34
Language contact in language obsolescence Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald 1. Preamble The di¤erence between language change in ‘‘healthy’’ and in endangered or obsolescent languages very often lies not in the sorts of change, which tend to be the same (Campbell and Muntzel 1989). It tends to lie in the quantity of change and in the speed with which the obsolescent language changes (see Schmidt 1985: 213; Aikhenvald 2002: 243–264). Language displacement frequently results in reduction of paradigms, simplification and loss of the language’s own features, and, ultimately, language shift and loss. As the obsolescent language is ‘‘retreating, contracting, as it gradually falls into disuse’’ (Dixon 1991a: 199), we expect it to be flooded with an influx of patterns and forms from the dominant language. Contact-induced changes can roughly be divided into three sorts, in terms of their stability. Following Tsitsipis (1998: 34), it appears useful to divide changes into completed, ongoing (or continuous), and discontinuous. Completed changes cover those aspects of the grammatical system of a lan- guage which do not show any synchronic variation and which go beyond speakers’ awareness (see the discussion of a Spanish-influenced passive in Purepecha by Chamoreau 2005). Ongoing or continuous changes are those in progress; here the degree of influence of the other language depends on the speaker’s competence and possibly other, sociolinguistic, variables (such as age or degree of participation in community life). Discontinuous changes are one-o¤ deviations characteristic of individual speakers. In the situation of language attrition, these often di¤erentiate fluent speakers from less proficient ones. This classification of changes is particularly important for distinguishing between old and established di¤usional processes – characterized by com- pleted changes – and new, in-coming continuous changes making their way into a speech community. In a situation of language obsolescence, one expects to encounter a multiplicity of sporadic changes which would be considered to be mistakes by fluent speakers (if they existed). Such aberrant individual innovations are tantamount to Tsitsipis’ ad hoc or discontinuous changes. The impact of language shift as seen through Bereitgestellt von | provisional account Angemeldet | 212.87.45.97 Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

The volume deals with previously undescribed morphosyntactic variations and changes appearing in settings involving language contact. Contact-induced changes are defined as dynamic and multiple, involving internal change as well as historical and sociolinguistic factors. A variety of explanations are identified and their relationships are analyzed. Only a multifaceted methodology enables this fine-grained approach to contact-induced change. A range of methodologies are proposed, but the chapters generally have their roots in a typological perspective. The contributors recognize the precautionary principle: for example, they emphasize the difficulty of studying languages that have not been described adequately and for which diachronic data are not extensive or reliable.

Citation preview

Page 1: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Language contact in language obsolescence

Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

1. Preamble

The di¤erence between language change in ‘‘healthy’’ and in endangered

or obsolescent languages very often lies not in the sorts of change, which

tend to be the same (Campbell and Muntzel 1989). It tends to lie in the

quantity of change and in the speed with which the obsolescent language

changes (see Schmidt 1985: 213; Aikhenvald 2002: 243–264). Language

displacement frequently results in reduction of paradigms, simplification

and loss of the language’s own features, and, ultimately, language shift

and loss. As the obsolescent language is ‘‘retreating, contracting, as it

gradually falls into disuse’’ (Dixon 1991a: 199), we expect it to be flooded

with an influx of patterns and forms from the dominant language.

Contact-induced changes can roughly be divided into three sorts, in

terms of their stability. Following Tsitsipis (1998: 34), it appears useful to

divide changes into completed, ongoing (or continuous), and discontinuous.

Completed changes cover those aspects of the grammatical system of a lan-

guage which do not show any synchronic variation and which go beyond

speakers’ awareness (see the discussion of a Spanish-influenced passive in

Purepecha by Chamoreau 2005). Ongoing or continuous changes are those

in progress; here the degree of influence of the other language depends on

the speaker’s competence and possibly other, sociolinguistic, variables (such

as age or degree of participation in community life). Discontinuous changes

are one-o¤ deviations characteristic of individual speakers. In the situation

of language attrition, these often di¤erentiate fluent speakers from less

proficient ones.

This classification of changes is particularly important for distinguishing

between old and established di¤usional processes – characterized by com-

pleted changes – and new, in-coming continuous changes making their

way into a speech community. In a situation of language obsolescence,

one expects to encounter a multiplicity of sporadic changes which would

be considered to be mistakes by fluent speakers (if they existed). Such

aberrant individual innovations are tantamount to Tsitsipis’ ad hoc or

discontinuous changes. The impact of language shift as seen through

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 2: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

discontinuous changes in the context of displacive language contact is the

topic of this article.

2. The various facets of language obsolescence

An obsolescent language is no longer actively used or transmitted. We dis-

tinguish several kinds of social context in which this occurs.1

Firstly, an obsolescent language is no longer actively spoken by a com-

munity, and is not transmitted to the next generation. Its knowledge is often

confined to a handful of last fluent speakers – as is the case for Ingrian

Finnish in Estonia (Riionheimo 2002), Bare (Aikhenvald 1995), Dyirbal

and Yidiny (Dixon 1991a, b), Mawayana (Carlin 2006), and Resıgaro

(Allin 1975) – or to a handful of not-very-fluent speakers or semi-speakers,

or even rememberers – as in the case of Nivkh, a Paleo-Siberian isolate

(Gruzdeva 2002), or Nyulnyul, an Australian language (McGregor 2002;

see Hill and Hill 1986, Hill 1985, for a definition of the terms). We will

refer to this as ‘‘global’’ language obsolescence.

Alternatively, language obsolescence can a¤ect individuals or groups of

individuals living away from the language community. This is often the

case with speakers of immigrant languages, spoken by groups of varied

size whose major language is the dominant language of the country. These

varieties are sometimes called ‘‘heritage’’ languages. The existing studies

include Heritage Russian (Pereltsvaig 2008, and references there; Kagan

and Dillon 2001), Heritage Italian, Heritage Norwegian, Heritage Swedish,

and Heritage Czech (see Bettoni 1991; Milani 1996; Hjelde 1996; Klintborg

1999; Henzl 1981).

Along similar lines, people who live away from the community where

the language is actively spoken also display signs of obsolescence. Obsoles-

cent speakers of many indigenous languages of Papua New Guinea form

part of urban communities whose dominant language is overwhelmingly

Papua New Guinea English and also Tok Pisin. The domain of their

ancestral language is often limited to token symbolic use in speech formu-

las. And when the speakers attempt to use the language, its make-up is

markedly di¤erent from the way it is spoken by the speech community in

the original area. I have observed this ‘‘individual’’ or ‘‘localized’’ language

obsolescence among Manambu speakers – see Section 3.1 (example (1)).

1. The examples discussed here reflect what Campbell and Muntzel (1989) call‘‘gradual death’’ of a language. We do not consider instances of ‘‘suddendeath’’ or ‘‘radical death’’ of a language, nor of ‘‘bottom-up death.’’

78 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 3: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Instances of individual or localized language obsolescence may occur

within a broader context of a ‘‘global’’ obsolescence of a language. Paumarı,

an Arawa language from Southern Amazonia, is gradually falling out of

use, and more rapidly so in the communities on the River Ituxı than on

the River Purus. As a result, speakers from the Ituxı communities display

more signs of language obsolescence (Aikhenvald 2010; Chapman and

Derbyshire 1991).

The interest of individual language obsolescence for a student of lan-

guage change lies in the possibility of comparing the obsolescent or heri-

tage language with the variety still actively spoken in the ‘‘homeland.’’

In the case of ‘‘global’’ obsolescence, we are sometimes fortunate to have

access to a description of a pre-obsolescent variety of a language. For

instance, Krejnovich’s work gives us access to Nivkh as it used to be

before the language stopped being transmitted to the next generation.

Numerous descriptions of Ingrian Finnish allow us to trace the obsoles-

cence of this language as it is currently spoken in Estonia (Riionheimo

2002). The grammar of traditional Paumarı by Chapman and Derbyshire

(1991) allows us to trace the nature of obsolescence in the present-day lan-

guage. The obsolescent Dyirbal (Schmidt 1985; Dixon 1991a) can be con-

trasted and compared with the language described by Dixon (1972) when

it was still fluently spoken. And the Tariana spoken by traditional repre-

sentatives of the older generation (nowadays in their late eighties) can be

contrasted with the speech of younger people who are gradually relinquish-

ing their ancestral language.

A situation of language obsolescence presupposes obsolescent speakers.

Their proficiency in the given language may, of course, vary (some may

be considered barely ‘‘rememberers,’’ others may conserve a degree of

fluency). The di¤erence between obsolescent speakers of obsolescent lan-

guages and obsolescent speakers of languages in active use elsewhere may

be compared to a well-known di¤erence between societal multilingualism

and individual multilingualism. The former is a social phenomenon and is

of prime concern to sociolinguists. The latter reflects personal history and

is of interest to psychologists more than to sociolinguists. However, we do

find that processes of language obsolescence appear to be similar in the

context of ‘‘global’’ and of ‘‘local’’ obsolescence (at the level of the individual

speaker). This suggests the presence of shared mechanisms which could,

and should, be investigated.

