23
This may be the author’s version of a work that was submitted/accepted for publication in the following source: Sim, Susan, Berthelsen, Donna, Walker, Susan, Nicholson, Jan, & Fielding-Barnsley, Ruth (2014) A shared reading intervention with parents to enhance young children’s early literacy skills. Early Child Development and Care, 184(11), pp. 1531-1549. This file was downloaded from: https://eprints.qut.edu.au/67931/ c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matters This work is covered by copyright. Unless the document is being made available under a Creative Commons Licence, you must assume that re-use is limited to personal use and that permission from the copyright owner must be obtained for all other uses. If the docu- ment is available under a Creative Commons License (or other specified license) then refer to the Licence for details of permitted re-use. It is a condition of access that users recog- nise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. If you believe that this work infringes copyright please provide details by email to [email protected] Notice: Please note that this document may not be the Version of Record (i.e. published version) of the work. Author manuscript versions (as Sub- mitted for peer review or as Accepted for publication after peer review) can be identified by an absence of publisher branding and/or typeset appear- ance. If there is any doubt, please refer to the published source. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2013.862532

c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

This may be the author’s version of a work that was submitted/acceptedfor publication in the following source:

Sim, Susan, Berthelsen, Donna, Walker, Susan, Nicholson, Jan, &Fielding-Barnsley, Ruth(2014)A shared reading intervention with parents to enhance young children’searly literacy skills.Early Child Development and Care, 184(11), pp. 1531-1549.

This file was downloaded from: https://eprints.qut.edu.au/67931/

c© Consult author(s) regarding copyright matters

This work is covered by copyright. Unless the document is being made available under aCreative Commons Licence, you must assume that re-use is limited to personal use andthat permission from the copyright owner must be obtained for all other uses. If the docu-ment is available under a Creative Commons License (or other specified license) then referto the Licence for details of permitted re-use. It is a condition of access that users recog-nise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. If you believe thatthis work infringes copyright please provide details by email to [email protected]

Notice: Please note that this document may not be the Version of Record(i.e. published version) of the work. Author manuscript versions (as Sub-mitted for peer review or as Accepted for publication after peer review) canbe identified by an absence of publisher branding and/or typeset appear-ance. If there is any doubt, please refer to the published source.

https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2013.862532

Page 2: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

1

A shared reading intervention with parents to enhance young

children’s early literacy skills

Prepublication Copy

Citation 

Sim, S., Berthelsen, D., Walker, S., Nicholson, J., Field-Barnsley, R. (2013). A shared reading intervention with parents to enhance young children’s early literacy skills. Early Child Development and Care. DOI:10.1080/03004430.2013.862532

Published online 13 December 2013 

Authors

Susan Sim, School of Early Childhood, Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Road, Kelvin Grove, QLD, Australia, 4059

Donna Berthelsen, School of Early Childhood, Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Road, , Kelvin Grove, QLD, Australia, 4059

Susan Walker, School of Early Childhood, Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Road,, Kelvin Grove, QLD, Australia, 4059

Jan M. Nicholson, Parenting Research Centre, Level 5 232 Victoria Parade, East Melbourne, VIC, Australia, 3002

Ruth Fielding-Barnsley, School of Education, University of Tasmania , Locked Bag 1307 Launceston, TAS, Australia, 7250.

Corresponding author: 

Susan Sim Email: [email protected]

Other author emails: 

[email protected]

[email protected]

[email protected]

[email protected]

Page 3: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

2

A Shared Reading Intervention with Parents to enhance Young

Children’s Early Literacy Skills

Abstract

Parental reading to children from an early age has been shown to enhance children’s emergent literacy skills. A pragmatic randomized controlled trial (RCT) was used to investigate the effects of two forms of shared reading interventions on children’s language and literacy skills. Parents of 80 preparatory year children from outer suburban schools of an Australian metropolitan city were trained to use shared reading strategies in an eight-week home intervention. Families were assigned to one of three groups: Dialogic Reading (DR), Dialogic Reading with the addition of Print Referencing (DR + PR), or an attention-matched control group. The sample comprised 42 boys and 38 girls ranging in age from 4.9 years to 6.3 years (M = 5.5, SD = 0.3). Data were collected at pre, post, and at three months follow-up. Measures assessed children’s oral language (receptive and expressive vocabulary), phonological awareness (rhyme, word completion), alphabet knowledge, and concepts about print. Analyses of change from pre to post showed significant effects for the DR and DR + PR groups compared to the control group on three of the six measures: expressive language, rhyme, and concepts about print. At 3-month follow-up assessment, the two intervention groups maintained significantly better performance on the measure of concepts of print only. At both time points, there were no group differences between the DR and DR+PR conditions. These findings illustrate the potential of a brief home focused intervention on promoting children’s emergent literacy.

Keywords:

Emergent literacy intervention

Dialogic reading

Print referencing

Shared reading

Pragmatic randomized control trial

Page 4: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

3

Introduction

The importance of shared reading at home by parents to enhance children’s early literacy development has been well established through a range of research studies (Mol, Bus, de Jong, & Smeets, 2008; Reese, Sparks, & Leyva, 2010). Reading to children from an early age is important because the early exposure to books and reading can support positive literacy outcomes at school (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998; Hood, Conlon, & Andrews, 2008). Early education services and schools can encourage parents to read to their children regularly in order that young children can develop an early understanding of books and print. Children may also benefit from opportunities to engage in focused interactions with familiar adults around reading before they enter school, as well as during the early years of school. This research investigates the impact of two forms of a home-based shared book reading intervention on young children’s language and early literacy skills. The children were enrolled in a full-time preparatory school program in Queensland, Australia. One shared reading intervention strategy found to be effective is dialogic reading. Dialogic reading involves the use by parents of strategic questioning and thoughtful responses to children’s interests and initiations during shared book reading sessions (Whitehurst et al., 1988). Positive effects of dialogic reading have been found for the oral language skills of typically developing young children (Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998) and for the development of the oral vocabulary skills of children at risk (Morgan & Meier, 2008). Another form of shared reading intervention is known as print referencing, which features adult use of explicit print terminology during reading interactions. The adult directs the child’s attention to specific aspects of the print within the books, such as the nature of letters and words (Justice, Kaderavek, Fan, Sofka, & Hunt, 2009). There is evidence that this approach has significant positive effects on children’s knowledge of print (Justice & Ezell, 2002; Justice, Pullen, & Pence, 2008).

