181
Occasional Paper No. 46 Parental marital status and children’s wellbeing LIXIA QU AND RUTH WESTON AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF FAMILY STUDIES

Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    6

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Occasional Paper No. 46

Parental marital status and children’s wellbeing

LIXIA QU AND RUTH WESTON

AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF FAMILY STUDIES

Page 2: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

© Commonwealth of Australia 2012ISSN 18392334ISBN 978-1-921975-67-7

All material presented in this publication is provided under a Creative Commons CC-BY Attribution 3.0 Australia licence - see summary of Creative Commons Legal Code.For the avoidance of doubt, this means this licence only applies to material as set out in this document.

With the exception of the Commonwealth Coat of Arms (for terms of use, refer to the Commonwealth Coat of Arms webpage ), the details of the relevant licence conditions are available on the Creative Commons website (accessible using the links provided) as is the full legal code for the CC-BY 3.0 AU licence.

AcknowledgementsThis research paper makes use of data from Growing Up in Australia: the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC). LSAC is conducted in partnership between the Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs (FaHCSIA), the Australian Institute of Family Studies (AIFS) and Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS), with advice provided by a consortium of leading researchers.

The authors would like to thank colleagues at AIFS and FaHCSIA for their valuable comments on previous versions of this report.

The views expressed in this publication are those of individual authors and may not reflect those of AIFS.

For more informationResearch Publications UnitResearch and Analysis BranchAustralian Government Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous AffairsPO Box 7576Canberra Business Centre ACT 2610Phone: (02) 6146 8061Fax: (02) 6293 3289Email: [email protected]

Page 3: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

ContentsExecutive summary vi

1 Introduction—research background 11.1 Trends in couple formation 11.2 Trends in ex-nuptial births and associated partnership circumstances 11.3 Related societal attitudes21.4 Institutionalisation of cohabitation: International patterns 21.5 Characteristics of married and cohabiting families and sole-mother families 31.6 Implications for children’s wellbeing 41.7 Research questions 6

2 Sample, measures and analytical approaches 82.1 Parental characteristics and family financial circumstances 92.2 Parenting self-efficacy and practices 92.3 Parents’ psychological distress 92.4 Quality of inter-parental relationships 102.5 Child wellbeing measures 10

3 Results 113.1 Family structure 113.2 Child, parent and family characteristics 143.3 Children’s wellbeing 263.4 Cohabitation pathways and child wellbeing 37

4 Summary and discussion 414.1 Research questions and findings 414.2 Final word 44

Appendix: Measures used and tables 45Parenting practice measures 45Argumentative relationship scale 45

List of shortened forms 88

Endnotes 89

References 91

iii

Page 4: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

List of tables

Table 1: Family form by survey wave, K cohort children 11Table 2: Change in family form from Wave 1 to Wave 3, K cohort children 13Table 3: Selected characteristics of children by (unchanged) family form, K cohort, Wave 1 (a) 15Table 4: Selected parental characteristics by unchanged family form, K cohort (a) 17Table 5: Mean scores of parenting approach scales, fathers and mothers, by unchanged family form,

Waves 1 to 3 24Table 6: Mean scores of psychological distress scale and quality of parental relationship of fathers and

mothers by unchanged family form, Waves 1 to 3 26Table 7: Child outcomes by family form, primary caregiver reports, Waves 1 to 3 28Table 8: Child SDQ outcomes by unchanged family form, teacher reports, Waves 1 to 3 29Table 9: Regressions of child outcomes, base model 32Table 10: Regressions of child outcomes, full model 35Table 11: Parents who were cohabiting in Wave 1: Parental psychological distress and the quality of

inter-parental relationships in Wave 1, by parental cohabitation pathways by Wave 3 38Table 12: Cohabiting families in Wave 1: Children’s developmental progress, by parental cohabitation

pathways by Wave 3 40

List of Appendix tables

Table A1: Sample attritions for three family types at Wave 1, K cohort 49Table A2: Children’s characteristics by family types, waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis) 50Table A3: Characteristics of fathers and mothers by family type, waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis)

52Table A4: Mean scores of parenting practice scales of fathers and mothers by family type, Waves 1 to 3

(cross-sectional analysis)56Table A5: Mean scores of psychological distress scale and quality of parental relationship of

fathers and mothers by family type, Waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis) 56Table A6: Child outcome by family type, parents’ reports, Waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis) 57Table A7: Child outcome by family type, teachers’ reports, Waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis)

58Table A8: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by parents (cross-sectional analysis), base

model 59Table A9: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by teachers (cross-sectional analysis), base

model 62Table A10: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by parents (cross-sectional analysis), full model

64Table A11: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by teachers (cross-sectional analysis), full

models69Table A12: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by parents (unchanged family form), base

models72Table A13: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by teachers (unchanged family form), base

models75Table A14: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by parents (unchanged family form), full models

77

Table A15: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by teachers (unchanged family form), full models83

iv

Page 5: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

List of figures

Figure 1: Experience of one or more financial hardships, by (unchanged) family form, primary carers’ reports, Waves 1 to 3 19

Figure 2: Ratings of families’ financial situation, by (unchanged) family form, primary carers’ reports, Waves 1 to 3 20

Figure 3: Mothers’ ratings of self as a parent, by (unchanged) family form, Waves 1 to 3 21Figure 4: Fathers’ ratings of self as a parent by (unchanged) family form, Waves 1 to 3 23Figure A1: Proportions of families having experienced one or more listed financial hardships by family

type, as reported by primary carers, Waves 1 to 3 46Figure A2: Ratings of financial situation by family type, as reported by primary carers, Waves 1 to 3

47Figure A3: Mothers’ ratings of self as a parent compared with other parents by family type, Waves 1

to 3 48Figure A4: Fathers’ ratings of self as a parent compared with other parents by family type, Waves 1 to 3

50

v

Page 6: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Executive summaryAs in many Western countries, the rise in cohabitation in Australia represents one of the most striking of the many changes to family life that have emerged since the 1970s. Not only is cohabitation now the normative pathway to marriage, but it is also increasingly used as a context for raising a family.

While there is considerable evidence suggesting that cohabiting couples are more likely to separate than married couples, much of this research has not assessed whether this applies where children have been born of the cohabiting relationship. Some overseas studies have suggested that children with biological married parents have higher wellbeing than those whose biological parents are cohabiting. Nevertheless, a less favourable picture for those in cohabiting rather than married families has not been apparent across all wellbeing measures examined or in all studies. Based on the data of the first three waves of the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children, this paper focused on the wellbeing of young Australian children who were living with their cohabiting or married biological parents or with their sole mother over a four-year period (Wave 1 to Wave 3).

Key findingsPrevalence and stability of different family forms over a four-year period

Not surprisingly, most of the children were living with both parents who were married to each other (here called ‘married families’) and sole-mother families were more commonly represented than families in which the child lived with both biological parents who were cohabiting (here called ‘cohabiting families’).

Most married, cohabiting and sole-mother families in Wave 1 were in the same family form in Wave 3, with levels of stability being considerably greater for married families than for the other two forms.

Although more than one in 10 children who lived in married families did not experience a change in family form, those who did experience a change tended to be living with their sole mother by Wave 3.

Children in cohabiting families were just as likely to experience parental marriage as to experience parental separation.

Not surprisingly, any change for those in sole-mother families tended to involve the introduction of a stepfather, although a few experienced their biological parents moving in together.

Socio-demographic characteristics, parenting practices and quality of the inter-parental relationship in different family forms

Compared with married parents, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were younger, more likely to identify as Indigenous, had a lower level of education, and were less likely to be employed.

The proportion of cohabiting families experiencing financial hardship was higher than that of married families but lower than that of sole-mother families.

Cohabiting parents and sole mothers appeared to be less consistent in their parenting than was the case for married parents.

vi

Page 7: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Compared with married parents, cohabiting parents tended to describe the quality of their inter-parental relationship in more negative terms and to indicate higher emotional distress.

The wellbeing of children living in different family forms Compared with children with married parents, those living with their sole mother

appeared to fare less well in terms of social–emotional, learning and physical development, as reported by their primary caregivers (typically their mothers) and teachers.

The reports of primary caregivers indicated that the children with cohabiting parents were doing less well than those with married parents in some areas (e.g. overall social–emotional development) and general development, but they were doing better than those in sole-mother families.

The family-related factors that were examined accounted for virtually all of the differences apparent between children with married parents and those with cohabiting parents.

The differences apparent in Wave 1 in the wellbeing of children in married families and those in sole mother families could be largely explained by the family-related factors that were examined. Nevertheless, by Wave 3, the differences between the two groups in some wellbeing measures still remained after taking account of the family characteristics.

vii

Page 8: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

1 Introduction—research backgroundAs Benjamin Disraeli observed, ‘Change is inevitable. Change is a constant’. This is well demonstrated in families, the basic unit of society. It is reflected, for example, in the pathways to family formation, childbearing trends, patterns of family functioning, parental (or partnership) separation, and the prevalence of different family forms (see Hayes, Qu, Weston & Baxter 2011; Hayes, Weston, Qu & Gray 2010).

The family forms of key interest in this report are those comprising couples with children (in which the child who forms the focus of the present analysis—the ‘study child’—was born to the couple), and the parents are legally married to each other or are living together outside legal marriage (here called ‘cohabitation’).

This study explores the following questions: to what extent is the distinction between married and cohabiting couples relevant in terms of settings for raising children? That is, how similar or different are the parental/family characteristics and parenting practices of cohabiting and married families in which the children of interest were born? How do children in these two forms of families fare? To help put similarities and differences between these two groups in perspective, the analyses also extend to sole-mother families. Data from Growing Up in Australia: The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC) are used in this study (see Section 2 for details about LSAC).

The discussion in this section first outlines trends in couple formation, in childbearing within and outside legal marriage, and in related societal attitudes. This is followed by a discussion of proposed ‘stages’ that countries go through as cohabitation increases in prevalence and the apparent process of convergence of the two institutions. Next, ways in which the two institutions still appear to differ are explored, with particular attention given to factors that may affect children’s wellbeing. Finally, previous research into the wellbeing of children in these two family forms is discussed—along with comparative studies of children in sole-mother families as a frame of reference.

1.1 Trends in couple formationThe establishment of a committed ‘living together’ relationship between two adults is typically a central milestone in the family formation process. Throughout most of the 20th century, this new way of life almost always began with marriage, but this is no longer the case, for many couples now live together outside marriage in a cohabiting relationship. Indeed, it is fairly unusual these days for couples to marry without having lived together first. Of all couples who married in 2009, 77 per cent had cohabited beforehand, compared with 31 per cent in 1981 (Australian Bureau of Statistics [ABS] 2002; 2010b). Some cohabiting couples never marry, while others end up separating, and for some other couples, cohabitation is a prelude to marriage.

Not surprisingly, cohabitation is the most common form of union for couples under the age of 25 years, although even among those in their late twenties who are living together, around four in 10 are cohabiting. And while the predominance of marriage increases with age, the proportion of couples who are cohabiting has increased across all age groups (Weston & Qu 2007).

1.2 Trends in ex-nuptial births and associated partnership circumstancesThere is also evidence that cohabitation has increasingly become a setting for raising children. This is apparent both in surveys of families and in official birth statistics. For example, surveys conducted by the ABS suggest that, among intact families with children under the age of 18 years, the proportion of parents who were cohabiting rather than married increased from 6 per

1

Page 9: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

cent in 1997 to 9 per cent in 2006–07.1 In addition, the proportion of infants born outside marriage has increased markedly in recent decades, from 8 per cent of registered births in 1970 to 34 per cent of births registered in 2009 (ABS 2010a). These latter statistics, which are based on birth registrations, are not accompanied by information concerning whether the mother was cohabiting with the father at the time of birth. However, perinatal statistics published by Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW), based on data collected from midwives or other attendants at births, suggest that the proportion of infants born to single women has remained stable since the early 1990s. For example, these data suggest that, among all women who gave birth, the proportion who were ‘single’ (that is, not living with a partner) at the time of the birth was 12 per cent in 1991 (Lancaster, Huang & Pedisich 1994), and 13 per cent in both 1999 and 2005 (reported by Nassar & Sullivan 2001,2 and Laws, Abeywardana, Walker & Sullivan 2007, respectively). Of these single women, over 90 per cent had never been married. In other words, the rise in the proportion of all babies born outside marriage can be attributed to the increase in the proportion of babies born to cohabiting couples.

1.3 Related societal attitudesThe increasing prevalence of cohabitation both fuels and is reinforced by an increasing social acceptance of such lifestyles. Changed views about such lifestyles were highlighted by Qu and Weston (2008a), who found that around three-quarters of adult Australians in a national survey conducted in 2005 agreed with the statement, ‘It is alright for an unmarried couple to live together even if they have no intention of marrying’, while around 70 per cent rejected the notion that ‘marriage is an outdated institution’.3 To gauge some idea of the level of attitudinal change that has occurred, Qu and Weston also referred to the results of a 1971 survey of married women in Melbourne, conducted by the Australian National University (ANU). Two-thirds of these women said that they would be ‘extremely horrified’, ‘considerably upset’ or would ‘consider that they had failed as a parent in his upbringing’, should their son indicate that he intended to live with a woman without marrying her first. In addition, 52 per cent said that they would be more horrified if their daughter reported such intentions, while 2 per cent said that they would be less upset, with the remaining 44 per cent indicating that their reactions would not differ according to their child’s sex.4

Not only is cohabitation now commonly accepted, but cohabiting couples with children are also generally considered to be ‘family’, although a small minority of Australians do not hold this view. This issue was captured in the Australian Survey of Social Attitudes 2003. Respondents were asked which of four different living arrangements they would consider to be a family: a married couple without children, an unmarried couple without children, a married couple with children, and an unmarried couple with children. While married couples with children were universally considered to be families, cohabiting couples with children were also considered so by around eight in 10 respondents (77 per cent of men and 82 per cent of women) (Evans & Gray 2005).5 It would be interesting to see if the gap has narrowed since this survey was conducted.

1.4 Institutionalisation of cohabitation: International patternsMany Western countries have experienced an increase in cohabitation, although the timing and extent of such trends have varied. According to Kiernan (2001, 2002), countries go through four stages in relation to cohabitation. In the initial stage, cohabitation represents a deviant or avant-garde phenomenon and applies to a small minority of the population (for example, Italy, Greece and Spain were at this stage at the time of Kiernan’s analysis). The second stage involves cohabitation as a prelude to marriage or a trial marriage, with marriage remaining the setting for childbearing (for example, the Netherlands, Switzerland and West Germany, according to Kiernan). Subsequently, cohabitation becomes a socially accepted alternative to marriage, with childbearing no longer restricted to marriage (examples being Austria, Great Britain and Ireland). In the fourth stage, cohabitation and marriage become indistinguishable (apparent in

2

Page 10: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Sweden and Denmark, with France and East Germany ‘hovering around this stage’). However, Kiernan noted that in most cases, couples who had a child outside marriage tended to marry within five years.

The increase in the proportion of babies born to cohabiting couples and the social acceptance of cohabitation in its own right suggests that Australia would be in the third stage, although around one-quarter of adults either reject or express uncertainty about whether it is all right for couples to live together without intending to marry.

Related to the issue of proposed stages through which Western countries progress, Cherlin (2004) maintained that, in the United States, marriage has been de-institutionalised while cohabitation is becoming increasingly institutionalised, from both a legal and a social perspective. Such trends would accompany and possibly narrow differences in the everyday meaning of the two arrangements.

The institutionalisation of cohabitation is also apparent in Australia, as evidenced by the introduction of the Family Law Amendment (De Facto Financial Matters and Other Measures) Act 2008. This Act enables cohabiting couples, including those of the same sex, to be treated the same way as married couples in relation to the settlement of financial matters. Even before this amendment to the Family Law Act 1975, cohabiting parents had been treated in the same way as married parents in the sense that their parenting matters were dealt with in the federal sphere. However, prior to the enactment of the amendment, property matters were dealt with in state and territory jurisdictions. In relation to assessments regarding entitlements to government benefits, such as family payment, cohabiting and married parents have been treated in the same manner.

The increasing prevalence of cohabitation has also led to classifications in research that discriminate between the types of living-together union. For instance, the ABS (for example, 2005) uses a measure called ‘social marital status’, which ascertains whether the couple is married or cohabiting.

1.5 Characteristics of married and cohabiting families and sole-mother familiesCherlin (2004) maintained that, although marriage has very much lost its practical meaning, it remains a symbol of status, prestige and achievement in the United States. He noted, for instance, that married couples tend to have higher socioeconomic status than cohabiting couples and that many people who are cohabiting want to get married, but are waiting until they have saved for an expensive wedding or have overcome other difficulties, including any problems in their relationship.

Studies in the United States also suggest that children living with cohabiting parents have a different family environment from those in married families. For example, cohabiting parents in the United States are less likely to participate in paid work and thus, unsurprisingly, tend to experience poorer financial circumstances (Artis 2007; Brown 2004). Some research also suggests that cohabiting mothers are more likely than married mothers to experience mental health problems (Artis 2007; Brown 2000). In addition, it appears that compared with married parents, cohabiting parents (including those with children) are likely to face greater uncertainty concerning the future of their union, with previous research suggesting that cohabiting relationships are considerably more likely than marriages to end in separation (Ermish 2002; Osborne, Manning & Smock 2007; Qu & Weston 2008b).

While much of this research has failed to take into account whether the children were living with a step-parent rather than with both biological (that is, natural) parents, in their study based in the United States Osborne et al. (2007) found that children born to cohabiting parents had a substantially greater risk of their parents separating compared to those born to married parents.

3

Page 11: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

These authors found that the difference was more marked for white children than for African–American or Mexican children.

1.6 Implications for children’s wellbeingLittle research has been undertaken into the implications for children’s wellbeing of growing up with both biological parents who are cohabiting. As Brown and Manning (2009) pointed out, the social marital status of parents in couple families has been largely overlooked in studies of family structure. If these cohabiting families are more fragile, then the circumstances and associated family functioning processes that are linked with their fragility may well have negative consequences for children’s developmental progress and wellbeing (see Magnuson & Berger 2009; Pryor & Rodgers 2001). However, as will be seen below, a 2008 Canadian study challenges this assumption (Wu, Hou & Schimmele 2008). The following discussion provides a summary of findings derived from some of the limited relevant research that has been undertaken to date.

Research by Artis (2007) in the United States has suggested that kindergarten children who lived with their biological cohabiting parents had poorer cognitive and psychosocial development indicators than their counterparts with married parents. Similarly, in another United States study, Osborne, McLanahan and Brooks-Gunn (2003) found that children aged 3 years old who were living with cohabiting parents were more likely than their counterparts with married parents to express aggressive, withdrawn and anxious/depressive behaviour three years later. Hofferth (2006) likewise observed more behavioural problems in a sample of children (aged 3 to 12 years) in the United States who were living with their biological cohabiting parents, compared with their counterparts who were living with married parents, but no differences were apparent in relation to cognitive achievement. Focusing on United States school-aged children and adolescents, Brown (2004) reported similar trends relating to psychological wellbeing (for example, in terms of behavioural and emotional problems and children’s engagement in school—such as completing homework and caring about doing well at school), with children who were living with their biological cohabiting parents faring no better than those living in sole-mother families.

Using data from a large-scale longitudinal study of children in the United Kingdom, Goodman and Greaves (2010) found that children aged 3 years who were born to married parents performed better in both cognitive development and social and emotional development than their counterparts who were born to cohabiting parents. In addition, they found that the gap in cognitive development between the two groups of children appeared to have increased by the time they had reached 5 years of age, while the difference in social and emotional development remained the same.

A study of psychosocial behaviours of Canadian children aged 10 to 15 years generated mixed findings between those who continued to live with their married parents over a five-year period and their counterparts who continued to live with both biological parents who remained unmarried over a two-year period (Wu et al. 2008). Specifically, children who continued to live with both biological parents who were cohabiting were less likely to exhibit prosocial behaviour, but also less likely to exhibit emotional problems compared with the other group of children. Their scores on the conduct disorder measure did not differ significantly. The findings from this study seem counter-intuitive in suggesting no harmful effects.

Finally, Bjorklund, Ginther and Sundstrom (2007) examined the grade point average at age 16 of 130,000 children who were born in Sweden between 1972 and 1987, and found that children whose parents had married before their birth performed significantly better than those whose parents were cohabiting.

All the above research has entailed attempts to explain the observed differences between the two groups of children by controlling for the effects of various family environment factors. Mixed results have emerged from such analyses.

4

Page 12: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Artis (2007) managed to explain differences that emerged for two of the three cognitive measures and for both measures of psychosocial development in terms of systematic differences between the families of these children in such matters as financial resources, mothers’ depressive symptoms and parenting practices. Similarly, Goodman and Greaves (2010) found that the differences in children’s cognitive, social and emotional development were very much explained by differences in parental characteristics, socioeconomic status, parental relationship quality and the history of parental relationship stability.

Osborne et al. (2003) reported that the effect of cohabitation on children’s aggressive and withdrawn behaviours disappeared once parental characteristics and economic and parental resources were controlled. However, the link between the parents’ relationship status and children’s anxious and depressive behaviours weakened but was still apparent.

Hofferth (2006), on the other hand, found that the elevated risk of behavioural problems apparent for those living with cohabiting parents approached statistical significance when such factors as fathers’ education and parental income were controlled for (p < 0.10). Furthermore, with these controls in place, the children who were living with their cohabiting biological parents performed better in terms of cognitive achievement than those who were living with their married biological parents.

In their study of fathering in Australia, which was based on the same longitudinal study as that used in the present report, Baxter and Smart (2010) found that children of cohabiting fathers fared better in terms of cognitive development than children of married fathers. In the United States, Brown (2004) indicated that differences in economic and parental resources between cohabiting families and married families could not fully explain the differences in children’s wellbeing that she examined.

The Canadian study by Wu et al. (2008) suggested that the poorer prosocial behaviour of the children who were living with their cohabiting parents continued to hold when the effects of various child- and parent-related demographic characteristics, along with the family’s financial status and indicators of family dysfunction and parental nurturance, were controlled for. However, the significantly higher emotional disorder scores of children with cohabiting rather than married parents could be explained by child- and parent-related demographic characteristics alone. This study also examined the progress of children who experienced the parental divorce or separation of their cohabiting parents (called ‘cohabitation separation’). The latter children appeared not to be progressing as well as those whose parents stayed together, while parental divorce appeared to have a significantly damaging effect on the children.

While Björklund et al. (2007) reported that the positive association of children’s educational performance with parental marriage was largely due to other differences in family characteristics, they found that marriage appeared to have a positive effect on educational performance for boys but not for girls. Bjorklund et al. concluded that marriage does not provide an advantage for Swedish children in terms of educational progress, but they did not rule out any positive effect linked with marriage for children in countries where cohabitation is less prevalent than in Sweden.

In short, there is some evidence that children living with both biological parents who are cohabiting have a higher risk of poorer developmental progress than children who are living with their married parents, but the results are not entirely consistent, and the family-related characteristics and processes that explain such differences remain uncertain. Some evidence has suggested that children living with their cohabiting parents may be no better off in terms of developmental progress or wellbeing than those in sole-mother families. However, parental divorce may have a more harmful impact on children than cohabitation separation. Reasons for this are unclear. Wu et al. (2008) suggested that, compared with separation from a cohabiting relationship, divorce may entail a more drawn-out process that prolongs children’s distress, although these authors also acknowledged that the results they observed may be a function of sampling problems.

