39

Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

Running Head: AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 1

Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles?

An Exploration on the Effects of Culture, Acceptance, and Social Anxiety

Chris Wong

Haverford College

Page 2: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 2

Abstract

Past research had indicated that East Asians are more likely than their Caucasian

counterparts to suffer from high levels of social anxiety, but do not experience the negative

consequences of experienced social anxiety, such as depression and impairment in performance,

to the same extent. The current study explores the differences in the manifestation of social

anxiety between Caucasians and East Asians living in the United States or Canada. We

anticipated that East Asians and Caucasians would differ in their levels of acceptance of

experienced social anxiety, which would moderate the effects of social anxiety upon “negative

outcomes”, including impairment, depression, and interpersonal problems. Consistent with

expectations, it was found that East Asian participants suffered from higher levels of social

anxiety, but inconsistent with expectations, they had a lower acceptance of anxiety that their

Caucasian counterparts. Also consistent with the hypothesis, acceptance moderated the

relationship between social anxiety and negative outcomes including depression and

interpersonal problems. Acceptance did not moderate the relationship between social anxiety and

impairment. It was also found that despite suffering higher levels of social anxiety, East Asian

participants were not significantly more depressed than the Caucasian participants. The

implications of those findings for both cross cultural psychology and acceptance-based

treatments are discussed.

Keywords: social anxiety, cross cultural differences, acceptance

Page 3: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 3

Introduction

Humans are social animals, meaning that the desire to form emotional connections is a

fundamental human motivation (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). This makes sense on an

evolutionary level, since our ancestors would have relied on their companions for protection

from predators and gathering more food. A lack of companions for an early human may have

meant death from predation or starvation, making social interactions and relationships with one’s

group quite literally a matter of life or death (Van Vugt & Schaller, 2008). The experience of

social anxiety can be seen as a trait that developed in our ancestors as they navigated complex

social relationships. Even though in modern day society companionship is no longer quite as

vital for human survival, experiencing social anxiety is still quite a common phenomenon.

Experienced social anxiety is a major component for Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD).

SAD is defined as the individual having a persistent fear of social interactions, usually due to

fear of judgement from others (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In the United States, it

is estimated that 13% of the population suffers from SAD at some point in their lives (Kessler et

al., 1994), and SAD is often associated with increased suicide ideation and a higher chances of

being financially dependent upon others. Roughly 69% of SAD patients are diagnosed with

comorbid disorders (Schneier, Johnson, Hornig, Liebowitz, & Weissman, 1992). Furthermore,

studies have shown that individuals who meet criteria for SAD reported significantly lower

levels of satisfaction on all domains of life, even the domains that have no direct connection to

social interactions (Eng, Coles, Heimberg, & Safren, 2005). Thus it can be concluded that SAD

has a significantly harmful impact which may actually extend beyond its immediate symptoms.

The way experienced social anxiety is connected to a full-fledged SAD diagnosis is often

hypothesized to be via a self-fulfilling cycle (Rapee & Heimberg, 1997). One prevailing

Page 4: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 4

cognitive model of social anxiety suggests that individuals who with high levels of social anxiety

usually have negative beliefs of themselves and their social skills. As such, they seek to escape

social situations and lower their anxiety levels by disengaging from social interactions, either by

engaging in safety behaviors, defined as coping behaviors used to reduce anxiety (Wells et al.,

1995), or by physically removing themselves from the situation. However, safety behaviors often

have the side effect of distracting the individual from the social interaction, and may give the

impression that the individual is aloof and uninterested. This may, in turn, make others less

enthusiastic about spending time with the anxious individual, thus reinforcing their social

anxieties and negative self-beliefs (Rapee & Heimberg, 1997; Wells et al., 1995).

Many interpersonal model of the formation of psychopathology and distress have been

proposed, either specifically directed at SAD (Alden & Taylor, 2004; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997)

or examining more generalized issues such as interpersonal sensitivity and social rejection

(Downey, Freitas, Michaelis, & Khouri, 1998; Madon, Willard, Guyll, & Scherr, 2011; Nelson &

Klutas, 2000). Most of these models emphasize similar scenarios of a downwards spiral

generated by a self-fulfilling prophecy, showing how SAD manages to generate significant levels

of distress and impairment in individuals.

Social Anxiety Across Culture

A study done in 1997 by Okazaki on a Asian American and Euro-American college

students demonstrated that when compared to Euro-American participants, Asian American

participants report significantly higher levels of experienced social anxiety and distress, even

after controlling for various other distress factors (Okazaki, 1997). Okazaki’s findings were

replicated in 2010 by Schreier in a large scale cross cultural study including college students

from nine different countries (Schreier et al., 2010). Schreier’s results show that East Asian

Page 5: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 5

college students report the highest mean levels of social anxiety amongst all the cultural groups

included in the study.

Given how East Asians seem to experience higher social anxiety, one would imagine that

a formal diagnosis of SAD would have a high prevalence within East Asian countries.

Unexpectedly, literature shows that it is not the case. Multiple meta-analyses on anxiety disorder

have concluded that East Asian cultures have the lowest prevalence for anxiety disorders,

including SAD, while North American cultures tend to have the highest prevalence of anxiety

disorders (Remes, Brayne, van der Linde, & Lafortune, 2016; Wancata, Fridl, & Friedrich, 2009;

Wittchen & Fehm, 2003). And while it is can be hypothesized that cultural stigmatization against

seeking mental help may play a part in this phenomena, there is evidence showing that East

Asian individuals may suffer less impairment from experiencing high levels of social anxiety

when compared to their North American counterparts.

In 2002, Okazaki conducted another study showing that for Asian American participants,

but not white participants, experiencing a high level of social anxiety did not negatively

influence how they performed on a social task (Okazaki, Liu, Longworth, & Minn, 2002). In this

study, 40 Asian American and 40 white participants were given trait anxiety and experienced

social anxiety measures to complete before and after a 3-minute public speaking task. The

speaking task was recorded and subsequently coded for behaviors such as gaze avoidance,

fidgeting, and prolonged silence. Although Asian American participants scored significantly

higher on both trait anxiety and experienced social anxiety measures when compared to white

participants, there were no significant group differences in the behavioral coding for the public

speaking task. Furthermore, there was a significant positive correlation between self-reported

Page 6: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 6

anxious emotions and impaired behavior for white participants exclusively (Okazaki et al.,

2002).

Assuming that these results are generalizable to demographics beyond undergraduate

students, this implies that for East Asian individuals either experienced social anxiety does not

significantly impair their self-perception, or that a negative self-perception does not interfere

with their social performance. Either way, the self-fulfilling prophecy model for SAD seems to

be less applicable for East Asian individuals, and reports of high levels of social distress does not

seem to correlate with social impairment for East Asian individuals. This reveals a lack of

nuance in the current literature on SAD as it manifests across different cultures. A closer look

into the cultural differences of SAD is warranted to shed more light on this phenomenon.

