20
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [BMELV] On: 28 January 2009 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 907835565] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Experimental Aging Research Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713722867 An Exploratory Study of Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise in Handball Goalkeepers Jörg Schorer a ; Joseph Baker b a Institute for Sport Science, Westfalian Wilhelms-University Münster, Münster, Germany b School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, and Carnegie Research Institute, Leeds Metropolitan University, Leeds, UK Online Publication Date: 01 January 2009 To cite this Article Schorer, Jörg and Baker, Joseph(2009)'An Exploratory Study of Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise in Handball Goalkeepers',Experimental Aging Research,35:1,1 — 19 To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/03610730802544641 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03610730802544641 Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    9

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

This article was downloaded by: [BMELV]On: 28 January 2009Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 907835565]Publisher RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Experimental Aging ResearchPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713722867

An Exploratory Study of Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise in HandballGoalkeepersJörg Schorer a; Joseph Baker b

a Institute for Sport Science, Westfalian Wilhelms-University Münster, Münster, Germany b School ofKinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, and Carnegie Research Institute,Leeds Metropolitan University, Leeds, UK

Online Publication Date: 01 January 2009

To cite this Article Schorer, Jörg and Baker, Joseph(2009)'An Exploratory Study of Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise in HandballGoalkeepers',Experimental Aging Research,35:1,1 — 19

To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/03610730802544641

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03610730802544641

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial orsystematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply ordistribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contentswill be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug dosesshould be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directlyor indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Page 2: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF AGING ANDPERCEPTUAL-MOTOR EXPERTISE IN HANDBALLGOALKEEPERS

Jorg Schorer

Institute for Sport Science, Westfalian Wilhelms-University Munster,Munster, Germany

Joseph Baker

School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University,Toronto, Ontario, Canada, and Carnegie Research Institute,

Leeds Metropolitan University, Leeds, UK

Few studies have examined the effect of age on skilled perception. Thepurpose of this study was to determine whether the perceptual-motorabilities of highly skilled performers in dynamic, time-constrained sportsexhibited the same pattern of age-related decline seen in other areas. Thesample for this study involved five age-specific groups of handball goal-keepers. Each participant completed an eye-tracking task, a temporalocclusion task, and an eight-choice reaction time task. Results revealedage-related declines in motor performance but not perceptual perform-ance. Skilled perception appears resistant to normal age-related declinesover time through the use of compensatory mechanisms.

Received 20 May 2006; accepted 17 April 2007.

This research was sponsored by the German Institute for Sport Science (VF 0407/06/47/

2003–2004) and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (grant no.

410-04-1207). It was conducted at the Institute for Sport and Sport Science, University of

Heidelberg. The authors would like to thank Sibille Abel, Florian Fath, and Ulrike Langenstein

for their helping hand in data collection and data analyses. The authors would also like to

thank those who gave them valuable advice and feedback on the current paper: Sean Horton,

Norbert Hagemann, and Steve Cobley as well as one reviewer and the section editor.

Address correspondence to Jorg Schorer, Institute for Sport Science, Westfalian Wilhelms-

University of Munster, Horstmarer Landweg 62b, 48149 Munster, Germany. E-mail:

[email protected]

Experimental Aging Research, 35: 1–19, 2009

Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

ISSN: 0361-073X print/1096-4657 online

DOI: 10.1080/03610730802544641

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 3: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

Skill maintenance is of increasing importance in aging societies and acomprehensive understanding of the relationship between age andskill is essential. Examinations of abilities ranging from reactiontime (Entier, Sibley, Pomeroy, & Kao, 2003; Fozard, Vercruyssen,Reynolds, Hancock, & Quilter, 1994) and memory (Henry,MacLeod, Phillips, & Crawford, 2004) to muscular strength(Kallman, Plato, & Tobin, 1990) and flexibility (Einkauf, Gohdes,Jensen, & Jewell, 1987) imply an inevitable downward spiral offunctional ability with age.

Studies of cognitive and motor skills suggest performance can bemaintained at high levels in spite of advancing age. For instance,Charness’ (1981) study of chess players found that high levels ofperformance could be maintained as performers got older. Otherexaminations of cognitive-motor experts, such as pianists (Krampe& Ericsson, 1996) and typists (Salthouse, 1984), substantiate thesefindings.

