7
RESEARCH PAPER Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectual disability ELI CARMELI 1 , TAMAR BAR-YOSSEF 2 , CLAUDETTE ARIAV 2,3 , RAN LEVY 2 & DARIO G. LIEBERMANN 1 1 Physical Therapy Department, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, 2 Neve Ram Institute for People with Special Needs, Israel, and 3 Flieman Geriatric Medical Center, Haifa, Israel Accepted February 2007 Abstract Background. There is limited experimental evidence to support the view that individuals with intellectual disabilities (ID) have a deficit in motor control. This work is a first attempt to evaluate their motor coordination. Purpose. The study assessed the relationship between cognitive ability and sensorimotor integration. The clinical hypothesis is that adults with ID fall below non-ID adults in motor skills that involve hand-eye coordination. Method. A group of 42 adults with ID (ID group) was compared to 48 age-matched typical adults (TA) using a mixed experimental design (‘Task’ as the within-subjects factor and ‘Group’ as the between-subjects factor). Participants performed the following tests twice: Box-and-Blocks, 25-Grooved-Pegboard, Stick Catching and overhead Beanbag-Throw. Pearson correlations and ANOVAs were used to test the hypothesis (p 0.05). Results. As expected, TA outperformed the ID group in all tests regardless of the hand used during for the assessment. However, TA individuals scored significantly better with one hand (i.e., the preferred and dominant hand) as opposed to persons with ID, who exhibited no hand preference. Test-retest correlations among the first and second assessment scores yielded moderate-strong coefficients, depending on the type of test (Box-and-Blocks ¼ 0.92 and 0.96, 25-Grooved- Pegboard ¼ 0.69 and 0.83, Stick-Catching ¼ 0.88 and 0.94, Beanbag-Throw ¼ 0.58 and 0.91 for ID and TA, respectively). Discussion. Difficulties in the integration of perceptual information into motor action may result in inadequate solutions to daily motor problems. As it stems from our results, intellectual disability relates to inability to integrate visual inputs and hand movements. In people with mild ID such inability is observed using both hands (i.e., they show no hand preferences). Poor perceptual-motor coordination might have a functional significance in that it may lead to exclusion from vocational and recreational activities, and a decreasing competence of ADL. Assessing coordination in adults with ID may contribute to understanding the nature of the ID condition and may encourage an early rehabilitation. Keywords: Intellectual disability, hand preference, hand-eye coordination Introduction The strong relationship between healthy lifestyle and life expectancy is widely recognized [1,2]. Such a relationship could be expected also be expected for the population with intellectual disabilities (ID). However, adults with ID show signs of premature aging with a greater tendency towards de-condition- ing and morbidity [3]. Consequently, they tend to lose independence more often and eventually be- come institutionalized. The incidence of ID amongst the general population of Israel is estimated to be approximately 3% (i.e., 170,000 people), including approximately 150,000 individuals with symptoms of mild ID. Approximately, 40% of adults with ID are eventually relocated within community programs of special residential-care centers [4]. Rehabilitation therapists (i.e., physical, occupa- tional therapists) have important roles within such institutions. They are responsible for diagnostics and treatment of the motor impairments of people with ID [5,6]. More importantly, as a byproduct of inducing success in daily motor functions they might be responsible for enhancing some of the intellectual capabilities. However, the relationship between in- tellectual disability and lack of success in basic motor Correspondence: Eli Carmeli, PhD, Physical Therapy Department, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Ramat Aviv, Tel Aviv, 69978, Israel. Tel: þ972 36405434. Fax: þ972 3 6405436. E-mail: [email protected] Disability and Rehabilitation, 2008; 30(5): 323 – 329 ISSN 0963-8288 print/ISSN 1464-5165 online ª 2008 Informa UK Ltd. DOI: 10.1080/09638280701265398 Disabil Rehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Northeastern University on 12/02/14 For personal use only.

Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectual disability

  • Upload
    dario-g

  • View
    218

  • Download
    3

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectual disability

RESEARCH PAPER

Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectualdisability

ELI CARMELI1, TAMAR BAR-YOSSEF2, CLAUDETTE ARIAV2,3, RAN LEVY2 &

DARIO G. LIEBERMANN1

1Physical Therapy Department, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, 2Neve Ram Institute for People with Special