Research on Dialogic Reading

The primary goal of dialogic reading is to bolster children’s oral language skills, primarily vocabulary and grammar (Justice, McGinty, Piasta, Kaderavek, & Fan, 2010). In efficacy studies conducted in the United States, dialogic reading is found to be effective in increasing children’s oral language skills when involving either teachers or parents as the reading partners (Crain-Thoreson & Dale, 1999; Hargrave & Senechal, 2000; Lonigan, Anthony, Bloomfield, Dyer, & Samwel, 1999; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998).

One early research study with dialogic reading conducted by Whitehurst, Arnold et al. (1994) showed that day care teachers and parents could produce significant positive change in language skills of low income preschoolers after a six week dialogic reading intervention. Children who had the opportunity to engage in dialogic reading at day care and home, performed better than children who only had opportunities to engage in dialogic reading within their day care setting. However, Whitehurst, Arnold et al. (1994) did not include a home only reading condition in their research design to compare against the day care plus home condition. Therefore, it could not be determined whether the day care intervention or the home intervention had the greater impact on language skills. Subsequently, Lonigan and Whitehurst (1998) demonstrated that children who were engaged in a dialogic reading intervention at home only had significantly higher scores on vocabulary than those who participated in a dialogic reading intervention at school as well as at home. Together, these studies do support the value of dialogic reading at home for enhancing children’s language skills.

Page 5: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

4

More recently, Chow, McBride-Chang, Cheung, and Chow (2008) conducted a study involving 148 upper middle-class Chinese-speaking kindergarten children with four intervention conditions: a dialogic reading group, a typical reading group, a dialogic reading with teaching morphological awareness of Chinese characters, and a control group. The intervention period was 12 weeks. The children in the dialogic reading group performed significantly better than all groups in the development of receptive vocabulary. The children in the combined dialogic reading and morphological awareness group had significantly greater gains in their Chinese character recognition compared to the other three groups, and also outperformed the children in the typical reading group in morphological awareness. While these results support the findings of Whitehurst and colleagues (Lonigan & Whitehurst, 1998; Whitehurst, Arnold, et al., 1994; Whitehurst et al., 1988) that dialogic reading helps to increase children’s vocabulary, the studies also suggested that training in print awareness (in this case, morphological awareness of Chinese characters) helped to increase children’s print skills in terms of character recognition and sounds.

Few studies implementing shared reading interventions, either with dialogic reading or print referencing, have been undertaken in the Australian context. Two studies have examined dialogic reading interventions and found beneficial effects for quite different groups of children. Elias, Hay, Homel, and Freiberg (2006) found that a centre-based dialogic reading intervention of more than six months duration resulted in young children from low socio-economic status communities showing increased time in literacy-related activities, and the use of more complex language. While these positive effects were evident in preschool at a 1-year follow-up when children were in Year 1 of primary school, this study did not include a control group. For children with a history of learning disabilities, Fielding-Barnsley and Purdie (2002, 2003) found that a dialogic reading intervention at home with additional components that focused on the nature of print, including awareness of rhyme, concepts about print materials, and alphabet knowledge, resulted in small positive gains on children’s concepts of print compared to children who received dialogic reading alone.

Research on Print Referencing

The primary goal of print referencing as a shared reading intervention strategy is to enhance children’s understanding of the nature of print and written text. In order for children to learn to read they need to develop alphabet knowledge, phonological awareness, and understanding of the conventions of print as presented in books and written materials (Puranik, Lonigan, & Kim, 2011). This is important in helping children to learn that the oral spoken word can be mapped onto the printed word (Snow et al., 1988).

Evans, Williamson, and Pursoo (2008) found that children aged between three and five years spend a substantial percentage of their time looking at illustrations rather than at the print in books. This finding is consistent with earlier findings of Evans and Saint-Aubin (2005) and Justice, Skibbe, Canning, and Lankford (2005). However, Evans and colleagues (2008) in their research showed that when children’s attention was drawn to the written text in books during shared book reading, the time that they then spent looking at print increased for all ages. In particular, 4-year-old children improved significantly on print recognition. While children’s performance on the print recognition task increased across all age groups in the research, older children looked more at the text as well as at the detail of the printed text. These findings suggests that pointing to print while reading may be an effective strategy to increase children’s awareness of the features of print when parents read with their children.

Page 6: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

5

Evans et al. (2008) suggested that if one of the goals of shared reading is to enhance print skills and orthographic knowledge, parents should choose genres of books that facilitate attention to and spontaneous conversation about print (e.g., alphabet books) or engage in specific behaviours that increase the amount of attention to print during story book reading. Justice, Pullen, and Pence (2008) found that children’s visual attention to print was significantly increased when adults read to children using explicit verbal and non-verbal print references. As Evans and Saint-Aubin (2005) noted “it is difficult to see how shared reading, without additional explicit references to print within the books, can be a major vehicle for developing children’s understanding of orthography or print specific skills” (p. 918).

The primary goal of print referencing is to orient children’s attention to print within a storybook by verbal and non-verbal means (Justice et al., 2010) and, as a consequence, children’s print knowledge can increase in relatively short periods of time (Justice & Ezell, 2002). This intentional focus on children’s attention to print within written text has been found to have a direct and positive influence on children’s learning about the code-based features of reading (Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; Justice & Ezell, 2004). Therefore, it seems critical that young children are encouraged to gain some knowledge of the print code and the meaning-based aspects of literacy before they are exposed to formal literacy programs in order to enhance their early literacy skills (Senechal, LeFevre, Smith-Chant, & Colton, 2001; Storch & Whitehurst, 2002). These emergent literacy skills are the precursors to conventional literacy skills.

Justice and Ezell (2000) in an intervention study using print referencing found significant gains in children’s ability to understand concepts of words in print and to segment word strings. This was a result of a four week intervention program with 28 parents reading with their 4-year-old children. In a Head-Start program, Justice and Ezell (2002) reported findings for 30 preschool children participating in a print referencing intervention that was conducted by teachers in small group settings that compared the use of print referencing strategies to strategies that focused attention on the book illustrations. They found significant differences for three of the four outcome measures of print recognition, words in print, and alphabet knowledge. To date, research on print referencing, as a shared reading strategy, has primarily involved teachers (Justice et al., 2010; McGinty, Breit-Smith, Fan, Justice, & Kaderavek, 2011; Piasta, Justice, McGinty, & Kaderavek, 2012). It is yet to be established whether print referencing can also be used effectively with parents at home to enhance children’s emergent literacy skills.

