14
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting: Antecedents and Consequences ABSTRACT: Based on Self-Determination Theory _SDT; Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_, the present research proposes and tests a motivation-based model of participation in budgeting that distinguishes among intrinsic motivation, autonomous extrinsic motivation, and controlled extrinsic motivation for participative budgeting. The proposed model was tested using a survey conducted among managers of an international bank. The results suggest that while intrinsic motivation and autonomous extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting are positively related to performance, controlled extrinsic motivation is negatively associated with performance. These findings highlight the importance of distinguishing among various forms of motivation in participative budgeting research and suggest that the mechanism by which the information benefits of participation in budgeting are obtained may be more complex than assumed. The results also provide evidence of the viability of using the proposed model to study commonly assumed reasons for participative budgeting within a general theoretically based framework of motivation. INTRODUCTION Following the work of motivation theorists _e.g., Lepper et al. 1973; Deci 1975; Lepper and Greene 1978; Deci and Ryan 1985_, the present research proposes and tests a motivationbased model of participation in budgeting _PB_. The current study distinguishes between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation for PB. Individuals are intrinsically motivated if they view PB as an end in itself . For example, they may derive a sense of accomplishment and satisfaction from the mere act of participating. On the other hand, they are extrinsically motivated if they participate as a means to achieve some separable objective. Based on Self-Determination Theory _SDT; Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_, the present study further distinguishes between autonomous and controlled forms of extrinsic motivation. In the present context, individuals would be motivated by autonomous extrinsic motivation if they truly identified with the value of PB, whereas they would be motivated by controlled extrinsic motivation if they participated because they were pressured by an external force _e.g., superior’s demand_ or an internal force _e.g., their own sense of anxiety_. Research _Vallerand 1997; Gagné and Deci 2005_ shows that unlike intrinsic motivation and autonomous forms of extrinsic motivation, controlled forms of extrinsic motivation can produce negative consequences. The present research is motivated by several factors. First, considering the extant motivation research literature _see, for example, Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_, a review of prior studies in PB _see, for example, Covaleski et al. 2003; Shields and Shields 1998_ highlights the inadequate use of the term “motivation.” In particular, research in PB does not differentiate among different types of motivation. Distinguishing among various types of motivation is important since they have been shown to lead to different consequences _for a review, see Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_. The present study seeks to provide preliminary evidence of the significance of differentiating among intrinsic motivation, autonomous extrinsic motivation, and controlled extrinsic motivation in PB research. Second, in an analysis of published PB research Shields and Shields _1998, 57_ note that most studies do not explicitly state their assumed reasons for PB. They argue that, “a desirable, if not necessary, condition for research on participative budgeting to make more systematic progress in developing a general theory is to focus on understanding why it

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting: Antecedents and Consequences

ABSTRACT: Based on Self-Determination Theory _SDT; Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_, the present research proposes and tests a motivation-based model of participation in budgeting that distinguishes among intrinsic motivation, autonomous extrinsic motivation, and controlled extrinsic motivation for participative budgeting. The proposed model was tested using a survey conducted among managers of an international bank. The results suggest that while intrinsic motivation and autonomous extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting are positively related to performance, controlled extrinsic motivation is negatively associated with performance. These findings highlight the importance of distinguishing among various forms of motivation in participative budgeting research and suggest that the mechanism by which the information benefits of participation in budgeting are obtained may be more complex than assumed. The results also provide evidence of the viability of using the proposed model to study commonly assumed reasons for participative budgeting within a general theoretically based framework of motivation.

INTRODUCTION

Following the work of motivation theorists _e.g., Lepper et al. 1973; Deci 1975; Lepper and Greene 1978; Deci and Ryan 1985_, the present research proposes and tests a motivationbased model of participation in budgeting _PB_. The current study distinguishes betweenintrinsic and extrinsic motivation for PB. Individuals are intrinsically motivated if they view PB as an end in itself. For example, they may derive a sense of accomplishment and satisfaction from the mere act of participating. On the other hand, they are extrinsically motivated if they participate as a means to achieve some separable objective.

Based on Self-Determination Theory _SDT; Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_, the present study further distinguishes between autonomous and controlled forms of extrinsic motivation. In the present context, individuals would be motivated by autonomous extrinsic motivation if they truly identified with the value of PB, whereas they would be motivated by controlled extrinsic motivation if they participated because they were pressured by an external force _e.g., superior’s demand_ or an internal force _e.g., their own sense of anxiety_. Research _Vallerand 1997; Gagné and Deci 2005_ shows that unlike intrinsic motivation and autonomous forms of extrinsic motivation, controlled forms of extrinsic motivation can produce negative consequences.

The present research is motivated by several factors. First, considering the extant motivation research literature _see, for example, Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_, a review of prior studies in PB _see, for example, Covaleski et al. 2003; Shields and Shields 1998_ highlights the inadequate use of the term “motivation.” In particular, research in PB does not differentiate among different types of motivation. Distinguishing among various types of motivation is important since they have been shown to lead to different consequences _for a review, see Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_. The present study seeks to provide preliminary evidence of the significance of differentiating among intrinsic motivation, autonomous extrinsic motivation, and controlled extrinsic motivation in PB research.