Needs, Israel, and 3Flieman Geriatric Medical Center, Haifa, Israel

Accepted February 2007

AbstractBackground. There is limited experimental evidence to support the view that individuals with intellectual disabilities (ID)have a deficit in motor control. This work is a first attempt to evaluate their motor coordination.Purpose. The study assessed the relationship between cognitive ability and sensorimotor integration. The clinical hypothesisis that adults with ID fall below non-ID adults in motor skills that involve hand-eye coordination.Method. A group of 42 adults with ID (ID group) was compared to 48 age-matched typical adults (TA) using a mixedexperimental design (‘Task’ as the within-subjects factor and ‘Group’ as the between-subjects factor). Participants performedthe following tests twice: Box-and-Blocks, 25-Grooved-Pegboard, Stick Catching and overhead Beanbag-Throw. Pearsoncorrelations and ANOVAs were used to test the hypothesis (p� 0.05).Results. As expected, TA outperformed the ID group in all tests regardless of the hand used during for the assessment.However, TA individuals scored significantly better with one hand (i.e., the preferred and dominant hand) as opposed topersons with ID, who exhibited no hand preference. Test-retest correlations among the first and second assessment scoresyielded moderate-strong coefficients, depending on the type of test (Box-and-Blocks¼ 0.92 and 0.96, 25-Grooved-Pegboard¼ 0.69 and 0.83, Stick-Catching¼ 0.88 and 0.94, Beanbag-Throw¼ 0.58 and 0.91 for ID and TA, respectively).Discussion. Difficulties in the integration of perceptual information into motor action may result in inadequate solutions todaily motor problems. As it stems from our results, intellectual disability relates to inability to integrate visual inputs andhand movements. In people with mild ID such inability is observed using both hands (i.e., they show no hand preferences).Poor perceptual-motor coordination might have a functional significance in that it may lead to exclusion from vocational andrecreational activities, and a decreasing competence of ADL. Assessing coordination in adults with ID may contribute tounderstanding the nature of the ID condition and may encourage an early rehabilitation.

Keywords: Intellectual disability, hand preference, hand-eye coordination

Introduction

The strong relationship between healthy lifestyle and

life expectancy is widely recognized [1,2]. Such a

relationship could be expected also be expected for

the population with intellectual disabilities (ID).

However, adults with ID show signs of premature

aging with a greater tendency towards de-condition-

ing and morbidity [3]. Consequently, they tend to

lose independence more often and eventually be-

come institutionalized. The incidence of ID amongst

the general population of Israel is estimated to be

approximately 3% (i.e., 170,000 people), including

approximately 150,000 individuals with symptoms of

mild ID. Approximately, 40% of adults with ID are

eventually relocated within community programs of

special residential-care centers [4].

Rehabilitation therapists (i.e., physical, occupa-

tional therapists) have important roles within such

institutions. They are responsible for diagnostics and

treatment of the motor impairments of people with

ID [5,6]. More importantly, as a byproduct of

inducing success in daily motor functions they might

be responsible for enhancing some of the intellectual

capabilities. However, the relationship between in-

tellectual disability and lack of success in basic motor

Correspondence: Eli Carmeli, PhD, Physical Therapy Department, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Ramat Aviv, Tel Aviv, 69978, Israel.

Tel: þ972 36405434. Fax: þ972 3 6405436. E-mail: [email protected]

Disability and Rehabilitation, 2008; 30(5): 323 – 329

ISSN 0963-8288 print/ISSN 1464-5165 online ª 2008 Informa UK Ltd.

DOI: 10.1080/09638280701265398

Dis

abil

Reh

abil

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Nor

thea

ster

n U

nive

rsity

on

12/0

2/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 2: Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectual disability

skills has only attracted limited attention, parti-

cularly concerning motor coordination in people

with ID [7].

It is reported in the literature that the population

with ID presents premature signs of aging [8,9].

Tasks that are performed under perceptual or motor

constraints such as movement accuracy, reaction

time and speed of movement, may exacerbate the

apparent deterioration of motor performance in

people with ID. Brewer and Smith [10] showed

that control mechanisms mediating speed-accuracy

trade-offs may underlie differences between

retarded-non-retarded populations. Likewise, Inui

and colleagues [11] examined the ability to process

information using a tracking task in adolescents that

have Down syndrome (DS). This group of adoles-

cents showed significantly slower and more variable

reaction times as compared to a matching group of

adolescents with intellectual disabilities, but without

DS. Latash and his colleagues [12] addressed the

apparent ‘clumsiness’ of persons with DS and its

relation to changes in indices of finger coordination,

and concluded that persons with DS have a deficit

in the control of both single- and multi-fingers

movements.

