1
ze: s 21” d is 42” ter. It any pect s: luded in 8en see scien:fic move, change eds. specific tos or e by ure andard sults, all be at nch in or ixel ne up to ted ity of a:on of our a good ke in t a large le review see them from o on your home and not ot print.] Chang Them This temp the built newer ve To change the Desig Colors dro The defau template always re some of t Prin: Poste Once you visit www order a h poster pr a free des deliver as day withi Genigrap producing PowerPoi in the ind when we design th US and C Email: i [This sid A period of voiced gloeal fric:on, usually occurring with preaspira:on (Figure 2) but some:mes on its own (Figure 3): Figure 2: breathiness followed by preaspira8on. Figure 3: breathiness. Breathiness and pre-aspiration in Aberystwyth English Michaela Hejná Previous research into preaspira:on usually treats breathiness as its necessary component. Thus, preaspira:on and breathiness tend to be added up in dura:onal analyses [19], or we do not know if breathiness was measured as part of preaspira:on [1012], and they are dis:nguished only some:mes [1315], in which case differences in the segmental and social condi:oning are found. Main quesCon: should they be treated as two components of a single phenomenon? Hypothesis: they should if 1. they are subject to the same social condi:oning 2. if they both serve the same linguis:c func:on (e.g. enhance a contrast) 3. or the same linguis:c condi:oning. Introduction phonological factors: vowel height, backness, length phone:c variables: F1, F2 phonologically ambiguous factors: place & manner of ar:cula:on of the posttonic plosive; type of the pretonic consonant On the whole, there are more similari:es than differences in the plosive context, especially concerning the phonological and uncertain variables. Most differences are found in the frica:ve and in the unstressed contexts . Occurrence 1. With plosives, the paeerns are the same except for breathiness being sensi:ve to phonological vowel backness. 2. With frica:ves, the paeerns are different. Dura:on 1. The paeerns condi:oned by the phonological variables are the same except for vowel length, to which only preaspira:on is sensi:ve. 2. Phone:c height and backness do not condi:on the two either in the same or in the opposite way, so they do not behave in the same way nor show tradeoffs. Speakers A period of voiceless (primarily) gloeal fric:on in sequences of a sonorant and a voiceless obstruent: Figure 1: preaspira8on. Preaspira:on can be accompanied with breathiness. Preaspira:on and breathiness show similariCes: their occurrence and dura:on show on the whole the same paeerns in apparent:me they both enhance the for:slenis contrast in the plosive stressed context (mock, mocker), they mostly cooccur However, they also show differences: their occurrence is not sensi:ve to gender in the same way in the frica:ve stressed context (moss) and in the unstressed context (frolic), breathiness is obligatory, but preaspira:on is not Conclusions: The two should be analysed separately unless it is demonstrated that they are consistently subject to the same condi:oning. Preaspira:on and breathiness are not dis:nguished in phonological studies (e.g. [1718]), but it has been suggested that dis:nguishing the two may shed light on the affilia:on debates [16]. We may gain more insight regarding the linguis:c and the social condi:oning of preaspira:on and breathiness: merging the two may lead to obscuring the paeerns, as also illustrated by [1315]. [1] Helgason, P. (2003) “Faroese preaspiration”. 15th ICPhS, Barcelona. 2517-20. [2] Helgason, P.; Ringen, C. (2008) “Voicing and aspiration in Swedish stops”. Journal of Phonetics 36. 607-28. [3] Karlsson, A. M.; Svantesson, J-O. (2011) “Preaspiration in Mongolian dialects: acoustic properties of contrastive stops”. Paper presented at The 10th Seoul International Altaistic Conference, 125-40. <http://www.sol.lu.se/en/person/AnastasiaKarlsson> [accessed in May 2014]. [4] Morris, J. (2010) “Phonetic variation in Northern Wales: preaspiration”. Proceedings of the Second Summer School of Sociolinguistics. Eds. M. Meyerhoff, C. Adachi, A. Daleszynska & A. Strycharz. University of Edinburgh: Edinburgh. <http://www.lel.ed.ac.uk/sssocio/proceedings/ Jon.pdf> [accessed in September 2012]. [5] Ringen, C.; van Dommelen, W. A. (2013) “Quantity and laryngeal contrasts in Norwegian”. Journal of Phonetics 41. 479-90. [6] Stevens, M.; Hajek, J. (2004) “Preaspiration in Sienese Italian and its interaction with stress in /VCː/ sequences”. Speech Prosody, Nara. 57-60. [7] Stevens, M.; Hajek, J. (2004c) “Comparing voiced and voiceless geminates in Sienese Italian: what role does preaspiration play?”. Proceedings of the 10th Australian International Conference on Speech Science & Technology. 340-5. [8] Stevens, M. (2010) “How widespread is preaspiration in Italy? A preliminary acoustic phonetic overview”. Lund University Centre for Languages and Literature Phonetics Working Papers 54. / FONETIK 2010, Lund. 97-102. [9] Stevens, M. (2011) “Consonant Length in Italian: Gemination, Degemination and Preaspiration”. Selected Proceedings of the 5th Conference on Laboratory Approaches to Romance Phonology. 