This Research Study

Research using dialogic reading interventions has demonstrated the beneficial effects on children’s oral language skills, such as expressive and receptive vocabulary but not on improving children’s other literacy skills, such as alphabet knowledge and concepts about print (What Works Clearinghouse, Institute of Education Sciences, United States Department of Education, 2007). Because literacy skills involve oral and written language knowledge, intervention strategies as children begin school should target both oral and written language knowledge. The current research investigates whether dialogic reading strategies are enhanced by the addition of a specific focus on print (i.e., print referencing strategies) to develop children’s understanding of the concepts about print and their alphabetical knowledge, in addition to the expected enhancement of oral language skills through dialogic reading. We are not aware of any studies to date, that have compared a dialogic reading condition in an intervention with a condition that includes dialogic reading with print referencing and that is implemented by parents at home. When compared with children who

Page 7: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

6

did not receive the reading intervention, we hypothesized that both approaches would enhance children’s oral language skills, while the combined approach would uniquely benefit children’s print knowledge skills as well.

This study addresses a significant research gap, particularly in the Australian context where there is a paucity of studies that have evaluated shared reading interventions implemented by parents at home. The current study will add to existing research knowledge by comparing the effects of a home reading intervention provided to 4-5- year-old children prior to formal schooling. Three conditions are implemented in the intervention study: dialogic reading alone (DR), dialogic reading with the addition of print referencing (DR+PR), and an attention-matched control condition (control). The intervention is conducted across a relatively short period of eight weeks, with a single training session provided to the parents of the children in each condition. Thus, the study will provide evidence for the efficacy of a short term intervention designed to be easily implemented through a classroom program. Children’s language and early literacy skills will be assessed at pre, post and at a follow-up assessment conducted three months after completion of the intervention.

The specific research questions are:

(1) What is the effect of a shared reading intervention on children’s language and literacy outcomes from pre to post intervention? Specifically, do children in the DR condition and the DR+PR condition show greater improvement than children in the control condition in language and early literacy outcomes at post intervention? Do children in the DR+PR condition show greater improvement in language and early literacy outcome measures than children in the DR condition group at post intervention?

(2) What is the impact of the intervention at follow-up, three months after the intervention? Specifically, do children in the DR condition and the DR+PR condition show greater improvement than children in the control condition in language and literacy outcomes at follow-up? Do children in the DR+PR condition show greater improvement in language and literacy outcome measures than children in the DR condition at follow-up?

Method

Research Design

The current study involved families with children in the preparatory (Prep) year of school in the Australian state of Queensland. Children can be enrolled in the Prep year if they turn 5 years by the 30th of June in the year in which they are enrolled. The school year across Australia runs from late January until mid-December. The children and families were recruited from three schools in outer suburban areas of the city of Brisbane. Schools participating in the research were recruited from the Catholic Education sector. Families were recruited over a two-year period under consistent recruitment conditions.

The research design was a pragmatic randomized control trial (RCT) (Torgerson, 2008, 2009; Treweek & Zwarenstein, 2009). Randomized controlled trials can be classified as explanatory or pragmatic (Treweek & Zwarenstein, 2009). While the distinction between these classifications is variable across the literature, the distinctions reflect different positions about design and delivery (Helms, 2002; Macramé, 1989; Torgerson, 2008, 2009; Torgerson, & Torgerson, 2003; Torgerson, & Torgerson, 2007; Wakefield, 2000).

Page 8: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

7

Schwartz and Lellouch (1967) were the first to make the distinctions between explanatory and pragmatic TCTs. Explanatory RCTs, also termed ‘efficacy trials’ (MacRae, 1989) test efficacy in highly controlled research settings, while pragmatic RCTs or ‘effectiveness trials’, test effectiveness in everyday practice. Each approach has strengths and limitations. Explanatory trials have an important role in providing knowledge about the effects of carefully implemented interventions delivered under optimal conditions but these may not translate to real world settings. This led to the evolution of pragmatic RCTs which sought to overcome ethical and practical implementation difficulties and the need for some flexibility in real world settings. While pragmatic trials provide information about how interventions work in the real world, they may have limited capacity to measure the “true” effects of an intervention (Kent & Kitsios, 2009). The goal of a pragmatic RCT should therefore be to ensure that the methodological features of an explanatory RCT are met as strongly as possible (Altman & Bland, 1999). For example, with respect to randomization, the What Works Clearinghouse which evaluates evidence of the effectiveness of education interventions considers functional randomization to be an acceptable alternative when practical considerations preclude true randomization (What Works Clearinghouse, Institute of Education Sciences, United States Department of Education, 2010).

The pragmatic decision taken in this research was to allocate parents to intervention condition according to the date of the training session that they could elect to attend. Parents were provided with dates for three training sessions at their school (one for each condition), and could select any one session to attend. They were not informed prior to the session of the nature of the associated intervention condition for any particular day, precluding their self-selection into a particular condition. It was assumed that this process provided an approximation to random allocation and would not result in a sample selection bias, given that parents did not know which training condition that they were selecting. This assumption was subsequently tested by statistical comparisons between groups on family demographic variables (i.e., maternal education, language spoken at home, parental employment, number of children in the family) across conditions. As described in the results, no selection bias was identified.

Participants

The families and children were recruited from three Catholic Education schools situated in the outer area of an Australian metropolitan city in Queensland. Recruitment of participants commenced after obtaining ethical clearance and formal approval from the Queensland University of Technology and the Brisbane Catholic Education Commission. Due to the logistical and time demands for the single researcher engaged in the research, the sample was recruited from two Prep year cohorts from three schools in 2009 and from one of these three schools in 2010. Families were recruited at similar times in each year, under the same conditions of training, with the same measures and procedures used at pre assessment, post assessment and follow-up assessment. Information and consent forms were distributed via Prep year teachers to the parents/caregivers of every student in the Prep classrooms at each school. The researcher followed up with teachers and schools to ensure an adequate response rate was achieved. The response rates were: 40% across the three participating schools in 2009 and 48% for the one school in 2010.

The initial sample recruited for the study consisted of 82 parents and children. Two parents did not attend any training, reducing the sample size to 80 parents and their children at Time 1 (pre-test assessment). There were 42 boys and 38 girls in the sample ranging in age from 4.92 years to 6.25 years (M = 5.53, SD = 0.33). Demographic data were available for 75

Page 9: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

8

families. Sixty-eight mothers and five fathers participated in the research. The other two participants were relatives of the focus child. The majority of families were from a Caucasian background (79%) with the remainder made up of those from an Asian background (19%), and others (3%). English was the home language of 79% of the participants while 21% spoke another language besides English. There were two families who described their child as being of Aboriginal or Torres Straits Islander background. Only 9% of parents had not completed high school and 91% had completed some other qualification after high school. Ten families (13%) were single-parent families, 30 (40%) had three or more children under 18 living in the household, and 7 (9%) received their main income from government welfare benefits.