Deterioration in motor performance, however,

may be a main characteristic of aging [13] aside

cognitive impairments [14]. Therefore, we cannot

preclude the possibility that motor outcomes are a

simple expression of the premature aging which is so

often characteristic of people with intellectual dis-

abilities. In order to partially preclude such a

possibility, we selected a sample of participants

composed of a subset of individuals with only ‘mild’

intellectual deficiencies because they were less prone

to suffer the effects of premature aging (i.e., we

reduced this covariate to a minimum).

Cognitive disability, development and motor skill

Motor impairment in people with intellectual dis-

abilities may relate to developmental factors [15].

Unilateral limb-dominance and hand-preference are

related to the specialization of the brain’s hemi-

spheres that occurs during development [16,17].

Arnold and colleagues [18] argued that rather than

showing clear asymmetric patterns with a clear

dominance of one side while moving, people with

ID do not discriminate between the left- or right-side

limbs.

Difficulties in performing hand-eye coordination

tasks bilaterally may underlie some of the difficulties

of adults with mild ID during their activities of

daily living (ADLs). An expression of such cognitive

impairment may be a generally poor motor per-

formance. We focus on the lack of differences

between the two hands and assume that hemispheric

specialization is a key factor in developing manual

dexterity. Normally, such specialization should be

evident in superior performances of the dominant

hand as compared to the opposite hand.

We further assume that poor coordination has a

functional impact on performance that may lead to a

decreasing competence in ADLs and ultimately may

result in exclusion from vocational and recreational

activities. We hypothesize that motor skill of people

with ID fall below the expected for non-ID people.

Four tasks that involve integration of hand move-

ments with visual information are used here in order

to test coordination in adults with mild ID and hand

preference. For this purpose, participants are in-

structed to perform tasks under constraints that

resemble those used in the previous studies (speed-

accuracy in a pegboard test, reaction time in a Stick

Catching test and accuracy in a mentally rotated

environment in a backwards beanbag throw). Their

results are compared to the performances of aged-

matched typical adults.

Furthermore, this study assesses the relationship

between cognitive ability and sensorimotor integra-

tion in adults with and without ID. We explore the

interrelationships between motor performances of

ID people at different times in order to evaluate

reliability and validity scores of the simple hand-eye

coordination tests.

Method

Participants

A group of adults with mild intellectual disability is

compared to an age-matched group in terms of

specific motor tasks performed bilaterally (right and

left hands; dominant hand first). The participants in

the residential home centers usually shared apart-

ments with 3 – 5 residents who are supervised around

the clock by a trained staff person. Medical, health,

welfare, rehabilitation services and recreational activ-

ities are routinely provided by supplementary staff.

The sample consists of permanent residents who

lived in foster homes at two residential care centers.

After being referred to the study by the institutional

health-care professionals, only candidates that vo-

lunteered to participate were considered for the

experimental trials. From a sample population of 418

permanent residents with ID, only 42 participants

with mild ID are included in the current ID group

(28 females and 14 males; mean age¼ 42.2+ 6.22

years; range 31 – 49 years). Three inclusion criteria

were used for selecting participants for the ID group:

(1) Mild intellectual deficiency (IQ¼ 62 – 79), as

diagnosed within 1 – 3 years after birth by IQ-

scores defined by the Wechsler Abbreviated

324 E. Carmeli et al.

Dis

abil

Reh

abil

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Nor

thea

ster

n U

nive

rsity

on

12/0

2/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 3: Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectual disability

Scale of Intelligence (Harcourt Assessment

Inc., San Antonio; [19]).

(2) Recognized ability to understand basic verbal

communication.

(3) Independence from personnel or from sup-

port services/devices.

Candidates were excluded if they had a clinical

history of neurological disease (e.g., Parkinson’s

disease, stroke, Alzheimer’s disease, neuropathy or

brain surgery) or in the case of any peripheral

neurological sign. In addition, candidates were

excluded if they showed any significant perceptual

deficits (e.g., auditory and/or visual). None of the

participants received narcotic medications at the

time of the experiments.

Similarly, from 213 permanent caregivers who

worked with the ID community, 48 participants are

included in the group of typical adults TA (30

females and 18 males; mean age¼ 45.7+ 5.19 years,

range 22 – 53 years). Participants with ID lived in the

care centers for at least five years prior to being

tested. The volunteers in the TA group work in those

centers in different health care professions (social

workers, caregivers, teachers and nurses).

The study was approved by the Institutional Ethic

Committee of Residential Care Centers under the

administrative control of the Israeli Ministry of

Welfare. Oral consent was obtained from partici-

pants as well as a written consent from their parents

or guardians.