5

Page 13: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Regarding sole-mother families, a great deal of evidence has amassed suggesting that children in these families are at an elevated risk of displaying a broad range of emotional and behavioural adjustment problems, and low school achievement. Factors explaining such trends include the poorer financial circumstances of sole-mother families, poorer maternal health, fewer effective parenting practices, and more limited social support networks (see Mackay 2005; Tobias, Kokaua, Gerritsen & Templeton 2010; Wu et al. 2008). In most cases, sole-mother families result from parental separation. Stressful circumstances—including inter-parental conflict leading to, and following, such separation—also emerge as important factors contributing to children’s elevated risk of diminished wellbeing (see Magnuson & Berger 2009; Pryor & Rodgers 2001). However, there has been little research investigating whether the differences observed in the wellbeing of children in sole-mother families and those in intact couple families are largely a reflection of differences between the former group and children living with two married, rather than cohabiting, parents, given that the parents of the vast majority of children in intact families are married to each other. Brown’s (2004) research would suggest this to be the case.

These limited studies of children in cohabiting and married families are important in that they raise policy-relevant hypotheses about factors that increase the risk of children displaying poor developmental progress. However, generalising these findings (particularly across countries) is difficult for at least three reasons. Firstly, as Brown (2004) has pointed out, much of the research tends to rely on cross-sectional data; therefore, the relationships between parental cohabitation and poorer child wellbeing may be spurious. Secondly, in the absence of life history data for the children, it has not been possible to explore potential long-term or cumulative differential impacts on children of their living in cohabiting or married families. Thirdly, the circumstances experienced by children in cohabiting families are likely to differ in different countries. For example, in countries in which childbearing in cohabitation is unusual and largely ‘frowned upon’, families are likely to differ markedly (for instance, in terms of family resources and parenting) from those in countries in which such behaviour is widespread and sanctioned by the vast majority of people.

1.7 Research questionsThe analyses outlined in this report focus on Australian families, use longitudinal data, and rely on multiple measures of children’s developmental progress, derived from three sources (the primary caregiver, a teacher and the child). While the research interest largely concerns the progress and experiences of children living with married and cohabiting biological parents, in order to place any observed differences in perspective, data concerning children living in sole-mother families were also examined. The analyses were designed to address the following questions in relation to these three groups of families with young children (aged 4 to 5 years when first examined): How common and how stable are these family forms over a four-year period and, of those

that change, what are their most likely pathways? (For example, are cohabiting parents more likely to marry or separate?)

To what extent do these family forms differ in terms of the financial circumstances of the family, selected socio-demographic characteristics of the parents and children, and indicators of parenting practices and parental wellbeing?

To what extent do the children in these different family forms differ in terms of their developmental progress?

To what extent, if at all, do any differences in child wellbeing between the groups diverge or diminish over the four-year period?

If differences in child wellbeing are apparent, to what extent can they be explained by any observed systematic differences in child and family characteristics or parenting practices and parental wellbeing?

6

Page 14: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Of all the children in cohabiting families in the initial survey wave, to what extent, if at all, does their wellbeing vary (at the outset and subsequently) according to whether their parents marry, separate or continue to cohabit over the four-year period?

For succinctness, the families in which the biological parents of the study child were married to each other are called ‘married biological families’, while those in which the parents were cohabiting are called ‘cohabiting biological families’. Some of the study children were also living with a half-sibling (6 per cent in the married families, 19 per cent in the cohabiting families, and 20 per cent in the sole-mother families).

7

Page 15: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

2 Sample, measures and analytical approachesThis report uses data derived from the first three waves (2004, 2006 and 2008) of Growing Up in Australia: The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children, funded by the Australian Government through the Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs.6 LSAC focuses on two cohorts of children: those born between March 2003 and February 2004 (infants in the first survey wave) and those born between March 1999 and February 2000 (children aged 4 to 5 years in the first survey wave). A nationally representative sample of around 5000 children was drawn for each age cohort (for details see Soloff, Lawrence & Johnstone 2005). The analysis on which the present report is based focuses on the elder of these cohorts (often referred to as the K cohort). These children were aged 6 to 7 years in Wave 2, and 8 to 9 years in Wave 3.

This report directs attention to children living with both biological parents and those living with their single mother. Depending on the marital status of the parents in couple families, these families are referred to as ‘married families’ or ‘cohabiting families’. (As will be shown below, only small proportions of children lived in other family forms.) The primary caregiver’s views of the focus child’s wellbeing, along with those of the child’s teacher, form the basis of almost all the analyses of the child’s wellbeing. The mother was the primary caregiver in 98 per cent of families.

Two sets of analyses were carried out for each wave of data, with the comparisons covering three forms of families—married families, cohabiting families and sole-mother families. The first set of analyses is cross-sectional in nature, covering the family forms in each of the three waves, taken separately (‘cross-sectional family groups’). The second set focuses on the three family groups that had not changed family form across the survey waves (‘unchanged family groups’). As mentioned above, cohabiting couples are more likely than married couples to separate, so restricting attention to the three unchanged family groups removes possible differential family conditions that contribute to any separation that, in turn, may affect children’s wellbeing. These two approaches assess the extent to which the three family groups differ systematically in terms of a selection of family characteristics, indicators of family processes and children’s developmental progress.

Multivariate analyses were then carried out to assess the extent to which any differences in children’s wellbeing could be attributed to any existing systematic differences in family characteristics, as well as children’s characteristics (such as age, gender, country of birth, and presence of older or younger siblings in the household). As noted above, much of this work involved analyses based on: (a) members of the three family forms apparent in each survey wave, regardless of whether some members held a different family form status in another survey wave (cross-sectional family groups); and (b) members of the three family forms who remained in the same family form in each survey wave (unchanged family groups). Because the results based on the cross-sectional family groups turned out to be similar to those based on the three unchanged groups, the results of the latter sets of analyses (based on the unchanged family groups) are outlined in the main body of this report, while those based on the three cross-sectional family groups are presented in the Appendix.

A final set of analyses focused exclusively on children who, in Wave 1, had been living with their biological cohabiting parents. The developmental progress of those whose parents subsequently married, separated or continued to live together was examined. Although limited in scope by the small number of children who experienced either transition,7 the key aim of this approach was to gauge whether there was any evidence that the children whose parents eventually separated might be negatively affected by this experience and, if so, whether some negative effects were apparent before the separation occurred. Given the small number of children who experienced the marriage or separation of their initially cohabiting parents, only bivariate analyses were undertaken.

8

Page 16: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

2.1 Parental characteristics and family financial circumstancesThe following parental characteristics were examined: personal age, country of birth, Indigenous status, educational attainment, and employment status. These characteristics were derived for mothers in all three family forms and for fathers in the married and cohabiting families. Two measures were used as indicators of the family’s financial circumstances, based on the reports of the primary caregivers (almost always the mother): an overall evaluation of their financial status, and experiences of financial hardship in the previous 12 months.8

In relation to the first of these measures, the primary caregivers were asked whether, given their current needs and financial responsibilities, they would describe their circumstances as ‘prosperous’, ‘very comfortable’, ‘reasonably comfortable’, ‘just getting by’, ‘poor’ or ‘very poor’. To tap specific financial difficulties, these respondents were asked whether, because of financial hardship: (a) they had been unable to pay gas, electricity or telephone bills on time; (b) they had been unable to pay the mortgage or rent on time; (c) adults or children (in the family) had gone without meals; (d) they had been unable to heat or cool their home; (e) they had pawned or sold something; and (f) they had sought assistance from a welfare or community organisation.9

2.2 Parenting self-efficacy and practicesThe measure of parenting self-efficacy was based on mothers’ and fathers’ ratings of their competence relative to other parents’ (a single-item measure). Four measures of parenting practices were derived, again based on the self-reports of fathers and mothers: ‘parental warmth’, ‘hostile parenting’, ‘inductive reasoning’ and ‘consistent parenting’.

The ‘parental warmth’ measure focused on such issues as how often the respondents expressed affection, listened to their children, had a warm and close time together with their children, and felt close to their children. On the other hand, the ‘hostile parenting’ scale gauged such matters as how often they praised their children or expressed disapproval of their children’s behaviour, how often they felt angry when they punished their children, and how often they had problems in managing their children. The ‘inductive reasoning’ measure tapped the frequency with which parents provided an explanation when they corrected their children’s behaviour or reasoned with their children when they misbehaved. The ‘consistent parenting’ scale assessed such issues as how often parents felt their children ‘got away’ with things when they should have been punished, how often the parent made sure that the children obeyed directions, and how often their children ignored any punishment they received.

All the items in the parenting practice scales were rated on a scale ranging from 1: ‘never/almost never’ to 5: ‘all the time’. Each parent’s mean rating for each scale was derived. To the extent that respondents provided accurate accounts of their behaviour, a high score on the parental warmth scale suggests that the parent often expressed warmth towards the children, while a high score on the hostile parenting scale suggests that the parent often expressed harsh attitudes towards the children. In relation to the scales concerning parenting consistency and use of reasoning, high scores suggest that the parent tended to be consistent in dealing with the children’s behaviour and often engaged in attempts to reason with them. Further details of these measures are presented in the Appendix.

2.3 Parents’ psychological distressThe six-item Kessler-6 screening scale for psychological distress was used (Kessler et al. 2002). Fathers and mothers were asked how often in the past four weeks they had felt: (a)  nervous, (b) hopeless, (c) restless or fidgety, (d) worthless, and (e) so sad that nothing could cheer them up, and (f) how often they felt that everything was an effort. These items were designed to detect parental mood and anxiety disorders. Scores on this scale ranged from 1 to 5, with higher scores

9

Page 17: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

suggesting a higher level of distress in the present report. Each parent’s average rating for the six items was derived.

2.4 Quality of inter-parental relationshipsTwo measures relating to the quality of inter-parental relationships were derived from both parents in cohabiting and married families: relationship happiness and arguments.

Parents were asked to rate their overall happiness in the relationship on a scale ranging from 1 (extremely unhappy) to 7 (perfectly happy). The measure on arguments between the father and mother was derived from parents’ responses to five questions about how often they and their partner engaged in disagreements, arguments and physical confrontation, and how often they felt stress and hostility towards each other. Each parent’s average rating for these five items was derived. Higher scores suggest poorer relationships.

2.5 Child wellbeing measuresOne of the strengths of the LSAC data is its wide range of child wellbeing measures, based on assessments provided by the primary caregiver and a teacher, and on measures administered to the child (for details on these measures, see Sanson, Misson and the LSAC Outcome Index Working Group 2005). The wellbeing measures cover components of the child’s physical, social–emotional and cognitive developmental progress. As explained by Sanson et al., an overall ‘outcome index’ (or overall measure of developmental progress) was derived, based on three other outcome indices covering developmental progress in the physical, social/emotional and learning domains. As their name implies, these four outcome indices are composite measures and provided in standardised scores, with a mean at 100 and standard deviation at 10. Higher scores indicate better outcomes (or developmental progress). These indices are comparable across waves.

The present report focuses on the overall outcome index, the three component indices, and one sub-component of the learning index—a measure of the child’s receptive language skills (using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [PPVT-III Form IIA]). PPVT scores for the K cohort of children ranged from 28 to 106 (with a mean of 72 and a standard deviation of 8).

It is common for research on the clinical assessment of psychological state to generate different results based on different informants (for example, reports of children’s behaviours from parents, teachers, clinical observations), and there is no evidence available regarding who make accurate and reliable informants (see De Los Reyes 2011). For example, children do not behave in the same way across different social contexts, and inconsistencies in reporting children’s behaviours among informants (for example, parents, teachers) may reveal useful information about variations of children’s behaviours in different settings (De Los Reyes 2011). Thus, data derived from five multi-item scales based on both parents’ and teachers’ responses to the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) were used in the analyses. That is, two scores were derived for each of the five scales—one based on parents’ reports, and the other on teachers’ reports. The five scales represent components of the social–emotional index and cover: (a) prosocial behaviour (that is, sharing easily, being considerate, being kind to younger children); (b) peer problems (that is, having difficulties in developing favourable relationships with other children); (c) emotional symptoms (that is, frequency of exhibiting negative emotional states); (d) hyperactivity (covering such behaviour as fidgeting and concentration span); and (e) conduct problems (exhibited when interacted with others). All scores ranged from 1 to 5, with higher scores reflecting better outcomes for the prosocial behaviour measure, and poorer outcomes for the other four scales. All measures except the conduct problems scale were comparable across the survey waves.

10

Page 18: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

3 ResultsThe results of the various analyses are set out according to the research question they were designed to address. These cover: (a) how common and stable the various family forms were over the four-year period; (b) the extent to which the three family forms of interest differ in character (that is, financial wellbeing, child and parent socio-demographic characteristics), parenting practices and parental wellbeing; (c) the extent to which children in the three family forms differ in terms of their developmental progress; (d) whether there was any evidence that differences between the three family groups diverged or diminished over the four-year period; (e) the extent to which any such differences can be explained by the measures tapping child and family characteristics, parenting practices and parental wellbeing; and (f) whether the wellbeing of children living with cohabiting parents varied (at the outset and subsequently) according to whether their parents married, separated or continued to cohabit over the four-year period.

3.1 Family structureHow common are these family forms?Table 1 shows the proportion of children in the various family forms in each survey wave taken separately (2004, 2006 and 2008). As noted above, the children’s ages were 4 to 5 years in Wave 1, 6 to 7 years in Wave 2, and 8 to 9 years in Wave 3. The family forms were based on whether children were living with both parents or one parent, whether the child had a step-parent in the household, and the social marital status (married or cohabiting) of the parents.

Table 1: Family form by survey wave, K cohort children

4 to 5 years(Wave 1)

6 to 7 years(Wave 2)

8 to 9 years(Wave 3)

%Two biological, married parents 72.6 71.8 68.8Two biological, cohabiting parents 9.3 7.4 6.3One biological parent, married stepfamily 0.8 1.3 2.9One biological parent, cohabiting stepfamily 2.0 2.2 4.2Sole biological mother 14.2 15.8 15.6Sole biological father 0.6 0.9 1.1Neither biological parent in the household 0.4 0.4 0.5Other (a) 0.0 0.2 0.6Total 100.0 100.0 100.0No. of respondents 4983 4464 4331

Note: a ‘Other’ covers families for which information about the relationship between members was incomplete. The K cohort refers to children who were aged between 4 and 5 years in Wave 1 (2004). Data have been weighted. The number of respondents in the table is unweighted. There were only nine adopted children in the entire sample. These were therefore treated as biological children. Their inclusion does not change the results at all.

Across the three waves, most of the children (69 to 73 per cent) were living in married biological families, with only 6 to 9 per cent living in cohabiting biological families. In total, then, 75 to 82 per cent of children were living with both biological parents in each survey wave (taken separately). A substantial minority were living with one biological parent, usually the mother (14 to 16 per cent were in sole-mother families and around 1 per cent were in sole-father families). A small proportion of children in each survey wave were living with one biological parent and one step-parent (3 to 7 per cent). Of those in stepfamilies, the vast majority were living with their biological mother and stepfather (96 per cent) (data not shown).

11

Page 19: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

The parents in stepfamilies were as likely to be cohabiting as to be married (2 to 4 per cent of all children were in cohabiting stepfamilies; 1 to 3 per cent were in married stepfamilies). Few children in any of the survey waves were living with neither biological parent (fewer than 1 per cent).10 In a small proportion of cases (fewer than 1 per cent), the family form in which the child was living could not be determined.

Table 1 also shows that the proportion of children in married and cohabiting biological families declined slightly over the three waves (married: from 73 per cent in Wave 1 to 72 per cent in Wave 2 and 69 per cent in Wave 3; cohabiting: from 9 per cent in Wave 1 to 7 per cent in Wave 2 and 6 per cent in Wave 3). On the other hand, there was a marginal increase in the proportion of children living with one parent (from 15 per cent in Wave 1, to 17 per cent in two subsequent waves), and in the proportion living with one biological and one step-parent (from 3 per cent to 7 per cent).

Overall, there were 519 fewer children represented in the second than first survey (representing an attrition rate of only 10 per cent) and 652 fewer children represented in the third than first survey (representing an attrition rate of 13 per cent). In Wave 3, 84 per cent of the original sample of K cohort children were followed up in both subsequent survey waves, and 3 per cent were followed up in Wave 3 but not in Wave 2.

How stable were these family forms over a four-year period and, of those that change family form, what are their most likely pathways?As living systems, families are by nature dynamic. They change in characteristics and in functioning as children are born, as members grow older, and as children gain independence and leave home. Various factors, such as external pressures and opportunities, health issues, financial issues, employment circumstances and relationship dynamics can also combine to bring about further changes, with some parents separating, some re-partnering, others reconciling, and some cohabiting parents marrying.

Table 2 focuses on three family forms in Wave 1—married and cohabiting biological families and sole-mother families—and shows the extent to which they had changed their form by Wave 3 (four years later). This table is restricted to the families of K cohort children who were represented in both these survey waves.11

Married biological families represented the most stable of the three family forms: more than nine in 10 had not changed status from Wave 1 to 3, with 6 per cent of children living with one biological parent, typically the mother, and only 1 per cent living with their mother and a stepfather. It is worth noting that, for a very small proportion of children (less than 1 per cent) in married biological families in Wave 1, the recorded marital status of parents had changed from married to cohabiting by Wave 3. While this may have resulted from mis-recording, it is also possible that the parents had divorced then reconciled during the four-year interval between the two surveys.

12

Page 20: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Table 2: Change in family form from Wave 1 to Wave 3, K cohort children

Wave 3

Wave 1

Two biological, married parents

Two biological, cohabiting

parentsSole mother

%Two biological, married parents 92.5 15.7 2.4Two biological, cohabiting parents 0.4 65.0 3.2Step-parent family 1.1 3.5 28.4Sole mother 5.5 14.3 63.2Sole father 0.5 1.5 1.4Other 0.0 0.0 1.4Total 100.0 100.0 100.0No of observations 3349 343 497

Note: Unweighted data.

Two-thirds of the children from cohabiting biological families in Wave 1 were in the same living arrangement in Wave 3. In other words, consistent with previous research outlined above, children from these families were more likely to experience parental separation between Wave 1 and Wave 3 (19 per cent) than those from married biological families (7 per cent). Thus, children from cohabiting biological families were more likely than those from married families to experience a switch to living in lone-parent families or stepfamilies some four years after the first survey. By Wave 3, 14 per cent of the children whose parents were cohabiting in Wave 1 were living with a sole mother, fewer than 2 per cent were living with a sole father, and around 4 per cent were in stepfamilies (almost always involving a stepfather). On the other hand, rates of subsequent marriage of cohabiting parents (16 per cent) by Wave 3 were similar to rates of separation (19 per cent).

Much the same proportion of children from cohabiting biological families and sole-mother families experienced unchanged family forms (65 per cent and 63 per cent respectively). In most cases, the change from a sole-mother family came about because the mother had re-partnered (applying to 28 per cent of all children in sole-mother families in Wave 1). Nevertheless, 6 per cent of sole mothers appeared to have reconciled with their child’s father, comprising over 3 per cent who were apparently cohabiting with him in Wave 3 and over 2 per cent who had apparently remarried him. It is also possible that some of these sole mothers may have lived apart from their child’s father for practical reasons (for example, work-related locational issues) and had begun living together by Wave 3. Just over 1 per cent of the children had moved from living with their sole mother to living with their sole father.12

Despite the many advantages of longitudinal studies, a key difficulty relates to sample attrition. By Wave 3, 13 per cent of the original K cohort of children were not in the study. Table A1 in the Appendix shows that, of the three family groups (the two biological couple families and sole-mother families), sample attrition was highest for the sole-mother families and lowest for the married biological families. Put another way, the following proportions of children were represented in all three survey waves: in Wave 1, 88 per cent who were living with both biological married parents; 75 per cent who were living with both biological cohabiting parents, and 72 per cent who were living with their sole mothers.

As explained in the LSAC 2008–09 Annual Report (LSAC 2009), families that did not participate in the later waves differed somewhat from those who continued to participate. For example, parents of the former group were more likely to have a non-English-speaking background, to identify as Indigenous, and to have lower incomes. Despite the socioeconomic differences between families that remained in the study and those who dropped out, it is

13

Page 21: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

difficult to ascertain the extent to which parental separation contributed to discontinuation in the study, especially for cohabiting parents.

The best that can be achieved at this stage is to examine separation rates among those who participated in Waves 1 and 3, but not Wave 2 (reflecting temporary discontinuation). There were 16 children who were in cohabiting biological families in Wave 1 whose families did not participate in Wave 2 but returned to the study in Wave 3. In three of these cases, the parents had married by Wave 3; in another three, the parents had separated; and in the remaining 10, the parents continued to cohabit. Of the 86 married biological families who participated in Waves 1 and 3 but not Wave 2, only 11 had separated by Wave 3. While these numbers are too small to provide any firm evidence, it is possible that parental separation was not the key reason behind temporary discontinuation in the study.

3.2 Child, parent and family characteristicsTo what extent do the family forms differ in terms of the financial circumstances of the family, selected socio-demographic characteristics of the parents and children, and indicators of parenting practices and parental wellbeing?This section focuses on children in the three unchanged forms of families of key interest: married and cohabiting biological families and sole-mother families. The child, parent and family characteristics of these groups are compared (bivariate analysis).

Child’s characteristicsTable 3 shows selected characteristics of children in Wave 1 across the three unchanged family groups. The children in each group were similar in age and gender. In Wave 1, they were 4.8 years old on average and 51 to 53 per cent were boys. While few children were born overseas, those in married biological families were more likely to have been born outside Australia (5 per cent) than those in the other two groups (1 to 2 per cent). Although uncommon across all groups, the children in married families were the least likely to have an Indigenous background (2 per cent as opposed to 8 to 10 per cent). Only a minority spoke a language other than English at home, with children in married biological families being the most likely to do so (16 per cent) and those in cohabiting biological families being the least likely to do so (5 per cent).

While much the same proportion of children in each group had an elder sibling (40 to 44 per cent), children in sole-mother families were less likely to have a younger sibling (32 per cent) than those who were living with both biological parents (49 to 50 per cent).

14

Page 22: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Table 3: Selected characteristics of children by (unchanged) family form, K cohort, Wave 1 (a)

Married families Cohabitating families Sole-mother familiesAge (mean years) 4.80 4.80 4.81

% % %Sex

Boy 51.0 52.2 52.6Girl 49.0 47.8 47.4Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Country of birthAustralia 94.8 98.6 98.8Other country 5.2 1.5 ** 1.2 ***Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Indigenous statusNo 98.3 89.7 *** 91.7 ***Yes 1.7 10.3 8.4Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Language at homeEnglish 84.1 94.8 88.7Other 15.9 5.3 *** 11.3 **Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Any older siblings in householdNo 39.8 43.6 42.9Yes 60.2 56.4 57.1Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Any younger siblings in householdNo 51.1 50.5 67.7 ***Yes 48.9 49.5 32.3Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

No. of respondents 3702 436 656

(a) The family forms in this table remained the same across three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Children in cohabiting and sole-mother families were separately compared with those in married families.