One of the most widely supported explanations for the mainte-nance of skilled performance over time is that performers make com-pensatory adaptations (Backman & Dixon, 1992). Several theories ofcompensation exist; for example, the theory of Selective Optimizationwith Compensation (SOC) advocated by Baltes and colleagues (Baltes& Baltes, 1980, 1990; Marsiske, Lang, Baltes, & Baltes, 1995) viewslife span development as a dynamic, ongoing process of balancinggains and losses, whereas Salthouse’s compensation model(Salthouse, 1987, 1990) is conceptually grounded in the interactionbetween experience and performance. However, a common aspectacross these theories is the notion that although individual compo-nents of a skill may decline with age, it is possible for overall perform-ance to remain the same due to an increased reliance on other aspectsof performance. Put more simply, the theory suggests that skilled per-formers strategically compensate for a decline in one area by develop-ing or improving in another. Empirical support for this notion can befound in studies examining chess players and typists. Charness (1981)found that skilled, older chess players could perform at the same levelas younger skilled players despite age-related deficiencies in memoryability. He explained these results by suggesting older players com-pensate for their declining memory through more efficient infor-mation processing (i.e., a more systematic search of the problemspace and a better global evaluation of chess positions). In addition,studies have found little decline in expert typing skill with advancingage (Bosman, 1993; Salthouse, 1984); the evidence indicates thatexpert typists compensate for age-related declines by scanning furtherahead in the text, which allows them to begin keystroke preparation

2 J. Schorer and J. Baker

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 4: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

earlier. As a result of this advanced planning, aging typists can offseta deficiency in one area by improving performance in another.

An alternative model, Selective Maintenance, advocated byEricsson and Krampe (Ericsson, 2000; Krampe & Ericsson, 1996),proposes that expert performance in skilled domains is maintainedin very specific capacities through appropriate attention to ‘deliberatepractice’ (i.e., high-quality training activities done with the specificpurpose of improving performance). Through extensive focus onappropriate training, experts are able to develop domain-specificmechanisms (e.g., expert hockey players’ ability to read offensivepatterns of play and predict their opponents’ actions; see Starkes,Helsen, & Jack, 2001, for a review of these domain-specific character-istics) that allow them to circumvent general age-related limitations,with these mechanisms more resistant to degradation over time—provided that training persists. To test this hypothesis, Krampe andEricsson (1996) compared older and younger pianists on a range ofperformance-related measures. In addition, they compared perfor-mers at the expert and amateur levels (i.e., older expert, olderamateur, younger expert, and younger amateur). They found thatolder performers, both amateur and expert, showed the same patternof age-related decline on general measures of performance, such asreaction time; however, domain-specific measures of performance,such as finger tapping speed and quality of performance, were main-tained in older experts. In most cases, differences in domain-specificmeasures of performance between younger and older experts wereaccounted for by differences in the amount of training and practicerather than age. Based on these results, the authors concluded thatpersistent regular training in a domain over time would allow agingperformers to maintain their skills.

Researchers have also examined the age-related decline of athleticskill, reporting that cognitive and motor skills appear more resistantto decline than physiological skills such as running, swimming, andcycling. For example, a recent examination of performance inPGA golfers (Baker, Horton, Pearce, & Deakin, 2005) found thatalthough performance among elite golfers consistently declined asthe golfers aged, this rate of decline was less than half the rate ofphysiological skills. All the same, this research indicates that cogni-tive, motor, and physiological capacities are susceptible to age-related decline, although the rate of decline may be buffered bydomain-specific practice.

One area of skilled performance that has not been adequatelyexamined in aging research is perception. Perception has been pre-sented as a critical variable distinguishing highly skilled from lesser

Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise 3

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 5: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

skilled performers (cf. Williams & Ward, 2003). Researchers haveconsistently reported experts do not differ from nonexperts invisual abilities, rather, the differences lie in the experts’ ability tointerpret visual information more efficiently and effectively(Abernethy, Neal, & Koning, 1994). For instance, studies examin-ing the visual cue utilization of expert cricket (Abernethy &Russell, 1984), badminton (Abernethy, 1991; Abernethy & Russell,1987), soccer (Williams & Burwitz, 1993), and squash players(Abernethy, 1990, 1991) reveal that experts use body position cuesobtained from their opponents precontact (i.e., before they bowl orbefore they strike the birdie or ball) to provide information abouthow best to respond in a situation. Due to the limited utility ofpost-opponent-contact information (i.e., after the opponent bowlsor strikes), experts from these sports have learned to rely on visualcues from the opponent’s arm and wrist position to predict wheretheir opponent will place the ball (cf. Land & McLeod, 2000).They then make the necessary adjustments to attend to their pre-diction.

Previous examinations of perceptual decline have shown age-related declinations in the ability to perceive motion (Gilmore, Wenk,Naylor, & Stuve, 1992), speed (Norman, Ross, Hawkes, & Long,2003), and depth (Norman, Dawson, & Butler, 2000) as well as theability to distinguish two- (2-D) and three-dimensional (3-D) shapes(Andersen & Atchley, 1995; Norman et al., 2000). However, thesestudies have relied exclusively on the perception of information innovel tasks using nonexpert samples. Research robustly indicates thatskilled perception is domain-specific and as a result, general or noveltasks may not be appropriate for investigating the influence of age onskilled perception.