Experimental procedures

Data were collected in two testing sessions separated

by a period of 6 – 8 days. This allowed enough time

for dissociating the second set from the experience

gathered in the first attempt. That is, assessment

values obtained approximately one week apart were

used to test reliability scores. Trials were performed

in a quiet environment that minimized shifts of

attention away from the experimental tasks. The

examiner always provided a first standardized set of

verbal instructions followed by a demonstration of

the motor task. A 10-min practice period was then

allowed. Hand dominance was determined by hand

preference during the warm up trials (3 trials per

task). The dominant hand was the first to be tested in

all cases. Data collection was always initiated with a

‘go’ signal provided by the examiner.

Motor tasks

The reason for testing our hypothesis using the

selected motor tasks was that they included elements

of hand-eye coordination (e.g., hand throwing to

memorized visual targets, fast hand-finger actions in

reaction to a visual events, grasping and transporting

small objects, accurate hand-finger movements).

These tests have been shown to be reliable, they

were simple and easy to implement in field condi-

tions, and fairly close to real-life situations.

Box and Blocks Test (B&B). Performance of B&B

was carried out while the individuals faced towards

the box (546 266 9 cm) located at the center of a

standard table. The box was divided into two equal

compartments by a 15 cm high dividing wall. The

blocks consisted of 100 wooden cubes (2.5 cm3). In

order to evaluate manual dexterity, participants were

instructed to transport a maximal number of blocks

from one side to the opposite side within 15 sec, one

block at a time [20]. Individuals performed first with

the preferred hand and 2 min later they performed

with the opposite hand. Each participant performed

two sets of trials with each hand (four sets in total

in two stages). The B&B test was chosen because it

is a robust test that is known to yield high intrarater

validity and interrater reliability scores (Intrarater

Correlation Coefficient ICC r¼ 0.92 – 0.96 and

an Interrater Reliability Coefficient IRC r¼ 0.78 –

0.92; [21]).

25-Grooved Pegboard Test (PegB). This test was

designed to assess fine manual dexterity and hand/

eye coordination [22]. It consists of a wooden board

with 25 holes. Performers were instructed to insert

25 cylindrical pegs (0.25 cm diameter6 0.8 cm

height) as quickly as possible into the holes, one

peg at a time. Performance time was continuously

measured, even in the event that pegs were dropped

down to the ground. The time to complete the task

was measured twice for each hand. For such a test,

validity and reliability scores reported in the litera-

ture [23 – 25] are moderate-high (ICC r¼ 0.69 –

0.83, r¼ 0.65 – 0.81).

Stick Catching test (StickC). Reaction time (RT in

ms) was assessed using a 2.5 cm wide6 100 cm

length wooden ruler (0.1 cm nominal resolution),

which was held vertically in the direction of the

gravity vector. Upon release, performers were in-

structed to catch the ruler and prevent the free fall

using a pinch-like grasping action. The measure of

the distance obtained from the difference between

the initial and final grasping heights was used as a

measure of reaction time.

According to Newton’s formula (i.e., the time of

flight during a free fall depends on the gravity

constant. Therefore, knowing the falling distance

under gravity conditions implies knowing the flight

time [27]):

Flight time ¼ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi2 �distance ðmÞ=9:81ðm � s�2Þ;

p

Bilateral hand-eye coordination in adult with mild ID 325

Dis

abil

Reh

abil

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Nor

thea

ster

n U

nive

rsity

on

12/0

2/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 4: Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectual disability

and thus, the free fall distance could indeed provide a

gross measure of visual RT because falling distance

may be translated into the falling time (i.e., flight

time), which is the time it would take the fingers to

move after visually perceiving the onset of the fall of

the ruler [26]. Such a method is just one among

many ways of testing RT, but perhaps the simplest

and most rudimentary. However, because our

sample was randomly selected and enough data

trials were collected, the current method was

assumed as a reliable way to assess RT.

During the measurements, participants sat on a

standard chair while their performing arm was at 308flexion and 208 abduction relative to a zero-arm

configuration (as defined by the fully extended arm

pointing forward). The elbow joint was positioned at

908 flexion while the forearm-wrist was maintained

in a mid-torsion position (between maximal supina-

tion and pronation). The examiner stood next to the

performer and held the ruler at a ‘zero starting point’.

At a random time, the examiner dropped the ruler

and the performer attempted to grasp it. Six

consecutive trials (three per hand) were carried out

with a 15-sec resting interval between measurements.

RT measures provide robust descriptors of the

ability to respond as fast as possible to an unexpected

stimulus, with an intra-rater validity r¼ 0.88 – 0.94

and a test-retest reliability r¼ 0.85 – 0.96 [27].

