21
Christina Schwaller 4 August, 2010 No Minor Feat: Reflections on the Cognitive Science Minor After a long absence, I returned to Truman State University in the spring of 2007, this time as a linguistics major. That semester, I took a course on cognitive psychology, and it fascinated me. I began to take courses toward a psychology minor, which I would fulfill by taking as many cognition-related courses as possible. Shortly thereafter, the cognitive science interdisciplinary minor was introduced, and I was thrilled. I have now completed both my linguistics major requirements and my cognitive science courses, and I know that these areas of study have together enriched my knowledge and helped to prepare me for graduate school. The minor in cognitive science requires that students take courses in at least three of the following categories: computational cognition, language and cognition, philosophy of cognition, and psychology of cognition. Most of my coursework was in the language and psychology areas, but I feel that the courses I took also informed me a great deal on the computational and philosophical approaches to cognitive science. Computational Cognition Computational cognition is the study of the computational foundation through which learning and inference occur in the brain. Through such study, one seeks to understand the basis behind the processing of information by means of mathematical modeling, computer simulation, and behavioral experiments. Almost every course in the Cognitive Science minor utilizes computational cognition to some degree. For instance, Phonetics and Phonology employs logical formulas to state phonetic rules as they are believed to exist in the human mind.

Reflective Essay for Completion of Cognitive Science Minor

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

I wrote this reflective essay as a final requirement for completing a college minor in cognitive science. I was one of the first students to minor in cognitive science at Truman State, and I am told that my essay was used as an example for future students to see how to successfully fulfill this writing requirement.

Citation preview

  • Christina Schwaller

    4 August, 2010

    No Minor Feat: Reflections on the Cognitive Science Minor

    After a long absence, I returned to Truman State University in the spring of 2007, this

    time as a linguistics major. That semester, I took a course on cognitive psychology, and it

    fascinated me. I began to take courses toward a psychology minor, which I would fulfill by

    taking as many cognition-related courses as possible. Shortly thereafter, the cognitive science

    interdisciplinary minor was introduced, and I was thrilled. I have now completed both my

    linguistics major requirements and my cognitive science courses, and I know that these areas of

    study have together enriched my knowledge and helped to prepare me for graduate school. The

    minor in cognitive science requires that students take courses in at least three of the following

    categories: computational cognition, language and cognition, philosophy of cognition, and

    psychology of cognition. Most of my coursework was in the language and psychology areas, but

    I feel that the courses I took also informed me a great deal on the computational and

    philosophical approaches to cognitive science.

    Computational Cognition

    Computational cognition is the study of the computational foundation through which

    learning and inference occur in the brain. Through such study, one seeks to understand the basis

    behind the processing of information by means of mathematical modeling, computer simulation,

    and behavioral experiments. Almost every course in the Cognitive Science minor utilizes

    computational cognition to some degree. For instance, Phonetics and Phonology employs logical

    formulas to state phonetic rules as they are believed to exist in the human mind.

  • Schwaller 2

    PSYC 331 Cognitive Psychology

    In Cognitive Psychology, I wrote a paper on Jeff Hawkinss book On Intelligence. His

    model for a learning machine is centered on a theory that all of intelligence is acquired via a

    memory-prediction framework acting in the neocortex. Hawkins insists that the plasticity of

    neurons and Mountcastles theory of a universal algorithm are evidence enough that a model can

    be built with silicon chips as soon as that algorithm is calculated. The cornerstones on which

    Hawkins has built his theory are memory-prediction, parallel sequencing, and hierarchical

    organization.

    A team of researchers tested nerve firing in monkeys during motor tasks, and the results

    seemed to suggest a high level of plasticity in certain certain neurons, as well as suggesting that a

    memory-prediction process influences the firing of some neurons in motor tasks. The

    experimenters found differences in firing patterns of inferior parietal lobule (IPL) neurons for the

    same task depending on the intention associated with that task. The aspects affecting this were

    found to be not from variation in the motor act (grasping), but variation of the future purpose that

    act served. The connected future action intended had an effect on the brain activity during the

    first action. Not only did the monkeys brains follow similar firing patterns when observing an

    act, but they followed the same guidelines of intention. The neurons that discharge in both

    performance and observation of an act are dubbed parietal mirror neurons (Fogassi 2005). In

    order to fire in certain ways based on intention of observed acts, the circuitry of the brain must

    incorporate a top-down prediction process. The experiments described here are, even on the

    surface, evidential of a memory-prediction system that is present in perception (Fogassi, 2005).

