Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

motivation jurnal

Citation preview

  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    1/14

    Soc Psychol Educ (2011) 14:427439

    DOI 10.1007/s11218-011-9155-1

    Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and academic

    achievement among Indian adolescents in Canadaand India

    Shaljan Areepattamannil John G. Freeman

    Don A. Klinger

    Received: 30 November 2010 / Accepted: 20 March 2011 / Published online: 22 April 2011 Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2011

    Abstract The purpose of the present study was to examine the relationships among

    intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and academic achievement for the Indian

    immigrant adolescents in Canada in comparison to their counterparts in India. Descrip-

    tive discriminant analysis indicated that the Indian immigrant adolescents in Canada

    had higher intrinsic motivation and academic achievement than their peers in India.

    By contrast, the Indian adolescents in India had higher extrinsic motivation than their

    counterparts in Canada. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses revealed the positivepredictive effects of intrinsic motivation on academic achievement for both the Indian

    immigrant and Indian adolescents. While extrinsic motivation had a negative predic-

    tive effect on academic achievement for the Indian immigrant adolescents in Canada,

    it was not a significant predictor of academic achievement for the Indian adolescents

    in India. Implications of the findings for policy and practice are discussed.

    Keywords Intrinsic motivation Extrinsic motivation Academic achievement

    Indian immigrant adolescents Indian adolescents

    1 Introduction

    Childrens abilities and skills do not fully explain their academic achievement

    (Graham and Hudley 2005; Pintrich 2003; Schunk and Zimmerman 2007), suggesting

    that there are also other pertinent factors at play in determining childrens academic

    achievement. It is in this context that the noncognitive correlates of academic achieve-

    ment, such as intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation, have been found to play a

    pivotal role in childrens school achievement (see Areepattamannil and Freeman 2008;

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    2/14

    428 S. Areepattamannil et al.

    Areepattamannil et al. 2010;Becker et al. 2010;Lee et al. 2010). Furthermore, intrin-

    sic motivation and extrinsic motivation form the basis of some of the contemporary

    theories of academic motivation, such as self-determination theory (SDT; Deci and

    Ryan1985,2000,2008a;Niemiec et al. 2010;Ryan and Deci 2000a;Vansteenkiste

    et al.2008) and attribution theory(Weiner 1986,1995,2006). However, the contem-porary theories of academic motivation and the two distinct types of academic moti-

    vationintrinsic and extrinsicare the offspring of Western theorizing and research.

    Thus, most of the research pertaining to academic motivation and motivational beliefs

    involved Western, particularly American, participants (Klassen 2004; Klassen and

    Usher2010).

    Because modern education needs cross-cultural psychology (Triandis 2001,

    p. 1), it is of critical importance to broaden the purview of motivation theories

    and research to account for the experiences of culturally diverse groups (Murdock

    2009;McInerney and Van Etten 2004). However, concurring withKlassen (2004),McInerney (2007) posits that when motivational and learning theories are trans-

    ported to new cultural and social settings to understand and manage individual and

    group behavior, there might be a mismatch (p. 369). Therefore, more empirical

    research, examining the effects of motivational constructs on learning outcomes for

    school children across cultures, is warranted. Although there is an increasing body of

    research devoted to the study of academic motivation across East Asian cultures (e.g.,

    Liem and Nie 2008; Zhou and Salili 2008), no study to date has examined the interrela-

    tionships among intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and academic achievement

    for adolescents, particularly for Indian adolescents, in South Asia and Indian immi-grant adolescents in Canada. Hence, the objectives of the present study were to: (1)

    examine the intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and academic achievement of

    Indian immigrant adolescents in Canada in comparison to their counterparts in India;

    and (2) explore the predictive effects of intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation

    on academic achievement for the Indian immigrant adolescents in Canada, and for the

    Indian adolescents in India.

    1.1 Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and academic achievement

    Historically, motivation theorists and their motivation theories have treated motiva-

    tion primarily as a unidimensional concept (Deci and Ryan 2008b). Furthermore,

    several contemporary theories of motivation unduly emphasize the overall amount of

    motivation, and fail to differentiate the types of motivation (Deci and Ryan 2008a,b).

