Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    1/19

    AJSLP

    Supplement

    Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    Treatment (VNeST) in Persons With

    Aphasia: Extension and Replication

    of Previous Findings

    Lisa A. Edmonds,a,b Kevin Mammino,a and Jimena Ojedaa,b

    Purpose:Verb Network Strengthening Treatment(VNeST)

    is an aphasia treatment that targets verbs (e.g., measure)and their related thematic roles (e.g., carpenterlumber).Previous studies reported encouraging results in a number ofparticipants using single-subject design with improvementsobserved on naming, sentence production, and discourse.The purpose of the current study was to conduct a groupanalysis evaluating the effect of VNeST on similar outcomes.Method:A multiple baseline design across participantswas conducted with 11 persons with aphasia due to stroke.Wilcoxon signed-ranks tests were used to evaluate potentialimprovement from pre- to posttreatment and maintenance.Individual effect sizes were also calculated to evaluatemagnitude of change within and across participants.Results:Results showed significant improvement atposttreatment and maintenance on trained and untrained

    sentence probes and object and action naming. Improvement

    in the production of sentences not targeted in treatment wasnonsignificant at posttreatment assessment but significantat maintenance. Moderate increases in percentage ofcomplete utterances and overall informativeness wereobserved on discourse.Conclusion: The results of this study replicate previousfindings and provide evidence that VNeST may promotespecific and generalized lexical retrieval abilities and affectbasic syntax production in both constrained and discourseproduction tasks.

    Key Words: aphasia, VNeST, verbs, treatment, thematicroles, sentences, discourse

    The most pervasive symptom in aphasia is anomia,or difficulty retrieving words (e.g., Laine & Martin,2006), which can affect language production at the

    single word, sentence, and discourse level. The majorityof lexical retrieval treatment paradigms reported in theliterature have targeted single-word noun production, andalthough generalization to improved retrieval of semanti-cally related nouns has been observed across a number ofstudies, generalization to sentences or discourse has beennegligible (see Kiran & Bassetto, 2008; Nickels, 2002).Treatments targeting single verb retrieval have primarily

    focused on provision of semantic and/or phonological cues.

    Overall, improvement has been limited to trained verbs, withvirtually no improvement to untreated verbs, though someimprovement to sentence production and/or connectedspeech has been reported in some studies. (For relevantreviews, see Conroy, Sage, & Lambon Ralph [2006] andWebster & Whitworth [2012].)

    In an attempt to promote sentence production andconnected speech, a number of verb treatments haveintegrated arguments into treatment. A subset of thesestudies (as described by Webster & Whitworth, 2012) focusedprimarily on sentence structure, syntax, and/or morphosyn-

    tax (e.g., Bastiaanse, Hurkmans, & Links, 2006; Conroy,Sage, & Lambon Ralph, 2009; Edwards & Tucker, 2006;Links, Hurkmans, & Bastiaanse, 2010; McCann & Doleman,2011; Raymer & Kohen, 2006). Trained tasks included sen-tence completion and production and sentence productioncuing hierarchies. Overall, improvements were observedfor trained verbs, but there was limited improvement tosingle-word retrieval of untrained verbs (Links et al., 2010;Raymer & Kohen, 2006). Some improvements to sentence

    aBrain Rehabilitation and Research Center, Malcom Randall VA

    Medical Center, Gainesville, FLbUniversity of Florida, Gainesville

    Correspondence to Lisa A. Edmonds: [email protected]

    Editor: Swathi Kiran

    Associate Editor: Yasmeen Faroqi-Shah

    Received August 16, 2013

    Revision received January 24, 2014

    Accepted February 25, 2014

    DOI: 10.1044/2014_AJSLP-13-0098Disclosure:The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at thetime of publication.

    American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology Vol. 23 S312S329 May 2014 AAmerican Speech-Language-Hearing Association

    S l t S l t P F th 43 d Cli i l A h i l C f

    S312

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    2/19

    production were reported (Bastiaanse et al., 2006; Edwards& Tucker, 2006; Links et al., 2010; McCann & Doleman,2011), with improvement to connected speech in a fewparticipants (Bastiaanse et al., 2006; Edwards & Tucker,2006; Links et al., 2010). Although the improvements tosentence production and connected speech are encouraging,

    the gains observed in these studies were primarily structuraland/or grammatical (Webster & Whitworth, 2012), notlexical.

    As discussed in Webster and Whitworth (2012), thedegree of improvement to argument structure depends onwhether argument structure is lexically specified. If it is,improvement will be restricted to treated verbs, but if it isnot, improvement in sentence production could be morewidespread. Thus, the lack of generalized lexical improve-ment in these studies could reflect a focus on structurewithout explicit semantic training. Promoting semanticrelationships between verbs and their thematic roles could

    potentially promote more generalized improvement to

    sentence production and discourse (e.g., Edmonds & Babb,2011; Edmonds, Nadeau, & Kiran, 2009). Some studies haveattempted this with tasks such as identifying agents andthemes in pictures and picture description (Fink, Martin,Schwartz, Saffran, & Myers, 1992), story production withan emphasis on verbnoun associations (Kim, Adingono,& Revoir, 2007), descriptions of a verbs semantics andthematic roles (Schneider & Thompson, 2003), semanticfeature analysis (SFA) with verbs (which focuses on singleverbs but includes thematic roles) (Faroqi-Shah & Graham,2011 [modified SFA]; Wambaugh & Ferguson, 2007;Wambaugh, Mauszycki, & Wright, 2014), and tasks target-ing associations between verbs and nouns, sometimes in

    conjunction with sentence production (Webster & Gordon,2009; Webster et al., 2005).

    The overall results of these studies showed improve-ment to trained verbs, but improvement to untrained verbswas still not observed in the vast majority of the studies (butsee Object and Action Naming Battery [OANB] verb nam-ing improvement in two participants in Faroqi-Shah &Graham [2011]). For the studies that examined potentialgeneralization to sentence production, some observedimprovement only with sentences containing trained verbs(Kim et al., 2007; Schneider & Thompson, 2003), whileothers saw improvement with untrained verbs as well

    (Webster & Gordon, 2009; Webster et al., 2005). Of the

    studies that examined connected speech, only a few reportedimprovement on lexical retrieval and/or improved syntaxor structure (Fink et al., 1992; Kim et al., 2007; Webster et al.,2005). Although there is some evidence of generalization inthese studies, there is still limited improvement to untrainedlexical items. Verb Network Strengthening Treatment(VNeST), the focus of the current study, is principally similarto these treatments. However, there are some differences,especially with regard to the scope of semantic training.

    VNeST is a theoretically motivated treatment designedto promote lexical retrieval in sentence contexts, withpotential generalization to more widespread lexical accessimprovement (Edmonds et al., 2009). The fundamental

    theoretical premise of VNeST is that semantic verb networksare represented as neural networks that strengthen viaHebbian learning through repeated activation and use(Edmonds et al., 2009). A growing body of research withyoung adults suggests that verbs and their related thematicroles are neurally coactivated such that agents and patients

    prime or facilitate activation of related verbs (Edmonds &Mizrahi, 2011; McRae, Hare, & Ferretti, 2005) and viceversa (Edmonds & Mizrahi, 2011; Ferretti, McRae, &Hatherell, 2001). There is also bidirectional neural coacti-vation between verbs (e.g., slicing) and their instruments (e.g.,knife) (Ferretti et al., 2001; McRae et al., 2005; Park &Edmonds, 2013) and priming from locations (e.g., restaurant)to related verbs (e.g., eating) (Ferretti et al., 2001).

    The treatment steps of VNeST follow directly from thistheoretical framework. Participants are given a verb (e.g.,measure) and are asked to retrieve related agents (doers of theaction, e.g., carpenter) and patients (receivers of the action,e.g., lumber). Participants generate multiple pairs of agents

    and patients per verb, thereby eliciting various event schemas(e.g., carpenterlumber, chefsugar, seamstressfabric formeasure). Because a verbs conceptual meaning is somewhatloose(relative to nouns) (Black & Chiat, 2003) and thuscan have different meanings based on its thematic roles,generation of multiple agentpatient pairs actualizes variousdimensions of the verbs meaning. Retrieving verb schemasalso activates a large network of world, autobiographic, andsemantic knowledge that could potentially maximize gener-alization of lexical retrieval to a relatively large corpus oftrained and untrained words in untrained language contextsand tasks. Thus, the scope of semantic treatment is greaterthan other treatments that target verbs and their thematic

    roles, as multiple event schemas that can represent differentmeanings or uses of trained verbs are not targeted in thosestudies.

    Another aspect of VNeST that is different from otherstudies is that no pictures are used in treatment, so partic-ipants cannot associate or learn responses related to pictures(e.g., Webster & Gordon, 2009) but rather must search,activate, and retrieve their own memories and representa-tions. Participants also answer wh-questions (where, when,why) about one scenario per verb to further develop theschema and expand its saliency and relevancy. For the car-penter measuring lumber example, a participant may discussthat her bathroom was remodeled last summer because of

    flood damage. Thus, VNeST requires divergent productionof multiple event schemas in a systematic way without beinglimited by the entities and action represented in trainedpictures.

    While VNeST attempts to engage a large semanticnetwork, treating verbs in conjunction with their thematicroles also activates the syntactic elements of sentences (i.e.,subjectverbobject) in a number of related ways. Becauseargument structure is considered an integral part of a verbslexical representation, training them together potentiallystrengthens both the verb representation and its connectionsto its arguments. In addition, the repeated and diverseselection of subjectagents and objectpatients potentially

    Edmonds et al.: Verb Network Strengthening Treatment S313

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    3/19

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    4/19

    on our primary discourse measure, complete utterances(Edmonds et al., 2009). We also predicted improvement onpercentage correct information units (CIUs), a measure ofinformative words, from Nicholas and Brookshire (1993).Although we were primarily interested in group results forthis study, we also report individual responses to treatment,

    including effect sizes.

