36
Exploring Emotional Memory as a Potential Mechanism Explaining the Effectiveness of Mindfulness Master’s Thesis Presented to The Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences Brandeis University Department of Psychology Ellen J Wright, Advisor In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Psychology by Bo Dong August 2015

Dong Thesis 2015

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

thesis

Citation preview

Page 1: Dong Thesis 2015

Exploring Emotional Memory as a Potential Mechanism Explaining the Effectiveness of

Mindfulness

Master’s Thesis

Presented to

The Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences

Brandeis University

Department of Psychology

Ellen J Wright, Advisor

In Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements for the Degree

Master of Arts

in

Psychology

by

Bo Dong

August 2015

Page 2: Dong Thesis 2015

Copyright by

Bo Dong

©2015

Page 3: Dong Thesis 2015

iii

ABSTRACT

Exploring Emotional Memory as a Potential Mechanism Explaining the Effectiveness of

Mindfulness

A thesis presented to the Psychology Department

Graduate School of Arts and Sciences

Brandeis University

Waltham, Massachusetts

By Bo Dong

Mindfulness has been found to improve psychological well-being. It has also been incorporated

in many clinical interventions as an effective strategy against various psychological disorders.

However, how mindfulness works is still unclear. Mindfulness emphasizes observing the present

moments with fully awareness and non-reactivity, so it may change the way people remember

things, especially for events containing strong emotional stimuli. It is possible that mindfulness

reduces the bias towards negative memories, which is typically shown in depressed people. The

current study investigated emotional memory as a potential pathway through which mindfulness

training leads to improvement in psychological well-being as well as a decrease in psychological

symptoms. Fifty-nine undergraduates were recruited for the study (69.5% female). Half of them

were randomly selected to receive brief mindfulness training by listening to an audio guided

mindfulness meditation tape, and the other half received music relaxation treatment. All of the

participants viewed a set of pictures with positive, negative and neutral valences, and were given

a recognition task. Participants who received mindfulness training had higher false alarm rates

for positively valenced images, and higher false alarms for positive images were related to

greater psychological well-being. The current study provided evidence that the effectiveness of

mindfulness training could be explained by changes in emotional memory.

Page 4: Dong Thesis 2015

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Title. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . i

Copyright . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ii

Abstract . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iii

List of Tables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi

Table 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi

Table 2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii

Table 3 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . viii

List of Figures. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .ix

Figure 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix

Figure 2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . x

Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1

Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9

Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9

Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9

Measures. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .9

Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12

Page 5: Dong Thesis 2015

v

Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14

Mindfulness and Psychological Well-being/Distress. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .14

Mindfulness and memory (recognition) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15

Memory (recognition) and psychological distress/well-being . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17

Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18

Limitations and implications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19

Conclusions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21

References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22

Appendix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26

Page 6: Dong Thesis 2015

vi

List of Tables

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the questionnaire measures

Control Mindful

Mean SD Mean SD

Cognitive reappraisal 4.79 .73 5.12 .88

Expressive suppression 3.61 1.26 3.42 1.19

Brooding 2.26 .88 2.13 .77

Trait Mindfulness 3.01 .47 3.16 .33

Time1 psychological distress 2.81 .76 2.68 .68

Time1 psychological well-being 3.25 .92 3.52 .79

Time2 psychological distress 2.79 .73 2.56 .64

Time2 psychological well-being 3.31 .97 3.74 .80

Note: No difference was found between mindfulness and control group for each of the

measures

Page 7: Dong Thesis 2015

vii

Table 2. Correlations among the questionnaire measures

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Cognitive reappraisal .

2. Expressive suppression -.07

3. Brooding -.09 .05

4. Trait Mindfulness -.30 -.20 -.45**

5. Time1 distress -.19 .15 .51** -.49**

6. Time1 well-being .15 -.22 -.34* .43** -.78**

7. Time2 distress -.20 .07 .48** -.43** .92** -.70**

8. Time2 well-being .21 -.21 -.31* .39** -.72** .94** -.74**

Note:*p<.05, **p<.01, **p<.001

Page 8: Dong Thesis 2015

viii

Table 3. Hits, false alarms, corrected recognition and sensitivity in each condition

Control Mindful

Mean (SD) Mean (SD)

Hits

Positive .86 (.14) .85 (.11)

Negative .95 (.07) .93 (.11)

False Alarms

Positive .02 (.04) .05 (.07)

Negative .13 (.09) .17 (.15)

Corrected Recognition

Positive .84 (.16) .80 (.14)

Negative .82 (.13) .76 (.22)

Sensitivity

Positive .96 (.05) .95 (.04)

Negative .95 (.04) .93 (.10)

Note: The table showed each of the memory test scores for each group and image type.

Page 9: Dong Thesis 2015

ix

List of Figures

Figure 1. Time 1 and time 2 psychological well-being between the two conditions

Figure Caption: the graph illustrated the mean psychological well-being score at time 1 and

time 2 separately for control group and mindfulness group. Time 2 well-being was

significantly increased only in mindfulness group.

Page 10: Dong Thesis 2015

x

Figure 2. Time 1 and time 2 psychological distress between the two conditions

Figure Caption: the graph illustrated the mean psychological distress score at time 1 and

time 2 separately for control group and mindfulness group. Time 2 distress was

significantly decreased only in mindfulness group.

