Navegacion en Tortugas

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/29/2019 Navegacion en Tortugas

    1/9

    Anim Cogn (2009) 12:779787

    DOI 10.1007/s10071-009-0237-9

    123

    ORIGINAL PAPER

    Visual and response-based navigation

    in the tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria)

    Anna Wilkinson Sacha Coward GeoVrey Hall

    Received: 10 November 2008 / Revised: 4 May 2009 / Accepted: 6 May 2009 / Published online: 23 May 2009

    Springer-Verlag 2009

    Abstract Much research has investigated spatial cogni-

    tion in mammals and birds. Evidence suggests that the hip-pocampus plays a critical role in this; however, reptiles do

    not possess a hippocampus. It has been proposed that the

    reptilian medial cortex plays a similar role, yet little behav-

    ioral research has directly investigated this. Consequently,

    this study examined the role of extramaze cues in spatial

    navigation by the red-footed tortoise (Geochelone carbona-

    ria) using an eight-arm radial maze. In Experiment 1 the

    maze was surrounded by a black curtain on which geomet-

    rical shapes were attached. After the tortoise reached

    above-chance performance we introduced test sessions in

    which the cues were removed. Performance was unaVected

    by cue removal. The tortoise appeared to have developed a

    turn-by-one-arm strategy. In a second experiment the

    curtain was removed and the tortoise was allowed access to

    a rich-cue environment. The use of the turn-by-one-arm

    strategy was signiWcantly reduced and the tortoise appeared

    to be using the extramaze cues to navigate around the appa-

    ratus. This type of response-based strategy, and the speciWc

    contexts in which it was used, has not been observed in

    mammals and birds, suggesting that the mechanisms served

    by the reptilian medial cortex do not parallel exactly those

    of the hippocampus.

    Keywords Reptile Spatial learning Radial maze

    Extramaze cues Response strategy

    Introduction

    Studies of navigation in mammals and birds have shown

    that they are able to use a range of strategies to navigate to a

    speciWc goal. These include the use of a single salient cue

    (a beacon), path integration, learning a sequence of

    responses, and creating a spatial representation of environ-

    mental cues (a cognitive map). In the latter case the goal is

    deWned by its spatial relation to a number of diVerent land-

    marks; this may be seen as highly adaptive as the removal

    of any single landmark does not necessarily disrupt naviga-

    tion. This behavior is thought to be dependent on the hippo-

    campus (OKeefe and Nadel 1978). Evidence that

    demonstrates the use of this navigational system has been

    found in a variety of mammals and birds (see OKeefe and

    Nadel 1978 for a summary), and also, more recently, in Wsh

    (for a review see Broglio et al. 2005). As such it is likely

    that reptiles also possess a similar system, there is some

    evidence to support this (Lpez et al. 2000, 2001, 2003);

    however, its existence in this group is much less investi-

    gated and the Wndings are not clear cut (Day et al. 1999,

    2001).

    The study of spatial behavior in reptiles has a long history

    (e.g., Tinklepaugh 1932; for a review see Burghardt 1977),

    but little is known about the mechanisms underlying their

    navigational ability. It is still not clear whether reptiles are

    capable of the forms of spatial learning that are seen in

    mammals and birds. For example, Holtzman et al. (1999)

    found that corn snakes (Elaphe guttata guttata) could rap-

    idly learn the position of a hidden goal in an open Weld task.

    Their results (though not designed to test the underlying

    A. Wilkinson S. Coward G. Hall

    Department of Psychology, University of York, York

    YO10 5DD, UK

    A. Wilkinson (&)

    Department of Neurobiology and Cognition, University of Vienna,

    Althanstr. 14, 1090 Vienna, Austria

    e-mail: [email protected]

  • 7/29/2019 Navegacion en Tortugas

    2/9

    780 Anim Cogn (2009) 12:779787

    123

    mechanisms) suggested that the snakes used a beacon to

    navigate to the goal. However, very diVerent results have

    been found in lizards. Day et al. (1999) examined the diVer-

    ent spatial abilities of two closely related lizard species

    whose foraging strategies diVered. They found that neither

    species preferentially attended to either distal or local cues

    to navigate. The lizards (of both species) rarely approached

    the goal directly, suggesting that they found it through trialand error searching. This led the authors to conclude that, if

    spatial memory exists in reptiles it is fundamentally diVerent

    from that observed in mammals and birds (Day et al. 1999).

    Quite diVerent results have been found in the study of

    spatial learning in chelonian (turtles, terrapins, and tor-

    toises). Lpez et al. (2000) showed that the terrapin (Pseu-

    demys scripta) was capable of learning to approach a given

    location in a T maze, regardless of the starting position. The

    terrapins behavior appeared to be based on extramaze cues

    and removal of these (using a curtain to block oV parts of

    the experimental room) disrupted performance. The authors

    suggested that the terrapins were navigating using a cogni-

    tive map of the sort that is postulated in mammals and

    birds. Later Wndings revealed a Xexibility in the use of this

    strategy. Lpez et al. (2001) required one group of terrapins

    to navigate to a speciWc goal solely on the basis of distal

    cues; a second group had the same distal cues but also had a

    beacon (a large salient cue) that was located close to the

    goal. Both groups learned to successfully navigate to the

    goal, and probe trials revealed that only the group with just

    the distal cues appeared to make use of a cognitive map-

    like representation to do so. The beacon group did not use

    the distal cues and, instead, used the single beacon to locate

    the goal. This pattern of results suggests that reptilian learn-

    ing and memory capabilities (in terrapins at least) may

    closely parallel those observed in mammals and birds.