Note: Statistical differences (chi-square test for categorical variables; one-way analysis of variance for continuous variables) are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

Parental characteristicsTable 4 presents information about biological mothers’ characteristics for all three unchanged family groups and biological fathers’ characteristics for the two forms of couple families. Some characteristics, such as country of birth and Indigenous status, are fixed and are therefore only presented in Wave 1. Parents’ age is also only presented for Wave 1, whereas their employment status and level of education during each survey wave are shown.

Compared with married biological mothers, cohabiting biological mothers and sole mothers were marginally, but significantly, younger (in Wave 1: mean = 34 years as opposed to 35 years), less likely to have a degree or higher qualification, and more likely to have no qualification at all. For example, 15 per cent of the cohabiting and sole mothers had a degree or higher in Wave 1, compared with 31 per cent of the married mothers. Not surprisingly, this pattern changed little across time.13

15

Page 23: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

The vast majority of mothers in all three groups were non-Indigenous and most were born in Australia. Nevertheless, compared with the married group, the cohabiting and sole mothers (especially the latter) were slightly more likely to identify as Indigenous (3 to 6 per cent as opposed to 1 per cent) and to be born in Australia (80 to 84 per cent as opposed to 74 per cent).

While sole mothers were significantly less likely than married mothers to be in paid work (Wave 1: 49 per cent as opposed to 60 per cent; Wave 2: 54 per cent as opposed to 67 per cent; Wave 3: 63 per cent as opposed to 75 per cent), there was no significant difference in the employment status of the cohabiting and married mothers except in Wave 3, where the cohabiting mothers were more likely to be non-employed and less likely to be working part time. The employment status of the cohabiting and sole mothers was more alike in Wave 3 than in the other two waves. There was an overall similar employment pattern across the first and second waves, with cohabiting mothers’ employment rates lying between those of married and sole mothers; for example, in Wave 2, 33 per cent of married mothers, 39 per cent of cohabiting mothers, and 46 per cent of sole mothers were not employed. In Wave 3, the employment rates of cohabiting mothers were similar to those of sole mothers, but were lower compared with those of married mothers.

Table 4 shows that cohabiting biological fathers differed from married biological fathers in a number of ways. Specifically, the cohabiting fathers had a lower level of education, were more likely to identify as Indigenous, and were less likely to be employed. For example, 30 per cent of the married fathers and only 15 per cent of the cohabiting fathers had a degree or higher qualification in Wave 1; around 1 per cent of the married fathers and 4 per cent of the cohabiting fathers had an Indigenous background; and 90 to 91 per cent of the married fathers in each survey wave were working full time, compared with 82 to 85 per cent of the cohabiting fathers.

Very similar trends in parental characteristics also emerged when analyses were based on the cross-sectional family groups (that is, when the focus included families that changed their form over the period investigated). The latter results are presented in Appendix Table A2.

16

Page 24: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Table 4: Selected parental characteristics by unchanged family form, K cohort (a)

17

Page 25: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3Married families

Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

Married families

Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

Married families

Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

Mothers’ characteristicsAge (mean years) 35.4 34.3 ** 34.1 **

% % %Education

Degree or higher 31.1 14.7 *** 15.2 *** 30.7 14.0 *** 12.6 *** 30.4 13.7 *** 12.9 ***

Other post-school qualification 36.0 38.4 41.8 39.1 40.3 49.3 ** 41.8 45.7 53.8 ***

No qualification 32.9 46.9 *** 43.0 *** 30.2 45.7 *** 38.1 * 27.8 40.6 ** 33.3

Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0Country of birth

Australia 73.5 80.0 83.8 ***Other country 26.5 20.0 16.2 ***Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Indigenous statusNo 99.0 96.9 ** 93.8 ***Yes 1.0 3.1 ** 6.2 ***Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Employment statusFull time 15.3 15.4 11.8 19.0 15.5 15.1 23.2 20.2 21.7Part time 45.0 40.3 36.9 ** 48.4 46.0 39.2 ** 51.5 43.4 * 41.0 ***Not employed 39.7 44.4 51.4 *** 32.6 38.5 45.8 *** 25.3 36.4 ** 37.3 ***Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0

No. of observations 3010 208 280 3020 209 284 3020 209 284

Fathers’ characteristicsAge (mean years) 37.9 37.2

18

Page 26: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3Married families

Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

Married families

Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

Married families

Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

% % %Education

Degree or higher 30.4 15.4 *** 30.3 14.5 *** 30.7 14.4 ***

Other post-school qualification 45.8 54.1 * 46.9 56.3 * 47.9 57.9 *

No qualification 23.8 30.6 22.8 29.3 21.4 27.8Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0

Country of birthOther country 27.5 21.9Australia 72.5 78.1Total 100.0 100.0

Indigenous statusNo 99.3 96.2 ***Yes 0.7 3.8Total 100.0 100.0

Employment statusFull time 89.6 81.9 ** 89.7 84.3 * 90.8 84.5 **Part time 5.5 7.0 5.7 7.4 5.3 8.2Not employed 4.9 11.1 *** 4.6 8.3 * 3.8 7.3 *Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0

No. of respondents 3003 207 3018 209 3020 209

(a) The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were compared with married parents.

Note: Statistical differences (chi-square test for categorical variables and one-way ANOVA for continuous variables) are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

19

Page 27: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Family financial circumstancesThe financial circumstances of the three groups of families that did not change in form differed considerably. In each survey wave, primary carers of children (mostly mothers) were asked whether they had experienced any of six financial hardships in the previous 12 months, such as the inability to pay utility bills, or inability to pay the mortgage or rent (see Section for complete list). In addition, they were asked whether they were in a ‘comfortable’ or ‘prosperous’ position, whether they were ‘just getting by’, or whether they were ‘poor’ or ‘very poor’. Responses to these two sets of questions are presented in Error: Reference source notfound and Error: Reference source not found respectively.

As shown in Error: Reference source not found, married biological families were the least likely of the three unchanged family groups to report having experienced any of the six indicators of financial hardship, while sole-mother families were the most likely to report such experiences. This pattern of results was apparent in all the three survey waves. Specifically, at least one financial hardship was reported by 22 per cent of the married respondents in Wave 1, compared with 40 per cent of the cohabiting respondents and 56 per cent of the sole mothers. By Wave 3, the respective proportions were 13 per cent, 17 per cent and 46 per cent. On this measure, then, it would seem that the cohabiting biological families were better off than the sole-mother families, but worse off than the married families.

Figure 1: Experience of one or more financial hardships, by (unchanged) family form, primary carers’ reports, Waves 1 to 3

Note: The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents.

Accessible long description of Figure 1

WAVE FAMILY FORM EXPERIENCE OF ONE OR MORE FINANCIAL HARDSHIPS1 MARRIED FAMILIES 22%

1 COHABITING FAMILIES 40%

1 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES 56%

2 MARRIED FAMILIES 13% (approximately)

2 COHABITING FAMILIES 22% (approximately)

20

Page 28: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

WAVE FAMILY FORM EXPERIENCE OF ONE OR MORE FINANCIAL HARDSHIPS2 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES 44% (approximately)

3 MARRIED FAMILIES 13%

3 COHABITING FAMILIES 17%

3 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES 47%

Consistent with the patterns concerning financial hardship, married biological respondents were the most likely of the three groups to describe their financial circumstances as being ‘reasonably comfortable’ or ‘prosperous’, and sole mothers were the least likely to do so. Conversely, sole mothers were the most likely to indicate that they were ‘just getting by’, and married respondents were the least likely to state this. While few respondents considered that they were ‘poor’ or ‘very poor’, sole mothers more commonly made such claims than did cohabiting or married respondents. For example, in Wave 1, 42 per cent of sole mothers, 64 per cent of cohabiting respondents and 69 per cent of married respondents described their circumstances as being ‘reasonably comfortable’, ‘comfortable’ or ‘prosperous’, while the view that they were ‘just getting by’ was expressed by 48 per cent, 32 per cent, and 30 per cent respectively, and reports that they were ‘poor’ or ‘very poor’ were provided by 10 per cent, 4 per cent and 2 per cent respectively. Much the same results emerged when the analyses were based on the cross-sectional family groups (see Appendix Table A3).

Figure 2: Ratings of families’ financial situation, by (unchanged) family form, primary carers’ reports, Waves 1 to 3

Note: The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents.

Accessible long description of Figure 2

WAVE FAMILY FORM PRIMARY CARERS’ RATINGS OF FINANCIAL SITUATION

1 MARRIED FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 69% ‘GETTING BY’ 30% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 2%

1 COHABITING FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 64% ‘GETTING BY’ 32% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 4%

21

Page 29: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

WAVE FAMILY FORM PRIMARY CARERS’ RATINGS OF FINANCIAL SITUATION

1 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 42% ‘GETTING BY’ 48% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 10%

2 MARRIED FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 80% ‘GETTING BY’ 19% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 1%

2 COHABITING FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 72% ‘GETTING BY’ 27% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 0%

2 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 42% ‘GETTING BY’ 50% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 7%

3 MARRIED FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 77% ‘GETTING BY’ 22% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 2%

3 COHABITING FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 68% ‘GETTING BY’ 28% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 3%

3 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 43% ‘GETTING BY’ 49% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 8%

Parenting self-efficacyParents were asked to indicate how good a parent they were, compared with other parents. This item was used as an indicator of parenting self-efficacy. The patterns of responses of the married, cohabiting and sole mothers in the unchanged family forms are depicted in Error:Reference source not found, while those of married and cohabiting fathers are depicted in Error:Reference source not found.14

Regardless of family form, mothers most commonly considered themselves to be ‘very good’ or ‘better than average’ parents, with 51 to 73 per cent holding this view across the three survey waves. Most of the other mothers considered themselves to be ‘average’ parents, and only a very small percentage rated themselves as ‘not very good’ parents or ‘had some trouble’ being parents.

However, systematic differences in sense of parenting efficacy were apparent across the three unchanged family groups. Favourable self-assessments (that is, that they were a very good or better than average parent) were provided by a marginally higher proportion of married than cohabiting mothers (65 per cent as opposed to 61 per cent in Wave 1), and by a lower proportion of sole mothers (55 per cent in Wave 1). Similar trends were apparent in the other two survey waves. Conversely, a slightly higher proportion of sole mothers than married or cohabiting mothers in each survey wave provided the most unfavourable self-assessments (that is, they were not very good parents or had some trouble being parents). For example, in Wave 1, this was reported by 7 per cent of sole mothers compared to 3 to 4 per cent of couple mothers.

Figure 3: Mothers’ ratings of self as a parent, by (unchanged) family form, Waves 1 to 3

22

Page 30: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Note: The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which children lived with their biological parents.

Accessible long description of Figure 3

WAVE FAMILY FORM MOTHERS’ RATINGS OF SELF AS A PARENT

1 MARRIED MOTHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 65%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 33%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 2%

1 COHABITING MOTHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 61%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 35%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 4%

1 SOLE MOTHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 55%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 38%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 7%

2 MARRIED MOTHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 73%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 26%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 1%

2 COHABITING MOTHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 64%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 35%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 0%

2 SOLE MOTHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 63%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 35%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 2%

3 MARRIED MOTHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 63%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 35%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 2%

3 COHABITING MOTHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 59%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 36%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 4%

3 SOLE MOTHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 51%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 42%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 7%

Married and cohabiting fathers were also inclined to hold favourable views of themselves as parents, with 47 to 70 per cent seeing themselves as very good or better than average parents, and 28 to 46 per cent believing that they were average parents across the three survey waves. Nevertheless, cohabiting fathers provided consistently less positive self-assessments compared with married fathers. For example, 68 per cent of married fathers in Wave 1 felt that they were very good or better than average parents, compared with 47 per cent of cohabiting fathers.

23

Page 31: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

While 3 per cent of married fathers provided negative self-assessments, 7 per cent of cohabiting fathers shared this view.

Figure 4: Fathers’ ratings of self as a parent by (unchanged) family form, Waves 1 to 3

Note: The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised the biological parents of the child.

Accessible long description of Figure 4

WAVE FAMILY FORM FATHERS’ RATINGS OF SELF AS A PARENT

1 MARRIED FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 68%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 30%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 3%

1 COHABITING FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 47%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 46%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 7%

2 MARRIED FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 70%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 28%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 2%

2 COHABITING FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 63%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 33%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 4%

3 MARRIED FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 66%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 15%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 3%

3 COHABITING FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 52%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 43%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 5%

Overall, then, married mothers and fathers provided similar (and favourable) self-assessments, with 65 to 68 per cent believing that they were very good or above average parents in Wave 1. However, cohabiting fathers were less likely than cohabiting mothers to hold such views (47 per cent as opposed to 61 per cent in Wave 1).

24

Page 32: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Parenting practicesAs outlined earlier, parents were asked a series of questions designed to tap their tendency to ‘parent’ their children in a warm manner; parent their children in a hostile manner; reason with their child when they corrected them or when their child misbehaved; and handle their child’s behaviour in a consistent manner (for example, in dealing with misbehaviour and in ensuring that their child followed their directions). Each of these measures was based on ratings of the frequency with which the parents engaged in the behaviour in question (on a five-point scale). Table 5 depicts the mean scores of mothers in the three unchanged family forms and those of fathers in the two unchanged couple family forms.

Table 5: Mean scores of parenting approach scales, fathers and mothers, by unchanged family form, Waves 1 to 3

Mothers FathersMarried mothers

Cohabiting mothers Sole mothers Married

fathersCohabiting

fathersWave 1

Warm parenting a 4.43 4.42 4.47 4.09 4.02Hostile parenting b 2.17 2.23 2.20 2.28 2.33Use of reasoning c 4.25 4.25 4.27 3.98 3.91Consistency of parenting d 4.10 3.92 *** 3.84 *** 3.99 3.75 ***

Wave 2Warm parenting a 4.44 4.43 4.46 4.12 4.14Hostile parenting b 2.16 2.20 2.28 2.08 2.10Use of reasoning c 4.22 4.18 4.21 3.95 3.91Consistency of parenting d 4.17 4.01 *** 3.95 *** 4.09 3.91 **

Wave 3Warm parenting a 4.34 4.29 4.37 * 4.06 4.12Hostile parenting b 2.13 2.21 2.23 2.16 2.18Use of reasoning c 4.14 4.08 4.13 3.91 4.09 **Consistency of parenting d 4.20 3.93 *** 3.93 *** 4.10 3.99

No. of observations 3020 208 284 3020 208

Note: All scores are on a five-point scale. a Higher scores = greater warmth. b Higher scores = higher hostility.

c Higher scores = higher use of reasoning. d Higher scores = greater consistency.The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were separately compared with married parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

Across the three waves, mothers reported high warmth, low hostility, high use of reasoning and also generally high consistency in the way they parented their child. These patterns were also apparent among the reports of fathers. Across the three survey waves, mean scores across all groups of mothers and fathers respectively varied as follows:15

warmth: 4.29 to 4.47 and 4.02 to 4.14 hostility: 2.13 to 2.28 and 2.08 to 2.33 reasoning: 4.08 to 4.27 and 3.91 to 4.09 consistency: 3.84 to 4.20 and 3.75 to 4.10.

Table 5 also shows that, regardless of the survey wave, there was little difference across the three groups of mothers in their reports on the frequency with which they expressed warmth or hostility and the frequency with which they used reasoning. However, in all three waves the cohabiting mothers and sole mothers reported significantly lower consistency in their parenting. Mean scores on this measure were 4.10 in Wave 1, 4.17 in Wave 2 and 4.20 in Wave 3 for

25

Page 33: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

married mothers, 3.92 in Wave 1, 4.01 in Wave 2 and 3.93 in Wave 3 for cohabiting mothers, and 3.83 in Wave 1, 3.95 in Wave 2 and 3.93 in Wave 3 for sole mothers.

The reports of cohabiting and married (biological) fathers were similar in relation to the frequency with which they adopted a warm or hostile approach. However, cohabiting fathers were more likely than married fathers to indicate frequent use of reasoning in Wave 3 (mean scores: 4.09 as opposed to 3.91), with ratings on this measure not varying significantly in Waves 1 and 2. Across each survey wave, cohabiting fathers indicated that they were less likely to adopt a consistent approach to parenting compared with married fathers (Wave 1: 3.75 as opposed to 3.99; Wave 2: 3.91 as opposed to 4.09; Wave 3: 3.99 as opposed to 4.10), though the difference was not statistically significant in Wave 3. The results based on the cross-sectional family groups are presented in Appendix Table A4.

Parental psychological wellbeing and quality of couple relationshipsThe measure of parental emotional distress indicates how often, during the previous four weeks, parents had felt nervous, hopeless, restless/fidgety, worthless, and so sad that nothing could cheer them up, and how often they felt that everything was an effort (that is, six ratings). Each parent’s mean rating (ranging from 1 to 5) was derived, and higher mean scores reflected higher average distress. Table 6 shows that the overall mean scores for all groups of mothers and fathers were below the midpoint of the scale, suggesting generally limited experiences of distress.

Across the three waves, sole mothers indicated significantly higher psychological distress than married mothers (mean scores: Wave 1, 1.96 as opposed to 1.62; Wave 2, 1.85 as opposed to 1.49; Wave 3, 1.86 as opposed to 1.54), while cohabiting mothers indicated significantly higher distress than married mothers in Wave 1 only (mean scores: 1.73 as opposed to 1.62). Similarly, cohabiting fathers indicated significantly higher distress than married fathers in Wave 1 (mean scores: 1.69 as opposed to 1.55), but not in subsequent waves.

Both mothers and fathers tended to indicate that they had favourable inter-parental relationships (as measured by overall happiness and the frequency with which they tended to relate to each other in argumentative, hostile ways). Nevertheless, cohabiting parents provided significantly less favourable assessments than married parents. Mean ratings on the relationship happiness scale were 4.88 in Wave 1, 4.83 in Wave 2 and 4.75 in Wave 3 for cohabiting mothers, and 5.39 in Wave 1, 5.37 in Wave 2 and 5.24 in Wave 3 for married mothers. Mean ratings were 4.85 in Wave 1, 4.81 in Wave 2 and 4.79 in Wave 3 for cohabiting fathers, while they were 5.47 in Wave 1, 5.36 in Wave 2 and 5.44 in Wave 3 for married fathers. Mean scores on the argumentative relationship scale were 2.21 in Wave 1, 2.03 in Wave 2 and 2.12 in Wave 3 for cohabiting mothers, and 1.98 in Wave 1, 1.82 in Wave 2 and 1.89 in Wave 3 for married mothers. For cohabiting fathers, mean scores were 2.29 in Wave 1 and 2.07 in the two later waves, while for married fathers mean scores were 1.96 in Wave 1, 1.87 in Wave 2 and 1.84 in Wave 3. Again, the same patterns emerged when the analyses were based on the cross-sectional family groups (see Appendix Table A5).

26

Page 34: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Table 6: Mean scores of psychological distress scale and quality of parental relationship of fathers and mothers by unchanged family form, Waves 1 to 3

Mothers FathersMarried mothers

Cohabiting mothers

Sole mothers

Married fathers

Cohabiting fathers

Wave 1Psychological distress (Kessler 6) a 1.62 1.73 * 1.96 *** 1.55 1.69 *Degree of happiness in relationship with the partner b 5.39 4.88 *** 5.47 4.85 **

*

Argumentative relationship scale c 1.98 2.21 *** 1.96 2.29 ***

Wave 2Psychological distress (Kessler 6) a 1.49 1.57 1.85 *** 1.52 1.59Degree of happiness in relationship with the partner b 5.37 4.83 *** 5.36 4.81 **

*Argumentative relationship scale c 1.82 2.03 *** 1.87 2.07 **

Wave 3Psychological distress (Kessler 6) a 1.54 1.65 1.86 *** 1.52 1.62Degree of happiness in relationship with the partner b 5.24 4.75 *** 5.44 4.79 **

*Argumentative relationship scale c 1.89 2.12 *** 1.84 2.07 **

No of observations 3020 208 284 3020 208

Note: a Scale of 1 to 5, higher scores = higher distress. b Scale of 1–7, 1 = extremely unhappy, 7 = perfectly happy.

c Scale of 1 to 5, higher scores = more prone to argument. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were compared with married parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

3.3 Children’s wellbeingThis section first compares the wellbeing or developmental progress of children in the three unchanged family forms. The analyses focus on the range of indicators of wellbeing outlined in Section . One of the measures tapped the children’s receptive language skills (based on the PPVT that was administered to the children); nine were based on the assessments of the primary caregiver; and assessments for five of these nine were also provided by one of the child’s teachers. The extent to which any differences in the wellbeing of the children in the three family forms can be explained by systematic differences between the family forms (noted in Section 3.1) is then examined. Once again, attention is directed to the children in the three unchanged family forms, while the results for the ‘cross-sectional’ group (some of whom experienced changes in family form) appear in Appendix Tables A6 and A7.

Wellbeing of children in each family formTo what extent do the children in the different family forms differ in terms of their developmental progress?To what extent, if at all, do any differences in child wellbeing between the groups diverge or diminish over the four-year period?Table 7 provides the children’s mean scores on the PPVT, along with various assessments relating to the children’s wellbeing provided by the primary caregiver (usually the mother). These assessments formed part of the outcome indices covering the child’s physical, social–emotional, learning and overall development. In addition, the respondents’ assessments relating

27

Page 35: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

to five aspects of the children’s social–emotional development (sub-scales of the SDQ) are presented. The latter five assessments provided by teachers are summarised in Table 8.

Overall, the primary caregiver in married biological families tended to provide the most favourable assessments of children’s wellbeing, while sole mothers tended to provide the least favourable assessments. These differences were statistically significant with the following exceptions: the SDQ sub-scale for prosocial behaviour in Wave 1 and the outcome index for physical development in Wave 2. Across the three waves, the PPVT average scores for children in sole-mother families were also significantly lower than those for children in married biological families, with the difference being most marked in Wave 3.

Table 7: Child outcomes by family form, primary caregiver reports, Waves 1 to 3

Married families Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

Wave 1PPVT score a 64.5 64.3 63.2 *Outcome indices

Physical development index a 100.4 98.9 98.4 **Social–emotional development index a 101.2 98.6 ** 96.8 ***Learning index a 101.4 100.5 98.1 ***Overall development index a 101.4 99.0 ** 96.8 ***

SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesProsocial behaviour a 7.8 7.6 7.6Peer problems b 1.6 1.8 * 2.1 ***Emotional problems b 1.6 1.8 2.1 ***Hyperactivity problems b 3.4 3.7 4.0 ***Conduct problems b, c 2.3 3.1 *** 3.2 ***

Wave 2PPVT score a 73.8 73.9 73.0 *Outcome indices

Physical development index a 100.3 98.9 99.4Social–emotional development index a 101.2 98.8 ** 96.0 ***Learning index a 101.2 99.5 * 97.2 ***Overall development index a 101.3 98.6 ** 96.4 ***

SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesProsocial behaviour a 8.3 8.0 7.9 **Peer problems b 1.4 1.8 ** 2.1 ***Emotional problems b 1.6 1.8 2.2 ***Hyperactivity problems b 3.2 3.6 4.0 ***Conduct problems b, c 1.4 1.7 * 2.1 ***

Wave 3PPVT score 78.2 78.3 77.1 *Outcome indices

Physical development index a 100.7 99.0 * 97.6 ***Social–emotional development index a 101.2 99.2 * 95.5 ***Learning index a 101.0 99.0 * 97.1 ***Overall development index a 101.3 98.7 ** 95.4 ***

SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesProsocial behaviour a 8.3 8.5 8.0 *Peer problems b 1.4 1.8 ** 2.1 ***Emotional problems b 1.5 1.8 * 2.4 ***Hyperactivity problems b 3.1 3.4 4.1 ***Conduct problems b, c 1.2 1.6 ** 1.8 ***

28

Page 36: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Note: a Higher score = better outcome. b Higher score = worse outcome. c Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were compared with married parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

As shown in Table 8, the teachers also tended to assess the developmental progress of children in sole-mother families less favourably than that of children who were living in married biological families. The differences in assessment were statistically significant for four of the five measures examined in each survey wave. The exception concerned the peer problems measure, where differences in ratings remained non-significant in each survey wave.