    Motivation, according to SDT, is multidimensional in nature(Deci and Ryan 2000),

    and the multidimensional motivation orientation encompasses three global types of

    motivation: intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation (Deci and Ryan

    1985, 2000, 2008a). Of these, intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation are the two

    primary types of motivated academic behavior (Cokley 2003).Ryan and Deci(2002)posit:

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    3/14

    Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and achievement 429

    motivation represents a prototype of self-determined activity, in that, when intrin-

    sically motivated, people engage in activities freely, being sustained by the expe-

    rience of interest and enjoyment. (p. 10)

    Thus, intrinsic motivation is the most self-determined form of academic motivation(Vallerand and Ratelle 2002). Initially, motivation theorists argued that intrinsic moti-

    vation was unidimensional in nature (Vallerand and Ratelle 2002). Later, Vallerand and

    his colleagues (Vallerand et al. 1989,1992,1993) proposed a tripartite taxonomy of

    intrinsic motivation: intrinsic motivation to know, intrinsic motivation to accomplish,

    and intrinsic motivation to experience stimulation.Vallerand(1997) differentiates the

    three types of intrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation to know refers to the desire

    to perform an activity for the pleasure and satisfaction that one experiences while

    learning, exploring, or trying to understand something new (p. 280). Intrinsic moti-

    vation to accomplish, on the other hand, refers to the desire to perform an activity forthe pleasure and satisfaction that one receives from accomplishing or creating new

    things. Finally, individuals who participate in an activity for the pleasure and satis-

    faction derived while experiencing pleasurable intellectual or physical sensations are

    intrinsically motivated to experience stimulation.

    A number of studies have demonstrated the innumerable advantages of intrinsic

    motivation in the context of learning. Intrinsically motivated students tend to have

    higher academic achievement (e.g.,Areepattamannil and Freeman 2008;Areepatta-

    mannil et al.2010;Gottfried et al. 2007), higher intellectual performance (e.g.,Got-

    tfried and Gottfried 1996, 2004), higher self-esteem (e.g., Deci and Ryan 1995), greaterpersistence (e.g.,Vansteenkiste et al. 2004,2006), less academic anxiety (e.g., Got-

    tfried1982,1985,1990), mastery-oriented coping with failure (e.g., Dweck 1975),

    enhanced deep or conceptual learning (e.g., Ames and Archer 1988;Grolnick and

    Ryan 1987; Vansteenkiste et al. 2004), creativity (e.g., Eisenberger and Shanock

    2003; Koestner et al. 1984), enhanced cognitive flexibility and engagement (e.g.,

    McGraw and McCullers 1979;Walker et al. 2006), enhanced subjective/psychologi-

    cal well-being (e.g.,Burton et al. 2006;Ryan and Deci 2000a;Sheldon et al. 2004),

    and less extrinsic motivation (e.g., Gottfried et al. 2005). Without doubt, intrinsic

    motivation would contribute to successful school performance(Gottfried et al. 2007,2008).

    Extrinsic motivation, by contrast, refers to a broad array of behaviors having

    in common the fact that activities are engaged in not for reasons inherent in them,

    but for instrumental reasons(Vallerand and Ratelle 2002, p. 42). Because extrinsic

    motivation is a multidimensional construct, SDT postulates that extrinsically moti-

    vated behaviors are characterized by four types of extrinsic behavioral regulation:

    external regulation, introjected regulation, identified regulation, and integrated reg-

    ulation (Ryan and Deci 2002). Moreover, SDT maintains that these four types of

    extrinsic behavioral regulation can be situated along a self-determination continuum,

    with external regulation representing a complete lack of self-determined motivation

    and integrated regulation representing the most self-determined form of extrinsic moti-

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    4/14

    and integrated regulation representing the most self determined form of extrinsic moti

    430 S. Areepattamannil et al.

    external to the individual. In other words, rewards and constraints regulate these

    behaviors. For example, a student who performs an activity to satisfy external demands

    (e.g., a tangible reward or punishment) or social contingency is externally regulated.

    Next along the autonomy continuum is the construct of extrinsic motivation-intro-

    jected regulation. These behaviors are controlled in part by the environment, but alsoby internal reward/punishment contingencies (e.g., ego enhancement, guilt, shame,

    or obligation). An example is students who perform their schoolwork because they

    do not want to let their parents down. Hence, introjected regulation encompasses a

    moderately low degree of self-determination.