    Method

    Experimental Design

    A multiple baseline approach across participants wasused with four phases: (a) baseline (probe testing; n = 5);(b) treatment (biweekly probe testing; n = 5); (c) posttreat-ment probes (n= 1 [because of time constraints in posttestingfor some participants] or n = 3); and (d) 3-month main-tenance probes (n= 1). The probe task was sentence pro-duction for pictures depicting trained (The carpenter is

    measuring the lumber) and untrained semantically relatedverbs (The farmer is weighing the apples). A control task wasadministered during the same time points for demonstrationof experimental control. Additional outcome measures,described below, were administered at posttreatment andmaintenance.

    Participants

    Treatment Participants

    Eleven participants (mean age = 63.1, 7 males) with aminimum of 12 years of education were recruited from theBrain Rehabilitation and Research Center at the Malcom

    Randall VA Medical Center in Gainesville, Florida, and fromBrooks Rehabilitation in Jacksonville, Florida. None of theparticipants had participated in previous VNeST studies.Participants met several inclusion criteria, including (a) di-agnosis of aphasia based on the WABR (Kerstesz, 2006),(b) monolingual English speaking, (c) right-handedness priorto stroke, (d) negative history of diagnosed learning disorderor drug or alcohol addiction, (e) maximum baseline aver-age of 40% on the sentence probes, and (f ) no worse thana composite score of a moderate deficit on the CognitiveLinguistic Quick Test (CLQT; Helm-Estabrooks, 2001). Allparticipants had aphasia as a result of stroke. Nine of the11 experienced a single left hemisphere ischemic stroke, one

    (P3) had an aneurysm rupture during a clipping procedure,and one (P4) had a left middle cerebral artery ischemic strokeafter two aneurysm ruptures. Finally, no participants exhib-ited more than moderate apraxia of speech (AOS) on thebasis of their performance on the Apraxia Battery for AdultsSecond Edition (Dabul, 2000) and on evaluation of McNeil,Robin, and Schmidts (2009) characteristics of AOS. SeeTable 1 for demographic details.

    Six additional participants were evaluated. Five werenot included because they did not meet the inclusion andexclusion criteria (e.g., multilingual, high scores on sentenceprobes), and one chose not to enroll due to living too faraway.

    Communication Partners

    Ten treatment participants had a family member whocommunicated with them regularly and agreed to complete aquestionnaire about the treatment participants communi-cation. Eight of the 10 communication participants com-pleted the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (Nasreddine et al.,

    2005), and of those eight, seven scored within normal limits(WNL) and were included in the study (the one who wasnot enrolled scored 21/30 [with 26 considered WNL]). Theother two participants were not available to come to theclinic for testing, but they were both below 60 years of age,gainfully employed, and showed no signs of inability tocomplete the questionnaire competently, so they were enrolledwithout cognitive testing. Thus, 9 of the 11 treatment partic-ipants had communication partners who were enrolled andwho completed the CETI (Lomas et al., 1989). Communica-tion partners (7 women) included four spouses, three adultchildren, one sibling, and one grandmother, with an averageage and education of 56.0 (SD= 17.5) and 14.1 (SD= 1.3),

    respectively.

    Materials

    The following measures were administered to deter-mine pretreatment language abilities and to serve as outcomemeasures at posttreatment. Table 2 shows group scores,and Table 3 shows individual scores.

    Outcome Measures for Research Question 1

    Sentence probe pictures.Sentence probe developmentis discussed in Edmonds et al. (2009) and Edmonds and Babb(2011). Briefly, 28 pictures were developed to elicit sentences

    containing an agent, verb, and patient, where agents havespecific titles (e.g., nurse, carpenter) to promote specificlanguage (as opposed to general terms, such as woman, man).A few additional sentences were added in this study, andsome previous sentences were reconfigured to ensure noreversible sentences and no entities with general titles (e.g.,boy, woman). Sentences were divided into sets, so that eachverb (measure) was semantically related to another verb(weigh) across sets. See Supplemental Table 1 for details.

    Control task.Two control tasks were administered toparticipants during the baseline phase to rule out the pos-sibility that improvements during treatment reflected anonspecific effect on semantic knowledge (i.e., to demon-

    strate internal validity). The first was a single-word adjective-retrieval task used previously (see Edmonds et al., 2009).This task requires participants to complete 11 sentences byproviding a synonym to a provided adjective (e.g., Someonewho is sick is also said to be _____ [target: ill]). Adjectiveswere chosen rather than verbs, because a sufficient number ofverbs unrelated to treatment verbs and balanced acrosspsycholinguistic variables (e.g., frequency, length) could notbe generated (Edmonds et al., 2009). This is a conserva-tive task, because participants are required only to provideone word, and they are provided with a carrier phrasethat contains a synonym to the target word. However, thistask was hypothesized not to improve, because adjectives

    Edmonds et al.: Verb Network Strengthening Treatment S315

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    5/19

    and features of objects were not explicitly trained. Further, asimilar task demonstrated control in a previous verb treat-

    ment study (Marshall, Pring, & Chiat, 1998).The second control task was the nonword repetition

    task (Number 8) from the Psycholinguistic Assessment ofLanguage Processing (Kay, Lesser, & Coltheart, 1992). Thistask was chosen because disproportionate nonword repetition,as compared with real word repetition, has been reported inaphasia (Rapcsak et al., 2009). The hypothesized mechanismis a phonological deficit that disproportionately affects theprocessing of unfamiliar phonological patterns (Rapcsaket al., 2009). Because VNeST does not target phonology,nonword repetition was not hypothesized to improve.

    Procedures for Collecting Probe Data

    and Administering TreatmentProbe and control measures. During baseline, the probe

    pictures and control items were administered at the begin-ning of each session and were audio- and video-recorded.Pictures were presented pseudorandomly with semanticallyrelated verbs (boilfry) in nonsequential order. For eachpicture, participants were instructed to Make a sentenceand include him/her, the action, and thiswhile pointing tothe agent (carpenter), verb (measure), and patient (lumber).Prompts were not provided unless the participant produced ageneral word for the target (e.g., cut instead ofsliceor man

    instead ofcarpenter), for which a prompt for a more specificword was given. After the baseline period, trained and

    untrained semantically related verbs were chosen based onthe 10 sentence probe pairs that resulted in the lowest aver-

    age starting point with similar averages for the trained anduntrained sets.

    Transcription from audio files was conducted for allprobe responses. One point was awarded for each correctagent, verb, and patient, with one phonemic error allowed(e.g.,bilot for pilot) per word. Grammatical and morpho-logical errors were not penalized, because they were not tar-geted in the treatment. If a response matched all three targetsfor the agent, verb, and patient, then 1 point was given for thetotal sentence score. Accurate alternative words were givencredit (e.g., loggerfor lumberjack, jetfor airplane). To ensureconsistency in probe scoring, we conducted interrater reli-ability for 100% of weekly probes. A point-to-point evaluation

    showed 97% agreement in scoring.Treatment materials and protocol. Treatment was

    provided two times per week for 10 weeks, for a total of20 sessions. Each session was 2 hr long. With time subtractedfor sentence probes during the treatment phase, participantsreceived approximately 35 hr of total treatment. (Becauseof scheduling issues, P5 received only 18 sessions.) Ten verbswere trained per participant, and each verb was trained 1 timeper week. See the Appendix for materials and protocol.

    Outcome Measures for Research Question 2

    Single-word lexical retrieval was evaluated with theOANB (Druks & Masterson, 2000), which contains pictures

    Table 1.Demographic and lesion information for all participants.

    Pt M/F Age Ed Race MPO Aphasia type Stroke or lesion information

    1 M 49 14 AfAm 61 Anomic Large left MCA CVA with changes in the left frontoparietal subcorticalwhite matter.

    2 M 69 12 Cauc 74 Anomic Left frontal, temporal, and parietal CVA. Involved regions include

    frontal operculum, triangular part of the frontal gyrus, superiortemporal gyrus, superior and middle frontal gyrus, precentralgyrus, dorsolateral frontal cortex, perisylvian region, hand and armregions of primary motor cortex.

    3 M 69 13 AfAm 144 Anomic Left posterior communicating artery aneurism rupture during asurgery attempt to clip it. Left caudate head and internal capsuleinvolvement.

    4 F 70 15 Cauc 15 Anomic Anterior communicating artery aneurysm, basilar tip aneurysm withPCA ischemia, and subsequent left MCA CVA (embolism).

    5 M 35 14 Cauc 59 Anomic Large left MCA CVA, thrombosis.6 F 81 16 Cauc 72 Conduction Moderate-sized CVA in the left parietal lobe extending to the junction

    of the temporal and parietal lobes.7 M 63 18 Cauc 14 Conduction-Jargon Left MCA CVA. Left anterior temporal infarct extension to the

    adjacent portions of the left parietal lobe and deep white matter ofthe left frontal lobe, including the head of the caudate nucleus andposterior insula.

    8 M 61 18 Cauc 23 Conduction Moderate left CVA involving temporal lobe, parietal lobe, and part of the left frontal lobe, including the insular cortex.

    9 F 68 16 Cauc 95 Wernickes Large left MCA CVA involving mid- to peripheral distribution of theMCA with vascular compromise to the periphery of the parietallobe and portions of the periphery of the temporal lobe.

    10 F 58 15 Cauc 26 TCM Large left anterior division MCA CVA.11 M 71 22 Cauc 16 TCM Moderate-sized left MCA CVA involving the left frontal lobe, insular

    cortex, and left basal ganglia.

    Note. Pt = participant; M = male; F = female; Ed = education; MPO = months postonset; AfAm = African American; Cauc = Caucasian;TCM = transcortical motor aphasia; MCA= middle cerebral artery; CVA = cerebrovascular accident; PCA = posterior cerebral artery.

    S316 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology Vol. 23 S312S329 May 2014

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    6/19

    of objects (N= 162) and actions (N= 100). The object andaction stimuli are divided into two forms (A and B) that arebalanced for psycholinguistic (e.g., frequency, age of acqui-sition) and nonlinguistic (e.g., imageability, visual com-

    plexity) variables. All participants completed both forms,and no cues or feedback were given.

