2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

  • Upload
    jose

  • View
    222

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    1/13

    See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/8931460

    Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of 

    Attention: An MEG Study 

     ARTICLE  in  JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE · DECEMBER 2003

    Impact Factor: 4.69 · DOI: 10.1162/089892903322598148 · Source: PubMed

    CITATIONS

    65

    DOWNLOADS

    2

    VIEWS

    99

    4 AUTHORS, INCLUDING:

     Yury Shtyrov

    Aarhus University

    92 PUBLICATIONS  2,873 CITATIONS 

    SEE PROFILE

    Friedemann Pulvermüller

    Freie Universität Berlin

    234 PUBLICATIONS  11,872 CITATIONS 

    SEE PROFILE

    Risto J Ilmoniemi

    Aalto University

    304 PUBLICATIONS  15,734 CITATIONS 

    SEE PROFILE

    Available from: Risto J Ilmoniemi

    Retrieved on: 15 September 2015

    http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Risto_Ilmoniemi?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_7http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Risto_Ilmoniemi?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_4http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Risto_Ilmoniemi?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_4http://www.researchgate.net/institution/Aalto_University?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_6http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yury_Shtyrov?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_4http://www.researchgate.net/institution/Freie_Universitaet_Berlin?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_6http://www.researchgate.net/publication/8931460_Grammar_Processing_Outside_the_Focus_of_Attention_An_MEG_Study?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_3http://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_1http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Risto_Ilmoniemi?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_7http://www.researchgate.net/institution/Aalto_University?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_6http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Risto_Ilmoniemi?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_5http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Risto_Ilmoniemi?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_4http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Friedemann_Pulvermueller?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_7http://www.researchgate.net/institution/Freie_Universitaet_Berlin?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_6http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Friedemann_Pulvermueller?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_5http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Friedemann_Pulvermueller?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_4http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yury_Shtyrov?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_7http://www.researchgate.net/institution/Aarhus_University?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_6http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yury_Shtyrov?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_5http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yury_Shtyrov?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_4http://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_1http://www.researchgate.net/publication/8931460_Grammar_Processing_Outside_the_Focus_of_Attention_An_MEG_Study?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_3http://www.researchgate.net/publication/8931460_Grammar_Processing_Outside_the_Focus_of_Attention_An_MEG_Study?enrichId=rgreq-53a343a1-8107-4ef2-b45e-fb81318e0369&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzg5MzE0NjA7QVM6MjczMzExNTM1OTg4NzM2QDE0NDIxNzM5NTY2Nzk%3D&el=1_x_2

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    2/13

    Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention:an MEG Study 

     Yury Shtyrov 1, Friedemann Pulvermüller 1, Risto Näätänen3,4,and Risto J. Ilmoniemi2,4

     Abstract

    &   To address the cerebral processing of grammar, we used

     whole-head high-density magnetoencephalography to record

    the brain’s magnetic fields elicited by grammatically correct

    and incorrect auditory stimuli in the absence of directed

    attention to the stimulation. The stimuli were minimal short

    phrases of the Finnish language differing only in one single

    phoneme (word-final inflectional affix), which rendered them

    as either grammatical or ungrammatical. Acoustic and lexicaldifferences were controlled for by using an orthogonal design

    in which the phoneme’s effect on grammaticality was inverted.

     We found that occasional syntactically incorrect stimuli elicited

    larger mismatch negativity (MMN) responses than correct

    phrases. The MMN was earlier proposed as an index of pre-

    attentive automatic speech processing. Therefore, its modu-

    lation by grammaticality under nonattend conditions suggests

    that early syntax processing in the human brain may take place

    outside the focus of attention. Source analysis (single–dipole

    models and minimum-norm current estimates) indicated

    grammaticality dependent differential activation of the leftsuperior temporal cortex suggesting that this brain structure

    may play an important role in such automatic grammar 

    processing.   &

    INTRODUCTION

    For over a century, the processing of language in the

    brain has remained one of the most intriguing issues in

    science. Although its many aspects have been tackled by numerous studies, little is still known of how our centralnervous system goes about processing grammar. Gram-

    mar is one of the main intrinsic properties of the human

    language. The very presence of a grammatical system

    distinguishes our ability of verbal communication fromall signaling systems used by animals. Earlier investiga-tions of the neurophysiology of syntactic processing

    (e.g., Hagoort, Brown, & Groothusen, 1993; Osterhout

    & Holcomb, 1992; Neville, Nicol, Barss, Forster, & Gar-

    rett, 1991) have led to important insights into this issue.