Second, in an analysis of published PB research Shields and Shields _1998, 57_ note that most studies do not explicitly state their assumed reasons for PB. They argue that, “a desirable, if not necessary, condition for research on participative budgeting to make more systematic progress in developing a general theory is to focus on understanding why it exists.” Extant research _e.g., Vallerand 1997; Ryan and Deci 2000a; Gagné and Deci 2005_ asserts that individuals’ stated reasons for a behavior indicate the underlying motivation for that behavior. Consistent with that argument, the present research proposes that the stated reasons for PB reflect various underlying forms of motivation for PB. This enables the reasons for PB to be studied within a general theoretically based motivation framework. For example, the commonly assumed reasons related to individuals’ sense of personal satisfaction, feeling of accomplishment, and feeling of belonging and identification, as well as reasons related to goal setting and information sharing, all indicate the underlying motivation for PB.

Third, research in PB has generally been studied from the perspective of the firm and that of the individual employee _Covaleski et al. 2003_. Whether individuals’ perspectives coincide with that of the firm is important. Consider, for example, the information sharing purpose of PB. On one hand, from the firm’s perspective, information sharing is a main reason for PB _Shields and Shields 1998_ and a major mechanism by which the benefits of PB are achieved _Covaleski et al. 2003_. In particular, according to economic models of PB, management induces the subordinate to reveal private information in order to allocate resources more efficiently _Baiman and Evans 1983; Penno 1984_. On the other hand, from the individual subordinates’ perspective, research in motivation _e.g., Vallerand 1997; Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_

Page 2: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

predicts that inducing employees to share information may lead to the controlled form of extrinsic motivation for PB, which may have possible negative consequences. To the extent that the two perspectives do not match, the intended benefits of PB are less likely to be obtained. The present study provides preliminary insights into the extent to which individuals’ perspective relating to the commonly assumed information sharing reason corresponds with that of the firm.

In summary, the present research examines PB from the perspective of the individual participant. It integrates the commonly assumed reasons for PB within a single framework based on motivation theory. Specifically, it studies those reasons for PB in terms of intrinsic motivation, autonomous extrinsic motivation, and controlled extrinsic motivation. The current research also examines antecedents and the consequence of different forms of motivation for PB. Based on the motivation and the PB research literature, individual variables _e.g., organization commitment_ and situational variables _e.g., environment dynamism_ are postulated as antecedents of individuals’ different types of motivation for PB. The different types of motivation are in turn predicted to influence important consequences _e.g., individuals’ performance_.

The proposed model was tested using a survey conducted among managers of an international bank. The results suggest that individuals can be both intrinsically motivated and extrinsically motivated to participate in the budgeting process. Specifically, both intrinsic motivation and autonomous extrinsic motivation are positively correlated with PB. Moreover, while intrinsic motivation and autonomous extrinsic motivation are positively associated with performance, controlled extrinsic motivation is negatively associated with performance. As for the antecedents, organizational commitment is positively associated with all forms of motivation and PB. Environmental dynamism, on the other hand, is negatively related to autonomous extrinsic motivation and PB.

The present study has implications for both research and practice. First, the proposed model enables the study and integration of research on the reasons for PB within a theoretically based framework of motivation. Second, the finding of a differential effect of intrinsic motivation, autonomous extrinsic motivation, and controlled extrinsic motivation on performance suggests the need for further examining the various forms of motivation for PB. Third, from a practice standpoint, the findings of the present study, which focused on individual participants’ motivations, indicate that their view of PB may differ from that intended by top management. In particular, this suggests that the mechanism by which the information benefits of PB are obtained may be more complex than assumed.

The remainder of this paper is organized as follows. The following section presents the theoretical background of the study. Next, a motivation-based model of PB is proposed and hypotheses are developed. The research method is then described, followed by the results and their discussion and implications.

THEORETICAL BACKGROUNDIntrinsic and Extrinsic MotivationMotivation theorists _Lepper et al. 1973; Deci 1975; Lepper and Greene 1978; Deci and Ryan 1985_ distinguish intrinsic from extrinsic motivation. In general, a person is described as intrinsically motivated if he or she performs an activity for its own sake, and derives pleasure and satisfaction from participating in the activity. In contrast, an individual who is extrinsically motivated performs an activity as a means to an end. He or she does not engage in the activity for the inherent pleasure that may be experienced while performing it, but instead in order to receive something positive or to avoid something negative once the activity is completed _Deci 1975_.

More recently, self-determination theory _SDT; Ryan and Deci 2000b, 2002_ has further differentiated among different forms of extrinsic motivation that vary in their relative autonomy or self-determination. Extrinsic motivation is of an autonomous type when behavior is performed out of choice because individuals value the behavior. In contrast, extrinsic motivation is of a controlled _i.e., nonautonomous_ type when one’s behavior allows for satisfaction of an externaldemand or reward contingency, or to avoid guilt or anxiety _Ryan and Deci 2000b, 72_.1 The distinction among the various types of motivation is important since research _see, for example, Vallerand 1997_ in general provides evidence of positive outcomes for intrinsic and the autonomous form of extrinsic motivation, and negative outcomes associated with the controlled form of extrinsic motivation. More specifically, Gagné and Deci _2005, 346–347_ note that with respect to performance, intrinsic motivation and autonomous extrinsic motivation are superior to controlled extrinsic motivation, especially on relatively complex tasks such as PB.

Reasons for Participation in BudgetingPsychology-based PB research _e.g., Ronen and Livingstone 1975; Hopwood 1976; Brownell and McInnes 1986_ generally considers three mechanisms by which the benefits of participation are achieved. These are value attainment, the motivational and the cognitive mechanisms. According to Locke and Schweiger _1979_,

Page 3: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

if employees are able to attain their values by participating, increased morale and job satisfaction will result. Values are broadly defined as what employees want. They can be tangible _e.g., money_ or intangible _e.g., self-expression_.