    Further along the self-determination continuum, extrinsic motivation-identified reg-

    ulation refers to behaviors that are performed by choice because the individual judges

    them to be important. For instance, a student with identified regulation engages in a

    particular behavior as long as she or he perceives the usefulness or instrumental value

    of doing so. In contrast to introjected regulation, identified regulation encompassesa moderately high degree of self-determination. Therefore, identified regulation is a

    relatively autonomous form of extrinsic motivation. Finally, integrated regulation, the

    most developmentally advanced form of extrinsic motivation, refers to behaviors that

    are fully integrated into ones self-schema. Although integrated forms of motivation,

    like intrinsic motivation, are characterized by a sense of autonomy and freedom, they

    are still extrinsic because behavior motivated by integrated regulation is done for its

    presumed instrumental value with respect to some outcome that is separate from the

    behavior (Ryan and Deci 2000b, p. 62).

    Numerous studies have documented the effects of extrinsic motivation on learningoutcomes. Students who are extrinsically motivated are more likely to have lower

    academic achievement (e.g., Becker et al. 2010;Lepper et al. 2005; Wolters et al.

    1996) and to engage in surface learning (e.g.,Biggs 1991). Furthermore, extrinsically

    motivated students experience greater anxiety (e.g.,Wolters et al. 1996), less positive

    emotions in school (e.g.,Sncal et al. 1995), and have a poorer ability to cope with

    failures (e.g.,Deci and Ryan 2000).

    In summary, intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation are not incompatible and

    can coexist in different degrees (Covington and Mueller 2001). Guay et al. (2008)

    opine that student motivation is of high quality when primarily based on intrinsic,integrated and identified regulations, and is of poor quality when based on external

    and introjected regulations (p. 233). Thus, intrinsic motivation, unlike extrinsic moti-

    vation, is related to more positive academic and psychological outcomes (Reeve et al.

    2007). Therefore, it is critical to examine the relationships among intrinsic motivation,

    extrinsic motivation, and academic achievement for the Indian immigrant adolescents

    in Canada and Indian adolescents in India. The following research questions addressed

    the objectives of the present study:

    1. To what extent do the Indian immigrant adolescents in Canada differ from their

    counterparts in India in terms of their intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation,

    and academic achievement?

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    5/14

    and academic achievement?

    Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and achievement 431

    2 Method

    2.1 Participants

    The final sample consisted of 355 Indian immigrant adolescents in Grades 912 (179male, 176 female) from one of the provinces in Central Canada, and 363 Indian ado-

    lescents in Grades 912 (192 male, 171 female) from one of the states in India. The

    age of the Indian immigrant participants ranged from 16 to 19 years, with a mean age

    of 16.04 years (SD = 1.15). The age of the Indian participants ranged from 13 to 18

    years, with a mean age of 16.88 years (SD = .89).

    2.2 Data collection procedures

    After obtaining clearance from the university research ethics board, principals of five

    secondary schools in one of the states in India were contacted. Three of the five schools

    agreed to participate in the study. The participating students completed the research

    measures in the classroom setting. All questionnaires were administered in English,

    which was the primary language of instruction in the participating schools. Students

    were told that all of their answers would be confidential and that they did not have

    to answer any of the questions if they did not wish to. Participants were seated at

    individual tables to ensure privacy while they completed the measures. Data from the

    Indian immigrant adolescents were collected through the Indian community centers

    in Central Canada.

    2.3 Measures

    2.3.1 Demographic questionnaire

    The demographic questionnaire asked respondents to report their age, gender, and cur-

    rent grades in school. To measure academic achievement, participants were asked to

    report their overall school GPA on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = A (Mostly 90s) to5 = F (Mostly 50s). The scale was later reverse scored such that higher scores indicate

    higher grades.

    2.3.2 Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation

    Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation were measured using 24 items drawn

    from the Academic Motivation ScaleHigh School version (AMS;Vallerand et al.

    1992). The items were rated on a scale ranging from 1 (does not correspond at all) to

    7 (corresponds exactly). Separate exploratory factor analyses (EFAs) using principal

    axis factoring (PAF) with oblique rotation were performed for each sample to identify

    the measures. The latent root criterion (eigenvalue > 1.00) and an examination of

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    6/14

    the measures. The latent root criterion (eigenvalue > 1.00) and an examination of

    432 S. Areepattamannil et al.

    The internal reliability of the intrinsic motivation scale was .95 for the Indian immi-

    grant sample and .76 for the Indian sample. The internal reliability of the extrinsic

    motivation scale was .88 for the Indian immigrant sample and .80 for the Indian sample.

    3 Results

    3.1 Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and academic achievement

    of Indian Immigrant and Indian adolescents

    Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among the study variables are presented

    in Table 1. Because descriptive discriminant analysis (DDA) is sufficient to indi-

    cate both that group differences exist and precisely where they exist among the

    variables (see Huberty and Olejnik 2006; Sherry 2006), DDA was conducted in

    lieu of multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to identify the variables that

    best discriminate between Indian immigrant and Indian adolescents (see Table 2).