    Lexical retrieval in sentences was evaluated withstimuli from the Argument Structure Production Test fromthe NAVS (Thompson, 2011). Verbs included one-, two-,and three-place verbs (e.g., The dog is barking; The womanis kissing the man; The man is putting the box on the shelf).

    The NAVS protocol requires showing and reading the verbto the participant, but we did not do either; rather, par-ticipants just saw the pictures and made a sentence. Further-more, the test contains two- and three-place verbs in optional(The man is sweeping) and obligatory contexts (The manis sweeping the dirt), but we tested only the more complex

    sentence to avoid multiple presentations. Thus, 36 of50 pictures were administered (one-place, n = 8; two-place,n= 17; three-place, n = 11).

    For OANB and the NAVS, scoring was conducted

    according to the test manuals, though one phonemic errorper lexical item was allowed. One hundred percent reliabilitywas conducted (point to point) for both measures, with anaverage scoring agreement of 96.5%.

    Outcome Measures for Research Question 3

    Participants responded to all 10 discourse elicitationmaterials from Nicholas and Brookshire (1993), whichinclude procedural, personal, single-picture, and sequential-picture stimuli. Participant responses were blinded and thentranscribed using Systematic Analysis of Language Tran-scripts software (Miller & Iglesias, 2012). Utterances werebroken into T-units, or a main clause with its subordinate

    Table 2.Group statistics details and results for tasks corresponding to all research questions evaluated at posttreatment and 3-monthmaintenance.

    Generalization measurePre-tx mean

    (SD)Post-tx mean

    (SD) WilcoxonZ pPre-tx meana

    (SD)Maintenance

    (SD) Wilcoxon Z- p

    Research Question 1

    Sentence probesSentencesTR (n= 10) 20.4% (13.3) 58.8% (33.9) 2.803 .005 20.4% (13.3) 56.0% (29.9) 2.705 .007SentencesUT (n = 10) 19.8% (11.0) 40.9% (24.9) 2.703 .007 19.8% (11.0) 43.0% (27.5) 2.805 .005WordsTR (n= 30) 58.5% (11.1) 80.7% (17.0) 2.803 .005 58.5% (11.1) 80.3% (13.5) 2.805 .005

    Agents (n= 10) 57.6% (21.6) 84.7% (19.6) 2.803 .005 57.6% (21.6) 86.0% (18.4) 2.805 .005Verbs (n = 10) 52.4% (16.1) 74.0% (21.8) 2.395 .017 52.4% (16.1) 68.0% (25.3) 2.492 .013Patients (n= 10) 65.6% (17.4) 83.5% (18.6) 2.703 .007 65.6% (17.4) 87.0% (10.6) 2.654 .008

    WordsUT (n = 30) 59.8% (10.0) 73.1% (15.5) 2.701 .007 59.8% (10.0) 73.3% (14.7) 2.701 .007Agents (n= 10) 63.6% (21.9) 74.3% (19.9) 2.075 .038 63.6% (21.9) 76.0% (21.2) 1.994 .046Verbs (n = 10) 49.4% (16.7) 61.3% (22.6) 1.779 .075 49.4% (16.7) 64.0% (23.2) 2.668 .008Patients (n= 10) 66.4% (12.8) 83.7% (14.2) 2.803 .005 66.4% (12.8) 79.0% (16.6) 2.293 .022

    Control 34.7% (14.5) 42.0% (18.0) 1.838 .066 34.7% (14.5) 36.6% (20.4) 0.841 .400

    Research Question 2Single words and sentences

    OANBnouns (n= 162) 79.91% (12.3) 85.07% (12.6) 2.402 .016 N/A N/A N/A N/A

    OANB

    verbs (n= 100) 64.64% (18.2) 74.18% (16.0)

    2.668 .008 N/A N/A N/A N/A NAVS sentences (n = 36) 29.04% (23.7) 42.42% (25.6) 1.719 .086 29.04% (23.7) 40.15% (23.8) 2.807 .005

    Research Question 3Discourse

    % Comp utterances 39.73 (11.9) 46.74 (16.0) 2.223 .026 40.85 (11.9) 47.98 (13.8) 1.580 .114Number of words 922.18 (584.2) 1,036.82 (638.4) 1.156 .248 986.80 (572.9) 1,093.40 (546.6) 0.764 .445Number of CIUs 408.45 (251.0) 469.91 (269.1) 1.682 .093 438.80 (242.4) 538.70 (281.2) 1.955 .051% CIUs 46.63 (11.8) 48.70 (13.8) 0.622 .534 47.49 (12.0) 51.42 (13.3) 1.988 .047CIUs per minute 27.69 (15.1) 28.90 (16.6) 0.978 .328 29.55 (14.5) 33.51 (21.4) 0.764 .445Number of utterances 132.18 (69.3) 143.36 (85.1) 0.408 .683 141.20 (65.9) 142.90 (74.2) 0.415 .678% Pauses 37.39 (24.0) 35.74 (24.4) 0.978 .328 34.52 (23.3) 33.82 (24.2) 0.663 .508% Mazes 10.99 (5.6) 10.71 (5.5) 0.712 .477 10.00 (4.7) 10.93 (5.2) 0.868 .386

    Research Question 4Aphasia severity:

    WAB AQ 75.91 (10.4) 82.12 (8.6) 2.395 .017 N/A N/A N/A N/A

    Functional communication:CETI (N= 100 possible) 32.61 (9.7) 65.28 (11.2) 2.666 .008 32.61 (9.7) 58.29 (15.6) 2.666 .008

    Note. Data in bold were significant at thep< .05 level. tx = treatment; TR = trained; UT = untrained; OANB = Object and Action Naming Battery;NAVS = Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences; Comp = complete; CIU = content information unit; WAB AQ = Western AphasiaBattery Aphasia Quotient; CETI = Communication Effectiveness Index; N/A = not applicable.aAn additional Pre-tx column is present because of a different number of participants that were evaluated at maintenance than at posttreatment.

    Edmonds et al.: Verb Network Strengthening Treatment S317

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    7/19

    Table3.

    Individualparticipantscoresforpretreatment,posttreatment,andmain

    tenanceresultsforthesentenceprobes,

    OANB,

    NAVSsentenceproduction,

    Nich

    olasandBrookshire

    (1993)discourseresults,andCETIsco

    resforallparticipants.

    Pt

    Sentenceprobesa

    Lexicalretrieval

    NicholasandBrookshire

    (1993)discoursetasks(n=10)

    Funct

    Comm:

    CETI

    AvgTR

    Sent

    (N=10)

    AvgUT

    Sent

    (N=10)

    OANB

    %

    Nounsb

    (N

    =162)

    OANB%

    Verbsb

    (N=100)

    %

    NAVS

    SentProdc

    (N=36)

    %

    Comp

    Uttsc

    Number

    ofwordsd

    Number

    ofCIUsd

    %CIUse

    CIUs/min

    Words/min

    %

    P

    auses

    1Pre

    3.8

    2.8

    90.1

    82

    72.2

    54.7

    811

    469

    57.8

    35.7

    65.9

    24.1

    DNT

    Post

    9.0***

    5.0***

    96.9

    95

    72.2

    58.3

    843

    490

    58.1

    42.2

    76.8

    18.4

    DNT

    Main

    9.0***

    7.0***

    N/A

    N/A

    75.0

    63.5

    696

    439

    63.1

    39.7

    65.0

    22.3

    DNT

    2Pre

    1.6

    1.8

    88.2

    81

    19.4

    47.3

    540

    287

    53.2

    20.4

    42.6

    48.6

    41.3

    Post

    4.7

    4.3**

    93.8

    81

    11.1

    32.8

    831

    390

    46.9

    15.2

    38.5

    58.5

    72.4

    Main

    7.0*

    5.0***

    N/A

    N/A

    19.4

    33.6

    1,0

    89

    515

    47.3

    16.2

    36.7

    50.3

    64.4

    3Pre

    0.4

    0.8

    53.7

    49

    5.6

    39.2

    1,3

    97

    440

    31.5

    29.9

    100.0

    20.4

    DNT

    Post

    2.0*

    2.0

    63.6

    51

    19.4

    51.2

    1,5

    11

    443

    29.3

    27.1

    98.4

    20.5

    DNT

    Main

    3.0**

    2.0

    N/A

    N/A

    19.4

    52.4

    1,5

    81

    516

    32.6

    32.8

    94.7

    15.4

    DNT

    4Pre

    2.8

    2.6

    89.5

    61

    16.7

    28.6

    276

    105

    38.0

    9.1

    26.8

    66.1

    25.3

    Post

    9.3***

    7.3***

    97.5

    81

    50.0

    29.5

    486

    184

    37.9

    8.8

    28.7

    59.3

    39.6

    Main

    8.0***

    4.0*

    N/A

    N/A

    22.2

    N/A

    N/A

    N/A

    N/A

    N/A

    N/A

    N/A

    56.5

    5Pre

    4.4

    4.0

    95.7

    75

    63.9

    43.1

    310

    202

    65.2

    13.5

    24.9

    65.3

    23.1

    Post

    10.0**

    9.0**

    97.0

    86

    66.7

    64.6

    339

    261

    77.0

    15.7

    23.0

    63.8

    83.7

    Main

    10.0**

    10.0***

    N/A

    N/A

    69.4

    58.2

    365

    265

    72.60

    16.9

    3

    26.1

    66.0

    74.5

    (tablecontinues)

    S318 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology Vol. 23 S312S329 May 2014

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    8/19

    Table3(Continued).