One limitation of many perceptual aging studies is that they onlyconsider one aspect of information processing. This current studytries to overcome this limitation by considering all three stages ofinformation processing: (1) perceptual processes, (2) response selec-tion, and (3) response execution (cf. Wickens, 1992). Although thesewill be analyzed in the beginning separately, they will be discussed intotal. Therefore, we will try to overcome the compartmentalizationthat is necessary for the results section in the discussion. Our primaryresearch question was whether the perceptual-motor skills of highlyskilled performers in dynamic, time-constrained sports exhibitthe same pattern of age-related decline seen in other areas of skill?Additionally, the study examined the effect of age on skilledperception in the absence of task-relevant practice (i.e., maintenancepractice or deliberate practice).

4 J. Schorer and J. Baker

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 6: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

METHODS

Participants

Five groups of male handball goalkeepers participated in thisstudy. The youngest group (sub-youth) consisted of eight goal-keepers with a mean age of 14.4 years (SD¼ 0.5) from a regionalselection, which is the highest selection level for their age. Thesecond group (youth) consisted of five goalkeepers with a meanage of 16.8 years (SD¼ 1.1) from the youth national team. Thethird and fourth groups (junior and adult, respectively) consistedof nine goalkeepers with a mean age of 19.2 years (SD¼ 1.6) fromthe junior national team and eight national team goalkeepers with amean age of 27.3 years (SD¼ 5.8).1 Finally, the fifth group (senior)consisted of three retired national goal-keepers with a mean age of46.7 years (SD¼ 3.8), who are no longer actively training or com-peting. Participants in this group were inactive from training andcompeting in handball for at least 5 years. Informed consent wasobtained before commencing the experiment.

Apparatus

For all tasks, participants stood at a distance of 7-m in front of a nor-mal 2.00� 3.00-m handball goal facing a 2.40� 3.20-m video screen.Above the handball goal a projector (Geha compact 212þ ) wasplaced connected to a notebook computer (Acer Travelmate660LCi), which was used to present the different tasks (Software:NBS Presentation 0.80) as well as send trigger signals to the eye-tracking system. The eye-tracking system was a head-mounted SMIiView X HED with a sample rate of 50 Hz. Participants were askedto wear a common bicycle helmet with two cameras and a senderfor wireless transmission. All participant hardware was connectedto a battery, which was carried in a daypack. The unit was relativelylight (450 g) and participants reported no disturbance from either thehelmet or the battery. Eye-tracking signals of eye and scene videoswere transmitted wirelessly to a second computer with two receiverunits. The body movements of the goalkeepers were recorded by adigital video camera (Panasonic AGDVC-15). These video recordingswere then used for the offline analyses of body movements to deter-mine different dependent variables.

1The adult participants are from a study by Schorer (2005).

Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise 5

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 7: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

Procedure

Three different tests were conducted with each participant. Followinga warm up, participants (i) performed an eye-tracking handball videotask; (ii) a temporal occlusion handball video task; and (iii) an eight-choice reaction time task. In each task, participants were asked toconduct real goalkeeper movements. Therefore, at the beginning ofa trial they would stand in the middle of the goal and then react tothe stimulus (e.g., reach to the lower right corner of the goal).

These tasks were conducted in the order presented above, becausethe whole body movements caused participants to sweat after a rela-tively short time and the eye-tracking system became less stable withsweat. This might have resulted into carryover effects, but given thelarge experience as well as the number of different throwers andthrows, we decided that the methodological effect from the decreasedstability would be greater and therefore kept the same order for allparticipants. After the tests, participants filled out a questionnaireon their training history and game experience.

Eye-Tracking Handball Video Task (ET)Prior to this first task, the bicycle helmet was fitted to the parti-cipant’s head and the eye-movement camera and the scene camerawere adjusted. The system was calibrated using a 5-point system.A total of 64 scenes were presented to the goalkeepers and the firsthalf of the scenes were accomplished in a duration of 5.5 min. Aftera short break, the calibration was reevaluated and adjusted if neces-sary prior to showing the second half of the scenes to the goalkeepers.Their task was to defend the displayed throws. The intertrial intervalwas set to 5 ss.

Temporal Occlusion Handball Video Task (TO)The temporal occlusion paradigm, as introduced to sport science byAbernethy and Russell (1987), was used to assess visual anticipationin handball goalkeeping. Participants were presented the above-described videos of handball penalty throws. Each clip was editedat three different points of time. Instead of using an absolute timeframe, three points were chosen by the relative timing of the move-ment, because the duration differences between videotaped penalty-throws were significant. The first temporal occlusion point (TOP 1)was the return point of the movement. For the third temporalocclusion point (TOP 3), release of the handball from the hand waschosen. The second temporal occlusion point (TOP 2) was calculatedas the midpoint between points 1 and 3 (Figure 1).