    Further evidence to support this conclusion comes from

    studies of the cerebral basis of spatial navigation in terra-

    pins. It has been suggested that the reptilian medial cortex

    serves a parallel function to the mammalian hippocampus

    (e.g., MacPhail 1982; Lpez et al. 2003). Experiments by

    Lpez et al. (2003) revealed that lesions to the medial cor-

    tex of terrapins caused a change in performance on an open

    Weld task when the animals had to use distal landmarks to

    navigate to a single goal. After a number of post-operative

    training sessions the lesioned animals were able to learn to

    navigate to the correct location, with performance equaling

    their pre-operative level (and the level shown by sham-

    operated animals). Probe trials revealed that the lesioned

    terrapins used a modiWed beacon strategy to locate the goal,

    whereas the sham-operated animals used a map-like strat-

    egy. These results closely parallel those found in mammals

    and birds and further suggest that the medial cortex per-

    forms a similar function to the mammalian/avian hippo-

    campus.

    This set of results suggests that reptiles (or at least terra-

    pins) are capable of at least some basic spatial tasks, and

    that the medial cortex plays an important role in this. How-

    ever, little is known about the limits of their spatial ability.

    Previous studies have tested the ability to navigate to one

    speciWc area in which the animals were rewarded; however,

    a more natural task would examine their ability to remem-

    ber numerous places that they have previously visited toensure that they do not return to an area in which a food

    source has already been depleted. The classic task for this

    test is the radial arm maze.

    In a recent investigation, Wilkinson et al. (2007) exam-

    ined the ability of a single red-footed tortoise (Geochelone

    carbonaria) to navigate in an eight-arm radial maze. The

    tortoise showed reliable, above-chance performance, pref-

    erentially choosing baited arms rather than returning to

    arms previously visited within a trial. Tests ruled out the

    use of olfactory cues from either the bait (strawberries), or

    from the avoidance of odor trails. This suggests that, in

    spite of diVerences in brain structure, the tortoise showed

    spatial learning abilities comparable to those observed in

    mammals.

    The cues that controlled this behavior remain to be deter-

    mined. As suggested by Lpez et al. (2000, 2001) for terra-

    pins, it is possible that the tortoise is able to identify spatial

    locations by the conWguration of visible extramaze cues that

    deWne them. But diVerences in the behavioral ecology of ter-

    rapins and tortoises suggest that it may be unwise to assume

    that the same mechanisms are used by both. Research with

    corvids has revealed the importance of behavioral ecology

    in spatial navigation (for a review see Balda and Kamil

    2006). Very diVerent spatial abilities have been observed in

    closely related species, and species that are phylogenetically

    disparate but inhabit similar ecological niches have devel-

    oped similar (highly specialized) spatial memory systems

    (Kamil et al. 1994). The behavioral ecology of the red-

    footed tortoise is quite diVerent from that of Lpezs terra-

    pins; the former actively forage for fallen fruit and Xowers

    (Strong 2005; Strong and Fragoso 2006), whereas the terra-

    pin is largely carnivorous whilst young, but as an adult eats

    largely plant matter (Hart 1983). Moreover, the habitat

    occupied by the two study species is very diVerent. The red-

    footed tortoise is terrestrial and lives on the margins of trop-

    ical forests (Strong 2005), whereas the terrapin of Lpezs

    experiments inhabits areas surrounding freshwater lakes,

    rivers, and ponds. These diVerences, in conjunction with the

    diVerences found within the reptile group itself makes it

    possible that quite diVerent spatial abilities are present in

    these closely related species and that the performance of the

    tortoise may more closely approximate that of a mammal

    inhabiting a similar ecological niche.

    In order to resolve this issue, it is necessary to conduct a

    study in which the role of extramaze cues in controlling

  • 7/29/2019 Navegacion en Tortugas

    3/9

    Anim Cogn (2009) 12:779787 781

    123

    navigation in the tortoise can be investigated directly. In

    order to do this we returned to the basic procedure used by

    Wilkinson et al. (2007) in which the tortoise was trained in

    an eight-arm radial maze, but changed the nature of the

    cues available. In the previous study the maze was open to

    the room. In the present study (Experiment 1) we sur-

    rounded the maze with a curtain, but Wxed to this a set of

    cues (simple geometrical shapes) that could be manipulatedin a post-acquisition test, to determine the role of extramaze

    cues in controlling the animals behavior.

    Experiment 1

    In this task the animal was initially trained in an eight-arm

    radial maze. All arms were baited with food and we

    expected, on the basis of previous Wndings (Wilkinson et al.

    2007) that the tortoise would learn, in the course of a trial,

    to visit each arm just once. In our previous experiment the

    maze was open to the room. In this experiment it was sur-

    rounded by a curtain to which visual cues were attached.