A word of caution is in order. Even if we do have access to what can be

considered a ‘‘pre-obsolescent’’ variety, we cannot always be sure that this

variety did not already bear some signs of decay. When R. M. W. Dixon

Language contact in language obsolescence 79

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 4: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

started his fieldwork on Dyirbal in 1963, the language was actively spoken,

in the domestic sphere, by several score people, including children. Over a

quarter of a century, Dixon has seen the language decline ‘‘from a state in

which there was an abundance of speakers . . . to one in which there is just

one good consultant left for each of three dialects, with no one to go to for

a second opinion’’ (Dixon 1991a: 183). But even in the good old days of

the early sixties, older speakers would comment on the fact that ‘‘words

used to be longer’’ in the language as they can recall it spoken in their

childhood by those old people who had passed away. That is, the process

of language contraction may have started long before the linguist came to

the scene, and this ‘‘discourse of nostalgia’’ (Hill 1998) may reflect speakers’

awareness of this. The few older representatives of the Tariana-speaking

community – the late Candido Brito, Americo and Jose Manoel Brito –

can be viewed as keepers of the traditional language. However, by the time

they were born (between 1911 and 1920), Tariana communities were already

a¤ected by Brazilian influence, and their traditional lives were under destruc-

tion. None of the three elders could remember the full version of tradi-

tional rituals and the ritual language. We can safely assume that even their

Tariana, fluent as it is compared to that of the younger generation, has

already su¤ered from a certain amount of loss. We can hypothesize that

this could have been accompanied by a shift to a dominant language.

Sadly, in many cases the obsolescent variety is the only one which is

professionally described. Allin (1975) is based on fieldwork with a handful

of last speakers of Resıgaro, a North Arawak language. Carlin (2006) is

based on her fieldwork with two last speakers of Mawayana, also Arawak.

The same applies to Nyulnyul (McGregor 2002), Araki (Francois 2002),

and quite a few other languages from many parts of the world. In none

of these cases do we have access to a full ‘‘pre-obsolescent’’ variety. Most

likely, we are dealing with ‘‘a mere remnant of what the language must

have been like when many speakers used it as their only means of commu-

nication’’ (Haas 1941).

Linguistic consequences of language obsolescence – ‘‘global,’’ ‘‘individ-

ual,’’ or ‘‘localized’’ – include simplification and reduction of grammar

and lexicon. Categories absent from the dominant language are particu-

larly endangered. So the system of numeral classifiers becomes reduced to

just a few in Korean as it is spoken by young people in Canberra (Lee

1997) whose major language of communication is English. (This is also

known as ‘‘negative borrowing.’’) The obsolescent language often su¤ers

from stylistic reduction and dialect mixing, and also speakers’ insecurity

(see Campbell and Muntzel 1989; Chamoreau 2000; Grenoble and Whaley

80 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 5: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

1998; Aikhenvald 2002: Chapter 11; Dixon 1991a, b, for Dyirbal and

Yidiny; Helimsky 2007: 218, for Selkup).

The impact of the increasingly dominant language on the receding,

obsolescent language gradually falling into disuse tends to involve a

massive influx of non-native forms. The outcome of this influx may result

in unusual phenomena, which may include occasional borrowed bound

morpheme and mixed paradigms (Section 3). If speakers tend to avoid

imported forms, impending language shift may result in a spread and

expansion of look-alikes and a massive calquing of structures from the

dominant language and accelerated di¤usion of patterns (Section 4).2

Speakers of obsolescent languages vary in their proficiency, from fluent

language users to semi-speakers with limited competence (Dorian 1973:

417, 1977). In some cases, evaluation may be possible using internal or

external clues. But in many cases we have no information about the level

of speakers’ knowledge: if a typologically unusual phenomenon is based

on such uncertain sources, the validity of the phenomenon is cast in doubt.

3. Non-native forms in language obsolescence

An influx of non-native forms is a typical feature of obsolescent speakers.

In Haugen’s (1989: 67) words, ‘‘the adoption of English loans’’ was the

‘‘first great step in the direction of English’’ for immigrant speakers of

Norwegian. The adoption of non-native forms often involves lexical items

and also grammatical forms. Conjunctions and discourse markers, highly

susceptible to borrowing under any circumstances of language contact, are

the ‘‘usual suspects.’’

In Section 3.1, we discuss relevant examples of individual language obso-

lescence in Manambu, comparing a fluent and an obsolescent speaker. We

2. Language contact does not explain all the discontinuous changes in languageobsolescence. For instance, terminal speakers of Arvanitika Albanian in Greecesporadically lose gender and number agreement; their entire system of tense-aspect-mood categories is disintegrating – imperfective past forms are notused at all, and the marking of grammatical person is ‘‘morphologically dis-torted’’ (Tsitsitpis 1998: 44–62). This ‘‘agrammatism’’ cannot be explained by‘‘negative borrowing,’’ that is, loss of categories not present in the dominantlanguage, Greek, since Greek possesses all the categories now lost in the obso-lescent Arvanitika (Sasse 1992b: 69–70). Changes in language obsolescencemay be motivated by language-internal processes (see, for instance, Dixon1991b; also Gruzdeva 2002).

Language contact in language obsolescence 81

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 6: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

then turn to the obsolescent Bare, a North Arawak language from Vene-

zuela and Brazil, and compare two sources on the language which display

varying degree of obsolescence. In these instances we can argue that

language contact in the situation of obsolescence does not produce any

remarkable results – the e¤ects are the same as may have occurred in

language contact of a non-replacive nature. This is consistent with the

idea that an increase in the quantity and the speed of change is the major

e¤ect of language obsolescence.3

Language shift in the context of language obsolescence may also result

in inclusion of some less likely candidates for borrowing – personal pro-

nouns, both free and bound. In Section 3.2 we look at Mawayana, a

North Arawak language with its last two speakers in Suriname in Trio

and Waiwai-speaking communities, and then turn to Resıgaro, a moribund

North Arawak language in northeastern Peru, which has undergone a

massive impact of Bora and Witotoan.

Can the influx of non-native forms in language obsolescence obscure its

genetic a‰liation? This is the topic of Section 3.3.

3.1. Non-native free forms: following a beaten path

3.1.1. Manambu

Those speakers of Manambu (a Ndu language from the East Sepik area)

who live in urban centers and rarely use the language employ numerous

non-native forms. An obsolescent speaker who had spent much of his life

in an urban town speaking Tok Pisin (the major lingua franca of Papua

New Guinea) produced (1). Later on, a fluent speaker volunteered (2), as

something she would have said. The Tok Pisin forms are in italics. The

form okey comes from English. It is also widely used by speakers of Tok

Pisin. Non-native forms are in bold.

3. I have undertaken extensive fieldwork on Manambu (see, for instance, Aikhen-vald 2008b), Tariana (see, for instance, Aikhenvald 2002) and also Bare (Iworked with the last fluent speaker of the language). For each language,I have recorded a substantial number of texts and natural conversations.ALL the examples in this paper (as in my other work) come from spontaneousdiscourse. In my fieldwork, I avoid elicitation as being methodologicallyflawed (see Aikhenvald 2007, Dixon 2007 for further fundamentals of linguisticfieldwork).

82 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 7: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Obsolescent speaker:

(1) asa:y kiya-d�-k aw wuna amay namba tufather die-3sg.m-compl.ds conn Iþlkþf.sg mother number two

du-ak ra:l okeyman-dat marryþ3f.sg.subj OK

ata lukautim-d�-d�wun tasol a taimthen look.after-3sg.m.subj-1sg.m.o but,only that.f.sg time

sikul-�r yi-d�wunschool-all go-1sg.m.subj

‘After my father died, then my mother married a second man, OK,

then he looked after me, only that at that time I went to school.’

Fluent speaker:

(2) asa:y kiya-d�-k aw wuna amay

father die-3sg.m-compl.ds conn Iþlkþf.sg mother

n�k�-d� du-ak ra:l ya:kya

another-sg.m man-dat marryþ3f.sgsubj all.right

ata yakwiya-d�-d�wun aw a s�k�rthen look.after-3sg.m.subj-1sg.m.o conn that.f.sg time

sikul-�r yi-d�wunschool-all go-1sg.m.subj

‘After my father died, then my mother married a second man, all

right, then he looked after me, only that at that time I went to school’

The two versions share one established loan word, sikul ‘school.’ Non-

native forms are not necessarily restricted to lexical items. Example (1)

shows that discourse markers, numerals, and conjunctions are imported

from the dominant language.4 Influx of loan forms is a striking feature of

‘‘globally obsolescent’’ languages. Extensive lexical impact of English has

been observed in the speech of the last speakers of Nyulnyul, an Australian

language (McGregor 2002: 177). Traditional Gooniyandi and Warrwa did

not have coordinating conjunctions: the remaining obsolescent speakers use

English forms nd (from and ) and � (from or). Traditional Nyulnyul did

4. In a situation of obsolescent speakers whose usage is unstable, the boundarybetween loans and code-switches is even harder to draw than in other language-contact situations. This is the reason why I use the term ‘‘import’’ to avoid usingeither ‘‘borrowing’’ or ‘‘code-switch.’’

Language contact in language obsolescence 83

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 8: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

have a conjunction agal ‘and’; the two remaining fluent speakers use the

English import nd.

Borrowing conjunctions and discourse markers in itself is not a symptom

of impending language death. Hamp (1989) and Johanson (2002) have

shown that allowing a certain number of loan forms by no means endangers

the language; the opposite can be true. Many fluent speakers of Manambu

in the villages use the English discourse marker okey and some occasionally

slip in the Tok Pisin tasol ‘but’ as a replacement for the polysemous aw

‘then, but, or’ (see Aikhenvald 2008b, 2009a). This confirms the general

assumption that language obsolescence tends to enhance the tendencies

present in a ‘‘healthy’’ language.

3.1.2. Bare

A comparison between two di¤erent stages of language obsolescence of the

same language points in a similar direction, that of increased influx of non-

native forms. Lopez Sanz’s (1972) brief grammatical description of Bare, a

North Arawak language from the Upper Rio Negro area in Venezuela, is

based on the analysis of materials (including several texts) collected in the

late 1960s from two remaining fluent speakers of the language from Santa

Rosa de Amanadona (with a total population of ethnic Bare of 140).

There are hardly any loans from Spanish, either lexical or grammatical.