6 J. Schorer and J. Baker

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 8: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

Forty-two edited video clips were then randomized over temporalocclusion points and throws. Participants were to react as quicklyand correctly as possible to the presented video tapes. The presen-tation and the required reactions were as described for the eye-tracking tasks. The intertrial interval was set to 5 s.

Choice Reaction Time Task (CRT)The third task involved an eight-choice reaction time task with 29trials. As in the other tasks, participants stood in front of the screen;however, in this task they had to react to the position where the hand-ball was presented. The eight positions were upper left, upper middle,upper right, middle left, middle right, lower left, lower middle, andlower right. For this task, the intertrial interval was set to 3 s.

Dependent Measures and Statistical Analysis

Three dependent measurements were common across all tasks. Firstmeasured was the movement initiation time of the goalkeepers, whichwas defined as the difference from stimuli appearance to the begin-ning of the movement by the participants. This duration was calcu-lated using an offline video analysis of the whole body movementsof each participant in each trial. The video recordings showed a flick-ering of luminance for the video projector positioned right on top ofthe goal as soon as a trial (video) began. This was used to representthe moment of stimuli appearance. The beginning of the movementby the participants was defined as the moment where the goalkeeper

Figure 1. Images indicating cut points for temporal occlusion task.

Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise 7

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 9: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

raised his foot. This characteristic proved to be a valid indicator formovement initiation. Second, reaction quality was measured as thepercentage of correct reactions. Again, for each trial the directionof the whole-body movement was assessed using the video recordings.If the defending movement was into the correct region, then a onewas given for this trial (¼ save), whereas a zero was noted forthe wrong direction (¼ goal). Although different measures wereconsidered (e.g., being correct in the x- or y-axis), a more game likeapproach was chosen. Within the sport of handball, you haveeither a save or a goal. Third, movement time was calculated asthe difference between the start of the movement and the reach tothe required region.

Number of fixations as well as relative and absolute fixationdurations were counted as additional dependent measures for botheye-tracking tasks. An eye-fixation was defined as a duration of120 ms or longer.

Due to the range of skills required for the various tasks, each of thethree stages of the information-processing model can be consideredindependently. For stage 1, perceptual processes, the capacity to per-ceive task-relevant information was measured via the eye-movementmeasures. Stage 2, response selection, was measured by reaction timein all three tasks, and stage 3, response execution, was evaluatedusing measures of movement time in all tasks. In addition, generalperformance was measured via reaction quality.

Instead of using a descriptive statistical approach, a more explora-tory data analysis was conducted, because cell sizes were small andvaried among the groups (cf. Sedlmeier, 1996). Figures are presentedas boxplots, based on a rank scale and, therefore, less influenced byoutliers. The line in the middle of the box is the median. The boxshows 25% and 75% quartiles and the lines above and below showthe highest and lowest values that are not outliers. Circles or asteriskabove or below these lines indicate outliers. The width of the boxesallows a relative comparison of cell sizes per group (cf. Benjamini,1988; Sedlmeier, 1996).

For the inferential statistical analysis, a conservative Exact Test byKruskal-Wallis for independent samples was calculated, which workswell with small samples and rank scales. For the Monte Carlo sam-pling, the default values with a sample size of 10,000 and confidencelevels of 99% were used. Due to the small sample size the exact orasymptotic p value seemed inappropriate; therefore, Monte Carlo pvalues will be reported. Post hoc comparisons of independent sampleswere calculated using Mann-Whitney U tests (alpha criterion set on.05). SPSS 12.0 was used for all analyses.

8 J. Schorer and J. Baker

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 10: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

RESULTS

The results section is divided into several parts. The first describes theexperience of the various groups of goalkeepers. The second examinesresults for general performance (measured via reaction quality),whereas the third, fourth, and fifth parts consider perceptual pro-cesses (measured via number of fixations, relative and absolute fix-ation duration), response selection (measured via reaction time),and response execution (measured via movement time), respectively.

Experience

Results for the history of goalkeeper-specific handball training andgame experience of the athletes are presented in Table 1.

As expected, significant differences were observed for game experi-ence (H(4)¼ 24.27; p< .01) and goal-keeping training (H(4)¼ 18.89;p< .01). Post hoc tests revealed significant differences for gameexperience between the adult group and sub-youths (U¼ 0.00;z¼�3.37; p< .01), youths (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.94; p< .01), and juniors(U¼ 3.50; z¼�3.32; p< .01), as well as between the senior groupand sub-youths (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.46; p¼ .01), youths (U¼ 0.00;z¼�2.26; p¼ .03), juniors (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.54; p< .01), and adults(U¼ 0.50; z¼�2.36; p¼ .02) and between sub-youths and juniors(U¼ 9.50; z¼�2.57; p< .01). For goalkeeper-specific training, sig-nificant differences were found between the sub-youth group andyouths (U¼ 4.00; z¼�2.38; p¼ .02), juniors (U¼ 6.00; z¼�2.93;p< .01), and adults (U¼ 0.50; z¼�3.31; p< .01), as well as betweenthe adult group and youths (U¼ 2.50; z¼�2.60; p< .01) and juniors(U¼ 8.50; z¼�2.67; p< .01). No significant differences betweenseniors and any other group were found due to the high standarddeviation of 12 years. This high deviation was caused by the widerange of experiences reported by the seniors (range, 1 to 25 years).However, all seniors were past Olympic medallists.