    These cues were removed for the post-acquisition test

    phase.

    Method

    Subject

    A single captive-bred, red-footed tortoise (G. carbonaria) par-

    ticipated in this study. She (formerly referred to by Wilkinson

    et al. 2007 as he) was approximately 4 years old and her plas-

    tron (the lower part of the shell) measured 10 cm in length at

    the start of the experiment. The tortoise, named Moses, had

    previously been the subject of a similar spatial learning exper-

    iment (Wilkinson et al. 2007). A single animal was used in

    this experiment because it is hard to purchase animals older

    than hatchlings in the UK. In the Wrst years of life hatchlings

    of this species remain under leaf litter and move about very

    little. Moses was purchased by the Wrst author, from a breeder,

    when she was young. When not involved in an experimental

    session, Moses was kept in a 61 cm 30 cm 30 cm glass

    tank in an oYce adjacent to the experimental room. The oYce

    was kept on a daily 12L:12D cycle (light on 08:0020:00).

    The temperature within the tank was maintained at 29C

    (4C); humidity was maintained at 50%. Moses had access

    to food (fruit and vegetables) for 60 min each day; this was

    given approximately 30 min after the completion of the exper-

    imental session.

    Apparatus

    The apparatus was an eight-arm radial maze made of

    opaque black Perspex. Each arm was 18.5 cm long and the

    width at the arm entrance was 10 cm, and widened through-

    out the arm to reach 25 cm at the end; this allowed the tor-

    toise room to turn. The side walls of the maze were 10 cm

    high. All arms radiated out of a central octagonal platform,

    this measured 25 cm in diameter. During training and

    experimental trials white plastic bottle tops (measuring

    3 cm in diameter and 1.5 cm high) were placed at the end of

    each arm and used as food cups. The lip of the cup pre-vented Moses from seeing whether food was available until

    she had fully entered the arm.

    The maze was positioned in a small experimental room

    that was lit by two 60-W ceiling lights and maintained

    approximately 29C by an electric heater in the corner of

    the room. A circular black curtain (diameter 84 cm) sur-

    rounded the maze. The length of the curtain extended

    86 cm above the maze and obscured all visual cues from

    the surrounding room. A large circle of card (diameter also

    84 cm) was Wtted directly above the curtain; this obscured

    possible ceiling cues. The circular card had a central hole

    (26 cm diameter) and eight smaller holes (8 cm diameter)

    placed at regular intervals around the circumference, which

    provided light to the experimental set-up.

    To observe Moses movements in the maze, an NDS-27

    Panasonic video camera was positioned directly above the

    maze (through the central circle in the card). The underside

    of the camera (excluding the lens) and beams were covered

    by plain brown card to prevent them being used as a cue.

    The camera was connected directly to a video screen in the

    experimental room; this allowed the experimenter (who

    remained in the experimental room throughout the experi-

    mental session) to view the tortoises movements without

    interfering with the experimental set up.

    Four brightly colored shapes were pinned to the curtain to

    provide potential cues for navigation. They were positioned

    53 cm (measured from the Xoor of the maze to the center of

    the stimulus) above the maze and set at 90 intervals around

    the circular curtain. Their position on the curtain was at an

    equidistant point between two arms. This ensured that no

    single cue could be used to detect whether a speciWc arm

    had been visited, but a combination of at least two cues was

    required. The cues used were a yellow triangle, blue circle,

    red square (all 256 cm2), and a green cross (231 cm2).

    Procedure

    The experiment was run over a period of 8 weeks from July

    2007 to September 2007. All trials took place in the after-

    noon between 13:00 and 16:00, as this was the time that

    Moses was most active. Prior to each experimental session

    Moses was removed from her tank and handled for approx-

    imately 5 min. This increased her activity level. She was

    then placed into a wooden holding cage and transported to

    the experimental room.

  • 7/29/2019 Navegacion en Tortugas

    4/9

    782 Anim Cogn (2009) 12:779787

    123

    Moses was given 3 days of pretraining to ensure that she

    moved conWdently around and ate readily in the maze. Each

    pretraining session consisted of a single trial lasting 30 min,

    in which the tortoise was placed in the maze and allowed to

    explore. The tortoise had access to all of the arms through-

    out the experiment. On the Wrst day of pretraining, food

    (dandelions and strawberries) was scattered throughout the

    maze. For the following 2 days food was scattered only inthe eight arms. By the third day Moses readily entered and

    ate from all eight arms within the 30 min trial period.

    On each of the next 23 days of training, Moses received

    four trials separated by an intertrial interval of 5 min that

    was spent in the holding cage. Between each trial the maze

    was rotated by either 45, 90, or 180 to discourage the

    tortoise from using intramaze cues in conjunction with

    extramaze cues. During each trial, the experimenter

    observed and documented the tortoises behavior via the

    video monitor. Entering an arm that had not been entered

    previously was scored as a correct response. At the end of

    each daily session the maze was wiped clean; it was not

    cleaned between trials.

    In the Wrst phase of training there were 15 daily sessions.