    The variables entered were extrinsic motivation, intrinsic motivation, and overall

    school GPA. Groups were Indian immigrant and Indian. The overall Wilks lambda,

    = .94, 2(3,N = 714) = 47.60,p < .001, 2 = .06, indicated that the two

    groups were statistically significantly different, and the resulting discriminant func-

    tion would be statistically significant.

    The canonical correlation (Rc), a measure of association that summarizes the degree

    of relationship between the groups and the discriminant function (Tabachnick and

    Fidell2007), was .25. Thus, the correlation between the grouping variable and thecomposite predictor variable accounted for only 6% of the variance (R2c ), suggest-

    ing that the canonical discriminant function did not discriminate between the two

    Table 1 Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among the study variables

    Indian immigrant Indian

    M SD Correlation M SD Correlation

    1 2 3 1 2 3

    1. Extrinsic motivation 5.20 1.09 5.35 .91

    2. Intrinsic motivation 4.97 1.39 .34** 4.68 .83 .57**

    3. Overall school GPA 4.12 .49 .01 .48** 3.79 .94 .17** .23**

    p < .01

    Table 2 Standardized discriminant function and structure coefficients for the two groups

    Variable Coefficient rs r2s (%)

    Extrinsic motivation .56 .28 7.84

    Intrinsic motivation 49 47 22 09

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    7/14

    Intrinsic motivation 49 47 22 09

    Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and achievement 433

    groups very well. Extrinsic motivation, F(1, 716) =3.90,p < .05, 2 =.01; intrin-

    sic motivation, F(1, 716) = 11.08,p < .01, 2 = .02; and overall school GPA,

    F(1, 716) =33.73,p < .001, 2 =.05 differentiated the two groups. Overall school

    GPA had the strongest correlation with the grouping variable (.83), followed by intrin-

    sic motivation (.47). Although extrinsic motivation statistically significantly separatedthe two groups, it had a loading of less than .40. For the substantive interpretation,

    loadings less than .40 are not interpreted (see Sherry 2006). Overall school GPA

    accounted for 68.89% of the variance in scores on the discriminant function, followed

    by intrinsic motivation with 22.09%. Thus, overall school GPA contributed the most

    to group separation, followed by intrinsic motivation.

    The group centroid means indicated that the discriminant function maximally

    separated the Indian immigrant group (.26) from the Indian group (.20). The dis-

    criminant function separated the Indian immigrant group from the Indian group on

    intrinsic motivation and overall school GPA, with higher scores characterizing theIndian immigrant adolescents (M =4.97, SD =1.39;M =4.68, SD =.83, respec-

    tively) and lower scores characterizing the Indian adolescents (M = 4.12, SD =

    .49;M =3.79, SD =.94, respectively). By contrast, the discriminant function sepa-

    rated the Indian immigrant group from the Indian group on extrinsic motivation, with

    higher scores characterizing the Indian adolescents (M =5.35, SD =.91) and lower

    scores characterizing the Indian immigrant adolescents (M =5.20, SD =1.09).

    3.2 Predicting academic achievement from intrinsic and extrinsic motivation

    To address the second research question, separate hierarchical multiple regression

    analyses were conducted for the Indian immigrant and Indian samples. Overall school

    GPA was the dependent variable. Extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation were

    the independent variables. The results of the hierarchical regression analyses for vari-

    ables predicting academic achievement are shown in Table 3. Control variables, gender

    and age, were entered into the regression equation in Step 1. In Step 2, extrinsic moti-

    vation and intrinsic motivation were added to the regression equation.

    For the Indian immigrant adolescents, the control variables entered into the regres-sion equation in Step 1 did not account for a statistically significant proportion of the

    variance in academic achievement, R2 =.01,F(2, 352) =1.09,p =.34. After Step

    2, with extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation also included in the equation,

    R2 =.26,Fchange(4, 350) =31.53,p < .001. Thus, addition of extrinsic motivation

    and intrinsic motivation to the regression equation resulted in a statistically signif-

    icant increment in R2. Whereas extrinsic motivation negatively predicted academic

    achievement for the Indian immigrant adolescents ( = .18,p < .001), intrinsic

    motivation positively predicted their academic achievement ( =.54,p < .001).