    The research supports Hawkinss idea of prediction as the source of various other

    cognitive tasks. The theory that sensation and motor control are results of the same basic process

  • Schwaller 3

    is both evidence and conclusion of the aforementioned universal prediction theory. Furthermore,

    the similarities in brain activity for motor control and perception provide support for the theory

    that cognitive processes are all built upon the same basic algorithm.

    In these tests, subjects evaluated action words while motor areas in the left brain

    (primarily used for language) were excited with the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation

    (TMS). It was found that choice reaction time was shorter when a word related to the area of the

    body to which the stimulated motor area was connected. The scientists performing this study

    concluded that the linguistic analysis and sensorimotor systems of the brain are highly interactive

    when there is a meaningful reason for such interaction (Pulvemller 2005).

    Language and Cognition

    Psycholinguistics deals with comprehension and production of language, specifically

    focusing on how the mind organizes, stores, and processes language. It encompasses any subject

    involving cognition and language. At first largely philosophical, psycholinguistics now studies

    data from brain imaging and takes into account computational theories from other subdisciplines

    of linguistics.

    ENG 414 Language and the Mind

    Language and the Mind deals with various topics of psycholinguistics. In this course, I

    studied such topics as language processing, storage and access, comprehension theory,

    neurobiology, and language deficits. Each of these informed my understanding of the possible

    cognitive pathways and processes that make language possible for humans.

    One of the steps toward an understanding of language cognition is discussion of the

    structure of the language regions of the brain. This neuroscientific approach considers

  • Schwaller 4

    hypotheses on the locations where particular linguistic processes occur. Methods for this include

    various brain imaging technologies, study of individuals with brain damage to specific areas, and

    comparison between human and animal brains. Through such methods, we can see that certain

    language faculties are, in part, localized to particular regions of the brain. However, brain

    imaging technologies have allowed us to see that each linguistic process involves more than just

    one area of the brain. Although the left side of the brain is associated with interpretation of

    symbols and analytic processing, the right side of the brain is also involved in language,

    especially when perceptual and spatial representation are involved. Brocas and Wernickes

    aphasias affect very specific parts of speech, suggesting that these factors are somewhat localized.

    However, linguistic processes follow paths through various areas of the brain, so damage to

    Brocas and Wernickes areas does not eliminate the language faculty.

    Connectionist models of language processing assert that frequency of usage strengthens a

    connection in the memory. If a neural pathway between two words is used frequently enough,

    then one word will quickly facilitate access of the other. Connection strength explains why

    frequently used words are the easiest to retrieve. It may also be the reason for collocation,

    through which we retrieve certain words together.

    The information processing approach emphasizes the complexity of language processing

    by charting the flow of information through several steps. Sensation and perception allow

    humans to see and recognize patterns, based on smaller characteristics of the stimulus, matching

    the whole to a representation in the long-term memory, and assigning a category or identity to

    the stimulus. Short-term or working memory holds the stimulus and, if necessary, passes it on to

    the long-term memory. Surprisingly, linguists and psychologists believe that decoding of

    perceived language actually involves construction of meaning and integration of new meaning

  • Schwaller 5

    into what has already been decoded. Charting the levels of representation in language decoding

    in this way largely relies on bottom-up processing, whereby the mind uses small units to

    assemble larger ones. However, top-down processing also gives us the ability to recognize the

    intended word when it is misspelled or mispronounced, as well as to guess the next word in a

    sentence or parse the grammatical construction of an utterance before it is completed. The

    interactive view of language processing suggests that processes may occur in both directions

    simultaneously, or in parallel. Below is Levelts model of speaking. This model employs an

    interactive approach in which decoding and encoding occur in parallel, utilizing both top-down

    and bottom-up processes.

  • Schwaller 6

    Levelts Model of Speaking

    (Field 128)

  • Schwaller 7

    LING 413 Advanced Linguistics (Semantics and Pragmatics)

    In the Advanced Linguistics course on semantics and pragmatics, I worked on a study in

    which I explored cognitive processes and mental organization of data at a specific level of

    semantics. With a partner, I designed and performed an experiment on locative verbs. Locatives

    are verbs that indicate transfer of content to or from a container. These verbs are puzzling in that

    they take different syntactic construction for some reason that is not automatically decipherable.