    At the start of each trial Moses was placed in the central

    area facing a randomly selected arm. Each food-cup was

    baited with a piece of strawberry (approximately 2 mm

    wide and 3 mm in length). The experimenter recorded

    every arm that Moses entered. She remained in the maze

    until she had entered all eight arms or 30 min had elapsed.

    An arm choice was recorded when half of the tortoise had

    entered the arm (although Moses rarely backed out of an

    arm after entering that far). After 7 days of training, there

    were no obvious signs of learning which raised the concern

    that the position of the cues might be such that they were

    not easily visible to the subject. At this point, therefore, the

    vertical position of the cues on the curtain was changed.

    They were Wxed in a position so that the bottom of each was

    in line with the top of the maze wall (and 10 cm above the

    level of the maze Xoor). Moses received training with this

    arrangement for 8 days.

    At this stage a further procedural change was made in

    the hope of increasing the rapidity of learning. When train-

    ing rats in a radial arm maze it is common practice to

    restrict the number of choices allowed on a given trial (e.g.,

    Olton and Samuelson 1976). For the next phase of training

    we restricted Moses to eight choices per trial, removing her

    from the maze at this point (or after 30 min).

    After performing consistently above chance in the sec-ond phase of training in the controlled cue environment we

    tested the extent to which Moses used the extramaze cues.

    She received two test sessions. Each was made up of two

    test trials intermixed with two retraining trials. The Wrst and

    third trials of each session were the same as in the training

    sessions, but in the second and fourth trials the cues were

    removed. Moses performance was scored in the same man-

    ner as for a training trial. If performance was based on

    using any (or all) of the cues we would expect a total dis-

    ruption of performance in the test trials.

    Results and discussion

    For the Wrst 15 sessions of training the subject was allowed

    as many choices as were needed to complete the maze

    (enter all eight arms). We scored, for each trial, the number

    of correct choices in the Wrst eight. (On 14 of the 60 trials of

    this phase, the subject did not complete the maze within the

    30 min allowed; for these we scored the total number of

    correct responses made). Interestingly, performance on the

    incomplete trials was rarely less than on equivalent com-

    pleted ones. Figure 1a presents the daily mean scores.

    According to Olton (1978), the number of correct responses

    in the Wrst eight choices to be expected on the basis of

    chance, is 5.3. (Chance performance is computed assuming

    that every choice is made at random, without replacement).

    As the Wgure shows, mean daily scores diVered little from

    the chance expectation. The mean number of correct

    responses in the Wrst eight over all days of this stage was

    5.68; a one-sample t test comparing this score against

    chance expectation revealed that it was approaching, but

    Fig. 1 a Correct choices in the

    Wrst eight trials during training inthe Wrst phase of the controlled

    cue environment. b Correct

    choices in the Wrst eight trials

    during training in the second

    phase of the controlled cue

    environment

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    6

    7

    8

    0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16

    Session

    Correctchoicesinthefirsteig

    ht

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    6

    7

    8

    0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

    Session

    Correctchoicesinthefirsteig

    ht

    a b

  • 7/29/2019 Navegacion en Tortugas

    5/9

    Anim Cogn (2009) 12:779787 783

    123

    did not reach a signiWcant diVerence, t14 = 2.05, P = 0.06.

    As the Wgure shows, there was no trend toward an improve-

    ment over the phase; a separate test of performance over the

    last eight trials (for which the mean correct score in the Wrst

    eight choices was 5.88) again revealed no diVerence from

    chance, t7 = 1.44, P = 0.19.

    Figure 1b shows performance over the second phase of

    training, in which Moses was allowed only eight choicesper trial. She successfully completed a total of 31 out of 32

    possible trials in this phase. As the Wgure shows, perfor-

    mance was improved under this training regime. The mean

    number of correct choices per trial over all 8 days was

    6.34, a score that diVered from chance t7 = 4.72, P = 0.002.

    The mean score for the last eight trials was 6.63 which

    again diVered from chance expectation t7 = 5.04,

    P = 0.002.

    Table 1 shows the number of correct choices made on

    each trial of the test phase. It is evident that removing the

    cues produced no disruption of performance. Moses scored

    on average 7.25 (out of 8) on the test trials and 7 on the

    intermixed retraining trials. A one-sample t test revealed

    that both these measures were signiWcantly above chance

    (5.3), t3 = 4.07, P = 0.03 and t3 = 4.16, P = 0.03, respec-

    tively. Furthermore, a paired sample test revealed that they

    did not diVer signiWcantly from each other t3 = 0.52,

    P = 0.64. This indicates that Moses was not using the avail-

    able visual cues to navigate the maze.