Page 11: Dong Thesis 2015

1

Introduction

Mindfulness, which originated in eastern Buddhism, is defined as a process of bringing one’s

attention and awareness to the present moment in a nonjudgmental and accepting way (Brown &

Ryan, 2003; Kabat-Zinn, 1990). A higher state of mindfulness is associated with a variety of

well-being indicators, such as reduction in mood disturbance and stress, increase in

psychological well-being, and improvements in many psychological problems such as depression,

anxiety, eating disorders and personality disorders (Bach & Hayes 2002; Brown & Ryan, 2003;

Nyklíček & Kuijpers, 2008). Moreover, the benefits of being mindful have been seen across

different populations (e.g., patients with cancer). Jon Kabat-Zin first introduced the concept of

mindfulness in the US and incorporated it into clinical practices. Jon Kabat-Zin designed the

Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR); it has emerged as a new way to treat a variety of

psychological disorders, eating disorders and anxiety disorders (Kabat-Zinn, 1990; Marchand,

2012). According to a meta-analytical study by Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt and Walach

(2004), MBSR improved clinical and non-clinical problems with an average effect size of 0.5.

Following MBSR, other mindfulness-based interventions such as Mindfulness Based Cognitive

Therapy and Mindfulness Based Relationship Enhancement have also become popular (JW.

Carson, KM. Carson, Gil, & Baucom, 2004; Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002).

While many researchers are trying to demonstrate more benefits related with mindfulness,

it is equally important to understand what makes mindfulness practice effective (Shapiro,

Carlson, Astin & Freedman, 2006). However, only recently the question as to how or why

Page 12: Dong Thesis 2015

2

mindfulness works has aroused researchers’ attention (e.g., Roberts-Wolfe, Sacchet, Hastings,

Roth, & Britton, 2012; Taylor et al., 2011). Better understanding of the mindfulness mechanisms

could provide an empirical rationale for using mindfulness practice to promote psychological

health and alleviate psychological symptoms.

Several studies have examined emotion regulation as a mechanism through which

mindfulness is associated with depressive and anxiety symptoms (e.g., Desrosiers, Vine, Curtiss,

& Klemanski, 2014). Desrosiers, Vine, Klemanski and Nolen-Hoeksema (2013) found that

rumination mediated the relationship between mindfulness and both anxiety symptoms and

depressive symptoms. This supported transdiagnostic models of psychopathology, which

categorizes disorders based on underlying mechanisms or core disturbances (Kring & Sloan,

2009). In addition, they also found that reappraisal mediated the association between

mindfulness and depressive symptoms, as well as worry mediated the association between

mindfulness and anxiety symptoms. Both rumination and worry involves maladaptive repetitive

thoughts, with rumination focusing on previous negative events (Lyubomirsky, Caldwell, &

Nolen-Hoeksema, 1998) and worry focusing on future threats (Watkins, 2008). The present-

focused element of mindfulness is antithetical to the emphasis in either rumination or worry; thus,

mindfulness may buffer the negative effects of rumination and worry (Desrosiers et al., 2013).

Although the study suggested that mindfulness was associated with better psychological outcome

through an increase in adaptive emotion regulation strategy (i.e., reappraisal) and a decrease in

maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (i.e., rumination and worry), the direction of these

effects are unknown. Moreover, the mediation effects of emotion regulation, however significant,

were not large.

Page 13: Dong Thesis 2015

3

Counterintuitively, some studies found that mindfulness was positively related to

psychopathologies. The observing element in mindfulness was particularly associated with

higher level of distress and anxiety symptoms (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney,

2006). Deroisers et al (2014) also demonstrated that observing was associated with depressive

symptoms through rumination, and was associated with anxiety symptoms through worry.

However, non-reactivity moderated the pathway to depressive symptoms, but not anxiety

symptoms. Observing non-reactively might prevent individuals from automatically engaging in a

maladaptive style of cognitive processing (i.e., rumination), and not being stuck in internal

feelings or external experience (Desrosiers et al., 2014). However, the positive association

between anxiety and observing remained significant even when combined with non-reactivity.

Thus, the unique connection between observing and anxiety symptoms needs to be further

explored.

Another set of studies examined emotional intelligence as a mediator between

mindfulness and psychological well-being. Emotional intelligence refers to skills in four areas:

accuracy in perceiving emotions, using emotions to facilitate thoughts, understanding emotions

and managing emotions to improve personal and social functioning (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso,

2008). Schutte and Malouff (2011) argued that the non-judgmental, self-regulating and being

present nature of mindfulness might enhance emotional abilities and competencies, hence

promoting greater emotional intelligence. Higher levels of trait emotional intelligence have been

associated with positive affect, life satisfaction, social competence and many other aspects of

psychological well-being that are common in people with high trait mindfulness (e.g., Austin,

Saklofske, & Egan, 2005; Mavroveli, Petrides, Rieffe & Bakker, 2007). Indeed, emotional

intelligence significantly mediated the positive association between mindfulness and the three

Page 14: Dong Thesis 2015

4

indicators of subjective well-being: higher positive affect, lower negative affect and higher life

satisfaction (Schutte & Malouff, 2011). Consistent with that finding, Wang and Kong (2014)

found that emotional intelligence partially mediated both the association between mindfulness

and life satisfaction and the association between mindfulness and mental distress. The partial

mediation effects suggested that emotional intelligence itself would not sufficiently explain the

mechanism linking mindfulness and positive psychological outcomes.