Nowadays, people in Venezuela who identify themselves as Bare in Vene-

zuela speak Spanish; the Bare in Brazil speak Portuguese (some also know

Nheengatu, or Lıngua Geral, a Tupı-Guaranı-based lingua franca of the

area).

In 1991, I worked with the late Candelario da Silva (1921–1992), from

the Tiburi community, near Cucui, Amazonas, Brazil. Candelario’s family

moved in 1912 from Venezuela, fleeing from an uprising. His family main-

tained links with relatives in Venezuela in the communities of Puerto

Ayacucho, San Fernando de Atabapo, and Santa Rosa de Amanadona.

According to Candelario, all the remaining speakers of Bare in the above

mentioned localities in Venezuela were older than himself. He frequently

referred to his elderly aunts in Puerto Ayacucho as authorities to consult

with on words in Bare he himself could not remember. Candelario under-

went traditional male initiation (his account of it is in Aikhenvald 1995:

52–55), and insisted that he had grown up with Bare as his first language.

He was fluent in Bare, his father’s language. His late mother, herself a

speaker of Mandawaka (another extinct North Arawak language of the

area), had always spoken Bare to him. After her death about 30 years

prior to our encounter, he had kept his ancestral language which he used

84 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 9: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

to talk to himself, especially when he used to get drunk (and this, accord-

ing to Candelario, was not infrequent).5 Candelario was quadrilingual: his

main home language was Nheengatu, and he was equally fluent in Spanish

and Portuguese. His children spoke Portuguese and Nheengatu.

The variety of Bare recorded by Lopez Sanz in Santa Rosa de Amana-

dona has a richer morphology than the language of Candelario. For

instance, it has a variety of aspectual and modal markers (e.g. -phei

‘durative’ and -ya ‘dubitative’), and a marker of reported speech -man

not attested in the corpus collected from Candelario. Verb forms attested

in Lopez Sanz (1972) contain up to five su‰xes, whereas Candelario never

used more than one su‰x on the verb.6 Another major di¤erence between

texts and examples in Lopez Sanz (1972) and the corpus recorded from

Candelario is the abundance of Spanish and Portuguese forms, just as

would be expected in the case of advanced language obsolescence.

Many of these are not lexical forms. Candelario made an e¤ort to

avoid Spanish or Portuguese forms: the few consistent exceptions include

playa ‘sand’ (from Spanish playa ‘beach’) instead of either khaadi ‘sand,

earth’ (Arihini variety) or kadieho (Ihini variety: Natterer 1831), precisa-

‘need, require’ (from Portuguese precisar ‘need’), and gata- ‘spend, waste’

from Portuguese or Spanish gastar ‘spend’).

Spanish subordinating conjunctions occur where speakers of the more

traditional variety recorded by Lopez Sanz (1972) would use a sequencing

clitic -ka. This morpheme in Bare, just like in many other Arawak lan-

guages of the area, has a variety of meanings: it marks adverbial clauses

5. My corpus contains over 150 pages of texts and dialogues, and word-lists. SeeAikhenvald (1995) for a grammatical analysis of the material assembled, anda survey of literature on Bare. At the time of my work with Candelario, andwriting the grammar, I did not have access to Lopez Sanz (1972). Materials inLopez Sanz reflect some language attrition. Traditionally, Bare had two majorvarieties – Arihini (‘‘the ones from here’’) and Ihini (‘‘the ones from there’’: seeAikhenvald 1995). Newly available materials collected by Johann Natterer in1831 demonstrate the existence of lexical di¤erences between the two varieties.In the texts and examples in Lopez Sanz (1972), lexical items from the twovarieties appear in free variation. Such dialect mixture, or, in Dixon’s (1991a)words, dialect merging, is typical of language obsolescence. Candelario knewof the two varieties, but could not tell them apart.

6. Some di¤erences between the variety of Santa Rosa de Amanadona recordedby Lopez Sanz and the language of Candelario may be due to additional dia-lectal or idiolectal variation: these include Santa Rosa de Amanadona heinand Candelario’s hena for declarative negation. Note that I preserve the tran-scription given by Lopez Sanz (1972) for the examples from Santa Rosa deAmanadona.

Language contact in language obsolescence 85

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 10: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

of most types except purposive, and regularly occurs on conditional, tem-

poral, and complement clauses (see Aikhenvald 2006b for Tariana, and

discussion there). An example of -ka, from Lopez Sanz (1972: 80), is in

(3) (in boldface); note that in a negative construction -ka attaches to the

negation (glosses are supplied by me):

Bare: Santa Rosa de Amanadona

(3) hena-ka i-kasa hein i-nika-waka

neg-seq 3gs-arrive neg 3sg.m-eat-neg

‘If he does not come, he does not eat.’

The polysemous -ka also appears in Candelario’s texts, as shown in the

example below from an autobiographical story (also see Aikhenvald 1995:

48–50). Clauses are in brackets, for ease of reference.

Bare: Candelario da Silva

(4) [nu-mina¸ i ø-maha niku] [a¸ i bi-pa¸ata-ni1sg-master 3sg.m-say 1sgþfor here 2sg-money-poss

kuma¸ehe] [bi-katehesa-ka] [beke badahanaka biku

big 2sg-know-seq fut one.day 2sgþfor

ahaw bi-wakhid’a-ka] [hena-ka bi-katehesa]

with what 2sg-live-seq neg-seq 2sg-know

[phinuka bi-pa¸ata-ni]2sg-throw-decl 2sg-money-poss

‘My master said to me: here is your big money, if you know some-

thing, one day you will have what to live with, if you do not know,

you will throw away your money.’

The sequencing marker is not used to introduce speech reports: as shown

in (4), speech reports are juxtaposed to the verb of speech.

Besides the sequencing -ka, Bare had an adverbial form abeuku ‘when,

as soon as; then’ used both by the two speakers in Santa Rosa de Aman-

dona and by Candelario. If accompanied by the sequencing -ka on the

verb, abeuku is a temporal linker ‘when.’ Example (5) comes from a myth-

ical text recorded by Lopez Sanz (1972: 83):

Bare: Santa Rosa de Amanadona

(5) isınka abeuku ihıwa-ka Puluna-minali. . .

3sg.mþlike when 3sg.mþgo ?-master. . .

‘It was when Pulunaminali (the master of all animals) went (round). . .’

86 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 11: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

In (6), from an autobiographical story by Candelario, abeuku is also

accompanied by -ka on the verb (in the last clause).

Bare: Candelario da Silva

(6) da-ya¸aki nu-maha i-ku me-maha ni-ku

dem-whisky 1sg-say 3sg.m-for 3pl-say 1sg-for

ke nihiwa abeuku i-makhi-ka sa ya¸akithat 1sgþgo when 3sg.m-finish-seq dem whisky

‘Then we drank, we managed to drink all the whisky, I said to him,

they said to me that I shall go when the whisky finishes.’

In the few examples of abeuku without an accompanying sequencing

-ka in the variety of Santa Rosa de Amanadona, the form means ‘then’

(Lopez Sanz 1972: 84):

Bare: Santa Rosa de Amanadona

(7) abeuku humadan

then 3f.sgþleave

‘Then she lets (him) go.’

Unlike the two speakers from Santa Rosa de Amanadona, Candelario

used abeuku as a temporal linker without the accompanying -ka on the

verb. In (8), -ka appears in the preceding clause, so its absence in the third

clause (introduced with abeuku) could be explained as an instance of

ellipsis:

Bare: Candelario da Silva

(8) [me-nika kubati ] abeuku idi-ka3pl-eat fish when then/there-seq

abeuku bed’a-waka me-nika matsuka

when nothing 3pl-eat manioc.flour

‘They (dogs) eat fish, when/if it is there, when (there is) nothing, they

eat manioc flour.’

However, in other examples like the one in (9) -ka is simply not used, and

abeuku is the only linker:

(9) bihiwa awehentei abeuku i-makhi

2sgþgo hereþelative when 3sg.m-finish

‘You will go away when it (the drink) finishes.’

Language contact in language obsolescence 87

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 12: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Candelario insisted on translating abeuku as ‘when’ (Portuguese quando).

The conjunction occupies the same clause-initial position as quando in

Portuguese (or cuando in Spanish). The apparent obsolescence of the

sequencing -ka in the presence of abeuku may indicate that Candelario

was adopting a Spanish-Portuguese strategy for temporal linking. He

never used this Spanish-Portuguese form himself. He freely used other

Spanish or Spanish-Portuguese conjunctions. Just occasionally, the verb

in a subordinate clause introduced by a conjunction would be accom-

panied by -ka. The temporal mientre ke (from Spanish mientras que)

‘while, whereas’ is accompanied by -ka in (10):

(10) mientre-ke nu-nakuda-ka i-ma¸e-d’a kubati

while-that 1sg-go-seq 3sg.m-steal-inch fish

‘While I was gone, he (the dog) started stealing the fish.’

The causal purke ‘because’ (from Spanish porque) is used on its own in the

penultimate clause of (11). It is accompanied by -ka in the second clause

of (12):

(11) idi me-maha-ka [ke hena me-yehe-waka

then 3pl-say-decl that neg 3pl-can-neg

me-dia-sa-ka nu] [purke hena hnuwina-waka

3pl-drink-caus-seq i because neg 1sgþfall-neg

ya¸aki ahaw] [hena hnuwina-waka]

whisky from neg 1sgþfall-neg

‘Then they said to me that they could not make me drunk, because I

do not fall down from whisky, I do not fall down.’

(12) [damakaru-kua nu-¸ehedi ] [purke nu-¸ehedi nu-yuwahada-ka]

jungle-locþlong 1sg-like because 1sg-like 1sg-walk-seq

[ pero nu-witi hena-hana yada-ka-na]

but 1sg-eye neg-more 3sg.mþsee-decl-perf

‘I enjoy the jungle, because I like to walk, but my eyes do not see

any more.’