    Pt

    Sentenceprobesa

    Lexicalretrieval

    NicholasandBrookshire

    (1993)discoursetasks(n=10)

    Funct

    Comm:

    CETI

    AvgTR

    Sent

    (N=10)

    AvgUT

    Sent

    (N=10)

    OANB

    %

    Nounsb

    (N

    =162)

    OANB%

    Verbsb

    (N=100)

    %

    NAVS

    SentProdc

    (N=36)

    %

    Comp

    Uttsc

    Number

    ofwordsd

    Number

    ofCIUsd

    %CIUse

    CIUs/min

    Words/min

    %

    P

    auses

    6Pre

    1.8

    2.0

    67.3

    63

    41.7

    57.4

    1,4

    11

    721

    51.1

    42.7

    80.7

    20.4

    45.6

    Post

    8.7***

    4.3**

    63.6

    67

    83.3

    60.4

    1,1

    46

    612

    53.4

    40.5

    75.8

    16.5

    63.5

    Main

    5.0*

    6.0**

    N/A

    N/A

    50.0

    55.1

    1,1

    16

    603

    54.0

    33.4

    64.9

    13.1

    60.3

    7Pre

    0.8

    1.6

    75.9

    69

    5.6

    28.4

    1,3

    51

    444

    32.9

    29.2

    87.2

    11.2

    17.1

    Post

    3.7*

    3.0*

    75.7

    69

    44.4

    28.6

    1,3

    23

    431

    32.6

    31.1

    94.9

    12.0

    59.7

    Main

    5.0**

    2.0

    N/A

    N/A

    30.6

    29.5

    1,3

    08

    444

    33.9

    25.6

    68.2

    31.2

    37.3

    8Pre

    DNT

    DNT

    82.0

    72

    33.3

    52.7

    1980

    916

    46.3

    57.0

    119.5

    15.7

    36.4

    Post

    DNT

    DNT

    88.2

    80

    38.9

    73.9

    1,6

    66

    916

    55.0

    60.4

    108.8

    8.4

    55.1

    Main

    DNT

    DNT

    N/A

    N/A

    50.0

    62.9

    1,7

    12

    1,0

    41

    60.8

    77.1

    121.8

    6.2

    45.5

    9Pre

    0.6

    0.0

    77.2

    18

    2.8

    30.9

    1,2

    85

    535

    41.6

    39.4

    97.0

    13.8

    30.1

    Post

    1.7

    1.0

    **

    84.6

    41

    2.8

    46.1

    2,3

    73

    974

    41.1

    46.5

    109.2

    13.9

    77.6

    Main

    0.0

    1.0

    **

    N/A

    N/A

    5.6

    45.5

    1,9

    55

    997

    51.0

    63.0

    125.4

    8.4

    78.3

    10 P

    re

    2.4

    1.8

    88.8

    76

    41.7

    27.4

    393

    238

    60.6

    14.7

    27.9

    74.7

    42.8

    Post

    7.7**

    3.0*

    91.9

    84

    27.8

    38.4

    472

    284

    60.2

    14.1

    26.1

    69.2

    48.3

    Main

    4.0

    3.0*

    N/A

    N/A

    69.4

    53.1

    639

    384

    60.1

    16.4

    31.4

    63.4

    50.9

    11 P

    re

    1.8

    2.4

    72.7

    65

    16.7

    27.5

    390

    136

    34.9

    13.1

    43.5

    51.1

    31.9

    Post

    2.0

    2.0

    85.1

    81

    50.0

    30.4

    415

    184

    44.3

    16.5

    41.1

    52.7

    67.3

    Main

    5.0*

    3.0

    N/A

    N/A

    30.6

    26.1

    473

    183

    38.7

    14.0

    40.0

    61.8

    73.9

    Note.

    Scoreswithunderscorearewithinnormallimitsaccordingtothetestmanual.Avg=average;Sent=sentence;TR

    =trained;UT=untrained;NAVSSentPro

    d=NAVSSentence

    Production;CompUtts=completeutte

    rances;CIU=contentinformationunits;FunctComm=functionalcommunication;P

    re=pretreatment;Post=posttreatment;M

    ain=maintenance;

    DNT=didnottest.

    aBoldeditemsforsentenceprobesindicatesmall*,medium**,orlarge***effects

    izescomparedwithpretreatmentscore.

    b

    BoldeditemsforanOANBindicateanincreaseof>2standard

    deviationsfromthemeanasindicatedinthetestsmanual.cBoldeditemsforNAV

    SSentenceProductionand%

    completeu

    tterancesindicatemediumorlargeeffectsizescomparedwith

    pretreatment.dItemswerenotanalyzed

    againstNicholasandBrookshire(1993)n

    ormativedata.

    eBoldeditemsfor%

    CIUs,

    CIUsperminuteandwordsperminuteind

    icateanincreaseof

    >2SEM(Nicholas&Brookshire,

    1993)

    comparedwithpretreatment.

    Edmonds et al.: Verb Network Strengthening Treatment S319

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    9/19

    clauses. Pauses 2 s were coded, and mazes, which weredefined as any filled pause (e.g., uh, um), false start (e.g.,b* b*), or part word (e.g., fir*), were also transcribed andcoded.

    Transcriptions were coded for number of words andCIUs (Nicholas & Brookshire, 1993), from which percentage

    CIUs (% CIU), CIUs per minute, and words per minute(WPM) were calculated. Complete utterances, which containboth a complete sentence frame and relevance to the topic(Edmonds et al., 2009), were also coded. A complete sen-tence frame [+SV] was defined as an utterance containing asubject, verb, and object (S-V-O). Grammatical, morpho-logical, and phonemic errors were acceptable, as these werenot targeted in treatment and are not penalized accordingto Nicholas and Brookshire (1993).

    Relevance of utterances [+REL] was determined byevaluating whether the entire S-V-(O) segment was relevantto the topic. Multiple utterances with identical meaning

    and containing no new information were considered relevant

    only the first time. Thus, a complete utterance [+COMP]had to be coded as both [+SV] and [+REL] as shown by thefollowing examples (Edmonds et al., 2009):

    1. The tree is open [+SV][REL][COMP].

    2. A little guy with some sand on the shore with his handsthe sand [SV][+REL][COMP] (missing main verb).

    3. To walk through the step [SV][REL][COMP].

    4. The son is flying is a kite [+SV][+REL][+COMP].

    Once coding was complete, percentage of completeutterances was determined. Reliability for transcription andcoding was done on 100% of transcripts. Transcript reli-

    ability was 96%, and coding reliability ranged from 92% to96% across coding types.

    Outcome Measures for Research Question 4

    The WABR (Kerstesz, 2006) was administered todetermine presence and severity of aphasia. The CETI(Lomas et al., 1989) questionnaire was used to gather proxyratings of functional communication. The responses to the16 questions are represented on a visual analogue scale,which participants mark fromNot at all able to As able asbefore stroke along a 100-mm line. The higher the line ismarked, the more as able as before the stroke the commu-nication is rated. A range of communication scenarios are

    represented, includingcommunicating physical problems suchas aches and pains and being part of a conversation when itis fast and there are a number of people involved.

    Additional Testing

    The following tests were administered to provide amore comprehensive picture of pretreatment cognitive andlinguistic abilities and potentially to provide insight intoresponses to treatment across participants. They were alsoadministered posttreatment but were not evaluated as out-come measures.

    The CLQT (Helm-Estabrooks, 2001) was adminis-tered as a screen for cognitive impairments. All participants

    achieved a composite score of a moderate deficit or betterwith a range of abilities across the five subtests (Attention,Memory, Executive Function, Language, and VisuospatialSkills). See Table 4.

    Semantic processing of objects and actions was testedusing the pictures in Pyramids and Palm Trees (Howard &

    Patterson, 1992) and Kissing and Dancing (Bak & Hodges,2003). For both tests, participants point to one of two itemsthat goes best with the target item (e.g., eyesfor eyeglasses

    rather thanears). To reduce testing burden at posttreatment,we did not readminister these tests if participants scoredwithin normal limits at pretesting ( 47 points for each).See Table 4.

    The Sentence Comprehension Test from the NAVS(Thompson, 2011) was administered to examine compre-hension of sentences controlled by syntactic complexity. Thetest contains 30 items, including active and passive sentences,subject- and object-extracted wh-questions, and subjectand object relatives. See Table 4 for results.

    Treatment Training and Reliability

    Three licensed speech-language pathologists withextensive clinical (830 years) and research experience withaphasia conducted the treatment at the Malcom Randall VAMedical Center in Gainesville, Florida, and Brooks Rehab-ilitation in Jacksonville, Florida. Clinicians received com-prehensive training from the first author, which includedreview of a comprehensive treatment manual, videos ofVNeST being delivered, and practice conducting VNeST.They were also given answer sheets and checklists to ensureproper administration of VNeST. Once clinicians exhibitedthorough knowledge and ability, they began treatment withparticipants.

    To ensure consistency in execution of the treatmentprotocol, the first author watched 25% of the treatmentsessions live or on video and determined whether each stepwas conducted correctly. Clinicians were made aware of anydiscrepancies between the treatment protocol and execution.Percent reliability was calculated by dividing the totalnumber of steps that were conducted correctly by the totalnumber of steps observed. The treatment protocol wasfollowed with a reliability of 95.3%.

    Analysis and Results

    A Wilcoxon signed-ranks test was conducted todetermine whether there were significant (p < .05) groupchanges from pre- to posttreatment on all measures and frompretreatment to maintenance (3 months) for sentence probes,NAVS, and discourse. Given the relatively small sample sizeand the diversity of participantslinguistic profiles, we alsoprovide individual participantsscores. In order to interpretindividual changes, we used normative reference samples,when available. For measures without normative references,effect sizes were calculated. Details are provided belowand in tables. Finally, there were two instances of missingdata: no posttreatment WABR or sentence probe results

    S320 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology Vol. 23 S312S329 May 2014

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    10/19

    Table4.

    Pre-andposttreatmentscoresforaphasiaseverity,cognitivescreening

    ,semanticprocessing,attention,andsen

    tencecomprehensionforallparticipants.