Table 1. Comparison of game experience and goalkeeper-specific training

Sub-youth

(n¼ 8)

Youth

(n¼ 5)

Juniors

(n¼ 9)

Adults

(n¼ 8)

Seniors

(n¼ 3)

Game experience 5.2 (3.7) 9.1 (1.0) 10.6 (3.5) 18.3 (3.9) 18.3 (1.5)

Goalkeeper specific training 1.3 (1.5) 3.7 (1.0) 5.0 (3.5) 11.1 (7.0) 13.7 (12.0)

Note. All values are in years. Values enclosed in parentheses represent standard deviations.

Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise 9

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 11: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

General Performance

Results for general performance as measured by reaction quality arepresented in Figure 2. No significant differences for reaction qualityin ET were observed (H(4)¼ 6.86; p¼ .13). However, for TO a maineffect was found (H(4)¼ 9.53; p¼ .03) and post hoc tests revealedsignificant differences between sub-youths and adults (U¼ 6.00;z¼�2.74; p< .01) and between sub-youths and seniors (U¼ 0.50;z¼�2.36; p¼ .02), with sub-youth athletes scoring lower on theTO task. No significant effects were found for the CRT data(H(4)¼ 8.48; p¼ .06).

Perceptual Processes

Results for the analysis of eye movements are presented in Figures 3to 5. For the number of fixations, a significant effect between groupswas found (H(4)¼ 22.95; p< .01), generally indicating that oldergroups had more fixations than younger groups (Figure 3). Posthoc tests revealed differences between sub-youths and adults(U¼ 0.00; z¼�3.24; p< .01), between youths and adults (U¼ 0.00;z¼�2.84; p< .01), between juniors and adults (U¼ 0.00;

Figure 2. Reaction quality (in percent) for sub-youth (n¼ 8), youth (n¼ 5),

junior (n¼ 9), adult (n¼ 9), and senior (n¼ 3) groups for the ET (dark grey),

TO (light grey), and CRT (white) tasks.

10 J. Schorer and J. Baker

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 12: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

z¼�3.33; p< .01), between youths and juniors (U¼ 5.00; z¼�2.33;p¼ .02), as well as between seniors and sub-youth (U¼ 0.00;z¼�2.45; p¼ .01), between seniors and youths (U¼ 0.00;z¼�2.24; p¼ .04), and seniors and juniors (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.45;p< .01).

This significant difference between younger and older groupswas also found for relative fixation duration (Figure 4; H(4)¼13.42; p< .01), with post hoc tests revealing differences betweensub-youths and adults (U¼ 7.00; z¼�2.43; p¼ .02), betweensub-youths and seniors (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.44; p¼ .01), as well asbetween juniors and adults (U¼ 0.00; z¼�3.33; p< .01) andbetween juniors and seniors (U¼ 2.00; z¼�2.13; p¼ .04). In gen-eral, these results indicate older groups (adults and seniors) hadsignificantly longer relative fixations than the younger groups(sub-youths and juniors).

This trend continued for the absolute fixation duration. Significantdifferences were observed between groups (Figure 5; H(4)¼ 21.47;p< .01) and post hoc tests revealed differences between sub-youthsand adults (U¼ 0.00; z¼�3.24; p< .01), between sub-youthsand seniors (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.44; p¼ .01), between juniors and

Figure 3. Number of fixations for sub-youth (n¼ 8), youth (n¼ 5), junior(n¼ 9), adult (n¼ 9), and senior (n¼ 3) groups for the ET task.

Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise 11

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 13: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

Figure 4. Relative fixation duration (in ms) for sub-youth (n¼ 8), youth

(n¼ 5), junior (n¼ 9), adult (n¼ 9), and senior (n¼ 3) groups for the ET task.

Figure 5. Absolute fixation duration (in ms) for sub-youth (n¼ 8), youth

(n¼ 5), junior (n¼ 9), adult (n¼ 9), and senior (n¼ 3) groups for the ET task.

12 J. Schorer and J. Baker

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 14: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

adults (U¼ 0.00; z¼�3.33; p< .01), and between juniors andseniors (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.50; p< .01). Additionally there was asignificant difference between youths and adults (U¼ 1.00; z¼�2.68; p< .01).