Studies of emotion regulation and emotional intelligence have both provided a better

understanding of why mindfulness practice works, yet their uniform reliance on cross-sectional

design did not elucidate the direction of effects. Thus, more research needs to be done in order to

comprehend the pathway through mindfulness to psychological well-being or psychopathologies.

Roberts-Wolfe et al. (2012) have suggested that mindfulness might affect how emotional

information is processed. They referred to the cognitive model of depression (Beck, 1987),

which stated that depressive people have biased attention and memory toward negative stimuli

and away from positive stimuli (e.g., Burt et al., 1995). Mindfulness practice can be seen as a

method to shift distressed individual’s attention and memory toward positive stimuli, which has

been related to well-being (Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002; Taylor & Brown, 1994). Specifically,

the current research is interested in whether mindfulness influences memory for positive relative

to negative events differently.

To study how mindfulness may influence people’s emotional memory, first we need to

investigate how mindfulness practice alters responses to emotional stimuli. Mindfulness helps

individuals to develop the ability to insist on neither pursuing pleasant experiences nor

intentionally forcing aversive experiences away (Shapiro, Carlson, Astin & Freedman, 2006). It

Page 15: Dong Thesis 2015

5

is likely that individuals high in mindfulness might be prone to remain detached or less engaged

in emotional experiences, so that they encounter less distress.

Alternatively, mindfulness may allow individuals to be better able to fully engage in

emotional experiences, but the non-reactivity and non-judgmental aspects might mitigate the

effects brought by negative experiences. Greenberg and Meiran (2013) tested whether

mindfulness was leading to “emotional blunting” or emotional engagement. It turned out that

after receiving mindfulness meditation, participants took twice as longer time to recall a mood

incongruent memory (e.g., after watching a sad movie, they took longer to recall the first happy

memory). This result supported the emotional engagement hypothesis. They also suggested that

mindfulness was associated with rapid recovery from emotion, since mindfulness meditators

recalled as many mood incongruent memories as controls.

If mindfulness leads to engagement in both positive and negative emotional stimuli, but

does not display the negative effects generated by negative stimuli; this finding should be further

explained. Using brain neuroimaging, Taylor et al. (2011) discovered a consistent result of the

processing of emotional stimuli in mindfulness meditators; mindfulness practice attenuated the

intensity of perception from emotional stimuli. However, this attenuation was achieved through

different mechanisms. For experienced meditators, mindfulness induced deactivations of regions

related to the default mode network but did not influence brain activity in emotional reactivity

brain areas. In contrast, for beginner meditators, mindfulness resulted in a down-regulation of the

left amygdala during emotional processing. Either way, the study suggested that mindfulness

encouraged a more relaxed, adaptive and objective response to all valences of emotional stimuli,

and that mindfulness fostered emotional stability. Although this study compared beginner

mindfulness mediators and experienced mindfulness meditators to address the effectiveness of

Page 16: Dong Thesis 2015

6

mindfulness training, a control group is needed to illustrate how mindfulness practice changes

the processing of emotional stimuli.

Evans and Segerstrom (2011) evaluated the association between mindfulness and a

variety of repetitive thoughts, in terms of valence, purpose and total amount of propensity to

engage in repetitive thoughts (Segerstrom et al., 2003). Results indicated that mindfulness was

associated with less total repetitive thoughts but more positive repetitive thoughts. It implied that

practicing mindfulness is related with thinking positively.

To our knowledge, only two studies tentatively explored the relationships among

mindfulness practice, emotional memory and psychological well-being/distress. Roberts-Wolfe

(2012) studied emotional memory using a free word recall test, and found that mindfulness

meditators recalled more positive words compared with participants who received music training;

however, no significant difference was found for recall of negative words. In addition,

mindfulness meditation training improved psychological well-being. The longitudinal design of

the study enabled the temporal relationship between mindfulness, emotional memory and

psychological well-being to be established. Nevertheless, one critical deficit in the design of the

study was concerning. Instead of using true random design, participants in this study were asked

to choose which training group they wanted to join. The consequence could be that currently

distressed individuals were more likely to choose mindfulness group since they were seeking

help, so the increase in positive word recall might be due to a regression to the mean.

Another study used a brief audiotaped mindfulness training to study its effects on a verbal

word recall test (Alberts & Thewissen, 2011). An opposite pattern of results was yielded

compared with the former study: participants that received mindfulness training recalled less

negative words as opposed to more positive words. Neither of these two studies tested the

Page 17: Dong Thesis 2015

7

mediation role of emotional memory between mindfulness and psychological well-being,

although psychological well-being was accompanied by either an increase in positive word recall

or a decrease in negative word recall.

The current research examined the influence of a mindfulness meditation on emotional

memory, the influence of emotional memory on psychological well-being/distress, and the

connections with mindfulness. We gave participants a short mindfulness meditation practice

which lasted 10 minutes and then tested their memory for pictures that contained emotional

stimuli (positive vs. negative). We hypothesized that first, mindfulness meditation would

increase psychological well-being and decrease psychological distress; second, those who

received mindfulness meditation would have better memory for positive images and worse

memory for negative images; third, more positive emotional memory and less negative

emotional memory would each be associated with increase in psychological well-being and

decreases in psychological distress; lastly, changes in emotional memory would mediate the

relationship between mindfulness training and psychological well-being/distress.

In addition, the current study examined the potential involvement of emotion regulation.