    Examination of the details of her performance within a

    given trial revealed that Moses behavior appeared to be

    determined by a simple, but highly eVective strategy, that

    of turning in a given direction and entering the arm next to

    the one she had just left. For each of the trials of the second

    phase of training we scored the number of consecutive

    choices, after the Wrst that were both in the same direction

    as the previous choice and were to an arm that was adjacent

    to it. Figure 2 shows these scores (mean values of the four

    trials in a day). The Wgure shows that the mean length of

    such a run increased dramatically over training; initially,

    the tendency to turn into an adjacent arm was rather low,

    but by the end of training the animal was regularly showing

    runs of four or Wve such turns. Analysis of the training data

    (comparing the Wrst four and last four sessions) revealed a

    signiWcant increase in the choice of arms that were adjacent

    to the arm that had just been left, and were in the same

    direction as the previous choice t3 = 5.16, P = 0.01. Further

    analyses revealed a signiWcant correlation between the

    mean number of correct choices per trial and the use of the

    turn-by-one-arm strategy r= 0.58, n = 32, P = 0.001. This

    suggests that this behavior did lead to greater success in the

    maze. Of the 32 trials in this phase of training, Moses was

    as likely to turn in a clockwise direction (61 choices) as in

    an anticlockwise direction (45 choices), t30 = 0.73,

    P = 0.47.

    This response strategy was maintained on the test ses-

    sion. For the four test trials with the cues removed, the

    mean run-length score was 5.5 and the mean score on the

    intermixed training trials was 4.5.

    In summary, Moses performed well in the radial arm

    maze and scored above chance for the entire second phase

    of training when the procedure limited her to eight choices

    per trial. This behavior was the result of a simple response-

    based strategy. This strategy was independent of the extra-

    maze cues as it was maintained during the test in which

    these were removed. Interestingly, she did not learn a turn-

    right, or turn-left rule; although within a trial she turned

    consistently in one direction, the choice of direction varied

    between trials. Making a choice that is one arm away from

    the previous choice is unusual in rats (Yoerg and Kamil

    1982). This is likely to be due to the constraints imposed by

    the tight turning circle. These constraints will no doubt

    apply to the tortoise as well as the rat (perhaps more so), in

    which case, the strategy adopted by Moses by the end of

    training was acquired in spite of constraints that would tend

    to oppose it.

    Table 1 Correct choices for each trial of the cue-use test

    There were two test days each containing two retraining and two test

    trials. Maximum score was 8

    Retraining trials Test trials

    7 8

    7 6

    6 7

    8 8

    Fig. 2 The number of consecutive one arm turns moving in the same

    direction in the second phase of the controlled cue environment and

    the test

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

    Session

    Thenumberof

    consecutive1arm

    turnsinthe

    samedirection

    Training Test

  • 7/29/2019 Navegacion en Tortugas

    6/9

    784 Anim Cogn (2009) 12:779787

    123

    In the previous study of Moses maze behavior (Wilkin-

    son et al. 2007) we found above-chance performance, but

    no indication that this was based on a response strategy.

    Exhaustive analysis of response sequences revealed only a

    slight tendency to preferentially choose arms that were two

    away from that being exited. There was no tendency to

    choose adjacent arms. The diVerence between the results of

    the two studies must be assumed to lie in some feature ofthe diVering procedures. The most obvious diVerence is that

    Wilkinson et al. (2007) gave access to a rich visual environ-

    ment (the room). In the present experiment the only extra-

    maze cues available were those attached to the curtain

    surrounding the maze. Although tortoises have good color

    vision and are able to distinguish the colors that were used

    as cues (Quaranta 1952) the positioning of the cues with

    respect to the maze and to each other, or the overall sparse-

    ness of the environment (not only were there were less fea-

    tures available, but there was also no geometric information

    provided by the room) may have prevented the tortoise

    from using these cues successfully. The use of a response-

    based strategy may have been dictated by the unavailability

    of appropriate extramaze cues.

    Experiment 2

    The Wndings of Experiment 1 present a stark contrast to

    those of our previous experiment (Wilkinson et al. 2007).

    We have suggested that the degraded nature of the visual

    cues in Experiment 1 may have prevented Moses from

    using them to navigate, and that the use of the turn-by-one-

    arm strategy was a result of this. If this were the case then it

    is possible that, were a full range of extramaze cues made

    available, Moses might stop using the response-based strat-

    egy and revert to using the extramaze cues to navigate. It is

    also possible, of course, that the development of the

    response strategy seen in Experiment 1 was the result of the

    tortoise having had extended experience in the maze.

    In order to distinguish between these possibilities we

    conducted a further study in which Moses was trained in a

    cue-rich environment that was essentially identical to that

    of the previous study in which she exhibited no turn-by-

    one-arm strategy (Wilkinson et al. 2007). If the former

    hypothesis is correct then we would expect the use of the

    turn-by-one-arm strategy to decrease when the tortoise has

    access to the full array of cues from the room. However, if

    the latter is correct then we would expect no change in

    behavior.

    Method

    This experiment began immediately after completion of the

    test phase in Experiment 1. The same subject was used in

    this experiment. All housing conditions remained the same.

    The fabric curtain was removed from the maze so that

    Moses was able to see the entire experimental room

    (described below). All other apparatus remained the same.

    This set up was almost identical to that used by Wilkinson

    et al. (2007). External cues that were, in principle, visible

    from inside the maze included shelving upon which labora-

    tory equipment was stored, a large poster, the experimenter,and a black door (for exact details please see Wilkinson

    et al. 2007). The experimenter could now observe the tor-

    toise directly, but continued to make behavioral observa-

    tions via the television screen to ensure consistency over

    the two experiments. In all other ways, the procedure was

    the same as described for the second phase of training for

    Experiment 1. Moses received four trials per session. There

    were 14 daily sessions of training and Moses completed 53

    out of the 56 trials.