21-32. [10] Helgason, P. (1998) "Preaspiration in Swedish and its implications for historical sound change." FONETIK 1998, Stockholm. 16-19. [11] McRobbie-Utasi, Z. (2003) "Normative preaspiration in Skolt Sami in relation to the distribution of duration in the disyllabic stress-group". Honti-Festschrift. Eds. Z. McRobbie-Utasi & C. So. Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Institute of Linguistics: Budapest. 291-300. [12] Tronnier, M. (2002) “Preaspiration in Southern Swedish dialects”. FONETIK 2002, Stockholm. / Quartely Progress and Status Report, Department of Speech, Music and Hearing and Centre for Speech Technology. KTH: Stockholm44. 33-6. [13] Nance, C.; Stuart-Smith, J. (2013) “Pre-aspiration and post-aspiration in Scottish Gaelic stop consonants”. JIPA 43, 2. 129-52. [14] Ní Chasaide, A. (1985) Preaspiration in phonological stop contrasts. PhD thesis, University College of North Wales. [15] Kingston, J. (1990) “Articulatory Binding”. Papers in Laboratory Phonology I. Between the Grammar and the Physics of Speech. Eds. J. Kingston & M. E. Beckman. CUP: Cambridge. 406-34. [16] Lodge, K. (2007) “Timing, segmental status and aspiration in Icelandic”. Transactions of the Philological Society 105, 1. 66-104. [17] Árnason, K. (1986) “The Segmental and Suprasegmental Status of Preaspiration in Modern Icelandic”. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 9, 1. 1-23. [18] Gussmann, E. (2002) Phonology. CUP: Cambridge. Discussion Aberystwyth 18 speakers born & raised in Aberystwyth most with parents also from midWales L1 Welsh speakers, proficient in English 12 females, 6 males 2490 years old Segmental conditioning Department of Linguistics and English Language, University of Manchester [email protected] Defining breathiness Co-occurrence In the plosive stressed context (mock, mocker), preaspira:on and breathiness cooccur in the vast majority of cases (Figure 6). Figure 6: occurrence of preaspira8on and breathiness in the plosive context in stressed syllables per speaker. In the frica:ve stressed context (moss) and also in the unstressed context (frolic), breathiness is obligatory but preaspira:on is not (Figure 7): Figure 7: (on the leD) occurrence of preaspira8on and breathiness in the frica8ve context in stressed syllables per speaker; (on the right) occurrence of preaspira8on and breathiness in unstressed syllables per speaker. In the unstressed context, breathiness is obligatory irrespec:ve of the manner of ar:cula:on, although slightly less frequent in the plosive context (frica:ves: 99%; plosives: 85%). Preaspira:on is much more frequent in the plosive than the frica:ve context, but does not reach values comparable to breathiness (frica:ves: 12%; plosives: 67.5%). Defining pre-aspiration Data Stressed syllables /a/, /ɛ/, /ɪ/, /ɒ/, /ʊ/, /ʌ/, /aː/, /oː/ combined with /p/, /t/, /k/ in word finally and wordmedially (mock, mocker) /a/, /ɛ/, /ɪ/, /ɒ/, /ʌ/ combined with /f/, /θ/, /s/, /ʃ/ wordfinally (mash) 3 tokens per word: 1x in isola:on, 2x in a carrier sentence Say ___ once. Unstressed syllables disyllables with the second unstressed syllable /p/, /t/, /k/, /f/, /s/, /ʃ/ wordfinal in the 2 nd , unstressed syllable (e.g. frolic, Philip, sheriff) 6 tokens per word in a carrier sentence 8,400 tokens for the stressed condi:on, 460 for the unstressed one For:slenis pairs 998 lenis tokens corresponding to 998 for:s tokens differing only in the plosive in ques:on Social conditioning In apparent:me, preaspira:on and breathiness occurrence show a very similar paeern. Preaspira:on and breathiness dura:ons show the same paeern, but breathiness is more variable across the individual. Figure 4: (on the leD) preaspira8on and breathiness occurrence in apparent8me; (on the right) pre aspira8on and breathiness dura8on in apparent8me. However, only preaspira:on occurrence is sensi:ve to gender: the young the male speakers, the more frequent the preaspira:on: Figure 5: pre-aspiration occurrence in apparent-time across gender. 70 100 130 160 190 220 250 280 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 Frequency of occurrence Age Age and breathiness & pre-aspiration occurrence "pre-aspiration" "breathiness" Linear("pre-aspiration") Linear("breathiness") 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 5.5 6 6.5 7 7.5 8 8.5 9 9.5 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 Duration (%) Age Age and breathiness & pre-aspiration duration "pre-aspiration" "breathiness" Linear("pre-aspiration") Linear("breathiness") 70 100 130 160 190 220 250 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 Pre-aspiration frequency Age Age, gender, and pre-aspiration occurrence females males Linear(females) Linear(males) Fortis-lenis contrast Both preaspira:on and breathiness enhance the for:slenis contrast (based on acous:c evidence). However, whilst preaspira:on is never found in the lenis series, breathiness is. Figure 8: (on the leD) occurrence of preaspira8on and the plosive series per posi8on in word and sentence; (on the right) occurrence of breathiness and the plosive series per posi8on in word and sentence. References