From the motivational mechanism perspective, participation may lead to employees’ increased trust and sense of control, more ego involvement, increased identification with the organization, the setting of higher goals, and increased goal acceptance. These factors presumably result in increased performance through less resistance to change and more acceptance of, and commitment to, specified goals _Locke and Schweiger _1979_. From the cognitive mechanism perspective, participation is seen as a conduit for information exchange, and it provides for better upward communication and understanding of the job and the decision-making process. These factors are believed to lead to improved performance by increasing the flow of information _Locke and Schweiger _1979_.

The foregoing discussion highlights three important points. First, the term “motivation,” as is employed in the participative budgeting literature, is inadequate since it does not differentiate among the different types of motivation. Second, it is unclear whether the information reasons as described in the participative budgeting literature reflect autonomous or controlled types of extrinsic motivation. According to SDT, information reasons would reflect controlled extrinsic motivation if PB is used by management to induce subordinates to reveal information that they otherwise would not _see Covaleski et al. 2003_. Third, the value attainment mechanism may reflect different forms of motivation depending on the specific value _e.g., money versus selfexpression _ sought by the individual. In light of the preceding three observations, the following section proposes a motivation-based framework for studying the commonly assumed reasons for PB.

A MOTIVATION-BASED MODEL OF PARTICIPATION IN BUDGETINGExtant research _e.g., Vallerand 1997; Ryan and Deci 2000a; Gagné and Deci 2005_ argues that the stated reasons for a behavior _e.g., PB_ indicate individuals’ motivation for that behavior. Consistent with that notion, the present research attempts to integrate the foregoing reasons for PB within a general motivation-based framework _see Figure 1_. Several features of the proposedmodel are noteworthy. First, central to the model is the distinction among intrinsic motivation, autonomous extrinsic motivation, and controlled extrinsic motivation for PB. On one hand, individuals may be intrinsically motivated to participate because they view their participation as an end in itself. For example, their participation gives them a sense of accomplishment and satisfaction. On the other hand, individuals may be extrinsically motivated to participate because they view their participation as a means to an end. This extrinsic motivation may be of the autonomous form if, for instance, PB is viewed as a means for the participant him/herself to set higher goals or goals on which he/she will be evaluated. In contrast, the extrinsic motivation may be of the controlled type if, for example, PB is perceived to be a means for management to obtain information from the participant.

1 According to SDT _Ryan and Deci 2000b_, autonomous extrinsic motivation consists of identified and integrated regulation. With identified regulation, behavior is performed out of choice because individuals value the behavior. In integrated regulation, individuals not only internalize the reasons for their behavior, but they also assimilate them to the “self.” In contrast, external and introjected regulations are controlled types of extrinsic motivation. In the case of external regulation, behavior is performed primarily to satisfy an external demand or to obtain an externally imposed reward contingency. In introjected regulation, individuals partly internalize the reasons for their actions although they have not accepted the regulation as their own. One is motivated by introjected regulation when one performs certain tasks to avoid a sense of guilt or anxiety, or to enhance one’s ego.

Second, the proposed model enables the integration and study of the reasons for participation described in the research literature _Shields and Shields 1998; Locke and Schweiger 1979_ within a theoretically based motivation framework. The present research specifically examines the commonly assumed reasons related to individuals’ sense of personal satisfaction, feeling of accomplishment, and feeling of belonging and identification, as well as reasons related to goal setting and information sharing. Following research in motivation, these reasons are presumed to indicate the form of motivation for PB.

Third, the model incorporates both antecedents and consequences of different types of motivation for PB. Consistent with the PB literature _Shields and Shields 1998_, the motivation-based model predicts an association between antecedents _e.g., individual and situational variables_ and motivation for participation, and a relationship between motivation for participation and consequences _e.g., performance_. Although the present study examined specifically organizational commitment, environmental dynamism, and performance, other antecedent and consequence variables can be explored using the proposed model.2 The remainder of this section develops expectations regarding the relationships between the three types of motivation and both their antecedents and consequences.

Antecedents and Motivation for Participation in BudgetingOrganizational Commitment

Page 4: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

Organizational commitment is loyalty to the organization, and has been defined as “the strength of an individual’s identification with and involvement in a particular organization” _Porter et al. 1974, 604_. Subsequent research _see review by Ketchand and Strawser _2001__ has distinguished between the continuance commitment and the affective commitment dimensions of organizational commitment. The continuance commitment dimension reflects individuals’ desire to remain with their organization because departing costs are high. In contrast, Ketchand and Strawser _2001, 223_ note that the affective commitment dimension reflected in Porter et al.’s _1974_ definition “is based on an individual’s emotional attachment to an organization formed because that individual identifies with the goals of the organization and is willing to assist the organization in achieving these goals” _emphasis added_.

Two key components of this definition pertain specifically to the different forms of motivation. First, according to Deci and Ryan _2000, 235_, “a secure relational base provides a needed backdrop for intrinsic motivation.” Individuals with high levels of organization commitment, and thus emotional attachment, are therefore expected to be more intrinsically motivated to participate.

Second, identification with demanded behaviors reflects individuals’ acceptance of the behavior as their own, i.e., the internalization of demanded behaviors _Ryan and Deci 2000b_. Hence, the degree to which individuals identify with the goals of the organization is expected to be positively associated with the autonomous form of extrinsic motivation.