Emotion regulation includes a broad range of strategies we use to manipulate emotional

responses (Gross, 1999a). People differ in their use of emotion regulation strategies. The current

study analyzed whether emotion regulation moderated the association between mindfulness and

psychological outcomes. Specifically, we hypothesized that people who use more adaptive

emotion regulation strategies (i.e., cognitive reappraisal) would demonstrate higher level of

psychological well-being and lower level of psychological distress; people who use more

maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (i.e., expressive suppression and rumination) would

demonstrate lower level of psychological well-being and higher level of psychological distress.

Page 18: Dong Thesis 2015

8

The findings would provide insight about under what conditions mindfulness appears to be more

effective.

Page 19: Dong Thesis 2015

9

Methods

Participants

Participants were 59 undergraduate students (30.5% male, 69.5% female) of Brandeis

University. Students received research credit or extra credit for participating in this study. Of the

59 participants, 1.8% identified as African American, 27.3% as Asian, 63.7% as Caucasian, 5.5%

as Hispanic, and 1.8% as others. The participants ranged in age from 17 to 22 years (M=19.54,

SD=.94).

Design

Participants first completed a packet of self-report questionnaires that measure cognitive

reappraisal, expressive suppression, rumination, trait mindfulness, and demographic information.

Then participants were randomly assigned into two groups: one received mindfulness training

and the other received music relaxation break as control. Both groups were be asked to view a

slide show of emotional images and do a memory test following that.

Measures

Emotion Regulation. Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross and John, 2003)

measures individual differences in the habitual use of two emotion regulation strategies:

expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Expressive suppression, as opposed to

cognitive reappraisal, is considered as a maladaptive strategies that has been associated with

stress-related symptoms (Moore, Zoellner & Mollenholt, 2008). The tendency to use

effective/adaptive emotion regulation strategy or ineffective/maladaptive strategies may tease

apart the effect as to under which conditions mindfulness practice works better. Participants were

Page 20: Dong Thesis 2015

10

asked to answer each item on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7

(strongly agree), with sample item as “I keep my emotions to myself”. Internal consistency (α

=.73 for cognitive reappraisal, and α =.78 for expressive suppression) was comparable to what

has been found in previous studies using the ERQ (Stephen, Steven, Andrew, & Benjamin, 2011;

Gross & John, 2003).

Brooding. The Brooding subscale of Ruminative Response Scale (RRS-B; Treynor,

Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003) measures an individual’s dispositional tendency to

ruminate. The brooding subscale was used because it assesses only the maladaptive part of

rumination, and it does not confound with depression (Treynor et al., 2003). In addition,

rumination is a transdiagnostic factor that can account for symptoms in many disorders, such as

depression and anxiety (Kring & Sloan, 2009; McLaughlin and Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011).

Participants were asked to indicate what they generally do for each item on a 4-point Likert-type

scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always), with sample item as “think ‘what am I

doing to deserve this?’”. The brooding subscale has 4 items in total, with good internal

consistency (α =.79).

Trait mindfulness. Five Facet of Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al., 2008)

measures the general tendency towards mindfulness in daily life. The FFMQ captures different

aspects of trait mindfulness, including observing, describing, acting with awareness, non-judging

of inner experience and non-reactivity. Participants were asked to indicate how frequently or

infrequently they have experienced each description on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1

(never or very rarely true) to 5 (very often or always true). There are 24 items in total. Higher

average score indicates higher level of trait mindfulness. The internal consistency was good (α

=.84).

Page 21: Dong Thesis 2015

11

Psychological well-being/distress. The Mental Health Inventory (MHI; Veit and Ware,

Jr., 1983) measures various aspects of psychological distress, such as anxiety, depression and

loneliness, and psychological well-being, such as positive affect and emotional ties. Participants

answered each item on a 6-point Likert scale, and the options varied by the items. There are 38

items in total. Twenty-four items measured psychological distress, and fourteen items measured

psychological well-being. The MHI was measured both at the beginning (time1) and at the end

of the study (time2). Measurement for psychological distress and well-being both demonstrated

high internal consistency at time 1 and 2 (α ranged from .91 to .94).

Brief mindfulness practice. The mindfulness group was asked to follow a video guided

mindfulness meditation training that lasted for about 15 minutes. The video was obtained from

the internet, and consisted of an audio of mindfulness practice script accompanied by a relaxing

music and pictorial visual contents. The video was chosen because it covered most of the major

steps in mindfulness meditation training, such as introduction of mindfulness, body relaxing,

awareness of the mind, mantra repeating and releasing of negative thoughts (see appendix for

transcribed audio text). The visual contents were used to draw participant’s attention to the task,

rather than having them looking around at the experiment room. Participants were asked to

follow the instructions to practice mindfulness meditation, for example, breathing, attention

focusing, and non-reactivity.

Music relaxation. The control group (music) was presented with same visual contents

accompanied by a collection of relaxing music, which also lasted for about 15 minutes. Music

relaxation was used as control because it would help to demonstrate that the effect of

mindfulness was different from the regular relaxation method (i.e. music). In a previous study

Page 22: Dong Thesis 2015

12

mindfulness training was associated with more psychological well-being when compared with

music relaxation training (Roberts-Wolfe etc., 2012).