Response Selection

Results for movement initiation time are presented in Figure 6. A sig-nificant effect between groups was revealed for ET (H(4)¼ 9.19;p¼ .04). Post hoc tests indicated seniors had significantly lower scoresthan sub-youths (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.45; p¼ .01), juniors (U¼ 2.00;z¼�2.13; p¼ .04), and adults (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.45; p¼ .01). No sig-nificant differences were observed for TO (H(3)¼ 4.11; p¼ .41), butdifferences were revealed for CRT (H(4)¼ 11.77; p< .01). Post hoctests revealed that adults had significantly lower movement initiationtimes than sub-youths (U¼ 8.00; z¼�2.52; p< .01), youths(U¼ 6.00; z¼�2.05; p¼ .05), juniors (U¼ 13.00; z¼�2.21;p¼ .03), and seniors (U¼ 0.00; z¼�2.09; p¼ .05).

Figure 6. Movement initiation time (in frames) for sub-youth (n¼ 8), youth

(n¼ 5), junior (n¼ 9), adult (n¼ 9), and senior (n¼ 3) groups for the ET (darkgrey), TO (light grey), and CRT (white) tasks.

Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise 13

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 15: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

Response Execution

For movement time, no significant differences were observed betweenthe four younger groups for either of the handball-specific tasks(Figure 7; ET: (H(3)¼ 6.86; p¼ .07); TO: (H(3)¼ 2.22; p¼ .56)). Theseniors were asked to perform the same movements as the youngergroups, but due to lower physical fitness they were incapable of complet-ing the required movements and as a result their data were excludedfrom the inferential statistics, but included in the graph. The medianvalues for the seniors on these tasks were lower than the values forthe sub-youth, junior, and adult groups. For CRT, there were significantdifferences (H(3)¼ 7.76; p¼ .04) and post hoc tests indicated sub-youthshad significantly greater movement times than juniors (U¼ 11.00;z¼�2.41; p¼ .02) and adults (U¼ 11.00; z¼�2.21; p¼ .03).

DISCUSSION

Results from the present study point to the maintenance of skilledperception in the face of advancing age. Without being involved in

Figure 7. Movement time (in frames) for sub-youth (n¼ 8), youth (n¼ 5),junior (n¼ 9), adult (n¼ 9), and senior (n¼ 3) groups for the ET (dark grey),

TO (light grey), and CRT (white) tasks.

14 J. Schorer and J. Baker

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 16: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

training or competition, senior handball goalkeepers performedsimilarly to current expert players and superior to lower level players.These results are intriguing and suggest several interesting possibili-ties regarding the nature of skilled perception. First, it suggests thatskilled perception is not vulnerable to the same rate of age-relateddecline as more general measures of perception, which have beenfound to be age-specific (e.g., Gilmore et al., 1992; Norman et al.,2003). The explanation for this result may lie in the nature of thecapacities being compared. Measures of general perceptual abilitieshave typically examined participants’ aptitude for perceiving novelstimuli, whereas skilled perception deals with individuals’ skill atperceiving highly specific, domain-relevant information. Moreover,the nature of skilled perception for athletes in time-constrained,decision-making sports may be more akin to a problem-solving task.Experts from these sports (including handball goalkeepers; c.f.Schorer, 2005) have learned to rely on advanced visual informationprovided by their opponent, and once they have learned the necessarysources of this information, it may be highly resistant to memorydecay. Evidence from the eye-movement data suggests that seniorsand current experts were very similar in the number of fixationsand their absolute and relative duration. Future studies should exam-ine this issue in more detail using other skilled perception tasks,which may relate more generally to aging populations. For example,although the general ability to perceive motion and speed declineswith age (Gilmore et al., 1992; Norman et al., 2003), perceivingtask–specific speed and motion information such as when drivingmay be maintained to a greater degree.

The current results also indicate that handball goalkeepers are notresistant to all forms of age-related decline. Motor performance, asmeasured by movement time, was affected as much as previousresearch would suggest and on these tasks senior goalkeepers wereeither the same as, or inferior to, their younger counterparts. Interest-ingly, the reaction time data provided valuable information regardingthe specific areas of decline in these players. For the handball-specifictask (ET), the senior goalkeepers showed an earlier movementinitiation than the adults; however, adults responded more quicklyon the more general reaction time task, suggesting that very specificadaptations are occurring. Further evidence of this is found inthe measures of overall performance. Senior players’ performancewas not significantly different from the other groups on the ETor CRT tasks, but they were significantly better on the TO task,which requires the use of highly domain-specific information at anearlier time.

Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise 15

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 17: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

Schorer (2005) suggested that performers in dynamic, time-constrained sports adapt to the specific performance constraints oftheir environment. In his view, the expert perceptual system adaptsto these constraints to be in the right spot at the right time. If theyinitiate their movement too late or too early, they will be unable toperform effectively. Data from the current study may indicatethat senior goalkeepers adjust their ‘‘just-in-time’’ strategy to accom-modate age-related declines such as motor deficits. The lack of differ-ence between the senior and adult groups on the ET task in light ofdifferences on the TO task may at first seem contradictory. If differ-ences in response initiation exist, they should be evident in the ETtask as well. However, the TO task was externally paced by occludingthe videos early, whereas external timing is missing in the ET task,which could lead to a later decision. Some participants may have atendency to react early and risk a worse reaction quality, whereasothers might decide to have a better reaction quality and a worsereaction time, perhaps suggesting the use of ‘trade-off’ strategies byparticipants. The superiority of the seniors in the reaction qualityin TO suggests they actually learned to anticipate earlier. Theseresults, coupled with previous findings from Bosman (1993),Charness (1981), and Salthouse (1984), reinforce the conclusion thatthe expert perceptual-cognitive system is highly plastic and capable ofreorganization to meet changing environmental demands.

Despite the intriguing results from this study, there were severallimitations that restrict their application. First, the data were cross-sectional and as a result may not represent the aging process due tocohort effects. It may be that the senior group was always superiorto the other groups (priori disposition; Krampe & Charness, 2006)and if so an age-related decline would not be evident in our analyses.However, comparisons of cross-sectional and longitudinal data inhighly skilled groups indicates that the rate of loss may be overesti-mated when evaluating cross-sectional data (Starkes, Weir, & Young,2003) and as a result, the current data may overestimate performancelosses over time. Second, although cursory information regardingtraining history was collected, it is unclear whether performance inperceptually similar activities may have facilitated maintenance ofthese abilities as suggested by Ericsson (2000). For example, it isunclear what role watching games may play in maintaining these per-ceptual skills. Finally, a general limitation of studies of this nature isthat examination of genuine expertise is necessarily limited by the factthat the number of experts is, by definition, small, making the estab-lishment of large sample sizes with strong statistical power extremelydifficult. This limitation is particularly meaningful in the current

16 J. Schorer and J. Baker

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 18: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

study with regards to the senior group, which had only 3 participantsand a high degree of variability in training experience. The currentstudy recognizes these difficulties and, although unable to eliminatethem, has attempted to work within these constraints. It is also note-worthy that the senior performers were significantly younger (meanage 46.7 years) than commonly used in studies of aging and expertise.However, longitudinal analyses of performance with age note age-related declines in performance immediately following peak perform-ance (see Bortz & Bortz, 1996, for a review). Although the seniorparticipants were somewhat younger than usual, they provide usefulinformation regarding age-related decline in this sport because peakperformance in handball typically occurs in the late 20 s or early30 s. Moreover, the period of mid-life is often underresearched butcould prove quite valuable for understanding the factors promotingeither skill maintenance as seen in many aging experts or theincreased rate of decline seen subsequent to this period in ‘normal’human aging.

It is prudent that the current results be viewed as exploratory,requiring corroboration and confirmation. As previously mentioned,data for this study were collected from a very elite sample of athletesand as a result, the generalizability of their results to ‘normal’ popula-tions is assumed rather than proven. Investigations of the age-relateddecline in performance for complex, everyday skills (e.g., driving acar) may provide insight into how well this study’s results describethe profile of decline in skilled perception with age. This researchcould prove valuable for the development of informed policiesrelevant to our aging population.

REFERENCES

Abernethy, B. (1990). Anticipation in squash: Differences in advance cue utilization

between expert and novice players. Journal of Sports Sciences, 8, 17–34.

Abernethy, B. (1991). Visual search strategies and decision-making in sport.

International Journal of Sport Psychology, 22, 189–210.

Abernethy, B., Neal, R. J., & Koning, P. (1994). Visual-perceptual and cognitive

differences between expert, intermediate, and novice snooker players. Applied

Cognitive Psychology, 8, 185–211.

Abernethy, B. & Russell, D. G. (1984). Advance cue utilisation by skilled cricket

batsmen. The Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 16, 2–10.

Abernethy, B. & Russell, D. G. (1987). The relationship between expertise and visual

search strategy in a racquet sport. Human Movement Science, 6, 283–319.

Andersen, G. J. & Atchley, P. (1995). Age-related differences in the detection

of three-dimensional surfaces from optic flow. Psychology and Aging, 10, 650–658.

Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise 17

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 19: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

Backman, L. & Dixon, R. A. (1992). Psychological compensation: A theoretical

framework. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 259–283.

Baker, J., Horton, S., Pearce, G., & Deakin, J. (2005). A longitudinal examination of

performance decline in champion golfers. High Ability Studies, 16, 179–185.

Baltes, P. B. & Baltes, M. M. (1980). Plasticity and variability in psychological aging:

Methodological and theoretical issues. In G. Gurski (Ed.), Determining the effects

of aging on the central nervous system (pp. 41–60). Berlin: Schering.

Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful

aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes

& M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences

(pp. 1–34). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Benjamini, Y. (1988). Opening the box of a boxplot. The American Statistician, 42,

257–262.