    Some always treat the content as the main argument (ex. fill the cup with water/ *fill water

    into the cup). Others may only show the up in constructions where the container is linked to the

    direct object (ex. poured paint onto the floor/ *poured the floor with paint). Some are

    alternators, and can link either the content or the container to the grammatical direct object (ex.

    loaded the gun with ammo/ loaded ammo into the gun).

    Linguists have not yet discovered how the mind determines which syntactic constructions

    these content-to-container verbs can take. Some existing research supported a theory that the

    way locatives are categorized in the grammar can be predicted based on the nature of the specific

    actions they describe. Non-alternating locatives can be classified into two groups: manner and

    endstate. Manner locatives indicate movement of content. These locatives are specific about the

    nature of the transfer, and how the container changes as a result is not their primary focus. These

    locatives require content-based syntax, meaning that the content needs to be the indirect object,

    and the container must be the direct object (ex. Tex pounded nails into the wall/ *Tex

    pounded the wall with nails). Endstate verbs are the opposite case, in that they require

    container-based constructions (ex. Bob filled the cup with water/ *Bob filled water into the

    cup). Because there is no clear distinction between the primitive semantic features of these

    locative verbs, scientists are not sure how children learn to categorize them in the mental lexicon.

  • Schwaller 8

    In basic semantics, an object being moved in space is labeled as the patient or theme.

    However, this is not the case when a verb links the ground or container entity to the grammatical

    object. Some linguists theorize that verbs can or must take a container locative in situations

    where the primary action describes a state-change of the ground or container as a result of

    motion. If this is the case, the mind categorizes the stationary object as the main affected

    argument and treats it as the grammatical argument.

    The first linking theories were developed in the 1960s and 1970s and were built upon a

    list-of-primitives structural model. Basic thematic roles (i.e., agent, theme, goal) were linked to

    grammatical relations. Unfortunately, these theories overgeneralized linking patterns. In the

    case of locative verbs, they did not allow for the semantic/syntactic correspondences displayed

    by container-locative constructions (i.e., fill, as in fill the glass with water, in which glass is

    both the ground entity and the direct object).

    Recent linking theories have abandoned the list-of-primitives model for a more abstract

    semantic structure technique. A verb is said to have a structured semantic representation that

    makes explicit the agentive, causal, and temporal properties of the event (157). Semantic

    structure theory uses more abstract primitives in defining meaning. This seems to decrease the

    unpredictability of locative alternators and container-locatives. It can also reveal the tendency of

    container-locative verbs and alternators to describe two events: a movement of the content

    (figure) and a state-change of the container (ground). Whichever one is considered to be the

    main argument determines the syntactic construction. If a verb describes how something moves,

    it is also explaining that it moves. The same goes for change in endstate. With this process,

    predictions can be made on which constructions a particular verb will accept.

  • Schwaller 9

    For our literature review, we studied the methods and findings of previous experiments

    involving locatives. In one study, three experiments were performed to assess speakers use of

    verb meaning to predict syntax. In each experiment, children and adults were taught novel verbs

    for a transfer of content to a container, and these actions were performed. The subjects were

    asked to describe the actions using the new verbs in their own sentences. Depending on which

    effect (motion of figure or state-change of ground) was made to appear more salient, the

    participants hypothetically would move toward either a figure or ground argument. The results

    all supported a theory that a manner of motion results in a mental representation of figure as

    primary object, while a change in endstate of the stationary entity shifts the ground into the

    position of main argument. There was also strong negating evidence for the assumption that the

    mental lexicon has a content-locative-only rule for locative verbs with container-locatives

    resulting from exceptions. The regularity in which container-locative constructions arose when

    predicted by their semantic structure theory supports the idea that the ground-object construction

    is the result of a gestalt shift of the endstate-effected verb to a position for manner-effected verbs.

    To clarify the results of previous research, we redesigned one of the experiments from

    our literature review. We felt that in order to reach any definite conclusions, more of the

    variables needed to be made constant. We stabilized variables such as participant age, using only

    adults age 18-23. We also allowed less variety in the testing materials, so that the different

    characteristics of the materials would not affect results. Finally, we limited possible responses to

    ranked multiple-choice answers, so that our results would be easily quantifiable.