Cultural Differences in Social Norms and Interactions

There are many reasons proposed for the higher level of experienced social anxiety of

participants from East Asian cultures compared to others. The primary one is the emphasis

placed upon social relationships in collectivistic cultures such as Japan, China, and most other

East Asian Cultures. Collectivism is defined as valuing group goals and group cohesion over

personal goals and opinions, while individualism features a higher value placed upon personal

independence (Shulruf, Hattie, & Dixon, 2007). As such, individuals from collectivistic cultures

are more likely to define themselves by their relationship with others and the context in which

they are placed, while individuals from an individualistic culture are more likely to define

themselves by inherent traits that are context-independent.

In support of the theory that East Asian individuals define themselves primarily via social

context, a cross-cultural study asked participants from the United States and Japan to come up

with 40 statements describing themselves, the first 20 statements without context (“I am ____”)

Page 7: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 7

and the second 20 statements when given a context (“At home, I am ____”). The results show

when compared to US participants, Japanese participants more often defined themselves via

social roles and behavioral attributes when not given a context (“I am a student”; “I am a son.”),

and only turn to more abstract intrinsic descriptors (“I am diligent”; “I am lazy”) when given a

context (Cousins, 1989).

Cousins theorize that these results show that Japanese individuals define their

individuality within social contexts, as opposed to US individuals who define themselves

independent of context and relationships; Hence while US individuals tend to act in accordance

with their self-concepts independently of the social situation (“I am shy, hence I am shy in all

situations”), Japanese individuals vary their behavior in accordance with the social context (“In

school I am shy and subdued. With friends I am outgoing. With family I am…”; (Cousins, 1989).

These findings are compatible with theories that state East Asian individuals are more likely to

adapt to the social situation by “reading the air” and adjusting their behavior accordingly

(Kitayama & Markus, 1991; Lau, Wang, Fung, & Namikoshi, 2014).

So in contrast to Western culture, where the explicit expression of one’s thoughts and

desires, or the explicit request for what one wants from others, is seen as authentic and

appropriate, those from East Asian cultures would typically view such expressions as being

childish and immature (Kitayama & Markus, 1991). Rather, the “correct” way to interact with

friends or acquaintance, is to gain knowledge the goals, needs, and preferences of the other

through observation and to fulfill this goals without the other explicitly requesting a favor. In

other words, anytime one is in a position to satisfy any of those goals, needs, or preferences, one

would do so; this holds the underlying expectation that one’s own goals would be in turn fulfilled

by the friend or acquaintance in time (Kitayama & Markus, 1991).

Page 8: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 8

In conclusion, it is theorized that in East Asian cultures social interactions operates via

mutual monitoring to ensure that each party member is contributing a roughly equal amount to

the others (Kitayama & Markus, 1991). The importance of mutual monitoring in East Asian

cultures have been supported in a study where participants from Japan and the United States

were placed into 3-person task groups. It was found that when the group lacked opportunities for

mutual monitoring and sanctioning, Japanese participants would exit from the group at a

significantly higher rate than their US counterparts (Yamagishi, 1988). These findings are in line

with Kitayama and Markus’ model of social interaction in East Asian cultures: for East Asian

individuals, mutual monitoring is vital to the maintenance of a healthy relationship. Without an

opportunity to monitor others, not only would the individual be unable to properly engage in

social interactions, they would also run the risk of sinking resources into the relationship with no

guarantee of later returns (Kitayama & Markus, 1991).

One consequence of the East Asian style of social interaction is low relational mobility.

Relational mobility as a measure was first operationalized in 2007, and is used to refer to the

perceived difficulty of moving between social groups (Yuki et al., 2007). One study on relational

mobility and rejection sensitivity showed that as societies, Japan have less social mobility than

the United States. In turn, this lack of social mobility correlated to Japanese participants being

much more sensitive to rejection (Sato, Yuki, & Norasakkunkit, 2014; Triandis, 1989). Sato

concluded that if an individual in Japan alienates their social group, it would be much more

difficult for them to find a new social group to replace their old one, which would explain why

Japanese participants report higher levels of rejection sensitivity and rejection based anxiety

(Sato et al., 2014).

Page 9: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 9

Furthermore, in 1989, before relational mobility was properly defined and

operationalized, a cross-cultural meta-analysis done by Henry Triandis observed that

collectivistic cultures have higher levels of outgroup bias resulting in less mobility between

social groups (Triandis, 1989). Triandis supported his statement with data showing that

collectivistic individuals are generally worse at meeting strangers than individualistic

individuals, that the manipulation and exploitation of outgroups are particularly common in

collectivistic cultures, and that collectivistic groups are more inclined to compete against out-

groups even when competitiveness is shown to be mutually counterproductive (Triandis, 1989).

All of this points to increased bias against out-group members in a collectivistic society, which

would restrict relational mobility.

Given all the reasons listed in this section it would be easy to see why East Asian

individuals would report higher levels of experienced social anxiety. Not only do social context

play a major part in an individual’s self-definition (Cousins, 1989), they also require high levels

of emotional sensitivity and control, since one is always monitoring others while also being

monitored by one’s peers. Furthermore, East Asian individuals would have a higher level of sunk

cost into their social circles compared to Western individuals due to the system of mutual goal

fulfillment (Kitayama & Markus, 1991) and the lower relational mobility in collectivistic

cultures (Sato et al., 2014; Triandis, 1989).

Cultural Protective Factors against SAD

The last section attempted to explain why East Asians report higher amounts of

experienced social anxiety than their Western counterparts. This section explores why

individuals from East Asian cultures maintain a significantly lower incidence of SAD, as well as

an uninfluenced social performance while experiencing high levels of social anxiety and distress.

Page 10: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 10

One area that has been often explored on this topic is the differences in the coping mechanisms

employed by East Asian individuals. While on a surface level, East Asians seem to employ

similar coping mechanisms to stress as Western individuals, studies have shown that these

“similar” mechanisms often lead to extremely different outcomes.

One coping mechanism that is shared between Eastern and Western cultures is emotional

suppression. Emotional suppression has been shown in Western cultures to carry with it

significant negative social and psychological effects, including disrupting communications with

others, decreased experience of positive emotions, and increased experience of negative

emotions (Butler et al., 2003; Gross & Levenson, 1997). However, further studies have shown

that these negative outcomes are muted or non-existent for individuals from an East Asian

background. A study has shown that for women with bicultural values (in this case, Western

values and East Asian values) emotional suppression does not lead to a decrease in

responsiveness in a conversation, while women who identified with European values had

significantly decreased responsiveness while suppressing emotions (Butler, Lee, & Gross, 2007).