Bortz, W. M. & Bortz, W. M. (1996). How fast do we age? Exercise performance

over time as a biomarker. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 51, 223–225.

Bosman, E. A. (1993). Age-related differences in the motoric aspects of transcrip-

tion typing skill. Psychology and Aging, 8, 87–102.

Charness, N. (1981). Search in chess: Age and skill differences. Journal of Experi-

mental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 467–476.

Einkauf, D. K., Gohdes, M. L., Jensen, G. M., & Jewell, M. J. (1987). Changes in

spinal mobility with increasing age in women. Physical Therapy, 67, 370–375.

Entier, J. L., Sibley, B. A., Pomeroy, J., & Kao, J. C. (2003). Components of reaction

time as a function of age, physical activity, and aerobic fitness. Journal of Aging

and Physical Activity, 11, 319–332.

Ericsson, K. A. (2000). How experts attain and maintain superior performance:

Implications for the enhancement of skilled performance in older individuals.

Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 8, 366–372.

Fozard, J. L., Vercruyssen, M., Reynolds, S. L., Hancock, P. A., & Quilter, R. E.

(1994). Age differences and changes in reaction time: The Baltimore Longitudi-

nal Study of Aging. Journal of Gerontology, 49, P179–P189.

Gilmore, G. C., Wenk, H. E., Naylor, L. A., & Stuve, T. A. (1992). Motion percep-

tion and aging. Psychology and Aging, 7, 654–660.

Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A

meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging,

19, 27–39.

Kallman, D. A., Plato, C. C., & Tobin, J. D. (1990). The role of muscle loss in the

age-related decline of grip strength: Cross section and longitudinal perspectives.

Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 45, M82–M88.

Krampe, R. T. & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate prac-

tice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: General, 125, 331–359.

Krampe, R. T. & Charness, N. (2006). Aging and expertise. In K. A. Ericsson, N.

Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. Hoffmann (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of expertise

and expert performance (pp. 723–742). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Land, M. F. & McLeod, P. (2000). From eye movements to actions: How batsmen

hit the ball. Nature neuroscience, 3, 1340–1345.

18 J. Schorer and J. Baker

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009

Page 20: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE - York University

Marsiske, M., Lang, F. R., Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1995). Selective optimiza-

tion with compensation: Life-span perspectives on successful human develop-

ment. In R. A. Dixon & L. Backman (Eds.), Compensating for psychological

deficits and declines: Managing losses and promoting gains (pp. 35–79). Mahwah,

NJ: Erlbaum.

Norman, J. F., Dawson, T. E., & Butler, A. K. (2000). The effects of age upon the

perception of depth and 3-D shape from motion and binocular disparity. Percep-

tion, 29, 1335–1359.

Norman, J. F., Ross, H. E., Hawkes, L. M., & Long, J. R. (2003). Aging and the

perception of speed. Perception, 32, 85–96.

Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: General, 113, 345–371.

Salthouse, T. A. (1987). Age, experience, and compensation. In C. Schooler & K. W.

Schaie (Eds.), Cognitive functioning and social structure over the life course

(pp. 142–150). New York: Ablex.

Salthouse, T. A. (1990). Cognitive competence and expertise in aging. In J. E. Birren

& K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (3rd ed., pp. 310–319).

San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Schorer, J. (2005). Being right-on-time shows the sensory-motor expertise of

national handball goalkeepers. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 27, S135.

Sedlmeier, P. (1996). Jenseits des Signifikanztest-Rituals: Erganzungen und Alterna-

tiven [Beyond the ritual of significance testing: Alternative and supplementary

methods]. Methods of Psychological Research Online, 1 [htttp://www.hsp.de/

MPR/issue1/index.html].

Starkes, J. L., Helsen, W., & Jack, R. (2001) Expert performance in sport and dance.

In R. Singer, H. Hausenblas, & C. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of research in sport

psychology (2nd ed., pp. 174–201). New York: Macmillan.

Starkes, J. L., Weir, P. L., & Young, B. W. (2003). What does it take for older athletes

to continue to excel? In J. L. Starkes & K. A. Ericsson (Eds.), Expert performance

in sports: Advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 251–272). Champaign, IL:

Human Kinetics.

Wickens, C. D. (1992). Engineering psychology and human performance (2nd ed.).

New York: Wiley.

Williams, A. M. & Burwitz, L. (1993). Advance cue utilization in soccer. In T. Reilly,

J. Clarys & A. Stibbe (Eds.), Science and football II (pp. 239–244). London:

E & FN Spon.

Williams, A. M. & Ward, P. (2003). Perceptual expertise: Development in sport. In

J. L. Starkes & K. A. Ericsson (Eds.), Expert performance in sports: Advances in

research on sport expertise (pp. 219–250). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Aging and Perceptual-Motor Expertise 19

Downloaded By: [BMELV] At: 08:59 28 January 2009