Piaget Tended to View the Mind as a Single Entity That Grows as a Whole

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Piaget theory

Citation preview

Piaget tended to view the mind as a single entity that grows as a whole. In contrast, developmentalists who adopt the information-processing perspective treat the mind as a set of interacting components, any one of which might change as the child grows older. They attempt to explain specific changes in childrens mental abilities in terms of specific changes in one or another of the minds components or in terms of specific knowledge the child has acquired. The information-processing approach to cognition, begins with the assumption that the mind is a system, analogous to a computer, for analyzing information from the environment. According to the standard information-processing model , the minds basic machinery includes attention mechanisms for bringing information in, working memory for actively manipulating (or thinking about) information, and long-term memory for passively holding information so that it can be used in the future. Together, these components are analogous to the all-purpose operating system that is built into a computer. In addition, the mind contains specific strategies and rules for analyzing particular types of information or solving particular types of problems. These strategies and rules are analogous to the specific programs, such as a wordprocessing program, that are loaded into a computer. From the informationprocessing perspective, cognitive development can stem from changes in the minds general operating system, in its specific programs, or in both.Information Processing: A Computer Model of Cognition

An alternative to Piagets theory of cognitive development is the information-processing model, which compares the workings of the mind to a computer and studies how information is received, encoded, stored, organized, retrieved, and used by people of different ages. This model offers important insights into two major areas of cognition: attention and memory.Attention

Attention refers to focusing awareness on a narrowed range of stimuli. Infants pay attention to their environment for only short periods of time. Even toddlers, who can pay attention for longer periods, are easily distracted. When watching television, for example, 2-year-olds talk more to other people, play more with toys, and look around the room more than 4-year-olds. As they get older, childrens attention spans improve, and they learn to discriminate between what is and what is not important to concentrate on at any given time.Memory

After children attend to information and take it into their information-processing system, they must remember it. Attention determines what information enters the computer, whereas memory determines what information is saved.

Like attention, memory skills also improve gradually throughout childhood and adolescence (Guisande et al., 2007; Hayne, Boniface, & Barr, 2000; Wang, 2008). Two-year-olds can repeat back about two digits immediately after hearing them, but 10-year-olds can repeat about six. Improvement comes as children acquire strategies during the school years for storing and retrieving information. For example, they learn to rehearse or repeat information over and over, to use mnemonics (like i before e except after c), and to organize their information in ways that facilitate retrieval (Chapter 7).

As people grow older, their use of information-processing strategies and overall memory continues to change. Recall from Chapter 8 that fl uid intelligence (requiring speed or rapid learning) tends to decrease with age, whereas crystallized intelligence (knowledge and information gained over the life span) continues to increase until advanced old age.

Despite their concerns about keeping up with 18-year-olds, older returning students often do as well or better than their younger counterparts in college classes. This superior performance by older adult students is due in part to their generally greater academic motivation, but it also refl ects the importance of prior knowledge.

Cognitive psychologists have demonstrated that the more people know, the easier it is for them to lay down new memories (Goldstein, 2008; Matlin, 2008). Older students, for instance, generally fi nd this chapter on development easier to master than younger students. Their interactions with children and greater knowledge about life changes create a framework on which to hang new information. In summary, the more you know, the more you learn. Thus, having a college degree and stimulating occupation may help you stay mentally sharp in your later years (Schaie, 2008; Whitbourne, 2009).

Havent studies also shown decreases in older adults memory capabilities? As mentioned at the beginning of the chapter, this may refl ect problems with crosssectional versus longitudinal research. One ongoing, and very encouraging, longitudinal study involves almost 700 nuns in a convent in Minnesota (Mortimer, Snowdon, & Markesbery, 2007; Snowdon, 2003). This research, along with others, documents the brains amazing ability to grow and change throughout the life span .Contrary to popular stereotypes of the frail and forgetful elderly, growing old, for most of us, will probably be better than expectedand, of course, far better than the alternative!