    Results and discussion

    Figure 3 shows the mean number of correct choices per

    trial for each training session in the cue rich environment.

    It is evident that performance remained above chance

    throughout; comparison of the overall mean for each ses-

    sion with chance expectation showed a signiWcant diVer-

    ence, t13 = 6.64, P < 0.001. But analysis of the detailed

    pattern of responding suggests that the basis of the perfor-

    mance changed over the course of training. As for Experi-

    ment 1 we analyzed, for each trial, the number of

    consecutive selections of an adjacent arm in the same

    direction of movement. Mean daily scores are shown in

    Fig. 4. It is clear that use of the turn-by-one-arm strategy

    declined across days; comparing the mean score for the

    Wrst four and last four sessions revealed a signiWcant

    Fig. 3 Correct choices in the Wrst eight trials in the cue rich training

    environment

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    6

    7

    8

    0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14

    Session

    Correctchoices

    inthefirsteight

  • 7/29/2019 Navegacion en Tortugas

    7/9

    Anim Cogn (2009) 12:779787 785

    123

    diVerence, t3 = 7.13, P = 0.006. Further analysis revealed

    that in the Wrst four sessions accuracy correlated with the

    use of the turn-by-one-arm strategy r= 0.63, n = 16,

    P = 0.009; however, in the last four sessions when use of

    this strategy was reduced to chance levels, success no

    longer signiWcantly correlated with strategy use r= 0.33,

    n = 16, P = 0.22. In fact, by the Wnal session of training

    the animals mean run length for turns into the adjacent

    arm was just 0.75; nonetheless, overall navigational accu-

    racy remained above chance.

    This change in performance indicates that the response-

    based strategy developed in Experiment 1 was a conse-

    quence of the particular cue environment used in that study;

    when the full set of extramaze cues was made available the

    animal reverted to behavior like that previously seen in the

    study by Wilkinson et al. (2007). It is not clear what ele-

    ments of the visual environment are relevant to the change

    in behavior. It is possible that the turn-by-one-arm strategy

    was a response to the general poverty of the visual features

    within the environment; however, the introduction of the

    curtain also resulted in a change in the geometric informa-

    tion available. Geometric information has been shown to be

    a powerful environmental cue for animal spatial navigation

    (e.g., Cheng 1986, for a recent review see Cheng 2008).

    This experiment cannot clarify which environmental cues

    contributed to the tortoises change in behavior; however,

    what is clear is the intriguing Xexibility of behavior dis-

    played by the tortoise. Navigation on the basis of the visual

    environment appears to be in some sense the preferred

    option; but when appropriate visual cues are not available,

    the animal adopts a simple and eVective response-based

    strategy. When visual cues are made available this strategy

    is abandoned.

    General discussion

    Wilkinson et al. (2007) demonstrated eYcient radial maze

    performance in a tortoise. There was no evidence of a

    response-based strategy; rather the behavior appeared to be

    controlled by extramaze cues, paralleling that shown by rats

    in the same situation. The original aim of the present study

    was to examine directly the role of extramaze cues by train-ing the tortoise in an apparatus with a restricted range of

    cues that could be manipulated by the experimenter. This

    aim could not be fulWlled as, when trained in an impover-

    ished cue environment in Experiment 1, Moses learned to

    use a simple but eYcient strategy to navigate through the

    mazeto turn consistently in one direction, selecting the

    arm adjacent to the one that she had just left. In Experiment

    2, when the curtain was removed and the cue environment

    was comparatively rich (both in terms of features and the

    geometric information available), the use of the turn-by-

    one-arm strategy signiWcantly decreased. Accurate perfor-

    mance was nonetheless maintained. This suggests the

    presence of two navigational processes in the tortoise. The

    use of visual cues appears to be a default behavior which is

    used if it is possible for the tortoise to do so. The second is

    a response-based strategy that allowed the tortoise to navi-

    gate the maze in a highly successful manner. It seems that

    by surrounding the maze with a curtain and presenting only

    four distal visual cues in Experiment 1, the amount of

    visual information available was reduced to a level that pre-

    vented the tortoise from using the extramaze cues to navi-

    gate.