The relationship between organizational commitment and controlled extrinsic motivation is less clear given that controlled extrinsic motivation consists of two types of behavioral regulation, i.e., introjected and external regulation _see footnote 1_. Whereas introjected regulation reflects some level of internalization of the demanded behavior, external regulation involves none _Ryan and Deci 2000b_. Studies by Gagné and Koestner _2002_ and Gagné et al. _2004_ find that organizational commitment is positively associated with introjected regulation, but negatively associated with external regulation. Based on the above, we propose a nondirectional hypothesis of the effect of organizational commitment on controlled extrinsic motivation. Finally, consistent with Clinton _1999_, who posits commitment as an antecedent of budgetary participation, it is expected that the stronger individuals’ identification with and their involvement in a particular organization, the higher the level of PB. Based on the foregoing, the related hypotheses are stated as follows:H1a: There is a significant positive association between organizational commitment and intrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting.H1b: There is a significant positive association between organizational commitment and autonomous extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting.2 Task difficulty was also measured as a third antecedent variable. It is omitted from the study due to the very low reliability of the measurement scale.

H1c: There is a significant association between organizational commitment and controlled extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting.H1d: There is a significant positive association between organizational commitment and the level of participation in budgeting.

Environmental DynamismEnvironmental dynamism is defined as the degree to which factors in the organization’s environment remain constant over time or continually fluctuate _Duncan 1972_. According to Ryan and Deci _2000b, 70_, “the inherent tendency to seek out novelty and challenges, to extend and exercise one’s capacities, to explore, and to learn” typifies intrinsic motivation. In the present context, it is posited that individuals’ intrinsic motivation to participate will be positively associated with environmental dynamism, to the extent that individuals view PB in a dynamic environment as novel and challenging.

The current study further posits that in high uncertainty situations caused by a dynamic environment, managers may feel pressured to participate in order to obtain information to better cope with unanticipated events _Brownell and Hirst 1986_. Consequently, the extrinsic motivation is more likely to be of the controlled than of the autonomous type. Finally, consistent with Shields and Shields _1998, 60_, who posit that environmental characteristics are antecedents of PB, we propose that environment dynamism is positively associated with PB. Accordingly, the related hypotheses are stated as follows:H2a: There is a significant positive association between environmental dynamism and intrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting.H2b: There is a significant negative association between environmental dynamism and autonomous extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting.H2c: There is a significant positive association between environmental dynamism and controlled extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting.

Page 5: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

H2d: There is a significant positive association between environmental dynamism and the level of participation in budgeting.

Motivation and Participation in BudgetingEarlier studies _e.g., Merchant 1981; Brownell and McInnes 1986_ have examined the effect of PB on participants’ motivation to work toward budget attainment. Prior studies _e.g., Mia 1988_ have also examined how work motivation serves as a moderator in the relationship between PB and performance. However, no prior study has examined employees’ motivation to participate in the budgeting process, which is the focus of the current research.

Although various forms of motivation may lead to different performance outcomes, research on motivation _Vallerand 1997, 279_ shows that those who are extrinsically motivated may be just as motivated to engage in a given activity as those who are intrinsically motivated. Hence, all three forms of motivation are expected to be positively associated with the extent of PB. The related hypotheses are:H3a: There is a significant positive relationship between intrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting and the level of participation in budgeting.H3b: There is a significant positive relationship between autonomous extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting and the level of participation in budgeting.H3c: There is a significant positive relationship between controlled extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting and the level of participation in budgeting.

Motivation and PerformanceThe proposed model posits an association between three forms of motivation for PB and the individual participant’s performance. Research _Vallerand 1997; Koestner and Losier 2002; Baard et al. 2004_ provides evidence of positive consequences produced by both intrinsic and autonomous extrinsic motivations. In contrast, a negative relationship is most often observed between controlled extrinsic motivation and performance. Consistent with those findings, the related hypotheses are stated as follows:H4a: There is a significant positive relationship between intrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting and performance.H4b: There is a significant positive relationship between autonomous extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting and performance.H4c: There is a significant negative relationship between controlled extrinsic motivation for participation in budgeting and performance.

Participation in Budgeting and PerformanceIn general, the relationship between PB and performance has been equivocal. This has lead to research that examines the extent to which the relationship is conditional on other budgeting and/or nonbudgeting variables. For example, studies by Brownell _1985_ and Govindarajan _1986_ provide support for the hypothesis that the relationship between participation and performance is contingent upon characteristics of the environment. The present study does not examine and predict the effect of contingencies. Consequently, the hypothesized association between PB and performance is based on the review of empirical evidence. A meta-analysis by Greenberg et al._1994_ provides evidence of a positive relationship between PB and performance. Similarly, a study by Wagner _1994_ finds that despite apparent inconsistencies in initial results of earlier reviews, research suggests that participation can have positive effects on performance. Thus, the following hypothesis is stated:H4d: There is a significant positive relationship between participative budgeting and performance.

METHODParticipantsThe survey was administered to 101 managers of a large international bank in Hong Kong. Because the sample is drawn from a single organization, external validity is limited. However, the focus of the present study was on the test of a general motivation-based model of PB. Thus, although the findings of the study may not be generalizable to other organizations, it provides control over the potential confounding effect of unobserved heterogeneous practices that may be present across different firms _Otley and Pollanen 2000, 486–487_.

Sixty-one percent of the participants were male. Total years of work experience at the bank ranged from four to 30 years. The participants held management positions in a variety of departments at the branch level. Total years in a managerial position ranged from less than one year to ten years. Participants’ age ranged from 26 to 55. None of the demographic variables was significantly related to any of the study variables.