Emotional stimuli task. The participants were asked to watch a slide show of images

that contains emotional stimuli. Images were taken from the International Affective Picture

System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2008). Thirty negatively valenced images and 30

positively valenced images were randomly selected based on IAPS rating. The order of the

images was also be randomly assigned in the slide show. The slide show started with 10

neutrally valenced pictures so the participants became familiar with the task and so the primacy

effect would be reduced. Another 10 neutrally valenced pictures were presented at the end of the

slide show to reduce recency effect

Recognition memory test. A memory test for the pictures from the slide show was

administered. The task was another slide show consisting of 40 images with old images from the

first slide show and new images (i.e., 10 negative appeared, 10 negative did not appear, 10

positive appeared, 10 positive did not appear). The participants were asked if they have seen the

image or not. The slide show would not continue until the participants gave an answer. Ten

neutrally valenced pictures appeared at the end but responses to the items were not included in

data analysis. These images were included to make sure the participants would not leave the

experiment still recalling disturbing pictures.

Procedure

Upon obtaining informed consent, participants first completed questions on demographic

information, the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire, the Ruminative Responses Scale-Brooding,

the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire, and the Mental Health Inventory (time 1). Then they

were instructed to either watch and learn mindfulness meditation through a 15 minutes video or

Page 23: Dong Thesis 2015

13

listen to relaxing music for 15 minutes. Following that was the emotional stimuli task and then

the memory task which lasted about 10 minutes. Finally, the participants filled out the Mental

Health Inventory again (time 2).

Page 24: Dong Thesis 2015

14

Results

Four subjects either were unable to finish the study or proceeded directly to the memory

task after the images slide show without waiting for the experimenters to give proper instructions,

thus were excluded from data analysis.

Descriptive statistics and correlations among cognitive reappraisal, expressive

suppression, brooding, trait mindfulness, psychological distress and well-being can be found in

Tables 1 and 2. Those who were excluded from the study did not differ on any of the measures

from those who were included. Brooding was negatively correlated with trait mindfulness as

expected, because its past events-oriented nature is considered an opposite to the present moment

focused nature of mindfulness (Desrosiers et al., 2013). Brooding was also positively correlated

with psychological distress and negatively correlated with psychological well-being. However,

neither cognitive reappraisal nor expressive suppression was significantly correlated with the

other variables.

Mindfulness and Psychological Well-being/Distress

To test whether psychological well-being and distress changed between time 1 and time 2,

2 ANCOVA tests were conducted separated for well-being and distress with time 2

psychological well-being/distress as dependent variable, time 1 as baseline covariate and group

as between subjects variable. For psychological well-being, there was a significant main effect of

group after controlling for time 1 psychological well-being, F(1,52)=4.33, p=.042, 2p=.08, with

the mindfulness group having higher well-being score (Madjusted=3.63, SD=.06) than the control

Page 25: Dong Thesis 2015

15

group (Madjusted=3.45, SD=.06). For psychological distress, there was a marginally significant

main effect for group after controlling for time 1 distress, F(1,52)=2.89, p=.09, 2p=.05, with the

mindfulness group (Madjusted=2.61, SD=.05) having lower distress score than the control group

(Madjusted=2.73, SD=.06).

To test whether emotion regulation might moderate the effect of mindfulness training (vs.

control), cognitive reappraisal and brooding were added to the previous model, respectively. No

significant interaction effect between each of the three emotion regulation strategies (expressive

suppression, cognitive reappraisal and rumination) and group was found: expressive suppression,

F(1,50)=.003, p=.95, cognitive reappraisal, F(1,50)=1.20, p=.28, rumination, F(1,50)=1.76,

p=.19.

Mindfulness and Memory (Recognition)

Signal detection theory (SDT) was utilized to quantify the memory test performance

systematically for each type of image between the two groups (Stanislaw & Todorov, 1999).

Four measures were calculated according to SDT: hit rates, false alarms, corrected recognition

and nonparametric measures of sensitivity.

Hit rates. Hit rates were calculated as the proportion of correctly recognized old images

within each experiment condition for image type (Stanislaw & Todorov, 1999). We hypothesized

that mindfulness group would have a higher hit rates than the control group. A 2 (group) x 2

(valence) ANOVA was conducted. There was significant main effect of image type,

F(1,53)=21.87, p<.001, 2p=.29, with higher hit rates for negative image (M=.93, SD=.09) than

for positive image (M=.86, SD=.12), as shown in table 4. There was no significant main effect

for group (Mcontrol=.90, SDcontrol=.02; Mmindful=.89, SDmindful=.02), F(1,53)=.25, p=.62, or image

type and group interaction, F(1,53)=.01, p=.94, which did not support our hypothesis.

Page 26: Dong Thesis 2015

16

False Alarms. We hypothesized that the mindfulness group would have a higher false

alarm rate for positive image because participants received mindfulness training should be

preoccupied with positive stimuli and have their attention more biased towards positive stimuli.

False alarms were calculated as the proportion of new items incorrectly recognized as old within

each experiment condition for each image type (Stanislaw & Todorov, 1999). A 2 (group) x 2

(valence) ANOVA was conducted. There was significant main effect of image type,

F(1,53)=46.04, p<.001, 2p=.47, with higher false alarms for negative images (M=.15, SD=.13)

compared with positive images (M=.04, SD=.06), as shown in table 4. No significant main effect

of experiment condition was found (Mcontrol=.07, SDcontrol=.02; Mmindful=.11, SDmindful=.02),

F(1,53)=2.46, p=.12, 2p=.04. No interaction effect between image type and group was found,

F(1,53)=.21, p=.65. However, there was a marginal significant effect of group when only looking

at false alarm rates for positive image, F(1,53)=2.76, p=.10, 2p=.05, with the mindfulness group

(M=.05, SD=.07) demonstrating more false alarms than control group (M=.02, SD=.04), as

shown in table 4.