    For the Indian adolescents, the control variables entered into the regression equation

    in Step 1 accounted for a statistically significant proportion of the variance in academic

    achievement, R2 = .04, F (2, 360) = 6.67, p < .01. In this step, age was a negative

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    8/14

    achievement, R .04,F(2, 360) 6.67,p < .01. In this step, age was a negative

    434 S. Areepattamannil et al.

    Table 3 Hierarchical multiple regression analyses for variables predicting academic achievement

    Indian immigrant Indian

    B (SE) B (SE)

    Step 1

    Gender .07(.05) .07 .13(.09) .07

    Age .01(.03) .02 .14(.04) .17

    Step 2

    Gender .07(.04) .07 .10(.09) .05

    Age .01(.03) .01 .12(.04) .14

    Extrinsic motivation .08(.02) .18 .08(.06) .07

    Intrinsic motivation .19(.02) .54 .18(.07) .16

    For Indian immigrants: R2 = .01 for step 1 (ns ), R2 = .25 for step 2; for Indian: R2 = .04 for step 1,R2 =.04 for step 2.p < .05. p < .01. p < .001

    and intrinsic motivation to the regression equation resulted in a statistically signifi-

    cant increment in R2. Extrinsic motivation did not statistically significantly predict

    academic achievement for the Indian adolescents ( = .07,p = .24). However,

    intrinsic motivation positively predicted Indian adolescents academic achievement

    ( =.16,p < .05).

    4 Discussion

    The aim of this study was twofold: first, to examine the intrinsic motivation,

    extrinsic motivation, and academic achievement of Indian immigrant adolescents in

    Canada in comparison to their counterparts in India; and second, to investigate the pre-

    dictive effects of intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation on academic achieve-

    ment for the Indian immigrant adolescents in Canada and for the Indian adolescents

    in India. With regard to academic achievement, the Indian immigrant adolescentsin Canada outperformed their counterparts in India. Whereas the Indian immigrant

    adolescents in Canada were more intrinsically motivated than the Indian adolescents

    in India, the Indian adolescents were more extrinsically motivated than the Indian

    immigrant adolescents in Canada.

    One of the reasons for the better school performance of the Indian immigrant

    adolescents in Canada could be their higher intrinsic motivation and lower extrinsic

    motivation as compared to the Indian adolescents in India. Reeve et al.(2007) posit

    that students learn better and feel better when they pursue intrinsic rather than extrin-

    sic goals and when their teachers facilitate learning associated with intrinsic goals

    (p. 233). Given the numerous benefits of autonomy-supportive classroom contexts (see

    McLachlan and Hagger 2010; Reeve 2006; Reeve et al. 2007), the Indian adolescents

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    9/14

    McLachlan and Hagger 2010;Reeve 2006;Reeve et al. 2007), the Indian adolescents

    Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and achievement 435

    less controlling, wanting to support autonomy, and learning the practical how-to of

    classroom autonomy support (Reeve 2009, p. 172). Further research is warranted to

    identify the factors that cause differences in intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation,

    and academic achievement between the Indian immigrant adolescents in Canada and

    the Indian adolescents in India.The findings of the study indicated that extrinsic motivation had a negative pre-

    dictive effect on academic achievement for the Indian immigrant adolescents in Can-

    ada. In other words, adolescents who study for extrinsic reasons have poorer school

    performance than do adolescents with lower extrinsic motivation. However, extrinsic

    motivation was not a significant predictor of academic achievement for the Indian

    adolescents in India. The negative association between extrinsic motivation and aca-

    demic achievement is very well documented in the burgeoning literature on extrinsic

    motivation (e.g.,Becker et al. 2010;Lepper et al. 2005;Wolters et al. 1996).

    In congruence with previous research (e.g., Areepattamannil and Freeman 2008;Areepattamannil et al. 2010;Gottfried et al. 2007), intrinsic motivation had a positive

    predictive effect on academic achievement for both the Indian immigrant adolescents

    in Canada and the Indian adolescents in India. This finding suggests that opportunities

    to engage in intrinsically motivated learning may lead to better school performance

    on the part of adolescents. Hence, supporting intrinsically motivated learning is of

    critical importance.Reeve et al.(2007) contend that:

    Supporting intrinsic motivation means being attuned to students psychologi-

    cal needs and offering external events in ways that support students autonomy,

    competence, and intrinsic motivation. It also means findings ways to enrich

    learning opportunities, making them more interesting and relevant to students

    lives. (p. 234)

    In conclusion, the findings of the present study may provide empirical support

    that intrinsic motivation is a positive predictor of academic achievement for the

    Indian immigrant adolescents in Canada and for the Indian adolescents in India. Fur-

    thermore, the present study corroborates the negative association between extrinsic

    motivation and academic achievement. Indeed, more research revolving around the

    impact of motivational constructs on learning outcomes is required to unearth themeaning and implications of motivational constructs and motivation theories across

    cultures.