    Test

    P1:AnomicP

    2:Anomic

    P3:Anomic

    P4:Anomic

    P5:Anomic

    P6:Cond

    P7:Cond

    P8:Cond

    P9:Wern

    P10:TCA

    P11:TCA

    Pre

    Post

    Pre

    Post

    Pre

    Post

    Pre

    Post

    Pre

    Post

    Pre

    Post

    Pre

    Pos

    t

    Pre

    Post

    Pre

    Post

    PreP

    ost

    Pre

    Post

    Aphasiaseverity:

    WABAQa

    81.6

    93.6

    88.1

    86.1

    81.9

    81.1

    78.5

    84.5

    84

    92.5

    72

    82

    66.6

    75

    75.6

    DNT

    52.9

    63.8

    81.4

    8

    2.8

    72.1

    79.4

    Information

    8

    10

    9

    8

    9

    8

    8

    9

    9

    10

    8

    8

    8

    8

    8

    DNT

    5

    7

    8

    8

    6

    8

    Fluency

    5

    9

    9

    8

    8

    9

    5

    6

    9

    9

    6

    9

    7

    8

    6

    DNT

    7

    7

    5

    5

    4

    5

    Comprehension

    196

    200

    177

    177

    159

    163

    181

    189

    184

    187

    196

    196

    148

    172

    184

    DNT

    91

    138

    192

    200

    195

    196

    Repetition

    91

    90

    87

    96

    90

    88

    88

    92

    60

    85

    63

    70

    66

    68

    64

    DNT

    37

    42

    97

    98

    94

    88

    Naming

    90

    88

    85

    86

    70

    66

    84

    86

    88

    94

    59

    72

    43

    61

    82

    DNT

    62

    68

    84

    86

    69

    81

    Cognition

    CLQTb

    Attention

    174

    Mild

    200

    WNL

    185

    W

    NL

    167

    Mild

    58

    Mod

    90

    Mod

    DNT

    DNT

    193

    WNL

    191

    WNL

    176

    WNL

    207

    WNL

    183

    WNL

    172Mild

    198

    WNL

    197

    WNL

    160

    Mild

    182

    WNL

    201

    WNL

    191

    W

    NL

    167

    WNL

    175

    WNL

    Memory

    136

    Mod

    155

    WNL

    130

    W

    NL

    124

    Mod

    94Sev

    110

    Mod

    DNT

    DNT

    175

    WNL

    169

    WNL

    126

    Mild

    174

    WNL

    90

    Sev

    102Sev

    149

    Mild

    168

    WNL

    77

    Sev

    81

    Sev

    138

    Mod

    149

    M

    ild

    116

    Mild

    134

    Mild

    Executive

    function

    22Mild

    29WNL

    22

    Mild

    22

    Mild

    19

    Mod

    16

    Mod

    DNT

    DNT

    26

    WNL

    34

    WNL

    23

    WNL

    25

    WNL

    23

    Mild

    21Mild

    27

    WNL

    30

    WNL

    18Mod

    26WNL

    30

    WNL

    27

    W

    NL

    15

    Mild

    15

    Mild

    Language

    23Mod

    27Mild

    22

    Mod

    20

    Sev

    21

    Mod

    21

    Mod

    DNT

    DNT

    31

    WNL

    31

    WNL

    21Mod

    28.5

    WNL

    18

    Sev

    20Sev

    26Mild

    30WNL

    17

    Sev

    19

    Sev

    25

    Mild

    26

    M

    ild

    19

    Mod

    22

    Mod

    Visuospatial

    84

    WNL

    99

    WNL

    92

    W

    NL

    87

    WNL

    46

    Mod

    47

    Mod

    DNT

    DNT

    90

    WNL

    99

    WNL

    84

    WNL

    93

    WNL

    87

    WNL

    78Mild

    98

    WNL

    98

    WNL

    73Mild

    83WNL

    97

    WNL

    96

    W

    NL

    81

    WNL

    82

    WNL

    Composite

    2.8Mild

    3.8

    WNL

    3.4Mild

    2.6Mild

    1.8Mod

    2.0Mod

    DNT

    DNT

    4.0

    WNL

    4.0

    WNL

    3.4Mild

    4.0

    WNL

    2.6Mild

    2.2Mod

    3.6

    WNL

    4.0

    WNL

    2.0Mod

    2.8Mild

    3.4Mild

    3.6

    W

    NL

    3.4Mild

    3.2

    Mild

    Semantic

    processing

    P&Pc

    (nouns)

    (n=52)

    46

    50

    46

    47

    40

    47

    48

    DNT

    48

    51

    52

    DNT

    51

    51

    50

    DNT

    47

    46

    50

    D

    NT

    47

    48

    K&Dc

    (verbs)

    (n=52)

    48

    50

    48

    DNT

    43

    42

    51

    DNT

    52

    DNT

    51

    DNT

    51

    49

    49

    DNT

    43

    47

    51

    D

    NT

    44

    47

    Sentence

    comprehension:

    NAVS

    sentences%

    97.1

    100

    62.9

    88.6

    77.1

    68.6

    80.0

    97.1

    91.4

    100

    91.4

    91.4

    5.6

    44.4

    33

    28

    60

    48.6

    41.6

    2

    7.8

    93.3

    90

    Note.

    Cond=conduction;Wern=Wernickes;CLQT=CognitiveLinguisticQuickTest;P&P=PyramidsandPalmTreestest;K&D=KissingandDancingtest;WNL=

    withinnormallimits;

    Mod=moderate;Sev=severe.

    aBoldedWABscoreswithimprovemen

    t5pointsareconsideredclinicallysignificant(Katz&Wertz,

    1997).bBoldedCLQT

    subtestsindicateimprovementinseverityratingfrompre-to

    posttreatment.cScoresfrom47to52a

    reWNLaccordingtotestmaterials.

    ManyparticipantswerenottestedduringposttestingiftheyscoredWNLpriortotreatm

    ent.

    Edmonds et al.: Verb Network Strengthening Treatment S321

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    11/19

    for P8 because of researcher error and no maintenancediscourse for P4 because of illness.

    Research Question 1: Improvement to Sentence

    Probes Containing Trained and Untrained Verbs

    Sentences containing trained and untrained wordsimproved significantly from pre- to posttreatment, and theseimprovements were maintained 3 months after completion oftreatment. No improvement was observed on the controltask, as hypothesized, at posttreatment or maintenance(p > .05). See Table 2.

    During the baseline phase, most participants scoredtoo high on the nonword repetition task to allow for po-tential improvement. Thus, the adjective control task wasused for all but two participants, P6 and P5. For P6, thetwo-syllable nonwords (n= 10) were used from the repeti-tion task. P5, who had a mild anomic aphasia with highconfrontation naming scores on other pretesting measures,

    scored too high on both control tasks, so forward andbackward digit span were used for his control.

    All participants averaged less than 50% accuracyacross all baseline probes. However, P1 and P4 exhibitedvariability during the baseline phase and had individual data

    points equaling 63.6%, thereby leaving less chance for po-tential improvement. A post hoc examination of the post-treatment control results shows that P1 did not exhibitimprovement. P4 showed miminal improvement on thecontrol task by repeating one nonword at posttreatmentthat was not produced during baseline. See sentence probeand control data for all participants in SupplementalTable 2.

    Although it was not an a priori research question, wewere interested in what components of the probe sentencesimproved. Virtually all words showed significant improve-ment posttreatment and at maintenance. See Table 2 fordetails.

    Effect sizes for trained and untrained sentence probeswere calculated for each participant. We used Cohens dcalculation (Cohen, 1988; d= M2 M1/s, whereM= themean, and s = the standard deviation at pretreatment). If thepretreatment standard deviation was zero, the trained anduntrained scores were pooled to calculate a nonzero standard

    deviation. When no existing effect size benchmarks existfor a specific task, it is optimal to estimate effect sizes from

    the task itself, if possible (Robey, Schultz, Crawford, &Sinner, 1999). Thus, we used the effect sizes from the par-ticipants in this study to calculate estimated benchmarks. Tocalculate benchmarks for trained sentence probes, we cal-culated the first, second, and third quartiles for all the effect

    sizes from the trained sentence probes (Beeson & Robey,2006), and those values (2.3, 3.7, and 5.5) were designated assmall, medium, and large effects. Because using the sameeffect sizes for trained and untrained probes is not advisable(Beeson & Robey, 2006; Robey et al., 1999), we calculatedseparate benchmarks for the untrained sentence probesand got slightly lower cutoffs (1.2, 1.7, and 3.3) for small,medium, and large effects. See Table 5 for effect sizes.

    Research Question 2: Confrontation Naming

    of Object and Actions on OANB and

    in Sentences on NAVS

    OANB

    There was a significant increase in noun and verb

    naming accuracy on OANB. See Table 2. To interpretindividual naming changes, we used the standard deviationsprovided in the OANB manual from a group of cognitivelynormal older adults (there are no comparable measuresfor people with aphasia). A standard deviation of 1.87 wasreported for nouns, 2.40 for verbs. Using two standarddeviations, an increase of 4 or more noun responses or 5 ormore verb responses was used as an indicator of improve-ment. See Table 3.

    To further refine our results, we adjusted OANB scoresby removing any words that happened to be trained duringtreatment or seen in trained sentence probes (30 possiblewords: 10 trained verbs [e.g., writing] and 20 corresponding

    agents and patients [e.g.,pipe]) to determine whether OANBimprovements were due to training and not necessarily anindication of generalization. Once we removed the overlap-ping items (37 nouns and verbs across participantsnotall participants were trained on the same verbs), we recal-culated percent correct accuracy. The pre- and posttreatmentscores (pre- and posttreatment nouns: 79.75% [12.57] and84.84% [12.80], respectively; pre- and posttreatment verbs:64.68% [18.48] and 73.24% [38.30], respectively) were vir-tually the same as those with all items included, and post-treatment significance was maintained for nouns (p= .016)and verbs (p = .007) (see Table 2).

    NAVSA significant increase in the production of sentences

    at posttreatment was not observed (p= .086), though therewas an increase in average accuracy from 29% to 42.4%.However, a significant increase was observed from pre-treatment to 3-month maintenance (p = .008). See Table 2for group results.