    For the figure or content entity, we used sponges. The first group of participants received

    dry sponges, and the second group received sponges soaked in an acidic solution. The surface or

    container entity was homemade pH test paper made from cabbage juice. Both groups were given

  • Schwaller 10

    the same response sheet. The following answers appeared in four different random orders to

    counterbalance any influence order might have on selection.

    a. mooped the sponge

    b. mooped the paper

    c. mooped the sponge onto the paper

    d. mooped the paper with the sponge

    For the first subject group, a novel manner verb was presented for testing. A student

    proctor passed out a small sponge, a piece of paper stained with cabbage juice, and an answer

    sheet to each subject. She demonstrated by bouncing a sponge onto the paper, causing no state-

    change to the paper, and said that the action was called mooping. Next, she asked all participants

    to repeat the action. They were then directed to their answer sheet and asked to rank four

    syntactic options in order of what they were most likely to say.

    For group 2, participant reactions to a novel endstate verb were tested. The proctor passed

    out a sheet of litmus paper, a sponge soaked in lemon juice, and an answer sheet to each

    participant. She then demonstrated a process to be performed by the participants. This process

    involved touching the sponge to the paper, which caused a change in the papers color. The

    demonstrator described this action as mooping and prompted the participants to repeat the action.

    Subjects then ranked the four options on the answer sheet, just like the previous section.

    We hypothesized that the group performing a manner-based task would produce more

    content-locative constructions, and the group performing an endstate task would produce more

    container-locative constructions.

    Mooped the paper and mooped the paper with the sponge are the container-based

    locatives, while mooped the sponge and mooped the sponge to the paper are content-based.

  • Schwaller 11

    By multiplying the number of responses in each ranking (1, 2, 3, 4) and finding the sum of these

    numbers, an overall ranking was found for each option. The lower numbers suggest greater

    preference.

    The results of the dry trial showed, as predicted, a tendency toward content-based

    locatives when describing manner of movement.

    Paper with Sponge = 99

    Paper = 83

    Sponge = 72

    Sponge to Paper = 56

    Container = 182

    Content = 128

    In the wet trial, the results were less clear.

    Sponge = 81

    Paper = 79

    Sponge to Paper = 72

    Paper with Sponge = 68

    Content = 153

    Container = 147

    The results in the wet sponge group were much closer in that there was still heavy use of content-

    based locatives. This could lead us to believe that something within the action caused a change

    in the content (sponge) to appear salient to participants in Group 2. However, it could also

    simply weaken the original hypothesis. After all, there was no apparent flaw in the procedure

  • Schwaller 12

    that might cause content and container locatives to occur with nearly the same frequency in this

    situation.

    One interesting side observation is that people tended to prefer the answer that gave the

    most detail. Neither Paper nor Sponge was favored, despite being appropriate locatives. This

    difference between simple and complex locative constructions was not studied in the

    experiments previous to ours. The totals of the content and container locatives, however, still

    show far less severe a dichotomy than was suggested by the findings of previous studies. Our

    results were not definitive enough to be taken as reliable support for or against their theory.

    There is need for a revised study with a stricter procedure in order to really clarify the findings.

    Philosophy of Cognition

    Philosophy of cognition studies the nature of the mind, consciousness, and mental

    functions from a philosophical view, using rational thought to formulate possible truths. Several

    of the courses that I took at Truman dealt with varying philosophies of the mind. We often used

    philosophy and logic to postulate possible connections between mental events and the physical

    brain.

    ENG/LING 413 Advanced Linguistics

    Each of the Advanced Linguistics courses I took (Semantics and Pragmatics, Phonetics

    and Phonology, and Morphology) dealt with inference of linguistic rules from observation of

    patterns in language. These rules are parts of the mental grammar, believed to contribute to the

    cognitive processes of encoding and decoding language. Not only do these areas of formal

    linguistics deal with language and cognition, but they also delve into the philosophy of cognition.

    Each area of formal linguistics theorizes about the nature of cognition by supposing that a certain

  • Schwaller 13

    aspect of the grammar may be the basest building block of language. For example, some

    semanticists believe that semantic information is the first aspect of language that the brain

    considers when encoding speech. In the study of morphology, one might consider that linguistic

    information is categorized based on morphological paradigms.