The linker ke (from Spanish, Portuguese que) is used to introduce speech

reports, as in (11) and (6). About 60 percent of speech reports in the

corpus contain ke. This same form occurs in the meaning of ‘so that,’ as

in (13).

88 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 13: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

(13) [bihıwa behewa] [kuhu ke id’ua¸ i beke bı ]

2sgþtake 2sgþfrom he that well fut you

‘Take it (poisoned pillow) from you, so that you will be well.’

Candelario used other Spanish conjunctions, for instance, the coordinator

pero ‘but’ (from Spanish pero ‘but’) shown in (12). None of the Spanish-

Portuguese conjunctions appear in the Santa Rosa de Amanadona variety.

Note that conjunctions occupy the same place as in Spanish. In contrast to

the other documented variety of Bare, and to most other Arawak lan-

guages, Bare spoken by Candelario is losing the sequencing enclitic -ka, a

marker which has no equivalent in either Spanish or Portuguese. This is

an instance of ‘‘negative borrowing.’’ We can thus conclude that the influx

of non-native forms into the speech of the last fluent speaker of Bare is

accompanied by leveling of structures. The obsolescent Bare imports

Spanish and Portuguese forms, and also becomes more similar to the

dominant Spanish and Portuguese in terms of its grammatical structure.

Conjunctions – especially free forms – are among the most borrowable

elements of the language (the interested reader is advise to consult Stolz

and Stolz 1996 with special focus on American Indian languages; Matras

1998, and Aikhenvald 2006a). As stated at the end of §3.1.1, the fact that

Spanish and Portuguese conjunctions have been borrowed into Bare

should not be considered as a special phenomenon in language obsoles-

cence. What is indicative of Bare as an obsolescent language is the high

number of loans from the dominant languages. That is, this contact-

induced change in language obsolescence appears to follow a beaten

path, albeit at an increased rate. we can recall, from §1 of this paper, that

the di¤erence between language change in vital, and in obsolescent lan-

guage, may lie in the ‘quantity of change’. It is indeed the case here.

3.2. Influx of non-native free forms: unusual patterns

We now turn to some rather unusual borrowing patterns in obsolescent

languages. In a number of instances, obsolescent languages borrow per-

sonal pronouns from the dominant language. In the examples available,

pronominal forms which express categories attested in the dominant lan-

guage but absent from the obsolescent one may get borrowed.

3.2.1. Mawayana

Mawayana (Carlin 2006) is a highly endangered North Arawak language

spoken by just two elderly people in a village where Trio and Waiwai,

Language contact in language obsolescence 89

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 14: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

from the Carib family, are the dominant languages. The two remaining

speakers of Mawayana have little opportunity of using the language, and

are aware that when they go, so will Mawayana.

Just like most other Arawak languages, Mawayana originally had first,

second, and third person, without distinguishing between first person

plural inclusive (I and you) and exclusive (I and a third person, excluding

you). In contrast, Waiwai and Trio have di¤erent forms for first person

inclusive and for first person exclusive. As a result of influence from Waiwai

and Trio as dominant languages with an obligatory distinction between

inclusive and exclusive, the two remaining speakers of Mawayana consis-

tently use the Waiwai pronoun amna to express the concept of first person

plural exclusive (e.g. Waiwai amna krapan ‘our (excl) bow’). The original

first person plural prefix wa- in Mawayana has been reinterpreted as

inclusive.

(14) amna saruuka (14b) wa-saruuka

1þ3pn fishtrap 1pl.poss-fishtrap

‘Our (excl) fishtrap.’ ‘Our (incl) fishtrap.’

The borrowed form comes from Waiwai. However, the behavior of the

verb bears an impact from Trio: in Trio the first person exclusive pronoun

requires a third person prefix on the verb, while in Waiwai the third person

singular prefix is often dropped. Example (15) shows that Mawayana fol-

lows the Trio pattern of person marking:

(15) amna rı-me

1þ3pn 3a-say.pres

‘We (excl) say.’

The first person inclusive is marked with the Mawayana prefix wa- (origi-

nally first person plural):

(16) wa-me

1incl.pl-say.pres

‘We (incl) say.’

Carlin (2006) is the first summary of the grammatical features of the lan-

guage in the light of language contact (and a full grammar is in progress).

The borrowed form amna does not occur in the previous records of the

language, which include longish lists of words and phrases in Howard

(1986), and materials in Farabee (1918: 283–286) and Schomburgk (1848),

all collected when the language was more actively spoken than it is at

90 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 15: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

present. This suggests that borrowing a pronoun – something not unheard

of, but rather unusual – could be the result of excessive influx of non-

native forms characteristic of Mawayana as an obsolescent language.

3.2.2. Resıgaro

The genetic a‰liation of Resıgaro – a small language spoken in north-

eastern Peru surrounded by speakers of Bora and Witotoan groups –

with the Arawak family was established by Igualada (1940) and Igualada

and Castellvı (1940); also see Loutkotka (1968: 136). The first extensive

materials on the language published by Rivet and de Wavrin (1951), and

based on the data collected by de Wavrin in the early 1930s, provided

ample evidence in the same direction (see Payne 1985 for a summary).

The group itself comprised not more than a thousand people at the

time of Whi¤en’s (1915) travels in the area. The first mention of Resıgaro

(Recıgaro), by Hardenburg (1910), places it among other Witotoan

groups. Tessmann (1930: 583) does not provide linguistic a‰liation, but

states that culturally they are close to the Bora-Witoto, and linguistically

are ‘‘perhaps close to Bora.’’7 At that time, the language was still actively

spoken. Note that there is no evidence of any genetic relationship between

Bora-Witotoan and Arawak languages (see Loukotka 1968; Aschmann

1993).

In his pioneering salvage grammar of Resıgaro, based on fieldwork

with ten remaining speakers whose major language was Bora, Trevor Allin

(1975) came to a di¤erent conclusion. The sheer number of Bora, and also

Witotoan, forms in Resıgaro indicated to him that the languages were

genetically related. He did not deny that Resıgaro belongs to the Arawak

family, but suggested that, given the high percentage of shared forms

between Bora, Witotoan languages, and Resıgaro, the limits of Arawak

should be expanded, and Bora and Witotoan be included.

There is, however, no doubt that the impressive number of Bora and

Witotoan forms in Resıgaro are due to borrowing (see Payne 1985, and

detailed discussion in Aikhenvald 2001). These lexical loans constitute

about 24 percent of the vocabulary, and include just a few verbs and

numerous nouns, covering body parts plus a few other items such as ‘fish’

and ‘hill.’ The most striking is the fact that ‘‘core’’ lexical items, such as

terms for body parts, are shared with Bora or with Witotoan languages.

7. ‘‘Uber die Ressıgaro ist nichts Naheres bekannt. Sie gehoren kulturell sicherzu der Uitoto-Boragruppe und sprachlich vielleicht in die Nahe der Bora.’’

Language contact in language obsolescence 91

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 16: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

However, the lexical data published by Rivet and de Wavrin (limited as

they are) often do not register a loan.

A prime example is the word for ‘tooth,’ Resıgaro -e�hepe ‘tooth,’

which is similar to Muinane Witoto iıpe, Proto-Witoto an *pe (Aschmann

1993). The reflex of the Proto-Arawak form *nene (Aikhenvald 2001) sur-

vives in Resıgaro -onene ‘front teeth’ (Allin 1975). Rivet and de Wavrin

(1951: 213) give the form wo-ne (1pl-tooth) ‘tooth,’ and no form similar

to Bora or to Witotoan.

The Resıgaro described by Allin uses borrowed numbers ‘one’ and

‘two’ (see Table 1). This is quite remarkable for an Arawak language,

since lower numbers (if they exist at all) generally appear to be rather

resistant to borrowing. And the overwhelming majority of Arawak lan-

guages preserve the reflexes of Proto-Arawak forms (fourth column in

Table 1). Once again, Rivet and de Wavrin (1951) register di¤erent forms,

which are clearly Arawak in origin. The form for ‘two’ shows the e¤ects

of the phonological process *y > tz found in other cognates with Proto-

Arawak.

Does this imply that pre-obsolescent Resıgaro was more Arawak-like

in its lexicon and grammar? In all likelihood, yes.

Bora influence on Resıgaro grammar goes further than free forms (see

Aikhenvald 2001 for a detailed discussion of structural influence of Bora on

Resıgaro, and also the discussion of borrowed classifiers). Borrowed bound

morphemes include one pronoun, number markers, oblique case markers,

and also classifiers. The independent pronouns and cross-referencing prefixes

in Resıgaro (where they are mostly used to mark A/Sa and as possessors

of inalienably possessed nouns) are compared to Bora in Table 2 (Allin

1975: 116–117; Thiesen 1996: 33). Borrowed morphemes are in boldface.

