing subjects with SCI (GDS stage

    2) and with baseline electroencephalographic studies during

    a period from 1980 to 1997. Of 118 normal older subjects

    enrolled, 44 subjects completed longitudinal follow-up after

    a 7-year minimal time interval and formed the study popu-lation. Subjects lost to follow-up (n 74) generally died

    during the follow-up interval, moved from the area, refused

    follow-up, or were unlocatable. There were no significant

    differences in age or gender between the subjects longitu-

    dinally followed and those who were lost to follow-up. The

    44 subjects followed comprised 22 men and 22 women,

    with a mean baseline age of 72.0 years (range, 64 to 79

    years). During the follow-up period, 20 subjects declined to

    an MCI diagnosis (GDS stage 3) [13], and 7 declined to a

    dementia diagnosis either of AD or, in one case, vascular

    dementia [18]. For the subjects who exhibited decline, the

    maximum deterioration period of observation within the7-year minimum observation interval was used for the anal-

    yses. For the non-decliners, the mean standard deviation

    (SD) period of observation was 8.9 1.8 years. Hence,

    assuming SCI is a stage in which all subjects eventually

    decline and that this stage lasts 15 years, we would antici-

    pate that approximately 59.33% of subjects would have

    been observed to decline during the 8.9-year observation

    interval. The actual observed percentage of subjects who

    declined was 61.36% (Figure 2). Clearly, this study is very

    supportive of (1) the hypothesis that SCI, as defined with

    the GDS stage 2 terminology/methodology and the appro-

    priate inclusion and exclusion criteria, is a stage in the

    evolution of MCI and, ultimately, AD, and (2) that this

    pre-MCI stage has a total duration of15 years before

    manifest decline to MCI and, ultimately, dementia associ-

    ated with AD.

    3.2. Affective and anxiety interrelationships with SCI

    3.2.1. Effect of affective and anxiety symptomatology on

    SCI outcome

    As noted, conditions other than incipient AD can also

    produce SCI symptomatology. For example, many prior

    studies have shown associations between depressed mood

    and other affective and anxiety symptoms and SCI symp-

    tomatology. However, until recently, these studies did not

    rigorously exclude MCI subjects (Table 1). Recent studies

    that do rigorously exclude MCI subjects continue to find

    interesting interrelationships between SCI, anxiety, and de-

    pression. For example, Lautenschlager et al [23] found that

    elderly (70 years of age) community-residing women withsubjective memory complaints (mean MMSE SD, 28.2

    1.5) had higher depression and anxiety scores than a com-

    parison group of healthy community-residing women that

    was closely matched in terms of the level of cognitive

    scores (mean MMSE SD, 28.3 1.4).

    These interrelationships can affect outcome. For exam-

    ple, it has long been known that treatment of depression, in

    subjects with a diagnosis of depression, can reduce memory

    complaints [24].

    A recent longitudinal study of Glodzik-Sobanska et al

    [25] illustrated the very profound relationship between af-

    fective symptoms, memory complaints, and subsequent out-come in older persons. This study examined large cohorts of

    normal, healthy, community-residing subjects, free of major

    depression or other significant psychiatric, neurologic, or

    medical morbidities. Two cohorts were examined. The

    larger cohort comprised 230 subjects (66% women) with a

    mean age of 67 8.0 years, a mean MMSE score of 29.1

    1.3, and a mean Hamilton Depression Rating Scale

    (HDRS) score of 3.4 3.6. These MMSE scores are, of

    course, indicative of a normal level of cognition in these

    very well-educated subjects (mean, 15.9 2.5 years of

    education), and the HDRS scores indicate a very low level

    of affective symptomatology, well within the normal rangeand well below the range traditionally associated with dys-

    phoria, as well as major depression. The subjects comprised

    81% SCI subjects (GDS stage 2) and 19% non-SCI normal

    subjects (GDS stage 1). Subjects were followed for a mean

    period of 8.4 3.9 years.

    It was found that the presence of SCI (GDS stage 2 at

    baseline) was associated both with subsequent decline and

    with being in a diagnostically unstable group that declined

    to an MCI diagnosis but subsequently reverted to a normal

    diagnosis. In addition, being in the unstable outcome group

    was associated with a significantly higher baseline HDRS

    score, although the magnitude of the HDRS score at base-

    S100 B. Reisberg et al / Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    4/11

    line was still low (mean baseline HDRS, 4.6 4.8). In the

    unstable diagnostic group, the five HDRS items with the

    highest standardized mean values were those describing (1)

    psychological symptoms of anxiety, (2) problems with

    maintaining sleep, (3) general somatic symptoms (including

    loss of energy), (4) problems with falling asleep, and (5)impairment in work and activities (including fatigue, weak-

    ness, and loss of interest). Importantly, these same items

    were also those contributing to the Hamilton scores in the

    broader baseline subject cohort comprising non-decliners

    and stable declining subjects, as well as the unstable out-

    come cohort.

    A second cohort was also studied by Glodzik-Sobanska

    et al [25], which consisted of a subset of the 230 subjects

    who had an evaluation of the severity of memory com-

    plaints assessed on the Memory Complaints Questionnaire

    (MAC-Q) [26]. This is a self-rated questionnaire with five

    questions addressing daily activities and one question ad-

    dressing overall memory functioning. Functioning is com-

    pared with that at 18 to 20 years. The characteristics of this

    83-subject subgroup did not differ significantly from the

    broader 230-subject cohort on any parameter. Analysis of

    this cohort subset indicated that in addition to the prior

    finding of the presence of subjective complaints of memoryimpairment and of a higher HDRS score being associated

    with membership in the unstable group, (1) a greater mag-

    nitude of complaints on the MAC-Q was associated with

    being in the unstable diagnostic group, which declined to an

    MCI diagnosis but ultimately reverted to a normal diagno-

    sis, and (2) the magnitude of complaints on the MAC-Q did

    not add to the other variables in predicting membership in

    the declining subject group.