Table 8: Child SDQ outcomes by unchanged family form, teacher reports, Waves 1 to 3

Married families Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

Wave 1Prosocial behaviour a 7.3 7.2 6.9 **Peer problems b 2.5 2.7 3.0Emotional problems b 1.3 1.5 1.6 **Hyperactivity problems b 2.9 3.3 3.6 ***Conduct problems b, c 1.7 1.9 2.2 ***

Wave 2Prosocial behaviour a 7.7 7.5 6.9 ***Peer problems b 1.2 1.4 1.9Emotional problems b 1.2 1.4 1.7 **Hyperactivity problems b 2.7 2.8 3.8 ***Conduct problems b, c 0.7 0.9 1.4 ***

Wave 3Prosocial behaviour a 7.7 7.6 6.9 ***Peer problems b 1.2 1.8 2.0Emotional problems b 1.1 1.6 * 1.9 ***Hyperactivity problems b 2.7 3.0 3.8 ***Conduct problems b, c 0.7 0.9 1.4 ***

Note: a Higher score = better outcome. b Higher score = worse outcome. c Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were compared with married parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

The children who were living with cohabiting biological parents fared less well in some domains than their counterparts whose parents were married, according to the primary caregivers’ assessments (Table 7). Differences were statistically significant for four measures in Wave 1 (social–emotional and overall development indices and the SDQ sub-scales concerning peer problems and conduct problems), five measures in Wave 2 (social–emotional, learning and overall development indices, and the peer problems and conduct problems sub-scales), and seven measures in Wave 3 (all four development indices, along with the sub-scales covering peer, emotional and conduct problems). However, the PPVT scores of the children in cohabiting and married families did not differ significantly in any of the three survey waves. While teachers’ assessments appeared to be slightly less favourable for children in cohabiting than married biological families, only one difference was statistically significant (emotional problems in Wave 3) (Table 8). Therefore, by Wave 3, the children with married parents

29

Page 37: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

appeared to outperform the children with cohabiting parents on eight measures, seven of which were based on the primary caregivers’ assessments.

In summary, both the primary caregivers’ and teachers’ reports suggest that children in sole-mother families were progressing less well in virtually all measures in each survey wave. Especially in Wave 3, the PPVT measure of receptive language skills administered to the children themselves also suggests that those in sole-mother families were not doing as well as those living with their married parents.

In addition, the children in cohabiting biological families were faring less well than those in married families in some areas, although this trend derived almost exclusively from the assessments provided by the primary caregivers and was not apparent in the children’s PPVT scores or from most teacher assessments. Overall, the children in married biological families outperformed those in cohabiting biological families on an increasing number of measures over the four-year period (with significant differences emerging for four measures in Wave 1, five in Wave 2, and eight in Wave 3).

On the whole, children from cohabiting families with two biological parents seemed to be progressing better than those in sole-mother families.16 In Wave 1, children in cohabiting families appeared to fare significantly better than those in sole-mother families in the following areas: social–emotional developmental progress, learning, and peer relationships, as reported by the primary caregiver. In Wave 2, apparent superior progress of children in cohabiting families emerged in relation to the primary caregivers’ assessments of learning; social–emotional and overall developmental progress; peer relationships; emotional expression and conduct; and in relation to teachers’ assessments of prosocial behaviour, peer relationships, level of hyperactivity and conduct issues. In Wave 3, the children in cohabiting families continued to outperform those in sole-mother families, as reflected in: (a) parents’ assessments of learning, social–emotional and overall developmental indices and all SDQ sub-scales except the peer problems measure; (b) teachers’ assessments of three of the five SDQ sub-scales; and (c) the children’s PPVT test scores. In other words, children living with their cohabiting parents appeared to be progressing better than those in sole-mother families, with differences being more apparent in Waves 2 and 3 than in Wave 1. This suggests a process of divergence over the four-year period.

Explanations for differences in children’s wellbeing: Multivariate analysisIf differences in child wellbeing are apparent, to what extent can they be explained by any observed systematic differences in child and family characteristics, or parenting practices and parental wellbeing?

The above analysis suggests that the developmental progress of children who were living with both married biological parents tended to be superior to that of children in the other two groups, especially those living with their sole mother. Furthermore, it seems that children who were living with both biological parents tended to outperform those in sole-mother families across a range of areas, especially in Waves 2 and 3. Overall, differences were apparent between the children in sole-mother and married families in most areas examined in all survey waves, while those between children with married and cohabiting parents appeared to become more pronounced over the four-year period.

However, these three groups of children differed systematically in terms of their family environments, which appeared to be most favourable for children in married parent families and least for those in sole-mother families. This appeared to relate to issues such as the families’ apparent financial circumstances, and the resident parents’ educational attainment level and apparent consistency in ensuring that their children behaved appropriately. In regard to these issues, the family environments of cohabiting families seemed to lie between the two other family types.

This section shows the extent to which such systematic variation in the family environments of the children in the three family forms explained their apparent differences in developmental

30

Page 38: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

progress. Once again, attention is directed to children who did not experience a change in family form across the survey waves. The ‘cross-sectional’ results (covering all children, regardless of whether they had experienced a change in family form during the four-year period) appear in Appendix Tables A8 to A11. These results were very similar to those outlined for the children who did not experience any change in family form.

As for the above-mentioned bivariate analysis, 15 measures of children’s developmental progress were examined: the PPVT administered to the child, nine measures (outcome indices and SDQ sub-scales) based on primary caregivers’ reports, and five SDQ sub-scale measures based on teachers’ reports. For each of these measures, two regressions were performed, with married families representing the reference group: base model—where the explanatory variables included the child’s characteristics (age, sex,

country of birth, Indigenous status, and presence of older siblings or younger siblings [assessed separately])

full model—where the explanatory variables included the child’s characteristics from the base model, along with demographic characteristics of the mother (age, education, country of birth, Indigenous status, and employment status); apparent financial circumstances of the family (experience of any of the six financial hardships and level of prosperity, as reported by the primary caregiver); mother’s self-efficacy as a parent; her self-assessed parenting practices; and the level of her emotional distress. The two measures of the quality of the inter-parental relationship were not included in this model, given that these measures were not applicable to sole mothers.

Results for the base model

The regression results relating to the base model (which controlled for the children’s characteristics only) are presented in Table 9, with the top panel focusing on the primary caregivers’ assessments and the child’s PPVT test scores, and the lower panel focusing on the teachers’ assessments. The full set of results appears in Appendix Tables A12 and A13.

When the children’s characteristics were controlled, those in cohabiting families appeared to be progressing less well than those in married families, according to four measures in Wave 1, five measures in Wave 2, and eight measures in Wave 3. On the other hand, the developmental progress of children in sole-mother families appeared to be worse across all 15 measures, with two exceptions: prosocial behaviour as assessed by teachers in Wave 1 and physical development (which only the primary caregiver has assessed) in Wave 2. These patterns of results were consistent with those based on the bivariate analysis discussed above.

31

Page 39: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Table 9: Regressions of child outcomes, base model

32

Page 40: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

PPVT score

Outcome indices (a) SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical development

index

Social–emotional

development index

Learning index

Overall development

indexProsocial behaviour

Peer problems

Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Conduct problems (a)

Primary caregivers’ reportsWave 1Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.469 –1.235 –2.376 ** –0.601 –2.102 * –0.151 0.249 * 0.148 0.248 0.834 ***

Sole-mother families –1.593 *** –1.79 * –4.277 *** –3.224 **

* –4.428 *** –0.213 0.447 *** 0.46 ** 0.69 *** 0.961 ***

Wave 2Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.124 –0.938 –2.405 ** –1.83 * –2.546 ** –0.219 0.344 ** 0.237 0.345 0.343 *

Sole-mother families –1.029 ** –0.43 –5.448 *** –4.095 **

* –4.806 *** –0.375 ** 0.675 *** 0.677 *** 0.683 *** 0.806 ***

Wave 3Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.121 –1.268 –2.053 * –2.198 * –2.547 ** 0.226 0.404 ** 0.316 0.41 * 0.366 *

Sole-mother families –0.983 * –3.35 *** –5.783 *** –3.346 **

* –5.703 *** –0.369 ** 0.703 *** 0.892 *** 0.926 *** 0.66 ***

Teachers’ reportsWave 1Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.031 0.075 0.186 0.226 0

Sole-mother families –0.301 0.462** 0.365** 0.500** 0 **

*Wave 2Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

33

Page 41: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

PPVT score

Outcome indices (a) SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical development

index

Social–emotional

development index

Learning index

Overall development

indexProsocial behaviour

Peer problems

Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Conduct problems (a)

Cohabiting families –0.185 0.175 0.145 –0.008 0

Sole-mother families –0.763*** 0.695*** 0.471** 0.940*** 0 **

*Wave 3Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.196 0.535** 0.484* 0.258 0

Sole-mother families –0.677*** 0.725*** 0.720*** 1.137*** 0 **

*

(a) These measures were only generated based on parents’ reports.

Note: Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents.. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

34

Page 42: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Results for full model

Table 10 presents the results of the regression analyses based on the full model, which, as noted above, controlled not only for children’s characteristics, but also for parental characteristics, apparent family financial circumstances, and mothers’ psychological distress and parenting practices.

For nine measures in Wave 1, five in Wave 2 and four in Wave 3, the differences in the progress between children with married parents and those with sole mothers were not statistically significant, according to primary caregivers’ assessments. Nevertheless, compared with the children with married parents, those in sole-mother families appeared to be progressing less well in relation to: (a) conduct in Wave 1, as assessed by the primary caregiver; (b) eight measures in Wave 2 (five assessed by the primary caregiver and three by a teacher); and (c) 11 measures in Wave 3 (six assessed by the primary caregiver and all five assessed by a teacher).

On the other hand, differences in the progress of children in cohabiting families and married families had largely disappeared. For parents’ reports, significant differences continued to exist for only one measure in Wave 1 and two measures in Wave 3. For teachers’ reports, no measures remained statistically significant in Waves 1 or 2 and only one measure was statistically significant in Wave 3. Specifically, compared with the children with married parents, those with cohabiting parents were reported by the primary caregiver to fare less well in Wave 1 only in terms of conduct. No significant differences between these groups emerged from primary caregivers’ reports in Wave 2. Furthermore, while they differed significantly on three measures in Wave 3 (PPVT score, primary caregivers’ assessments of prosocial behaviour, and teachers’ assessments of peer relationships), for two of these measures (the child’s PPVT score and the SDQ prosocial sub-scale, as assessed by the primary caregiver), the children with cohabiting parents appeared to be doing better than those with married parents, but worse in terms of peer relationships, according to the teachers.

Further analysis suggested that the better performance of children with cohabiting than married parents regarding prosocial behaviour (as assessed by the primary caregiver) and receptive language skills was not apparent when the demographic characteristics of both the children and mothers were controlled (see Appendix Tables A16 and A17 for results). Once parenting approaches were introduced, children with cohabiting parents were better in receptive language and prosocial behaviours. (The early analysis on parenting approaches shows that cohabiting parents were less consistent in their parenting than married parents.)

In summary, the measures of family environment (including parenting practices) appeared to explain most of the Wave 1 developmental progress differences between the children in married families on the one hand, and those in cohabiting and lone-parent families on the other. However, these control measures were increasingly ‘unable’ to explain differences between those living with married parents and those living with a sole mother across the four-year period. Few differences were apparent between those with married and cohabiting parents net of the family environment factors than that assessed, and in Wave 3, the children with cohabiting parents appeared to be doing better, net of the factors, in receptive language skills and prosocial behaviour, although the teachers’ assessments of their peer relations suggest that they were not doing as well as those in married families.

35

Page 43: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

Table 10: Regressions of child outcomes, full model

36

Page 44: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

PPVT score

Outcome indices (a) SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical development

index

Social–emotional

development index

Learning index

Overall development

indexProsocial behaviour

Peer problems

Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Conduct problems a

Primary caregivers’ reportsWave 1Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.159 –0.496 –0.666 0.434 –0.425 –0.056 0.054 –0.048 –0.098 0.570 ***

Sole-mother families –0.515 0.240 –1.012 –1.378 –1.076 –0.045 0.070 0.030 0.106 0.498 **

Wave 2Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.486 0.032 –0.913 –0.413 –0.721 –0.098 0.186 0.066 0.079 0.165

Sole-mother families –0.056 1.963 ** –1.765 ** –1.462 * –0.672 –0.151 0.267 * 0.158 0.095 0.428 ***

Wave 3Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.777 * –0.042 –0.540 –0.595 –0.546 0.357 ** 0.203 0.152 0.123 0.168

Sole-mother families –0.112 –0.641 –2.578 *** –0.916 –1.876 * –0.201 0.286 * 0.462 ** 0.350 * 0.232 *

Teachers’ reportsWave 1Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.085 –0.021 0.064 0.01 –0.085

Sole-mother families –0.096 0.242 0.14 0.096 0.261

Wave 2Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

37

Page 45: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Parental social marital status and children’s wellbeing

PPVT score

Outcome indices (a) SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical development

index

Social–emotional

development index

Learning index

Overall development

indexProsocial behaviour

Peer problems

Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Conduct problems a

Cohabiting families –0.068 0.101 0

7 –0.203 0.051

Sole-mother families –0.453* 0.496 *** 0

8 0.382 0.398**

Wave 3Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.034 0.437 * 0

6 0.004 0.057

Sole-mother families –0.388* 0.524 ** 0

1 * 0.631* 0.431*

(a) These measures were only generated based on parents’ reports.

Note: Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

38

Page 46: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Significant predictors

The results of the multivariate analysis (presented in Appendix Tables A14 and A15) suggest that the boys were progressing less well than the girls, net of all measures in the full model. These patterns were consistent across the survey waves, though fewer differences were statistically significant in Wave 3 than in the previous two waves. Children whose mother had a degree or higher qualification appeared to be progressing significantly better than other children across most measures for all three waves. Not surprisingly, better family financial circumstances were associated with superior child progress: in each survey wave, the children in families with more favourable financial circumstances (as measured by one or both indicators) appeared to be significantly outperforming other children in terms of around half the measures. It is also not surprising that mothers’ level of emotional distress was negatively associated with their child’s developmental progress. Specifically, lower psychological distress of mothers was linked with more superior child development across 11 measures in Wave 1, 10 measures in Wave 2, and seven in Wave 3.

Mothers’ perceptions about themselves as parents were also significantly associated with the children’s developmental progress. In Wave 1, for seven measures, the more confident that mothers felt as parents, the better their child fared in terms of those seven measures. However, this association appeared to weaken as the child matured, for significant differences emerged for only three measures in Waves 2 and 3. It is important to point out that any causal connection between mothers’ confidence as parents and their children’s developmental progress may be bi-directional. That is, less confident mothers may be less competent in handling their children’s behaviour, which in turn may compromise their children’s developmental progress, while at the same time, relatively poor developmental progress may also make mothers feel less confident than otherwise.

Mothers’ self-reported parenting practices were also significantly associated with children’s developmental progress across all three waves. Specifically, the children appeared to fare better where mothers said that they frequently displayed warmth, and worse where their mothers reported a hostile approach to parenting. In addition, children of mothers who reported greater use of reasoning and consistency in parenting tended to fare better than other children. However, of these four measures of parenting behaviour (warmth, hostility, consistency and reasoning), those concerning hostility and consistency were more strongly linked with children’s developmental progress. Specifically, mothers’ reported warmth was statistically significant for only two to five developmental progress measures in each wave, while their reported use of reasoning was statistically significant for one to three measures. In contrast, statistically significant relationships emerged between most measures of children’s developmental progress and mothers’ reports regarding their tendency to display ‘hostile parenting’ and consistency.

3.4 Cohabitation pathways and child wellbeingOf all the children in cohabiting families in the initial survey wave, to what extent, if at all, does their wellbeing vary (at the outset and subsequently) according to whether their parents marry, separate or continue to cohabit over the four-year period?The final set of analyses focused on three groups of children who were living with their cohabiting parents in Wave 1: those who continued to experience this arrangement (n = 223), those whose parents had married by Wave 3 (n = 54) and those whose parents had separated by Wave 3, resulting in the children living with their sole mother (n = 49). Given the small number of children who experienced either of the transitions, the results outlined in this section may best be treated as a means of generating hypotheses for subsequent research.

It was predicted that the cohabiting parents who had married by Wave 3 would have provided the most favourable assessments of their inter-parental relationship in Wave 1, while those who

39

Page 47: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

had separated by Wave 3 would have the least favourable assessments. Table 11 presents the mean scores of mothers and fathers in the three groups regarding the two measures tapping the quality of their inter-parental relationships, along with their mean scores regarding psychological distress. The analyses compared the means for married parents with those for each of the other two groups, taken separately.

As predicted, from the perspective of both fathers and mothers, those who subsequently married had significantly better inter-parental relationships in Wave 1 than those who continued to cohabit and those who separated. This was apparent for each of the two measures tapping relationship quality (happiness in the relationship and argumentativeness). In addition, among the couples who married, the fathers appeared to be less distressed in Wave 1 than those who continued to cohabit. Although the mean distress score of fathers who married was also lower than that of fathers who separated, this difference was not statistically significant.

Table 11: Parents who were cohabiting in Wave 1: Parental psychological distress and the quality of inter-parental relationships in Wave 1, by parental cohabitation pathways by Wave 3

Parental psychological distress and relationships, Wave 1

Family types, Wave 3

Married Continued cohabiting

Separated, with sole mothers

Mothers’ reportsPsychological distress (Kessler 6) a 1.59 1.69 1.77Degree of happiness in relationship with the partner b 5.62 4.96 *** 4.13 ***

Argumentative relationship scale c 1.99 2.19 * 2.43 ***Fathers’ reports

Psychological distress (Kessler 6) a 1.47 1.67 * 1.68Degree of happiness in relationship with the partner b 5.47 4.92 ** 4.69 **

Argumentative relationship scale c 1.96 2.23 ** 2.37 **Number of observations 54 223 49

Note: a Scale of 1 to 5, higher scores = higher distress. b Scale of 1 to 7, 1 = extremely unhappy, 7 = perfectly happy. c Scale of 1 to 5, higher scores = more prone to argument. The parents represented here were the biological parents of the study child. The reference group comprised Wave 1 cohabiting parents who were married by Wave 3. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

Table 12 presents the mean scores for all 15 measures of developmental progress and highlights differences that were statistically significant. Once again, the scores for the children whose parents married were compared (separately) with those for children whose parents continued to cohabit or were separated (with the child living with the sole mother).

None of the differences in mean scores of children whose parents eventually married or continued to cohabit differed significantly. On face value, the direction of most results would suggest that the children whose parents separated were not faring as well as the children whose parents married. However, in Wave 1, none of the differences reached the conventional 5 per cent level of statistical significance, although two approached significance (p < 0.05)—the caregivers’ assessment of their child’s overall developmental progress and emotional problems. By Wave 3, six of the 15 mean scores for children in these two groups differed significantly, with results suggesting poorer progress for the children who were living with their sole mothers. The children who were living with their sole mothers appeared to be doing worse in terms of: overall social–emotional progress the caregivers’ assessments of prosocial behaviour the caregivers’ and teachers’ assessments of emotional problems the teachers’ assessments of peer problems and conduct problems.

40

Page 48: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

In summary, the cohabiting parents who subsequently married tended to evaluate the quality of their relationship more favourably than those who continued to cohabit and those who had separated (with the child living with the sole mother), though none of the differences between the children with married parents and those whose parents continued to cohabit were statistically significant. While the Wave 1 mean scores of the children who ended up living with their sole mother tended to be lower than those of children whose parents were married, the differences did not reach statistical significance (though two approached significance). By Wave 3, the children who were living with their sole mothers appeared to be progressing less well according to six of the 15 measures, but better according to one. Some of these measures were based on the primary caregivers’ reports and some on the teachers’ reports.

41

Page 49: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table 12: Cohabiting families in Wave 1: Children’s developmental progress, by parental cohabitation pathways by Wave 3

Married Continued cohabiting

Separated, with sole mothers

Wave 1PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9Primary caregiver’s assessments

Outcome indicesPhysical development index a 101.01 99.34 98.0Social–emotional development index a 100.75 99.18 97.3Learning index a 100.15 100.17 98.6Overall development index a 100.82 99.28 97.0 #

SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesProsocial behaviour a 7.89 7.53 7.3Peer problems b 1.56 1.73 1.9Emotional problems b 1.37 1.71 2.0 #Hyperactivity problems b 3.89 3.60 3.8Conduct problems b, c 2.89 3.02 3.2

Teacher’s assessmentsProsocial behaviour a 7.33 7.40 7.1Peer problems b 2.69 2.72 3.0Emotional problems b 1.17 1.45 1.3Hyperactivity problems b 3.30 3.20 3.4Conduct problems b, c 2.00 1.82 2.5

Wave 3PPVT a score 78.36 78.34 77.60Primary caregiver’s assessments

Outcome indicesPhysical development index a 99.59 99.27 98.69Social–emotional development index a 101.51 99.59 96.03 *Learning index a 99.71 99.36 99.02Overall development index a 100.76 99.19 97.34

SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesProsocial behaviour a 8.69 8.42 7.81 *Peer problems b 1.59 1.72 2.00Emotional problems b 1.17 1.72 2.17 **Hyperactivity problems b 3.55 3.27 3.86Conduct problems b, c 1.43 1.62 1.95

Teacher’s assessmentsProsocial behaviour a 7.39 7.54 6.53Peer problems b 1.33 1.69 2.21 *Emotional problems b 1.04 1.49 1.89 *Hyperactivity problems b 3.28 2.76 4.18Conduct problems b, c 0.89 0.96 2.08 *

Number of respondents 54 223 49

42

Page 50: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Note: a Higher score = better outcome. b Higher score = worse outcome. c Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. All children represented here were living with biological parents only. The reference group comprised Wave 1 cohabiting parents who were married by Wave 3. # p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

4 Summary and discussionAs in many Western countries, the rise in cohabitation in Australia represents one of the most striking of the many changes to family life that have emerged since the 1970s. At first, the rise largely represented a new period between leaving the parental home and marrying. However, cohabitation is increasingly being used as a context for childbearing, although the vast majority of couples postpone childbearing until they are married.

The increase in cohabitation rates has fuelled and has been influenced by a growing social acceptance of this choice in lifestyle, and policies have changed to remove areas of discrimination in the opportunities available to cohabiting and married families. As Cherlin (2004) observed, marriage has become less institutionalised while cohabitation has become increasingly institutionalised.