Seventy-seven children participated in the Time 2 (post-test) assessment conducted in Term 4. Three children were not available for this assessment because the family was away on summer holidays. Seventy-two children participated in the Time 3 (follow-up) assessment conducted three months later when the children were in Year 1. Five children were not available for the follow-up assessment because they had moved schools at the end of the previous year.

Procedures

When consent forms were received, the researcher made arrangements with the classroom teachers/learning support teachers for the dates and times to complete the pre intervention assessment with the children of the participating parents, before the intervention training sessions were scheduled. Each child was tested individually in a quiet and safe room designated by the school. Each child in the study received pre, post and follow-up assessments using the same assessment measures. Each assessment took approximately one hour to complete. Rapport with each child was established initially to ensure that the child felt comfortable and relaxed to ensure that he/she could perform at his/her best on the assessment measures. After the completion of child assessments, parents/caregivers were then invited to attend a training session at the school on a weekday, scheduled in the hour before or after school.

The intervention was conducted in English and was undertaken across eight weeks during the third term of the Australian school year which has four school terms. Parents in each intervention condition received one training session of one hour on the reading strategies they were to use with their children at home and completed a short demographic questionnaire. Training sessions were differentiated in content according to the allocated condition.

Description of the Intervention

In the two reading intervention conditions, parents were shown video tapes demonstrating the shared reading strategies. Parents in the DR condition were shown an eight and a half minute video made by the first author illustrating the strategies of DR (questioning and interactional reading). Parents in the DR + PR group were shown this plus a second seven minute video made by the last author incorporating the strategies of PR (engaging children about print awareness, rhyming, and alliteration). The specifics of each strategy are shown in Appendix A (Tables A.1 and Table A.2). After viewing the video(s), notes relating to each set of reading strategies were explained in detail. Parents were allowed to ask questions and give feedback to ensure that they understood the recommended strategies. A take-home laminated copy of the reading strategies was provided to the parents as a guide when reading with their children. Parents in the two intervention groups were asked to read a book, using

Page 10: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

9

the demonstrated strategies, on at least three occasions in each week, for a period of eight weeks. Reading books were changed each week by the classroom teacher. Parents in the two intervention groups read the same books for the intervention. The list of the books used in the intervention is presented in Appendix B.

After the training session, an initial telephone call was made to all parents in the two intervention groups to ensure that they understood what to do and that they were able to implement the reading activities. Subsequently, the researcher made follow-up telephone calls each week to all parents to see if they had managed to read each book three times with their child, and to remind the parents to complete their reading log and for children to return the book and log to the class teacher at the end of each week, in order to receive a new book for the following week. Parents were reminded to return the book after each week regardless of the number of times they had read with their child. This was to ensure that they had the opportunity to read all of the eight books throughout the intervention period. All parents were invited to contact the researcher at any time if they had any questions.

Parents in the control group received training on completing number learning activities with their child. Children in the control group therefore were considered to be receiving similar amounts of one-on-one attention to match the level of parental attention as was received by children in the intervention groups. At the training session, these parents were shown eight number activities to teach their children the numbers from 1 to 10 and their corresponding values. Parents were asked to complete one number activity with their child three times each week for eight weeks. Parents in the control group were contacted during the first week and fortnightly after that to ensure that they were able to carry out the number activities, and were also informed to contact the researcher if they encountered any difficulty. When the children in the two reading intervention groups were given books to take home, children in the control were given sets of number cards.

Intervention fidelity

Implementation fidelity of shared reading interventions has been assessed in a number of ways in previous studies. These include requiring participants to video or audio tape their reading with their children (e.g., Arnold, Lonigan, Whitehurst, & Epstein, 1994; Briesch, Chafouleas, Lebel, & Blom-Hoffman, 2008; Justice & Ezell, 2000; Whitehurst et al., 1988), completion of a reading and activity log (Hardgrave & Senechal, 2000; Whitehurst, Arnold, et al., 1994; Whitehurst, Epstein, et al., 1994), and use of a questionnaire regarding compliance (Whitehurst, Epstein, et al., 1994). Parent use of the reading strategies at home was supported and assessed in two ways during each week of the intervention: weekly phone calls from the researcher and parent-completed logs. At each phone call, parents were asked the number of times they were able to read with their child, whether they were encountering any problems and how the child enjoyed reading the book. They were reminded about the strategies to use and asked to return the book with the log sheet at the end of the week in order to receive the next book. Parents recorded the number of times they read the book to the child in the log sheet and could write down any comments about the reading sessions or the book. This method of assessing fidelity expanded on the procedure used by Fielding-Barnsley and Purdie (2002) which involved a single mid-program phone call. It had the benefit of both monitoring compliance with the reading program as well as providing the opportunity for the parent and researcher to discuss any problems encountered which the researcher could help to rectify (e.g., if a parent did not get a new book for the week or if the teacher failed to change the book).

Page 11: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

10

Measures

Children were assessed by the first author who is a registered psychologist at pre (before the intervention started), post (after the intervention) and follow-up (three months after the intervention). The description and psychometric properties of the six measures used at each assessment are presented in Table 1 and briefly described here.

The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (4th Ed.) (PPVT-IV) (Dunn & Dunn, 1998) is used extensively in reading research (e.g., Chow et al., 2008; Hardgrave & Senechal, 2000; Justice & Ezell., 2002; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Whitehurst, Arnold et al., 1994). The Hundred Pictures Naming Test (HPNT; Fischer & Glenister, 1992) is a measure of expressive single-word vocabulary language skills and is used in the current study. This measure was selected over the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (EOWPVT; Gardner, 1981) which has previously been used in reading intervention studies that include print referencing (e.g., Justice & Ezell, 2002), as the HPNT has normative data for Australian children whereas the latter’s normative group was drawn from the San Francisco Bay area in North America. The Phonological Abilities Test (PAT; Muter, Hulme, & Snowling, 1997) is a measure used to assess three phonological abilities: rhyme; syllable and phonemes (word completion subscale); and alphabet knowledge. The Concepts about Print (CAP; Clay, 2002) is a measure of print concepts used in reading research (e.g., Fielding-Barnsley & Purdie, 2002, 2003).

< Insert Table 1 here>

Results

In this section the findings about the effectiveness of the intervention from pre to post; pre to follow-up are presented. These statistical analyses use a method of controlling for baseline scores recommended by Frison and Pocock (1992) and Mol and colleagues (2008) to improve precision in identifying intervention effects. To establish that there were no baseline differences in the pre-test measures by intervention condition by year of recruitment, preliminary analyses tested for differences. These analyses are presented prior to reporting the intervention analyses. Data were also checked for violations of the assumptions of normality, linearity and homogeneity of variances.