Studies have also shown that East Asian individuals engaged in emotional suppression

not only avoid the social consequences of suppression that plague their European counterparts,

they may also circumvent the physiological consequences as well. A study done in 2013 showed

that while presented with unpleasant stimuli and instructed to engage in emotional suppression,

EEG scans show that European individuals maintain a similar level of arousal to the European

non-suppression group. On the other hand, East Asian individuals who engaged in emotional

suppression displayed a significant decrease in arousal when compared to the East Asian non-

suppression group (Murata, Moser, & Kitayama, 2013). These findings show that when East

Asian individuals engage in emotional suppression, not only do they have a much lower level of

Page 11: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 11

impairment compared to their European counterparts, they also manage to decrease their levels

of physiological arousal, something that Euro-American individuals fail to do.

However, emotional suppression is hardly the most effective tool for reducing one’s

experienced anxiety levels. Acceptance of one’s internal experiences is another key tool to

consider. In a study done by Gong in 2016, 82 Chinese students were given a job interview task

with different instructions for anxiety reducing strategies (including acceptance and

suppression). All participants also filled out self-reported anxiety measures at four points of time

before and after the interview task. The results show that acceptance was one of the most

effective strategies at reducing experienced anxiety at all points of measurement, and the most

effective at reducing anxious behavior during the filmed interview task (Gong, Li, Zhang, &

Rost, 2016). These findings demonstrate acceptance’s superior effectiveness at reducing both

experienced anxiety and anxious behavior when compared to emotional suppression, at least for

East Asian individuals.

Furthermore, acceptance on a more generalized level may also enhance other emotional

regulation strategies as well. For example, another cross cultural study found that within a

cultural context that accepts and normalizes emotional suppression, emotional suppression does

not correlate to negative social or psychological outcomes at all (Soto, Perez, Kim, Lee, &

Minnick, 2011). Hence, it could be theorized that acceptance not only functions as an anxiety

reducing strategy in of itself, it can also enhance the positive effects of other coping mechanisms

as well. However, current literature is lacking in regards to how prevalent acceptance is in

different cultures, as well as how acceptance of social anxiety disorders would influence

negatives outcomes from high social anxiety. Many theories have proposed the connections

between acceptance and positive outcomes, and between acceptance and culture, but there is a

Page 12: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 12

lack of empirical data specifically mapping out the relationship between the three factors. In the

following sections the current literature on culture, acceptance, and negative outcomes would be

explored.

Acceptance’s Role in Anxiety Reduction

Before delving any deeper into the literature on acceptance, the term “acceptance” must

be defined. For the purpose of this study there’s two major areas of acceptance: Acceptance of

others and self-acceptance. Acceptance of others has been defined as something extremely

similar to a therapeutic acceptance, which is that of “tolerance of all aspects of the client, as

manifested by the adoption of a ‘baseline’ attitude of consistent, genuine, noncritical interest”

(Block-Lerner, Wulfert, & Moses, 2009). Of course, a more generalized definition of the

acceptance of others would not require the amount of interest and engagement required in an

accepting therapeutic relationship, but the general principles of tolerance and a “baseline” level

of interest would still hold. Acceptance of self, which in this case also includes acceptance of

one’s own emotions, has been defined in modern therapy as “allowing thoughts to come and go

without struggling with them”(Hayes & Lillis, 2012). For the purposes of this paper, we will

only be discussing self-acceptance.

The central role that acceptance may play upon anxiety reduction is highlighted in

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT), which was developed by Steven Hayes as part of

the “third wave” behavioral treatments for anxiety and mood disorders. One of the treatment

components of ACT involves having patients observe and accept their anxieties and fears in

order to live a valued life(Hayes & Lillis, 2012). According to Hayes, anxiety itself is not what

causes anxiety disorders. Instead, it is the patient’s own rejection of anxiety and constant struggle

Page 13: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 13

to “think the right thoughts” that causes most of the impairment and distress seen in clinical

examples of anxiety disorders.

A common metaphor used by ACT proponents to describe anxiety this dynamic is the

“tug of war” metaphor, in which the anxious individual is engaging in a never ending tug of war

with their own anxiety. In the metaphor, many clinically anxious individuals strain continuously

against their own anxious feelings, hoping to win it and then move on with their live in the

absence of anxiety. However, the tug of war is fundamentally unwinnable, and most anxious

individuals find themselves in an exhausting stalemate with their own anxiety while life moves

on without them, provoking even more distress within them (Hayes & Lillis, 2012).

With that metaphor in mind, ACT proposes to its patients that they should “let go of the

rope” and allow anxiety to run its course without fighting it. ACT claims that by accepting

anxiety with willingness, pathologically anxious individuals can instead focus upon finding what

they truly value in life, and commit themselves to living a life in accordance to those values

rather than wasting time in the fight against anxiety (Hayes & Lillis, 2012). Studies have shown

that ACT is efficacious in treating common mental disorders (Hacker, Stone, & MacBeth, 2016),

with some studies yielding similar success rates as Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT), which

is held as the “Golden Standard” for treating anxiety disorders (Arch et al., 2012). While ACT’s

success in treating anxiety disorders cannot be fully attributed to the effects of self-acceptance, it

remains the case that self-acceptance is one of the cornerstones of ACT therapy and is likely be a

crucial element in ACT’s success.

ACT is not the first psychological treatment to incorporate acceptance. A meta-analysis

looking at various Western psychological treatments concluded that many of the approaches,

such as Freud’s psychoanalysis and modern cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) include aspects

Page 14: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 14

of acceptance in its theory (Block-Lerner et al., 2009). Furthermore, a few other therapies such

as dialectal behavioral therapy (DBT) and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT)

explicitly given acceptance a position of high import within their theories (Block-Lerner et al.,

2009). Hence, acceptance can be assumed to be a major component in reducing anxiety and

lessening negative life outcomes.

Based on the literature upon both acceptance and SAD, it may be that the acceptance of

social anxiety can prevent the self-fulfilling prophecy that characterizes many cases of SAD. If

someone accepts that social anxiety is natural, and that there is nothing abnormal in experiencing

social anxiety, their experienced distress would not necessarily translate into negative self-

concept, nor would it necessarily cause any impairment to their social performance.

Functionally, by accepting their experienced social anxiety, the individual would prevent the

self-fulfilling prophecy from happening, and prevent themselves from being impaired by social

anxiety. In the next section the focus will return to East Asian society, and whether or not self-

acceptance of experienced social anxiety can be seen as a key protective factor for East Asian

individuals against SAD.

Acceptance of Social Anxiety in East Asian Society

The concept of acceptance has appeared many times throughout East Asian cultures,

from the way Buddhism emphasizes acceptance and transcendence as a mean to end suffering

(David, Lynn, & Das, 2013), to this quote from the Taoist work Zhuangzi: The Basic Writings:

“To recognize what can’t be helped and accept it calmly as if it were fate (ming) – only a man of

virtue can manage this.” (Zhuangzi & Watson, 2003). It can be seen that the acceptance of the

unchangeable aspects of one’s situation and environment is seen as a desirable thing and has a

major role in shaping East Asian culture through its religions and philosophies. In addition to

Page 15: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 15

that, acceptance may fill in a central role for developing a protective buffer against anxiety

disorders for East Asian individuals.