Correct recognition. Corrected recognition scores were calculated by subtracting the

proportion of false alarms from the proportion of hits for each type of image, to test whether

there was a difference in memory accuracy across each experiment condition (Stanislaw &

Todorov, 1999). We expected no difference in corrected recognition for the two groups. Again, a

factorial ANOVA with group as the between subjects variable and image type as the within

subject variable was conducted. No significant main effect of image type or experiment

condition, or interaction between image type and condition was found, p’s>.10.

Sensitivity. Nonparametric measures of sensitivity (A’) were calculated to examine the

memory test performance (Stanislaw & Todorov, 1999). A’ was used because it was not affected

Page 27: Dong Thesis 2015

17

by response bias (i.e. a true measure of sensitivity). A factorial ANOVA with group as the

between subjects variable and image type as the within subject variable indicated no significant

effect of either image (Mpositive=.95, SDpositive=.04; Mnegative=.94, SDnegative=.08), F(1,53)=2.00,

p=.16, 2p=.04, or group (Mcontrol=.95, SDcontrol=.01; Mmindful=.94, SDmindful=.01), F(1,53)=1.55,

p=.20, 2p=.03, or imageXgroup interaction, F(1,53)=.47, p=.50, 2

p=.01.

Memory (recognition) and psychological distress/well-being

To explore whether the difference in participants’ memory could have an effect on

psychological distress/well-being, several ANCOVAs were conducted with either time 2

psychological well-being or distress as the dependent variable, time 1 psychological well-being

or distress as the baseline covariate, group as the between subject variable and memory

performance as the independent variable (Hit rate, False Alarms, Corrected Recognition, and

Sensitivity). Only false alarm rate for positive images were marginally significant in predicting

time 2 psychological well-being, controlling for time 1 psychological well-being, F(1,52)=3.88,

p=.05, 2p=.07, so that higher false alarms for positively valenced images were related to higher

score on psychological well-being. The effect of mindfulness was not undermined when false

alarm rates for positive image were added in the model: without false alarm for positive image,

F(1,53)=6.44, p=.05, 2p=.11; with false alarm for positive image, F(1,52)=4.33, p=.04, 2

p=.08.

Thus the mediational relationship with emotional memory mediating the relationship between

mindfulness training and psychological well-being was not supported.

Page 28: Dong Thesis 2015

18

Discussion

Consistent with our hypotheses and many previous studies (Brown & Ryan, 2003;

Carmody & Baer, 2008; Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004), we found that

receiving a 15 minutes brief mindfulness training increased perceived psychological well-being

and lowered psychological distress. The findings provided further evidence of the effectiveness

of brief mindfulness training, in particular, among college students.

Our second hypothesis was that mindfulness training would affect people’s emotional

memory. Specifically, those who received mindfulness training should have more false alarms

for positive images than those who did not receive mindfulness training. For the image

recognition test, we found that negative images had both higher hit rate and false alarm rate than

positive images for both mindfulness and control groups. This is not surprising, as previous

literature has indicated that human brains are naturally more sensitive to emotionally negative

stimuli (Cacioppo & Gardner, 1999; Delplanque, Lavoie, Hot, & Silvert, 2004; Huang & Luo,

2006). We also found a higher false alarm rate for positive images for mindfulness group

compared to control group, although the difference was marginally significant. This supported

our hypothesis that participants received mindfulness training have a bias towards positive

stimuli. That is to say, participants tend to respond that they have seen a positively valenced

image even if the image was not seen previously. It is particularly interesting because our study

demonstrated that a person who received mindfulness training might have more positive memory,

or at least believed he/she have more positive memory. According to Taylor and Brown (1998),

Page 29: Dong Thesis 2015

19

even positive illusion or positive unrealistic memories about positive events could contribute to

better mental health outcome. Thus, the positive bias generated by mindfulness training might be

the reason why it increases psychological well-being.

The general memory performance indicated by corrected recognition and sensitivity was

not different between the two groups for either positive or negative image. However, this is not

unexpected, as we did not propose any true memory difference for mindfulness group vs. control

group.

The third hypothesis was that the difference in memory performance might contribute to

the difference in psychological well-being/distress. Our results indicated that higher false alarm

rate positively predicted time 2 psychological well-being after controlling for time 1

psychological well-being. So far, we have demonstrated that mindfulness meditation made

participants have higher false alarm rate for positively valenced image, and that higher false

alarm rate for positive image predicted higher psychological well-being.

Lastly, we examined the involvement of emotion regulation strategies in predicting

psychological well-being/distress, but we did not find any significant interaction between

emotion regulation strategy and mindfulness training.

Limitations and Implications

Although we established three links among all the targeted variables: mindfulness and

psychological well-being, mindfulness and positive emotional memory, positive emotional

memory and psychological well-being, we did not find significant results in how negative

emotional memory or psychological distress were involved. Perhaps mindfulness training is only

related to a boost of psychological well-being through more positive emotional memory rather

than through less negative emotional memory.

