5002147

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    1/10

    RONALD C. CURHAN*

    Retai l price, newspaper advertising, display space, and display locationquali ty were tested at two levels for selected fruits and vegetables accordingto a fractiona l facto rial research design in four larg e superm arkets. The resultingimpact on rates of sale was analyzed for four classes of items; hard fruit,cooking vegetables, salad vegetables, and soft fruit.

    The Effects of Merchandising and TemporaryPromotional Activities on the Sales of FreshFruits and Vegetables in Supermarkets

    Although food chains maintain historical sales rec-ords and have conducted countless evaluations ofspecific merchandising activities, it is doubtful thatmore than a few legitimate experiments have attemptedsystematic measurement of the influence of variouscombinations of merchandising and temporary promo-tional activities on sales of supermarket products orclasses of prod ucts. The effect of such key promotionalvariables as temporary retail price reductions andadvertising, and of such merchandising variables asincremental display space and quality of display loca-tion, has not often been quantified, nor have therelationships among these variables been determined.Measures of the impact of these variables on salesand profits are essential to the developm ent of com pu-ter-based marketing models and management deci-sion-information systems.The research reported here was undertaken to pro-vide such measures. Sixteen selected fresh fruits andvegetables were subjected to merchandising andpromotional treatments varied according to a fractionalfactorial re search d esign. This design permits extrapo-

    lation of results to products and to combinations ofvariables other than those tested.Since it is acknowledged among supermarket man-agers that different classes of products respond dif-

    *Ronald C. Curhan is Associate Professor of Marketing, BostonUniversity. This research was supported by funds from the COS-MOS Project managed by Case and Company for the NationalAssociation of Food Chains and the Marketing Science Institute.

    ferently to merchandising and promotional activities,four categories of merchandise were selected forstudy: hard fruit, cooking vegetables, salad vege tables,and soft fruit. Physical characteristics generally varymore across these product categories than they dowithin these categories. That is, potatoes and onionsare more alike in their merchandising and physicalcharacteristics than are potatoes and lettuce or onionsand cherries. Hard fruit and cooking vegetables, w ithinseason, are considered almost "staples," whereassalad vegetables and soft fruit generally are moreperishable; vary more in day-to-day quality, avail-ability, and price; and require more care in handlingand refrigeration. These four categories account forvirtually all of the fresh fruit and vegetables usuallysold in supermarkets.In addition to noting that response to merchandisingand promotional activities varies for different produ ctcategories, supermarket produce m erchandisers repo rtthat differences in response may also be partly attrib-utable to specific product characteristics such as:volume classwhether a product is a relatively fast-

    selling item or a relatively slow-selling item withinits merchandise category; price classwhether aproduct sells for a relatively high price per customerpurch ase, or whether it sells for a relatively low p rice;and seasonality classwhether a product is availablethroughout most of the year, or whether availabilityis limited to a specific season. To consider all combi-nations of these 3 product characteristics within eachmerchandise category at only 2 levels would requiretesting of 8 (i.e., 2 x 2 x 2) produc ts for each ca tegoryor 32 products in all.286

    Journal of Marketing ResearcVol. XI (August 1974), 286-94

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    2/10

    EFFECTS OF MERC HAN DISING AN D PRO MOTION S O N SALES 28 7Four merchandising and temporary promotionalvariables were determined to be of interest: displayspace, retail price, newspaper advertising, and displaylocation quality. Each of these variables was testedat two levels: "normal" and "featured." A featureddisplay consisted of a space allocation of at least 200%of the space allocated to an item the period immediately

    preceding a test period. Price promotion m eant a priceat least 10% lower than the prevailing "no rm al" price,with "normal" price being determined on the basisof recent pricing within the chain, as well as competi-tors' prevailing prices. An item was considered "ad-ver tise d" if it was included as one of the three produceproducts customarily featured by the chain in itsweekly newspaper advertisement. Finally, prime loca-tions were designated for each test store on the basisof researcher observation of store traffic patterns andconsultation with store personnel. Generally, separatefloor tables, ends of large tables, and high-trafficpositions on wall coun ters qualified as prime locations;To test each of these merchandising and temporarypromotional variables, even at only two levels, wouldrequire 16 (i.e ., 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 ) sep arate te sts ofeach item. Sufficient items to test product charac-teristics for the 4 merchandise categories would meanthat 4 categories x 8 produc ts x 16 tests or 512separate observationswould be required, withoutany provision for replication. Clearly such a task isoperationally impractical. Supermarket managers are,understandably, reluctant to permit store researchwhich would restrict their freedom of activity soseverely.To overcome this constraint, a 7 ~ factorial experi-mental design was selected for this study. Fractional