Nevertheless, Australia does not appear to have reached a situation whereby cohabitation and marriage are indistinguishable (see Kiernan 2001). For example, as noted above, most people wait until they are married before they have children, and there is evidence that cohabiting couples are more likely to separate than married couples. However, much of this research has not taken into account the presence or absence of children. While many studies over the years have examined differences in the wellbeing of children in different family forms, very few have taken into account differences in the wellbeing of children between those whose biological parents are married to each other or those whose parents are cohabiting. The few studies that have been conducted have, for the most part, suggested that the children with biological married parents had higher wellbeing than those whose biological parents were cohabiting (for example, Artis 2007; Goodman & Greaves 2010; Wu et al. 2008). In fact, one study suggested that the latter group was faring no better than children who were living in sole-mother families (and many studies suggest that children in sole-mother families fare relatively poorly across a range of measures) (Brown 2004).

However, a less favourable picture for those in cohabiting rather than married families has not been apparent across all measures examined or in all studies. For example, in a Canadian study of children aged between 0 and 15 years whose parents had been cohabiting or married, the children with cohabiting parents appeared to be progressing better than those with married parents in one area (display of emotional problems), poorer in another (prosocial behaviour), and did not differ in a third area examined (conduct disorders) (Wu et al. 2008).

To date, the reasons for children who live with cohabiting biological parents faring worse than those who live with married parents remain uncertain, although the various studies outlined in this report tend to suggest that the poorer financial resources of cohabiting families contribute to poorer developmental progress. In fact, one study suggested that children in cohabiting families would display superior cognitive skills were it not for their families’ more difficult circumstances, associated with such factors as fathers’ education and mothers’ earnings (Hofferth 2006). Wu and colleagues (2008), on the other hand, were unable to explain the poorer social–emotional behaviour of children with cohabiting parents in terms of the joint effects of a range of circumstances covering characteristics of child and parent, financial resources, and aspects of family functioning and parental nurturance.

All of the above-mentioned research was conducted in either the United States or Canada. The present study focused on the developmental progress of young Australian children who were living with their cohabiting or married biological parents or with their sole mother over a four-year period, with assessments being made at the start, middle and end of this period. The present

43

Page 51: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

study was designed to address several research questions. These and their related findings are outlined below.17

4.1 Research questions and findingsHow common and how stable are these family forms over a four-year period?Not surprisingly, most of the children were living with both parents who were married to each other (here called ‘married families’) and sole-mother families were more commonly represented than families in which the child lived with both biological parents who were cohabiting (here called ‘cohabiting families’). The cohabiting families were as common as, if not more common than, stepfamilies (with rates varying according to survey wave).

Most married, cohabiting and sole-mother families in Wave 1 were in the same family form in Wave 3, with levels of stability being considerably greater for married families than for the other two forms. Indeed, the level of stability of cohabiting and sole-mother families was very similar (63 to 65 per cent were in the same family form in Wave 3). However, this assessment treated change in social marital status (from cohabitation to marriage) as a form of instability. If stability is measured in terms of whether families separated or re-partnered, then the married families continue to be the most stable and the sole-mother families are the least stable.

Where change in family form occurs, what are their most likely pathways?Although 93 per cent of children who lived in married families did not experience a change in family form, those who did experience a change tended to be living with their sole mother by Wave 3. Children in cohabiting families were just as likely to experience parental marriage as to experience parental separation. Not surprisingly, any change for those in sole-mother families tended to involve the introduction of a stepfather, although a few experienced their parents moving in together.

To what extent do these family forms differ in terms of the financial circumstances of the family, and selected socio-demographic characteristics of the parents and children, indicators of parenting practices and parental wellbeing?Systematic differences were evident in the experiences of children in the three family forms in relation to these matters. Similar results were apparent for each survey wave. For example, compared with married parents, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were younger, more likely to identify as Indigenous, had a lower level of education, and were less likely to be employed. Cohabiting families and sole-mother families were more likely than married families to experience financial hardships and the parents tended to have less favourable views about their financial situation. It also seems that the two former groups of parents were less consistent in their parenting than was the case for married parents. Finally, compared with married parents, those who were cohabiting tended to report poorer inter-parental relationship quality and higher emotional distress. In other words, some of the characteristics of cohabiting families appeared to lie between those of married families and sole-mother families. For example, the proportion of cohabiting families experiencing financial hardship was higher than that of married families but lower than that of sole-mother families.

To what extent do the children in the three family forms differ in terms of their developmental progress?To what extent, if at all, do any differences in child wellbeing between the groups diverge or diminish over the four-year period?Compared with children with married parents, those living with their sole mother appeared to be progressing less well in terms of receptive language skills and in terms of social–emotional,

44

Page 52: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

learning and physical development, as reported by their primary caregiver. Teachers, likewise, tended to provide less favourable assessments of the children in sole-mother families than those in married families. These trends were consistent across the survey waves.

The children with cohabiting parents appeared to be progressing less well than those with married parents in some areas only. The relative underperformance of children of cohabiting parents was not apparent in the test scores of receptive language skills administered to the children and emerged in only one of the teachers’ assessments (emotional problems in Wave 3). Based on the primary caregivers’ reports, the children with cohabiting parents were not doing as well as those with married parents in terms of overall social–emotional development and general development (derived from physical, social–emotional and learning indices), peer relationships and conduct problems, with these trends being apparent in all three survey waves.

It should be pointed out that, while children with cohabiting parents appeared to fare less well than those with married parents, they were doing better than those in sole-mother families, based on primary caregivers’ reports. This trend was apparent in all three waves.

If differences in child wellbeing are apparent, to what extent can they be explained by any observed systematic differences in child and family characteristics or parenting practices and parental wellbeing?The above-noted differences between the groups regarding family environment appeared to explain most of the variations between the groups in the initial survey wave. However, by Wave 3, the poorer progress of children in sole-mother families compared to married families could not be fully explained by family characteristics. This is not surprising, given that peer and school influences will strengthen as the children mature. Edwards (2005), for instance, has shown that children in poorer neighbourhoods tend to progress less well in social–emotional and learning domains.

The family-related factors examined in this research accounted for virtually all of the differences apparent between children with married parents and those with cohabiting parents. However, one surprising trend emerged. The bivariate analysis yielded no significant differences between these two groups in scores on the receptive language measure and the primary caregivers’ assessments of the children’s prosocial behaviour. The multivariate analysis, on the other hand, suggested that the children of cohabiting parents would have displayed superior performance in these areas were it not for their differential family environment experiences. This may reflect differences between the two types of families in characteristics that could not be captured in this study. The greater apparent consistency of married mothers than sole mothers also appeared to contribute to the differences in the developmental progress of children in these two family forms.

Other factors explaining other differences in the developmental progress of children with married parents and those with cohabiting parents and sole mothers included the family’s financial circumstances, and mothers’ educational attainment, emotional distress, and confidence in parenting. Again, building effective parenting capacity should help boost mothers’ confidence as parents. As noted earlier, mothers’ confidence may facilitate the developmental progress of their children, while children’s poor behaviour may also undermine mothers’ confidence. Regardless of the direction of these trends, effective parenting skills should promote mothers’ confidence directly and also indirectly, through improving the children’s behaviour. The results also suggest that measures that improve mothers’ educational attainment and financial circumstances would also narrow the gap in the developmental progress of children in these three family forms.

Of all children in cohabiting families in the initial survey wave, to what extent, if at all, does their wellbeing vary (at the outset and subsequently) according to whether their parents marry, separate or continue to cohabit over the four-year period?

45

Page 53: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

The parents who married tended to evaluate the quality of their relationship more favourably than those who continued to cohabit and those who separated (with the child living with the sole mother). While the Wave 1 mean scores of the children who ended up living with their sole mother tended to be lower than those of children whose parents were married by Wave 3, the differences did not reach statistical significance (with two approaching significance). By Wave 3, the children who were living with their sole mothers appeared to be progressing less well according to six of the 15 measures, but better according to one. Some of these measures were based on the primary caregivers’ reports and some on the teachers’ reports. These findings should be interpreted with caution given that the analysis was based on small sample size.

4.2 Final wordCurrently, children with cohabiting parents are more likely to experience parental separation, and there is some evidence to suggest that those whose parents continue to cohabit fall behind those whose parents remain married. But as Australia progresses towards cohabitation becoming more normative, any differences in the children’s development are likely to diminish further. In the meantime, the two groups of families appear to differ in various ways and the children in cohabiting families do not appear to be faring as well as those in married families in some areas. Children in sole-mother families fare much worse. This report also shows that family characteristics such as socioeconomic circumstances, the quality of parental relationship, and parenting practices are important factors explaining differences between the groups. In other words, the results outlined suggest that policies that manage to effectively address such factors (for example, financial disadvantage, inadequate parenting skills and problematic inter-parental relationships) would represent important means of enhancing children’s wellbeing across the family forms.

46

Page 54: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Appendix: Measures used and tables

Parenting practice measuresParental warmth is based on six items: How often do you express affection by hugging, kissing and holding this child? How often do you hug or hold this child for no particular reason? How often do you tell this child how happy he/she makes you? How often do you have warm, close times together with this child? How often do you enjoy listening to this child and doing things with him/her? How often do you feel close to this child both when he/she is happy and when he/she is

upset?

Four items formed the hostile parenting scale: Of all the times that you talk to this child about his/her behaviour, how often is this praise? Of all the times that you talk to this child about his/her behaviour, how often is this

disapproval? How often are you angry when you punish this child? How often do you feel you are having problems managing this child in general?Inductive reasoning includes two items: How often do you explain to this child why he/she is being corrected? How often do you talk it over and reason with this child when he/she misbehaves?

The consistency parenting scale consists of five items: When you give this child an instruction or make a request to do something, how often do you

make sure that he/she does it? If you tell this child he/she will get punished if he/she doesn’t stop doing something, but

he/she keeps doing it, how often will you punish him/her? How often does this child get away with things that you feel should have been punished? How often is this child able to get out of punishment when he/she sets his/her mind to it? When you discipline this child, how often does he/she ignore the punishment?

Response options for all these parenting practice items are from 1 ‘never/almost never’ to 5 ‘always/almost always’. For each scale, the mean of responses to the items involved for each parent is the score of the scale.

Argumentative relationship scaleThis scale includes five items: Thinking about any disagreements (between you and your partner):

– how often do you and your partner disagree about basic child-rearing issues?– how often is your conversation awkward or stressed?– how often do you disagree?– how often is there anger or hostility between you?

How often do you have arguments with your partner that end up with people pushing, hitting, kicking or shoving?

Response options range from 1 to 5 (respectively indicating Never, Rarely, Sometimes, Often, Always). The mean of the responses for each parent is derived.

47

Page 55: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Appendix figuresFigure A1: Proportions of families having experienced one or more listed financial hardships by

family type, as reported by primary carers, Waves 1 to 3

Note: Family type can change between waves, and families across the three waves are not the same families.

Accessible long description of Figure A1

WAVE FAMILY TYPEProportions of families having experienced one or more listed financial hardships

1 MARRIED FAMILIES 24%

1 COHABITING FAMILIES 44%

1 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES 61%

2 MARRIED FAMILIES 14%

2 COHABITING FAMILIES 30%

2 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES 44%

3 MARRIED FAMILIES 13%

3 COHABITING FAMILIES 21%

3 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES 45%

48

Page 56: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Figure A2: Ratings of financial situation by family type, as reported by primary carers, Waves 1 to 3

Note: Families across the three waves are not the same families.

Accessible long description of Figure A2

WAVE FAMILY TYPE PRIMARY CARERS’ RATINGS OF FINANCIAL SITUATION

1 MARRIED FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 79% ‘GETTING BY’ 31% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 3%

1 COHABITING FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 60% ‘GETTING BY’ 36% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 4%

1 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 40% ‘GETTING BY’ 52% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 8%

2 MARRIED FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 79% ‘GETTING BY’ 20% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 1%

2 COHABITING FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 70% ‘GETTING BY’ 28% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 2%

2 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 46% ‘GETTING BY’ 47% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 7%

3 MARRIED FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 76% ‘GETTING BY’ 22% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 2%

3 COHABITING FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 68% ‘GETTING BY’ 30% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 3%

3 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES‘COMFORTABLE TO PROSPEROUS’ 43% ‘GETTING BY’ 48% ‘POOR OR VERY POOR 10%

49

Page 57: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Figure A3: Mothers’ ratings of self as a parent compared with other parents by family type, Waves 1 to 3

Note: Families across the three waves are not the same.

Accessible long description of Figure A3

WAVE FAMILY TYPE Mothers’ ratings of self as a parent

1 MARRIED FAMILIES‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 66%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 32%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 2%

1 COHABITING FAMILIES‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 62%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 35%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 3%

1 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 58%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 36%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 7%

2 MARRIED FAMILIES‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 72%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 27%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 1%

2 COHABITING FAMILIES‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 68%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 31%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 1%

2 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 67%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 31%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 2%

3 MARRIED FAMILIES‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 63%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 34%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 2%

3 COHABITING FAMILIES‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 56%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 39%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 4%

3 SOLE-MOTHER FAMILIES‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 53%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 41%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 6%

50

Page 58: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

51

Page 59: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Figure A4: Fathers’ ratings of self as a parent compared with other parents by family type, Waves 1 to 3

Note: Families across the waves are not the same families.

Accessible long description of Figure A4

WAVE FAMILY FORM FATHERS’ RATINGS OF SELF AS A PARENT

1 MARRIED FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 68%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 29%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 3%

1 COHABITING FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 52%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 44%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 5%

2 MARRIED FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 70%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 28%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 2%

2 COHABITING FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 61%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 34%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 4%

3 MARRIED FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 66%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 31%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 3%

3 COHABITING FATHERS‘A VERY GOOD OR BETTER THAN AVERAGE PARENT’ 56%

‘AN AVERAGE PARENT’ 40%

‘NOT VERY GOOD’ OR ‘HAD SOME TROUBLE BEING A PARENT’ 4%

52

Page 60: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Appendix tablesTable A1: Sample attritions for three family types at Wave 1, K cohort

Family type at Wave 1Two biological, married parents

Two biological, cohabiting parents Sole mother

NumberParticipated in three waves 3263 327 472Missing at Wave 2 only 86 16 25Missing at Wave 3 only 150 39 64Missing at both Waves 2 & 3 203 54 95No. of observations 3702 436 656

%Participated in three waves 88.1 75.0 72.0Missing at Wave 2 only 2.3 3.7 3.8Missing at Wave 3 only 4.1 8.9 9.8Missing at both Waves 2 & 3 5.5 12.4 14.5Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Note: Percentages may not total 100.0% due to rounding.

53

Page 61: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A2: Children’s characteristics by family types, waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis)

54

Page 62: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3

Married families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

s

Married

families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

s

Married

families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother families

Age: mean 4.8 4.8 4.8 6.9 6.9 6.9 *

* 8.9 8.9 8.9 **

% % %Sex

Boy 51.0 52.2 52.6

51.7 52 50.

251.

7 51.4 48.9

Girl 49.0 47.8 47.4

48.3 47 49.

848.

3 48.6 51.1

Total 100.0 100.0

100.0

100.0

1000

100.0

100.0

100.0

100.0

Country of birth

Other country 5.2 1.5*

* 1.2***

5.5 1 ** 2.0 *

* 5.3 1.0** 2.5*

Australia 94.8 98.6 98.

894.

5 98 98.0

94.7 99.0 97.

5

Total 100.0 100.0

100.0

100.0

1000

100.0

100.0

100.0

100.0

Indigenous status

No 98.3 89.7***

91.7

***

98.5 89

***

92.9

***

98.4 91.2

***

92.2

***

Yes 1.7 10.3 8.4 1.6 10 7.2 1.6 8.8 7.8

Total 100.0 100.0

100.0

100.0

1000

100.0

100.0

100.0

100.0

Language at home

Other 15.9 5.3***

11.3

**

16.8 6

***

12.1 * 16.

1 7.8**10.8

**

English 84.1 94.8 88.7

83.2 94 87.

983.

9 92.2 89.2

Total 100.0 100.0

100.0

100.0

1000

100.0

100.0

100.0

100.0

Any older siblings in household

No 39.8 43.6 42.9

40.3 47 * 44.

040.

5 49.0* 44.7

Yes 60.2 56.4 57.1

59.7 52 56.

159.

5 51.0 55.3

Total 100.0 100.0

100.0

100.0

1000

100.0

100.0

100.0

100.0

Any younger siblings in household

No 51.1 50.5 67.7

***

46.6 44 60.

9

***

44.9 43.9 55.

4

***

Yes 48.9 49.5 32.3

53.4 55 39.

155.

1 56.1 44.6

55

Page 63: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3

Married families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

s

Married

families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

s

Married

families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother families

Total 100.0 100.0

100.0

100.0

1000

100.0

100.0

100.0

100.0

No. of observations 3702 436 65

6333

3 304 616

3164 251 575

Note: The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Children in cohabiting and sole-mother families were compared with those in married families. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

56

Page 64: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A3: Characteristics of fathers and mothers by family type, waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis)

57

Page 65: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3Marrie

d familie

s

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

s

Married

families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

s

Married

families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

sMothers’ characteristics

Age 35.3 33.2

***

32.5

***

37.3

35.0

***

35.3

***

39.2

37.6

***

37.6

***

% % %Education

Degree or higher 29.2 11

4

***

112

***

29.3

109

***

14.3

***

30.1

124

***

14.4

***

Other post-school qualification

36.2 393

378

39.7

387

45.1 * 41.

9437

48.7

**

No qualification 34.6 49

3

***

510

***

31.0

505

***

40.6

***

28.1

439

***

36.9

***

Total 100.0

100 100 100.0

100 100.0

100.0

100 100.0

Country of birth

Other country 28.7 193

***

187

***

29.7

173

***

21.6

***

29.3

200 * 19.

8

***

Australia 71.3 807

***

813

***

70.3

827

***

78.4

***

70.7

800 * 80.

2

***

Total 100.0

100 100 100.0

100 100.0

100.0

100 100.0

Indigenous status

No 99.0 926

***

933

***

99.0

924

***

94.5

***

99.0

952

***

93.6

***

Yes 1.1 7***

6***

1.0 7***

5.5***

1.0 4***

6.4***

Total 100.0

100 100 100.0

100 100.0

100.0

100 100.0

Employment status

Full time 15.6 164

113 * 19.

4153

16.7

23.3

196

20.8

Part time 43.5 344

***

297

***

47.0

421

38.1

***

51.0

439 * 39.

4

***

Not employed 40.9 493

**

591

***

33.7

426

**

45.2

***

25.7

364

***

39.8

***

Total 100.0

100 100 100.0

100 100.0

100.0

100 100.0

No. of respondents

3702 436 656 33

33 304 616

3164 251 57

5

58

Page 66: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3Marrie

d familie

s

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

s

Married

families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

s

Married

families

Cohabiting

families

Sole-mother familie

sFathers’ characteristics

Age 37.8 36.2

***

39.9

37.8

***

41.8

40.8

% % %Education

Degree or higher 29 11

7

***

29.4

120

***

30.5

134

***

Other post-school qualification

46 567

***

47.2

555 * 48.

1576

**

No qualification 24 31

6**

23.3

325

**

21.5

290 *

Total 1000

100 100.0

100 100.0

100

Country of birth

Other country 29 211

**

30.3

211

**

29.7

214 *

Australia 70 790

**

69.7

789

**

70.4

786 *

Total 1000

100 100.0

100 100.0

100

Indigenous status

No 99 932

***

99.3

936

***

99.3

949

***

Yes 0 6***

0.7 6***

0.7 5***

Total 1000

100 100.0

100 100.0

100

Employment status

Full time 88 752

***

88.8

766

***

90.6

811

***

Part time 6 8 * 6.0 9 * 5.4 8 *

Not employed 5 161

***

5.2 141

***

4.1 103

***

Total 1000

100 100.0

100 100.0

100

No. of respondents

3702 436 33

33 304 3164 251

59

Page 67: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Note: The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were compared with married parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across variables.

60

Page 68: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A4: Mean scores of parenting practice scales of fathers and mothers by family type, Waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis)

Mothers FathersMarried mothers

Cohabiting mothers

Sole mothers

Married fathers

Cohabiting fathers

Wave 1Warm parenting a 4.43 4.43 4.49 * 4.09 4.04Hostile parenting b 2.17 2.26 ** 2.24 * 2.28 2.36 *Use of reasoning c 4.25 4.22 4.28 3.99 3.93

Consistency of parenting d 4.07 3.88 *** 3.84 **

* 3.98 3.78 ***

No. of observations 3702 435 655 3702 435Wave 2

Warm parenting a 4.45 4.45 4.46 4.13 4.09Hostile parenting b 2.16 2.24 2.23 * 2.08 2.12Use of reasoning c 4.23 4.20 4.27 3.96 3.88

Consistency of parenting d 4.15 3.98 *** 3.98 **

* 4.07 3.92 *

No. of observations 3333 303 615 3333 303Wave 3

Warm parenting a 4.34 4.27 4.35 4.06 4.07Hostile parenting b 2.13 2.17 2.20 2.17 2.18Use of reasoning c 4.14 4.09 4.14 3.91 4.03

Consistency of parenting d 4.20 3.97 *** 3.98 **

* 4.09 3.96 *

No. of observations 3164 250 575 3164 250

Note: All scores are on a five-point scale. a Higher scores = greater warmth. b Higher scores = higher hostility.c Higher scores = higher use of reasoning. d Higher scores = greater consistency. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were compared with married parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

61

Page 69: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A5: Mean scores of psychological distress scale and quality of parental relationship of fathers and mothers by family type, Waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis)

Mothers FathersMarried mothers

Cohabiting mothers

Sole mothers

Married fathers

Cohabiting fathers

Wave 1Psychological distress (Kessler 6) a 1.65 1.74 * 1.97 **

* 1.56 1.68 **

Degree of happiness in relationship with the partner b 5.32 4.83 **

* 5.42 5.01 ***

Argumentative relationship scale c 2.00 2.21 *** 1.98 2.20 **

*No. of observations 3672 429 651 2962 287

Wave 2Psychological distress (Kessler 6) a 1.50 1.58 1.86 **

* 1.53 1.59

Degree of happiness in relationship with the partner b 5.33 4.84 **

* 5.33 4.86 ***

Argumentative relationship scale c 1.84 2.06 *** 1.88 2.06 **

*No. of observations 3233 298 593 2673 179

Wave 3Psychological distress (Kessler 6) a 1.55 1.63 1.92 **

* 1.52 1.61

Degree of happiness in relationship with the partner b 5.25 4.76 **

* 5.43 4.88 ***

Argumentative relationship scale c 1.89 2.09 *** 1.85 2.02 *

No. of observations 2785 194 472 2351 138

Note: a Scale of 1 to 5, higher scores = higher distress. b Scale of 1–7, 1 = extremely unhappy, 7 = perfectly happy.

c Scale of 1 to 5, higher scores = more prone to argument. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were compared with married parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

62

Page 70: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

63

Page 71: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A6: Child outcome by family type, parents’ reports, Waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis)

64

Page 72: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Married families Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

Wave 1

PPVT score a 64.3 63.5 * 62.5 ***

Outcome indices

Physical development index a 100.4 98.9 * 98.6 ***

Social–emotional development index a 101.0 98.2 *** 96.6 ***

Learning index a 101.0 98.3 *** 96.7 ***

Overall development index a 101.2 97.8 *** 96.2 ***

SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesProsocial behaviour a 7.8 7.6 ** 7.5 **

Peer problems b 1.6 1.9 ** 2.1 ***

Emotional problems b 1.7 1.9 * 2.1 ***

Hyperactivity problems b 3.4 3.9 *** 4.1 ***

Conduct problems b, c 2.3 3.1 *** 3.2 ***

Wave 2

PPVT score a 73.7 73.5 72.8 ***

Outcome indicesPhysical development index a 100.3 99.4 99.1 **

Social–emotional development index a 101.1 98.6 *** 96.6 ***

Learning index a 101.0 98.4 *** 97.2 ***

Overall development index a 101.2 98.2 *** 96.5 ***

SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesProsocial behaviour a 8.3 8.0 ** 8.0 *

Peer problems b 1.5 1.7 ** 2.1 ***

Emotional problems b 1.6 1.8 2.1 ***

Hyperactivity problems b 3.2 3.7 ** 3.9 ***

Conduct problems b, c 1.4 1.7 ** 2.0 ***

Wave 3PPVT score a 78.2 77.8 77.2 **Outcome indices

Physical development index a 100.6 98.6 * 98.0 ***

Social–emotional development index a 101.2 99.0 ** 96.5 ***

65

Page 73: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Learning index a 101.0 97.8 *** 97.9 ***

Overall development index a 101.2 97.9 *** 96.5 ***

SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesProsocial behaviour a 8.3 8.5 8.0 *

Peer problems b 1.4 1.8 ** 2.0 ***

Emotional problems b 1.5 1.9 * 2.3 ***

Hyperactivity problems b 3.1 3.5 * 3.8 ***

Conduct problems b, c 1.2 1.6 ** 1.8 ***

Note: a Higher score = better outcome. b Higher score = worse outcome. c Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were compared with married parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

66

Page 74: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A7: Child outcome by family type, teachers’ reports, Waves 1 to 3 (cross-sectional analysis)

Married families

Cohabiting families

Sole-mother families

Wave 1Prosocial behaviour a 7.3 7.2 7.0 **

Peer problems b 2.5 2.9 ** 3.1 ***

Emotional problems b 1.3 1.5 * 1.7 ***

Hyperactivity problems b 3.0 3.5 *** 3.7 ***

Conduct problems b, c 1.8 2.2 *** 2.4 ***

Wave 2Prosocial behaviour a 7.7 7.4 * 7.0 **

Peer problems b 1.3 1.4 1.8 ***

Emotional problems b 1.2 1.4 1.7 ***

Hyperactivity problems b 2.8 2.9 3.7 ***

Conduct problems b, c 0.8 1.0 * 1.4 ***

Wave 3

Prosocial behaviour a 7.7 7.6 7.0 ***

Peer problems b 1.2 1.7 ** 2.0 ***

Emotional problems b 1.1 1.6 ** 1.9 ***

Hyperactivity problems b 2.7 3.1 3.7 ***

Conduct problems b, c 0.7 1.0 * 1.4 ***

Note: a Higher score = better outcome. b Higher score = worse outcome. c Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. For both mothers and fathers, cohabiting parents and sole mothers were compared with married parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted. Missing data result in slight variations in numbers of observations across waves and scales.