    Similar to the OANB, we wanted to ensure that the

    improvement on the NAVS was not due to improvementon items that were trained. Thus, we removed the results ofany sentences that contained a trained item (e.g., shaving,driving; range was 26 across participants) and recalculatedthe results. The results were similar to our original findings

    with pre- to posttreatment not being significant (p = .075)despite an increasing trend in percentage correct (pre: 26.88%[24.81]; post: 40.46% [27.05]). Maintenance results weresimilar to the original results (Table 2) as well (37.41% [26.13];accuracy,p = .010).

    The NAVS and the measure of complete utterancesused for discourse (discussed below) do not have normalreference measures to interpret individual change aftertreatment. Thus, we calculated effect sizes using Cohens d

    (Cohen, 1988) with a pooled group standard deviation.There was more variability in the group pretreatment scoresas compared with individual sentence probe baseline scores,resulting in much lower effect sizes for the group statistics.

    S322 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology Vol. 23 S312S329 May 2014

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    12/19

    For these measures, Cohens (1988) benchmarks of 0.5 and0.8 as medium and large effects were used. See Table 5 forall effect sizes.

    Research Question 3: Lexical Retrieval in Discourse

    The primary discourse measure was percentage ofcomplete utterances, which increased significantly from pre-to posttreatment with no change in number of utterances.

    At maintenance, percentage of complete utterances did notmaintain significance despite a similar average comparedwith posttreatment. This loss of significance may be dueto lower power at maintenance (n = 9) as compared withposttreatment (n= 11).

    In addition to percentage of complete utterances, weexamined potential lexical retrieval improvement at the wordlevel with % CIU. We also evaluated potential changes in

    efficiency with CIUs per minute and WPM (Nicholas &Brookshire, 1993). We used the standard error of the meanvalues reported for the aphasia group in Nicholas andBrookshire (1993): % CIU SEM= 2.0; CIUs/min = 4.0;WPM = 6.2. Changes of more than two standard errors of

    the mean were used as an indicator of improvement. Nogroup changes were observed for any of these measures. Per-cent pauses and mazes were also evaluated, with no changesobserved. See Tables 2 and 3.

    Research Question 4: WABR and CETI

    Posttreatment aphasia quotients improved signifi-cantly from an average of 75.91 to 82.12 (p= .017), with

    seven of the 10 participants exhibiting clinically significantimprovement (5 points; Katz & Wertz, 1997). See Tables 2and 4. Average scores for the CETI showed a significantincrease from pre- to posttreatment and 3-month mainte-nance. Changes for all participants at both time pointssurpassed the retest standard error of the mean of 5.2 es-tablished during CETI development (Lomas et al., 1989).See Tables 2 and 3.

    Discussion

    Previous studies with VNeST (Edmonds & Babb,2011; Edmonds et al., 2009) using single-subject design

    Table 5.Effect sizes for sentence probes, control task, NAVS sentence production, and complete utterances for all participants.

    Posttreatment Maintenance

    Sentence probesa Sentence probesa

    Pt TR UT Controlb TR UT Controlb

    1 6.22*** 4.92*** 0.15 6.22*** 9.39*** 0.922 2.02 3.03** 0.94 3.56* 3.82*** 0.3 2.92* 1.10 0.67 4.75** 1.10 0.114 7.81*** 4.15*** 2.68** 6.22*** 1.23* 1.57*5 3.69** 3.54** 0 3.69** 4.24*** 0.6 8.21*** 1.65* 0.66 3.82** 2.83** 0.617 2.62* 1.23* 0.94 3.83** 0.35 1.23*8 DNT DNT DNT DNT DNT DNT9 1.95 1.91c 0.94 1.10 1.91c 1.23*

    10 4.62** 1.43* 0.56 1.40 1.43* 2.6211 0.18 0.45 3.80*** 2.92* 0.67 1.57*

    Posttreatment Maintenance

    Pt % NAVS Sent Productiond % Complete Utterancesd % NAVS Sent Productiond % Complete Utterancesd

    1 0.00 0.25 0.12 0.69**2 0.34 1.03 0 1.053 0.56** 0.85*** 0.60** 1.01***4 1.35*** 0.07 0.23 N/A 5 0.11 1.53*** 0.23 1.17***6 1.69*** 0.22 0.36 0.187 1.58*** 0.01 1.07*** 0.088 0.23 1.51*** 0.71** 0.79**9 0 1.09*** 0.12 1.12***

    10 0.56 0.78** 1.19*** 1.08***11 1.35*** 0.21 0.60** 0.11

    aBolded items for sentence probes indicatesmall*, medium**, or large*** effect sizes as compared with pretreatment. bControls with boldedeffect sizes did not improve over the highest point at baseline (P7, P9), improved by 1 point (P4), or improved by 2 points (P11; he showed notreatment effect on probes). cWe did not note the medium effect of this value because of apparent inflation of the effect size due to a low standarddeviation calculation.dBolded items for NAVS Sentence Production and % complete utterances indicate medium and large effect sizes comparedwith pretreatment.

    *p< .05. **p< .01. ***p < .001.

    Edmonds et al.: Verb Network Strengthening Treatment S323

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    13/19

    reported encouraging results in six participants. The currentgroup study showed significant improvement on productionof sentences containing trained and untrained semanticallyrelated stimuli, replicating previous findings. Improvementon trained verb probes indicates that trained verb networkswere sufficiently activated during treatment to allow gener-

    alization to picture description, an untrained task. It washypothesized that such improvement would occur becauseof strengthened connections between the trained verb andrelated agentpatient combinations. Other researchers haveargued that such improvement could potentially be due toincreased awareness about verbs and/or a general strategy tospecify arguments around verbs (e.g., Marshall et al., 1998),improved efficiency in verb retrieval (e.g., Conroy et al.,2006), or, similarly, a freeing up of resources to facilitateimproved sentence retrieval (Linebarger, McCall, & Berndt,2004). Although these explanations are certainly possible,the improvement to untrained semantically related probes

    indicates more generalized semantic improvement and,

    potentially, generalization of improved structuralsyntacticabilities for simple sentences.

    Because VNeST aims to promote widespread lexicalretrieval abilities through activation of a multitude of diverseevent schemas, our outcome measures evaluated lexicalretrieval across a hierarchy of tasks. Group improvementwas observed on both object and action naming on OANB(Druks & Masterson, 2000), indicating improved access toand lexical retrieval of untrained object and action concepts.The majority of participants also exhibited generalizedimprovement to NAVS sentences. These findings, along withthose from previous studies, support the hypothesis thatVNeST may promote generalization of sentence production

    abilities beyond treated concepts and tasks (response andstimulus generalization, respectively). Presumably, im-provement beyond trained networks occurred, at least inpart, because of increased lexical retrieval. However, ade-quate sentence frame construction abilities are important forintegrating improved lexical retrieval into sentences (seeWebster & Whitworth, 2012). It is conceivable that VNeSTtasks, such as agent and patient retrieval around a trainedverb and reading aloud the agentverbpatient responses incanonical order, may have strengthened both lexical retrievaland basic sentence construction abilities. Participants whodid not improve on the NAVS may have had impairments

    in constructing a sentence frame requiring more explicit

    syntactic treatment in addition to the lexical retrieval tasks,as they all improved on OANB naming but not on NAVSsentences.

    Connected speech improved from pre- to posttreat-ment on percentage of complete utterances, suggestingimproved integration of relevant content words into a com-plete sentence frame. We also hypothesized improvementon % CIUs, a measure of relevant lexical retrieval. Onepossibility for the lack of % CIU improvement could be atradeoff between improved lexical retrieval within utterancesand increased informativeness overall. However, the find-ings do not support this, as four of the five participantswho improved on % CIUs also improved on percentage of

    complete utterances, thereby increasing the proportion ofinformative content while also integrating relevant wordsmore fully into sentence frames. Two additional participantsimproved on percentage of complete utterances but not on% CIUs, and one participant improved only on % CIUs.Thus, about a third of participants showed no improvement

    on discourse, a third showed improvement in either infor-mativeness or complete utterances, and a third showedimprovement to both informativeness and sentence produc-tion. Variable improvement on discourse within and acrossparticipants is commonly reported in verb treatmentsstudies (e.g., Edwards, Tucker, & McCann, 2004; Fink et al.,1992; Links et al., 2010; Schneider & Thompson, 2003;Webster et al., 2005) and is not surprising, given that dis-course requires the integration of cognitive (e.g., attention),micro-linguistic skills (e.g., lexical retrieval, syntax), andmacro-linguistic skills (e.g., coherence and cohesion). Thus,the persistent finding of variable outcomes in connectedspeech across studies is likely a reflection of the diversity and

    complexity of participant impairment patterns, the scopeand focus of treatment protocols, and the outcome measuresused to evaluate improvement (see Conroy et al., 2006;Webster & Whitworth, 2012). However, the participants whodid not improve on discourse in the current study showedimprovement on some combination of the other generaliza-tion tasks, suggesting a response treatment that did not gen-eralize to or was not captured in our outcome measures. Infuture studies, we plan to refine pretreatment testing measuresto better characterize impairment patterns and diversifydiscourse outcomes to better capture potential improvement.

    More global language abilities were measured on theWABR and the CETI. WABR scores improved signifi-

    cantly across the group, with 7 of the 10 participants showinga clinically significant improvement (Katz & Wertz, 1997).Increases in the WABR score corresponded to improve-ments in Production and/or Comprehension subtests. For allbut one (P9, who has Wernickes aphasia) of those whoimproved on WABR comprehension, comprehension alsoimproved on NAVS sentences. Although VNeST does notexplicitly target comprehension, this improvement may berelated to participants having to respond to many questions,includingwh-questions, during treatment.

    The responses from communication partners on theCETI reflect the perception of improved functional com-munication of treatment participants from pre- to post-

    treatment and maintenance. To ensure that communicationpartners would not be influenced by their initial scoring,we did not provide them with their pretreatment responsesat posttreatment or maintenance (which is different fromthe CETI protocol). Unsolicited responses from participantsfamilies also revealed functional communication changes.P4s spouse reported the following regarding her ability toparticipate in her medical care: You guys helped her speechand communication so much. She was talking in completesentences when she was done with it. When she got thediagnosis of [omitted for privacy], she was able to talk to us.She was able to ask the doctors questions and participatein her medical plan. That was so very important to us.See

    S324 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology Vol. 23 S312S329 May 2014

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    14/19

    additional family comments in Supplemental Table 3. Pleasenote that we acknowledge that proxy and anecdotal reportare not direct evaluations of functional communication.However, combined with the impairment-based results, it isconceivable that participants experienced some improvedcommunication abilities outside of the clinic.