Acceptance of social anxiety may be the natural consequence of how social interaction is

structured in East Asian collectivistic society. As mentioned above, the general consensus within

cross cultural studies agree that social cohesion is much more important in East Asian societies

compared to Western societies (Shulruf et al., 2007), while other studies have shown that the

lack of relational mobility means that individuals from an East Asian country would have a

harder time replacing relationships they lose (Sato et al., 2014; Triandis, 1989).

Furthermore, due to the system of mutual monitoring, the “proper” method of social

interactions within East Asian society is far more observation-based, then expression-based. This

means that in a when someone speaks to an acquaintance in East Asian cultures, the

responsibility usually lies upon the listeners to observe the speaker and understand what he/she

wants. This stands in contradiction to most Western societies, in which it is the speaker’s

responsibility to express himself/herself in a way that can be easily understood by the listener

(Kitayama & Markus, 1991). Due to this heavy reliance upon observation rather than expression,

smooth social exchanges between individuals in an East Asian culture would rely more heavily

upon familiarity between the interacting individuals when compared to those in Western

cultures. This reliance may be one of the underlying mechanisms for the lack of relational

mobility, as any social interactions between strangers would be highly rigid and uncomfortable

as neither party is familiar with the mannerisms of the other.

While a well maintained social network is high valued across cultures, it may be that East

Asian individuals are under much more stress to maintain their social networks due to how hard

it is to replace if lost. As such, it may be seen as normal to feel high levels of social anxiety in an

Page 16: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 16

East Asian culture, just as it would be seen as normal to feel high levels of anxiety while signing

a house mortgage in a Western culture. If it is true that East Asian individuals see experienced

social anxiety as normal, it would follow these experiences would not translate into negative

beliefs about the self, stopping the self-fulfilling prophecy of SAD before it even starts.

Furthermore, in East Asian society where experienced social anxiety is highly prevalent,

it may be that individuals would have an easier time finding someone in their social circle to

relate to in terms of experiencing social anxiety, and enjoy more social support from their friends

and family. Moreoever, since experienced social anxiety is more normative within East Asian

society, it would logically follow that society at large would be more accepting of experiencing

social anxiety. Given this, socially anxious individuals would find social support readily

assessable, preventing them from feeling guilty about being socially anxious and forming a

protective buffer against SAD.

Current Study

The current study seeks to examine a simple model of the interactions between culture,

experienced social anxiety, acceptance of experienced social anxiety, and negative outcomes

such as depression, high interpersonal problems, and high impairment (As illustrated in Figure

1). Specifically, this model predicts that the acceptance of social anxiety would moderate the

relationship between social anxiety and impairment, interpersonal problems, and depression,

while culture will predict acceptance of social anxiety.

Three hypotheses will be tested:

(1) Participants from East Asian cultures will be more accepting of social anxiety compared

to participants from Euro-American cultures.

Page 17: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 17

(2) Experienced social anxiety will predict higher levels of “negative outcomes”, including

impairment, depression, and interpersonal problems.

(3) Acceptance of social anxiety will moderate the relationship between experienced social

anxiety and negative outcomes such that higher levels of acceptance would weaken this

association.

Methods

Procedure & Participants

Participants were recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk’s service and asked to

complete a set of self-report measures administered using Qualtrics software. The sample

included 670 adults living in the United States or Canada. 5 participants were then excluded from

the final analysis due to completing the survey in less than 120 seconds (2 minutes). While

attention check items were implemented upon launch of the survey, they were soon found to be

of questionable validity, and were quickly excluded from the study. All participants were

compensated 50 cents upon completion of the survey.

Measures

Social Interaction and Anxiety Scale (SIAS). The SIAS is a 20 item scale measuring

social anxiety. Items included statements such as “When mixing socially, I am uncomfortable”,

which participants rate on a Likert scale of 0 (“Not at all”) to 4 (“Extremely characteristic”)

(Mattick & Clarke, 1998). In 1997, a review upon the validity of SIAS was conducted. The

results find that SIAS has a high sensitivity to SAD and, unlike many other measures for social

anxiety, it can differentiate between SAD and other anxiety disorders such as agoraphobia and

panic disorder (Brown et al., 1997). Furthermore, the SIAS has high internal consistency

Page 18: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 18

(Cronbach’s alpha > 0.90) and a high test-retest reliability (r > 0.91) (Mattick & Clarke, 1998;

Peters, Sunderland, Andrews, Rapee, & Mattick, 2012).

Social Anxiety- Acceptance and Action Questioniare (SA-AAQ). The SA-AAQ is a 19

item scale including statements such as “Despite feeling socially anxious at times, I am in control

of my life”; which participants on a Likert scale between 1 (“Never true”) to 7 (“Always true”).

The measure was developed in 2010 by MacKenzie and Kocovski, and is intended to measure

acceptance of social anxiety symptoms. Upon creation of the measure, the SA-AAQ was also

tested for internal reliability and demonstrated a reasonable high level of reliability (Cronbach’s

alpha = 0.94), (MacKenzie & Kocovski, 2010).

Furthermore, the SA-AAQ is a valid measure for acceptance of social anxiety symptoms,

and is far more sensitive to acceptance of social anxiety than most other self-acceptance

measures. Analysis also showed that the SA-AAQ had a much lower correlation to measures of

social anxiety compared to the correlations within the various social anxiety measures, showing

that the SA-AAQ is not merely a measure of social anxiety (MacKenzie & Kocovski, 2010).

Sheehan Disability Scale (SDS) The SDS was developed by Sheehan and is meant to

measure an individual’s impairment in terms of work, social, and family life (Sheehan, 1983).

Partiticpants are asked to rate on a scale of 0-10 how much their symptoms have disrupted their

work/ academics, social life, and family life. Two additional more items ask how many days in

the last week was the person underproductive or absent from school or work due to their

symptoms.

While originally designed to measure impairment levels for psychiatric disorders such as

SAD, depression, panic disorder, and alcohol dependence (Sheehan, 1983), SDS have also been

tested to measure impairments levels from other disorders, such as bipolar disorder (Arbuckle et

Page 19: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 19

al., 2009), ADHD (Coles, Coon, DeMuro, McLeod, & Gnanasakthy, 2014), and pathological

gambling (Hodgins, 2013). Within those studies, SDS has shown a high level of internal

reliability (Cronbach’s = 0.79 - 0.91) (Coles et al., 2014).

Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP) There are three versions of the IIP: The full

length IIP, with 127 items, the IIP-64, with 64 items, and the IIP-32, with 32 items. All of the

three includes items such as “It is hard for me to feel close to others” and “It is hard for me to

give a gift to another” that participants would have to rate on a Likert scale between 0 (“Not at

all”) to 4 (“Extremely”)(Alden & Taylor, 2004; Horowitz, Rosenberg, Baer, Ureño, &

Villaseñor, 1988; Soldz, Budman, Demby, & Merry, 1995) The current study would be using the

IIP-32.

Interpersonal problems as measured by the IIP-32 are divided into 8 subscales, with 4

items attributed to each subscale: (1) Domineering/ Controlling (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.73); (2)

Vindictive/ Self-centered (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.77); (3) Cold/ Distance (Cronbach’s alpha =

0.85); (4) Socially inhibited (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.86); (5) Nonassertive (Cronbach’s alpha =

0.87); (6) Exploitable (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81); (7) Overly Nurturing (Cronbach’s alpha =

0.75); and (8) Intrusive/ Needy (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.70) (Hopwood, Pincus, DeMoor, &

Koonce, 2008).

In 2008 Hopwood et al. conducted a brief study comparing the effectiveness of IIP-32

when compared to the IIP-64, a longer version of the IIP, in American college students. Apart

from a lower internal consistency, which was expected due to the brevity of the scales, the IIP-32

performed just as well as the IIP-64 when it came to validity and reliability in college students

(Hopwood et al., 2008).

Page 20: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 20

Beck’s Depresion Inventory 2nd Edition (BDI-II). The Beck’s Depression Inventory

(BDI) was first developed in 1961, with the second edition (BDI-II) being published in 1971

(Beck, Steer, & Carbin, 1988). The second edition includes 21 items measuring various aspects

of depression, including negative affect, absence of positive affect, and anhedonia. In 1988, Beck

conducted a meta-analysis upon the BDI and the BDI-II, concluding that the BDI-II has a high

validity, reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.73-0.92), and sensitivity towards differentiating

psychiatric and non-psychiatric patients (Beck et al., 1988).

However, Beck did find that some demographic characteristics, such as gender, race, and

education, significantly influence the intensity of depression as measured by the BDI-II (Beck et

al., 1988). However, these demographic influences over BDI’s results have been quite steady

throughout the usage of the BDI, showing that the differences in results should be interpreted as

gender and cultural differences in experienced depression, rather than any failings on the part of

the measure itself (Beck et al., 1988).

Results

Preliminary Analysis

The final sample included 296 Caucasian participants (130 Male; 171 Female) and 369

East Asian participants (198 Male; 171 Female). Within the East Asian participants, there were

143 first generation immigrants, and 226 second generation immigrants. The mean age of the

entire final participant sample was 35 years old, the mean age of Caucasian participants were 40

years old, and the mean age of the East Asian participants were 30 years old.

Table 1 shows the means, SD, and range for all variables measured. The data indicate that

a high amount of the participants would qualify for some level of social phobia diagnosis (32 <

SIAS score < 42), and some of them would also be deemed as suffering from traditional social

Page 21: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 21

anxiety (SIAS score > 42). Most of the participants did not score high on the BDI-II, although

there 103 of the participants scored within the range of “major depression” in the BDI-II (29<

BDI score < 63).

A series of t-tests was conducted between the Caucasian and East Asian participants on

the SIAS, SDS, IIP-32, and BDI-II scores (See Table 2). As expected, East Asians (M = 35.75,

SD = 17.91) scored higher than Caucasians (M = 31.84, SD = 20.65) on our measure of social

anxiety, t (663) = -2.571, p < 0.01. Unexpectedly, the East Asian group (SDS: M = 12.01, SD =

7.19; IIP: M = 50.33, SD = 26.73) experienced higher disability and interpersonal problems than

the Caucasian (SDS: M = 9.81, SD = 7.75; IIP: M = 40.15, SD = 29.69), SDS: t (661) = -3.777, p

< 0.01; IIP-32 t (663) = -5.103, p < 0.01. There was no significant difference in depression

between the two groups. Within the East Asian sample, years stayed in the US or Canada did not

affect the relationship between the SIAS score and any of the output variables (SDS, IIP-32,

BDI-II).

Partial Correlation between Culture and Acceptance

We used a partial correlation to determine if culture predicted of SA-AAQ scores,

controlling for baseline levels of social anxiety. Unexpectedly, being from an East Asian culture

was negatively correlated with SA-AAQ score (r = -0.193, n = 665, p < 0.01).

Main effect for Social Anxiety and Moderation effect of Acceptance

We used a multiple regression framework to find out whether acceptance of social

anxiety moderated the relationship between social anxiety and negative outcomes. Our model

included the main effects of social anxiety and acceptance of social anxiety, as well as an

interaction term, which was calculated by centering the SIAS and SA-AAQ scores and creating

Page 22: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 22

the cross product. This main effects and the cross product was then entered into separate

regression models for the SDS, SDS social scale, IIP-32, and BDI-II.

For the SDS, the results indicated that the main effects of the two predictors had

significant effects upon SDS (∆R^2 = 0.703, ∆F (2, 632) = 0.748, p = 0.478), with both social

anxiety (B= 0.271, r = 0.695, p <0.01) and acceptance of social anxiety (B= -0.471, p < 0.01)

having a significant effect. However, contrary to expectations, the interaction variable led to no

significant variance (∆R^2 = 0.00, ∆F (1, 631) = 0.505, B= 0.00, t = -0.71, p = 0.478), with the

similar results being found for the SDS social scale (∆R^2 = 0.00, ∆F (1, 627) = 0.075, B = -

4.359E-005, t = -0.274, p = 0.784). (For stats on SDS, see Table 3)

For interpersonal problems, the main effect of the two predictors had a significant effect

(∆R^2 = 0.834, ∆F (2, 636) = 726.12, p < 0.01), with both social anxiety (B = 10.32, r = 0.757, p

< 0.01) and acceptance of social anxiety (B = -0.915, r = 0.702, p < 0.01) having a significant

effect. While the interaction between the two predictors led to significant variance for the IIP-32

scores (∆R^2 = 0.004, ∆F (1, 635) = 7.948, B= 0.004, t = 2.819, p = 0.005), the direction of the

moderation effect was opposite to what was hypothesized (Chart 1) (Stats on Table 4)

For the BDI-II scores, the main effect of the two predictors had a significant effect (∆R^2

= 0.337, ∆F (2, 636) = 161.559, p <0.01), with both social anxiety (B = 0.349, r = 0.514, p <

0.01) and acceptance of social anxiety (B = -0.347, r = 0.533 , p < 0.01) having a significant

effect on depression. The interaction effect between the two predictors also led to significant

variance in the theorized direction for the BDI scores (∆R^2 = 0.09, ∆F (1,635) = 8.455, B= -

0.96, t = -2.908, p = 0.04) (Chart 2) (Stats on Table 5)

Discussion

Main Findings

Page 23: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 23

We found mixed support for our hypotheses. Contrary to what was hypothesized, East

Asian participants demonstrated significantly less acceptance of social anxiety compared to their

Caucasian counterparts. As expected, social anxiety was associated with higher impairment,

interpersonal problems, and depression for both Caucasian and East Asian participants, and

acceptance was associated with less severity on all of these outcomes. Also as expected,

acceptance of social anxiety moderated the relationship between social anxiety and interpersonal

problems, and the relationship between social anxiety and depression. Acceptance also

moderated the relationship between social anxiety and interpersonal problems, but in an

unanticipated direction. No significant moderation effect was found for the relationship between

social anxiety and impairment.