    factorial designs yield information on certain variablesand combinations of va riables, but not on all variablesor combinations of variables. Particular fractionalfactorial designs can be chosen which will maximizethe number of variables and combinations of variablesfor which information of interest can be obtained.It should be noted that fractional factorial designs

    do not yield "clean" data for each variable or combi-nation of variables tested, but give results which are"confounded" with so-called higher-order interactionterms. However, by careful choice of a fractionalfactorial design, the limitations of this confoundingcan be minimized [6].For purpo ses of this study, it was decided to replicatea quarter factorial design within each merchandisecategory using four separate products. Products werecarefully chosen according to the fractional factorialdesign specifications. Thus, in each product category,two high-volume items and two low-volume items we reselected; two items were high priced and two werelower priced; and, two items were seasonal productsand two were nonseasonal.The particular combinations of characteristics re-quired for these three variables and the productsselected as experim ental test item s to fulfill thes erequirements are shown in Table 1. The test itemswere selected by the cooperating chain in consultationwith the researcher. Every effort was made to selectitems which met the design requirements within theconstraints of product availability and other opera-tional considerations.The extent to which the items selected actually fulfillthe design requirements may be determined from Table2. With the exception of pineapple, the volume classconditions are met within each product category,although other items with higher rates of sale mighthave been selected in preference to celery hearts inthe salad vegetable category. Price class complianceis excellent, with the exception of Hubbard squash.Compliance for seasonality class is satisfactory, al-though other items might have been chosen in prefer-ence to tomatoes and bananas. It is difficult to findfruits which are not seasonal, but D'Anjou pears andRed Delicious apples both have relatively long season s.In summary, the items chosen met the test require-ments, although each selection was not necessarilyoptimal.

    The particular fractional factorial research designTable 1

    REQUIRED PRODUCT CHARACTERISTICS AND TEST ITEMS CHOSEN

    VolumeclassLowHighHighLow

    Requiredproduct characteristicsPriceclassLowLowHighHigh

    SeasonalityNonseasonal

    SeasonalNonseasonal

    Seasonal

    HardfruitLimesNavelorangesRedDeliciousapplesOtherapples

    Test itemsCookingvegetablesEggplant

    CornBag redpotatoesHubbardsquash

    SaladvegetablesRomainelettuceTomatoes

    CelloceleryRedcabbage

    SoftfruitD'AnjoupearsBananasPineapple

    Redgrapes"Northern Spy, Wealthy, and Jonathan varieties.

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    3/10

    288 JOURN AL OF MAR KETING RESEARCH, AU GU ST 1974Table 2

    TEST ITEM PRODUCT CHARACTERISTICS

    RequiredvolumeclassLowHighHighLow

    "Typical customer= 1 head, etc.).

    RequiredpriceclassLowLowHighHigh

    RequiredseasonalityclassNonseasonalSeasonalNonseasonalSeasonal

    a. Volume ClassAverage Number of Typical Customer Purchases" PerHardfruit

    Limes65Navel oranges785Red Delicious498Other apples293purchase size was determined

    CookingvegetablesEggplant24Co m216Potatoes215Squash110

    by researcher observationb. Price Class

    10,000 Customer TransactionsSaladvegetables

    Romaine80Tomatoes1,300Celery103Red cabbage20and differs by item (i.e., corn

    Average Retail Price per Typical Customer PurchaseHardfruitLimes150Navel oranges380Red Delicious580Other apples440