Overhead Beanbag Throw (BThrow). The goal of this

task was to throw a beanbag (500 gm) over the

shoulder in the direction of a hoop (0.75 m

diameter) located at the center of a 16 1 m

gymnastic mat. The performer stood at a distance

of 2 m facing away from the mat. Each hand

performed six consecutive attempts (12 trials in

total). After every trial, performers got an immediate

knowledge of results. A 0 – 3 scale was used to score

the motor performance whereas a three points score

was given for a throw that ended within the hoop,

two points if the bag partially hit the hoop and later

fell onto the mat and one point if the bag landed

anywhere on the mat without hitting the hoop. A

zero score was given when the beanbag landed

outside the mat. This arm-throwing test was

assumed to test spatial orientation [28], accuracy

and throwing skill. Reported scores of validity and

reliability for ball throwing tests are ICC r¼ 0.58 –

0.91 and r¼ 0.75 – 0.92 for test-retest sessions [29].

Mental rotation is among the different cognitive

skills required for a success in such a task. This has

been specifically addressed in the early experiments

of Shepard and colleagues [30,31]. These authors

showed that gradual mental rotation of an object

increases the response time during the actual

performance, whereas anticipatory mental rotation

of the same object (before starting the performance)

reduces the response time. Difficulty in mentally

rotating objects may be one more expression of

cognitive impairment. The overhead throwing task as

performed here puts such a cognitive skill to the test.

Analysis

Data analyses included multiple analyses of variance

(ANOVA) and Pearson correlations between vari-

ables. Hand dominance across tests (right vs. left)

and group differences (TA vs. ID) were determined

by introducing ‘Hand’ as a within-subjects factor and

‘Groups’ as the between-subjects factor. Correlation

coefficients were calculated among the four tests to

determine the inter-correlation coefficients. Descrip-

tive statistics were also calculated for relevant

demographic variables, including the mean, standard

deviation and 95% confidence intervals for each

dependent variable, with age and gender as grouping

factors. A SPSS-v10 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA)

package was used for the statistical assessment of the

hypotheses. The confidence level was set at p� 0.05.

Results

Differences between groups in motor tasks that involve

hand-eye coordination

Performance scores for ID participants were sig-

nificantly lower and less consistent in comparison to

TA individuals, in all four tests (see Table I). TA

individuals showed not only superior performances

overall, as expected, but also significantly better

results for the dominant hand, and therefore, this

hand seem to be the preferred one (F[1,88]¼ 72.45;

p5 0.001). Notably, the ID group presented motor

performances of the dominant hand that were not

significantly different from the opposite hand

(F[1,88]¼ 3.581; p¼NS) suggesting that ID may

have an undefined hand preference.

Repeated sessions showed strong correlations for

the B&B test (ICC r¼ 0.92 – 0.96), moderate for the

PegB test (ICC r¼ 0.69 – 0.83) and Stick Catching

test to assess reaction times (ICC r¼ 0.88 – 0.94),

and inconsistent but moderate-high coefficients for

the BThrow test (ICC r¼ 0.58 – 0.91). The incon-

sistency in the correlation scores might be attributed

to the larger variance observed for the result obtained

in this task. The task enables discrimination among

individuals with ID because it includes some domi-

nant cognitive components, as it will be discussed

in the next section.

Strong pair wise inter-correlations ranging

r¼ 0.76 – 0.98 (all significant at p5 0.05) were

observed between the four tests for both groups, as

shown in Table II. However, the coefficients for data

grouped by gender and hand preference showed

326 E. Carmeli et al.

Dis

abil

Reh

abil

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Nor

thea

ster

n U

nive

rsity

on

12/0

2/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 5: Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectual disability

strong correlations between all four hand-eye co-

ordination tests performed with the dominant and

preferred hand, but only for the TA group (r¼ 0.80 –

0.92, p5 0.05).

It is worth noting also that given the similarities

between B&B and PegB tests the strong relationship

found (r¼ 0.98, p� 0.001) implies that these two

tests may be different expressions of a similar sensori-

motor ability that underlies the performance. Hence,

one or the other might be considered redundant.

Discussion

Motor skill in general is required throughout life for

adaptation to our daily environment, while manual

skill in particular is essential for achieving indepen-

dence in many ADL [32]. Poor motor coordination

might have functional impacts and may lead to

exclusion from vocational and recreational activities

and decreasing competence of ADL (activities of

daily living). Our results however suggest that people

diagnosed as having mild intellectual disabilities

perform poorly manual tasks that require interactions

between hand motion and visual inputs.