    J.L. Austins How to Do Things with Words is definitely the most strictly philosophical

    text that I read in the Semantics and Pragmatics course. It does not go into detail about

    computation or experiments but instead focuses mainly on philosophical argument. Austin was,

    in fact, a philosopher of language, not a linguist. In How to Do Things with Words, Austin

    contradicts the previously established assumption that language can be completely broken down

    into a truth-value system. He argues that many sentences can be deemed neither true nor false,

    as they are not statements of possible truths. Austin spends much of the lecture defining and

    explaining performative utterances. I will not go into detail about the performative, but it is

    worth mentioning because it has inspired much further research in the field of pragmatics.

    Austins work in philosophy of language has inspired generations since within the realm of

    philosophical linguistics, and linguists have expanded and his theories through computational

    and psychological methods as well.

    PHRE 342 Symbolic Logic

    Another area that contributes to the philosophy of cognition is logic, the study of

    reasoning. In part, it seeks to clarify and explain the processes by which the human mind solves

    problems. This field utilizes a simple language in order to evaluate statements and determine

    truth or fallacy. Logic is, in its methodology, extremely computational. However, its ultimate

    goal is to deduce a true philosophy of the mind by determining the formulas humans use for

    critical thinking. PHRE 342 or Symbolic Logic is examines the formal deductive systems

  • Schwaller 14

    underlying mathematics and, presumably, thought. In this course, we studied sentential logic,

    predicate logic, the logic of relations, the algebra of sets, and modal logic. The advanced forms

    of mathematical logic studied also served to describe newer philosophies of language and

    cognition. Rather than simply positing binary truth values, these forms of logic took into account

    such concepts as necessity and possibility. Therefore, they are likely more accurate as

    representations of not only math, but language as well. If we assume that language mirrors

    thought, and surely the logic of semantics mirrors the logic of cognition, then it is fair to say that

    symbolic logic comes as close to describing our many complex cognitive processes as any

    previously existing logic has.

    Psychology of cognition

    Cognitive psychology explores internal mental processes from an approach concerned

    mainly with the human psyche and behaviors. It is a theoretical method that employs quantitative

    and scientific methodologies in order to create information processing models for higher-level

    mental functions.

    PSYCH 331 Cognitive Psychology

    Cognitive Psychology, as a Truman course, examines the current knowns and unknowns

    of human cognition, with particular attention to complex cognitive tasks. We spent time studying

    human information processing theories, as well as other types of research and theory. Particular

    emphasis was given to the connectionist approach to human cognition. We primarily discussed

    the complex cognitive tasks that are unique to the human mind, and we were encouraged to come

    up with their own theories on such processes as pattern recognition, attention, memory, sensation

    and perception, language processing, problem solving, and decision making.

  • Schwaller 15

    PSYCH 370 Human Sensation and Perception

    An important topic in cognitive psychology is the organization of information within the

    brain. Both storage and processing are believed to involve a combination of localization and

    lateralization. When human perception makes a mistake, the mistake can inform us of a possible

    overlap in processing pathways. For the Sensation and Perception course I studied the link

    between spicy taste sensations and its connection to false perception of hot or burning tactile

    sensations, as well as the similar cooling felt in connection with menthol. In my research, I

    found that capsaicin, a chemical found in chili peppers, is considered responsible for the

    inaccurate sensation of burning heat that is found alongside the subjective taste description of

    spicy. Similarly, menthol creates a false feeling of extremely low temperature. For both

    substances, the occurrence of perceived temperature change can occur anywhere on the skin but

    are particularly intense on the tongue.

    I considered multiple theories about where in the brains perceptual pathways an overlap

    between taste and pain characteristics might occur. Similarities in taste and pain can be seen in

    that they involve some of the same areas of the brain. The thalamus and the insula region of the

    cortex are involved in both of these perceptual processes. Therefore, information of both types

    could collide at either the subcortical or cortical level. However, this would suggest a much

    greater level of synesthesia in most or all of perception, due to the fact that some of these areas

    are used for filtering or processing many types of information. Since this does not occur, it would

    be more likely that an answer lies either within the chemical makeup of the taste stimuli or in the

    surface aspects of tongue regions and their receptive fields.

    I performed research to evaluate the effects of capsaicin and menthol on subjects

    perception, using a psychophysical approach. These tests involved the separate application of

  • Schwaller 16

    substances containing each of these chemicals to different regions of the tongue, in order to also

    determine which region or taste buds were most sensitive to the chemicals. Participants were

    asked to describe their taste and cutaneous reactions to the stimuli. In a separate test, I applied

    pressure with a toothpick to the separate regions of the tongue. This served the purpose of

    determining whether any region of the tongue has more nocireceptors receptors located all over

    the skin that are activated by pressure, extreme temperature, or burning chemicals to send a pain

    signal to the brain than any other region.