    Important findings from this study of Glodzik-Sobanska

    et al [25] are that increased intensity of memory complaints,

    as well as increased affective and anxiety symptomatology,

    can be associated with a potentially reversible decline in

    Fig 2. The hypothesized mean duration of the subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) stage, synonymous with Global Deterioration Scale (GDS) stage 2, was

    published in Reisberg, B. (1986). Dementia: a systematic approach to identifying reversible causes. Geriatrics, 41(4), 30-46. The observed results for the

    duration of the SCI stage, synonymous with GDS stage 2, are calculated from Prichep, L.S., et al. (1994). Quantitative EEG correlates of cognitive

    deterioration in the elderly. Neurobiology of Aging, 15, 85-90. Result: For a stage with a 15 year duration, the observed result differed from the hypothesized

    result by 2 %. Copyright 2007 Barry Reisberg, M.D. All rights reserved.

    S101 B. Reisberg et al / Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    5/11

    SCI subjects. Hence, this study supports the very complex

    relationship between anxiety and symptoms sometimes

    identified as being affective, such as sleep disturbances, and

    SCI, as well as the association of SCI with subsequent

    cognitive decline to MCI and to dementia.

    3.2.2. Pathophysiologic relationship between affective

    disorder and ADRecent studies of the pathophysiologic basis both of AD

    and of affective disorder and related conditions have re-

    vealed fundamental interrelationships that appear to provide

    an explanation for the findings in SCI studies (Table 2). As

    outlined inTable 2, both depression and AD are character-

    ized by hippocampal volumetric losses [3,2733]. Both con-

    ditions have been associated with neuronal and/or gray

    matter losses in the hippocampus and elsewhere in the brain

    [4,3439].Interestingly, with respect to cell-cycle reactivation, the

    findings appear to be different in affective disorder and in

    Table 1

    Relationships between affective and anxiety symptoms and SCI

    Finding Reference Exclusion of MCI Subjects

    Presentation

    Complaints of poor memory more associated with depressed mood than

    objective disabilities

    Bolla et al [19], 1991 No

    Older depression subjects report more memory problems thancommunity-residing subjects or dementia subjects

    OConnor, et al [20], 1990; Feehanet al [21], 1991

    No

    Anxiety and depressive symptoms are most strongly associated with

    memory complaints

    Jorm et al [22], 2001 No

    Older women with subjective memory complaints had higher anxiety

    and depression scores than healthy older subjects

    Lautenschlager et al [23], 2005 Yes

    Outcome

    Treatment of depression, in depressed subjects, reduces memory

    complaints

    Plotkin et al [24], 1985 NA

    Presence of memory complaints is associated both with subsequent

    decline and with an unstable outcome wherein subjects decline but

    subsequently return to a normal diagnosis

    Glodzik-Sobanska et al [25], 2007 Yes, MCI was excluded at baseline.

    MCI was studied as an outcome

    measure.

    Increased intensity of memory complaints and a higher level of affective

    symptoms in nondepressed, healthy older persons, GDS stage 1 or 2,

    was associated with an unstable outcome, wherein subjects, whenfollowed, received a diagnosis of MCI but subsequently reverted back

    to a normal (GDS stage 1 or 2) diagnosis.

    Glodzik-Sobanska et al [25], 2007 Yes, MCI was excluded at baseline.

    MCI was studied as an outcome

    measure.

    Abbreviations: SCI, subjective cognitive impairment; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; GDS, Global Deterioration Scale; NA, not applicable.

    Table 2

    Pathophysiologic relationships between affective disorder, and related predisposing conditions to affective disorder, and AD

    Pathophysiologic Condition Affective DisorderRelated Findings AD Pathology

    Volumetric loss in the hippocampus Post-traumatic stress disorder [27] and depression [28]

    have been associated with hippocampal volume losses

    Hippocampal volumetric losses are visible with

    neuroimaging [2933] and with neuropathologic

    study [3].

    Neuronal cellular loss in the

    hippocampus and elsewhere in

    the brain

    Cortical gray matter reductions in major depression [34].

    Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for

    behavioral effects of antidepressants [35].

    Neuronal loss in various specific brain regions

    [36] including, notably, in the hippocampus

    [4,3739].Neuronal cell-cycle reactivation Various antidepressants increase neurogenesis in the

    subgranular zone of the dentate gyrus of the

    hippocampus [35,40]. Conversely, stress, a risk factor

    for depression, has been associated with a decrease in

    hippocampal granule cell neurogenesis [41].

    Reactivation of the cell cycle in pathologically

    involved brain regions, including the

    hippocampus, without associated cell division

    [4246].

    Tau phosphorylation Stress, a physiologic condition associated with depression,

    has been associated with tau phosphorylation [47].

    Cerebral ischemia and infarction, conditions that are

    strongly associated with and/or can produce depression,

    have been associated with hyperphosphorylated tau

    [48].

    Hyperphosphorylated tau is a characteristic feature

    of AD, resulting in neurotubular dysfunction

    and neurofibrillar pathology [49,50]. AD has

    been strongly associated with cerebrovascular

    pathology and with cerebrovascular risk factors

    [51].