Poster’PrintSize:’ Breathiness and pre-aspiration in Aberystwyth ... · Poster’PrintSize:’ This poster template is 21” high by 45” wide and is printed at 200% for a 42”

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Poster’PrintSize:’ Breathiness and pre-aspiration in Aberystwyth ... · Poster’PrintSize:’ This poster template is 21” high by 45” wide and is printed at 200% for a 42”

Poster  Print  Size:  This poster template is 21” high by 45” wide and is printed at 200% for a 42” high by 90” wide poster. It can be used to print any poster with a 7:15 aspect

Placeholders: The  various  elements  included  in  this  poster  are  ones  we  o8en  see  in  medical,  research,  and  scien:fic  posters.  Feel  free  to  edit,  move,    add,  and  delete  items,  or  change  the  layout  to  suit  your  needs.  

conference  organizer  for  specific  

You  can  place  digital  photos  or  logo  art  in  your  poster  file  by  selec:ng  the  Insert,  Picture  command,  or  by  using  standard  copy  &  paste.  For  best  results,  all  graphic  elements  should  be  at  least  150-­‐200  pixels  per  inch  in  their  final  printed  size.  For  instance,  a  1600  x  1200  pixel  photo  will  usually  look  fine  up  to  8“-­‐10”  wide  on  your  printed  

To  preview  the  print  quality  of  images,  select  a  magnifica:on  of  100%  when  previewing  your  poster.  This  will  give  you  a  good  idea  of  what  it  will  look  like  in  print.  If  you  are  laying  out  a  large  poster  and  using  half-­‐scale  dimensions,  be  sure  to  preview  your  graphics  at  200%  to  see  them  

Please  note  that  graphics  from  websites  (such  as  the  logo  on  your  hospital's  or  university's  home  page)  will  only  be  72dpi  and  not  

[This  sidebar  area  does  not  print.]  

Change  Color  Theme:  This  template  is  designed  to  use  the  built-­‐in  color  themes  in  the  newer  versions  of  PowerPoint.  

To  change  the  color  theme,  select  the  Design  tab,  then  select  the  Colors  drop-­‐down  list.  

The  default  color  theme  for  this  template  is  “Office”,  so  you  can  always  return  to  that  a8er  trying  some  of  the  alterna:ves.  

Prin:ng  Your  Poster:  Once  your  poster  file  is  ready,  visit  www.genigraphics.com  to  order  a  high-­‐quality,  affordable  poster  print.  Every  order  receives  a  free  design  review  and  we  can  deliver  as  fast  as  next  business  day  within  the  US  and  Canada.    

Genigraphics®  has  been  producing  output  from  PowerPoint®  longer  than  anyone  in  the  industry;  da:ng  back  to  when  we  helped  Microso8®  design  the  PowerPoint®  so8ware.    