The survey was administered during two sessions of a training seminar attended by the managers. There were 51 managers at the first session and 50 at the second. The participants were not compensated and

Page 6: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

participation was voluntary. Everyone who attended the seminar participated in the survey. No significant differences in responses were found between the two groups. Therefore, the data were combined for statistical analyses.

Measurement of VariablesOrganizational CommitmentOrganizational commitment was measured using the nine positively worded items from Mowday et al. _1979_. A review of organizational commitment research by Ketchand and Strawser _2001_ indicates that the nine-item version of the original questionnaire had relatively high reliability in prior studies _e.g., Price and Muller 1981; Blau 1987; Nouri and Parker 1998_, and it strongly correlated with the eight-item Affective Commitment Scale developed by Meyer and Allen _1984_. This is consistent with findings of others _e.g., Carsten and Spector 1987; Farkas and Tetrick 1989; Settoon et al. 1996_ that the six negative items in the original 15-item questionnairemeasure an intent-to-quit factor. The word “organization” in the original instrument was replaced by the word “bank.”3 Participants were asked to respond to questions related to the degree to which they feel committed to their organization _bank_. A seven-point Likert scale was used, ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” Higher _lower_ scores indicate a relatively higher _lower_ commitment to the organization.

Environmental DynamismThe instrument developed by Duncan _1972_ was used to measure environmental dynamism. This measure has been used in prior accounting studies including Fisher _1996_ and Chenhall and Morris _1986_. Participants were asked to examine a group of eight environmental characteristics generally believed to be crucial to decision-making.4,5 Two items related to government regulation _“impacts of government regulation” and “constraints from bank regulators”_ were excluded from the analysis because different branches of the same bank _all resided in Hong Kong_ presumably operate under similar regulatory conditions. For each characteristic, participants were asked to indicate on a five-point Likert scale how often they perceived the item changed. The scale items range from 1 _never_ to 5 _very often_. Low _high_ scores indicate a relatively static environment _dynamic environment_.

Participation in BudgetingThis study used the Milani _1975_ instrument to measure participation. Prior studies _e.g., Parker and Kyj 2006; Mia 1988; Brownell 1982_ report good reliability for this scale. Participants were asked to rate their level of budget participation under each of six items on a five-point Likert scale.6 The responses were coded so that higher _lower_ scores reflect a relatively higher _lower_level of participation.

3 Aranya and Ferris _1984_ and Ferris _1981_ made similar changes to the original questionnaire. For example, to measure commitment to the profession, the word “organization” was replaced by the word “profession” _Aranya and Ferris,1984_.4 Item 6 _negotiating_ was changed slightly to ensure the suitability of the instrument to bank managers. Specifically, “Purchasing, selling or contracting for goods or services, contacting suppliers, dealing with sales representatives” was changed to “Negotiating with customers on loans, deposits, etc.”5 Item 8 on the original instrument referred to “constraints from suppliers.” Since this does not apply to bank managers, that item was reworded to read, “constraints from bank regulators.”6 Prior to the study, we ensured that the stated organization policy of the bank and the organizational culture at the bank allowed and encouraged PB. It is worth mentioning, however, that the perceived level of support for PB might differ at the branch level, which would result in some variance in the level of PB across branches.

PerformancePerformance was measured using the self-rating instrument developed by Mahoney et al. _1963, 1965_. This scale has been used in many accounting studies including Hall _2008_, Parker and Kyj _2006_, Marginson and Ogden _2005_, Chong and Chong _2002_, Wentzel _2002_, and Otley and Pollanen _2000_. The instrument consists of performance measures along eight dimensions, as well as an overall performance measure. Participants were asked to self-rate their performance on a scale from 1 _low performance_ to 9 _high performance_ on each dimension and on the overall measure. According to Mahoney et al. _1963, 1965_, the eight dimensions of performance account for about 55 percent of managerial performance. In this study, the regression of participants’ overall rating on their ratings of the eight individual dimensions resulted in 78.2 percent explained variation. Thus, the overall rating for each participant was used as the performance measure.

Motivation for Participation in BudgetingThe scales for the different forms of motivation were developed following SDT researchers _e.g., Ryan and Connell 1989; Vallerand 1997_, who argue that the perceived reasons for engaging in an activity provide a valid measure of motivation. According to Ryan and Connell _1989, 750_, reasons are phenomenally accessible and they represent the primary basis by which individuals account for their actions. As such, they

Page 7: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

provide a direct means of assessing the perceived autonomy of one’s actions. Similarly, Vallerand _1997, 284_ argues that self-reports of individuals’ reasons for engaging in an activity provide a direct means of measuring motivation that is independent from its determinants and consequences.

In this study, the different forms of motivation were measured by assessing individuals’ perceived reasons for PB. A total of seven items _see the Appendix_ were used. The items were derived from the list of motivational, cognitive, and value attainment factors suggested by Locke and Schweiger _1979_ and Locke et al. _1986_. The three intrinsic motivation items referred to feelings of accomplishment, personal satisfaction, and belonging resulting from the mere act of participation. In contrast, the four extrinsic motivation items referred to participation as a means to an end. Autonomous extrinsic motivation was measured using two items that referred to PB as a means for the participant to set higher goals and to set goals on which he/she will be evaluated. The two controlled extrinsic motivation items referred to PB as a means for the participants to provide information and for the supervisors to better utilize information. Thus, while the autonomous items emphasized choice on the part of the participant, the controlled items implied external demands from the firm or the supervisor.