    HardfruitLimesNavel orangesRed DeliciousOther apples

    CookingvegetablesEggplant410Corn400Potatoes750Squash140

    c. Seasonality ClassCookingvegetablesEggplantCornPotatoesSquash

    SaladvegetablesRomaine250Tomatoes390Celery490Red cabbage420

    SaladvegetablesRomaineTomatoesCeleryRed cabbage

    SoftfruitD'Anjou pears162Bananas1,222Pineapple38Red grapes72

    = 5 ears; romaine lettuce

    SoftfruitD'Anjou pears400Bananas340Pineapple600Red grapes550

    SoftfruitD'Anjou pearsBananasPineappleRed grapes

    chosen also determined the specific combinations ofthe four variables which were tested (i.e., price,advertising, display space, and location quality). Eighttreatments were called for: four of which involveddisplay space as an independent variable, four whichinvolved price, four of which involved newspaperadvertisements, and four of which involved locationquality. Treatments were the same for each testproduct.Under conditions stipulated by the cooperatingchain, price and advertising had to be consistentchain-wide, whereas display space and location q ualitycould vary from store to store within a given testweek. The selection of four test stores m ade it possibleto replicate in separate pairs of stores two displayspace/location quality tests for a given item withina given week under controlled price/advertisingconditions.Given 4 items in each product category and 8separate merchandising and promotional combinations

    to be tested for each, 32 separate tests were thereforerequired in each category, and 128 were required forthe total experiment. Finally, because each test wasreplicated in a second store, a total of 256 individualobservations was obtained.In order to minimize the disruption of usual chainoperations, the testing was spread over a seven-monthperiod. Two categories were tested during the summerof 1972, and the other two were tested during thefollowing fall and winter. The critical variable wasadvertising. Eight advertisem ents w ere required du ringeach period. These were spread over 12 weeks duringthe summer, and over 17 weeks in the fall and winterperiod. This allowed for "breather" weeks to avoidholidays and permitted the chain considerable freedomto pursue its normal promotional strategy.

    Test treatment compliance involving pricing andadvertising at the chain level was exact in 53 of 64instances, or 83%. Test treatment compliance involv-ing display space and location quality at the store

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    4/10

    EFFECTS OF MERC HAN DISING AN D PR OMOT IONS O N SALES 289

    level was exact in 218 out of 256 instances, or 85%.Combined com pliance at both the chain and store levelswas exact in 182 of 256 instances, or ml Wo of thespecified instances.It is the researcher's judgment that sufficient in-formation was available to establish reasonable valuesfor essentially all the instances in which actual per-formance differed from specified design. For example,each test combination having been replicated in twostores, acceptable substitute information often wasavailable from the second test store when compliancewas less than perfect in the first store. Second,although the test design may not have been adheredto for a particular produc t in a particular w eek, re sultsof similar promotional activity were sometimes avail-able from w eeks not designated as test weeks. Finally,in many instances where compliance deviated fromspecified conditions, the differences were minor andare judged not to have biased overall results. Effectsof individual observation s are dissipated in the proce ssof factorial analysis so that errors of the magnitudelikely to have occurred are tolerable and probablydo not distort the findings reported.

    The experiment was designed to yield informationon the effects of temporary m erchandising and prom o-tional activity for certain types of products withinspecified categories of products. For example, it washoped that the experiment might lead to conclusionsof the following type: that the effect of a pricereduction is greater when executed with increased spacethan it is under conditions of normal space allocationsfor high-volume items within the salad vegetablecategory. Such a conclusion would, of course, ac-knowledge that the effect of a price reduction mightwell be positive under both increased and normal spacecondition s. The experim ent was not designed to permitdetailed analysis of the effects of price, advertising,display space, or location quality per se on specificfruits or vegetables. Limes, for example, served torepresent all low-volume, low-priced, nonseasonalproducts within the hard fruit category. Individualtest products were not subjected to sufficient treat-ments to permit systematic analysis of the impact onsales attributable to merchandising and promotionalvariables.

    Sales data for each test item were determined fromspecial inventory counts and examination of deliveryrecords for each store. Results are reported as ratiosof observed test week movement per thousand cus-tomer transactions to ' 'normal average weekly move-ment" per thousand customer transactions for eachstore. The determination of this normal average weeklymovement was to some extent subjective, sincearbitrary computations tend to mask trend movementsand other aberrations. However, the basis for normal-ization was identical for each store for each test week ,with rare exception. The ratios obtained for eachidentical pair of test conditions were then averaged.