The finding that East Asian participants were significantly less accepting of experienced

social anxiety than Caucasian participants directly contradicted our first hypothesis. It could be

that East Asians employ other coping strategies in regards to their higher social anxiety, such as

repression. Furthermore, this finding does not contradict the fact that East Asian cultures place

more stress upon acceptance than Western cultures; simply because a culture places more value

in a particular attribute does not mean that individuals from said culture would possess this

attribute at higher levels than those from other cultures.

The finding that experienced social anxiety has a positive correlation with impairment,

interpersonal problems, and depression is congruent with our hypotheses and existing literature,

as is the finding that acceptance moderates the effects of social anxiety upon depression. While

acceptance did have a moderator effect upon interpersonal problems, the moderator effect was

opposite to what we hypothesized. However, it should be noted that the main effect of social

anxiety and acceptance upon interpersonal problems was still significant, and that while

Page 24: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 24

individuals with higher levels of acceptance and higher levels of anxiety experienced a larger

increase in interpersonal problems compared to the non-accepting participants, they still suffer

from lower levels of interpersonal problems.

We failed to find a significant moderation effect for acceptance of social anxiety upon the

relationship between experienced social anxiety and impairment of social life. This finding is

seemingly contradictory to ACT literature, as ACT predicts that acceptance of social anxiety

should alleviate the negative outcomes of social anxiety. However, this may imply that, within

the context of ACT, acceptance of social anxiety may increase global functioning by decreasing

symptoms of comorbid disorders, rather than tackling the primary symptoms first hand.

It may be that acceptance of experienced social anxiety does not directly influence

impairment stemming from social anxiety, but rather lessens the general negative outcomes from

social anxiety by decreasing comorbid symptoms. The finding that acceptance of social anxiety

has a significant moderation effect upon interpersonal problems and depression supports this

view, as acceptance of social anxiety lessened the effects of depression. This explanation is also

congruent with the findings that ACT has a similar rates of treatment success as CBT; it can be

reasonably speculated that the reduction of comorbid symptoms for SAD would entail a certain

degree of success in treatment.

Additional Findings

Our findings that East Asians scored significantly higher on interpersonal problems and

impairment was inconsistent with past research, which suggests that despite significantly higher

levels of self-reported social anxiety, East Asians have similar levels of negative outcomes

compared to their Caucasian counterparts. However, this finding does not directly contradict the

findings and implications of the experiments literature, such as experiments run by Gong, Butler,

Page 25: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 25

and Okazaki (Butler et al., 2007; Gong et al., 2016; Okazaki et al., 2002). All three of these

experiments measured variations on short-term social performances in a specific laboratory task.

In these studies, participant’s performances are rated and coded by raters who have presumably

never met these participants in the past. As such, while these experiments would be able to

indicate how well participants performed social tasks under stress, they may not provide a good

representation of how well the participants would perform when interacting with more long-term

friends and acquaintances while experiencing social anxiety.

Although the data indicates that East Asian participants suffer from a significantly higher

level of social impairment and interpersonal problems than their Caucasian counterparts, it

should also be noted that they did not score significantly different on the BDI. Hence this may

indicate that we should view the past findings in which East Asians were found to have higher

levels of social anxiety and lower SAD diagnoses in a different light. Due the significantly

higher levels of interpersonal problems and impairment reported by our East Asian participants,

it can be speculated that a lack of impairment is not the major reason for a lack of SAD

diagnoses. Instead, our findings imply that while East Asians suffer from a higher level of

impairment and interpersonal problems due to social anxiety, they may not suffer as much

distress, as indicated by the lower depression score while controlling for social anxiety. It may be

that East Asians simply do not feel as distressed by social anxiety, and hence do not feel the need

to seek a diagnosis and treatment.

Strengths and Limitations

The current study is one of the first to look into the effects of self-acceptance of

symptoms as a moderator to impairment. It is also one of the first to look at the cross-cultural

differences in social anxiety and attempt to explain it via acceptance. The findings have shed a

Page 26: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 26

bit of light upon how culture interacts with acceptance of social anxiety, and upon some of the

details of how acceptance alleviates the deleterious effects of social anxiety.

Limitations of this study include the typical issues of online anonymous self-report

studies. Due to the fact that the surveys were filled in online anonymously, it is unclear whether

participants would be completely honest in their responses. Furthermore, as with many self-

report studies, unconscious biases and self-serving biases may have been a confounding factor as

well. Cultural bias was also not controlled for during data analysis, which may have been another

confounding factor.

Implications and Future Research Directions

The findings for this research raises interesting implications in regards to the effects of

acceptance and cross-cultural differences in social anxiety. In terms of acceptance, our findings

for the moderation effect of acceptance would seem to indicate that acceptance as a coping

strategy against social anxiety does not influence direct impairments suffered via social anxiety

itself, but significantly decreases comorbid symptoms such as depression, acting like “damage

control” in regards to stressors.

However, this also raises the problem when one takes into account that acceptance of

social anxiety moderated the effects of social anxiety upon interpersonal problems (albeit in the

opposite direction from our hypothesis) but not the self-reported levels of impairment in social

life. Problems in one’s interpersonal relationship is one of the most salient aspects of an impaired

social life, but our data seems to imply a significant difference between measured by the SDS

social life item, and interpersonal problems, as measured by the IIP-32. A more in depth study

upon the differences between these two measures should be conducted.

Page 27: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 27

Assuming that the difference in moderation effect is not a byproduct of the measures

themselves, these findings may point at future research upon the exact effects of acceptance

based therapy for anxiety disorders. If acceptance as a coping strategy has no significant effect

on direct impairment, but improves global functioning and decreases distress by alleviating

comorbid symptoms, one may wonder what this implies for the application of acceptance-based

therapies in a clinical setting. Future research on the exact effects of acceptance upon anxiety

disorders is needed.

On the cultural front, the data from this study indicates that despite suffering from a

significantly high level of social anxiety, East Asian individuals do not differ significantly from

their Western counterparts in terms of depression. This indicates that while individuals from East

Asian cultures do not have a complete protective buffer as implied by the cross cultural meta-

analyses, there is still a protective factor that lessens the impact of experienced social anxiety

upon depression. Whether this protective factor is only for depression, or can be extended to

other common comorbid symptoms is yet to be seen.