For each of the seven reasons, participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed that the item was a reason for participating in the budgeting process. Responses were measured on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1, “strongly disagree,” to 7, “strongly agree.”

RESULTSDescriptive StatisticsDescriptive statistics for the measured variables are presented in Table 1. For each variable, theoretical range and an actual range is presented, along with the mean and standard deviation. The correlation coefficients among the seven variables are also included in Table 1. As shown in Table 1, the mean Intrinsic Motivation was 5.31, the mean Autonomous Extrinsic Motivation _Autonomous-EM_ was 5.50, and the mean Controlled Extrinsic Motivation _Controlled-EM_ was 5.16. These means were not significantly different _F-ratio _ 2.17, p _ 0.12_. The two types of extrinsic motivation were positively correlated _r _ 0.32; p _ 0.01_, and Intrinsic Motivation was only positively correlated with Autonomous-EM _r _ 0.28; p _ 0.01_, but not with Controlled-EM _r _ 0.14; p _ 0.15_.

Statistical AnalysesThe model shown in Figure 1 was tested using the two-step structural equation modeling _SEM_ procedure recommended by Anderson and Gerbing _1988_. This approach has been employed in a number of accounting studies _e.g., Bamber and Iyer 2002; Chong and Chong 2002; de Ruyter and Wetzels 1999; Maiga and Jacobs 2005_. The procedure consists of first evaluating the measurement model to adjust for measurement error. This step provides evidence of the convergent and discriminant validity of the measures. In the second step, the SEM is then estimated based on the results of the measurement model analysis.

In order to achieve reliable maximum likelihood estimation, a sample-size-to-parameter ratio of 5 or more is recommended _Hayduk 1987_. Given the large number of parameters being estimated relative to the sample size in this study, the partial aggregation form of SEM was employed following de Ruyter and Wetzels _1999_, Settoon et al. _1996_, Bagozzi and Heatherton _1994_, and Williams and Hazer _1986_. According to that approach, the scale scores were used as indicators of the latent variables rather than the individual items. Specifically, the indicator for each latent variable was first computed by averaging the items for each scale, except for performance. Subsequently, each scale’s reliability and variance were used to incorporate measurement error into the SEM analysis.

Measurement ModelExploratory factor analysis. Given that the three measures of motivation for PB were developed specifically for the study, an exploratory factor analysis was performed to test if these three measurements represented three separate constructs. The principal-component method of factor analysis was used. The seven items were subjected to a factor analysis with varimax rotation. This yielded three factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. The results _see Table 2_ show that the three Intrinsic Motivation items _a, b, and c_ loaded on one factor, which accounted for 43.55 percent of the variance. Similarly, the two Autonomous-EM items _d and e_ loaded on a second factor, which accounted for 24.60 percent of the variance. The two Controlled-EM items _f and g_ loaded on a third factor, which accounted for 14.70 percent of the variance. In all, the threefactors accounted for 82.85 percent of the variance. The Cronbach alpha for the Intrinsic Motivation, Autonomous-EM, and Controlled-EM scales were 0.88, 0.89, and 0.56, respectively.Confirmatory factor analysis. Confirmatory factor analysis _CFA_ was first performed on each conventional measure, and then on the measurement model as a whole, to assess convergent and discriminant validity.

Page 8: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

The initial model for organizational commitment _OC_ did not provide an adequate fit _Chisquare _df _ 27_ _ 153.84, p _ 0.01; GFI _ 0.75; AGFI _ 0.59; NFI _ 0.82; CFI _ 0.84_. Following the re-specification procedure suggested by Anderson and Gerbing _1988_, one item _“For me this is the best of all possible banks for which to work”_ was deleted due to low standardized loading. The revised measurement model achieved an acceptable fit _Chi-square _df _ 17_ _ 41.67; p _ 0.01; GFI _ 0.92; AGFI _ 0.83; NFI _ 0.95; CFI _ 0.97_ with reasonable reliability _0.94_ and all item loadings over 0.50.

The initial model for environmental dynamism _ED_ did not provide an adequate fit _Chisquare _df _ 9_ _ 42.87, p _ 0.01; GFI _ 0.86; AGFI _ 0.67; NFI _ 0.47; CFI _ 0.49_. Two items _“Actions of competitors” and “Keeping pace with technological advances”_ were deleted due to low standardized loadings. The revised model provided an adequate fit _Chi-square _df _ 2_ _ 5.27; p _ 0.07; GFI _ 0.97; AGFI _ 0.87; NFI _ 0.88; CFI _ 0.91_. The reliability was 0.81 and the item loadings were all over 0.50.

The initial model for PB _PB_ provided an adequate fit _Chi-square _df _ 9_ _ 29.21, p _ 0.01; GFI _ 0.92; AGFI _ 0.82; NFI _ 0.93; CFI _ 0.95_. The reliability was 0.91 and the item loadings were all over 0.50.

A confirmatory factor analysis was then performed on all the measures using scale average score to determine the overall fit of the measurement model. All possible links were drawn between the seven constructs, which lead to a saturated model, i.e., degree of freedom was 0. The procedure suggested by Anderson and Gerbing _1988_ was used to assess discriminant validity of the seven constructs shown in the theoretical model. First, the correlation was fixed at 1.0 for pairs of constructs with high correlations. Following Bamber and Iyer _2002_, the correlations over 0.45 _OC→Intrinsic Motivation; OC→PB_ were fixed at 1.0. Then, a Chi-square difference test was performed to compare the constrained and unconstrained models. The Chi-square difference test showed significantly _ΔChi-square _ 8.78 _Δdf _ 2_, p _ 0.01_ inferior fit for the constrained model. This provides evidence of the discriminant validity of the constructs _Anderson and Gerbing 1988_.