US  and  Canada:    1-­‐800-­‐790-­‐4001  Email:  [email protected]  

[This  sidebar  area  does  not  print.]  

A   period   of   voiced   gloeal   fric:on,   usually   occurring   with   pre-­‐aspira:on  (Figure  2)  but  some:mes  on  its  own  (Figure  3):  

Figure  2:    breathiness  followed  by  pre-­‐aspira8on.  

Figure  3:  breathiness.  

Breathiness and pre-aspiration in Aberystwyth English

Michaela Hejná

Previous   research   into   pre-­‐aspira:on   usually   treats   breathiness   as   its  necessary   component.   Thus,   pre-­‐aspira:on   and   breathiness   tend   to   be  added  up  in  dura:onal  analyses  [1-­‐9],  or  we  do  not  know  if  breathiness  was  measured  as  part  of  pre-­‐aspira:on  [10-­‐12],  and  they  are  dis:nguished  only  some:mes   [13-­‐15],   in   which   case   differences   in   the   segmental   and   social  condi:oning  are  found.  

Main   quesCon:   should   they   be   treated   as   two   components   of   a   single  phenomenon?  

Hypothesis:  they  should  if  1.  they  are  subject  to  the  same  social  condi:oning  2.  if  they  both  serve  the  same  linguis:c  func:on  (e.g.  enhance  a  contrast)  3.  or  the  same  linguis:c  condi:oning.  

Introduction -­‐   phonological  factors:  vowel  height,  backness,  length    -­‐   phone:c  variables:  F1,  F2  -­‐    phonologically   ambiguous   factors:   place   &   manner   of   ar:cula:on   of   the  post-­‐tonic  plosive;  type  of  the  pre-­‐tonic  consonant  

On   the   whole,   there   are   more   similari:es   than   differences   in   the   plosive  context,  especially  concerning  the  phonological  and  uncertain  variables.  Most  differences  are  found  in  the  frica:ve  and  in  the  unstressed  contexts.  

Occurrence  1.  With   plosives,   the   paeerns   are   the   same   except   for   breathiness   being  

sensi:ve  to  phonological  vowel  backness.  2.  With  frica:ves,  the  paeerns  are  different.  

Dura:on  1.  The   paeerns   condi:oned   by   the   phonological   variables   are   the   same  

except  for  vowel  length,  to  which  only  pre-­‐aspira:on  is  sensi:ve.  2.  Phone:c  height  and  backness  do  not  condi:on  the  two  either  in  the  same  

or  in  the  opposite  way,  so  they  do  not  behave  in  the  same  way  nor  show  trade-­‐offs.  

Speakers

A  period  of   voiceless   (primarily)   gloeal   fric:on   in   sequences  of   a   sonorant  and  a  voiceless  obstruent:  

Figure  1:  pre-­‐aspira8on.  

Pre-­‐aspira:on  can  be  accompanied  with  breathiness.    

Pre-­‐aspira:on  and  breathiness  show  similariCes:  -­‐    their   occurrence   and   dura:on   show   on   the   whole   the   same   paeerns   in  apparent-­‐:me  -­‐   they  both  enhance  the  for:s-­‐lenis  contrast  -­‐   in  the  plosive  stressed  context  (mock,  mocker),  they  mostly  co-­‐occur  

However,  they  also  show  differences:  -­‐   their  occurrence  is  not  sensi:ve  to  gender  in  the  same  way  -­‐   in  the  frica:ve  stressed  context  (moss)  and  in  the  unstressed  context  (frolic),  breathiness  is  obligatory,  but  pre-­‐aspira:on  is  not  

Conclusions:  The  two  should  be  analysed  separately  unless  it  is  demonstrated  that  they  are  consistently  subject  to  the  same  condi:oning.  

Pre-­‐aspira:on   and   breathiness   are   not   dis:nguished   in   phonological   studies  (e.g.  [17-­‐18]),  but  it  has  been  suggested  that  dis:nguishing  the  two  may  shed  light  on  the  affilia:on  debates  [16].  

We  may  gain  more  insight  regarding  the  linguis:c  and  the  social  condi:oning  of  pre-­‐aspira:on  and  breathiness:  merging  the  two  may  lead  to  obscuring  the  paeerns,  as  also  illustrated  by  [13-­‐15].  