    4.1 Limitations of the study

    There are two limitations of the study. First, the measure of academic achievement

    in the study is based on student self-reports. Although self-reported grades are rea-

    sonably good reflections of actual grades for students with high ability and good

    grade point averages, self-reported grades are unlikely to represent accurately the

    actual scores of students with low GPAs and, to a lesser extent, low ability (Kuncel

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    10/14

    actual scores of students with low GPAs and, to a lesser extent, low ability (Kuncel

    436 S. Areepattamannil et al.

    extrinsic motivation, and academic achievement for the Indian immigrant and Indian

    adolescents in the present study may not be generalizable to adolescents across cultures

    in India.

    References

    Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students learning strategies and

    motivation processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260267.

    Areepattamannil, S., & Freeman, J. G. (2008). Academic achievement, academic self-concept, and

    academic motivation of immigrant adolescents in the Greater Toronto Area secondary schools.

    Journal of Advanced Academics, 19, 700743. doi:10.4219/jaa-2008-831 .

    Areepattamannil, S., Freeman, J. G., & Klinger, D. A. (2010). Influence of motivation, self-beliefs,

    and instructional practices on science achievement of adolescents in Canada. Social Psychology

    of Education. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s11218-010-9144-9.

    Becker, M., McElvany, N., & Kortenbruck, M. (2010). Intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation

    as predictors of reading literacy: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102,

    773785. doi:10.1037/a0020084.

    Biggs, J. (1991). Good learning: What is it? How can it be fostered?. In J. Biggs (Ed.), Teaching for

    learning: The view from cognitive psychology. (pp. 215230). Melbourne: Australian Council for

    Educational Research.

    Burton, K. D., Lydon, J. E., DAlessandro, D. U., & Koestner, R. (2006). The differential effects of intrinsic

    and identified motivation on well-being and performance: Prospective, experimental and implicit

    approaches to self-determination theory.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 750762.

    Cokley, K. (2003). What do we know about the motivation of African American students? Challenging

    the anti-intellectual myth. Harvard Educational Review, 73, 524558.

    Covington, M. V., & Mueller, K. J. (2001). Intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation: an approach /avoidance

    reformulation. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 157176.Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior.

    New York: Plenum.

    Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The basis for true self-esteem. In M. Kemis (Ed.),

    Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem(pp. 3149). New York: Plenum.

    Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The what and why of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-

    determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01.

    Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well-being

    across lifes domains. Canadian Psychology, 49, 1423. doi:10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.14.

    Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Self-determination theory: A macro-theory of human motivation,

    development, and health. Canadian Psychology, 49, 182185. doi:10.1037/a0012801.

    Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in the alleviation of learned

    helplessness.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 674685.

    Eisenberger, R., & Shanock, L. (2003). Rewards, intrinsic motivation and creativity: A case study of

    conceptual and methodological isolation. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 121130.

    Gottfried, A. E. (1982). Relationships between academic intrinsic motivation and anxiety in children

    and young adolescents. Journal of School Psychology, 20, 205315.

    Gottfried, A. E. (1985). Academic intrinsic motivation in elementary and junior high school

    students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 631645.

    Gottfried, A. E. (1990). Academic intrinsic motivation in young elementary school children. Journal

    of Educational Psychology, 82, 525538.

    Gottfried, A. E., & Gottfried, A. W. (1996). A longitudinal study of academic intrinsic motivation in

    intellectually gifted children: Childhood through adolescence. Gifted Child Quarterly, 40, 179183.

    Gottfried, A. E., & Gottfried, A. W. (2004). Toward the development of a conceptualization of giftedmotivation.Gifted Child Quarterly, 48, 121132.

    Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., Cook, C. R., & Morris, P. E. (2005). Educational characteristics

    http://dx.doi.org/10.4219/jaa-2008-831http://dx.doi.org/10.4219/jaa-2008-831http://dx.doi.org/10.4219/jaa-2008-831http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-010-9144-9http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-010-9144-9http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-010-9144-9http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020084http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020084http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020084http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.14http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.14http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.14http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012801http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012801http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012801http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.14http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020084http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-010-9144-9http://dx.doi.org/10.4219/jaa-2008-831
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    11/14

    Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and achievement 437

    through early adulthood. In C. Hudley & A. E. Gottfried (Eds.), Academic motivation and the

    culture of schooling (pp. 3639). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Gottfried, A. E., Marcoulides, G. A., Gottfried, A. W., Oliver, P., & Guerin, D. (2007). Multivariate latent

    change modeling of developmental decline in academic intrinsic math motivation and achievement:

    Childhood through adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 317327.

    Graham, S., & Hudley, C. (2005). Race and ethnicity in the study of motivation and competence. InA. Elliot & C. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 392414).

    New York: Guilford.

    Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy in childrens learning: An experimental and

    individual difference investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 890898.

    Guay, F., Ratelle, C. F., & Chanal, J. (2008). Optimal learning in optimal contexts: The role of

    self-determination in education. Canadian Psychology, 49, 233240.

    Huberty, C. J., & Olejnik, S. (2006). Applied MANOVA and discriminant analysis, 2nd edition.

    New York: Wiley.

    Klassen, R. M. (2004). A cross-cultural investigation of the efficacy beliefs of South Asian immigrant

    and Anglo non-immigrant early adolescents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 731742.

    Klassen, R. M., & Usher, E. L. (2010). Self-efficacy in educational settings: Recent research andemerging directions. In T. Urdan & S. Karabenick (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement,

    Volume 16A (pp. 133). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

    Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. (1984). Setting limits on childrens behavior:

    The differential effects of controlling versus informational styles on intrinsic motivation and

    creativity. Journal of Personality, 52, 233248.

    Kuncel, N. R., Crede, M., & Thomas, L. L. (2005). The validity of self-reported grade point averages,

    class ranks, and test scores: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. Review of Educational

    Research, 75, 6382.

    Lee, J. Q., McInerney, D. M., Liem, G. A. D., & Ortiga, Y. P. (2010). The relationships between

    future goals and achievement goal orientations: An intrinsic-extrinsic motivation perspective.

    Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 264279.

    Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in theclassroom: Age differences and academic correlates.Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 184196.

    Liem, A. D., & Nie, Y. (2008). Values, achievement goals, individual-oriented and social-oriented

    achievement motivations among Chinese and Indonesian secondary school students. International

    Journal of Psychology, 43, 898903.

    McGraw, K. O., & McCullers, J. C. (1979). Evidence of a detrimental effect of extrinsic incentives

    on breaking a mental set. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 285294.

    McInerney, D. M. (2007). The motivational roles of cultural differences and cultural identity in

    self-regulated learning. In D. Schunk & B. Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulated

    learning: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 369400). New York: Erlbaum.

    McInerney, D. M. & Van Etten, S. (Eds.). (2004). Big theories revisited: Research on sociocultural

    influences on motivation and learning. Greenwich, CT: Information Age.

    McLachlan, S., & Hagger, M. S. (2010). Effects of an autonomy-supportive intervention on

    tutor behaviors in a higher education context. Teaching and Teacher Education, 26,

    12041210. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2010.01.006.

    Murdock, T. B. (2009). Achievement motivation in racial and ethnic context. In K. Wentzel & A.

    Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 433462). New York: Erlbaum.

    Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2010). Self-determination theory and the relation of

    autonomy to self-regulatory processes and personality development. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook

    of personality and self-regulation (pp. 169191). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Pintrich, P. R. (2003). Motivation and classroom learning. In W. Reynolds & G. Miller (Eds.),Handbook

    of psychology: Educational psychology, Volume 7 (pp. 103122). New York: Wiley.

    Reeve, J. (2006). Teachers as facilitators: What autonomy-supportive teachers do and why their students

    benefit. Elementary School Journal, 106, 225236.Reeve, J. (2009). Why teachers adopt a controlling motivating style toward students and how they

    can become more autonomy supportive. Educational Psychologist, 44, 159175. doi:10.1080/

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2010.01.006http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2010.01.006http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2010.01.006http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520903028990http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520903028990http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520903028990http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2010.01.006
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    12/14

    ca beco e o e auto o y suppo t ve. ducational sychologist, , 59 75. do : 0. 080/

    438 S. Areepattamannil et al.

    Reeve, J., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Jang, H. (2007). Understanding and promoting autonomous self-reg-

    ulation: A self-determination theory perspective. In D. Schunk & B. Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation

    and self-regulated learning: Theory, research, and application (pp. 223244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,

    social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 6878.

    Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and newdirections. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 5467.

    Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). Overview of self-determination theory: An organismic dialectical

    perspective. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research(pp. 333).

    Rochester, NJ: University of Rochester Press.

    Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). Promoting self-determined school engagement: Motivation,

    learning, and well-being. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook on motivation at

    school (pp. 171196). New York: Routledge.

    Sncal, C., Koestner, R., & Vallerand, R. J. (1995). Self-regulation and academic procrastination.Journal

    of Social Psychology, 135, 607619.

    Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (Eds.). (2007). Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory,

    research, and applications. New York: Erlbaum.Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2004). The independent effects of goal

    contents and motives on well-being: Its both what you pursue and why you pursue it. Personality

    and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 475486.

    Sherry, A. (2006). Discriminant analysis in counseling psychology research. The Counseling

    Psychologist, 5, 661683.

    Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics, 5th edition. Boston: Pearson

    Education.

    Thompson, B. (2004). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis: Understanding concepts and

    applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Triandis, H. C. (2001). Modern education needs cross-cultural psychology. In D. M. McInerney &

    S. VanEtten (Eds.), Research on sociocultural influences on motivation and learning,

    Volume 1 (pp. 113). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In

    M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Volume 29 (pp. 271360). San

    Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Vallerand, R. J., Blais, M. R., Briere, N. M., & Pelletier, L. G. (1989). Construction et validation de

    lEchelle de Motivation en Education [Construction and validation of the Academic Motivation

    Scale]. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 21, 323349.

    Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., Blais, M. R., Briere, N. M., Senecal, C., & Vallieres, E.

    F. (1992). The Academic Motivation Scale: A measure of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in

    education. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 10031017.

    Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., Blais, M. R., Briere, N. M., Senecal, C., & Vallieres, E. F. (1993). On

    the assessment of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in education: Evidence on the concurrent

    and construct validity of the Academic Motivation Scale. Educational and Psychological

    Measurement, 53, 159172.

    Vallerand, R. J., & Ratelle, C. F. (2002). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: A hierarchical model. In

    E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 3763). Rochester,

    NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsicversus extrinsic: Goal contents in self-determina-

    tion theory: Another look at the quality of academic motivation.Educational Psychologist, 41, 1931.

    Vansteenkiste, M., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). Self-determination theory and the explanatory role

    of psychological needs in human well-being. In L. Bruni, F. Comin, & M. Pugno (Eds.),Capabilities

    and happiness (pp. 187223). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating

    learning, performance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents andautonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 246260.

    Walker, C. O., Greene, B. A., & Mansell, R. A. (2006). Identification with academics, intrinsic/extrinsic

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520903028990http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520903028990http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520903028990
  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    13/14

    Wa e , C. O., G ee e, . ., & a se , . . ( 006). de t cat o w t acade cs, t s c/e t s c

    Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and achievement 439

    Weiner, I. B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New

    York: Guilford.

    Weiner, I. B. (2006). Social motivation, justice, and the moral emotions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Wolters, C. A., Yu, S. L., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). The relation between goal orientation and students

    motivational beliefs and self-regulated learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 211238.

    Zhou, H., & Salili, F. (2008). Intrinsic reading motivation of Chinese preschoolers and its relationshipswith home literacy. International Journal of Psychology, 43, 912916.

    Author Biographies

    Shaljan Areepattamannil is a PhD candidate in educational psychology at the Faculty of Education,

    Queens University, Canada. His research focuses on better understanding the psychological and environ-

    mental factors that impact the academic achievement of adolescents across cultures.

    John G. Freemanis an associate professor at the Faculty of Education, Queens University, with a cross-

    appointment to theSchool of HealthSciencesand Kinesiology andDirector of theSocial Program EvaluationGroup (SPEG). His research and that of the students he supervises focuses primarily on how schools can be

    made more welcoming places for students, teachers, and parents, regardless of ability level, race, sexuality,

    or emotional health (among other considerations). As Director of SPEG, he hopes to build rich research

    partnerships between local community partners and Queens University.

    Don A. Klinger is an associate professor in assessment and evaluation at the Faculty of Education at

    Queens University, Canada. His research explores the use of large-scale assessments and databases to

    inform educational policy and practice, and help us determine those factors associated with educational

    outcomes.

  • 5/24/2018 Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation

    14/14

    Copyright of Social Psychology of Education is the property of Springer Science & Business Media B.V. and

    its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder'

    express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.