Preliminary Analyses on Baseline Differences between Groups

Data collected across the two cohorts recruited in 2009 and 2010 were combined and examined for any significant baseline differences by year of recruitment and by intervention condition. One-way between-groups analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) were conducted for each pre-test dependent measure by year (2009, 2010) as a covariate and the three conditions (DR+PR, DR, control) as the independent grouping variable. There were no differences between conditions on the six baseline measures when year of recruitment was included as a covariate: PPVT, F (2, 76) = 1.42, p = .25; HPNT, F (2, 76) = 0.16, p =.85; PAT subtests: PAT - Rhyme, F (2, 76) = 0.89, p =.41, PAT- Word Completion (Syllable and Phoneme), F (2, 76) = 1.95, p =.15;PAT - Alphabet Knowledge, F (2, 76) =1.12, p =.33; CAP, F (2, 76) = 2.01, p =.14. It was therefore concluded that (1) children in the three groups were of similar competence on the measures at pre-test suggesting that ‘pragmatic randomization’ was effective in reducing or eliminating group selection bias on the basis of children’s

Page 12: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

11

competencies, and (2) the cohorts recruited across two years could be combined for the subsequent analyses that tested for the intervention effects.

Pre to Post Intervention Change

Analyses of the effects of the intervention used a series of analysis of covariance (ANCOVAs) testing for condition effects at Time 2 (post assessment), with pre-test scores entered as covariates. One-way between groups ANCOVAs were conducted with intervention condition (DR+PR, DR, control) as the independent variable and the six language and literacy scores as dependent variables. Means and standard deviations on the measures are presented in Table 2. For all analyses, a least significant difference (LSD) post hoc group means comparison test was conducted to identify where the differences were among the three groups. Effect sizes for significant differences between groups in the analyses were calculated using Cohen’s ƒ statistic. Effect size index calculations used the standard deviation of the g (number of groups) population means divided by the common within-population standard deviations (Cohen, 1992).

Compared to the control group, children in the DR and DR +PR group showed greater improvement at post intervention on three language and early literacy measures: HPNT, PAT-Rhyme, and CAP, as described below. There were no significant between group differences for receptive vocabulary, PPVT, F (2, 73) = 0.66, p = .52; word completion, PAT - Word Completion (Syllable and Phoneme), F (2, 73) = 0.91, p =.41or alphabet knowledge, PAT - Alphabet Knowledge, F (2, 73) = 2 .81, p = .067.

HPNT: After adjusting for the pre intervention scores on the HPNT, there was a significant group effect on the post intervention HPNT scores, F (2, 73) = 5.095, p = .008; partial eta squared = .12. This represents a small effect size, Cohen δ = 0.207 (Cohen, 1988, 1992). Pair wise analyses found that the post intervention scores of the DR+PR group was significantly higher than that of the control group, mean difference = 2.72, p =. 008; also the post intervention scores of the DR group were significantly higher than those of the control group, mean difference = 2.93, p = .005. There were no significant differences between the DR+PR group and the DR group on the post intervention scores, mean difference = -.22, p = .83.

PAT-Rhyme: After adjusting for the pre intervention scores in the ANCOVA analysis on the measure of PAT-Rhyme, there were significant group differences on the post intervention PAT-Rhyme scores, F (2, 73) = 6.89, p =.002, partial eta squared = .16. This represents a medium effect size, Cohen δ = .275 (Cohen, 1988, 1992). Pair wise analyses revealed that the post intervention scores for the DR+PR group were significantly higher than that of the control group, mean difference = 1.82, p = .002; also the post intervention scores for the DR group were significantly higher than those of the control group, mean difference = 1.88, p = .002. There were no significant differences between the DR+PR group and the DR group on the post intervention scores, mean difference = -.05, p = .92.

CAP: After adjusting for the pre intervention scores on CAP, there was a significant group effect on the post intervention CAP scores, F (2, 73) = 24.92, p = .00, partial eta squared = .41. This represents a large effect size, Cohen δ = 0.442 (Cohen, 1988, 1992). Pair wise analyses found that the post intervention scores of the DR+PR group was significantly higher than that of the control group, mean difference = 4.33, p = .00; also the post intervention scores of the DR group were significantly higher than those of the control group, mean difference = 4.09, p = .00. There were no significant differences between the DR+PR group and the DR group on the post intervention scores, mean difference = .23, p = .72.

Page 13: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

12

In summary, compared to the control group, children in the DR and DR +PR group showed greater improvement on three language and early literacy measures: HPNT, PAT-Rhyme, and CAP at post intervention than the control group but there were no differences for the PPVT, PAT-Word Completion, or PAT-Alphabet Knowledge. There were no significant differences on any of the measures between the intervention conditions of DR+PR and DR groups at post intervention. The attrition rate was low at post intervention. Two children from the DR condition and one child from the DR+PR condition did not attend the post assessment.

<Insert Table 2 here>

Pre to Follow-Up Intervention Change

Similar one-way between groups ANCOVAs were used to examine the follow-up effects of the two reading interventions at Year 1. Table 3 presents the means and standard deviations for the pre to follow-up scores. Results revealed significant differences between the three intervention conditions on only one measure: CAP, F (2, 68) = 5.23, p = .008, partial eta-squared= 0.13. This represents a small effect size, Cohen δ = 0.234 (Cohen, 1988, 1992). Pair wise analyses found that the follow-up scores of the DR+PR group were significantly higher than that of the control group, mean difference = 1.603, p = .005; and also the follow-up scores of the DR group were significantly higher than those of the control group, mean difference = 1.542, p = .007. However, there were no significant differences between the DR+PR group and the DR group on the post intervention scores, mean difference = .91, p = .72.

In summary, children in both the intervention groups, DR+PR and DR had maintained the effects of the shared reading intervention compared with the children in the control group for concepts of print only. At follow-up assessment which was conducted three months after the post assessment, attrition rate was low. Five children did not participate due to a change of schools across the summer holiday break. They were: one from the control group, two from the DR group, and two from the DR+PR group.

<Insert Table 3 here>

Discussion

Results of the current study confirmed that a shared book reading intervention had some beneficial effects on children’s language and literacy skills related to expressive vocabulary, rhyme, and concepts about print. The results also showed that the shared book reading intervention helped children to maintain their understanding of concepts about print at follow-up when the children entered Year 1. Contrary to expectations, there were no differences in the effects for the different reading intervention groups (DR and DR+PR), either at post or follow-up.

