1
Introns Illusions as neuro-signs Nicholas Wade One of the reasons visual scientists find illusions fascinating is that their pictorial simplicity belies the complexity of visual processing. Illusions can direct us towards neurophysiological interpretations at many levels, which I have called ‘neuro-signs’. The most useful visual illusions are those that point to the simplest neural level possible, that is, those that provide signs of neural processing at early stages of the visual system, such as in the retina itself. The region of the retina that can change the activity of nerve cells throughout the visual system when light falls on it is called the ‘receptive field’ of those nerve cells. Primate retinal ganglion cells have receptive fields that are concentric and antagonistic — if the centre of a receptive field is excited by light the surround is inhibited (called ‘surround inhibition’), or vice versa. Are there any visual signs of such neural antagonism? There could be. In the black and white grid pattern shown below, a variety of illusory dots can be seen at the intersections of the black and white grid. In the upper half, dark grey dots appear at the white intersections between the black rectangles, although a light meter would not register such effects. This is called a Hermann grid; its reverse in the lower half, where illusory light grey dots are seen in the black intersections, is called a Hering grid. In both cases, the dots at an intersection disappear as soon as it is fixated, but they remain at those nearby. The dots are more difficult to see at the smallest intersections in both grids. Because of this effect, it is thought the illusory dots might be related to the distance from the centre of the retina (or ‘retinal eccentricity’); receptive fields have been found to be much smaller around the centre of the retina than more peripherally. Indeed, the optimal separation for producing the illusory dots increases with retinal eccentricity in a similar manner to the increase in receptive field size. But why do the illusory dots appear at all? One suggestion is that concentric receptive fields of the same dimensions will be stimulated differently when they fall at an intersection between four rectangles than when flanked by two rectangles — there would be more surround inhibition at an intersection than elsewhere. This would make a light intersection darker than its neighbours and a dark intersection lighter. Such an interpretation does not account for the dots that are visible between rectangles that are simply outlines, although these dots are not as compelling. When coloured rectangles are placed on a grey background (see above), the illusory dots at the intersections take on the colour of the rectangles: they appear to be light green in the grey intersections of the upper half, and reddish in the grey intersections of the lower half. This is because some receptive fields at the retinal ganglion cell level display colour opponency — the centre can be excited by, say, red and the surround inhibited by green; others operate on blue–yellow opponency. Hermann–Hering grids provide signs of a link between visual perception and neurophysiology. The link is made at an early stage of visual processing — the differences in the activity of the retinal ganglion cells — with the assumption that the neural signals for these differences are preserved in their transmission to the visual cortex of the brain. Address: Department of Psychology, University of Dundee, Dundee DD1 4HN, UK. Magazine R439 This is the second in a short series of articles by Nicholas Wade, describing some common visual illusions.

Illusions as neuro-signs

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Illusions as neuro-signs

Introns

Illusions as neuro-signsNicholas Wade

One of the reasons visual scientistsfind illusions fascinating is that theirpictorial simplicity belies thecomplexity of visual processing.Illusions can direct us towardsneurophysiological interpretations atmany levels, which I have called‘neuro-signs’. The most useful visualillusions are those that point to thesimplest neural level possible, that is,those that provide signs of neuralprocessing at early stages of the visualsystem, such as in the retina itself.

The region of the retina that canchange the activity of nerve cellsthroughout the visual system whenlight falls on it is called the ‘receptivefield’ of those nerve cells. Primateretinal ganglion cells have receptivefields that are concentric andantagonistic — if the centre of a

receptive field is excited by light thesurround is inhibited (called‘surround inhibition’), or vice versa.

Are there any visual signs of suchneural antagonism? There could be.In the black and white grid patternshown below, a variety of illusorydots can be seen at the intersectionsof the black and white grid. In theupper half, dark grey dots appear atthe white intersections between theblack rectangles, although a lightmeter would not register sucheffects. This is called a Hermanngrid; its reverse in the lower half,where illusory light grey dots areseen in the black intersections, iscalled a Hering grid.

In both cases, the dots at anintersection disappear as soon as it isfixated, but they remain at thosenearby. The dots are more difficult tosee at the smallest intersections inboth grids. Because of this effect, it isthought the illusory dots might berelated to the distance from thecentre of the retina (or ‘retinaleccentricity’); receptive fields havebeen found to be much smalleraround the centre of the retina thanmore peripherally. Indeed, theoptimal separation for producing theillusory dots increases with retinaleccentricity in a similar manner tothe increase in receptive field size.

But why do the illusory dotsappear at all? One suggestion is thatconcentric receptive fields of thesame dimensions will be stimulateddifferently when they fall at anintersection between four rectanglesthan when flanked by two rectangles— there would be more surroundinhibition at an intersection thanelsewhere. This would make a lightintersection darker than itsneighbours and a dark intersectionlighter. Such an interpretation doesnot account for the dots that arevisible between rectangles that aresimply outlines, although these dotsare not as compelling.

When coloured rectangles areplaced on a grey background (seeabove), the illusory dots at theintersections take on the colour of the

rectangles: they appear to be lightgreen in the grey intersections of theupper half, and reddish in the greyintersections of the lower half. This isbecause some receptive fields at theretinal ganglion cell level displaycolour opponency — the centre canbe excited by, say, red and thesurround inhibited by green; othersoperate on blue–yellow opponency.

Hermann–Hering grids providesigns of a link between visualperception and neurophysiology.The link is made at an early stage ofvisual processing — the differencesin the activity of the retinal ganglioncells — with the assumption that theneural signals for these differencesare preserved in their transmission tothe visual cortex of the brain.

Address: Department of Psychology,University of Dundee, Dundee DD1 4HN, UK.

Magazine R439

This is the second in a short series ofarticles by Nicholas Wade, describingsome common visual illusions.