Cognitive disability, development and motor skill

We showed here that hand specialization is not

obvious even in persons with a mild intellectual

disability. Our sample of participants presented no

advantage in using one hand over the other, and the

findings may provide support for the suggestion that

people with intellectual disabilities have no func-

tional differentiation between hands (i.e., a bilateral

symmetry) during simple motor functions. Lack

of a clear pattern of lateralization has already been

reported [33].

We might speculate that this is related to their

undefined interhemispheric brain specialization.

That is, differences between ID and TA groups

might be a reflection of a developmental setback in

the former group.

In the present study, participants were tested

under task constraints that resembled those in the

previous studies (speed-accuracy in the pegboard

test, reaction time in the Stick Catching test and the

Backwards Beanbag throw), but in all these tests,

their performance was poor as compared to the

performances of an age-matched group of controls.

Since deterioration in motor performance under the

previous constraints is also is one main characteristic

of aging [13] aside cognitive impairments [14], we

cannot preclude the possibility that the observed

results were an outcome of premature aging,

characteristic of people with intellectual disabilities,

rather than a correlate of intellectual disability.

Motor performance of people with ID without vision

As implied in previous paragraphs, conditions that

challenge the sensorimotor abilities of people with

intellectual disabilities may allow for discrimination

among individuals at different levels.

The visual occlusion paradigm during motor

performance has been widely used to assess the

effect of continuous or intermittent feedback on the

execution of movement. Occlusion of visual input

may lead to the use of internal representations of the

Table I. Means, SD and confidence intervals for the Box and Blocks Test (B&B), 25-Grooved Pegboard Test (PegB), Stick Catching test

(StickC) and Beanbag Throwing test (BThrow) by gender subgroups (Male/female) and hand dominance (Dominant D/Non-dominant,

ND).

ID group TA group

Groups D/ND D/ND D/ND D/ND

Test Male Female Male Female

B&B (# blocks) 8+ 2/6+ 4 10+ 2/7+3 6+2/5+ 2* 6+ 2/5+2*

PegB (sec) 59+ 9/73+ 11 52+ 7/64+9 49+6/62+ 7* 47+ 6/51+7*

StickC-RT (ms) 247/263 251/267 235/257* 240/274*

BThrow (points) 4+ 3/2+ 2 3+ 1/2+2 2+1/6+ 2* 5+ 1/2+1*

*p�0.05.

Table II. Pair wise Pearson correlation coefficients between for the

different field tests for the TA and ID groups: Box and Blocks

(B&B); 25 Grooved Pegboard test (PegB); Stick Catching reaction

time test (StickC); and Beanbag Throw task towards a target

(Bthrow test.)

Groups Tests B&B PegB StickC BThrow

Typical B&B 1 – – –

Adults PegB 0.98* 1 – –

StickC 0.89* 0.90* 1 –

BeanB 0.84* 0.88* 0.76* 1

Intellectually B&B 1 – – –

Impaired PegB 0.98* 1 – –

StickC 0.86* 0.88* 1 –

BeanB 0.88* 0.90* 0.92* 1

*p�0.05.

Bilateral hand-eye coordination in adult with mild ID 327

Dis

abil

Reh

abil

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Nor

thea

ster

n U

nive

rsity

on

12/0

2/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 6: Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectual disability

environment. An internal visual representation could

indeed be used to activate and modulate previously

acquired motor plans with little or no feedback. Do

individuals with ID lack this ability during aiming

tasks? The findings here show that this may be the

case and such a possibility is supported by earlier

literature [34,35]. The beanbag throw in our experi-

mental trials was carried out backwards, to a

previously seen target. Thus, it posed a cognitive

challenge to our participants with mild ID because it

required the ability to perform an accurate task

without visual perception of the location of the

target. We showed that lack of continuous visual

input affected the performance of people with mild

intellectual disabilities.

Intellectual disability and mental rotation

Although the beanbag throwing task may be see-

mingly simple, the sensorimotor ability required for

its performance is the ability to locate objects relative

to rotated body coordinates within a fixed reference

frame. This may be a challenge for individuals

with ID. Accurate backward throws involve the

transformation of joint coordinates that define the

appropriate arm – hand configuration into spatial

coordinates of the mentally visualized target. Such a

task involves also an adjustment of motor commands.

During a normal throwing task, arm extension may

be appropriate because the performer faces the target.

However, when the performer faces in the opposite

direction (a 1808 rotation of the body) a backward

throwing movement is required because the target is

located behind, and thus, arm flexion is the appro-

priate action. Participants in the present study often

failed to make such a coordinate transformation in

the correct target direction. Thus, as it stems from

the results of the present study people with mild

intellectual disabilities lack the fundamental ability to

rotate mentally the location of an object in space.