Page 30: Dong Thesis 2015

20

Some of the relationships among the three variables were marginally significant. One reason

could be the small sample size. Another reason was that most participants, no matter in the

mindfulness or the control condition, had very good performance on the memory test (i.e., high

hit rates), so large variations in participants’ memory performance results were not obtained. The

memory test might be too easy for the participants, since it was a recognition test and that the test

was followed immediately by the image slide show. In essence, the memory recognition test

showed a ceiling effect. Future studies could increase the difficulty of the test by introducing a

distraction task in between the stimuli exposure and recognition test, or lengthen the time span

before the recognition test (e.g., Flores and Berenbaum, 2012).

Our findings implied that mindfulness meditation potentially altered people’s emotional

memory with a bias towards positively valenced stimuli, and thus increased psychological well-

being. It would be interesting for future studies to examine the specific processes that changed

through mindfulness training, such as encoding and retrieval of memory. Moreover, the current

study validated previous studies on the effectiveness of 15-minute brief mindfulness training.

This should theoretically benefit college students who need a way to relieve their daily stress.

However, the long-term effects of brief mindfulness training should also be examined in future

studies.

Page 31: Dong Thesis 2015

21

Conclusion

The current study demonstrated that only receiving 15 minutes brief mindfulness training

contributed to better mental health outcome, and that this process was at least partially explained

by the increase of false alarms towards positive emotional memory. This study was one of the

first to examine mechanisms by which mindfulness might make a difference in mental health

indicators. We encourage future studies to keep exploring the mechanism of mindfulness

meditation.

Page 32: Dong Thesis 2015

22

References

Alberts, H. J., & Thewissen, R. (2011). The effect of a brief mindfulness intervention on memory

for positively and negatively valenced stimuli. Mindfulness, 2, 73-77.

doi:10.1007/s12671-011-0044-7

Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., & Egan, V. (2005). Personality, well-being and health correlates

of trait emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 547-558.

doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.05.009

Bach, P., & Hayes, S. C. (2002). The use of acceptance and commitment therapy to prevent the

rehospitalization of psychotic patients: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 1129. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.5.1129

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report

assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27-45.

doi:10.1177/1073191105283504

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., & Williams, J. M. G.

(2008). Construct validity of the five-facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and

nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15, 329-342. doi:10.1177/1073191107313003

Beck, A.T.(1987).Cognitive models of depression. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1, 5–37.

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in

psychological well-being. Journal of personality and social psychology, 84, 822.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Burt, D.B., Zembar, M.J., and Niederehe, G.(1995).Depression and memory impairment: a meta-

analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychol. Bull. 117, 285–305.

doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.285

Cacioppo, J. T., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Emotion. Annual review of psychology, 50, 191-214.

doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.191

Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2008). Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of

mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness-

based stress reduction program. Journal of behavioral medicine, 31, 23-33.

doi:10.1007/s10865-007-9130-7

Carson, J. W., Carson, K. M., Gil, K. M., & Baucom, D. H. (2004). Mindfulness-based

relationship enhancement. Behavior Therapy, 35, 471-494. doi:10.1016/S0005-

7894(04)80028-5

Delplanque, S., Lavoie, M. E., Hot, P., Silvert, L., & Sequeira, H. (2004). Modulation of

cognitive processing by emotional valence studied through event-related potentials in

humans. Neuroscience letters, 356, 1-4.

Desrosiers, A., Vine, V., Curtiss, J., & Klemanski, D. H. (2014). Observing nonreactively: A

conditional process model linking mindfulness facets, cognitive emotion regulation

strategies, and depression and anxiety symptoms. Journal of affective disorders, 165, 31-

37. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.024

Page 33: Dong Thesis 2015

23

Desrosiers, A., Vine, V., Klemanski, D. H., & Nolen‐Hoeksema, S. (2013). Mindfulness and

emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: common and distinct mechanisms of

action. Depression and anxiety, 30, 654-661.doi:10.1002/da.22124

Evans, D. R., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2011). Why do mindful people worry less?.Cognitive therapy

and research, 35, 505-510. doi:10.1007/s10608-010-9340-0

Flores, L. E., & Berenbaum, H. (2012). Desire for emotional closeness moderates the

effectiveness of the social regulation of emotion. Personality and Individual

Differences, 53, 952-957. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.07.009

Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward

emotional well-being. Psychological science, 13, 172-175. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00431

Greenberg, J., & Meiran, N. (2013). Is mindfulness meditation associated with “feeling

less?” Mindfulness, 1-6. doi:10.1007/s12671-013-0201-2

Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion and emotion regulation. In L. A. Pervin, O. P. John, L. A. Pervin, O.

P. John (Eds.) , Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed.) (pp. 525-552).

New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:

implications for affect, relationships, and well-being .Journal of personality and social

psychology, 85, 348-362.

Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress

reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of psychosomatic research, 57,

35-57, 35-43.

Huang, Y. X., & Luo, Y. J. (2006). Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: an ERP

study. Neuroscience letters, 398, 91-96. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.12.074

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: The program of the stress reduction clinic at the

University of Massachusetts Medical Center. New York: Delta.

Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. M. (Eds.). (2009). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A

transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., & Cuthbert, B.N. (2008). International affective picture system

(IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8.

University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.

Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative and

distracting responses to depressed mood on retrieval of autobiographical

memories. Journal of personality and social psychology, 75, 166-177. doi:10.1037/0022-

3514.75.1.166

Marchand, W. R. (2012). Mindfulness-based stress reduction, mindfulness-based cognitive

therapy, and Zen mediation for depression, anxiety, pain, and psychological

distress. Journal Of Psychiatric Practice, 18, 233-252.

doi:10.1097/01.pra.0000416014.53215.86

Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K. V., Rieffe, C., & Bakker, F. (2007). Trait emotional intelligence,

psychological well‐being and peer‐rated social competence in adolescence. British

Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 263-275. doi:10.1348/026151006X118577

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2008). Emotional intelligence: new ability or eclectic

traits?. American Psychologist, 63, 503-517. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.63.6.503

Page 34: Dong Thesis 2015

24

McLaughlin, K. A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in

depression and anxiety. Behaviour research and therapy, 49(3), 186-193.

doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.006

Moore, S. A., Zoellner, L. A., & Mollenholt, N. (2008). Are expressive suppression and

cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-related symptoms?. Behaviour research and

therapy, 46, 993-1000. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.001

Nyklíček, I., & Kuijpers, K. F. (2008). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction intervention

on psychological well-being and quality of life: Is increased mindfulness indeed the

mechanism? Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 35, 331-340. doi:10.1007/s12160-008-

9030-2

Roberts-Wolfe, D., Sacchet, M., Hastings, E., Roth, H., & Britton, W. (2012). Mindfulness

training alters emotional memory recall compared to active controls: support for an

emotional information processing model of mindfulness. Frontiers in human

neuroscience, 6, 15. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00015

Segerstrom, S. C., Castañeda, J. O., & Spencer, T. E. (2003). Optimism effects on cellular

immunity: testing the affective and persistence models. Personality and individual

differences, 35, 1615-1624. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00384-7

Schutte, N. S., & Malouff, J. M. (2011). Emotional intelligence mediates the relationship

between mindfulness and subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences,

50, 1116-1119. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.01.037

Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of

mindfulness. Journal of clinical psychology, 62, 373-386. doi:10.1002/jclp.20237

Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior

research methods, instruments, & computers, 31, 137-149. doi:10.3758/BF03207704

Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective

on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210. doi:10.1037/0033-

2909.103.2.193

Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: separating fact

from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 21-27. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.21

Taylor, V. A., Grant, J., Daneault, V., Scavone, G., Breton, E., Roffe-Vidal, S., & Beauregard, M.

(2011). Impact of mindfulness on the neural responses to emotional pictures in

experienced and beginner meditators. Neuroimage, 57, 1524-1533.

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.001

Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A.

(2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based

cognitive therapy. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 68, 615-623.

doi:10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.615

Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A

psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247-259.

doi:10.1023/A:1023910315561

Veit, C. T., & Ware, J. E. (1983). The structure of psychological distress and well-being in

general populations. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 51, 730-742.

doi:10.1037/0022-006X.51.5.730

Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological

bulletin, 134, 163-206. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163

Page 35: Dong Thesis 2015

25

Wang, Y., & Kong, F. (2014). The role of emotional intelligence in the impact of mindfulness on

life satisfaction and mental distress. Social Indicators Research, 116, 843-852.

doi:10.1007/s11205-013-0327-6

Page 36: Dong Thesis 2015

26

Appendix

Mindfulness training script

“This meditation will instantly reduce the anxiety and fear that you are feeling right now.

Make sure you are sitting comfortably, resting your hands gently on your legs, or whatever

position that’s most comfortable for you. If you like, allow your eyes to gently close or focus

them on an object in the room. Just let yourself become as comfortable and relaxed as possible.

As you begin to settle in to this meditative state, focus your attention on your breathe. Feel the

air move through your nasals as you simply let your breathe continue to flow, in and out. Ever so

naturally. As you breathe, you may want to also turn your attention to the air in your nose, as you

slowly push your belly in and out along with your breathe. Just gently focus on this now… As

you are relaxed, feel so pleasant and comfortable, soon you’ll begin to speak some simple

montras that will instantly help to calm your fear, anxiety or sense of panic… when you begin,

you may have felt some uncomfortable feelings. You may feel some of these uncomfortable

feelings still, perhaps a tightness in your chest or quicken up your heart beat. As you continue

through this session, you will feel your heart rate settle in a wonderful sense of inner calm will

slowly come over to you. As you hear these montras, slowly repeat them to yourself, one by one.

You can say them in your mind, or if it helps, repeat them out loud, whichever is best for you.

Dedicate one to full relaxed breathe to each montra and feel the powerful weight of each word as

you repeat them. Continue to feel the air of your breathe, gently breathe in and out as we begin.

Now either silently or out load, repeat after me. “As I breathe in and out, I am aware of my

breathe… I am aware of my heart beat… I am aware of my body… As I breathe in and out, I am

aware of the anxiety within me…. I am aware of my fears… I am aware of any tightness in my

chest…As I breathe in and out, I am aware of my negative thoughts…Now I calm my negative

thoughts…Now I release my negative thoughts…As I breathe in and out, I calm my anxiety, I

calm my fear, I release the tightness in my chest…As I breathe in and out, I relax my minds, I

relax my body, I slow down my thoughts…As I breathe in and out, I release my fears.. I become

free with my anxiety, I become free of suffering… I find peace…As I breathe in and out, I let go,

I let go…I let go…” Very good, you should now feel a wonderful sense of calm throughout your

body and in your mind. You are now liberated and free. If you like you may listen once again, to

tell yourself this montras and reinforce them. But for now, take a few more moments to breathe

in and out, and relax in this moment of peace and stillness.”