26
10/06/22 1 INCIDENTAL FOCUS ON FORM DURING DECISION MAKING TASKS AND THE EFFECTS ON ORAL AND WRITTEN PERFORMANCE Eva Alcón Soler Universitat Jaume I

Eva Alcón Soler Universitat Jaume I

  • Upload
    kaveri

  • View
    66

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

INCIDENTAL FOCUS ON FORM DURING DECISION MAKING TASKS AND THE EFFECTS ON ORAL AND WRITTEN PERFORMANCE. Eva Alcón Soler Universitat Jaume I. THEORETICAL BACKGROUND : Research on focus on form instruction. A definition of focus on form : - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Citation preview

Page 1: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/231

INCIDENTAL FOCUS ON FORM DURING DECISION MAKING TASKS AND THE EFFECTS ON ORAL AND WRITTEN PERFORMANCE

Eva Alcón Soler

Universitat Jaume I

Page 2: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/232

THEORETICAL BACKGROUND: Research on focus on form instruction

A definition of focus on form:

…interactional moves directed at raising learner awareness of forms by briefly drawing students’ attention to linguistic elements (words, collocations, grammatical structures, pragmatic patterns, and so on), in context, as they arise incidentally in lessons whose overriding focus is on meaning, or communication.” Long (1996:40)

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 3: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/233

Research on focus on form instruction

1. Describe types of focus on form in language classrooms:

Planned versus incidental

Reactive versus pre-emptive

Explicit versus implicit

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 4: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/234

2. Measure the effectiveness of focus on form by focussing on:

The silence of exchanges and its relation to L2 learning (Loewen, 2004, 2006; Alcón, 2007; Alcón and García Mayo, 2008)

The relationship between type of feedback, uptake and L2 development (Lyster, 1988, 2001, 2002; Lyster & Ranta, 1997; Mackey & Oliver, 2002; Tsang, 2004; Mackey & Silver, 2005; Loewen and Philp, 2006)

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 5: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/235

1. Incidental focus on form occurs in meaning focus interaction and it facilitates learners’ noticing

2. Participant output during task performance is influenced by type of feedback

Findings from research:

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 6: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/236

3. Few studies measure the effect of interaction in intact EFL classrooms.

4. Few studies compare learners’ gains of target linguistic items in subsequent oral and written production tasks.

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 7: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/237TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

AIM To examine how incidental focus on form

is accomplished during decision-making tasks carried out in EFL classrooms.

To study the relationship between lexically-oriented focus on form episodes (FFEs), noticing, learner uptake and subsequent lexical gains in EFL learners’ written and oral production tasks

Page 8: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/238TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

RESEARCH QUESTIONS

What features of focus on form influence learner uptake and noticing in lexically oriented focus on form episodes?

Is there a relationship between learners’ reports of noticing lexical items and their subsequent use in oral and written decision-making tasks?

Page 9: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/239TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

PARTICIPANTS

12 Spanish speakers (7 females and 5 males) studying English as a compulsory subject for six years at school, and their age ranged from 14 to 15.

A female English language teacher who had 8 years’ teaching experience and an MA in Applied Linguistics took part in the study.

Page 10: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2310TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS

The data for this study were obtained from intact EFL classrooms throughout a whole academic year.

17 45-minute audio-recorded lessons

204 learners’ diaries (17 sessions × 12 learners) reporting what they had learned after each conversational class.

Page 11: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2311

6 post-production tasks: 3 oral and 3 written production tasks created on the basis of the items reported in learners’ diaries.

Oral decision-making tasks required learners to reduce the number of objects or actions from a maximum of 10 to a minimum of 2.

Written production tasks required learners to explain why different objects and actions were needed to go to the moon, to travel to India and to live in an isolated village.

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 12: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2312TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

1. Identifying incidental lexically oriented FFEs.

2. Coding types of FFEs: Reactive, pre-emptive, teacher initiated, student initiated, level of complexity of the negotiation sequences, and learner uptake in response to interactional feedback.