    Three major syntax-related phenomena have been

    described on the basis of the brain’s electric responsesto language stimuli. The first one is the so-called early 

    left anterior negativity (ELAN) occurring, with a latency 

    of   ¹125 msec, in response to function words1 whose

    placement in a phrase violates sentence structure rules

    (Neville et al., 1991). Secondly, grammatically relatedfrontal negativities with longer latencies (over 250 msec)

    have been found, for example, for different morpholog-

    ical or syntactic errors (Gunter, Friederici, & Schriefers,

    2000; Münte, Schiltz, & Kutas, 1998). Finally, a late

    positive shift (often termed P600), reaching its maxi-

    mum at   ¹600 msec and maximal at centro-parietal

    recording sites, has been considered the most robustneurophysiological response to ungrammatical sen-

    tences because it could be observed in many linguisticexperiments (for review, see Osterhout, McLaughlin, &

    Bersick, 1997; Osterhout & Hagoort, 1999).

     When studying the processing of grammar in the brain,

    it seems important to control for a number of issues. For 

    instance, in such studies, subjects are typically asked toattend to presented sentences (e.g., Friederici, Wang,

    Herrmann, Maess, & Oertel, 2000; Osterhout & Swinney,

    1993; Neville et al., 1991); often, the task is to judge the

    sentence grammaticality. In such cases, as attention is

    required, one cannot be sure to what extent the regis-tered responses are influenced by brain correlates of 

    attention rather than by the language-related activity as

    such. Attention-related phenomena are known to mod-

    ulate a variety of the brain’s evoked responses in a

    substantial span of time after stimulus onset and to

    involve a number of brain structures including thoseclose to, or overlapping with, the core language areas

    (see, e.g., Yamasaki, LaBar, & McCarthy, 2002; Yantis

    et al., 2002; Escera, Alho, Winkler, & Näätänen, 1998;

    Tiitinen, May, & Näätänen, 1997; Woods, Alho, & Algazi,

    1993; Woods, Knight, & Scabini, 1993; Alho, 1992; Picton

    & Hillyard, 1974). It also appears possible that subjectspay more attention to incorrect sentences as they try to

    1 Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit,

    Cambridge, UK,   2 Biomag Laboratory, Helsinki University 

    Central Hospital,   3 University of Helsinki,   4 Helsinki Brain

    Research Center 

    ©   2003 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15:8, pp. 1195–1206 

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    3/13

    make some sense of them or that they process grammat-

    ical and ungrammatical items using different strategies.

    These different strategies and attention variation may 

    find their reflection in the event-related measures, over-

    lapping with true syntax-related activity. Therefore, lim-iting the attention-related effects seems to be necessary 

    at this stage for further investigation of the neural

    processing of syntax.

     A methodologically important aspect in such experi-ments is stimulus selection. Grammatically correct andincorrect phrases would inevitably differ in many other 

    features: e.g., sound onset times and durations in

    acoustic stimulation, as well as visual geometry and

    overall luminance in visual tasks, would change when

     words or affixes are added, removed, or relocated to

    modulate grammaticality. Differences even in basicphysical features may lead to differential brain activation

    (Korth & Nguyen, 1997; Näätänen & Picton, 1987) that

    could in principle overlap with or be misinterpreted as

    language-related effects. For example, it was shown that

     word-elicited responses are highly dependent on the word length (Osterhout, Bersick, & McKonnon, 1997;

     Assadollahi & Pulvermüller, 2003), suggesting the ne-

    cessity to control for such factors. Furthermore, the

    physical stimulus parameters are of crucial importance

    for obtaining ERP effects of syntactic processes (Gunter,Friederici, & Hahne, 1999). It seems most logical to us

    to apply an experimental design in which physical

    differences between the grammatical and ungrammati-

    cal items are controlled for by presenting the same

    physical contrasts to the subjects  both   in ungrammatical

    and grammatical contexts; this sort of an orthogonaldesign would help to disentangle purely syntactic effectsfrom those related to the physical stimulus features.

    So, in sum, it appears crucial to limit the influence of 

    attention and different processing strategies on the

    brain response to grammatical and ungrammaticalstrings as well as to apply maximal control over stimulus

    features using a counterbalanced stimulus design. Con-

    trolling for these issues simultaneously in a single study 

    can be a challenging task, which, as to our knowledge,

    has not been realized so far. The present study repre-sents an attempt to achieve this goal.