    Cerebral white matter changes (eg,

    hyperintensities, leukoariosis)

    Association with depression [52]. Association with AD [53].

    Abbreviation: AD, Alzheimers disease.

    S102 B. Reisberg et al / Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    6/11

    AD, particularly in outcome. As noted above, there is neu-

    ronal loss in both conditions. In depression, the treatment

    for the neuronal loss is cell-cycle reactivation, specifically

    in the form of antidepressant treatment, which appears to

    induce neurogenesis in the subgranular zone of the dentate

    gyrus of the hippocampus [35,40]. In depression, this suc-

    cessful cell-cycle reactivation and cell division are associ-ated with remission. The reason antidepressant treatments

    appear to take time to be effective is that the hippocampal

    cellular restoration is believed to take some weeks before

    the effects of this restoration are fully manifest.

    In AD, neuronal cellular loss is also associated with

    cell-cycle reactivation in the hippocampus and elsewhere

    [4246]. However, this reactivation does not produce neu-

    rogenesis (so far as is known) and is believed to be delete-

    rious to the terminally differentiated neurons. The cell-cycle

    reactivation in AD has been related to both hyperphospho-

    rylation of tau with resultant neurotubular dysfunction [42]

    and to cellular death.In addition to the association already noted between

    affective disorder and AD, both conditions have been

    strongly related to the occurrence of cerebrovascular disease

    and cerebrovascular risk factors [48,51] and to the occur-

    rence of cerebral white matter changes [52,53]. Some of

    these changes, notably cerebral ischemia and cerebral in-

    farction, as well as stress models of affective disorder,

    indicate possible relationships between depression and tau

    hyperphosphorylation [47,48], a fundamental pathologic

    change in AD [49,50].

    3.3. Need for a new diagnostic entity, primary idiopathicSCI

    Given the numerous and complex interrelationships be-

    tween SCI and anxiety and depression, as well as with AD

    and many other potential pathologic entities, eg, cerebro-

    vascular disease, head trauma, for research to proceed ef-

    fectively, new definitions that can be commonly agreed on

    might be necessary. In AD, despite or, actually, because of

    intimate associations with, for example, cerebrovascular

    pathology [51], categories of probable and possible AD

    have been created to permit research to proceed expedi-

    tiously [18]. Analogous procedures need to be followed

    with a proposed definition of primary idiopathic SCI. Stud-

    ies need to be conducted that apply the criteria of McKhann

    et al [18] for probable and possible AD, with the exception

    that dementia and also MCI need to be excluded. As noted

    earlier, evidence already exists and is accruing that indicates

    that this primary idiopathic SCI entity might be associated

    with eventual AD. However, until such evidence becomes

    more definitive, an etiopathogenic basis of this primaryidiopathic SCI entity should not be assumed.Table 3 out-

    lines an initial proposed definition for this entity.

    The SCI terminology for the proposed entity is itself

    somewhat novel. Most commonly, this condition has been

    referred to in the literature as subjective memory com-

    plaints. The term cognitive is preferable to memory,

    because the range of presentations of this entity is pres-

    ently not clearly known and should be studied. For ex-

    ample, many persons with these symptoms complain of

    concentration disturbances. Also, the role of visuospatial

    deficits and language deficits as possible modes of pre-

    sentation of this condition should be explored beforeprematurely limiting this SCI entity to the memory do-

    main of presentation.

    Also, the term impairment, as opposed to complaints,

    appears to be preferable. These persons do not necessarily

    complain of deficits. Rather, they acknowledge perceived

    deficits in cognition when queried. In fact, persons with

    these symptoms might, or might not, share their perceived

    deficits with their spouses or other intimates, and they

    might, or might not, present in memory clinic settings. Also,

    the term complaints is somewhat pejorative and should be

    eschewed for this reason. These persons are not, in general,

    complainers.

    3.4. Primary idiopathic SCI: Differences from age-

    matched, cognitively normal persons without SCI and

    differences from MCI cohorts

    There is emerging information on differences from oth-

    erwise healthy, SCI cohorts and age-matched persons who

    are free of SCI (Table 4). Differences in psychometric

    assessments [54], hippocampal gray matter [55], hippocam-

    pal volumes [56], cerebral metabolism [57], and urinary

    cortisol levels [58] are presently being reported.

    Perhaps the most clear, dramatic, and interpretable

    changes are being reported in terms of cerebral metabolism.

    Table 3

    Proposed criteria for primary idiopathic subjective cognitive impairment

    1. Presence of subjective cognitive deficits.

    2. Belief that ones cognitive capacities have declined in comparison with 5 or 10 years previously.

    3. Absence of significant medical, neurologic, or psychiatric conditions, including depression and anxiety disorders, that might interfere with cognition.*

    4. Absence of overt cognitive deficits. These overt deficits might be elicited in the context of a detailed clinical interview. They might also be evident

    to the spouse or other informants.

    5. Cognitive performance in a general normal range.

    6. Absence of dementia.

    * This condition must be met for a categorization as probable. The presence of one or more of these conditions in persons fulfilling the other inclusion

    and exclusion criteria is compatible with a possible diagnostic categorization.