    Because VNeST-related improvements are assumed tobe, at least in part, due to activation of a large number ofdiverse event schemas, we evaluated actual responses toverify that participants were, in fact, producing a varietyof responses. We found that participants produced a largenumber of novel agents and patients throughout the courseof treatment (first half: 52 agents and 48 patients: secondhalf: 44 agents and 46 patients). That is, the 52 agents and48 patients produced in the first half of treatment were notproduced again in the second half of treatment, and thosenovel 44 and 46 agents and patients produced in the secondhalf were not produced in the first half. Participants also

    produced an additional 46 agents and 46 patients across both

    halves of treatment, totaling 145 agents and 138 patientsproduced during treatment.

    We also examined the nature of responses and foundthat participants produced a diverse mix of scenarios, in-cludingprototypical(e.g., quarterbackthrowsfootball,womancarriespocketbook, oxpullswagon), personal(e.g.,bossdrivesbig truck, [dogs name]shakestoy, Iread

    funny pages, Uncleboilsshrimp, [wifes name]sewscurtains),encyclopedic or world knowledge (e.g.,judgereadsconstitutional law book, hula dancershakeships, state

    policeweightrucks, Benjamin Franklinflykite, funeral

    directorbrusheshair, Mrs. Clintonwritesbiography), andother (e.g., mad scientistboilhead of Frankenstein, vampires

    bite

    humans) topics. Thus, treatment on only 10 verbs resultedin activation and production of a wide range of noun con-cepts (not including responses towh-questions).

    These data highlight the extent of noun retrievalin VNeST, where the semantics and phonology of nounconcepts are presumably strengthened. However, there isrelatively little production of the trained verb, and thatproduction occurs only after the verb is provided by theclinician. Step 5 of the protocol was added to promoteindependent verb retrieval. However, this task is not entirelyin the spirit of VNeST, which encourages truly independentretrieval of concepts. Further, this step could be achieved

    through recruitment of short-term memory. Given the

    relative complexity of verb retrieval and the necessity forverb retrieval to construct a sentence (Marshall et al., 1998),more access to phonology (i.e., more production) of verbs inVNeST may be warranted to maximize the potential forgeneralization. Evidence for this is seen in the sentence proberesults, where verbs improved less robustly than the agentsand patients. Future studies will investigate integration ofmore verb production into the protocol.

    The improvements reported in the current and pre-vious VNeST studies in (the reduction of) aphasia severity,functional communication (by report), and lexical retrievaland sentence production abilities from single words throughdiscourse are generally more robust than reports from

    other verb treatment studies reported in the literature (seeConroy et al., 2006; Webster & Whitworth, 2012). Thefindings also suggest high potential clinical utility of VNeST.We controlled treatment dosage to 35 hr per week over10 weeks, consistent with a traditional aphasia treatmentplan and treated only 10 verbs per participant (fewer than

    other verb treatments; e.g., Raymer & Ellsworth, 2002;Wambaugh & Ferguson, 2007; Webster, Morris, & Franklin,2005), though personal and salient responses to verb promptswere encouraged from participants to promote relevanceand interest. Treatment is also low-tech (only pen and paperrequired) and as such is transportable and easily deliveredin multiple settings. In addition, VNeST adapts relativelyeasily to computer and telepractice use (Furnas & Edmonds,2014), providing clinicians and participants with morepotential opportunities for home treatment and practice.Thus, VNeST is easy to administer and potentially efficient,and it offers high potential gains.

    Though more research is needed to better define par-

    ticipant characteristics, results across VNeST studies sug-gest that participants with moderatesevere to mild aphasiamay be potential candidates. We have not conducted adetailed analysis of outcomes based on aphasia types. How-ever, on the basis of our observations, persons with nonfluent,even agrammatic, aphasia show gains to lexical retrievalof content words in sentence production with mixed im-provement to discourse. Syntax (i.e., word order) in theseparticipants is typically well maintained. It should be notedthat none of the sentence probes are reversible. Because wordorder is highly biased toward animate subjects, partici-pants with potential syntax or mapping impairments may beusing animacy cues to aid production. Persons with fluent

    aphasia tend to exhibit more efficient and informativesentence production posttreatment, including reduced cir-cumlocution and reduced use of general terms (e.g., guy,lady) and pronouns. Discourse is also variable in theseparticipants. Finally, we used fairly permissible inclusioncriteria, to allow for relatively broad generalization ofresults. This resulted in a mix of aphasia types, including onlyone person with Wernickes aphasia. In future studies, wewill increase targeted recruitment and perform more detailedtesting to gain more insight into the effects of pretreatmentimpairment patterns and aphasia types.

    The development of aphasia treatment protocols caninvolve many Phase 2 studies to narrow potential candidates,

    refine treatment and outcome measures, and estimate dos-age (Robey, 2004). Three studies with VNeST have nowtested 17 participants, with overall positive results. However,there are still limitations that need to be addressed inaddition to those we have already mentioned. First, we needobjective measures of functional communication in additionto proxy report. Second, we need to evaluate the effectsof treating verbs with more diverse semantic and syntacticrepresentations, as only a small number of verbs and verbtypes have been trained, largely because of the limitations ofthe sentence probes (e.g., semantically related, imageable,transitive verbs). The process of refining an aphasia treat-ment protocol and understanding who might optimally

    Edmonds et al.: Verb Network Strengthening Treatment S325

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    15/19

    benefit from it is a long process. However, we are committedto that process in order to contribute evidence-based optionsto clinicians who serve persons with aphasia.

    Acknowledgments

    This work was supported by the Department of VeteransAffairs, Veterans Health Administration, Rehabilitation Researchand Development Grant 1I01RX000563-01 (to Lisa A. Edmonds).We acknowledge the research participants and their families fortheir enthusiasm and motivation throughout. We also extend ourthanks to the following collaborators, research assistants, andclinicians in the Aphasia Lab at the Brain Rehabilitation andResearch Center at the Malcom Randall VA Medical Center andBrooks Rehabilitation in Jacksonville, FL: Sam Wu, Jodi Morgan,Ceil Brooks, Brayleah Kernan, Flo Singletary, and CarolynHanson.

    References

    Bak, T. H., & Hodges, J. R.(2003). Kissing and Dancing

    A test todistinguish the lexical and conceptual contributions to noun/verband action/object dissociation. Preliminary results in patientswith frontotemporal dementia.Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16,

    169181.Barbieri, E., Basso, A., Frustaci, M., & Luzzatti, C.(2010). Argu-

    ment structure deficits in aphasia: New perspective on modelsof lexical production. Aphasiology, 24, 14001423.

    Bastiaanse, R., Hurkmans, J., & Links, P. (2006). The training ofverb production in Brocas aphasia: A multiple-baseline across-behaviours study. Aphasiology, 20, 298311.

    Beeson, P. M., & Robey, R. R. (2006). Evaluating single-subjecttreatment research: Lessons learned from the aphasia literature.Neuropsychology Review, 16, 161169.

    Berndt, R. S., Mitchum, C. C., Haendiges, A. N., & Sandson, J.

    (1997). Verb retrieval in aphasia: II. Relationship to sentenceprocessing. Brain and Language, 56, 107137.

    Black, M., & Chiat, S. (2003). Nounverb dissociations: A multi-faceted phenomenon.Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16, 231250.

    Cohen, J.(1988).Statistical power analysis for the behavioralsciences(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Conroy, P., Sage, K., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Towardstheory-driven therapies for aphasic verb impairments: A reviewof current theory and practice. Aphasiology, 20, 11591185.

    Conroy, P., Sage, K., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2009). A comparisonof word versus sentence cues as therapy for verb naming inaphasia. Aphasiology, 23, 462482.

    Dabul, B.(2000).Apraxia Battery for Adults (2nd ed.). Austin, TX:Pro-Ed.

    Druks, J., & Masterson, J. (2000).An Object and Action NamingBattery. Hove, England: Psychology Press.

    Edmonds, L. A., & Babb, M. (2011). Effect of Verb NetworkStrengthening Treatment in moderate-to-severe aphasia. AmericanJournal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20, 131145.

    Edmonds, L. A., & Mizrahi, S. (2011). Online priming of verbs andthematic roles in younger and older adults. Aphasiology, 25,

    14881506.Edmonds, L. A., Nadeau, S., & Kiran, S. (2009). Effect of Verb

    Network Strengthening Treatment (VNeST) on lexical retrievalof content words in sentencesin persons withaphasia. Aphasiology,23,402424.

    Edwards, S., & Tucker, K.(2006). Verb retrieval in fluent aphasia: Aclinical study.Aphasiology, 20, 644675.

    Edwards, S., Tucker, K., & McCann, C.(2004). The contribution ofverb retrieval to sentence construction: A clinical study. Brainand Language, 91,7879.

    Faroqi-Shah, Y., & Graham, L. E.(2011). Treatment of semanticverb classes in aphasia: Acquisition and generalization effects.Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 25, 399418.

    Ferretti, T. R., McRae, K., & Hatherell, A. (2001). Integrating verbs,situation schemas, and thematic role concepts. Journal ofMemory and Language, 44, 516547.

    Fink, R. B., Martin, N., Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., & Myers,

    J. L.(1992). Facilitation of verb retrieval skills in aphasia: Acomparison of two approaches. Clinical Aphasiology, 21, 263275.

    Furnas, D. W., & Edmonds, L. A. (2014). The effect of ComputerVerb Network Strengthening Treatment on lexical retrieval inaphasia. Aphasiology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/02687038.2013.869304

    Helm-Estabrooks, N.(2001).Cognitive Linguistic Quick Test.San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Howard, D., & Patterson, K. (1992). Pyramids and Palm Trees.London, England: Harcourt Assessment.