    These averages were analyzed as described in thenext section to obtain measures of the effects attrib-utable to different combinations of the factors tested.FACTORIAL ANALYSIS

    Factorial analysis estimates the effect of each factorand combination of factors tested. Customarily, lowercase letters are used to represent experimental treat-ments, whereas capital letters stand for estimates ofeffects. For example, in this study treatment d repre-sents the test condition where display (factor D) isat high level (i.e ., by definition 200% of normal space),and all other factors are at low level. D stands forthe estimate of the increm ental gain in yield attributableto the presence of factor D averaged over all levelsof other factors. Note that whether a specific testcondition is designated as high or low is immaterial.If, for exa mple, we obtain D = 10 under one condition,we would obtain D = -10 for the other. Also, thatwhen a complete factorial is analyzed according toYates's algorithm [4], the treatment a row will yieldan estimate for effect A, the treatment ab row willyield an estimate for AB, and so on.

    In a fractional factorial analysis each estimate willcontain a number of terms, the number dependingon the particular fractional replicate. This study isa quarter factorial, hence each estimated effect con-tains four terms. The sign of these terms and themanner in which factors are combined within eachof these terms is specifically a consequence of theparticular fractional factorial chosen. Moreover, whenfractional factorial data are analyzed according toYates's algorithm, the estimates are not generallycorresponding in capital letter to the small letteredtreatment. For example, in this study treatment g (i.e.,high level of location quality and low level of allother factors) yields the estimated effect D+ ABCD + EFG - ABCDEFG. Terms such as+ ABCD and -ABCDEFG, so-called higher-orderinteraction terms, customarily are set equal to zerobecause they probably ar e zero; they are certainlyincomprehensible and devoid of meaning in practicalterms. Treatment combinations and estimated effectsfor factors and combinations of factors are shownin Table 3. To conserve space, degrees of freedom(1 in each instance except for error terms) and meansquare columns are omitted. Levels of significancewere determined by testing F-ratios against tablevalues for (1 , 32) d.f.

    ANALYSIS OF RESULTSOf major interest are the so-called "main effects"attributable to display space, price, advertising, andlocation quality (e.g ., - D+ EFG, E- DFG, F- DEG,and G - DEF\ where the three-letter terms are setequal to zero on the assumption that they, like otherhigher order terms, essentially are zero). These maineffects are the average o ver all factors, including high

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    5/10

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    6/10

    EFFECTS OF M E R C H A N D I S I N G AND PRO M O TI O NS ON SALES 291and low levels of volume class, price class, andseasonality class. In addition, estimates of so-called"Interaction Effects," that is the effect of displayspace, price, advertising, and location qu ality at highand low levels of volume class, price class, andseasonality class averaged over all other factors canbe obtained from those two-letter terms which arepaired with three-letter terms where, again, the three-letter terms are set equal to zero (e.g., - BD-i- ACD,-CD-\-ABD, AD-BCD, BE-ACE, CE-ABE,-AE+BCE, BE-ACF, CF-ABF, -AF+BCF,BG- ACG,CG-ABC, and -AG-h BCG).Estimates involving a single-letter term and a dou-ble-letter term, a combination which cannot beseparated, were not of primary interest and so know-ingly were compromised in the research design. Forexample, it is irrelevant to consider whether low-volume products sell less than high-volume productsunder all conditions, as test products were selectedto meet this criterion. Estimates involving two-letterterms paired with other two-letter terms also areimpossible to interpret. In spite of this ambiguity,the experiment was conducted as described becauseit yielded estimates of effects for variables of crucialinterest, and, relative to the information sought, thedesign was efficient. As previously noted, explanationof all terms would have required an experiment atleast four times the size of the one executed, anundertaking beyond the tolerance of the cooperatingchain.Display Space

    The study demonstrates that bonus space increasessales for all categories of products (Table 4). ForTable 4

    PERCENT INCREASE IN UNIT SALES FOR "HIGH" LEVELSOF DISPLAY (100% BONUS SPACE)

    Productcharacteristics

    Average(Main effect)High volume

    Low volumeHigh priceLow priceSeasonalNonseasonal

    Hardfruit4 4 "20'''=68"49 3 9"5 8"30

    Cookingvegetables

    5937"81 60"5 8655 3 "

    Saladvegetables

    28 14 "4 2"26"302 5 "31

    Softfruit49"38"60"74-"24a,b5642 "Main effect significant at the .25 level or better.