Structural ModelTheoretical model fit. The decision-tree framework for the sequential Chi-square difference tests _SCDTs_ among nested models recommended by Anderson and Gerbing _1988_ was used to determine the optimal model. The results of the procedure are shown in Table 3.

The first step consists of performing a Chi-square difference test between the saturated model and the theoretical model _see Figure 1_. The results of the comparison showed significant difference _p _ 0.01_. The next step is to compare the theoretical model with the constrained model. When estimating the theoretical model, the coefficient estimate between ED and Controlled-EM _path coefficient _ 0.06, p _ 0.66_ and that between Controlled-EM and PB _path coefficient _ 0.06, p _ 0.63_ were not significant. Therefore, the theoretical model was compared with a constrained model where these two insignificant links were constrained to 0. The Chi-square difference between the two models was not significant _p _ 0.19_. The third step was to compare the constrained model and the saturated model. Although the result of the Chi-square difference test was significant _p _ 0.03_, the constrained model provides an adequate fit _Chi-square _df _ 7_ _ 15.88, p _ 0.03; GFI _ 0.96; AGFI _ 0.85; NFI _ 0.93; CFI _ 0.96_. Thus, the tests of the hypotheses are based on the constrained model _see Figure 2_ where the path coefficients between ED and Controlled-EM, and between Controlled-EM and PB were constrained to 0.7

Hypothesis tests. Standardized parameter estimates for the constrained model are used to test the hypotheses. The results are shown in Figure 2.

The standardized parameter estimates between OC and Intrinsic Motivation _path coefficient _ 0.65, p _ 0.01_, OC and Autonomous-EM _path coefficient _ 0.30, p _ 0.01_, OC and Controlled-EM _path coefficient _ 0.57, p _ 0.01_, and OC and PB _path coefficient _ 0.57,p _ 0.01_ are all positive and statistically significant. These results are consistent with H1a, H1b, H1c, and H1d. As noted earlier, the path between ED and Controlled-EM _path coefficient _ 0.06, p _ 0.66_ is not significant. The path between ED and Intrinsic Motivation _path coefficient _ _0.13, p _ 0.19_ is also insignificant. Thus, H2a and H2c are not supported. As predicted in H2b, the path between ED and Autonomous-EM _path coefficient _ _0.28, p _ 0.01_ is negative and significant. The path coefficient between ED and PB _path coefficient _ _0.19, p _ 0.02_ is significant but negative. This does not support H2d. Consistent with H3a and H3b, the path coefficient between Intrinsic Motivation and PB _path coefficient _ 0.24, p _ 0.01_ and between Autonomous-EM and PB _path coefficient _ 0.20, p 0.01_ are positive and significant. As noted earlier, the path between Controlled-EM and PB _path coefficient _ 0.06, p _ 0.63_ is insignificant. Thus, H3c is not supported.

Page 9: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

As predicted in H4a and H4b, both Intrinsic Motivation _path coefficient _ 0.35, p _ 0.01_and Autonomous-EM _path coefficient _ 0.32, p _ 0.01_ are positively associated with Performance. In contrast, the path coefficient between Controlled-EM and Performance _path coefficient _ _0.32, p _ 0.01_ is negative. H4c is, therefore, supported. H4d is supported as well. The path coefficient between PB and Performance _path coefficient _ 0.28, p _ 0.05_ is both positive and statistically significant.

7 According to the decision-tree framework proposed by Anderson and Gerbing _1988_, when the Chi-square difference between the constrained model and saturated model is significant, an unconstrained model _i.e., a model with additional paths drawn compared with the theoretical model_ may be identified to replace the original theoretical model. Since no additional meaningful path could be identified in our theoretical model _Figure 1_, we decided not to continue with such re-specification. This decision is based on Anderson and Gerbing’s _1988, 416_ recommendation that “respecification decisions should not be based on statistical considerations alone, but rather in conjunction with theory and content considerations.”

DISCUSSIONThe present study proposes a motivation-based model of PB from the perspective of individual participants. The model distinguishes among intrinsic motivation and the autonomous and controlled forms of extrinsic motivation for PB. In the tested model, associations are predicted between two antecedents _organization commitment and environment dynamism_ and participants’ arious forms of motivation for participation. The different forms of motivation are also predicted o be associated with individual participants’ performance.

The remainder of this section first examines the results and discusses their potential implications. he study’s limitations are then described, followed by suggestions for future research.

Motivation for Participation in BudgetingThere was no significant difference across the levels to which participation was motivated by ntrinsic motivation, autonomous extrinsic motivation, or controlled extrinsic motivation. Moreover, imilar to the findings of Ryan and Connell _1989_, the two types of extrinsic motivation were positively correlated. This suggests that individuals can be motivated to participate by autonomous and controlled motivation simultaneously. This is consistent with Levesque and Pelletier’s _2003_ observation among a significant percentage of their study participants.

Antecedents and Motivation for Participation in BudgetingOrganizational commitment was positively and significantly associated with all three forms of motivation, as well as with the extent of PB. This finding indicates that individual variables can be important antecedents of PB. While the focus of research on participative budgeting has primarily been on environmental, organizational, and task variables _Shields and Shields 1998_, the present study suggests that individual factors may be equally important antecedents. In addition, the positive effect of organizational commitment on controlled extrinsic motivation suggests that the items we used to measure controlled extrinsic motivation might reflect introjected as opposed to external regulation _see footnote 1_. Introjected regulation is in effect when the individual has taken in, but has not fully internalized the value of the behavior _Gagné and Deci 2005_. In the current context, managers may have partially accepted the value of PB as a conduit for the organization to obtain useful information.