67

Page 75: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A8: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by parents (cross-sectional analysis), base model

68

Page 76: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesPhysical

development index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall development

indexProsocial

behaviours Peer problems Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Conduct problems a

Wave 1Age of child (years) 3.83 *** –0.433 0.943 0.699 0.589 0.211 –0.217 0.085 –0.337 * –0.064

Sex of child: Boy –0.946 *** –1.055 *** –2.383 *** –

4.402 *** –3.68 *** –0.486 *** 0.21 *** 0.027 0.768 *** 0.273 ***

Child: Born in Australia 1.029 0.346 0.214 –0.58 0.091 0.215 –0.227 0.029 0.194 0.176

Child: Indigenous –1.154 –0.935 –2.792 * –

3.481 *** –3.348 ** –0.029 0.371 * 0.315 0.598 ** 0.489 *

Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH

–1.377 *** 0.193 0.239 –1.552 *** –0.636 –0.175 * –0.043 –0.129 –0.149 0.054

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH

–0.873 *** –0.068 -1.309 ** –0.653 –1.407 *** –0.127 0.043 0.328 *** 0.016 0.139

Family form (ref. = Married families)Cohabiting families –0.922 * –1.368 * –2.709 *** –

2.126 *** –3.038 *** –0.212 * 0.276 ** 0.19 0.401 ** 0.765 ***

Sole mothers –2.189 *** –1.662 ** –4.448 *** –4.449 *** –5.099 *** –0.254 ** 0.51 *** 0.473 *** 0.723 *** 0.903 ***

Constant 46.976 *** 103.061 *** 98.211 *** 102.129 *** 101.535 *** 6.983 *** 2.726 *** 1.102 4.507 *** 2.174 **r2 0.048 0.01 0.049 0.085 0.079 0.024 0.022 0.023 0.048 0.04No. of respondents 3,636 3998 3997 3978 3977 3997 3997 3997 3997 3997

Wave 2Age of child 2.084 *** –0.069 –0.399 –0.37 –0.464 0.095 0.35 ** 0.26 * 0.3 0.084Sex of child: Boy 0.494 ** –0.944 ** –2.855 *** –

0.893 * –2.233 *** –0.602 *** 0.227 *** –0.036 1.022 *** 0.352 ***

Child: Born in Australia 0.008 0.654 0.85 –

1.073 0.074 0.185 –0.105 –0.217 0.047 0.06

69

Page 77: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesPhysical

development index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall development

indexProsocial

behaviours Peer problems Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Conduct problems a

Child: Indigenous –0.722 –2.141 * –2.31 * –

5.464 *** –4.773 *** –0.546 * 0.508 ** 0.047 0.259 0.123

Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH

–1.757 *** 1.386 *** –0.097 –2.41 *** –0.612 –0.085 –0.035 –0.155 * –0.095 0.307 ***

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH

–0.822 *** 0.569 –1.333 *** –0.75 –0.768 * –0.137 * 0.035 0.262 *** –0.09 0.3 ***

Family form (ref. = Married families)Cohabiting families –0.406 –0.531 –2.303 *** –

2.435 *** –2.592 *** –0.234 * 0.241 * 0.185 0.45 ** 0.352 ***

Sole mother –1.175 *** –1.002 * –4.731 *** –3.824 *** –4.653 *** –0.22 * 0.607 *** 0.588 *** 0.684 *** 0.712 ***

Constant 60.723 *** 99.516 *** 105.389 *** 107.134 *** 106.355 *** 7.87 *** –0.995 –0.067 0.681 0.2

r2 0.041 0.01 0.058 0.049 0.056 0.038 0.034 0.027 0.065 0.055No. of respondents 3947 4073 4071 4061 4059 4069 4068 4068 4068 4068

Wave 3

Age of child 1.923 *** –1.515 –0.467 –0.624 –1.275 0.115 0.124 0.154 0.019 0.040

Sex of child: Boy 0.374 –0.697 –3.427 *** –

0.849 * –2.263 *** –0.741 *** 0.228 *** 0.009 1.166 *** 0.453 ***

Child: Born in Australia 0.789 0.919 –1.066 –

1.053 –0.562 0.200 0.107 0.230 0.359 * 0.178

Child: Indigenous –1.646 ** –1.913 –4.000 ** –

5.299 *** –5.178 *** –0.369 0.251 0.625 * 0.551 0.627 **

Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH

–1.716 *** 1.003 * –0.088 –2.313 *** –0.705 –0.091 –0.052 –0.100 –0.162 0.272 ***

70

Page 78: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scalesPhysical

development index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall development

indexProsocial

behaviours Peer problems Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Conduct problems a

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH

–0.964 *** 0.727 –0.825 –1.203 ** –0.657 –0.209 ** –0.014 0.207 * –0.261 ** 0.197 **

Family form (ref. = Married families)Cohabiting families –0.183 –1.673 –2.008 * –

3.052 *** –3.105 *** 0.249 * 0.400 ** 0.326 * 0.417 * 0.344 **

Sole mother –0.878 ** –2.825 *** –4.680 *** –2.448 *** –4.556 *** –0.285 ** 0.563 *** 0.767 *** 0.662 *** 0.543 ***

Constant 61.943 *** 112.773 *** 108.721 *** 110.456 *** 115.099 *** 7.620 *** 0.129 –0.172 2.187 0.204

r2 0.041 0.019 0.069 0.034 0.055 0.054 0.025 0.037 0.083 0.055No. of respondents 3385 3413 3411 3406 3404 3411 3410 3411 3411 3411

Note: a Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. HH = household. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

71

Page 79: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A9: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by teachers (cross-sectional analysis), base model

72

Page 80: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Prosocial behaviours Peer problems Emotional

problemsHyperactivity

problemsWave 1

Age of child 0.271 –0.677 *** –0.077 –0.105

Sex of child: Boy –0.831 *** 0.197 * –0.005 0.932 **

*Child: Born in Australia 0.012 0.005 0.017 0.056

Child: Indigenous –0.347 0.726 * 0.206 0.971 ***

Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.030 –0.082 -0.0004 –0.306 **

*Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.183 * –0.030 0.144 * –0.343 **

*Family form (ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.061 0.255 * 0.191 0.388 **

Sole mothers –0.218 0.561 *** 0.39 **

* 0.512 ***

Constant 6.358 *** 5.656 **

* 1.566 * 3.242 ***

r2 0.04 0.019 0.008 0.057No. of respondents 3998 3998 3998 3998Wave 2Age of child –0.104 0.203 0.024 0.433

Sex of child: Boy –1.151 *** 0.246 **

* –0.091 1.662 ***

Born in Australia –0.058 0.177 0.206 0.751 **Being Indigenous –0.25 0.197 0.331 0.703Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.092 –0.183 * –0.19 * –0.14Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.025 0.042 0.04 –0.206Family form (ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.293 * 0.123 0.091 0.041

Sole mothers –0.651 *** 0.478 **

* 0.484 *** 0.898 **

*

Constant 8.98 *** –0.343 0.987 –1.616

r2 0.08 0.023 0.016 0.114No. of respondents 3339 3338 3340 3341Wave 3Age of child –0.11 0.183 –0.065 0.096

Sex of child: Boy –1.352 *** 0.128 –0.117 1.689 **

*Born in Australia –0.136 0.149 0.054 –0.014Being Indigenous 0.241 0.144 0.17 0.405Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.045 –0.216 * –0.206 * –0.159Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.188 * –0.089 –0.004 –0.24 *Family form (ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.101 0.492 ** 0.582 ** 0.197

Sole mothers –0.633 *** 0.646 **

* 0.686 *** 0.968 **

*

73

Page 81: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Constant 9.416 *** –0.463 1.828 1.096

r2 0.102 0.029 0.027 0.115No. of respondents 2900 2901 2900 2902

Note: HH = household. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

74

Page 82: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A10: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by parents (cross-sectional analysis), full model

75

Page 83: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity problems

Parent: Conduct

problems (a)

Wave 1Family form (ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.186 –0.573 –0.731 –0.822 –1.085 * –0.094 0.058 –0.02 –0.011 0.436 ***

Sole mothers –0.949 * 0.186 –1.17 * –2.297 *** –1.634 ** –0.114 0.129 0.056 0.102 0.404 ***

Age of child: 4.03 *** –0.231 1.17 0.623 0.79 0.213 –0.25 * 0.038 –0.347 * –0.093

Sex of child: Boy –0.976 *** –0.844 ** –1.908 *** –4.379 *** –3.365 *** –0.433 *** 0.176 *** –0.018 0.683 *** 0.174 **Child: Born in Australia –0.587 –0.153 –0.385 –1.042 –0.677 0.243 –0.077 0.171 0.211 0.174

Child: Indigenous 1.48 0.677 –0.212 0.727 0.551 –0.15 0.093 0.092 –0.219 –0.138

Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH –1.211 *** 0.464 0.74 * –1.051 ** –0.014 –0.079 –0.109 –0.186 ** –0.22 ** 0.044

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH –0.635 * –0.457 –0.347 –0.22 –0.564 –0.02 –0.028 0.229 *** –0.17 * –0.029

Age of mother 0.107 *** –0.007 0.102 ** 0.117 ** 0.105 ** –0.004 –0.009 –0.008 –0.034 *** –0.026 ***

Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification –1.204 *** 0.745 * –1.068 *** –1.888 *** –1.075 *** 0.023 0.193 ** 0.059 0.442 *** 0.096

No post-school qualification –2.011 *** 0.64 –2.032 *** –3.403 *** –2.414 *** –0.073 0.324 *** 0.159 * 0.647 *** 0.173 *

Mother: Born in Australia 2.52 *** –0.371 –0.352 0.669 0.016 –0.224 *** –0.126 * –0.036 0.075 0.19 **Mother: Indigenous status –3.164 * –1.073 –0.71 –4.506 ** –2.81 0.37 0.086 0.018 0.558 0.364

Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)

Part time 0.392 –0.175 0.272 0.235 0.221 –0.129 –0.202 * 0.022 –0.163 0.007

Not employed –0.229 –0.389 –0.485 –0.939 * –0.771 –0.157 –0.066 0.136 0.048 –0.035Financial hardship (up to six types) –0.214 –0.505 * –0.684 *** –0.332 –0.73 *** 0.026 0.122 *** 0.111 ** 0.101 * 0.088 *

Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.012 0.461 * 0.345 0.247 0.446 * 0.051 –0.039 –0.036 –

0.049 –0.04

76

Page 84: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity problems

Parent: Conduct

problems (a)

Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy –0.291 * 0.304 0.723 *** 0.152 0.582 *** 0.199 *** –0.056 0.035 –0.21 *** –0.14 ***

Mothers’ parenting styleWarm parenting –0.029 0.261 1.226 ** –0.716 0.572 0.521 *** –0.176 ** –0.088 0.108 –0.011Hostile parenting –0.057 –1.53 *** –5.043 *** 0.053 –3.182 *** –0.585 *** 0.266 *** 0.386 *** 0.963 *** 1.276 ***

Inductive reasoning 0.252 0.417 0.331 0.457 0.352 0.259 *** –0.021 0.02 –0.106 0.135 *

Consistency 0.954 *** 1.361 *** 2.289 *** 1.637 *** 2.413 *** 0.224 *** –0.212 *** –0.194 *** –0.433 *** –0.469 ***

Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.92 *** –2.38 *** –2.43 *** –0.855 ** –2.738 *** –0.053 0.312 *** 0.519 *** 0.196 ** 0.194 ***

Constant 35.164 *** 84.077 *** 74.628 *** 89.1 *** 75.32 *** 2.967 *** 6.041 *** 3.957 *** 7.031 *** 3.309 ***r2 0.149 0.1 0.341 0.151 0.306 0.184 0.126 0.135 0.223 0.289No. of respondents 3636 3998 3997 3978 3977 3997 3997 3997 3997 3997Wave 2Family form (ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.304 0.421 –0.657 –0.736 –0.563 –0.107 0.083 0.006 0.123 0.138Sole mothers –0.333 0.965 –1.657 *** –1.692 ** –1.218 ** –0.047 0.269 ** 0.122 0.185 0.408 ***

Age of child 2.261 *** 0.249 –0.138 –0.187 –0.087 0.085 0.258 * 0.168 0.319 * 0.130Sex of child: Boy 0.541 ** –0.742 * –2.083 *** –0.770 * –1.694 *** –0.522 *** 0.178 *** –0.120 * 0.871 *** 0.226 ***Child: Born in Australia –0.869 –0.192 –1.179 –1.186 –1.342 –0.031 0.199 0.039 0.235 0.259 *

Child: Indigenous 0.692 2.869 * 1.198 –0.730 1.562 –0.108 –0.029 –0.154 –0.760 * –0.075

Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH –1.555 *** 1.813 *** 0.713 * –1.913 *** 0.225 0.036 –0.124 * –0.249 *** –

0.194 * 0.226 ***

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH –0.422 1.221 ** –0.206 –0.029 0.416 –0.003 –0.021 0.138 * –

0.334 *** 0.130 **

Age of mother 0.111 *** 0.043 0.063 * 0.153 *** 0.116 ** 0.000 –0.005 0.000 –0.027 *** –0.016 ***

Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification –1.480 *** 0.199 –1.179 *** –3.312 *** –2.094 *** –0.051 0.140 * 0.065 0.329 *** 0.210 ***

77

Page 85: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity problems

Parent: Conduct

problems (a)

No post-school qualification –1.826 *** –0.040 –1.515 *** –3.938 *** –2.651 *** –0.073 0.171 * 0.102 0.553 *** 0.166 **

Mother: Born in Australia 1.288 *** –0.044 0.221 –0.372 –0.088 –0.013 –0.219 ** –0.047 0.111 0.078Mother: Indigenous –1.521 –6.028 *** –2.840 –4.935 ** –6.635 *** –0.429 0.602 0.077 1.005 * –0.045Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)

Part time 0.211 –0.311 0.892 * 0.667 0.650 0.063 –0.189 ** 0.059 –0.313 ** –0.130 *

Not employed –0.163 –0.977 * 0.540 –0.140 –0.193 –0.020 –0.159 * 0.125 –0.366 *** –0.104

Financial hardship (up to six types) 0.067 –0.629 * –0.652 ** –0.151 –0.721 ** –0.054 0.057 0.123 ** 0.089 0.028

Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.255 * 0.807 ** 0.368 0.567 * 0.801 *** –0.076 –0.082 * –0.068 –

0.108 * –0.020

Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy –0.305 ** 0.741 *** 0.234 –0.530 * 0.248 0.085 * –0.007 0.027 –

0.093 * –0.049

Mothers’ parenting style

Warm parenting –0.178 0.856 * 1.506 *** –0.757 0.838 * 0.590 *** –0.108 –0.045 –0.180 * –0.105

Hostile parenting –0.104 –0.546 –5.339 *** –0.517 –3.138 *** –0.615 *** 0.250 *** 0.421 *** 1.156 *** 0.981 ***Inductive reasoning 0.063 0.204 –0.275 –0.139 –0.248 0.107 –0.027 0.081 0.233 *** 0.093 *

Consistency 0.871 *** 1.356 *** 2.370 *** 1.860 *** 2.556 *** 0.311 *** –0.316 *** –0.188 *** –0.190 ** –0.331 ***

Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.230 –1.526 *** –3.206 *** –0.789 * –2.666 *** –0.144 * 0.428 *** 0.704 *** 0.300 *** 0.160 ***

Constant 52.277 *** 74.037 *** 81.198 *** 95.678 *** 78.089 *** 4.252 *** 3.488 *** 3.423 *** 1.788 0.727r2 0.118 0.072 0.356 0.124 0.260 0.199 0.141 0.164 0.225 0.311No. of respondents 3947 4073 4071 4061 4059 4069 4068 4068 4068 4068Wave 3Family form (ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.489 –0.481 –0.565 –1.405 –1.130 0.377 ** 0.189 0.179 0.146 0.161Sole mothers –0.056 –0.206 –1.559 ** –0.295 –0.924 –0.151 0.116 0.321 ** 0.124 0.165 *

Age of child 2.157 *** –1.111 0.075 –0.178 –0.610 0.129 0.011 0.067 –0.057 0.003

Sex of child: Boy 0.492 ** –0.368 –2.503 *** –0.608 –1.584 *** –0.633 *** 0.152 ** –0.080 0.984 *** 0.300 ***

78

Page 86: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity problems

Parent: Conduct

problems (a)

Child: Born in Australia 0.244 0.228 –1.955 ** –0.758 –1.158 0.135 0.301 * 0.342 * 0.432 ** 0.269 *Child: Indigenous –0.189 0.153 –1.497 –0.829 –1.000 0.054 0.222 0.378 0.186 0.090Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH –1.549 *** 1.450 *** 0.124 –1.837 *** –0.151 –0.054 –0.108 –0.125 –

0.180 * 0.278 ***

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH –0.616 ** 1.513 *** 0.382 –0.505 0.609 –0.072 –0.144 * 0.084 –

0.481 *** 0.023

Age of mother 0.111 *** 0.038 0.068 * 0.182 *** 0.132 ** –0.005 –0.012 –0.003 –0.019 * –0.019 ***

Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification –1.547 *** –0.440 –1.089 ** –3.365 *** –2.298 *** –0.147 * 0.149 * 0.020 0.277 ** 0.187 ***

No post-school qualification –1.928 *** –0.635 –1.169 ** –4.414 *** –2.902 *** –0.137 0.237 ** 0.008 0.332 ** 0.142 *

Mother: Born in Australia 0.808 *** –0.182 –0.405 –0.712 –0.587 –0.084 –0.087 0.041 0.175 0.085Mother: Indigenous –1.559 –1.808 –2.525 –4.984 * –4.315 * –0.620 –0.164 0.224 0.294 0.635Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)

Part time 0.540 * 0.811 * 0.578 1.267 ** 1.251 ** –0.048 –0.215 *** 0.075 –0.238 ** –0.152 **

Not employed 0.274 –1.037 –0.391 0.526 –0.400 –0.102 0.004 0.193 * –0.110 –0.069

Financial hardship (up to six types) –0.081 –0.604 –0.895 ** –0.034 –0.730 * 0.006 0.137 * 0.151 * 0.124 * 0.127 *

Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.200 1.235 *** 0.551 ** 0.896 *** 1.234 *** –0.013 –0.112 ** –0.083 –

0.132 ** –0.038

Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy –0.205 * 0.432 * 0.465 * –0.354 0.253 0.076 * –0.034 –0.018 –

0.108 * –0.089 **

Mothers’ parenting style

Warm parenting –0.276 0.338 0.737 –0.821 * 0.135 0.425 *** –0.073 0.046 –0.101 0.008

Hostile parenting –0.253 –1.281 *** –5.761 *** –0.549 –3.486 *** –0.732 *** 0.381 *** 0.541 *** 1.131 *** 1.008 ***Inductive reasoning 0.201 –0.264 –0.409 –0.122 –0.369 0.023 –0.022 0.085 0.134 * 0.086 *

Consistency 0.710 *** 1.241 *** 1.568 *** 1.879 *** 2.156 *** 0.188 ** –0.180 *** –0.141 * –0.260 *** –0.251 ***

79

Page 87: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity problems

Parent: Conduct

problems (a)

Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.082 –1.535 *** –2.412 *** –0.153 –1.877 *** 0.005 0.396 *** 0.530 *** 0.301 *** 0.147 **

Constant 53.430 *** 91.524 *** 91.279 *** 94.995 *** 89.941 *** 6.409 *** 4.302 *** 2.219 4.396 ** 0.911r2 0.11 0.095 0.343 0.115 0.244 0.202 0.141 0.152 0.249 0.324No. of respondents 3385 3413 3411 3406 3404 3411 3410 3411 3411 3411

Note: a Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. HH = household. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

80

Page 88: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A11: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by teachers (cross-sectional analysis), full models

81

Page 89: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Teacher: Prosocial

behavioursTeacher: Peer

problemsTeacher:

Emotional problems

Teacher: Hyperactivity

problemsWave 1Family form (ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.053 0.097 0.061 0.132Sole mothers –0.038 0.273 * 0.129 0.104