    S103 B. Reisberg et al / Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    7/11

    Table 4

    Cross-sectional studies indicating differences between primary idiopathic SCI subjects and hierarchically adjacent diagnostic categories

    Modality Subject Selection Finding (reference)

    Studies of normal, non-SCI subjects in comparison with SCI subjects

    Neuropsychologic tests Individuals with other neurologic, medical, or

    psychiatric conditions excluded

    Subjects with symptoms of memory loss but no cognitive

    impairment (S NCI) performed significantly worse than age

    matched controls on 6 of 18 cognitive tests. These were (1)Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence matrices; (2)

    delayed recall, (2 versions); (3) immediate recall; (4) the trail

    making test B; and (5) the trail making test difference score

    (Archer, et al., 2006) [54]

    Hippocampal gray matter

    density on MRI

    Exclusion criteria included medical, psychiatric,

    or neurologic conditions that could adversely

    effect brain structure or cognition

    Subjects with cognitive complaints show reduced gray matter

    hippocampal density on MRI in comparison with healthy

    controls (Saykin et al [55], 2006)

    Medial temporal lobe

    volumes assessed with

    MRI

    No neurologic or psychiatric comorbidity.

    Subjects with memory complaints had higher

    education levels and more depressive

    symptoms than age-matched controls.

    Subjects with subjective memory complaints had smaller left

    hippocampal volumes than controls. No differences were

    found in right hippocampal volumes or parahippocampal gyrus

    volumes (van der Flicr et al [56], 2004)

    Positron emission

    tomography study of

    cerebral metabolism

    Individuals without (GDS stage 1) or with

    (GDS stage 2) SCI were studied. Individuals

    with other medical, neurologic, or psychiatricconditions known to affect cognitive

    functioning were excluded.

    Subjects with subjective memory complaints showed cerebral

    metabolic rates for glucose reductions bilaterally in the

    parahippocampal gyrus and in the middle temporal gyrus; inthe left hemisphere; in the inferior parietal lobe, in the inferior

    frontal gyrus, fusiform gyrus, and thalamus; and in the right

    putamen. The parahippocampal gyrus showed the greatest

    reduction (18%) (Mosconi et al [57], in press)

    Interaction of cerebral

    metabolic change with

    apolipoprotein E allele

    status

    Same as above. Subjects with subjective complaints (GDS stage 2) with an

    apolipoprotein E, e4 allele, had lower cerebral glucose

    metabolism rates than e4 negative SCI subjects, e4 positive

    non-SCI subjects, or e4 negative non-SCI subjects. The most

    severe reductions in the e4 positive SCI subjects were in the

    parahippocampal gyrus (18% decrease in comparison to the

    average of the metabolism of the other 3 groups) (Mosconi, et

    al [57], in press)

    Urinary cortisol levels Subjects with significant neurologic, medical, or

    psychiatric disease were excluded. Also,

    subjects taking glucocorticoids, with MCI, orwith diabetes, were excluded. Subjects with

    SCI (GDS stage 2) and healthy non-SCI

    subjects (GDS stage1) were studied.

    Subjects who were free of memory complaints (GDS stage 1),

    had lower 12-hour urinary cortisol levels than SCI (GDS stage

    2) subjects (Wolf et al [58], 2005)

    Studies of SCI subjects in comparison with MCI subjects

    Mental status, dementia scale

    (including functioning and

    personality), and

    psychometric test battery

    evaluations

    Subjects with significant medical, psychiatric,

    neurologic, or neuroradiologic conditions

    apart from brain agingrelated SCI or MCI

    were excluded. Subjects with SCI (GDS

    stage 2) or MCI (GDS stage 3) were studied.

    MCI subjects were significantly more impaired than SCI subjects

    on the MMSE; the Blessed et al dementia scale; the Blessed et

    al information test, memory test, and concentration test; in

    WAIS vocabulary scores; on the paragraph, initial and delayed

    recall tests and the paired associates, initial and delayed recall

    tests; on the designs test; on the shopping list selective

    reminding task; on performance in digits backwards recall; on

    the digit symbol substitution test; and on finger tapping speed

    (the mean of the right and left sides). There was also

    significantly more impairment for the MCI subjects incomparison with the SCI subjects on a comprehensive

    psychometric test battery score. (Reisberg et al [12], 1988)

    Mental status and

    neuropsychological tests

    Individuals with other neurologic, medical, or

    psychiatric conditions excluded.

    MCI subjects showed significantly more impairment than

    subjects with symptoms of memory loss but no cognitive

    impairment on the MMSE; on tests of immediate and delayed

    recall, on the California Verbal Learning Test Recognition

    assessment, on the Washington Recognition Memory Test, and

    on the Rey-Osterreich figure copy test, immediate recall test

    and the delayed recall test (Archer et al [54], 2006)

    Brain gray matter density on

    MRI

    Subjects with medical, psychiatric, or

    neurologic conditions that would adversely

    affect brain structure or cognition were

    excluded.

    Subjects with MCI showed significantly lower gray matter

    density in the left middle frontal gyrus and in the right inferior

    frontal gyrus (Saykin et al [55], 2006)

    Abbreviations: MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; SCI, subjective cognitive impairment; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; GDS, Global DeteriorationScale; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination.

    S104 B. Reisberg et al / Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    8/11

    There is an 18% decrement in cerebral metabolism in the

    parahippocampal gyrus that is observed in the SCI subjects

    in comparison with comparably aged, healthy persons who

    are free of SCI. A similar difference is seen for the inter-

    action between SCI and the presence of the apolipoprotein

    E(APOE), e4 allele. SCI, APOEe4 positive persons have an

    18% metabolic rate decrement in the parahippocampal gy-rus in comparison with all other subject groups (ie, APOE

    e4 (), no-SCI; APOEe4 (), no-SCI; and APOE e4 (),

    with SCI, subject groups).