    The results showed that the middle region of the tongue was less susceptible to both taste

    and temperature sensations. This region is also devoid of taste buds. However, the pressure test

    showed that all regions reacted at approximately the same level when given a pressure stimulus.

    For this reason, I believe that the regions have a similar number of nocireceptors. However, our

    minds so strongly link the particular taste sensations with temperature or pain sensations that it is

    probably inpossible to judge each accurately. In a way, taste buds are a source of temperature-

    related pain reactions, because the sensation of spicy brings forth the memory of burning heat.

    The overlap between these different kinds of sensations probably occurs higher up in the

    cognitive pathways. Both pain signals and taste signals pass through the thalamus. It is possible

    that the perceptual error happens here. These signals are also sent to the insula, in the cortex,

    which is another possible site for the overlap. However, these areas receive many sorts of

    perceptual information and usually manage to sort it accordingly. In addition, these explanations

    ignore the dilemma of why only certain chemicals cause the taste/pain perceptual effect.

    No clear and certain explanation has been found by this or more professional research.

    However, this topic is growing in popularity. The perceptual phenomenon caused multimodal

    response to the pungent chemicals capsaicin and menthol is similar to the experience of

  • Schwaller 17

    synesthesia. Both events seem to have great potential to inform cognitive scientists on the

    sensation and perception pathways in the brain, which may be both localized and lateralized

    according to mode, with some occasional crossing.

    Internship Experience in Cognitive Science

    ENG 451 Internship for English (Autism Research)

    In the spring of 2009, I took part in a research internship, along with a few other students,

    under Dr. Mary Shapiro. Our research focused on Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Dr.

    Shapiros long-term goal for the project was to develop new materials and approaches for people

    with ASD to develop interactive skills. In particular, she noticed that most existing literature

    taught only basic, fundamental language skills. Yet, as its name suggests, ASD covers a

    spectrum of disorders, indicating various levels of cognitive, linguistic, and social potential.

    People toward one end of the spectrum often show high rates of success in existing intervention.

    This seems to imply that such individuals might benefit from further, more advanced instruction.

    Our purpose was to determine the possible merit of developing such a program of speech and

    language instruction, aimed at adolescents and adults who have completed existing programs

    successfully.

    After discussing the goals of the project, we began our research with an extensive

    literature review. We studied every resource that was available to us in order to find out what

    skills were already being taught. Special attention was paid to subtle aspects of speech, including

    paralinguistic behaviors, like active listening cues (minimal responses, nodding). Also an area of

    focus was discussion of advanced linguistic strategies, ranging from turn-taking and topic

    maintenance to explanations of complex sociolinguistic phenomena. We found that many

  • Schwaller 18

    materials addressed paralinguistic actions, often as part of a larger aim to educate children in

    performing specific speech acts. The speech acts addressed were quite numerous, and they were

    often taught through scripts or simple picture stories. We did not find, however, any curricula

    that explored sociolinguistic topics in great depth. The literature review revealed a lack of

    educational resources on dialect, register, and many other related topics.

    Once we had found these subjects to be missing from existing ASD programs, we sought

    to determine what merit such programs might have. The first step in this was to see how high-

    functioning adults with ASD handled the difficult facets of spoken language in social situations.

    We transcribed the speech of a few men at an Autism support group meeting, quantitatively

    analyzing various linguistic phenomena that we observed. For a control, we chose a social group

    consisting of neurotypical men and performed the same analysis on their transcribed speech. Our

    data showed significantly higher rates of phonological simplification among the neurotypical

    speakers than among those with ASD. Such simplification is associated with casual speech

    registers and nonstandard dialects. Neurotypical adults often code-switch between formal and

    informal registers, depending on context. In the ASD group, avoidance of nonstandard variation

    may be the result of a lapse in language instruction. Code-switching behaviors are not formally

    taught, so the adults with ASD may not have acquired this skill. Similarly, we found slightly

    more pragmatic wordplay in the control group than in the ASD group. As suspected, the control

    group showed greater understanding of appropriateness, non-literal or indirect meaning, context,

    accommodation, and other interactional influences. Even so, the adults in the ASD group applied

    advanced pragmatic concepts and displayed comprehension beyond what is normally expected of

    people with ASD. Overall, this corpus study clarified an existing disparity between the advanced

    linguistic skills of neurotypical adults and those with ASD.