Furthermore, the current study only gathered East Asian immigrants who can read

English as participants, so it would be questionable whether any conclusions and speculations

gathered from the data can be applied for non-immigrants currently living in an East Asian

society. Future studies should aim to explore the cross-cultural effects in terms of long term

distress and impairment in Caucasians, immigrants from East Asian cultures, and individuals

from East Asian cultures living in East Asia.

Concluding Comment

While the present study did not establish a connection between culture and acceptance of

social anxiety as hoped, it still shed further light upon the SAD diagnosis through a cross cultural

Page 28: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 28

perspective, especially balancing the emphasis upon “distress” vs “impairment” when it comes to

the SAD diagnosis. Additionally, this study is amongst the first to explore the specific effects of

self-acceptance of experienced social anxiety upon negative outcomes. Despite the well-

established literature surrounding the effects of acceptance upon generalized anxiety and other

negative emotions, most of the literature is focused upon societal acceptance or peer acceptance.

Studies of self-acceptance have increased with the advent of ACT, but most of those studies are

focused upon the effects of acceptance upon anxiety disorders in general. Literature is lacking on

the effects of acceptance of experienced social anxiety upon SAD, an empty spot that the author

hope would soon be filled.

Page 29: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 29

References

Alden, L. E., & Taylor, C. T. (2004). Interpersonal processes in social phobia. Clinical

Psychology Review, 24(7), 857–882. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.07.006

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders (Fifth Edition). American Psychiatric Association. Retrieved from

http://psychiatryonline.org/doi/book/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

Arbuckle, R., Frye, M. A., Brecher, M., Paulsson, B., Rajagopalan, K., Palmer, S., & Innocenti,

A. D. (2009). The psychometric validation of the Sheehan Disability Scale (SDS) in

patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 165(1), 163–174.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.11.018

Arch, J. J., Eifert, G. H., Davies, C., Vilardaga, J. C. P., Rose, R. D., & Craske, M. G. (2012).

Randomized clinical trial of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) versus acceptance and

commitment therapy (ACT) for mixed anxiety disorders. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 80(5), 750–765. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028310

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal

attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–

529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Carbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck

Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review,

8(1), 77–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

Block-Lerner, J., Wulfert, E., & Moses, E. (2009). ACT in context: an exploration of experiential

acceptance. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16(4), 443–456.

Page 30: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 30

Brown, E. J., Turovsky, J., Heimberg, R. G., Juster, H. R., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H.

(1997). Validation of the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale and the Social Phobia Scale

across the anxiety disorders. Psychological Assessment, 9(1), 21.

Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wlhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). The

social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion, 3(1), 48–67.

https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.48

Butler, E. A., Lee, T. L., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion regulation and culture: Are the social

consequences of emotion suppression culture-specific? Emotion, 7(1), 30–48.

https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.30

Coles, T., Coon, C., DeMuro, C., McLeod, L., & Gnanasakthy, A. (2014). Psychometric

evaluation of the Sheehan Disability Scale in adult patients with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 10, 887–895.

https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S55220

Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 124.

David, D., Lynn, S. J., & Das, L. S. (2013). Self-Acceptance in Buddhism and Psychotherapy. In

M. E. Bernard (Ed.), The Strength of Self-Acceptance (pp. 19–38). New York, NY:

Springer New York. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-1-4614-6806-

6_2

Downey, G., Freitas, A. L., Michaelis, B., & Khouri, H. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy in

close relationships: Rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 545–560. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-

3514.75.2.545

Page 31: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 31

Eng, W., Coles, M. E., Heimberg, R. G., & Safren, S. A. (2005). Domains of life satisfaction in

social anxiety disorder: relation to symptoms and response to cognitive-behavioral

therapy. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19(2), 143–156.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.01.007

Gong, L., Li, W., Zhang, D., & Rost, D. H. (2016). Effects of emotion regulation strategies on

anxiety during job interviews in Chinese college students. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping,

29(3), 305–317. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2015.1042462

Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: the acute effects of inhibiting negative

and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 95.

Hacker, T., Stone, P., & MacBeth, A. (2016). Acceptance and commitment therapy – Do we

know enough? Cumulative and sequential meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials.

Journal of Affective Disorders, 190, 551–565. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.10.053

Hayes, S. C., & Lillis, J. (2012). Acceptance and commitment therapy. American Psychological

Association Washington, DC. Retrieved from http://mindovermood.info/16-research-

pappers

Hodgins, D. C. (2013). Reliability and validity of the Sheehan Disability Scale modified for

pathological gambling. BMC Psychiatry, 13. Retrieved from

http://search.proquest.com/psycinfo/docview/1420144147/A6FDD1871F664C14PQ/13

Hopwood, C. J., Pincus, A. L., DeMoor, R. M., & Koonce, E. A. (2008). Psychometric

Characteristics of the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems–Short Circumplex (IIP–SC)

With College Students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 90(6), 615–618.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890802388665

Page 32: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 32

Horowitz, L. M., Rosenberg, S. E., Baer, B. A., Ureño, G., & Villaseñor, V. S. (1988). Inventory

of interpersonal problems: Psychometric properties and clinical applications. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 885–892. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-

006X.56.6.885

Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., …

Kendler, K. S. (1994). Lifetime and 12-Month Prevalence of DSM-III-R Psychiatric

Disorders in the United States: Results From the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives

of General Psychiatry, 51(1), 8–19.

https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010008002

Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (1991). Culture and the Self: Implications for Cognition,

Emotion, and Motivation. College Student Development and Academic Life:

Psychological, Intellectual, Social and Moral Issues, 264.

Lau, A. S., Wang, S., Fung, J. J., & Namikoshi, M. (2014). What happens when you“ can’t read

the air”? Cultural fit and aptitude by values interactions on social anxiety. Journal of

Social and Clinical Psychology, 33(10), 853.

MacKenzie, M. B., & Kocovski, N. L. (2010). Self-reported acceptance of social anxiety

symptoms: Development and validation of the Social Anxiety—Acceptance and Action

Questionnaire. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 6(3), 214.

Madon, S., Willard, J., Guyll, M., & Scherr, K. C. (2011). Self-Fulfilling Prophecies:

Mechanisms, Power, and Links to Social Problems: Self-Fulfilling Prophecies. Social

and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(8), 578–590. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-

9004.2011.00375.x

Page 33: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 33

Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of social phobia

scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety11Editor’s note: This article was written before

the development of some contemporary measures of social phobia, such as the Social

Phobia and Anxiety Inventory (Turner et al., 1989). We have invited this article for

publication because of the growing interest in the scales described therein. S.T.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(4), 455–470. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-

7967(97)10031-6

Murata, A., Moser, J. S., & Kitayama, S. (2013). Culture shapes electrocortical responses during

emotion suppression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(5), 595–601.

https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss036

Nelson, L. J., & Klutas, K. (2000). The Distinctiveness Effect in Social Interaction: Creation of a

Self-Fulfilling Prophecy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(1), 126–135.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200261012

Okazaki, S. (1997). Sources of ethnic differences between Asian American and white American

college students on measures of depression and social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 106(1), 52.