    This pattern of behavior is quite diVerent from that

    shown by rats in a similar situation. Rats in a cue-impover-

    ished environment (e.g., Mazmanian and Roberts 1983) and

    even blinded rats (e.g., Zoladek and Roberts 1978) do not

    normally exhibit such response-based behavior. An excep-

    tion is found in studies by Einon (1980) and Roberts and

    Dale (1981); both experiments observed response-based

    strategies in a cue-rich environment, and neither saw the

    dramatic changes in the behavior that were observed in our

    tortoise. However, in the latter experiment (Roberts and

    Dale 1981) the eVects of proactive interference between tri-

    als reduced over time, suggesting an improvement in the

    use of the response-based strategy. Einon (1980) systemati-

    cally examined the cause of the response-based behavior

    and found that it was only present in immature rats. This

    prompts the hypothesis that the strategy observed in the

    maze may be related to changes in foraging behavior over

    the life cycle. The nutritional requirements of immature rats

    (those required for growth) are quite diVerent from what is

    needed to maintain health at maturity (Clarke 1980). The

    diet of wild-living rats (Rattus rattus), reXects this and

    because of natural variation in the distributions of food

    types, wild foraging behavior does change with age (Clarke

    Fig. 4 The number of consecutive one arm turns moving in the same

    direction in the cue rich environment

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14

    Session

    Thenumberof

    consecutive1arm

    turnsinthe

    samedirection

  • 7/29/2019 Navegacion en Tortugas

    8/9

    786 Anim Cogn (2009) 12:779787

    123

    1980). But whatever is true of rats in this sort of analysis

    cannot account for our Wndings in the tortoise. Moses

    search behavior changed because of cue availability (or her

    ability to use the cues that were available); it was not a

    result of changes in her development, suggesting that the

    processes involved in her search behavior diVer from those

    observed in rats (and other mammals).

    This response-based navigational behavior has also beenobserved in Wsh (Roitblat et al. 1982; Hughes and Blight

    1999). Like the rat studies (Einon 1980; Roberts and Dale

    1981), the former study did not take place under limited cue

    conditions. However, the latter study tested Wsh in both a

    cue-rich and cue-impoverished environment. The aim was to

    directly test the role of behavioral ecology in spatial search

    behavior. The authors used two species ofWsh (Spinachia

    spinachia and Crenilabrus melops) that inhabit highly struc-

    tured tidal zones in which visual cues are provided by rocks

    and weeds. However, rough weather frequently results in

    these features being obscured. Thus the authors predicted

    that the Wsh would be able to use both response-based and

    spatial cue-based navigational strategies.

    Their experiment goes some way to supporting our data.

    They found that in a cue-rich environment the Wsh were

    able to use cues to navigate; however, in the impoverished

    cue environment the Wsh used a response-based strategy.

    Interestingly, both species were signiWcantly less good in

    the non-cue than in the cue condition; this is quite diVerent

    from Moses behavior. Furthermore, their experiment was

    run between subjects, so it is not possible to know whether

    the Wsh would change from a successful response-based

    strategy to a cue-based strategy with a change in the cue

    environment. The authors interpreted their results as being

    due to the behavioral ecology of the two species that were

    tested (both lived in tidal zones). However, to our knowl-

    edge these speciWc tests have not been given to non-tidal

    species ofWsh, thus, it is possible that this behavior is com-

    mon to all Wsh (and maybe reptiles).

    The small amount of literature available on spatial

    behavior in reptiles does not provide any previous observa-

    tions of the animals using a response-based strategy to nav-

    igate. Such evidence as there is for the use of multiple

    memory strategies in chelonian comes from the study of

    Lpez et al. (2001) who found that terrapins were able to

    use both a salient (beacon) cue and a map-like representa-

    tion to navigate to a single goal. The animals in this set of

    experiments, however, were tested in a between-subjects

    design, which did not allow examination of the possibility

    of changes between these strategies in a single animal. In

    contrast, in our Experiment 2 we found that Moses showed

    a change of navigational strategy according to environmen-

    tal circumstances. It is interesting that she did not need to

    change her behavior to successfully navigate the maze in

    the cue rich environment; continuing with the turn-by-one-

    arm strategy would have maintained a high level of perfor-

    mance. The only comparable example of such Xexibility

    can be seen in the lesioned terrapins of Lpez et al. (2003)

    that had to re-learn to navigate to a goal post-operatively.

    The lesions appeared to prevent them from using the map-

    like representation that was observed in the training phase

    (and in the sham-operated animals), and thus they were

    forced to learn a beacon-type strategy to reach the goal. It isunclear, however, why the changes in Moses navigational

    behavior occurred. Our results suggest that the use of visual

    cues is a default strategy that will be overruled when eVec-

    tive visual cues are not available. It is not clear whether the

    same is true of the Wsh tested by Hughes and Blight (1999);

    however, there is a clear similarity in behavior between

    their Wsh and our tortoise.

    In sum, these Wndings suggest the presence of two pro-

    cesses that can control navigation in our tortoise. One

    appears to be based on visual cues. It is apparently similar

    to the cognitive-map mechanism employed by rats; but our

    attempt to investigate this directly in Experiment 1 was

    thwarted by the animals use of a diVerent mechanism. This

    second mechanism involves a simple response-based strat-

    egy of a sort not usually observed in mammals, but appears

    to be present in at least some Wsh. This pattern ofWndings

    suggest that when tortoises (or at least this tortoise) navi-

    gate in a situation with poor environmental cues they use a

    simple, but eYcient response-based strategy; but when

    more cues are available they switch away from this, and

    apparently navigate using the surrounding visual cues. This

    behavioral Xexibility may reXect the behavioral ecology of

    the speciWc species tested, or it may be true of chelonia and

    possibly reptiles and Wsh in general.

    Acknowledgment The participation of Sacha Coward was made

    possible by a bursary from the UK Experimental Psychology Society.

    The authors would like to thank Paul McLaughlin and Simon Fletcher

    for making the maze. We are also indebted to the Behavioural Neuro-

    science Research Group at the University of York and the Biology of

    Cognition Research Group at the University of Vienna for their helpful

    comments.