3. Measuring the impact of type of feedback and complexity of the negotiation sequence on learner uptake (Rates of agreement 93% for complexity and 90% for explicitness of feedback)

The sequence of the research process could be summarized as follows:

Page 13: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2313

4. Measuring the impact of type of feedback on noticing by comparing the words learners reported they had learnt after each lesson (noticing) with their occurrence in FFEs.

5. Measuring learning outcomes by comparing learners’ noticing of lexical items with their performance in tailor-made oral and written post-tasks.

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 14: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2314TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

RESULTS

RQ1

What features of focus on form influence learner uptake and noticing in lexically oriented focus on form episodes?

Page 15: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2315

Learners' uptake in FFEs

101

82

192

84

9

68

17

63

0

50

100

150

200

P reemptive teacherinitiated

P reemptive studentinitiated

Reactive teacher supplier Reactive student supplier

Total Uptake

Figure 1. Types of FFEs and learner’s uptake in FFEs TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 16: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2316TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Table 1. Pearson Correlation Matrix for learner uptake, type of feedback and complexity of the negotiation sequence

 Learner uptake

Implicit feedback

ComplexityExplicit feedback

Learner uptake 1.0000      

Implicit feedback -0.657 1.0000    

Complexity 0.711 -0.921 1.0000  

Explicit feedback 0.904 -0.812 0.764 1.0000

Page 17: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2317

Type of feedback has an influence on uptake (Lyster and Ranta, 1997; Mackey and Philp, 1998). Recasts do not trigger successful uptake

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 18: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2318

Table 2. Pearson Correlation Matrix for noticing, complexity

and type of feedback

  NoticingImplicit feedback

ComplexityExplicit feedback

Noticing 1.0000      

Implicit feedback 0.561 1.0000    

Complexity 0.083 -0.921 1.0000  

Explicit feedback 0.672 -0.812 0.764 1.0000

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 19: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2319

These findings suggest the potential benefits of recasts to facilitate learner noticing, even if they do not trigger uptake (Mackey and Oliver 2002; Mackey and Silver 2005, and Loewen and Philp, 2006).

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 20: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2320TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

RQ2

Is there a relationship between learners’ reports of noticing lexical items and their use in subsequent oral and written production?

Page 21: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2321

Table 3. Pearson Correlation Matrix for lexical items used in FFEs, noticing, and subsequent lexical use in written and oral delayed post tasks.

  

Lexical items in FFEs

NoticingWritten post-task lexical use

Oral post-task lexical use

Lexical items in FFEs

 0.824

(Sig. .000)0.438

(Sig. .123)0.265

(Sig. .125)

Noticing    0.511

(Sig. .126)0.518

(Sig. .128)

Written post-task lexical use

     0.246

(Sig. .256)

Oral post-task lexical use

       

TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Page 22: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2322TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Explicit knowledge can be obtained from focus on form instruction, and lexical gains are observed across modalities.

Whether learning follows noticing or is dependent on noticing should be tested in further experimental studies on task performance.

Page 23: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2323TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

1. Oral tasks elicited more negotiation of meaning and a focus on the managing of interaction.

2. Written tasks elicited more accurate use of the items and a focus on outcomes

Tasks as instruments of data collection

Page 24: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2324TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

Task familiarity: more accurate use of the items in delayed written production tasks than in delayed oral tasks (Alcón, in press)

Page 25: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2325TBLT 2009, Lancaster 14, 2009. Eva Alcon, UJI

1. We equate learners’ recall of lexical words with noticing, but how can we explain accurate use of items in subsequent production while learners do

not report noticing.

Other research issues:

2. Information about learners’ capacity for mental processing

3. Tasks were designed to trace lexical items in FFEs and their impact on subsequent oral and written performance: spontaneous language use/post testing tasks.

Page 26: Eva Alcón Soler  Universitat Jaume I

20/04/2326

THANK YOU