Furthermore, they fail to integrate an internal visual

image into the appropriate motor commands.

Clinical relevance

We have shown in a sample of adults with ID that

upper body (hand – eye) coordination is poor,

particularly when a quick response to perceptual

(visual) stimuli is required. An early detection of

deficits in coordination, as assessed by the simple

tasks used in the present study, is utmost essential for

an accurate and optimized rehabilitation process that

targets functional abilities. In addition, assessing

perceptual-motor coordination may contribute to

understand the nature of ID because poor hand-eye

coordination is an expression of intellectual disability

[36]. In this regard, the beanbag throw poses a

challenge because it may be associated with the

mental rotation of a previously acquired visual

representation of the environment. Such motor task

may enable to discriminate among individuals at

different stages of the rehabilitation process because

it emphasizes critical cognitive elements related to

success in the performance of ADL.

Conclusions

Motor assessments using hand – eye coordination

tasks may reflect the status of people diagnosed as

having mild intellectual disabilities. Such assess-

ments may allow for designing early rehabilitation

programs specific to the individuals. Rehabilitation

protocols should thus include specific motor tests as

part of the common cognitive assessments performed

in residential care centers.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Mr S. Bar-Chad from the

Neve-Ram Institute for People with Special Needs,

Rechasim, Israel, and Prof. J. Merrick from the

National Institute of Child Health and Human Devel-

opment affiliated with the Ministry of Social Affairs of

Israel, for his assistance during the performance of

the field tests. Finally, we gratefully acknowledge the

insights provided by Dr M. Wertheim.

References

1. de Groot Verheijden MW, de Henauw S, Schroll M,

van Staveren WA. Lifestyle, nutritional status, health, and

mortality in elderly people across Europe: A review of the

longitudinal results of the SENECA study. J Gerontol Ser A:

Biolog Sci Med Sci 2004;59:1277 – 1284.

2. Leahy MJ, Thurber D, Calvert JF Jr.Benefits and challenges of

research with the oldest old for participants and nurses.

Geriatric Nursing 2005;26:21 – 28.

3. Ashman AF, Suttie JN. The medical health status of older

people with mental retardation in Australia. J Appl Gerontol

1996;15:57 – 73.

4. Merrick J, Kandel I. Medical services for persons with intellec-

tual disability in Israel. Public Health Rev 2003;31:45 – 68.

5. Dean M. UNICEF’s goals for 2000 AD. Lancet 1993;342:

855 – 858.

6. Turner S, Moss S. The health needs of adults with learning

disabilities and the Health of the Nation strategy. J Intellect

Disabil Res 1996;40:438 – 450.

7. Kelly S, Jessop EGA. Comparison of measures of disability

and health status in people with physical disabilities under-

going vocational rehabilitation. J Public Health Med 1996;

18(2):169 – 174.

8. Carmeli E, Kessel S, Bar-Chad S, Merrick J. A comparison

between older persons with Down syndrome and a control

group: Clinical characteristics, functional status and sensori-

motor function. Down Synd Res Pract 2004;9:17 – 24.

9. Schonknecht P, Pantel J, Kruse A, Schroder J. Prevalence and

natural course of aging-associated cognitive decline in a

population-based sample of young-old subjects. Am J

Psychiatry 2005;162:2071 – 2077.

328 E. Carmeli et al.

Dis

abil

Reh

abil

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Nor

thea

ster

n U

nive

rsity

on

12/0

2/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.

Page 7: Perceptual-motor coordination in persons with mild intellectual disability

10. Brewer N, Smith GA. Processing speed and mental retarda-

tion: Deadline procedures indicate fixed and adjustable

limitations. Memory Cognit 1990;18:443 – 450.

11. Inui N, Yamanishi M, Tada S. Simple reaction times and

timing of serial reactions of adolescents with mental retarda-

tion, autism, and Down syndrome. Percept Motor Skills

1995;81:739 – 745.

12. Latash ML, Kang N, Patterson D. Finger coordination in

persons with Down syndrome: Atypical patterns of coordina-

tion and the effects of practice. Experim Brain Res 2002;

146:345 – 355.

13. Serrien DJ, Swinnen SP, Stelmach GE. Age-related deteriora-

tion of coordinated interlimb behavior. J Gerontol, Ser B:

Psycholog Sci Soc Sci 2000;55:295 – 303.

14. Span MM, Ridderinkhof KR, van der Molen MW. Age-

related changes in the efficiency of cognitive processing across

the life span. Acta Psycholog 2004;117:155 – 183.