    Differences between SCI subjects and MCI subjects on

    mental status, dementia scale, and numerous test measures

    have long been recognized [12]. These differences are once

    again being recognized with the renaissance of interest in

    this important area of behavioral investigation [54]. Differ-

    ences between SCI and MCI subject groups in terms of

    neuroimaging observations are beginning to be observed

    [55].

    3.5. Predicting outcome in subjects with SCI

    Remarkably, seemingly excellent, reliable, and nonin-

    vasive predictors of outcome of SCI subjects are begin-

    ning to emerge. This is very important because this ap-

    pears to be a stage that lasts about 15 years before MCI.

    Therefore, treatment intervention studies might, of ne-

    cessity, need to rely on sensitive, surrogate markers of

    change. Positron emission tomographic studies of brain

    glucose utilization, particularly in the hippocampal re-

    gion, have shown great promise in this area [57,59].

    However, such measures entail some radiation exposurefor healthy older persons with SCI.

    Prichep et al [6] examined outcome in 44 subjects with

    SCI (GDS stage 2) followed for a minimum of 7 years.

    Subjects were studied at baseline with quantitative elec-

    troencephalograms (QEEGs). Outcome was defined di-

    chotomously as (1) decline to either MCI or dementia

    during the follow-up period or, alternatively, (2) the

    absence of decline. Of the 44 subjects studied, 27 de-

    clined at follow-up. We found that the baseline QEEGs of

    decliners differed significantly from those of the non-

    decliners. Differences were categorized by increases in

    theta power, slowing of mean frequency, and changes incovariance among regions in the decliners in comparison

    with the non-decliners. In a logistic regression analysis

    baseline QEEG measures predicted future decline with an

    overall accuracy of 90% (R2 0.9, P .001). These

    findings indicate that baseline QEEG measures might

    prove useful not only in prognostic assessments but also,

    perhaps, in the measurement of therapeutic responses to

    proposed interventions for persons with these commonly

    occurring SCI symptoms.

    Recently we conducted a different study with the ar-

    chived data from Prichep et al [6]. Specifically, three out-

    come groups were studied: (1) no change, (2) decline to

    MCI, and (3) decline to dementia. Data were converted to

    standardized low-resolution electromagnetic tomographic

    analysis (sLORETA) images. Five subjects were randomly

    selected by computer from each of the three outcome

    groups. Images for maximal theta frequency were pro-

    duced. These images are shown in Figure 3, which shows

    clear differences between these outcome groups at base-line. Progressive increments in maximal theta frequency

    in specific brain regions are seen with the following

    outcome hierarchy: SCI MCI dementia [60]. These

    differences are particularly remarkable considering that

    they are baseline images grouped by subsequent outcome

    nearly a decade later. Hence, it appears these sLORETA

    images could likely be useful in future therapeutic SCI

    trials.

    3.6. Treating SCI

    At the present time there are no approved treatments for

    MCI. Therefore, should investigators begin to contemplate

    treating the even earlier SCI stage? There are many reasons

    for a positive answer. These include the following:

    (1) It might, in fact, prove to be less difficult finding

    treatments at earlier points in the evolution of ulti-

    mate AD.

    (2) Persons are already self-medicating for these symp-

    toms. The scientific community has an obligation to

    comment on the efficacy and safety of these proce-

    dures.

    (3) As briefly reviewed, the methodologies that can sup-

    port such studies are becoming available andaccessible.

    4. Conclusion

    SCI has been an underrecognized entity on the part of

    the professional community. Although persons with this

    condition frequently come to memory clinics, there are

    no current pharmacologic treatment trials for these per-

    sons. However, a long-term follow-up study is now pub-

    lished confirming less systematic observations indicating

    that this condition, when properly defined, is a stage

    lasting about 15 years before the presently well-recog-nized MCI condition. However, in the clinic setting, this

    can be seen as the stage when the patient knows but the

    doctor doesnt know.

    Similarly, persons with SCI presently try to treat their

    symptoms themselves. This condition, perhaps more than

    any other, is a reason for persons self-medicating, generally

    with various non-prescription substances. The level of pub-

    lic discourse is extraordinarily low. For example, presently

    in the United States, a product that we will call by the

    pseudonym Concentration is advertised widely and regu-

    larly on national television network stations. The constitu-

    ents of Concentration are never stated. However, there is

    S105 B. Reisberg et al / Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    9/11

    a person in the advertisement who says, They are giving it

    away free. It must be good. There is also a man whointroduces himself as the president of the company who

    says, I take [Concentration] everyday and I wouldnt go a

    day without it.

    There is an opportunity at present to raise the intellectual

    level of discourse. Basic studies on the nature of SCI need

    to continue to be conducted. Simultaneously, the first treat-

    ment trials of SCI should be initiated.

    Acknowledgments

    Supported in part by United States Department of Health

    and Human Services (DHHS) grants P30 AG08051,

    AG03051, AG09127, AG11505, AG022374, AG13616,

    and AG12101 from the National Institute on Aging and bygrant MH32577 from the National Institute of Mental

    Health of the U.S. National Institutes of Health, by grants

    90AZ2791, 90AM2552, and 90AR2160 from the U.S.