[1] Helgason, P. (2003) “Faroese preaspiration”. 15th ICPhS, Barcelona. 2517-20. [2] Helgason, P.; Ringen, C. (2008) “Voicing and aspiration in Swedish stops”. Journal of Phonetics 36. 607-28. [3] Karlsson, A. M.; Svantesson, J-O. (2011) “Preaspiration in Mongolian dialects: acoustic properties of contrastive stops”. Paper presented at The 10th Seoul International Altaistic Conference, 125-40. <http://www.sol.lu.se/en/person/AnastasiaKarlsson> [accessed in May 2014]. [4] Morris, J. (2010) “Phonetic variation in Northern Wales: preaspiration”. Proceedings of the Second Summer School of Sociolinguistics. Eds. M. Meyerhoff, C. Adachi, A. Daleszynska & A. Strycharz. University of Edinburgh: Edinburgh. <http://www.lel.ed.ac.uk/sssocio/proceedings/Jon.pdf> [accessed in September 2012]. [5] Ringen, C.; van Dommelen, W. A. (2013) “Quantity and laryngeal contrasts in Norwegian”. Journal of Phonetics 41. 479-90. [6] Stevens, M.; Hajek, J. (2004) “Preaspiration in Sienese Italian and its interaction with stress in /VCː/ sequences”. Speech Prosody, Nara. 57-60. [7] Stevens, M.; Hajek, J. (2004c) “Comparing voiced and voiceless geminates in Sienese Italian: what role does preaspiration play?”. Proceedings of the 10th Australian International Conference on Speech Science & Technology. 340-5. [8] Stevens, M. (2010) “How widespread is preaspiration in Italy? A preliminary acoustic phonetic overview”. Lund University Centre for Languages and Literature Phonetics Working Papers 54. / FONETIK 2010, Lund. 97-102. [9] Stevens, M. (2011) “Consonant Length in Italian: Gemination, Degemination and Preaspiration”. Selected Proceedings of the 5th Conference on Laboratory Approaches to Romance Phonology. 21-32. [10] Helgason, P. (1998) "Preaspiration in Swedish and its implications for historical sound change." FONETIK 1998, Stockholm. 16-19. [11] McRobbie-Utasi, Z. (2003) "Normative preaspiration in Skolt Sami in relation to the distribution of duration in the disyllabic stress-group". Honti-Festschrift. Eds. Z. McRobbie-Utasi & C. So. Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Institute of Linguistics: Budapest. 291-300. [12] Tronnier, M. (2002) “Preaspiration in Southern Swedish dialects”. FONETIK 2002, Stockholm. / Quartely Progress and Status Report, Department of Speech, Music and Hearing and Centre for Speech Technology. KTH: Stockholm 44. 33-6. [13] Nance, C.; Stuart-Smith, J. (2013) “Pre-aspiration and post-aspiration in Scottish Gaelic stop consonants”. JIPA 43, 2. 129-52. [14] Ní Chasaide, A. (1985) Preaspiration in phonological stop contrasts. PhD thesis, University College of North Wales. [15] Kingston, J. (1990) “Articulatory Binding”. Papers in Laboratory Phonology I. Between the Grammar and the Physics of Speech. Eds. J. Kingston & M. E. Beckman. CUP: Cambridge. 406-34. [16] Lodge, K. (2007) “Timing, segmental status and aspiration in Icelandic”. Transactions of the Philological Society 105, 1. 66-104. [17] Árnason, K. (1986) “The Segmental and Suprasegmental Status of Preaspiration in Modern Icelandic”. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 9, 1. 1-23. [18] Gussmann, E. (2002) Phonology. CUP: Cambridge.

Discussion

Aberystwyth  

-­‐  18  speakers  born  &  raised  in  Aberystwyth                                                                              -­‐  most  with  parents  also  from  mid-­‐Wales  -­‐  L1  Welsh  speakers,  proficient  in  English  -­‐   12  females,  6  males      -­‐   24-­‐90  years  old  

Segmental conditioning

Department of Linguistics and English Language, University of Manchester [email protected]

Defining breathiness

Co-occurrence In   the   plosive   stressed   context   (mock,   mocker),   pre-­‐aspira:on   and  breathiness  co-­‐occur  in  the  vast  majority  of  cases  (Figure  6).  

Figure   6:   occurrence   of   pre-­‐aspira8on   and   breathiness   in   the   plosive   context   in   stressed   syllables   per  speaker.  