Environmental dynamism was negatively, but not significantly, associated with intrinsic motivation. Recall that it was hypothesized that the association between these two variables would be positive to the extent that individuals perceived the dynamic environment as novel and challenging. One possible explanation is that this perception was counterbalanced by the contrasting view that a dynamic environment creates pressure for employees to participate in order to cope with the uncertainties of a constantly changing environment. In such a case, PB would be less likely viewed as autonomous, and intrinsic motivation would be undermined _Deci and Ryan 2000_. Consequently, the counterbalancing views may have resulted in a nonsignificant association between environmental dynamism and intrinsic motivation.

As hypothesized, environmental dynamism was negatively correlated with autonomous extrinsic motivation. However, it was not significantly correlated with controlled extrinsic motivation as we predicted. Another unexpected finding is that environmental dynamism was negatively associated with PB. The more participants perceived the environment to be dynamic, the lower was their level of involvement in the PB process. A possible explanation for these two unexpected findings is that participants may have a poor understanding of the environment when it is dynamic. Consequently, they may be less likely to participate because they do not have the necessary knowledge of the environment to make a useful contribution. Indeed, Locke et al. _1986, 70_ note that employees who find themselves in such a situation “will realize that they should not participate in the decision and will feel embarrassed or inadequate.” Similarly, Hopwood _1973_ argues that as environmental dynamism increases, the formal accounting system is less able to capture managerial behaviors that are needed to achieve organizational goals _p. 11_. Consequently, participating in the

Page 10: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

budgeting process _which is a formal component of management accounting system_ could be viewed as less useful when environmental dynamism is high.

Motivation for Participation in Budgeting and PerformanceThe findings suggest that both intrinsic motivation and autonomous extrinsic motivation were positively related to PB and performance. This is consistent with research _for a review, see Ryan and Deci 2000b; Gagné and Deci 2005_ that provides evidence that intrinsic motivation, as well as autonomous extrinsic motivation, produce positive consequences.

Controlled extrinsic motivation, on the other hand, was not significantly related to PB, and it was negatively associated with performance. The more participants were motivated by controlled extrinsic motivation to participate, the lower was the reported level of performance. These results indicate that there may be differences between the way individuals and the organization view PB. In particular, while the latter intends for PB to be viewed as a means to exchange information, employees in the current sample seem to view it as controlling. They were thus motivated by controlled extrinsic motivation, which negatively affected their performance.

The results of this study should be interpreted in light of its limitations. First, the scales for measuring different types of motivation were conveniently constructed using reasons for PB that were described in prior research. The three scales do not fully capture the breadth of those constructs. The operationalization of extrinsic motivation, in particular, was limited to only goalsetting _in the case of autonomous extrinsic motivation_ and information reasons _in the case of controlled extrinsic motivation_. Second, the study employed self-reported measures of motivation, participation, and performance. A multi-method approach would improve the validity of the findings. Third, the sample of participants was drawn from a single organization. While this provides a homogeneous sample and eliminates some of the noise associated with a crosssectional study, it also limits the generalizability of the findings.

The evidence pertaining to different types of motivations for PB suggests several possibilities for future research. First, the present study did not examine other common reasons for participation, such as to improve coordination between subunits or to reduce job-related tension _Shields and Shields 1998_. Future studies can investigate the extent to which these reasons reflect autonomous and controlled motivation, and their impact on performance. Second, the relationship between other antecedent variables and different forms of motivation can be studied. For example, future research can examine the impact of budget emphasis, information asymmetry, and budgetbased compensation on the different types of motivation. Third, Ryan and Deci _2000a, 64_ note that, “Given the clear significance of internalization for both personal experience and behavioral and performance outcomes, the critical applied issue concerns how to promote the autonomous regulation of extrinsically motivated behaviors.” Research can examine specific aspects of the participation process that lead to increased intrinsic motivation or more autonomous extrinsic motivation.

While further research is needed before definitive conclusions can be drawn, the findings of the current study suggest that studying the various types of motivation for PB may be useful for understanding its effectiveness, and the cognitive mechanism by which the information benefits of PB are presumably obtained may be more complex than assumed since it may reflect controlledforms of extrinsic motivation.

APPENDIXMeasure of Motivation for Participation in Budgeting*Below are a number of reasons why people participate in the budgetary process. Please indicate the extent of your agreement or disagreement to the following responses to the statement,“I participate ”a. Because participation in budgeting gives me a feeling of accomplishment.b. Because participation in budgeting gives me a great sense of personal satisfaction.c. Because participation in budgeting gives me a feeling of belonging and increased identification with the bank.d. Because participation in budgeting is a means through which I am able to set higher goals.e. Because participation in budgeting is a means through which I set the goals on which I will be evaluated.f. Because participation in budgeting is a means for me to provide information that is important for my job.g. Because participation in budgeting is a means through which my superior allows for better utilization of information on the job.

Page 11: Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Participation in Budgeting

* Participants’ responses were measured on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1, labeled “strongly disagree,” to 7, labeled “strongly agree.” Items a, b, and c are the intrinsic motivation reasons, d and e are the autonomous extrinsic motivation reasons, and f and g are the controlled motivation reasons.