  • Schwaller 19

    The interns involvement in this project culminated with the design and implementation

    of a survey to gauge audience reactions to formal versus informal register. We also aimed to

    measure how their judgments might be affected by knowledge of a speaker having Autism. The

    research group produced two versions of a video for participants to view. In each of these, an

    actor played Joe, a student who is working on his storytelling and interaction skills. He first tells

    a story as a monologue, in which he reports the players conversation. Next, he discusses movies

    with a friend. In the first version, Joe speaks in a moderately formal register, avoiding

    nonstandard or slang words. The second version differs only in this respect. During his

    monologue, Joe uses be like and be all to report conversation (i.e., I was like... and He was

    all...). In the dialogue, he uses the words awesome and sucks to express his approval and

    disapproval of various things.

    For the survey, undergraduate students were allowed to participate. They were separated

    into four groups. Groups 1 and 3 viewed the version of the video in which Joe used only standard

    vocabulary. Groups 2 and 4 viewed the recording featuring be like/be all and awesome/sucks.

    They were all given the same set of questions, except that groups 3 and 4 were additionally told

    that Joe had ASD. Participants in these two groups were also asked to rate their level of

    experience with ASD. The ultimate configuration of variables was as such: Group 1 = standard

    language with no mention of ASD, Group 2 = nonstandard language with no mention of ASD,

    Group 3 = standard language with mention of ASD, and Group 4 = nonstandard language with

    mention of ASD. Their questionnaires asked respondents to rate Joes performance in a number

    of areas, using a scale of 1 to 10. These included friendliness, intelligence, confidence, and

    honesty. Based on our knowledge of similar studies, we predicted that respondents in the

    nonstandard language groups would give higher ratings for friendliness and honesty. This was

  • Schwaller 20

    not the case. In responses to the monologue portion, nonstandard language correlated with lower

    scores on some questions and had no significant effect on others. Even with regard to

    friendliness, the slang words did not prove advantageous. The highest ratings came from group 3,

    revealing positive reactions to the ASD label in combination with standard language usage.

    Questions regarding the dialogue portion resulted in different trends. The nonstandard versions

    scored higher than the standard versions here. An ASD label corresponded to better scores again,

    with highest ratings in group 4, showing ASD label and nonstandard language to be the most

    positively received combination. Interestingly, when asked how much their own opinions

    matched Joes, group 4 showed the most agreement. Answers to the dialogue portion revealed

    positive correlations between ASD label and response, as well as between nonstandard language

    and response. These trends may reflect feelings of compassion toward disability and solidarity

    among speakers with common nonstandard variation (in this case, the nonstandard language

    examples are common within the respondents age group). Taken altogether, the survey results

    are inconclusive. We can cite one conclusive trend - positive reactions to an ASD label - but

    hearer reaction to our nonstandard stimuli needs to be retested.

    For the Cognitive Science minor, I have tried to educate myself in a broad range of

    subtopics while remaining focused on the essential concepts in the study of cognition. My

    courses were from three areas of the cognitive science minor, but as interdisciplinarity suggests,

    they frequently dealt with more than one area. My internship experience gave me the opportunity

    to apply the theories and concepts I had learned to research in a field that is also socially

    important. In the end, I am quite satisfied with the knowledge and critical thinking skills that I

    have developed while pursuing this minor.

  • Schwaller 21

    Works Referenced

    Austin, J. L. How to Do Things with Words. Cambridge: Harvard UP, 1975. Print.

    Field, John. Psycholinguistics: a Resource Book for Students. London: Routledge, 2003. Print.

    Fogassi, L., P. Ferrari, B. Gesierich, S. Rozzi, F. Chersi, and G. Rizzolatti. "Parietal Lobe: From

    Action Organization to Intention Understanding." Science 308.5722 (2005): 662-67.

    Academic Search Elite. Web. 2 Apr. 2007.

    Hawkins, Jeff, and Sandra Blakeslee. On Intelligence. New York: Times, 2004. Print.

    Pulvermller, F., O. Hauk, V. Nikulin, and R. Ilmoniemi. "Functional Links between Motor and

    Language Systems." European Journal of Neuroscience 21.3 (2005): 793-97. Academic

    Search Elite. Web. 23 Apr. 2007.