Table 1. Numbers ‘one’ and ‘two’ in Resıgaro, Bora and Arawak

No. Resıgaro(Allin 1975)

Bora Resıgaro(Rivet and deWavrin 1951)

Proto-Arawak

one sa-cl tsa-cl ‘apa #(ha)pene *pa

two migaa- mı�ee/mihaa-cl ‘e(i)tza #m� *yama

92 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 17: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Unlike most other Arawak languages but similarly to the Bora-Witotoan

group, Resıgaro has inclusive versus exclusive opposition in first person

non-singular, and also a dual number. The first person plural exclusive

pronoun muu�a was borrowed from Bora, similarly to the way the last

speakers of Mawayana introduced a Waiwai form to cover the same

meaning. In Resıgaro, it was subsequently reanalyzed as consisting of a

Table 2. Pronouns in Resıgaro and in Bora

Resıgaro Bora

Pronouns prefixes PronounsPrefixes(poss)

Prefixes(subj)

1sg no no- oo ta- —

2sg phu, pha p- uu di- —

3sg m tsu, tsa gi- diıbyei-, aadi- —

3sg f tso do- diılle

1incl du m fa-musif-/_hva/_elsewhere

mee

me-

me-

1incl du f fa-mupi

1excl du m muu-musi muu- muhtsi

1excl du f muu-mupi muhp�

2du m ha-musi hu-, i- (impv) a-muhtsi amu� a-

2du f ha-mupi a-muhpi

3du m na-musin-/_hna-

diitye-tsi aathje-

3du f na-mupi diitye-p�

1pl incl fa-�a, fu, fa f/ua- mee me-

1pl excl muu-�a, muu — muuha

2pl ha-�a, hu i- (impv) amuuha amu� a-

3pl na-�a, hna na- diıtye, aatye aathje- —

Language contact in language obsolescence 93

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 18: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

prefix muu- and a particle -�a, following the analogy of other non-singular

pronouns in the language itself, such as na-�a ‘third person plural’ and

fa-�a ‘first person plural inclusive.’ This shows the linguistic creativity

of the last speakers, captured by Sasse’s (2001) colorful metaphor, the

‘‘Phoenix from the ashes’’ (in the spirit of Dorian 1999, and Dal Negro

2004).

The Resıgaro dual markers feminine -mupi, masculine -musi (also from

Bora: see Table 2) combine with muu- reanalyzed as a bound form. Unlike

other pronouns, the first person plural exclusive has no corresponding

prefix used with nouns and with verbs, which may point towards its later

origin. The Bora forms in Resıgaro are in bold in Table 2.

In their comparatively detailed discussion of personal pronouns, free

and bound, in Resıgaro, Rivet and de Wavrin (1951: 204–206) do not

mention the first person plural exclusive form (the analysis of pronominal

markers occupies about a half of their short grammatical summary: 204–

209). They do not mention the number markers on nouns at all. We can

hypothesize that the introduction of non-native free and bound pronomi-

nal forms by the last speakers of the language is likely to be a result of

contact-induced change in the situation of extreme linguistic stress.

This is not to say that the Resıgaro described by Rivet and de Wavrin

(1951) had no loans from Bora or Witotoan; to the contrary. One example

is Resıgaro tee� ı (Allin 1975), tehe(y)hı (Rivet and de Wavrin 1951)

‘river,’ Bora thee-� i, Proto-Bora-Muinane *tee-� i. The Proto-Arawak form

is *huni ‘water, river.’ A reflex of this form is attested in Resıgaro’s closest

genetic relatives Tariana, Baniwa, and Piapoco as uni ‘water, river.’

Further bound morphemes borrowed from Bora into Resıgaro include

markers of masculine and feminine dual, oblique cases, and numerous

classifiers (see Aikhenvald 2001; Allin 1975; Thiesen 1996). None of these

are mentioned by Rivet and de Wavrin (1951): we may hypothesize that

the influx of borrowed morphemes into the obsolescent language is a

recent phenomenon, but we have no means of definitely proving this.

Borrowing a pronoun, free or bound, is not unheard of, but is quite

unusual (Gardani 2005). Third person plural pronouns they, their, them

in English are considered to be borrowings from a Scandinavian source

(Campbell 1997; Baugh 1957: 120). Miskito, a vibrant Misumalpan lan-

guage, is said to have borrowed first and second person singular pronouns

from Northern Sumu (Campbell 1997, based on Ken Hale, p.c.), also

Misumalpan. Further examples of borrowing individual free pronominal

forms come from Matiso¤ (1990: 113) and Newman (1977, 1979a, b).

94 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 19: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Campbell (1994) reports that Alsea, an isolate from Oregon, borrowed a

whole set of Salishan pronominal su‰xes.8 However, the instances such

as Mawayana and Resıgaro should be treated with caution as bona fide

examples of borrowing pronouns. The fact that these borrowings were

documented at a stage when both Mawayana and Resıgaro are used by

just a handful of speakers whose major language is di¤erent alerts us to a

potential e¤ect of a massive influx of non-native forms characteristic of

the last stages of a language’s life. Can a massive influx of borrowed forms

obscure a language’s a‰liation? This takes us to our next section.

3.3. Language obsolescence and language a‰liation

It is well known that teasing apart similarities due to genetic inheritance

from those due to borrowing of varied kinds is one of the hardest prob-

lems in comparative linguistics (cf. the classic controversy between Boas

and Sapir: see Swadesh 1951). Ideally, if two languages descend from the

same ancestor, the forms and their meanings must be easily relatable, via

the application of established rules for phonological change and semantic

change. In reality, the distinction between inherited and di¤used similarities

may be di‰cult to draw, especially in a situation of prolonged and un-

interrupted di¤usion of cultural and linguistic traits across an area; see,

for instance, Dixon (1997; 2002), Dench (2001), and Heath (1978), for

the Australian area, and further examples in Aikhenvald (2006a). Simi-

larities between languages can be suggestive of a genetic relationship,

but not su‰cient to postulate it with full assurance. Murrinh-patha and

Ngan.gitjemerri, two languages spoken in the Daly River region of North-

ern Australia, share just cognate paradigms for portmanteau forms of

inflective simple verbs, but scarcely anything else in grammar and almost

no lexicon (Dixon 2002: 675). The paradigm of free pronouns is the only

fully ‘‘Chadic’’ feature of the Tangale group (Jungraithmayr 1995). Such

examples are bound to remain ‘‘fringe’’ puzzles to comparative linguists.

The case of Resıgaro is rather instructive in this respect. The influx of

Bora and Witotoan forms into this language led Trevor Allin to believe

that the language was related to Bora and to Witotoan (Allin 1975). Payne

8. Another frequently given example of a putative borrowing of part of the pro-nominal paradigm comes from Kambot (or Botin), from the Grass family inNew Guinea (Foley 1986: 210–211). A closer look at the paradigm of Kambotpronouns in the original sources (Laycock and Z’graggen 1975; Pryor 1990)shows that this hypothesis is based on misinterpretation of the data (seeAikhenvald 2009b, for a full analysis).

Language contact in language obsolescence 95

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 20: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

(1985) undertook a careful reconstruction and comparison with the previ-

ous stage of the language captured by Rivet and de Wavrin (1951), to

prove that the language is not Bora-Witotoan. But what if all we have is

a highly obsolescent stage?

An almost extreme example of influx of non-native forms into a pro-

nominal paradigm and its restructuring comes from Marrku, the tradi-

tional language of Croker Island (Australian area) (Evans et al. 2006;

Evans 2007). Like many Australian languages, Marrku has been on the

decline for many decades. It was reported that by 1939 there were only

five speakers left (Evans et al 2006: 2); by 1991 there were only two semi-

speakers who were then highly proficient in other indigenous languages of

the area (especially Iwaidja). The verb paradigms accessible to Evans (2007)

show a curious picture: while there is strong evidence from body-part pre-

fixes (Evans 2000) in favor of an erstwhile genetic relationship between

Marrku and other Iwaidjan languages, verbal paradigms in Marrku –

collected from obsolescent speakers – contain massive borrowings from

Iwaidja and its relative Ilgar. This massive influx, without any previous

stage of the language to be compared with, makes exact genetic classifica-

tion of Marrku an almost impossible task (Evans 2007).

4. Further outcomes of language contact in language obsolescence

An influx of foreign forms is not a universal outcome of language obsoles-

cence.9 We saw above that an obsolescent language may tend to rapidly

become structurally similar to the dominant one. Almost all the categories

present in Bora are expressed in Resıgaro; Mawayana replicates the Trio

and Waiwai patterns (without necessarily borrowing the forms). Nivkh,

a Paleo-Siberian isolate on the path towards extinction, has undergone

massive restructuring of imperative paradigms under the influence of

Russian (see Gruzdeva 2002). Similar examples abound.

9. Last speakers often avoid consciously using loan forms, even if they were usedin the language. R. M. W. Dixon reports that Dick Moses, one of the very lastfluent speakers of Yidiny, made sure his language was free of English intru-sions. As Dixon (1977: 29) reported, ‘‘Moses has eliminated what were cer-tainly established English loan words’’; ‘‘in place of mudaga ‘motor car’ andbiligan ‘billy can,’ he uses dundalay and gunbu:l which he said were originallythe avoidance style forms for these items.’’ Similar examples of purism havebeen documented for Arizona Tewa (Kroskrity 1993).

96 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 21: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Intensive language contact in the situation of language obsolescence

goes together with enhancement of already existing similarities. Forms in

the obsolescent language which are similar to those in the dominant one

tend to become more frequent, and to assume the meanings influenced by

the dominant language.

Ingrian Finnish spoken by a handful of Finns scattered around Estonia

is a case in point. Most speakers are undergoing a rapid shift to Estonian.

The two languages are closely related and structurally similar; as a result,

it is not always possible to distinguish Estonian and Finnish forms. The

most striking foreign form recorded in the language of the few remaining

speakers is the past tense marker -si- employed instead of the Ingrian

Finnish -i- (Riionheimo 2002: 201–202). This past tense marker is highly

productive in Estonian; its appearance in Ingrian Finnish can thus be

explained by the influence of the dominant language. But there is also

a language-internal explanation: there is a subclass of verbs in Ingrian

Finnish which requires -si- past rather than -i- past. Similarity in form of

the Ingrian Finnish and the Estonian past marker is a strong contributing

factor to its increased frequency in the moribund Ingrian Finnish. Other

than that, speakers tend to avoid using Estonian forms.