15. Raz N, Williamson A, Gunning-Dixon F, Head D, Acker JD.

Neuroanatomical and cognitive correlates of adult age

differences in acquisition of a perceptual-motor skill. Micro-

scopic Res Techniques 2000;51:85 – 93.

16. Nebes RD. Hemispheric specialization in the aged brain. In:

Threvarthen C, editor. Brain circuits and functions of the

mind. Essays in honor of Roger W. Sperry. MA: Cambridge

University Press; 1990. pp 364 – 370.

17. Sperry RW. Forebrain commissurotomy and conscious

awareness. In: Threvarthen C, editor. Brain circuits and

functions of the mind. Essays in honor of Roger W. Sperry.

MA: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp 371 – 388.

18. Arnold G, Boone KB, Lu P, Dean A, Wen J, Nitch S,

McPherson S. Sensitivity and specificity of finger tapping test

scores for the detection of suspect effort. Clinical Neuropsy-

chol 2005;19(1):105 – 120.

19. Hays JR, Reas DL, Shaw JB. Concurrent validity of the

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence and the Kaufman

brief intelligence test among psychiatric in patients. Psychol

Rep 2002;90:355 – 359.

20. Mathiowetz V, Volland G, Kashman N, Weber K. Adult

norms for the Box and Block Test of manual dexterity. Am J

Occupat Ther 1985;39:386 – 391.

21. Desrosiers J, Bravo G, Hebert R, Dutil E, Mercier L.

Validation of the Box and Block Test as a measure of

dexterity of elderly people: Reliability, validity, and norms

studies. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1994;75:751 – 755.

22. Schmidt SL, Oliveira RM, Rocha FR, Abreu-Villaca Y.

Influences of handedness and gender on the grooved peg-

board test. Brain Cognit 2000;44:445 – 454.

23. Bornstein RA. Classification rates obtained with ‘‘standard’’

cut-off scores on selected neuropsychological measures. J Clin

Experim Neuropsychol 1986;8:413 – 420.

24. Desrosiers J, Hebert R, Bravo G, Dutil E. The Purdue

Pegboard Test: Normative data for people aged 60 and over.

Disabil Rehabil 1995;17:217 – 224.

25. Ruff RM, Parker SB. Gender- and age-specific changes in

motor speed and eye-hand coordination in adults: Normative

values for the Finger Tapping and Grooved Pegboard Tests.

Percept Motor Skills 1993;76:1219 – 1230.

26. Liebermann, DG, Goodman, D. Pre- and Post-landing effects

of falling with and without vision. J Electromyography

Kinesiol 2007;17(2):212 – 227.

27. Kauranen K, Vanharanta H. Influences of aging, gender, and

handedness on motor performance of upper and lower

extremities. Percept Motor Skills 1996;82:515 – 525.

28. Papaxanthis C, Schieppati M, Gentili R, Pozzo T. Imagined

and actual arm movements have similar durations when

performed under different conditions of direction and mass.

Experim Brain Res 2002;143:447 – 452.

29. de Greef MH, Dijkstra Y, Ottens NM, Jansma FF.

Reliabilities of the Groningen Functional Capacity Test.

Percept Motor Skills 2003;96:731 – 738.

30. Shepard RN, Metzler J. Mental rotation of three-dimensional

objects. Science 1971;171:701 – 703.

31. Cooper LA, Shepard RN. Chronometric studies of the

rotation of mental images. In: Chase WG, editor. Visual

information processing. New York: Academic Press; 1973.

pp 75 – 176.

32. Pigeon P, Bortolami SB, DiZio P, Lackner JR. Coordinated

turn-and-reach movements. II. Planning in an external frame

of reference. J Neurophysiol 2003;89:290 – 303.

33. Mohan A, Singh AP, Mandal MK. Transfer and interference

of motor skills in people with intellectual disability. J Intellect

Disabil Res 2001;45:361 – 369.

34. Elliott D. Intermittent visual pickup and goal directed

movement: A review. Human Move Sci 1990;9:531 – 548.

35. Elliott D, Chua R, Pollock BJ. The influence of inter-

mittent vision on manual aiming. Acta Psycholog 1994;85:

1 – 13.

36. Sachdey PS, Wen W, Christensen H, Jorm AF. White matter

hyperintensities are related to physical disability and poor

motor function. J Neurol, Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76:

362 – 367.

Bilateral hand-eye coordination in adult with mild ID 329

Dis

abil

Reh

abil

Dow

nloa

ded

from

info

rmah

ealth

care

.com

by

Nor

thea

ster

n U

nive

rsity

on

12/0

2/14

For

pers

onal

use

onl

y.