The significant difference in post assessment scores for expressive vocabulary (HPNT) for the two intervention groups compared to the control group gives further support to previous research on dialogic reading and children’s oral language (Arnold et al., 1994; Elias et al., 2006; Fielding-Barnsley & Purdie, 2002, 2003; Lonigan et al., 1999; Whitehurst et al., 1988). The results of the current study also provided support that both forms of the shared book reading intervention strategies helped improve children’s understanding of rhyme. This is a skill related to phonological awareness. A plausible explanation for the non-significant difference between the two intervention groups on that measure is that both forms of shared

Page 14: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

13

book reading intervention are equally effective strategies for conveying understanding to children about rhyme through attention to rhyming words in the rhyming books used in the current study. The findings on the significant differences at post for print knowledge between the two intervention groups and the control group also supports the findings of Justice and Ezell (2000) in this area.

The current study showed that the efficacy of two forms of shared book reading intervention on children’s concepts of print were maintained at follow-up assessment at Year 1 entry. This significant difference in follow-up scores for concepts of print for the two intervention groups compared to the control group demonstrated that shared book reading helped maintained the concepts of print skills of children in the two intervention groups compared to children in the control group who did not show improvement on these concepts at follow-up. Although Elias et al. (2006) found children showed greater confidence and more familiarity with the concept of print tasks in Year 1 follow-up, there was no control group in that study to compare with the experimental group. Piasta et al. (2012) did find significant effects of a 30-week shared book reading intervention implemented by teachers using print referencing on preschool children’s early literacy skills at a two year follow-up.

Limitations of the Study 

Parents in each intervention condition were trained to follow the points of the respective reading strategies printed in the handouts as well as demonstrated in the videos. Greater gains in children’s print knowledge related to alphabet knowledge might have been apparent if there had been a stronger emphasis in the training for parents in the dialogic reading with the addition of print referencing group to explicitly teach their children to recognise the letters and sounds instead of merely pointing out the letters and sounds encountered within the print. This limitation might be the reason why there was no difference between the two reading intervention groups in outcomes. Another plausible reason is that parents in the dialogic reading with the addition of print referencing group might not have been able to implement both sets of reading strategies effectively. Future research may want to investigate dialogic reading with print referencing when parental teaching of the latter strategy is given greater emphasis and time within the training. Future research could also consider implementing a print referencing alone condition to compare with a dialogic reading with the addition of print referencing intervention group.

A second limitation relates to the fidelity of the intervention implementation. There were measures in the current study to support the implementation fidelity. These included a log sheet sent home each week for parents to record their book reading with each new book, as well as weekly follow-up telephone calls by the researchers to confirm that the book had been read at least three times. This system did not guarantee that the books were read with strong attention to the use of the strategies for dialogic reading or print referencing strategies which parents had received in their training. More extensive training in the strategies prior to intervention could improve the effects of the intervention. Additionally, reminders and confirmation of the use of the strategies during the intervention could be made in the weekly follow-up phone calls to parents. It was also difficult to ascertain the reliability of parental self-report on the frequency of the reading of the book for the week. A few parents had many other commitments and did indicate that they could not read the book the recommended number of times with their children but most could confirm that they had read the book three or more times each week. Another possibility would be to get parents to complete a short video of the shared book reading activity with their child on one or more occasions during the intervention in order to ascertain how well the parents were able to implement the shared

Page 15: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

14

reading strategies in which they were trained. This would enable a check to be made on the quality of strategies implemented within the intervention conditions.

A third limitation is about the optimal duration and timing for implementing a shared reading intervention for parents of children in the preparatory year of school. In this study, the intervention was delivered in Term 3 in a school year that has four terms and the follow-up assessment was conducted after the summer school holidays. Summer holidays are a time when children and families may reduce their engagement in routine activities, such as daily reading. Delivering the intervention towards the end of the school may not have allowed “new reading behaviours” by the parents to be sufficiently well-established to ensure that gains made by children as a result of the intervention were maintained into the next school year. The intervention may have been more effective if implemented in the 1st or 2nd term of the school year and the follow-up assessment made at the end of the prep school year. Additionally, an intervention of eight weeks was a relatively short intervention period. Eight weeks may not have been sufficient to establish new patterns of shared reading behaviours between parents and children. Future research is needed to identify the optimal timing for delivering the intervention within the school year and the optimal duration for bringing about greater long term benefits.

Strengths of the Study

This study has a number of strengths that provide direction for future research. The study gives support for a relatively short and simple intervention approach that could be easily delivered to parents by teachers as children begin school. A major strength of the present study is its use of an attention-matched control condition in which parents in the control group were engaged in number games with their children at the same frequency each week, for the same number of weeks. Thus, the significant findings cannot be attributed to differences in the amount of one-on-one attention that children received from their parents during the intervention, giving greater confidence that resulting differences were due to the shared reading strategies. Overall, the intervention was also shown to be acceptable to parents as evidenced by the low attrition rate of families in the research.

In summary, the findings of the current study demonstrated that there are beneficial effects of shared book reading with parents on children’s emergent literacy skills before formal schooling. This supports the importance of engaging parents in the development of emergent literacy by explicitly teaching parents to engage in specific strategies when reading to their children. This has implications for policy makers to support early reading interventions particularly for children from disadvantaged backgrounds whose parents may not have the resources or awareness of how to engage their children in shared reading at home. Dialogic reading and the combination of dialogic reading with print referencing had significant effects on children’s oral language and print awareness or code-related skills. These results support the importance of shared reading with children, as well as confirming the findings of previous research (e.g., Whitehurst et al., 1988) that dialogic reading is an effective means to help young children’s oral language skills. Further, the findings of the current study provides further support for the proposal by the National Early Literacy Panel (2008) in the United States that a combination of shared reading strategies may help develop children’s early literacy skills. Dialogic reading alone, and dialogic reading with the addition of print referencing, appears to provide effective means that parents can use in order to support young children’s emergent literacy skills.

Page 16: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

15

References

Altman, D. G., & Bland, M. J. (1999). Treatment allocation in controlled trials: Why randomise? British Medical Journal Online, 318:1209.

Arnold, D. H., Lonigan, C. J., Whitehurst, G. J., & Epstein, J. N. (1994). Accelerating language development through picture book reading: Replication and extension to a videotape training format. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 235-243.

Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2006). Information paper: An introduction to Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA), 2006 (Report No. 2039.0) .Canberra, Australia: Australia Bureau of Statistics.

Briesch, A. M., Chafouleas, S. M., Lebel, T. J., & Blom-Hoffman, J. A. (2008). Impact of videotaped instruction in dialogic reading strategies: An investigation of caregiver implementation. Psychology in the Schools, 45(10), 978-993.

Byrne, B. & Fielding-Barnsley, R. F. (1992). Sound Foundations: A scientific researched method of introducing pre-reading skills. Artarmon, Australia: Leydon.