    ''First order interaction significant at the .25 level or better.'Calculated from Table 3 as follows:

    -D+EFG =D =AD-BCD =

    AD =D+AD =

    -44.344+0-23.9(-24)-044+(-24) = 20

    Read as: The estimated effect of bonus display space for fastselling hard fruit is 20.

    example, doubling display space for hard fruit in-creased sales by 44%. This is not to say that increasedspace will increase sales of every hard fruit by 44%,however, as this value is an estimate of the categoryaverage which may or may not hold for individualitems. Also, the experiment tested only normal spacecompared with double that amount of space. Thequestion as to whether ra tes of sale increase uniformlyas space is increased is not addressed. Thus, it ispossible that a 50% space increase would not yieldany sales increase, and that a 200% increase wouldnot increase sales appreciably more than did the100%increase. Other studies (see, for example, [2]) suggestthat space increases must be noticeable before theywill stimulate significant sales changes, and that sooneror later sales reach an upper limit beyond which theyno longer respond to incremental space allocations.

    Slow-selling items are more prone to the effectsof change in display space than are fast-selling items,although the difference is significant only for hardfruit. Indications are that cooking vegetables, saladvegetables, and soft fruit also respond in this fashion,although the results are not statistically significant.Strict interpretation of the findings limits conclusionsto the general statement that the effect of displayspace is positive and does not vary by volume classfor these categories. It may be reasoned that normalspace allocations for low-volume items are often belowthe threshold level necessary to attract consumernotice, whereas operational considerations ensure thatspace allocations are above this level for high-volumeproducts. Moreover, incremental space allocated tolow-volume items may more frequently be perceivedas unusual by shoppers and thus attract attention asevidence of promotion.The effect of an increase in space is larger for ahigh-priced soft fruit than for a low-priced soft fruit,and for seasonal hard fruit than for nonseasonal hardfruit, although this latter finding is not statisticallysignificant. Note that the alternative interpretation ofthese interactions, that is, holding display space con-stant and attributing the effect observed to a changefrom a low to a high volume class, a change of priceclass, or a change of seasonality class is irrationalas these factors cannot vary for a given item.Price

    Contrary to generally held trade op inion, the impactof price reductions was not statistically significantexcept for soft fruit (Table 5) and, even in this instan ce,the finding needs qualification as estimates for signifi-cant interactions were large relative to the estimatefor the main effect (i.e., from Table 3, E = 18, AE= 24 and CE= -17; hence the estimated effect fora price reduction is 18 24, or 42 at high volumeand -6 at low volume. Likewise the effect for sea-sonality is 18 ( -17 ) , or 1 for seasonal and 35 fornonseasonal soft fruit). U nder this condition, e stimates

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    7/10

    292 JOURNAL OF AURKETING RESEARCH, AUGU ST 197Table 5

    PERCENT INCREASE IN UN IT SALES FOR "H IG H " LEVELSOF PRICE (1 0% REDUCTION)

    ProductcharacteristicsAverage

    (Main effect)High volumeLow volumeHigh priceLow priceSeasonalNonseasonal

    Hardfruit1119- 7- 1 3 "15"

    Cookingvegetables724- 1 011386

    Saladvegetables9126135- 119

    Softfruit1842'"- 6 ' "24121'"35. .b

    Main effect significant at the .25 level or better."First order interaction significant at the .25 level or better.of the main effect, even though significant, must beinterpreted with caution [4, p. 255]. Price reductionshad a large effect for fast-selling soft fruit. Perhapsin spite of being less widely popular, the slow-sellingsoft fruits have particularly " loy al" purc hase rs, whosedecisions to buy are not significantly affected by price.The effect of a price reduction did not vary signifi-cantly by price class within any merchandise category.Nominally positive or negative values suggest thatthe effect of price reductions is essentially zero, oreven negative, for seasonal products, whereas theyprobably are significantly positive for non seaso nal softfruit. In other words, the novelty associated withseasonal products may be more important than priceper se. Hard fruit, cooking vegetables, and saladvegetables may be perceived as categories where,assuming satisfactory quality, a purchase will be madewith small regard for retail price. Within these catego-ries, even if products are on sale, the consum er seem sdisinclined to purchase a greater quantity than origi-nally intended. High-priced soft fruit purchases areof a much more discretionary charac ter, with purchasedecisions more subject to in-store influence. A per-ceived bargain probably consti tutes such an influence.Finally, the absolute price level for high-priced soft