In a situation of traditional inhibition against borrowed forms, growing

language obsolescence may go hand in hand with expansion of those mor-

phemes that have the same form in the obsolescent and in the dominant

language. Tariana is the only Arawak language spoken in the Vaupes basin

in northwest Amazonia (spanning adjacent areas of Brazil and Colombia).

This used to be a well-established linguistic area, characterized by obliga-

tory multilingualism based on the principle of linguistic exogamy: ‘‘those

who speak the same language as us are our brothers, and we do not marry

our sisters’’ (see Aikhenvald 2002 and references there). Languages spoken

in this area traditionally included the East Tucanoan languages Tucano,

Wanano, Desano, Piratapuya, Tuyuca (and a few others), and the Arawak

language Tariana (now spoken by over 100 speakers in two villages).

Speakers of these participate in the exogamous marriage network which

ensures obligatory multilingualism. Nowadays, Tariana is no longer spoken

by children, and fewer and fewer people use the language even in domestic

settings. The growing obsolescence of Tariana and its rapid replacement

by now dominant Tucano is accompanied by a rapidly increasing number

of calqued forms and constructions from Tucano.

The long-term interaction based on institutionalized societal multi-

lingualism between East Tucanoan languages and Tariana has resulted in

the rampant di¤usion of grammatical and semantic patterns (though not

Language contact in language obsolescence 97

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 22: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

so much of forms) and calquing of categories. Comparison of Tariana

with closely related Arawak languages (such as Baniwa/Kurripako and

Piapoco) helps identify the di¤used and the inherited features in Tariana.

A striking feature of the Vaupes linguistic area is a strong cultural inhibi-

tion against language mixing, viewed in terms of borrowing forms, or

inserting bits of other languages, in one’s Tariana. This inhibition operates

predominantly in terms of recognizable loan forms. Speakers who use

non-native forms are subject to ridicule which may a¤ect their status in

the community. What often happens in the language of obsolescent speakers

is reinterpreting Tariana morphemes in accordance with the meaning their

look-alikes may have in Tucano.

Consider the Tariana clitic -ya ‘emphatic.’ This clitic is now increas-

ingly used by obsolescent insecure speakers as a marker of immediate

command (17), mirroring the Tucano imperative -ya (18):

(17) Tariana

pi-nha-ya

2sg-eat-impv

‘Eat!’

(18) Tucano

ba’a-ya

play-impv

‘Play!’

The -ya imperative in Tariana is frequently used by younger speakers, and

hardly ever by the few traditional older speakers, who concur that this is

not ‘‘proper Tariana.’’ The morpheme -ya in an imperative construction is

condemned as a token of identifiable language-mixing (see Aikhenvald

2008a, for cognates of the emphatic -ya in other Arawak languages, and

the imperative marker -ya in Tucanoan languages).

Another similar example comes from the increased use of nominaliza-

tions marked with -¸i in Tariana commands. This is an alternative to simple

imperatives, but with a somewhat di¤erent meaning, ‘make sure you do.’

(19) Tariana

pi-nha-¸ i!2sg-eat-nominalization

‘Eat!’ (make sure you eat, lest you go hungry)

This usage is restricted to casual speech by younger people for whom Tucano

is the main language of day-to-day communication. Tucano, just like most

98 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 23: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

other East Tucanoan languages, has a su‰x -ri used in commands with

an overtone of warning, with the meaning of ‘or else’ (see Ramirez 1997,

Vol. 1: 146–147). The usage of nominalizations as commands in Tariana

has in all likelihood been influenced by the -ri marked imperative in Tucano.

That the form in (19) is a nominalization is corroborated by the transla-

tions given by traditional speakers of Tariana, who themselves avoid using

commands like (19), using an apprehensive construction instead.

Traditional Tariana did not use to have any special morpheme for first

person plural imperative (or hortative). Nowadays, obsolescent speakers

employ a hortative -da/-¸a. Compare Traditional Tariana, in (20a), with

(20b), recorded from an obsolescent speaker:

(20a) Traditional Tariana

wa-i¸a1pl-drink

(20b) Obsolescent Tariana

wa-i¸a-da1pl-drink-hortative

‘Let’s drink!’

Functionally and formally this morpheme is reminiscent of the Tucano

hortative -ra/-da (Ramirez 1997, Vol. I: 145) which is shared with other

Tucanoan languages:

(21) Tucano

sı ’ri-da!

drink-hortative

‘Let’s drink!’

The Tariana hortative is likely to be a recent borrowing from Tucano. Or

it could be the result of a reinterpretation of already existing Tariana mor-

pheme -da/-¸a ‘dubitative’ which is sometimes used to express politeness.

Traditional speakers of Tariana are aware of the similarity between the

Tariana and the Tucano morphemes, and treat the hortative (as in (20b))

as ‘‘incorrect’’ Tariana ‘‘mixed’’ with Tucano. This is typical of Tariana

language attitudes: given the general prohibition on mixing languages viewed

in terms of lexical loans, the hortative is, not surprisingly, a marginal feature

of the language (see Aikhenvald 2002: 213–222 on language awareness in

the Vaupes area).

Or a look-alike can oust another, non-shared morpheme. Tariana has

numerous verbal markers to do with extent and type of action, among

Language contact in language obsolescence 99

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 24: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

them the enclitic -pita ‘repetitive action: do again.’ This enclitic is being

replaced by the form -ta ‘repetitive’, shared with related languages, but

infrequent in the traditional language. The form -ta is similar to Tucano

taha, often reduced to -ta (Ramirez 1997: 343–4).

These instances of a semantic extension of a native morpheme under

the influence of a look-alike in a contact language (known as grammatical

accommodation: see Aikhenvald 2006a) are symptomatic of language shift

in language obsolescence. This is an alternative to influx of non-native

forms.

Obsolescent Tariana o¤ers curious examples of drastic restructuring.

Tucanoan languages and Tariana are genetically unrelated, and typologically

di¤erent. Like many Arawak languages, Tariana employs prefixes for subject

cross-referencing, while Tucanoan languages are predominantly su‰xing. As

a result of long-term contact, Tariana has developed numerous un-Arawak

features, including cases for core arguments and a complex system of eviden-

tials. (These are instances of completed changes.) Obsolescent Tariana is

developing a system of cross-referencing enclitics, as exemplified by (22b),

mirroring the Tucanoan pattern.

The following example is a typical beginning of a story. It was recorded

from a fluent middle-aged speaker who always tried to speak the tradi-

tional language. The structural parallelism with Tucano is striking, but not

complete. The major di¤erence lies in the person that is marked: Tariana

employs a prefix ( just like any Arawak language would), while Tucano em-

ploys a su‰x (portmanteau with a tense-evidential marker). The relevant

forms are in bold.

(22a) Traditional Tariana

Tariana Payape-se-nuku paita nawiki

Tucano Diporo-pi-re ni’ki masi

long.ago-loc-top.non.a/s oneþcl:human person

Tariana dy-uka-na aı-nuku

3sgnf-arrive-rem.p.vis here-top.non.a/s

Tucano eta-wı a’to-re

arrive-3sgnf.rem.p.vis here-top.non.a/s

‘A long time ago a man arrived here.’

A similar story told by an obsolescent speaker (now in his early thirties)

started in a subtly di¤erent way:

100 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 25: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

(22b)

Tariana Payape-se-nuku paita nawiki

Tucano Diporo-pi-re ni’ki masi

long.ago-loc-top.non.a/s oneþcl:human person

Tariana dy-uka-na¼diha aı-nuku

3sgnf-arrive-rem.p.vis¼he here-top.non.a/s

Tucano eta-wı a’to-re

arrive-3sgnf.rem.p.vis here-top.non.a/s

‘A long time ago a man arrived here.’

The Tariana in (22b) is structurally closer to Tucano since the speaker

employs an encliticized personal pronoun following the evidential. When

(22b) was uttered, no one commented on the language di¤erence. Speakers

are more aware of non-native forms than they are of non-native patterns.

Nevertheless, when I played (22b) back to a traditional elder, he com-

mented that ¼diha should not have been there.

Instances like (22b) demonstrate that Tariana is becoming almost like

relexified Tucano. But since language change in language obsolescence is

unstable and discontinuous, chances are that this relexified variety will not

live beyond the life-span of the last speakers.

5. What can we conclude?

A study of contact-induced change in the situation of language obsoles-

cence poses specific problems. Basically, the same or similar issues arise

when we investigate the speech behavior of obsolescent speakers of other-

wise well-spoken languages, and processes of change in those languages

which are on their way out.

Independently of whether we are dealing with obsolescent languages or

just with obsolescent speakers, the influx of non-native forms tends to be

pervasive. This is understandable: language obsolescence is typically asso-

ciated with word-retrieval problems, and it is easier to just use an item

from the dominant language.

In other instances of language obsolescence, we encounter instances

of influx of non-native forms beyond lexicon. Mawayana and Resıgaro

have borrowed pronouns, while Resıgaro has also restructured its cross-

referencing system, e¤ectively incorporating a non-native bound form of

a pronoun. This is in addition to borrowing numbers ‘one’ and ‘two,’ and

Language contact in language obsolescence 101

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 26: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

numerous further bound morphemes. These instances of borrowing mem-

bers of closed classes and even bound forms are typologically unexpected

and unusual. However, a question arises: are these really borrowings, or

are they just instances of nonce forms? That the last speakers’ usage is

unstable and ephemeral is a well-known fact. Typologists and historical

linguists need to be wary of that when they encounter unusual patterns of

borrowed forms in obsolescent languages.

A further, commonly attested, e¤ect of language contact in obsolescence

is the enhancement of forms already shared with the dominant language.