Clay, M. M. (2002). Concepts of print: An observation survey of early literacy achievement (2nd ed.). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Chow, B., McBride-Chang, C., Cheung, H., & Chow, C. S. (2008). Dialogic reading and morphology training in Chinese Children: Effects on language and literacy. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 233-244.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.

Crain-Thoreson, C., & Dale, P. S. (1999). Enhancing linguistic performance: Parents and teachers as book reading partners for children with language delays. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 18, 28-39.

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (1998). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.

Elias, G., Hay, I., Homel, R., & Freiberg, K. (2006). Enhancing parent-child reading in a disadvantaged community. Australian Journal of Early Childhood, 31(1), 20-25.

Evans, M. A., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2005). What children are looking at during shared storybook reading. Psychological Science, 16, 913-920.

Evans, M. A., Williamson, K., & Pursoo, T. (2008). Preschoolers’ attention to print during shared book reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(1), 106-129.

Fielding-Barnsley, R., & Purdie, N. (2002). Developing pre-literacy skills via shared book reading. Australian Journal of Learning Disabilities, 7(3), 13-19.

Fielding-Barnsley, R., & Purdie, N. (2003). Early intervention in the home for children at risk for reading failure. Support for Learning: British Journal of Learning Support, 18(2), 77-82.

Fischer, J. P., & Glenister, J. (1992). The Hundred Pictures Naming Test. Victoria, Australia: The Australian Council for Educational Research Ltd.

Page 17: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

16

Frison, L., & Pocock, S. J. (1992). Repeated measures in clinical trials: Analysis using mean summary statistics and its implications for design. Statistics in Medicine, 11, 1685-1704.

Gardner, M. F. (1981). Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.

Hargrave, A. C., & Senechal, M. (2000). A book reading intervention with preschool children who have limited vocabularies: The benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15(1), 75-90.

Helms, P. J. (2002). Real world pragmatic clinical trials: What are they and what do they tell us? Pediatric Allergy and Immunology, 13, 4-9.

Hood, M., Conlon, E., & Andrews, G. (2008). Preschool home literacy practices and children’s literacy development: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 252-271.

Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2000). Enhancing children’s print and word awareness through home-based parent intervention. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9(3), 257-269.

Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2002). Use of storybook reading to increase print awareness in at-risk children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 17-29.

Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H.K. (2004). Print referencing: An emergent literacy enhancement strategy and its clinical applications. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools, 35(2), 185-192.

Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., Fan, X., Sofka, A., & Hunt, A. (2009). Accelerating preschoolers’ early literacy development through classroom-based teacher-child storybook reading and explicit print referencing. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40(1), 67-85.

Justice, L. M., McGinty, A. S., Piasta, S. B., Kaderavek, J. N., & Fan, X. (2010). Print-focused read-alouds in preschool classrooms: Intervention effectiveness and moderators of child outcomes. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 504-520.

Justice, L. M., & Pullen, P. C., Pence, K. (2008). Influence of verbal and non-verbal references to print on preschoolers’ visual attention to print during storybook reading. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 855-866.

Justice, L. M., Skibbe, L., Canning, A., & Lankford, C. (2005). Pre-schoolers, print and storybooks: An observational study using eye movement analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28(3), 229-243.

Kent, D. M., & Kitsios, G. (2009). Against pragmatism: On efficacy, effectiveness and the real world. Trials, 10:48. doi:10.1186/1745-6215-10-48

Lonigan, C. J., Anthony, J. L., Bloomfield, B.G., Dyer, S. M., & Samwel, C. S. (1999). Effects of two shared-reading interventions on emergent literacy skills of at-risk preschoolers. Journal of Early Intervention, 22(4), 306-322.

Lonigan, C. J., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1998). Relative efficacy of parent and teacher involvement in a shared-reading intervention for preschool children from low income backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(2), 263-290.

Page 18: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

17

MacRae, K. D. (1989): Pragmatic versus explanatory trials. International Journal of Technology Assessment in Health Care, 5, 333-339.

McGinty, A. S., Breit-Smith, A., Fan, X., Justice, L.M., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2011). Does intensity matter? Preschoolers’ print knowledge development within a classroom-based intervention. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 255-267.

Morgan, P. L., & Meier, C. R. (2008). Dialogic reading’s potential to improve children’s emergent literacy skills and behavior. Preventing School Failure, 52(4), 11-16.

Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., de Jong, M. T., & Smeets, D. J. H. (2008). Added value of dialogic parent-child book readings: A meta-analysis. Early Education & Development, 19(1), 7-26.

Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. (1997). The Phonological Abilities Test. University of York, York, UK: The Psychological Corporation.

National Early Literacy Panel (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Jessup, MD: National Center for Family Literacy, National Institute for Literacy. Retrieved http.www.nifl.gov/nifl/publications//pdf/nelpreport09.pdf.

Piasta, S. B.. Justice, L. M., McGinty, A. S., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2012). Increasing young children’s contact with print during shared reading: Longitudinal effects on literacy achievement. Child Development, 83(3), 810-820.

Puranik, C. S., Lonigan, C. J., & Kim, Y. (2011). Contributions of emergent literacy skills to name writing, letter writing, and spelling in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 465-474.

Reese, E., Sparks, A., & Leyva, D. (2010). A review of parent interventions for preschool children’s language and emergent literacy. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 10, 97-117.

Schwartz, D., & Lellouch, J. (1967). Explanatory and pragmatic attitudes in therapeutic trials. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 20, 637-648.

Senechal, M., LeFevre, J., Smith-Chant, B. L., & Colton, K. V. (2001). On refining theoretical models of emergent literacy the role of empirical evidence. Journal of School Psychology, 39(5), 439-460.

Snow, C. E., Burns, S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Educational attainments of school leavers with a preschool history of speech-language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 173-183.

Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and coded-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934-947.

Torgerson. C. J. (2008). Randomized experiments evaluating policy interventions are feasible. The Psychology of Education Review, 32(2).

Torgerson, C. J. (2009). Randomised controlled trials in education research: A case study of an individually randomised pragmatic trial. Education 3-13: International Journal of Primary, Elementary and Early Years Education, 37(4), 313-321.

Torgerson, C. J., & Torgerson, D. J. (2003). The design and conduct of randomised controlled trials in education: Lessons from health care. Oxford Review of Education, 29(1), 67-80.

Page 19: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

18

Torgerson, C. J., & Torgerson, D. J. (2007). The need for pragmatic experimentation in educational research. Economics of Innovation and New Technology, 16(5), 323-330.

Treweek, S., & Zwarenstein, M. (2009) Making trials matter: Pragmatic and explanatory trials and the problem of applicability. Trials, 10:37. doi:10.1186/1745-6215-10-37

Wakefield, A. (2000). Evidence-based physiotherapy: The case for pragmatic controlled trials. Physiotherapy, 86, 394-396.