In  the  frica:ve  stressed  context  (moss)  and  also  in  the  unstressed  context  (frolic),  breathiness  is  obligatory  but  pre-­‐aspira:on  is  not  (Figure  7):  

Figure   7:   (on   the   leD)   occurrence   of   pre-­‐aspira8on   and   breathiness   in   the   frica8ve   context   in   stressed  syllables  per  speaker;  (on  the  right)  occurrence  of  pre-­‐aspira8on  and  breathiness  in  unstressed  syllables  per  speaker.  

In   the   unstressed   context,   breathiness   is   obligatory   irrespec:ve   of   the  manner   of   ar:cula:on,   although   slightly   less   frequent   in   the   plosive  context   (frica:ves:   99%;   plosives:   85%).   Pre-­‐aspira:on   is   much   more  frequent  in  the  plosive  than  the  frica:ve  context,  but  does  not  reach  values  comparable  to  breathiness  (frica:ves:  12%;  plosives:  67.5%).  

Defining pre-aspiration

Data Stressed  syllables  -­‐  /a/,  /ɛ/,  /ɪ/,  /ɒ/,  /ʊ/,  /ʌ/,  /aː/,  /oː/  combined  with  /p/,  /t/,  /k/   in  word-­‐

finally  and  word-­‐medially  (mock,  mocker)    -­‐  /a/,  /ɛ/,  /ɪ/,  /ɒ/,  /ʌ/  combined  with  /f/,  /θ/,  /s/,  /ʃ/  word-­‐finally  (mash)  -­‐  3  tokens  per  word:  1x  in  isola:on,  2x  in  a  carrier  sentence  Say  ___  once.  

Unstressed  syllables  -­‐  disyllables  with  the  second  unstressed  syllable  -­‐  /p/,   /t/,   /k/,   /f/,   /s/,   /ʃ/   word-­‐final   in   the   2nd,   unstressed   syllable   (e.g.  

frolic,  Philip,  sheriff)  -­‐  6  tokens  per  word  in  a  carrier  sentence  

-­‐  8,400  tokens  for  the  stressed  condi:on,  460  for  the  unstressed  one  

For:s-­‐lenis  pairs  -­‐     998   lenis   tokens   corresponding   to   998   for:s   tokens   differing   only   in   the  

plosive  in  ques:on  

Social conditioning In   apparent-­‐:me,   pre-­‐aspira:on   and   breathiness   occurrence   show   a   very  similar   paeern.   Pre-­‐aspira:on   and   breathiness   dura:ons   show   the   same  paeern,  but  breathiness  is  more  variable  across  the  individual.  

Figure   4:   (on   the   leD)   pre-­‐aspira8on   and   breathiness   occurrence   in   apparent-­‐8me;   (on   the   right)   pre-­‐aspira8on  and  breathiness  dura8on  in  apparent-­‐8me.  

However,   only   pre-­‐aspira:on   occurrence   is   sensi:ve   to   gender:   the   young  the  male  speakers,  the  more  frequent  the  pre-­‐aspira:on:  

Figure 5: pre-aspiration occurrence in apparent-time across gender.

70 100 130 160 190 220 250 280

20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90

Freq

uenc

y of

occ

urre

nce

Age

Age and breathiness & pre-aspiration occurrence

"pre-aspiration" "breathiness" Linear("pre-aspiration") Linear("breathiness")

2 2.5

3 3.5

4 4.5

5 5.5

6 6.5

7 7.5

8 8.5

9 9.5 10

20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90

Dur

atio

n (%

)

Age

Age and breathiness & pre-aspiration duration

"pre-aspiration" "breathiness" Linear("pre-aspiration") Linear("breathiness")

70

100

130

160

190

220

250

20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90

Pre-

aspi

ratio

n fr

eque

ncy

Age

Age, gender, and pre-aspiration occurrence

females males Linear(females) Linear(males)

Fortis-lenis contrast Both   pre-­‐aspira:on   and   breathiness   enhance   the   for:s-­‐lenis   contrast  (based   on   acous:c   evidence).   However,   whilst   pre-­‐aspira:on   is   never  found  in  the  lenis  series,  breathiness  is.    

Figure  8:   (on  the   leD)  occurrence  of  pre-­‐aspira8on  and  the  plosive  series  per  posi8on   in  word  and  sentence;   (on   the   right)  occurrence  of  breathiness  and   the  plosive   series  per  posi8on   in  word  and  sentence.    

References