    References

    Balda R, Kamil A (2006) The ecology and life history of seed cachingcorvids. In: Brown MF, Cook RG (eds) Animal spatial cognition:

    comparative, neural, and computational approaches. Available

    on-line at: www.pigeon.psy.tufts.edu/asc/balda/

    Broglio C, Gomez A, Duran E, Ocana F, Jimenez-Moya Rodriguez F,

    Salas C (2005) Hallmarks of a common forebrain vertebrate plan:

    specialized pallial areas for spatial, temporal and emotional mem-

    ory in actinopterygian Wsh. Brain Res Bull 66:277281

    Burghardt GM (1977) Learning processes in reptiles. In: Gans C,

    Tinkle DW (eds) Biology of the Reptilia. Academic, London, pp

    555681

    Cheng K (1986) A purely geometric module in the rats spatial repre-

    sentation. Cognition 23:149178

    http://www.pigeon.psy.tufts.edu/asc/balda/http://www.pigeon.psy.tufts.edu/asc/balda/
  • 7/29/2019 Navegacion en Tortugas

    9/9

    Anim Cogn (2009) 12:779787 787

    123

    Cheng K (2008) Whither geometry? Troubles of the geometric mod-

    ule. Trends Cogn Sci 12:355361

    Clarke DA (1980) Age- and sex-dependent foraging strategies of a

    small mammalian omnivore. J Anim Ecol 49:549563

    Day LB, Crews D, Wilczynski W (1999) Spatial and reversal learning

    in congeneric lizards with diVerent foraging strategies. Anim

    Behav 57:393407

    Day LB, Crews D, Wilczynski W (2001) EVects of medial and dorsal

    cortex lessons on spatial memory in lizards. Behav Brain Res

    118:2742

    Einon D (1980) Spatial memory and response strategies in rats: age,

    sex and rearing diVerences in performance. Q J Exp Psychol

    32:473489

    Hart DR (1983) Dietary and habitat shift with size of red-eared turtle

    (Pseudemys scripta). Herpetologica 39:285290

    Holtzman DA, Harris TW, Aranguren G, Bostocks E (1999) Spatial

    learning of an escape task by young corn snakes (Elaphe guttata

    guttata). Anim Behav 57:5160

    Hughes RN, Blight CM (1999) Algorithmic behaviour and spatial

    memory are used by intertidal Wsh species to solve the radial

    maze. Anim Behav 58:601613

    Kamil AC, Balda RP, Olson DJ (1994) Performance of four seed cach-

    ing corvid species in the radial-arm maze analog. J Comp Psychol

    108:385393Lpez JC, Rodrguez F, Gmez Y, Vargas JP, Broglio C, Salas C

    (2000) Place and cue learning in turtles. Anim Learn Behav

    28:360372

    Lpez JC, Gmez Y, Rodrguez F, Broglio C, Vargas JP, Salas C

    (2001) Spatial learning in turtles. Anim Cogn 4:4959

    Lpez JC, Vargas JP, Gmez Y, Salas C (2003) Spatial and non-spatial

    learning in turtles: the role of the medial cortex. Behav Brain Res

    143:109120

    MacPhail EM (1982) Brain and intelligence in vertebrates. Clarendon

    Press, Oxford

    Mazmanian DS, Roberts WA (1983) Spatial memory in rats under

    restricted viewing conditions. Learn Motiv 14:123139

    OKeefe J, Nadel L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map.

    Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Olton DS (1978) Characteristics of spatial memory. In: Hulse SH,

    Fowler H, Honig WK (eds) Cognitive processes in animal behav-

    ior. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 341373

    Olton DS, Samuelson RJ (1976) Remembrance of places passed: spa-

    tial memory in rats. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2:97116

    Quaranta JV (1952) An experimental study of color vision of the giant

    tortoise. Zoologica 37:295314

    Roberts WA, Dale RH (1981) Remembrance of places lasts. Proactive

    inhibition and patterns of choice in rat spatial memory. Learn

    Motiv 12:261281

    Roitblat HL, Tham W, Golub L (1982) Performance ofBetta splendens

    in a radial arm maze. Anim Learn Behav 10:108114

    Strong JN (2005) Seed dispersal and the ecological implications of

    hunting Geochelone carbonaria and G. denticulate in Northwest-

    ern Brazil. Masters thesis, College of Environmental Science and

    Forestry, State University of New York, New York

    Strong JN, Fragoso JMV (2006) Seed dispersal by Geochelone

    carbonaria and Geochelone denticulate in Northwestern Brazil.

    Biotropica 38:683686

    Tinklepaugh OL (1932) Maze learning of a turtle. J Comp Psychol13:201206

    Wilkinson A, Chan H, Hall G (2007) Spatial learning and memory in

    the tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria). J Comp Psychol 121:412

    418

    Yoerg SI, Kamil AC (1982) Response strategies in the radial arm

    maze: running around in circles. Anim Learn Behav 10:530534

    Zoladek L, Roberts WA (1978) The sensory basis of spatial memory in

    the rat. Anim Learn Behav 6:7781