    Katz, R. C., & Wertz, R. T. (1997). The efficacy of computer-

    provided reading treatment for chronic aphasic adults. Journalof Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 493507.

    Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). PsycholinguisticAssessments of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA).New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Kerstesz, A.(2006).Western Aphasia BatteryRevised. Austin, TX:Pro-Ed.

    Kim, M., Adingono, M. F., & Revoir, J. S. (2007). Argument struc-ture enhanced verb naming treatment: Two case studies.Contem-porary Issues in Communication Science and Disorders, 34,2436.

    Kiran, S., & Bassetto, G. (2008). Evaluating the effectiveness ofsemantic-based treatment for naming deficits in aphasia: Whatworks? Seminars in Speech and Language, 29, 7182.

    Kiran, S., & Thompson, C. K. (2003). The role of semantic com-plexity in treatment of naming deficits: Training semantic

    categories in fluent aphasia by controlling exemplar typicality.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46,

    773787.Laine, M., & Martin, N. (2006). Anomia: Theoretical and clinical

    aspects. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Lambon Ralph, M. A., Snell, C., Fillingham, J. K., Conroy, P., &

    Sage, K.(2010). Predicting the outcome of anomia therapy forpeople with aphasia post CVA: Both language and cognitivestatus are key predictors. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 20,

    289305.Linebarger, M. C., McCall, D., & Berndt, R. (2004). The role of

    processing support in the remediation of aphasic languageproduction disorders.Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 267282.

    Links, P., Hurkmans, J., & Bastiaanse, R. (2010). Training verb andsentence production in agrammatic Brocas aphasia.Aphasiology,24,13031325.

    Lomas, J., Pickard, L., Bester, S., Elbard, H., Finlayson, A., &

    Zoghaib, C.(1989). The Communicative Effectiveness Index:Development and psychometric evaluation of a functionalcommunication measure for adults aphasia. Journal of Speechand Hearing Disorders, 54,113124.

    Marshall, J., Pring, T., & Chiat, S. (1998). Verb retrieval andsentence production in aphasia.Brain and Language, 63, 83159.

    McCann, C., & Doleman, J.(2011). Verb retrieval in nonfluentaphasia: A replication of Edwards & Tucker, 2006.Journal ofNeurolinguistics, 24, 237248.

    McNeil, M. R., Robin, D. A., & Schmidt, R. A. (2009). Apraxiaof speech. In M. R. McNeil (Ed.), Clinical management of

    S326 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology Vol. 23 S312S329 May 2014

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    16/19

    sensorimotor speech disorders(2nd ed., pp. 249268). New York,NY: Thieme Medical Publishers.

    McRae, K., Hare, E., & Ferretti, T. R.(2005). A basis for generatingexpectancies for verbs from nouns. Memory & Cognition, 33,11741184.

    Miller, J., & Iglesias, A. (2012). Systematic Analysis of LanguageTranscripts (SALT) (Version 2012 Research Version) [Computersoftware]. Middleton, WI: SALT Software, LLC.

    Mitchum, C., & Berndt, R. (1994). Verb retrieval and sentence con-struction: Effects of targeted intervention. In M. J. Riddoch &G. Humphreys (Eds.),Cognitive neuropsychology and cognitiverehabilitation(pp. 317348). Hove, England: Erlbaum.

    Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Badirian, V., Charbonneau, S.,

    Whitehead, V., Collin, I., . . . Chertkow, H. (2005). The MontrealCognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mildcognitive impairment. Journal of the American GeriatricsSociety, 53,695699.

    Nicholas, L. E., & Brookshire, R. H. (1993). A system for quan-tifying the informativeness and efficiency of the connectedspeech of adults with aphasia.Journal of Speech and HearingResearch, 36,338350.

    Nickels, L.(2002). Therapy for naming disorders: Revisiting,revising, and reviewing.Aphasiology, 16, 935979.

    Park, H., & Edmonds, L. A.(2013, May). Comparing semantic andsyntactic expectation between verbs and thematic roles: Evidence

    from eyetracking. Poster presented at the Clinical AphasiologyConference, Tucson, AZ.

    Rapcsak, S. Z., Beeson, P. M., Henry, M. L., Leyden, A., Kim, E.,

    Rising, K., . . . Cho, H.(2009). Phonological dyslexia and dys-graphia: Cognitive mechanisms and neural substrates. Cortex,45,575591.

    Raymer, A., & Kohen, F. (2006). Word-retrieval treatment inaphasia: Effects of sentence context. Journal of RehabilitationResearch and Development, 43, 367377.

    Raymer, A. M., & Ellsworth, T. A.(2002). Response to contrastingverbretrieval treatments: A casestudy. Aphasiology,16, 10311045.

    Robey, R. R.(2004). A five-phase model for clinical-outcomeresearch.Journal of Communication Disorders, 37, 401411.

    Robey, R. R., Schultz, M. C., Crawford, A. B., & Sinner, C. A.

    (1999). Single-subject clinical-outcome research: Designs, data,effect sizes, and analyses. Aphasiology, 13,445473.

    Rsler, F., Streb, J., & Haan, H. (2001). Event-related brainpotentials evoked by verbs and nouns in primed lexical decisiontask.Psychophysiology, 38, 694703.

    Schneider, S. L., & Thompson, C. K. (2003). Verb production inagrammatic aphasia: The influence of semantic class andargument structure properties on generalisation. Aphasiology,17,213241.

    Thompson, C. K. (2011). The Argument Structure Production Test/The Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences. Chicago,IL: Northwestern University.

    Wambaugh, J. L., & Ferguson, M. (2007). Application of seman-tic feature analysis to retrieval of action names in aphasia.Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 44,

    381394.

    Wambaugh, J. L., Mauszycki, S., & Wright, S. (2014). Semanticfeature analysis: Application to confrontation of actions inaphasia. Aphasiology, 28, 124.

    Webster, J., & Gordon, B. (2009). Contrasting therapy effects forverb and sentence processing difficulties: A discussion of whatworked and why. Aphasiology, 23, 12311251.

    Webster, J. L., Morris, J., & Franklin, S. (2005). Effects of therapytargeted at verb retrieval and the realisation of the predicateargument structure: A case study. Aphasiology, 19, 748764.

    Webster, J., & Whitworth, A. (2012). Treating verbs in aphasia:Exploring the impact of therapy at the single word and sentencelevels.International Journal of Language and CommunicationDisorders, 6,619636.

    Edmonds et al.: Verb Network Strengthening Treatment S327

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    17/19

    Appendix(p. 1 of 2)

    Verb Network Strengthening Treatment Materials and Protocol

    Treatment Materials

    Treatment stimuli consisted of the following:

    1. 10 cards containing the names of the 10 trained verbs (Verb Set 1) (e.g.,measure)2. 68 cards for each verb containing 34 agents and 34 patients that form 34 pairs related to each verb (e.g., chefsugar,

    carpenterlumber, surveyorland,and designerroomfor the verb measure)3. 5 cards containing the following words: who, what, where, when, why4. An average of 25 sentences per verb to be used for semantic judgment.

    These sentences spanned four categories: (a) correct (The designer measures the room), (b) inappropriate agent (The infantmeasures the lumber), (c) inappropriate patient (The chef measures the television), and (d) thematic reversal (The room measuresthe designer).

    Treatment Protocol

    Step 1: The clinician layswhoandwhatcards on the table facing the participant and asksWho can/might verb something/someone?(e.g., Who might drive something?). When the participant cannot produce an agent, he or she is given a semanticor context cue (e.g.,Who might drive for his/her job?). If that cue does not work, then four cards with possible choices areprovided, three cards with foils and one plausible option. The participant reads through the options one at a time (with assistancefrom the clinician, if needed) and chooses the correct response. Once an agent is chosen, then a corresponding patient isrequested (e.g., If the participant said soldier,then the patient might be tank). Participants are encouraged to provide at leastone personal pair (e.g., dadboatfordrive), and responses can change from week to week. (Previous VNeST studies requesteda list of agents or patients and then the corresponding noun, but it is more natural to generate one schema at a time.) In aneffort to promote more participant involvement during this step in the current study, the last six participants wrote their responseafter they said it (rather than the clinician providing the written word). If they were incorrect about the spelling, they were giventhe word to copy (and in the same way no phonemic cues or feedback was given, neither was there any writing feedback[e.g., phoneme to grapheme correspondence]). Examination of probe results revealed no advantage or difference for participantswho wrote responses.

    Step 2: The participant reads the triads aloud (e.g., cheffrytomatoes). Morphology or inflection is not required, but it is notdiscouraged if participants use it (e.g., The chef is frying the tomatoes).

    Step 3: The participant chooses one scenario (e.g., chef scenario) and answers three wh-questions about it (e.g.,where?[restaurant], when?[before restaurant opens], why? [to make them delicious]) about it. If the participant has difficulty

    understanding thewh-questions, then clarification is given (e.g.,Where? What location or place?) See Figure A1.Step 4: The participant decides whether semantic judgment sentences (12 total, 3 from each category) are correct or not.The four categories are (a) correct (The designer measures the room), (b) inappropriate agent (The infant measures the lumber),(c) inappropriate patient (The chef measures the television), and (d) thematic reversal (The room measures the designer).

    Step 5: The participant is asked what verb or action he or she has been working on. This step was added to allow oneopportunity for independent verb retrieval.

    Step 6: Step 1 is repeated, but no cues are given, and the step is terminated when the participant retrieves three to four pairsor when the participant cannot generate any more pairs, whichever occurs first.

    S328 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology Vol. 23 S312S329 May 2014

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    18/19

    Appendix(p. 2 of 2)

    Verb Network Strengthening Treatment Materials and Protocol

    Figure A1. Example of card layout for VNeST steps 1 and 3.

    Edmonds et al.: Verb Network Strengthening Treatment S329

  • 8/11/2019 Effect of Verb Network Strengthening

    19/19

    Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without

    permission.