    DHHS Administration on Aging, by grant M01 RR00096

    from the General Clinical Research Center Program of the

    National Center for Research Resources of the U.S. Na-

    tional Institutes of Health, by the Fisher Center for Alzhei-

    mers Disease Research Foundation, and by grants from Mr

    William Silberstein and Mr Leonard Litwin. Additional

    support is acknowledged from the Hagedorn Foundation,

    the Harry and Jennie Slayton Foundation, and the Sonya

    Samberg Family Trust.

    Fig 3. Standardized low resolution electromagnetic tomographic analyses (sLORETA) images at maximal theta frequency at baseline for subjects with

    subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) (Global Deterioration Scale [GDS] stage 2). Shown are sLORETA images at maximal theta frequency at baseline for

    subjects who were followed for outcome at 7 years. From a pool of 44 subjects in the study [6], five subjects from three outcome groups were randomly

    chosen by computer. The outcome groups shown in the figure are (1) No change (top row) (n 5), if the subjects remained in a normative diagnostic category

    either with or without SCI (GDS stage 1 or 2) at the time of follow-up; (2) Decline (middle row) (n 5), if the subjects declined to mild cognitive impairment

    (MCI) (GDS stage 3) during the follow-up period; and (3) Convert (bottom row) (n 5), if the subjects converted to dementia (GDS stage 4) during the

    follow-up period. Clear and highly significant differences are seen at baseline for maximal theta frequency in the three outcome groups. Progressive

    increments are seen in maximal theta frequency in specific brain regions with the following outcome hierarchy: SCI MCI dementia. It appears that

    outcome in normal SCI subjects can be predicted many years later from baseline sLORETA readings. These sLORETA procedures are noninvasive (Adapted

    from Reisberg et al, Alzheimers & Dementia 2007;3:S1856).

    S106 B. Reisberg et al / Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    10/11

    References

    [1] Reisberg B. Dementia: A systematic approach to identifying revers-

    ible causes. Geriatrics 1986;41(4):3046.

    [2] Reisberg B, Ferris SH, Franssen E, Shulman E, Monteiro I, Sclan SG,

    et al. Mortality and temporal course of probable Alzheimers disease:

    a five-year prospective study. Int Psychogeriatr 1996;8:291311.

    [3] Bobinski M, Wegiel J, Wisniewski HM, Tarnawski M, Reisberg B,

    Mlodzik B, et al. Atrophy of hippocampal formation subdivisions

    correlates with stage and duration of Alzheimer disease. Dementia

    1995;6:20510.

    [4] Bobinski M, Wegiel J, Tarnawski M, Bobinski M, Reisberg B, de

    Leon MJ, et al. Relationships between regional neuronal loss and

    neurofibrillary changes in the hippocampal formation and duration

    and severity of Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1997;

    56:41420.

    [5] Kluger A, Ferris SH, Golomb J, Mittelman MS, Reisberg B. Neuro-

    psychological prediction of decline to dementia in nondemented el-

    derly. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1999;12:16879.

    [6] Prichep LS, John ER, Ferris SH, Rausch L, Fang Z, Cancro R, et al.

    Prediction of longitudinal cognitive decline in normal elderly using

    electrophysiological imaging. Neurobiol Aging 2006;27:47181.

    [7] Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, Crook T. The global deterioration

    scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. Am J Psy-

    chiatry 1982;139:11369.

    [8] Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, Crook T. The Global Deterio-

    ration Scale (GDS). Psychopharmacol Bull 1988;24:6613.

    [9] Reisberg B. Functional assessment staging (FAST). Psychopharma-

    col Bull 1988;24:6539.

    [10] Sclan SG, Reisberg B. Functional assessment staging (FAST) in

    Alzheimers disease: reliability, validity and ordinality. Int Psychoge-

    riatr 1992;4:5569.

    [11] Reisberg B, Ferris SH, Anand R, de Leon MJ, Schneck MK, Crook

    T. Clinical assessment of cognitive decline in normal aging and

    primary degenerative dementia: concordant ordinal measures. In:

    Pinchot P, Berner P, Wolf R, Thau K, eds. Psychiatry. vol.5. New

    York: Plenum Press; 1985. p. 3338.

    [12] Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, Sinaiko E, Franssen E, Kluger A,

    et al. Stage-specific behavioral, cognitive, and in vivo changes in

    community residing subjects with age-associated memory impair-

    ment and primary degenerative dementia of the Alzheimer type. Drug

    Dev Res 1988;15:10114.

    [13] Flicker C, Ferris SH, Reisberg B. Mild cognitive impairment in the

    elderly: predictors of dementia. Neurology 1991;41:10069.

    [14] Gauthier S, Reisberg B, Zaudig M, Petersen RC, Ritchie K, Broich K,

    et al. Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet 2006;367:126270.

    [15] Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical

    method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.

    J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:18998.

    [16] Reisberg B, Gauthier S. Current evidence for subjective cognitive

    impairment (SCI) as the pre-mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stage

    of subsequently manifest Alzheimers disease. Int Psychogeriatr

    2008;20:116.

    [17] Reisberg B, Ferris S, de Leon MJ, Torossian C, Kadiyala S, Zhu W.

    Subjective cognitive impairment: the pre-mild cognitive impairment

    stage of brain degeneration: longitudinal outcome after a mean of 7

    years follow-up. Neuropsychopharmacology 2005;30(Suppl 1):S81.

    [18] McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan

    EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimers disease: report of the

    NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of

    Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimers disease.

    Neurology 1984;34:93944.

    [19] Bolla KI, Lindgren KN, Bonaccorsy C, Bleecker ML. Memory com-

    plaints in older adults: fact or fiction? Arch Neurol 1991;48:614.