Age of child 0.273 –0.7 *** –0.121 –0.154

Sex of child: Boy –0.79 *** 0.187 * –0.033 0.89 ***

Child: Born in Australia –0.042 0.293 0.175 0.254Child: Indigenous –0.709 0.366 0.135 0.636

Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.091 –0.12 –0.081 –0.373 ***

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.246 ** –0.079 0.106 –0.395 ***

Age of mother 0 –0.014 0.01 –0.004Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification 0.002 0.174 0.09 0.216 *

No post-school qualification –0.116 0.23 * 0.209 ** 0.415 ***

Mother: Born in Australia 0.008 –0.334 *** –0.113 –0.175Mother: Indigenous status 0.73 0.33 –0.06 0.111Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)

Part time 0.053 –0.208 –0.03 –0.452 ***

Not employed –0.055 0.029 0.185 * –0.168

Financial hardship (up to six types) –0.045 0.075 0.098 * 0.176 ***

Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.059 –0.028 –0.024 –0.025Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy 0.195 *** 0.082 0.062 0.029

Mothers’ parenting styleWarm parenting 0.258 ** –0.029 0.01 0.046

Hostile parenting –0.313 *** 0.044 0.193 ** 0.433 ***

Inductive reasoning 0.116 0.031 –0.045 0.039

Consistency 0.131 * –0.177 * –0.102 * –0.248 ***

Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.06 0.276 *** 0.293 *** 0.151 *

Constant 3.616 *** 7.807 *** 2.506 ** 3.985 ***

r2 0.083 0.049 0.052 0.105No. of respondents 3998 3998 3998 3998Wave 2Family form (ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.165 0.027 0.000 –0.175Sole mothers –0.398 ** 0.284 ** 0.239 0.433 **

Age of child –0.077 0.174 –0.009 0.396

Sex of child: Boy –1.093 *** 0.207 ** –0.116 1.565 ***

82

Page 90: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Teacher: Prosocial

behavioursTeacher: Peer

problemsTeacher:

Emotional problems

Teacher: Hyperactivity

problemsChild: Born in Australia –0.172 0.181 0.116 0.742 **Child: Indigenous 0.728 –0.390 –0.291 –0.651Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.107 –0.225 ** –0.238 ** –0.189Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.122 0.008 0.034 –0.363 **Age of mother 0.016 0.001 0.013 –0.017Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification –0.061 0.066 0.161 0.195No post-school qualification –0.044 0.154 0.103 0.215

Mother: Born in Australia –0.054 0.172 * 0.275 *** 0.377 **Mother: Indigenous status –1.112 * 0.637 * 0.705 1.482 **Mother’s employment status (Full time)

Part time 0.374 *** –0.309 *** 0.015 –0.414 **Not employed 0.202 –0.207 * 0.012 –0.338 *

Financial hardship (up to six types) –0.099 0.001 0.084 0.152Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.053 –0.122 ** –0.144 ** –0.147 *Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy 0.080 –0.003 –0.077 0.053

Mothers’ parenting styleWarm parenting –0.064 0.012 0.107 0.008

Hostile parenting –0.455 *** 0.305 *** –0.032 0.778 ***

Inductive reasoning –0.075 0.015 0.094 0.207 *Consistency 0.203 ** –0.094 –0.090 –0.159

Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.059 0.133 * 0.200 ** 0.235 *Constant 7.975 *** 0.612 1.905 –1.178r2 0.117 0.057 0.039 0.168No. of respondents 3339 3338 3340 3341Wave 3Family form (ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.077 0.369 * 0.476 ** –0.064Sole mothers –0.357 ** 0.457 *** 0.484 ** 0.509 **

Age of child –0.086 0.174 –0.032 0.089

Sex of child: Boy –1.264 *** 0.087 –0.165 * 1.557 ***

Child: Born in Australia –0.079 0.130 –0.139 –0.175Child: Indigenous 0.398 –0.375 –0.435 –0.988 *Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.043 –0.206 * –0.218 * –0.154Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.327 ** –0.211 * –0.088 –0.425 **Age of mother 0.027 ** –0.024 ** –0.001 –0.034 **Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification –0.308 ** 0.035 0.162 0.404 ***

No post-school qualification –0.257 * –0.051 0.083 0.338 **Mother: Born in Australia –0.209 * 0.185 * 0.328 *** 0.408 **Mother: Indigenous status 0.023 0.597 0.697 1.673 **Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)

83

Page 91: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Teacher: Prosocial

behavioursTeacher: Peer

problemsTeacher:

Emotional problems

Teacher: Hyperactivity

problemsPart time 0.277 ** –0.209 * 0.149 –0.264 *Not employed 0.195 0.053 0.207 –0.230

Financial hardship (up to six types) –0.107 –0.020 0.025 0.154Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.067 –0.080 –0.113 * –0.189 **Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy 0.111 * –0.073 –0.067 –0.089

Mothers’ parenting styleWarm parenting –0.021 0.049 0.102 0.196

Hostile parenting –0.496 *** 0.206 ** 0.237 ** 0.719 ***

Inductive reasoning –0.057 0.034 0.117 * 0.104Consistency 0.161 * –0.029 –0.024 –0.258 **

Mothers’ K–6 depression scale 0.055 0.116 0.074 –0.045Constant 8.414 *** 1.090 1.093 1.611r2 0.145 0.056 0.051 0.173No. of respondents 2900 2901 2900 2902

Note: HH = household. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

84

Page 92: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A12: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by parents (unchanged family form), base models

85

Page 93: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Prosocial behaviours

Peer problems

Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Conduct problems (a)

Wave 1Age of child (years) 3.445 *** –1.207 0.526 0.004 –0.230 0.222 –0.114 0.122 –0.189 –0.071

Sex of child: Boy –0.95 *** –1.181 *** –2.374 *** –4.359 *** –3.722 *** –0.424 *** 0.23 *** 0.037 0.793 *** 0.269 ***

Child: Born in Australia 1.531 0.324 –0.349 –0.252 –0.017 0.139 –0.201 0.17 0.211 0.192

Child: Being an Indigenous –1.242 0.172 –1.082 –2.728 –1.519 0.229 0.317 0.135 0.382 0.083

Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH –1.248 *** 0.281 0.46 –0.972 * –0.223 –0.174 * –0.073 –0.175 * –0.147 0.014

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH –0.705 * –0.655 –0.951 * –0.278 –1.063 * –0.092 –0.023 0.284 *** –0.034 0.125

Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)Cohabiting families –0.469 –1.235 –2.376 ** –0.601 –2.102 * –0.151 0.249 * 0.148 0.248 0.834 **

*

Sole mothers –1.593 *** –1.79 * –4.277 *** –3.224 *** –4.428 *** –0.213 0.447 *** 0.46 ** 0.69 *** 0.961 ***

Constant 48.318 *** 106.699 *** 100.521 *** 104.865 *** 105.3 *** 6.938 *** 2.239 ** 0.824 3.762 *** 2.191 *r2 0.035 0.01 0.036 0.065 0.058 0.018 0.015 0.02 0.041 0.032No of respondents 2,839 3092 3091 3077 3076 3091 3091 3091 3091 3091Wave 2Age of child 2.251 *** –0.447 –0.444 –0.575 –0.74 0.12 0.434 *** 0.231 0.225 0.144

Sex of child: Boy 0.663 ** –1.208 *** –2.857 *** –0.632 –2.244 *** –0.601 *** 0.252 *** –0.033 1.033 *** 0.305 ***

Child: Born in Australia –0.197 –0.046 0.327 –0.893 –0.47 0.07 –0.152 –0.24 0.342 * 0.137

Child: Indigenous –0.632 –2.274 * –2.538 * –5.139 *** –4.818 *** –0.64 * 0.463 * 0.104 0.302 0.091Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH –1.656 *** 1.731 *** 0.054 –2.317 *** –0.33 –0.052 –0.074 –0.162 * –0.099 0.314 **

*

86

Page 94: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Prosocial behaviours

Peer problems

Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Conduct problems (a)

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH –0.722 ** 0.879 * –1.074 * –0.533 –0.384 –0.106 –0.029 0.247 ** –0.164 0.302 **

*Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.124 –0.938 –2.405 ** –1.83 * –2.546 ** –0.219 0.344 ** 0.237 0.345 0.343 *

Sole mother –1.029 ** –0.43 –5.448 *** –4.095 *** –4.806 *** –0.375 ** 0.675 *** 0.677 *** 0.683 *** 0.806 ***

Constant 59.67 *** 102.545 *** 106.124 *** 108.246 *** 108.561 *** 7.79 *** –1.518 0.16 0.938 –0.281r2 0.037 0.012 0.055 0.037 0.045 0.04 0.033 0.025 0.063 0.048No of respondents 3265 3369 3368 3360 3359 3366 3366 3365 3367 3366Wave 3Age of child 1.908 *** –1.924 * –0.286 –0.97 –1.54 0.112 0.175 0.056 0.023 0.015

Sex of child: Boy 0.412 * –1.008 * –3.375 *** –0.927 * –2.417 *** –0.735 *** 0.22 *** 0.023 1.169 *** 0.412 ***

Child: Born in Australia 1.018 0.367 –1.076 –0.609 –0.612 0.185 0.071 0.212 0.397 * 0.206

Child: Indigenous –1.408 * –2.906 –5.236 *** –5.456 *** –6.286 *** –0.339 0.516 * 0.928 ** 0.46 0.778 **Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH –1.847 *** 1.194 * 0.059 –2.534 *** –0.648 –0.072 –0.112 –0.13 –0.119 0.275 **

*Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH –1.124 *** 0.637 –0.72 –1.477 ** –0.776 –0.189 * –0.039 0.196 * –0.23 * 0.171 *

Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)Cohabiting families 0.121 –1.268 –2.053 * –2.198 * –2.547 ** 0.226 0.404 ** 0.316 0.41 * 0.366 *

Sole mother –0.983 * –3.35 *** –5.783 *** –3.346 *** –5.703 *** –0.369 ** 0.703 *** 0.892 *** 0.926 *** 0.66 ***

Constant 62.029 *** 117.133 *** 107.028 *** 113.489 *** 117.729 *** 7.634 *** –0.247 0.715 2.054 0.421r2 0.043 0.022 0.073 0.035 0.061 0.053 0.030 0.038 0.087 0.055No. of respondents 3020 3040 3038 3036 3034 3038 3037 3038 3038 3038

87

Page 95: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Note: a Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1. HH = household. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

88

Page 96: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A13: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by teachers (unchanged family form), base models

Prosocial behaviours

Peer problems

Emotional problems

Hyperactivity problems

Wave 1Age of child 0.360 –0.553 ** –0.094 –0.075Sex of child: Boy –0.800 *** 0.273 ** 0.018 1.063 ***Child: Born in Australia 0.107 0.095 0.045 0.077Child: Indigenous –0.446 0.917 ** 0.096 0.959 **Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.000 –0.127 –0.098 –0.305 **Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.190 * –0.069 0.054 –0.358 **Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.031 0.075 0.186 0.226Sole mothers –0.301 0.462 ** 0.365 ** 0.5 **

Constant 5.833 *** 4.919 *** 1.68 * 2.968 **r2 0.039 0.016 0.006 0.062No. of respondents 3092 3092 3092 3092Wave 2Age of child –0.075 0.239 0.017 0.584 *Sex of child: Boy –1.135 *** 0.246 *** –0.076 1.659 ***Child: Born in Australia –0.091 0.023 0.104 0.744 **Child: Being an Indigenous –0.137 0.203 0.432 0.656Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.088 –0.174 * –0.248 ** –0.1Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH –0.015 0.031 –0.037 –0.226Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.185 0.175 0.145 –0.008Sole mothers –0.763 *** 0.695 *** 0.471 ** 0.94 ***

Constant 8.818 *** –0.477 1.202 –2.664r2 0.078 0.027 0.013 0.112No. of respondents 2789 2787 2789 2790Wave 3Age of child –0.130 0.253 –0.122 0.171Sex of child: Boy –1.362 *** 0.133 –0.074 1.721 ***Child: Born in Australia –0.069 0.084 0.033 –0.16Child: Indigenous 0.157 0.238 0.218 0.749 *Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.047 –0.236 * –0.251 ** –0.19Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.171 –0.077 –0.013 –0.28 *Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.196 0.535 ** 0.484 * 0.258Sole mothers –0.677 *** 0.725 *** 0.72 *** 1.137 ***

Constant 9.543 *** –1.044 2.356 0.555r2 0.101 0.029 0.023 0.122No. of respondents 2591 2592 2590 2592

Note: HH = household. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

89

Page 97: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A14: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by parents (unchanged family form), full models

90

Page 98: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity

problems

Parent: Conduct problems

(a)

Wave 1Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.159 –0.496 –0.666 0.434 –0.425 –0.056 0.054 –0.048 –0.098 0.570 ***

Sole mothers –0.515 0.240 –1.012 –1.378 –1.076 –0.045 0.070 0.030 0.106 0.498 **Child: Age 3.690 *** –0.888 0.773 –0.032 0.040 0.219 –0.157 0.071 –0.194 –0.100Sex of child: Boy –0.863 *** –0.868 ** –1.670 *** –4.208 *** –3.191 *** –0.348 *** 0.169 ** –0.033 0.671 *** 0.138 *Child: Born in Australia –0.023 –0.222 –0.905 –0.451 –0.640 0.197 –0.066 0.314 * 0.228 0.205

Child: Indigenous 1.280 1.193 1.294 2.936 2.539 –0.051 0.126 –0.094 –0.570 –0.567Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH –1.081 *** 0.467 0.838 * –0.509 0.259 –0.093 –0.132 –0.204 ** –0.215 * 0.012

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH

–0.528 –0.662 –0.387 0.076 –0.542 –0.034 –0.048 0.232 ** –0.179 0.016

Age of mother 0.110 *** –0.028 0.084 * 0.107 * 0.084 * –0.007 –0.005 –0.012 –0.030 ** –0.017 *Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification

–1.056 *** 0.919 * –1.126 ** –1.630 *** –0.917 ** 0.021 0.156 * 0.094 0.459 *** 0.104

No post-school qualification –1.888 *** 0.822 * –1.977 *** –3.223 *** –2.240 *** –0.072 0.331 *** 0.128 0.662 *** 0.173 *

Mother: Born in Australia 2.393 *** –0.270 –0.230 0.197 –0.098 –0.222 ** –0.117 –0.074 0.052 0.181 *

Mother: Indigenous status –3.108 * –0.683 –1.112 –6.938 ** –3.732 0.593 –0.004 0.120 0.884 0.581

Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)Part time 0.529 0.084 0.637 0.662 0.751 –0.158 –0.259 ** –0.078 –0.142 –0.052Not employed 0.049 –0.094 0.065 –0.566 –0.161 –0.111 –0.131 –0.002 0.069 –0.061

91

Page 99: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity

problems

Parent: Conduct problems

(a)

Financial hardship (up to six types) –0.300 –0.661 * –0.842 *** –0.274 –0.856 ** 0.034 0.135 ** 0.127 ** 0.162 ** 0.119 *

Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.149 0.420 0.452 * 0.262 0.476 * 0.085 * –0.070 –0.053 –0.030 –0.023

Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy

–0.243 0.380 0.838 *** 0.211 0.711 *** 0.192 *** –0.070 * –0.018 –0.182 *** –0.135 ***

Mothers’ parenting styleWarm parenting 0.319 0.513 1.284 ** –0.513 0.892 * 0.497 *** –0.214 * –0.060 0.084 –0.066

Hostile parenting 0.116 –1.247 *** –4.847 *** –0.099 –3.066 *** –0.605 *** 0.242 *** 0.338 *** 0.948 *** 1.242 ***

Inductive reasoning 0.219 0.286 0.430 0.550 0.319 0.234 ** –0.055 –0.023 –0.089 0.146 *

Consistency 1.068 *** 1.603 *** 2.596 *** 1.511 *** 2.547 *** 0.284 *** –0.255 *** –0.244 *** –0.433 *** –0.477 ***Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.900 *** –2.338 *** –2.329 *** –0.801 * –2.669 *** –0.030 0.288 *** 0.499 *** 0.204 ** 0.208 **

Constant 32.935 *** 85.450 *** 74.228 *** 90.952 *** 76.425 *** 3.052 ** 6.142 *** 4.523 *** 6.047 *** 3.305 ***r2 0.136 0.095 0.333 0.127 0.284 0.18 0.122 0.132 0.21 0.278No. of respondents 2839 3092 3091 3077 3076 3091 3091 3091 3091 3091

Wave 2Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.486 0.032 –0.913 –0.413 –0.721 –0.098 0.186 0.066 0.079 0.165

Sole mothers –0.056 1.963 ** –1.765 ** –1.462 * –0.672 –0.151 0.267 * 0.158 0.095 0.428 ***Age of child 2.475 *** –0.011 0.106 –0.354 –0.177 0.139 0.313 ** 0.114 0.207 0.137Sex of child: Boy 0.742 *** –0.934 ** –2.067 *** –0.491 –1.647 *** –0.513 *** 0.194 *** –0.123 * 0.885 *** 0.193 ***

92

Page 100: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity

problems

Parent: Conduct problems

(a)

Child: Born in Australia –0.911 –0.803 –1.177 –0.549 –1.348 –0.104 0.097 –0.056 0.468 ** 0.270 *

Child: Indigenous 0.217 2.369 0.186 0.273 1.338 –0.208 –0.034 –0.019 –0.580 0.076Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH –1.430 *** 2.035 *** 0.702 –1.782 *** 0.376 0.045 –0.138 * –0.221 ** –0.188 * 0.225 ***

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH

–0.296 1.352 ** –0.264 0.259 0.595 0.011 –0.046 0.170 * –0.347 *** 0.171 **

Age of mother 0.131 *** 0.051 0.061 0.178 *** 0.130 *** 0.000 –0.006 –0.004 –0.021 * –0.010Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification

–1.382 *** 0.226 –1.063 ** –3.343 *** –2.044 *** –0.024 0.104 0.076 0.359 *** 0.194 ***

No post-school qualification –1.646 *** 0.041 –1.323 *** –3.836 *** –2.486 *** –0.009 0.138 0.091 0.558 *** 0.164 **

Mother: Born in Australia 1.085 *** 0.039 –0.152 –0.815 –0.447 –0.055 –0.187 * –0.012 0.129 0.143 *

Mother: Indigenous status –0.762 –5.819 ** –2.369 –6.240 ** –6.958 *** –0.575 0.595 0.019 0.889 –0.249

Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)Part time 0.084 –0.118 0.926 * 0.418 0.645 –0.024 –0.221 ** 0.010 –0.315 ** –0.156 *Not employed –0.185 –0.769 0.722 –0.149 0.021 –0.078 –0.210 * 0.049 –0.410 *** –0.126

Financial hardship (up to six types) 0.072 –0.660 –0.547 –0.469 –0.846 ** 0.012 0.084 0.110 0.069 0.044

Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.238 0.891 ** 0.523 * 0.616 * 0.932 *** –0.050 –0.110 ** –0.080 –0.128 * –0.017

Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy

–0.281 * 0.875 *** 0.274 –0.487 * 0.353 0.070 –0.023 0.013 –0.097 –0.047

Mothers’ parenting style

93

Page 101: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity

problems

Parent: Conduct problems

(a)

Warm parenting –0.344 0.872 * 0.974 * –0.922 * 0.512 0.567 *** –0.047 0.019 –0.056 –0.062

Hostile parenting –0.053 –0.307 –5.482 *** –0.396 –3.037 *** –0.670 *** 0.245 *** 0.406 *** 1.232 *** 0.980 ***

Inductive reasoning 0.119 0.102 –0.064 –0.007 –0.158 0.085 –0.061 0.051 0.172 ** 0.080 *

Consistency 1.055 *** 1.601 *** 2.329 *** 1.706 *** 2.541 *** 0.331 *** –0.319 *** –0.190 ** –0.135 –0.318 ***Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.365 –1.679 *** –3.504 *** –0.793 * –2.886 *** –0.150 * 0.444 *** 0.745 *** 0.371 *** 0.192 ***

Constant 49.015 *** 72.901 *** 79.575 *** 95.595 *** 77.089 *** 4.186 *** 3.412 *** 4.213 *** 1.860 0.350r2 0.115 0.076 0.357 0.114 0.249 0.206 0.141 0.157 0.227 0.305No. of respondents 3265 3369 3368 3360 3359 3366 3366 3365 3367 3366

Wave 3Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.777 * –0.042 –0.540 –0.595 –0.546 0.357 ** 0.203 0.152 0.123 0.168

Sole mothers –0.112 –0.641 –2.578 *** –0.916 –1.876 * –0.201 0.286 * 0.462 ** 0.350 * 0.232 *Age of child 2.151 *** –1.493 0.235 –0.528 –0.868 0.119 0.071 –0.034 –0.049 –0.022Sex of child: Boy 0.540 ** –0.669 –2.313 *** –0.674 –1.668 *** –0.613 *** 0.130 * –0.084 0.965 *** 0.241 ***Child: Born in Australia 0.404 –0.043 –1.878 ** –0.390 –1.075 0.157 0.266 0.310 * 0.497 ** 0.283 *

Child: Indigenous 0.484 –1.419 –2.308 0.629 –1.429 0.449 0.628 0.714 0.060 0.155Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH –1.694 *** 1.525 ** 0.093 –2.035 *** –0.221 –0.046 –0.136 –0.124 –0.116 0.288 ***

Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH

–0.791 ** 1.253 * 0.255 –0.736 0.315 –0.060 –0.120 0.118 –0.447 *** 0.018

Age of mother 0.112 *** 0.028 0.048 0.187 *** 0.119 ** –0.010 –0.010 –0.001 –0.021 * –0.013 *

94

Page 102: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity

problems

Parent: Conduct problems

(a)

Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)Other post-school qualification

–1.517 *** –0.323 –1.382 *** –3.332 *** –2.365 *** –0.166 * 0.161 * 0.085 0.304 *** 0.228 ***

No post-school qualification –1.954 *** –0.568 –1.231 ** –4.041 *** –2.728 *** –0.119 0.228 ** 0.048 0.336 ** 0.146 *

Mother: Born in Australia 0.825 *** –0.582 –0.289 –0.861 –0.784 –0.108 –0.109 0.017 0.124 0.079

Mother: Indigenous status –2.257 –0.978 –3.321 –7.428 *** –5.431 * –1.130 * –0.323 0.239 0.357 0.748

Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)Part time 0.558 * 0.534 0.215 1.087 * 0.881 * –0.098 –0.182 ** 0.114 –0.172 –0.103Not employed 0.204 –1.297 * –0.461 0.281 –0.665 –0.106 0.000 0.183 –0.079 –0.031

Financial hardship (up to six types) 0.048 –0.851 * –0.989 ** –0.066 –0.892 * 0.021 0.122 0.180 * 0.136 0.163 **

Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.284 * 0.948 ** 0.327 1.008 *** 1.057 *** –0.002 –0.094 * –0.036 –0.079 –0.009

Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy

–0.187 0.552 * 0.365 –0.341 0.272 0.067 –0.022 0.004 –0.103 * –0.082 *

Mothers’ parenting styleWarm parenting –0.426 * 0.342 0.693 –1.002 * 0.033 0.411 *** –0.058 0.061 –0.171 0.030

Hostile parenting –0.384 * –1.068 ** –5.934 *** –0.697 * –3.533 *** –0.780 *** 0.395 *** 0.543 *** 1.139 *** 1.049 ***

Inductive reasoning 0.268 –0.148 –0.346 0.035 –0.212 0.010 –0.028 0.074 0.139 * 0.053

Consistency 0.670 *** 1.254 *** 1.242 *** 1.891 *** 2.018 *** 0.139 * –0.156 ** –0.110 –0.210 ** –0.194 ***Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.119 –1.711 *** –2.617 *** 0.013 –1.973 *** 0.005 0.416 *** 0.570 *** 0.359 *** 0.159 **

95

Page 103: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT score

Outcome indices SDQ social–emotional sub-scales

Physical devt index

Social–emotional devt index

Index of learning

Overall devt index

Parent: Prosocial

behavioursParent: Peer

problemsParent:

Emotional problems

Parent: Hyperactivity

problems

Parent: Conduct problems

(a)

Constant 53.691 *** 95.280 *** 92.960 *** 98.628 *** 94.085 *** 7.126 *** 3.520 * 2.774 * 4.390 ** 0.488r2 0.115 0.094 0.338 0.118 0.24 0.198 0.134 0.147 0.252 0.322No. of respondents 3020 3040 3038 3036 3034 3038 3037 3038 3038 3038

(a) Items that formed this scale were the same in Wave 2 and Wave 3, but differed in Wave 1.