This often concerns frequently used forms and constructions, such as the

expression of commands. In addition, enhanced structural di¤usion may

result in one language becoming like a reflection of the other: the obsoles-

cent Tariana may sound like relexified Tucano. This is an extreme – but

again, often ephemeral – outcome of language shift.

In Johanson’s (2002) words, ‘‘languages do not die of ‘structuritis’ ’’ –

that is, contact-induced change does not result in language extinction.

But the processes of language obsolescence may promote structural changes

amazing in their extent. Before passing into extinction, an obsolescent lan-

guage may become a ‘‘carbon copy’’ of the dominant idiom. This exces-

sive copying is hardly surprising. The dominant language is the one used

on a day-to-day basis by speakers of an obscolescent language, and so the

structures from the dominant language get calqued and transferred into

the language falling into disuse. (More discussion and examples can be

found in Aikhenvald 2002, Grenoble 2000, and classic work by Hill and

Hill 1986, Tsitsipis 1998 and Campbell and Muntzel 1989).

Contact-induced changes in the situation of language obsolescence are

inherently unstable (as was pointed out by Tsitsipis 1998). Ephemeral

as they are, their outcomes may go against generalizations obtained in

‘‘healthy’’ language situations.

Acknowledgments

I am deeply grateful to the late Candelario da Silva, the last speaker of Bare,

to the members of the Brito family who taught me their native Tariana, and

my adopted family at Avatip who taught me their native Manambu. Special

thanks go to R. M. W. Dixon, who provided invaluable comments on this

article. I am also grateful to Claudine Chamoreau for her careful editing.

102 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 27: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Abbreviations

a Transitive subject

all Allative

caus Causative

cl Classifier

compl.ds Completed di¤erent subject

conn Connective

dat Dative

decl Declarative

dem Demonstrative

du Dual

exc Exclusive

f Feminine

fut Future

impv Imperative

inc Inclusive

inch Inchoative

lk Linker

loc Locative

m Masculine

neg Negative

nf Nonfeminine

o Object

perf Perfective

pl Plural

pn Pronoun

poss Possessive

rem.p.vis Remote past visual

seq Sequencing

sg Singular

subj Subject

top.non.a/s Topical non-subject.

References

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 1995. Bare. Munich: Lincom Europa.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 1999. The Arawak language family. In The Amazonian Lan-

guages, R. M. W. Dixon and A. Y. Aikhenvald (eds), 65–105. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Language contact in language obsolescence 103

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 28: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2001. Areal di¤usion, genetic inheritance and problems of sub-

grouping: a North Arawak case study. In Areal Di¤usion and Genetic Inheritance:

Problems in Comparative Linguistics, A. Y. Aikhenvald and R. M. W. Dixon

(eds), 167–194. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2002. Language Contact in Amazonia. Oxford: Oxford Univer-

sity Press.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2003. A grammar of tariana, from north-west Amazonia. Cam-

bridge: Cambridge University Press.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2006a. Grammars in contact: a cross-linguistic perspective.

In Grammars in Contact: a Cross-linguistic Typology, A. Y. Aikhenvald and

R. M. W. Dixon (eds), 1–66. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2006b. Semantics and pragmatics of grammatical relations in

the Vaupes linguistic area. In Grammars in Contact: a Cross-linguistic Typology,

A. Y. Aikhenvald and R. M. W. Dixon (eds), 237–266. Oxford: Oxford Univer-

sity Press.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2007. Linguistic fieldwork: setting the scene. Sprachtypologie

und Universalienforschung 60, 1: 1–11.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2008a. Multilingual imperatives: the elaboration of a category

in north-west Amazonia. International Journal of American Linguistics 74: 189–

225.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2008b. The Manambu Language of the East Sepik, Papua New

Guinea. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2009a. Semantics of clause linking in Manambu. In Semantics

of Clause Linking: A Cross-linguistic Typology, R. M. W. Dixon and A. Y.

Aikhenvald (eds), 118–144. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2009b. Language contact along the Sepik River. Anthropologi-

cal Linguistics 50: 1–66.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. 2010. Gender and noun class in Paumarı in a typological per-

spective. In Linguistics and Archeology in the Americas, E. Carlin and S. van de

Kerke (eds), 236–252. Leiden: Brill.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. and Dixon, R. M. W. (eds). 2001. Areal Di¤usion and Genetic

Inheritance: Problems in Comparative Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University

Press.

Aikhenvald, A. Y. and Dixon, R. M. W. (eds). 2006. Grammars in Contact: a

Cross-Linguistic Typology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Allin, T. R. 1975. A Grammar of Resıgaro. PhD dissertation, University of St.

Andrews.

Aschmann, R. P. 1993. Proto-Witotoan. Arlington: Summer Institute of Linguistics

and the University of Texas.

Austin, P. 1986. Structural change in language obsolescence: some Eastern Aus-

tralian examples. Australian Journal of Linguistics 6: 201–230.

Baugh, A. C. 1957. A History of the English Language. New York: Appleton-

Century-Crofts.

104 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 29: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Bettoni, C. 1991. Language variety among Italians: Anglicisation, attrition and

attitudes. In Language in Australia, S. Romaine (ed.), 263–269. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Brenziger, M. (ed.). 1992. Language Death. Factual and Theoretical Explorations

with Special Reference to East Africa. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Brenziger, M. and Dimmendaal G. J. 1992. Social context of language death. In

Language Death. Factual and Theoretical Explorations with Special Reference

to East Africa, M. Brenziger (ed.), 1–5. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Campbell, L. 1994. Problems with the pronouns in proposals of remote relation-

ships among native American languages. In Proceedings of the Meeting of the

Society for the Study of the Indigenous Languages of the Americas and the

Hokan-Penutian Workshop, M. Langdon (ed.), 1–20. Berkeley: University of

California Press.

Campbell, L. 1997. Amerindian personal pronouns: A second opinion. Language

73: 339–351.

Campbell, L. and Muntzel, M. 1989. The structural consequences of language

death. In Investigating obsolescence. Studies in Language Contraction and Death,

N. C. Dorian (ed.), 181–196. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Carlin, E. 2006. Feeling the need: the borrowing of Cariban functional categories

into Mawayana (Arawak). In Grammars in Contact: a Cross-linguistic Typology,

A. Y. Aikhenvald and R. M. W. Dixon (eds), 313–332. Oxford: Oxford Uni-

versity Press.

Chamoreau, C. 2000. Chronique d’une mort annoncee. Un exemple de reduction

en phurhepecha. Actes du XXIeme Colloque international de la Societe inter-

nationale de linguistique fonctionnelle (SILF). Iasi (Romania), 26 June–2 July

1996, 127–132.

Chamoreau, C. 2005. Reorganizacion de la voz en purepecha: una vision dinamica.

In Dinamica linguıstica de las lenguas en contacto, C. Chamoreau and Y. Lastra

(eds), 67–86. Sonora: University of Sonora.

Chapman, S. and Derbyshire, D. C. 1991. Paumarı. In Handbook of Amazonian

Languages, vol. 3, D. C. Derbyshire and G. K. Pullum (eds), 151–355. Berlin:

Mouton de Gruyter.

Dal Negro, S. 2004. The Decay of a Language. The Case of a German Dialect in

the Italian Alps. Bern: Peter Lang.

Dench, A. 2001. Descent and di¤usion: the complexity of the Pilbara situation. In

Areal Di¤usion and Genetic Inheritance: Problems in Comparative Linguistics,

A. Y. Aikhenvald and R. M. W. Dixon (eds), 105–133. Oxford: Oxford Uni-

versity Press.

Dixon, R. M. W. 1972. The Dyirbal Language of North Queensland. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Dixon, R. M. W. 1977. A Grammar of Yidiny. Cambridge: Cambridge University

Press.

Language contact in language obsolescence 105

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 30: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Dixon, R. M. W. 1991a. A changing language situation: the decline of Dyirbal,

1963–1989. Language in Society 20: 183–200.

Dixon, R. M. W. 199lb. Reassigning underlying forms in Yidiny – a change during

language death. In Language and History, Essays in Honour of Luise A. Hercus,

P. Austin, R. M. W. Dixon, T. Dutton, and I. White (eds), 89–99. Canberra:

Pacific Linguistics.

Dixon, R. M. W. 1997. The Rise and Fall of Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press.

Dixon, R. M. W. 2002. Australian Languages: their Nature and Development.

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dixon, R. M. W. 2007. Field linguistis: a minor manual. Sprachtypologie und Uni-

versalienforschung 60, 1: 12–31.

Dorian, N. C. 1973. Grammatical change in a dying dialect. Language 49: 413–

438.

Dorian, N. C. 1977. The problem of the semi-speakers in language death. Linguistics

191: 23–32.

Dorian, N. C. (ed.). 1989. Investigating Obsolescence. Studies in Language Con-

traction and Death. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dorian, N. C. 1994. Purism vs. compromise in language revitalisation and lan-

guage revival. Language in Society 23: 479–494.

Dorian, N. C. 1999. The study of language obsolescence: stages, surprises, and

challenges. Languages and Linguistics 3: 99–122.

Dorian, N. C. 2002. Diglossia and the simplification of linguistic space. Interna-

tional Journal of the Sociology of Language 9: 63–70.

Evans, N. 2000. Iwaidjan, a very un-Australian language family. Linguistic Typology

4: 91–112.

Evans, N. 2007. More di¤erent than we thought: reassessing the genetic status of

Marrku. Seminar presented at RCLT.

Evans, N., Malwagag, J. W., and Marrala, K. 2006. Majila Inkawart. Six Stories

in Marrku, the Traditional Language of the Kroker Island, translated into

Iwaidja and English. Jabiru Town Council: Iwaidja Inyman.