Okazaki, S., Liu, J. F., Longworth, S. L., & Minn, J. Y. (2002). Asian American-White

American differences in expressions of social anxiety: A replication and extension.

Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology, 8(3), 234–247.

https://doi.org/10.1037//1099-9809.8.3.234

Peters, L., Sunderland, M., Andrews, G., Rapee, R. M., & Mattick, R. P. (2012). Development of

a short form Social Interaction Anxiety (SIAS) and Social Phobia Scale (SPS) using

Page 34: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 34

nonparametric item response theory: The SIAS-6 and the SPS-6. Psychological

Assessment, 24(1), 66–76. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024544

Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social

phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(8), 741–756.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00022-3

Remes, O., Brayne, C., van der Linde, R., & Lafortune, L. (2016). A systematic review of

reviews on the prevalence of anxiety disorders in adult populations. Brain and Behavior,

6(7), e00497. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.497

Sato, K., Yuki, M., & Norasakkunkit, V. (2014). A Socio-Ecological Approach to Cross-Cultural

Differences in the Sensitivity to Social Rejection The Partially Mediating Role of

Relational Mobility. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(10), 1549–1560.

Schneier, F. R., Johnson, J., Hornig, C. D., Liebowitz, M. R., & Weissman, M. M. (1992). Social

Phobia: Comorbidity and Morbidity in an Epidemiologic Sample. Archives of General

Psychiatry, 49(4), 282–288. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820040034004

Schreier, S.-S., Heinrichs, N., Alden, L., Rapee, R. M., Hofmann, S. G., Chen, J., … Bögels, S.

(2010). Social anxiety and social norms in individualistic and collectivistic countries.

Depression and Anxiety, 27(12), 1128–1134. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20746

Sheehan, D. (1983). The Anxiety Disease. New York, NY, USA: Charles Scribners Sons.

Shulruf, B., Hattie, J., & Dixon, R. (2007). Development of a New Measurement Tool for

Individualism and Collectivism. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 25(4), 385–

401. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282906298992

Page 35: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 35

Soldz, S., Budman, S., Demby, A., & Merry, J. (1995). A Short Form of the Inventory of

Interpersonal Problems Circumples Scales. Assessment, 2(1), 53–63.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191195002001006

Soto, J. A., Perez, C. R., Kim, Y.-H., Lee, E. A., & Minnick, M. R. (2011). Is expressive

suppression always associated with poorer psychological functioning? A cross-cultural

comparison between European Americans and Hong Kong Chinese. Emotion, 11(6),

1450–1455. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023340

Triandis, H. C. (1989). 15. The Self and Social Behavior in. The Culture and Psychology Reader,

98, 326.

Van Vugt, M., & Schaller, M. (2008). Evolutionary approaches to group dynamics: An

introduction. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 12(1), 1–6.

https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2699.12.1.1

Wancata, J., Fridl, M., & Friedrich, F. (2009). SOCIAL PHOBIA: EPIDEMIOLOGY AND

HEALTH CARE EPIDEMIOLOGIE UND VERSORGUNG DER SOZIALPHOBIE.

Psychiatria Danubina, 21(4), 520–524.

Wells, A., Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P., Ludgate, J., Hackmann, A., & Gelder, M. (1995). Social

Phobia: The Role of In-Situation Safety Behaviors in Maintaining Anxiety and Negative

Beliefs–Republished Article. Behavior Therapy, 47(5), 669–674.

Wittchen, H.-U., & Fehm, L. (2003). Epidemiology and natural course of social fears and social

phobia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108(s417), 4–18.

Yamagishi, T. (1988). Exit from the group as an individualistic solution to the free rider problem

in the United States and Japan. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24(6), 530–

542. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(88)90051-0

Page 36: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 36

Yuki, M., Schug, J., Horikawa, H., Takemura, K., Sato, K., Yokota, K., & Kamaya, K. (2007).

Development of a scale to measure perceptions of relational mobility in societ. Center for

Experimental Research in Social Science Working Paper, (83). Retrieved from

https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/11235/1/MPRA_paper_11235.pdf

Zhuangzi, & Watson, B. (2003). Zhuangzi: Basic Writings. New York: Columbia University

Press.

Appendix

Figure 1:

Page 37: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 37

Table 1: Statistics for all collected variables

Social Anxiety Acceptance Impairment Interpersonal

Problems

Depression

Mean 34.02 85.18 11.03 45.80 13.37

SD 19.26 20.14 7.52 26.26 13.10

Range 80.00 102.00 30.00 128.00 58.00

Minimum .00 25.00 .00 .00 .00

Maximum 80.00 127.00 30.00 128.00 58.00

Note: SD = Standard Deviation

Table 2: T-test results between Culture

Caucasian East Asian

M

SD M SD t-test

Social Anxiety 31.84 20.65 35.75 17.91 -2.799**

Impairment 9.81 7.75 12.01 7.19 -3.807**

Interpersonal Problems

40.15 29.69 50.33 26.73 -5.061**

Depression 12.44 0.74 14.11 0.69 -1.640

** p < 0.01

Note: M = Mean, SD = Standard Deviation

Page 38: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 38

Table 3: Predictor variables for impairment

Predictor B r

Social Anxiety 0.271 0.695**

Acceptance -0.29 0.777**

Social Anxiety x Acceptance 0.00 N.S.

**p .001 Table 4: Predictor variables for interpersonal problems

Predictor B r

Social Anxiety 1.032 0.757**

Acceptance -0.915 0.702**

Social Anxiety x Acceptance 0.004 0.836**

**p .001

Table 5: Predictor variables for depression

Predictor B r

Social Anxiety 0.349 0.514**

Acceptance -0.347 0.533**

Social Anxiety x Acceptance -0.03 0.588**

**p .001

Page 39: Different Cultures, Different Coping Styles? An

AN EXPLORATION ON THE EFFECTS OF CULTURE, ACCEPTENCE AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 39

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

LOW SOC I A L ANX I E T Y H IGH SOC I A L ANX I E T Y

INTE

RPER

SONAL

PRO

BLEM

S

SOCIAL ANXIETY

CHART 1

high acceptence low acceptence

05

10152025303540

LOW SOC I A L ANX I E T Y H IGH SOC I A L ANX I E T Y

DEPR

ESSION

SOCIAL ANXIETY

CHART 2

High Acceptance Low Acceptance