Note: HH = household. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

96

Page 104: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Table A15: Regressions of child outcomes, as reported by teachers (unchanged family form), full models

97

Page 105: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Teacher: Prosocial

behavioursTeacher: Peer

problemsTeacher:

Emotional problems

Teacher: Hyperactivity

problemsWave 1Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families 0.085 –0.021 0.064 0.010Sole mothers –0.096 0.242 0.140 0.096

Age of child 0.358 –0.607 ** –0.131 –0.138Sex of child: Boy –0.742 *** 0.244 ** –0.026 0.998 ***Child: Born in Australia 0.035 0.408 * 0.192 0.256Child: Indigenous –0.893 0.270 0.309 0.323Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.063 –0.144 –0.148 –0.362 ***Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.241 * –0.035 0.061 –0.353 **Age of mother 0.001 0.001 0.009 0.008Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification 0.003 0.084 0.103 0.187No post-school qualification –0.077 0.141 0.216 ** 0.417 ***

Mother: Born in Australia 0.006 –0.367 *** –0.154 –0.193Mother: Indigenous status 0.850 0.875 –0.447 0.626Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)

Part time 0.002 –0.384 ** –0.085 –0.437 **Not employed –0.174 –0.160 0.129 –0.191

Financial hardship (up to six types) –0.032 0.079 0.067 0.246 ***Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.050 –0.053 –0.045 –0.058Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy 0.167 ** 0.046 –0.008 –0.008

Mothers’ parenting styleWarm parenting 0.255 * 0.024 0.096 0.006Hostile parenting –0.301 *** 0.005 0.168 * 0.360 ***Inductive reasoning 0.116 –0.068 –0.064 0.000Consistency 0.217 ** –0.144 –0.114 –0.193 **

Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.094 0.221 ** 0.238 *** 0.150Constant 2.752 * 7.038 *** 2.626 ** 3.916 **r2 0.084 0.04 0.044 0.107No. of respondents 3092 3092 3092 3092Wave 2Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.068 0.101 0.047 –0.203Sole mothers –0.453 * 0.496 *** 0.188 0.382

Age of child –0.024 0.189 –0.030 0.532 *Sex of child: Boy –1.067 *** 0.206 ** –0.107 1.548 ***Child: Born in Australia –0.179 –0.003 –0.022 0.700 **Child: Indigenous 0.630 –0.192 –0.139 –0.600Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.113 –0.209 ** –0.282 ** –0.162Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.052 0.058 –0.020 –0.349 *Age of mother 0.010 0.010 0.016 –0.008Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification –0.059 0.072 0.165 0.328 **No post-school qualification –0.034 0.143 0.075 0.256

Mother: Born in Australia –0.080 0.168 * 0.317 *** 0.370 **Mother: Indigenous status –0.856 0.406 0.628 1.409 *Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)

Part time 0.393 *** –0.389 *** –0.042 –0.389 **Not employed 0.226 –0.313 ** –0.015 –0.331 *

Financial hardship (up to six types) –0.119 0.037 0.093 0.242 *Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.089 –0.126 ** –0.134 * –0.172 *

98

Page 106: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy 0.074 –0.007 –0.076 0.027

Mothers’ parenting styleWarm parenting –0.050 0.038 0.084 –0.031Hostile parenting –0.441 *** 0.276 *** –0.022 0.741 ***Inductive reasoning –0.076 –0.004 0.084 0.211 *Consistency 0.196 * –0.075 –0.086 –0.168

Mothers’ K–6 depression scale –0.081 0.100 0.229 ** 0.160Constant 7.598 *** 0.188 2.351 –2.363r2 0.113 0.058 0.037 0.165No. of respondents 2789 2787 2789 2790Wave 3Family form (unchanged; ref. = Married families)

Cohabiting families –0.034 0.437 * 0.376 0.004Sole mothers –0.388 * 0.524 ** 0.491 * 0.631 *

Age of child –0.128 0.259 –0.084 0.192Sex of child: Boy –1.270 *** 0.098 –0.123 1.579 ***Child: Born in Australia –0.002 0.051 –0.176 –0.350Child: Indigenous 0.602 –0.243 –0.385 –0.655Child: Had older sibling(s) in HH 0.045 –0.223 * –0.263 ** –0.177Child: Had younger sibling(s) in HH 0.311 ** –0.182 * –0.079 –0.457 **Age of mother 0.028 ** –0.023 * 0.001 –0.034 **Mother: Education attainment (ref. = Degree or higher)

Other post-school qualification –0.270 * 0.053 0.142 0.381 **No post-school qualification –0.153 –0.104 0.042 0.208

Mother: Born in Australia –0.241 * 0.199 * 0.340 *** 0.456 **Mother: Indigenous status –0.358 0.521 0.660 1.567 *Mother’s employment status (ref. = Full time)

Part time 0.225 * –0.150 0.179 * –0.195Not employed 0.161 0.027 0.173 –0.174

Financial hardship (up to six types) –0.114 0.036 0.090 0.206Rating of financial prosperity (1 to 6) 0.063 –0.056 –0.107 –0.172 *Mothers’ ratings of parenting self-efficacy 0.078 –0.091 –0.080 –0.085

Mothers’ parenting styleWarm parenting 0.000 0.039 0.046 0.160Hostile parenting –0.517 *** 0.165 * 0.185 * 0.738 ***Inductive reasoning –0.016 0.010 0.107 0.097Consistency 0.165 * –0.017 –0.047 –0.241 *

Mothers’ K–6 depression scale 0.099 0.105 0.053 –0.077Constant 8.791 *** 0.377 1.915 0.650r2 0.141 0.053 0.046 0.179No. of respondents 2591 2592 2590 2592

Note: HH = household. The family forms in this table remained the same across the three waves and comprised those in which the children lived with their biological parents. Statistical differences are shown as: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Data have been weighted.

List of shortened formsABS Australian Bureau of StatisticsAIFS Australian Institute of Family Studies ANU Australian National University ASSDA Australian Social Sciences Data Archives FaHCSIA Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs HILDA Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia LSAC Longitudinal Study of Australian Children

99

Page 107: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

PPVT Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test SDQ Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire

100

Page 108: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Endnotes1. The authors derived the percentage for 1997 based on the ABS (1998) publication relating to its Family Characteristics Survey (Table 23). The authors derived the percentage for 2006–07 from the expanded, confidentialised unit record file for the Family Characteristics and Transitions Survey, Australia (ABS 2008). ‘Intact families’ refers to those where both parents were living with their biological children and with no stepchildren. However, it is possible for one or both parents to have married or lived with someone previously and it is also possible for one or both parents to have had children from a previous relationship(s) living elsewhere.

2. The 1999 data on the marital status of mothers for New South Wales were excluded due to methodological issues in the data collection (Nassar & Sullivan 2001).

3. The 2005 results were based on the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey. The HILDA project was initiated and is funded by the Australian Government Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs (FaHCSIA) and is managed by the Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research (Melbourne Institute). The findings and views reported, however, are those of Qu and Weston (2008a) and should not be attributed to either FaHCSIA or the Melbourne Institute.

4. The 1971 ANU survey was conducted by the Department of Demography. Relevant information about the project is available at the Australian Social Sciences Data Archives (ASSDA) website: <assda-nesstar.anu.edu.au/webview>.

5. Evans and Gray (2005) also reported that 61 to 64 per cent of men and women saw married couples without children as being ‘family’, but only 29 per cent of men and 36 per cent of women used this classification for cohabiting couples without children.

6. LSAC is conducted in partnership between FaHCSIA, the Australian Institute of Family Studies (AIFS) and the ABS. The findings and views reported in this analysis are those of the authors and should not be attributed to FaHCSIA or the ABS.

7. Of the 326 cohabiting biological parents in Wave 1, 54 had married and 49 had separated by Wave 3.

8. While equivalised household income (that is, household income relative to costs associated with household composition) may have been a superior indicator of objective financial circumstances, the first LSAC wave collected parental income but not household income, and the results for parental income were based on a low response rate (79 per cent).

9. Primary carers in Waves 1 and 3 were also asked whether, in the last 12 months, they had financial limits on the type of food they could buy. Because this question was not asked in Wave 2, the item was excluded in the analyses to achieve consistency of measurement across the three waves.

10. Children who were not living with a biological parent were living with grandparents, other relatives or foster parents.

11. Of the children in these three family groups, 85 per cent were represented in all three waves and 3 per cent were followed up in Wave 3 but not Wave 2.

12. Seven children had such a change in living arrangements, with two children experiencing a shared care-time arrangement in Wave 1.

13. The proportions of mothers who were recorded as having a degree or higher qualification for all three unchanged family groups fell very slightly from Wave 1 to Wave 3. This may

101

Page 109: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

reflect reporting errors or the possibility that in subsequent waves some mothers with a degree reported a more recently (and lower level) acquired qualification rather than their highest one.

14. Here, as elsewhere, married and cohabiting parents refer to those in couple families in which the study child was born to the couple.

15. Ranges reported here refer to mean scores for different groups of fathers and mothers across the three waves to avoid cumbersome reporting of each individual mean score, and they are not confidential intervals.

16. The discussion in this paragraph is based on separate analyses that assessed the statistical significance of any differences in the wellbeing scores of children in cohabiting families and sole-mother families. Results are available from the authors on request.

17. While some of discussions in this paper focus on stable family types in order to control the effect of change in family forms, the analyses based on cross-sectional data are also carried out and available in the Appendix and the results are in general similar to those based on stable family types.

102

Page 110: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

ReferencesArtis, J 2007, ‘Maternal cohabitation and child well-being among kindergarten children’, Journal of

Marriage and Family, vol. 69, pp. 222–36. Australian Bureau of Statistics 1998, Family characteristics Australia 1997, cat. no. 4442.0, ABS,

Canberra.—— 2002, Marriages and divorces 2001, cat. no. 3310.0, ABS, Canberra.—— 2005, Family, household and income unit variables, 2005, cat. no. 1286.0, ABS, Canberra.—— 2008, Family characteristics and transitions, Australia, 2006–07, cat. no. 4442.0, ABS, Canberra.—— 2010a, Births Australia 2009, cat. no. 3301.0, ABS, Canberra.—— 2010b, Marriages and divorces 2009, cat. no. 3310.0, ABS, Canberra.Baxter, JA & Smart, D 2010, Fathering in Australia among couple families with young children,

Occasional Paper No. 37, Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs, Canberra.

Björklund, A, Ginther, D & Sundström, M 2007, Does marriage matter for children? Assessing the causal impact of legal marriage, IZA Discussion Paper No. 3189, Institute for the Study of Labour, Bonn.

Brown, SL 2000, ‘The effect of union type on psychological well-being: Depression among cohabitors versus married’, Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, vol. 41, pp. 244–55.

—— 2004, ‘Family structure and child well-being: The significance of parental cohabitation’, Journal of Marriage and Family, vol. 66, pp. 351–67.

Brown, SL & Manning, WD 2009, ‘Family boundary ambiguity and the measurement of family structure: The significance of cohabitation’, Demography, vol. 46, no. 1, pp. 85–101.

Cherlin, A 2004, ‘The deinstitutionalization of American marriage’, Journal of Marriage and Family, vol. 66, pp. 848–61.

De Los Rayes, A 2011, ‘Introduction to the special edition: More than measurement error. Discovering meaning behind informant discrepancies in clinical assessment of children and adolescents’, Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psyschology, vol. 40, no. 1, pp. 1–9.

Edwards, B 2005, ‘Does it take a village? An investigation of neighbourhood effects on Australian children’s development’, Family Matters, vol. 72, pp. 36–43.

Ermisch, J 2002, Trying again: Repartnering after dissolution of a union, ISER Working Papers No. 2002–19, Institute for Social and Economic Research, University of Essex, Essex.

Evans, A & Gray, E 2005, ‘What makes an Australian family?’, in S Wilson, G Megaher, R Gibson, D Denemark & M Western (eds), Australian social attitudes: The first report , UNSW Press, Sydney.

Goodman, A & Greaves, E 2010, Cohabitation, marriage and child outcomes, Institute of Fiscal Studies, London.

Hayes, A, Qu, L, Weston, R & Baxter, J 2011, Families in Australia 2011: Sticking together in good and tough times, Australian Institute of Family Studies, Melbourne.

Hayes, A, Weston, R, Qu, L & Gray, M 2010, Families then and now 1980–2010, Fact sheet, Australian Institute of Family Studies, Melbourne.

Hofferth, S 2006, ‘Residential father family type and child well-being: Investment versus selection’, Demography, vol. 43, no. 1, pp. 53–77.

Kessler, RC, Andrews, G, Colpe, LJ, Hiripi, E, Mroczek, DK, Normand, SLT et al. 2002, ‘Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in nonspecific psychological distress’, Psychological Medicine, vol. 32, no. 6, pp. 959–76.

Kiernan, K 2001, ‘The rise of cohabitation and childbearing outside marriage in Western Europe’, International Journal of Law, Policy and the Family, vol. 15, pp. 1–21.

—— 2002, ‘Cohabitation in western Europe: Trends, issues, and implications’, in A Booth & AC Crouter (eds), Just living together: Implications of cohabitations on families, children and social policy , Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Mahwah, NJ.

Lancaster, P, Huang, J & Pedisich, E 1994, Australia’s mothers and babies 1991, National Perinatal Statistics No. 1, Australian Institute of Health and Welfare, Canberra.

103

Page 111: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Laws, P, Abeywardana, S, Walker, J & Sullivan, E 2007, Australia’s mothers and babies 2005, National Perinatal Statistics No. 20, Australian Institute of Health and Welfare, Canberra.

Longitudinal Study of Australian Children 2009, 2008–09 annual report, Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs and Australian Institute of Family Studies, Canberra.

Mackay, R 2005, ‘The impact of family structure and family change on child outcomes: A personal reading of the research literature’, Social Policy Journal of New Zealand, vol. 24, pp. 111–33. Retrieved from: <www.msd.govt.nz/documents/publications/mds/journal/issue24/24-pages111–113.pdf>.

Magnuson, K & Berger, LM 2009, ‘Family states and transitions: Associations with children’s well-being during middle childhood’, Journal of Marriage and Family, vol. 71, pp. 575–91.

Nassar, N & Sullivan, E 2001, Australia’s mothers and babies 1999, National Perinatal Statistics No. 11, Australian Institute of Health and Welfare, Canberra.

Osborne, C, McLanahan, S & Brooks-Gunn, J 2003, Young children’s behavioral problems in married and cohabiting families, Center for Research on Child Wellbeing Working Paper No. 03–09-FF, Center for Research on Child Wellbeing, Office of Population Research, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey.

Osbourne, C, Manning, WD & Smock, PJ 2007, ‘Married and cohabiting parents’ relationship stability: A focus on race and ethnicity’, Journal of Marriage and Family, vol. 69, pp. 1345–66.

Pryor, J & Rodgers, B 2001, Children in changing families: Life after parental separation, Blackwell, Oxford.

Qu, L & Weston, R 2008a, ‘Attitudes towards marriage and cohabitation’, Family Relationship Quarterly, vol. 8, pp. 5–10.

—— 2008b, Snapshots of family relationships, Australian Institute of Family Studies, Melbourne.Sanson, A, Mission, S & the LSAC Outcome Index Working Group 2005, Summarising children’s

wellbeing: The LSAC outcome index, LSAC Technical Paper No. 2, Australian Institute of Family Studies, Melbourne.

Soloff, C, Lawrence, D & Johnstone, R 2005, Sample design, LSAC Technical Paper No. 1, Australian Institute of Family Studies, Melbourne.

Tobias, T, Kokaua, J, Gerritsen, S & Templeton, R 2010, ‘The health of children in sole-parent families in New Zealand: Results of a population-based cross-sectional survey’, Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 274–80.

Weston, R & Qu, L 2007, ‘An update on partnership formation trends: What does the 2006 Census suggest?’, Family Relationships Quarterly, vol. 6, pp. 16–19.

Wu, Z, Hou, F & Schimmele, CM 2008, ‘Family structure and children’s psychosocial outcomes’, Journal of Family Issues, vol. 29, no. 12, pp. 1600–24.

104

Page 112: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Occasional papers1. Income support and related statistics: a ten-year compendium, 1989–99

Kim Bond and Jie Wang (2001)2. Low fertility: a discussion paper

Alison Barnes (2001)3. The identification and analysis of indicators of community strength and outcomes Alan

Black and Phillip Hughes (2001)4. Hardship in Australia: an analysis of financial stress indicators in the 1998–99 Australian

Bureau of Statistics Household Expenditure SurveyJ Rob Bray (2001)

5. Welfare Reform Pilots: characteristics and participation patterns of three disadvantaged groupsChris Carlile, Michael Fuery, Carole Heyworth, Mary Ivec, Kerry Marshall and Marie Newey (2002)

6. The Australian system of social protection—an overview (second edition)Peter Whiteford and Gregory Angenent (2002)

7. Income support customers: a statistical overview 2001Corporate Information and Mapping Services, Strategic Policy and Knowledge Branch, Family and Community Services (2003)

8. Inquiry into long-term strategies to address the ageing of the Australian population over the next 40 yearsCommonwealth Department of Family and Community Services submission to the 2003 House of Representatives Standing Committee on Ageing (2003)

9. Inquiry into poverty and financial hardshipCommonwealth Department of Family and Community Services submission to the Senate Community Affairs References Committee (2003)

10. Families of prisoners: literature review on issues and difficultiesRosemary Woodward (2003)

11. Inquiries into retirement and superannuationAustralian Government Department of Family and Community Services submissions to the Senate Select Committee on Superannuation (2003)

12. A compendium of legislative changes in social security 1908–1982 (2006)13. A compendium of legislative changes in social security 1983–2000 Part 1 1983–1993,

Part 2 1994–2000Bob Daprè (2006)

14. Evaluation of Fixing Houses for Better Health Projects 2, 3 and 4SGS Economics & Planning in conjunction with Tallegalla Consultants Pty Ltd (2006)

15. The ‘growing up’ of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander children: a literature reviewProfessor Robyn Penman (2006)

16. Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander views on research in their communitiesProfessor Robyn Penman (2006)

17. Growing up in the Torres Strait Islands: a report from the Footprints in Time trials

105

Page 113: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

Cooperative Research Centre for Aboriginal Health in collaboration with the Telethon Institute for Child Health Research and the Department of Families, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs (2006)

18. Costs of children: research commissioned by the Ministerial Taskforce on Child SupportPaul Henman; Richard Percival and Ann Harding; Matthew Gray (2007)

19. Lessons learnt about strengthening Indigenous families and communities: what’s working and what’s not?John Scougall (2008)

20. Stories on ‘growing up’ from Indigenous people in the ACT metro/Queanbeyan regionCooperative Research Centre for Aboriginal Health in collaboration with the Telethon Institute for ChildHealth Research and the Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs (2008)

21. Inquiry into the cost of living pressures on older AustraliansAustralian Government Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs submissions to the Senate Standing Committee on Community Affairs (2008)

22. Engaging fathers in child and family services: participation, perception and good practiceClaire Berlyn, Sarah Wise and Grace Soriano (2008)

23. Indigenous families and children: coordination and provision of servicesSaul Flaxman, Kristy Muir and Ioana Oprea (2009)

24. National evaluation (2004–2008) of the Stronger Families and Communities Strategy 2004–2009Kristy Muir, Ilan Katz, Christiane Purcal, Roger Patulny, Saul Flaxman, David Abelló, Natasha Cortis, Cathy Thomson, Ioana Oprea, Sarah Wise, Ben Edwards, Matthew Gray and Alan Hayes (2009)

25. Stronger Families in Australia study: the impact of Communities for ChildrenBen Edwards, Sarah Wise, Matthew Gray, Alan Hayes, Ilan Katz, Sebastian Misson, Roger Patulny and Kristy Muir (2009)

26. Engaging hard-to-reach families and childrenNatasha Cortis, Ilan Katz and Roger Patulny (2009)

27. Ageing and Australian Disability EnterprisesShannon McDermott, Robyn Edwards, David Abelló and Ilan Katz (2010)

28. Needs of clients in the Supported Accommodation Assistance ProgramAustralian Institute of Health and Welfare (2010)

29. Effectiveness of individual funding approaches for disability supportKaren R Fisher, Ryan Gleeson, Robyn Edwards, Christiane Purcal, Tomasz Sitek, Brooke Dinning, Carmel Laragy, Lel D’aegher and Denise Thompson (2010)

30. Families’ experiences of servicesMorag McArthur, Lorraine Thomson, Gail Winkworth and Kate Butler (2010)

31. Housing costs and living standards among the elderlyBruce Bradbury and Bina Gubhaju (2010)

32. Incentives, rewards, motivation and the receipt of income supportJacqueline Homel and Chris Ryan (2010)

33. Problem gamblers and the role of the financial sectorThe South Australian Centre for Economic Studies (2010)

106

Page 114: Parental social marital status and children’s well-being€¦  · Web viewMarried Continued cohabiting Separated, with sole mothers Wave 1 PPVT score a 63.65 64.35 64.9 Primary

34. Evaluation of income management in the Northern TerritoryAustralian Institute of Health and Welfare (2010)

35. Post-diagnosis support for children with Autism Spectrum Disorder, their families and carerskylie valentine and Marianne Rajkovic, with Brooke Dinning and Denise Thompson; Marianne Rajkovic, Denise Thompson and kylie valentine (2011)

36. Approaches to personal money managementThe Social Research Centre and Data Analysis Australia (2011)

37. Fathering in Australia among couple families with young childrenJennifer Baxter and Diana Smart (2011)

38. Financial and non-financial support to out-of-home carersMarilyn McHugh and kylie valentine (2011)

39. Community attitudes to people with disability: scoping projectDenise Thompson, Karen R Fisher, Christiane Purcal, Chris Deeming and Pooja Sawrikar (2012)

44. Paid Parental Leave evaluation: Phase 1Bill Martin, Belinda Hewitt, Marian Baird, Janeen Baxter, Alexandra Heron, Gillian Whitehouse, Marian Zadoroznyj, Ning Xiang, Dorothy Broom, Luke Connelly, Andrew Jones, Guyonne Kalb, Duncan McVicar, Lyndall Strazdins, Margaret Walter, Mark Western, Mark Wooden (2012)

107