    Table 6PERCENT INCREASE IN UN IT SALES FOR "H IG H " LEVELS

    OF ADVERTISING (FEATURED ITEM)

    ProductcharacteristicsAverage(Main effect)High volumeLow volumeHigh priceLow priceSeasonalNonseasonal

    Hardfruit33 2838 50"16"39 27

    Cookingvegetables8971107116"62"128-"5 0 "

    Saladvegetables518- 8- 818- 1 0 "20"

    Softfruit312- 6- 1 01624

    Main effect significant at the .25 level or better."First order interaction significant at the .25 level or better.

    fruit is relatively high, which suggests that savingmay be more important in this product category.Advertising

    The effect of advertising is significant for hard fruiand cooking vegetables (Table 6). Advertising has anegligible effect on sales of salad vegetables and soffruit. This suggests that while the purchase decisionfor the former two categories is influenced by advertising, the purchase decision for salad vegetables andsoft fruit essentially is an in-store decision based onthe appearance, quality, and value of the productThis is not to say that the cumulative effect ofadvertising soft fruit and salad vegetables does nomake a valuable contribution to store image. Thisexperiment does suggest, however, that sales of produce staples respond more directly to advertisingstimuli than do discretionary purchase i tems.

    The effect of advertising is positive and significanboth for high- and low-volume h ard fruit an d, esp ecially, for cooking vegetables, although the impact igreater for slow-sell ing products in these categoriesDisplays of low-volume products ordinarily aredwarfed by larger displays of fast-selling productwithin these categories. Advertising probably serveto call attention to these otherwise "invisible" products.Results by price also are m ixed. The repo rted effecof advertising was greater for high-priced productwithin the hard fruit and cooking vegetable categoriesbut still positive for low-priced products. The reverstends to be true for soft fruit and salad vegetablesalthough the effects are not significant.

    The effect of advertising was very large for season aproducts in the cooking vegetables category, althoughsignificant and positive for nonseasonal cooking vegetables and for seasonal and nonseasonal hard fruias well. Cooking vegetables are relatively "invisibleproducts" for which advertising probably serves toattract consumer attention.Care must be exercised in drawing conclusions abouthe main effect of advertising from the results fosalad vegetables, since this is another instance w herealthough the estimate of the interaction is significantthe main effect is not. The finding that advertisingis not significant either for seasonal or nonseasonasoft fruit again supports the theory that customer"s ho p" th is depar tment , ra ther than depend on advertising as a major source for purchase information.

    Location QualityEssentially the same pattern of results prevails folocation quality as for advertising (Table 7); howeverin this instanc e it is more difficult to relate the findingto practical experience. What these f indings seem tosuggest is that customers will shop the entire salad

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    8/10

    EFFECTS OF MERCH AND ISING A ND PROMO TIONS O N SALES 293Table 7

    PERCENT INCREASE IN UN IT SALES FOR " H IG H " LEVELSOF LOCATION QUALITY (PRIME LOCATION)

    ProductcharacteristicsAverage(Main effect)High volumeLow volumeHigh priceLow priceSeasonalNonseasonal

    Hardfruit26213146"6-"44,b

    8 "

    Cookingvegetables4815"8 1"56407026

    Saladvegetables- 8- 2- 1 4- 7- 9- 1 4- 2

    Softfruit119"- 1 7 "79- 3 0 "32"

    Main effect significant at the .25 level or better."First order interaction significant at the .25 level or better.

    vegetable and soft fruit sections without special in-ducement, because their shopping strategy is basedon visual perusal of these categories. However, be-cause hard fruit and cooking vegetables are viewedas staples, customers venture into display areas forthese products only insofar as they have specificpurchase intentions. Nevertheless, if items withinthese categories are given high traffic locations, cus-tomers' attention is drawn to them and increased salesresult. Location quality has a larger effect on salesof low-volume than high-volume cooking vegetables.This finding further substantiates the observation thatdisplay is an important factor influencing sales forotherwise "inv isible" low-volume cooking vegetables.