Farabee, W. C. 1918. The Central Arawaks. Philadelphia: Anthropological Publi-

cations, The University Museum, University of Pennsylvania.

Foley, W. A. 1986. The Papuan Languages of New Guinea. Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press.

Francois, A. 2002. Araki: a Disappearing Language of Vanuatu. Canberra: Pacific

Linguistics.

Gardani, F. 2005. Borrowing of inflectional morphemes in language contact. MA

thesis, University of Vienna.

Grenoble, L. A. 2000. Morphosyntactic change: the impact of Russian on Evenki.

In Languages in Contact, D. G. Gilbers, J. Nerbonne, and J. Schaeken (eds),

105–120. Amsterdam: Rodopi.

106 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 31: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Grenoble, L. A. and Whaley, L. J. 1998. Towards a typology of language endanger-

ment. In Endangered Languages, L. Grenoble and L. J. Whaley (eds), 22–54.

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gruzdeva, E. Y. 2002. The linguistic consequences of Nivkh language attrition.

SKY Journal of Linguistics 15: 85–103.

Haas, M. R. 1941. Tunica (extract from Handbook of American Indian Languages,

vol. IV). New York: J. J. Augustin.

Hamp, E. 1989. On signs of health and death. In Investigating obsolescence. Studies

in Language Contraction and Death, N. C. Dorian (ed.), 197–210. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Hardenburg, W. E. 1910. The Indians of the Putumayo, Upper Amazon. Man 10:

134–8.

Haugen, E. 1989. The rise and fall of an immigrant language: Norwegian in

America. In Investigating Obsolescence. Studies in Language Contraction and

Death, N. C. Dorian (ed.), 61–74. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Heath, J. 1978. Linguistic di¤usion in Arnhem Land. Canberra: AIAS.

Helimski, E. A. 2007. Fonetika i morfonologia enetskogo jazyka v uslovijakh

jazykovogo sdviga (Phonetics and morphophonology of Enets in language shift).

In Jazykovye izmenenija v uslovijakh jazykovogo sdviga, (Language changes in

language shift), N. B. Vakhtin (ed.), 213–224. St. Peterburg: Nestor.

Henzl, V. M. 1981. Slavic languages in the new environment. In Language in the

USA, C. A. Ferguson and S. B. Heath (eds), 293–321. Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press.

Hill, J. H. 1985. The grammar of consciousness and the consciousness of grammar.

American Ethnologist 12: 725–37.

Hill, J. H. 1998. ‘‘Today there is no respect’’: nostalgia, ‘‘respect’’ and oppositional

discourse in Mexicano (Nahuatl) language ideology. In Language Ideologies:

Practice and Theory, B. B. Schie¤elin, K. A. Woolard, and P. V. Kroskrity

(eds), 68–86. New York: Oxford University Press.

Hill, J. H. and Hill, K. C. 1986. Speaking Mexicano. Dynamics of Syncretic Lan-

guage in Central Mexico. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

Hjelde, A. 1996. The gender of English nouns used in American Norwegian. In

Language Contact across the North Atlantic, P. S. Ureland and I. Clarkson

(eds), 297–312. Tubingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.

Howard, C. V. 1986. Mawayana: Formulario dos vocabularios padroes. Rio de

Janeiro: Museu Nacional. Ms.

Igualada, F. de. 1940. Informe sobre el Centro de investigaciones linguısticas y

etnograficas de la Amazonia colombiana (1933–1940). Amazonia colombiana

americanista, organo semestral de materiales y estudios para la americanistica

del CILEAC de Sibundoy. Pasto, t. 1.2–3: 61–91.

Igualada, F. de and Castellvı, M. de. 1940. Clasificacion y estadistica de las

lenguas habladas en el Putumayo (Caqueta) y Amazonas. Amazonia colombiana

americanista, organo semestral de materiales y estudios para la americanistica

del CILEAC de Sibundoy. Pasto, t. 1.2–3: 92–101.

Language contact in language obsolescence 107

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 32: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Johanson, L. 2002. Do languages die of structuritis? On the role of code-copying

in language endangerment. Rivista Italiana di Linguistica 14: 249–270.

Jungraithmayr, H. 1995. Was ist am Tangale noch tschadisch/hamitosemitisch? In

Sprachkulturelle und historische Forschungen in Afrika, A. Fleisch and D. Otten

(eds), 197–205. Cologne: Koppe.

Kagan, O. and Dillon, K. 2001. A new perspective on teaching Russian: focus on

the heritage learner. The Slavic and East European Journal 45: 507–518.

Klintborg, S. 1999. The Transience of American Swedish. Lund: Lund University

Press.

Kroskrity, P. V. 1993. Language, History and Identity: Ethnolinguistic Studies of

the Arizona Tewa. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

Laycock, D. C. and Z’graggen, J. 1975. The Sepik-Ramu phylum. In New Guinea

Area Languages and Language Study. Volume 1, Papuan languages and the

New Guinea linguistic scene, S. A. Wurm (ed.), 731–764. Canberra: Pacific

Linguistics.

Lee, Y. 1997. Classifiers in Korean. Honors thesis, Australian National University.

Lopez Sanz, R. 1972. El Bare. Estudio Linguıstico. Caracas: Faculty of Economics

and Social Sciences of the Central University of Venezuela.

Loukotka, C. 1968. Classification of South American Indian Languages. Los Angeles:

Latin American Studies Center, UCLA.

Matiso¤, J. A. 1990. On megalo-comparison: a discussion note. Language 66:

106–120.

Matras, Y. 1998. Utterance modifiers and universals of grammatical borrowing.

Linguistics 36: 281–331.

McGregor, W. 2002. Structural changes in language obsolescence: a Kimberley

(Australia) perspective. SKY Journal of Linguistics 15: 145–186.

Milani, C. 1996. Language contact among North-American people of Italian

origin. In Language Contact across the North Atlantic, P. S. Ureland and I.

Clarkson (eds), 479–501. Linguistische Arbeiten 359. Tubingen: University of

Tubingen.

Natterer, J. 1831. Sprachproben der Indier Bare. Bewohner von Marabitanas vom

Stamm Arihini/Alihini. Nation Arhini. Sprache der Nation Bare – Stamm

ihini. Lists 30–32. ms.

Newman, S. 1977. The Salish independent pronoun system. International Journal

of American Linguistics 45: 207–223.

Newman, S. 1979a. A history of the Salish possessive and subject forms. Inter-

national Journal of American Linguistics 45: 207–23.

Newman, S. 1979b. The Salish object forms. International Journal of American

Linguistics 45: 299–308.

Payne, D. L. 1985. The genetic classification of Resigaro. International Journal of

American Linguistics 51: 222–231.

Payne, D. L. 1991. A classification of Maipuran (Arawakan) languages based on

shared lexical retentions. In Handbook of Amazonian Languages, vol. 3, D. C.

Derbyshire and G. K. Pullum (eds), 355–499. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

108 Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 33: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Pereltsvaig, A. 2008. Aspect in Russian as grammatical rather than lexical notion.

Russian Linguistics 32: 27–42.

Pryor, J. 1990. Deixis and participant tracking in Botin. Language and Linguistics

in Melanesia 21: 1–29.

Ramirez, H. 1997. A fala Tukano dos Yepa-masa. Tomo 1. Gramatica. Manaus:

Inspetoria Salesiana.

Riionheimo, H. 2002. How to borrow a bound morpheme? Evaluating the status

of structural interference in a contact between closely related languages. SKY

Journal of Linguistics 15: 187–218.

Rivet, P. and de Wavrin, R. 1951. Un nouveau dialecte Arawak: le Resigaro.

Journal de la Societe des Americanistes de Paris 40: 203–238.

Sasse, H.-J. 1992a. Theory of language death. In Language Death. Factual and

Theoretical Explorations with Special Reference to East Africa, M. Brenzinger

(ed.), 31–57. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Sasse, H.-J. 1992b. Language decay and contact-induced change: similarities and

di¤erences’. In Language Death. Factual and Theoretical Explorations with Special

Reference to East Africa, M. Brenzinger (ed.), 58–79. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Sasse, H.-J. 2001. A Phoenix from the ashes: linguistic creativity in an obsolescent

speech community. Seminar presented at RCLT.

Schomburgk, R. 1848. Comparative vocabulary of 18 languages and dialects of

Indian tribes inhabiting Guiana. Report of the 18th British Association for the

Advancement of Science, 96–99.

Schmidt, A. 1985. Young People’s Dyirbal. An Example of Language Death from

Australia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Stolz, C. and Stolz, T. 1996. Funktionswortenentlehnung in Mesoamerika. Spanisch-

amerindischer Sprachkontakt (Hispanoindiana II). STUF 49: 86–123.

Swadesh, M. 1951. Di¤usional cumulation and archaic residue as historical

explanations. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 7: 1–21.

Tessmann, G. 1930. Die Indianer Nordost-Perus. Grundlegende Forschungen fur

eine systematische Kulturkunde. Hamburg: Friederichsen, De Gruyter & Co,

M. B. H.

Thiesen, W. 1996. Gramatica del Idioma Bora. Serie Linguıstica Peruana 38. Pucallpa:

Instituto Linguıstico de Verano.

Tsitsipis, L. D. 1998. A Linguistic Anthropology of Praxis and Language Shift.

Arvanıtika (Albanian) and Greek in Contact. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Whi¤en, T. W. 1915. The North-west Amazonas: Notes of some Months Spent

among Cannibal Tribes. London: Constable and Company.

Language contact in language obsolescence 109

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51

Page 34: Dynamics of Contact-Induced Language Change

Bereitgestellt von | provisional accountAngemeldet | 212.87.45.97

Heruntergeladen am | 04.03.13 15:51