    [20] OConnor DW, Pollitt PA, Roth M, Brook PB, Reiss BB. Memory

    complaints and impairment in normal, depressed, and demented el-

    derly persons identified in a community survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry

    1990;47:2247.

    [21] Feehan M, Knight RG, Partridge FM. Cognitive complaint and test

    performance in elderly patients suffering depression or dementia. Int

    J Geriatr Psychiatry 1991;6:28793.

    [22] Jorm AF, Christensen H, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Henderson AS.Memory complaints as a precursor of memory impairment in older

    people: a longitudinal analysis over 7-8 years. Psychol Med 2001;31:

    4419.

    [23] Lautenschlager NT, Flicker L, Vasikaran S, Leedman P, Almeida OP.

    Subjective memory complaints with and without objective memory

    impairment: relationship with risk factors for dementia. Am J Geriatr

    Psychiatry 2005;13:7314.

    [24] Plotkin DA, Mintz J, Jarvik LF. Subjective memory complaints in

    geriatric depression. Am J Psychiatry 1985;142:11035.

    [25] Glodzik-Sobanska L, Reisberg B, De Santi S, Bab JS, Pirraglia E,

    Rich KE, et al. Subjective memory complaints: presence, severity and

    future outcome in normal older subjects. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord

    2007;24:17784.

    [26] Crook TH, Feher EP, Larrabee GJ. Assessment of memory complaintin age-associated memory impairment: the MAC-Q. Int Psychogeriatr

    1992;4:16576.

    [27] Gurvits TV, Shenton ME, Hokama H, Ohta H, Lasko NB, Gilbertson

    MW, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging study of hippocampal vol-

    ume in chronic, combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol

    Psychiatry 1996;40:10919.

    [28] Sheline YI, Wang PW, Gado MH, Csernansky JG, Vannier MW.

    Hippocampal atrophy in recurrent major depression. Proc Natl Acad

    Sci U S A 1996;93:390813.

    [29] Seab JP, Jagust WJ, Wong ST, Roos MS, Reed BR, Budinger TF.

    Quantitative NMR measurements of hippocampal atrophy in Alzhei-

    mers disease. Magn Reson Med 1988;8:2008.

    [30] de Leon MJ, George AE, Stylopoulos LA, Smith G, Miller DC. Early

    marker for Alzheimers disease: the atrophic hippocampus. Lancet

    1989;16:6723.

    [31] Kesslak JP, Nalcioglu O, Cotman CW. Quantification of magnetic

    resonance scans for hippocampal and parahippocampal atrophy in

    Alzheimers disease. Neurology 1991;41:514.

    [32] Jack CR, Petersen RC, OBrien PC, Tangalos EG. MR-based hip-

    pocampal volumetry in the diagnosis of Alzheimers disease. Neu-

    rology 1992;42:1838.

    [33] de Leon MJ, Golomb J, George AE, Convit A, Tarshish CY, McRae

    T, et al. The radiologic prediction of Alzheimer disease: the atrophic

    hippocampal formation. AJNR 1993;14:897906.

    [34] Shah PJ, Ebmeier KP, Glabus MF, Goodwin GM. Cortical grey

    matter reductions associated with treatment-resistant chronic unipolar

    depression: controlled magnetic resonance imaging study. Br J Psy-

    chiatry 1998;172:52732.

    [35] Santarelli L, Saxe M, Gross C, Battaglia F, Dulawa S, et al. Require-

    ment of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of an-

    tidepressants. Science 2003;301:8059.

    [36] Coleman PD, Flood DG. Neuron numbers and dendritic extent in

    normal aging and Alzheimers disease. Neurobiol Aging 1987;8:521

    45.

    [37] Hyman BT, Van Hoesen GW, Damasio AR, Barnes CL. Alzheimers

    disease: cell-specific pathology isolates the hippocampal formation.

    Science 1984;225:116870.

    [38] West MJ, Gundersen HJ. Unbiased stereological estimation of the

    number of neurons in the human hippocampus. J Comp Neurol

    1990;296:122.

    [39] West MJ. Regionally specific loss of neurons in the aging human

    hippocampus. Neurobiol Aging1993;14:28793.

    S107 B. Reisberg et al / Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108

  • 8/9/2019 SCIreviewA&D08

    11/11

    [40] Garca-Verdugo JM, Doetsch F, Wichterle H, Lim DA, Alvarez-

    Buylla A. Architecture and cell types of the adult subventricular zone:

    in search of the stem cells. J Neurobiol 1998;36:23448.

    [41] Gould E, Tanapat P, McEwen BS, Flgge G, Fuchs E. Proliferation

    of granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is

    diminished by stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1998;95:316871.

    [42] Nagy Z, Esiri MM, Smith AD. The cell division cycle and the

    pathophysiology of Alzheimers disease. Neuroscience 1998;87:7319.

    [43] Busser J, Geldmacher DS, Herrup K. Ectopic cell cycle proteins

    predict the sites of neuronal cell death in Alzheimers disease brain.

    J Neurosci 1998;18:28017.

    [44] Arendt T. Alzheimers disease as a loss of differentiation control in a

    subset of neurons that retain immature features in the adult brain.

    Neurobiol Aging 2000;21:78396.

    [45] Husseman JW, Nochlin D, Vincent I. Mitotic activation: a convergent

    mechanism for a cohort of neurodegenerative diseases. Neurobiol

    Aging 2000;21:81528.