    Results imply that location quality has a positiveimpact for high-volume soft fruit and a negative impactfor low-volume soft fruit. A gain, any stateme nts a boutthe main effect of location quality must be viewedwith caution. This, in turn, may be interpreted asimplying that, although their shopping strategy is basedon in-store visual perusal, most customers take "theeasy way out" and terminate their search processwhen confronted with purchase opportunities which"satisfice." A smaller number of customers, on theother hand, more systematically "s h op " the soft fruitsection and are less influenced by display location.The effect of location quality is greater for high-priced and seasonal hard fruit and probably not signifi-cant for low-priced and nonseasonal hard fruit. Theformer classes of products probably are not normally

    sought out by customers, and so may be expectedto benefit from display ex posu re. The effect of locationquality is negative for seasonal soft fruit and positivefor nonseasonal soft fruit, although the net effect isnegligible. Since most supermarkets prominantly dis-play seasonal soft fruit, nonseasonal items probablylack visibility in a category where visual shoppingis important. Also, we would expect results to besimilar for display space and location quality, but theydiffer for soft fruit even though they generally areparallel for other merchandise categories.

    SUMMARYThis experiment has produced quantified data forcertain variables for which numerical information hasnot hitherto been available. Further, it has suggestedcertain significant relationships among these variableswhich have not previously been apparent. Thus, the

    study can reasonably claim to have served its intendedpurpose of developing information useful to the for-mulation of heuristic decision rules, which, in turn,are an important prerequisite to the development ofcomputer-based management decision-informationsystems.In addition, this study demonstrates the usefulnessof factorial experimental designs to marketing re-search. Although cited in texts on marketing researchand experimentation (see, for example, [1,3,5]), fac-torial designs have been little used in practice. Inpart, this may be attributed to the large number oftreatmen ts called for by all but the most simple factorialdesigns. However, factorial designs are especiallyappropriate to marketing research because they yieldinformation on the effects of combinations of vari-ables, as well as on specific variables. Fractionalfactorial designs, although they sacrifice some detail,reduce the number of required treatments while stillproviding measures of selected intera ctions. They m aybe especially useful when employed as the first stepof a two-stage research proce ss consisting of a " roug hcut" to identify important effects and subsequentresearch to "fine tune" suggested results.

    Certain findings of this study may have immediatelyapplicable operational importance to supermarketmanagers. For example, perhaps advertising effortshould be concentrated on hard fruits and cookingvegetables and withheld from salad vegetables andsoft fruits. Indeed, the behavioral similarities of thetest variables vis-a-vis hard fruit and cooking vegeta-bles on the one hand, and salad vegetables and softfruit on the other, have been noted throughout.However, some words of caution are necessary atthis point. First, although this experiment was con-ducted in large supermarkets over an extended periodof time, the usual care should be taken in attemptingto generalize on the basis of a single experiment. Theresults may be unique to the specific circumstancestested, although this is judged to be very unlikely.Second, the impact of greater or lesser rates of

    sale on profit is indeterminate without considerationof direct product profit contribution for specific prod-ucts and, indeed, for categories of products to includesubstitutes and compliments. The merchandisingstrategies suggested by results of this study shouldnot be implemented without due attention to such directproduct profit contribution analysis.REFERENCES

    1. Banks, Seymour. Experimentation in Marketing. NewYork: McGraw Hill, 1965.

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    9/10

    294 JOURN AL OF AAARKETING RESEARCH, AUG UST 1972. Brown , William M. and W. T. Tucker. "Vanishing Shelf Chapter 10.Space ," Atlantic Economic Review, 9 (October 1961), 5. Gree n, Paul E. and Donald S.Tull. Research/or Marfeefin9-13. Decisions, second edition. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Pren3. Cox, Keith K. and Ben M. Enis. Experimentation for tice-Hall, 1970.Marketing Decisions. Scranton, Pa.: International Text- 6. Holland, Charles W. and David W. Craven s. "Fractio nabook , 1969. Factorial Experimental Designs in Marketing Re sea rch ,4. Davies, O. L., ed. Th e Design and Analysis of Industrial Journal of Marketing Research, 10 (August 1973), 270 -Experiments, second edition. New York: Hafner, 1956,

  • 8/8/2019 5002147

    10/10