Wasik, B. A., & Bond, M. A. (2001). Beyond the pages of a book: Interactive book reading and language development in preschool classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 243-250.

What Works Clearinghouse, Institute of Education Sciences, United States Department of Education. (2007). Interactive Shared Book Reading. Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/intervention_reports/WWC_ISBR_011807.pdf

What Works Clearinghouse, Institute of Education Sciences, United States Department of Education. (2010). Procedures and standards handbook (Version 2.1). Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/reference_resources/wwc_procedures_v2_1_standards_handbook.pdf

Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Smith, M, & Fischel, J. E. (1994). A picture book reading intervention in day care and home from low income families. Developmental Psychology, 30(5), 679-689.

Whitehurst, G. J., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Payne, A. C., Crone, D. A., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). Outcomes of an emergent literacy intervention in Head Start. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(4), 542-555.

Whitehurst, G. J., Falco, F. L., Lonigan, C. J., Fischel, J. E., DeBaryshe, B. D., Valdez-Menchaca, M. C., & Caulfield, M. (1988). Accelerating language development through picture book reading. Developmental Psychology, 24(4), 552-559.

Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848-872.

Page 20: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

19

Table 1

Measures of language and early literacy

Name Description Validity & Reliability Skills Assessed

PPVT-

III

A matrix of four pictures is

shown to the child who is

then asked to choose the one

that matches the spoken word

provided by the examiner.

Test-retest reliabilities of

.91and .92 for Form A and

B, respectively, for

children aged from 2

years, 6 months to 5 years,

11 months.

Receptive

vocabulary.

HPNT Test consists of 100 line

drawings of noun objects

familiar to both children and

adults.

Normed on a sample of

275 children from 11

primary schools in

Australia. Test occasion

correlation with expressive

vocabulary is .83.

English speaking

(expressive)

vocabulary.

PAT Three subtests used are:

Rhyme Detection, Word

Completion-Syllables and

Phonemes, and Alphabet

Knowledge.

Norm-referenced test and

has a test and retest

reliability of these subtests

are .80, .58, .71, and .86

respectively.

Phonological skills

that predict 4-7 year-

old children’s early

reading progress.

CAP Assess a variety of print

conventions and concepts,

including book orientation,

print directionality, reading

vocabulary (e.g., letter,

word), concepts of lowercase

and uppercase letters, and the

function of punctuation

marks.

Test and retest reliability

for the Texas sample

ranged from .73 to .89.

Test reliability with print

awareness is .83.

Print awareness

skills.

Note. PPVT-III=Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Ed. (Dunn & Dunn, 1998); HPNT=

Hundred Picture Naming Test (Fischer & Glenister, 1992); PAT=Phonological Abilities Test

(Muter, Hulme, & Snowling, 1997); CAP=Concepts about Print (Clay, 2002).

Page 21: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

20

Table 2

Pre to post intervention scores on language and literacy measures

Pre Test (n=80) Post Test (n=77)

Control

(n=23)

DR

(n=28)

DR+PR

(n=29)

Control

(n=23)

DR

(n=26)

DR+PR

(n=28)

Measures M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

PPVT-IIIA/B 61.78

(15.32)

66.61

(16.96)

69.38

(15.89)

73.04

(19.08)

80.46

(15.33)

82.93

(14.87)

HNPT*

[Control< DR

& DR+PR]

79.83

(9.12)

80.12

(9.62)

81.17

(8.89)

82.0

(7.16)

85.69

(6.62)

86.25

(6.91)

PAT-Rhyme*

[Control< DR

& DR+PR]

6.04

(3.31)

6.89

(2.89)

7.14

(2.81)

5.65

(3.61)

8.12

(2.39)

8.21

(1.95)

PAT- Word

Completion

12.22

(3.5)

13.96

(2.17)

13.04

(4.24)

13.39

(3.76)

15.08

(1.23)

14.29

(2.42)

PAT-Alphabet

Knowledge

19.57

(7.94)

18.17

(7.21)

16.38

(7.82)

22.91

(5.09)

24.39

(2.04)

20.53

(7.57)

CAP*

[Control< DR

and DR+PR]

2.50

(1.62)

3.23

(2.42)

3.50

(2.29)

6.35

(2.10)

10.33

(2.30)

10.54

(2.58)

Note. Significance indicated at *p < .05 for ANCOVAs for intervention effects at Time 2

(post) with pre test scores entered as covariates.

 

 

 

Page 22: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

21

Table 3

Pre to follow-up scores on language and literacy measures

Pre Test (n=80) Follow-up (n=72)

Control

(n=23)

DR

(n=28)

DR+PR

(n=29)

Control

(n=22)

DR

(n=24)

DR+PR

(n=26)

Measures M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

PPVT-IIIA/B 61.78

(15.32)

66.61

(16.96)

69.38

(15.89)

78.86

(11.85

80.50

(15.3)

81.85

(12.35)

HNPT 79.83

(9.12)

80.12

(9.62)

81.17

(8.89)

85.73

(4.98)

86.79

(6.42)

86.96

(7.03)

PAT-Rhyme 6.04

(3.31)

6.89

(2.89)

7.14

(2.81)

7.41

(2.70)

8.04

(2.55)

8.19

(2.15)

PAT- Word

Completion

12.22

(3.5)

13.96

(2.17)

13.04

(4.24)

13.27

(3.19)

14.17

(2.41)

14.15

(1.93)

PAT-Alphabet

Know-ledge

19.57

(7.94)

18.17

(7.21)

16.38

(7.82)

23.64

(4.49)

24.83

(1.79)

21.54

(7.14)

CAP*

[Control< DR

& DR+PR]

2.50

(1.62)

3.23

(2.42)

3.50

(2.29)

9.73

(1.58)

11.19

(1.86)

11.21

(2.10)

Note. Significance indicated at *p < .05 for ANCOVAs for intervention effects at Time 3

(follow-up) with pre test scores entered as covariates.

 

Page 23: c Consult author(s) regarding copyright matterseprints.qut.edu.au/67931/1/Sim_et_al_2013_ECDC... · 2021. 1. 8. · Children’s Early Literacy Skills Abstract Parental reading to

22

Highlights 

Efficacy of shared reading strategies used by parents at home on children’s literacy skills was studied.

A pragmatic RCT design was used with pre, post and follow-up assessment.

Dialogic Reading and Dialogic Reading with Print Referencing conditions were effective on three outcome measures.

One intervention effect remained at 3-month follow-up.

Evidence for the efficacy of a short-term low-cost intervention was found.