    [46] Zhu X, Rottkamp CA, Raina AK, Brewer GJ, Ghanbari HA, Boux H,

    et al. Neuronal CDK7 in hippocampus is related to aging and Alz-

    heimer disease. Neurobiol Aging 2000;21:80713.

    [47] Rissman RA, Lee KF, Vale W, Sawchenko PE. Corticotropin-releas-ing factor receptors differentially regulate stress-induced tau phos-

    phorylation. J Neurosci 2007;27:655262.

    [48] Wen, Y, Yang SH, Liu R, Perez EJ, Brun-Zinkernagel AM, Koulen

    P, et al. Cdk5 is involved in NFT-like tauopathy induced by transient

    cerebral ischemia in female rats. Biochim Biophys Acta 2007;1772:

    47383.

    [49] Grundke-Iqbal I, Iqbal K, Tung YC, Quinlan M, Wisniewski HM,

    Binder LI. Abnormal phosphorylation of the microtubule-associated

    protein tau (tau) in Alzheimer cytoskeletal pathology. Proc Natl Acad

    Sci U S A 1986;83:49137.

    [50] Avila J, Lucas JJ, Perez M, Hernandez F. Role of tau protein in both

    physiological and pathological conditions. Physiol Rev 2004;84:361

    84.

    [51] OBrien JT, Erkinjuntti T, Reisberg B, Roman G, Sawada T, Pantoni

    L, et al. Vascular cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol 2003;2:89

    98.

    [52] OBrien JT, Firbank MJ, Krishnan MS, van Straaten EC, van der Flier

    WM, Petrovic K. White matter hyperintensities rather than lacunar

    infarcts are associated with depressive symptoms in older people: the

    LADIS study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006;14:83441.

    [53] Burton EJ, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, Firbank MJ, OBrien JT. Progres-

    sion of white matter hyperintensities in Alzheimer disease, dementia

    with lewy bodies, and Parkinson disease dementia: a comparison with

    normal aging. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006;14:8429.

    [54] Archer HA, Macfarlane F, Price S, Moore EK, Pepple T, Cutler D, et

    al. Do symptoms of memory impairment correspond to cognitive

    impairment: a cross sectional study of a clinical cohort. Int J Geriatr

    Psychiatry 2006;21:120612.

    [55] Saykin AJ, Wishart HA, Rabin LA, Santulli RB, Flashman LA, WestJD, et al. Older adults with cognitive complaints show brain atrophy

    similar to that of amnestic MCI. Neurology 2006;67:83442.

    [56] van der Flier WM, van Buchem MA, Weverling-Rijnsburger AW,

    Mutsaers ER, Bollen EL, Admiraal-Behloul F, et al. Memory com-

    plaints in patients with normal cognition are associated with smaller

    hippocampal volumes. J Neurol 2004;251:6715.

    [57] Mosconi L, De Santi S, Brys M, Tsui WH, Pirraglia E, Glodzik-

    Sobanska L, et al. Hypometabolism and altered cerebrospinal fluid

    markers in normal apolipoprotein E E4 carriers with subjective mem-

    ory complaints. Biol Psychiatry (in press).

    [58] Wolf OT, Dziobek I, McHugh P, Sweat V, de Leon MJ, Javier E, et

    al. Subjective memory complaints in aging are associated with ele-vated cortisol levels. Neurobiol Aging 2005;26:135763.

    [59] de Leon MJ, Convit A, Wolf OT, Tarshish CY, DeSanti S, Rusinek

    H, et al. Prediction of cognitive decline in normal elderly subjects

    with 2-[18F] fluoro-2-deoxy- D-glucose/positron-emission tomogra-

    phy (FDG/PET). Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001;98:1096671.

    [60] Reisberg B, Torossian C, Kadiyala S, Jamil IA, John R, Prichep L.

    Prediction of decline in normal elderly to MCI or dementia: a 9-year

    prospective longitudinal analysis using standardized low resolution

    electromagnetic tomographic analysis (SLORETA). Alzheimers &

    Dementia 2007;3:S1856.

    Memoriam to my dear colleague, Professor Leon Thal,

    MDThis publication is written to honor the memory of our

    dear and valued colleague, Dr Leon Thal. With his many

    activities and his leading role in the U.S. National Institute

    on Aging consortium on pharmacologic trials in the treat-

    ment of Alzheimers disease, Leon was an inspiration for all

    of us who are actively working toward the improved treat-

    ment and, ultimately, the prevention of this disease.

    On a personal note, I would like to acknowledge that

    Leon and I come from similar backgrounds and grew up

    very close to each other in Brooklyn, New York. Leon went

    to medical school at the State University of New York

    Downstate Medical Center, located less than a mile downthe road, on New York Avenue, from where my parents and

    I lived contemporaneously. Later, of course, we both chose

    very similar medical and scientific challenges.

    Leons work with early pharmacotherapeutic trials of

    cholinesterase inhibitors, in organizing and successfully

    running the first and only U.S. Alzheimers Disease Coop-

    erative Studies group, and in many other areas were galva-

    nizing events that significantly advanced our field. I will

    always remember his perceptive comment after I presented

    the then novel data from our multicenter memantine trial at

    the World Alzheimers Congress in Washington, DC, in

    2000. It is hard for all of us to imagine that Leon is gone.We miss him. We endeavor to honor him with these works,

    which his life inspired.

    Barry Reisberg, MD

    S108 B. Reisberg et al / Alzheimers & Dementia 4 (2008) S98S108