    Here, we addressed brain activity related to grammat-ical processing in the absence of directed attention

    towards stimuli and without an active task which would

    possibly invite subjects to invent a strategy for dealing

     with unusual stimuli. We used a counterbalanced stim-ulus design, which allowed us to control for effects of 

    the physical and lexical parameters of the stimuli. To

    gain high temporal resolution necessary in such an

    experiment, we opted for multichannel magnetoence-

    phalographic (MEG) recording of brain responses, which can also provide information about the spatial

    location of the registered activity.

     We utilized the so-called mismatch negativity (MMN),a unique indicator of automatic cerebral processing of 

    acoustic stimuli, which can be used to investigate the

    cerebral processing of speech and language (Shtyrov,

    Kujala, Palva, Ilmoniemi, & Näätänen, 2000; Shtyrov &

    Pulvermüller, 2002a, 2002b; Näätänen & Winkler, 1999;

    Näätänen, 2001; Pulvermüller et al., 2001). MMN, withits major sources of activity in both the supratemporal

    and frontal cortices, is a brain response elicited by rare

    (deviant) stimuli occasionally presented in a sequence

    of frequent (standard) stimuli (Opitz, Rinne, Mecklinger, von Cramon, & Schröger, 2002; Picton, Alain, Otten,Ritter, & Achim, 2000; Alho, 1995). It can be registered

    as a negative deflection in the scalp electroencephalo-

    graphic recording (EEG), or, as a component in the

    brain’s magnetoencephalogram. Importantly, MMN (and

    its magnetic counterpart, MMNm, or mismatch field),

    can be elicited in the absence of the subject’s attention(Näätänen, 1995; Näätänen & Escera, 2000). It is con-

    sidered to reflect the brain’s automatic discrimination of 

    changes in the auditory sensory input and, furthermore,

    to provide an index of experience-dependent memory 

    traces in the human brain (Näätänen, 2001; Shtyrov et al., 2000; Kraus, McGee, Carrell, & Sharma, 1995).

    Recent evidence suggests that the MMN reflects brain

    processing of such language elements as phonemes

    (Näätänen et al., 1997; Näätänen, 1999, 2001), syllables

    (Shtyrov et al., 2000; Alho et al., 1998), and words(Shtyrov & Pulvermüller, 2002b; Korpilahti, Krause,

    Holopainen, & Lang, 2001; Pulvermüller et al., 2001).

    Importantly, we have recently found a specific pattern

    of MMN responses to inflectional affixes (Shtyrov &

    Pulvermüller, 2002a), which is best explained by the

    activation of distinct cortical memory traces for suchaffixes realized as distributed strongly connected pop-ulations of neurons (Pulvermüller, 1999, 2001). Inflec-

    tional (functional) affixes are important tools utilized in

    many languages for realizing syntax and conveying

    grammatical information. This suggests that the MMNcould be a sensitive tool for probing neural elements

    responsible for the processing of grammar in the brain.

     We have therefore set out to investigate the cerebral

    processing of syntax using a whole-head high-density 

    MEG system, and recorded MMNm elicited by either grammatically correct or ungrammatical phrases, while

    the subjects were distracted from the auditory stimula-tion by engaging in a different activity. The stimuli were

    short pronoun–verb phrases differing only in their final

    phoneme, which rendered them either as grammatical

    or as grammatically incorrect (see Figure 1, Table 1, andMethods section for details). Any possible effects of 

    acoustic (phonetic) differences were ruled out by using

    an orthogonal design reversing the effect of the pho-

    neme and word combination on grammaticality.

    RESULTS

    Magnetic MMN responses were elicited in all conditions.The overall analysis of single equivalent current dipole

    1196 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 8  

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    4/13

    (ECD) sources showed that the grammaticality of the

    deviant stimuli had a significant effect on the MMNm

    dipole moment,   F (1,8) = 6.35,   p   < .04. Separate

    analyses for each hemisphere showed that the gram-

    maticality had no effect for the right hemisphere alone

    (  p  > .65), whereas in the left hemisphere its effect wassignificant,   F (1,8) = 6.67,   p  < .04: syntactically incor-rect phrases elicited stronger MMNm activity than didthe correct ones (see Figures 2, 3, and 4a). This effect

    also became manifest as a significant Pronoun   £  Suffix 

    interaction (  p   < .04). It neither depended on the

    direction of the acoustic contrast between the suffixes

    (  p  > .12) nor on the preceding pronoun (  p  > .63) as

    such, but solely on the correctness of the phrase as a

     whole (Figure 4a).The L1 minimum-norm current estimate (MCE)

    analysis of the MEG signal suggested activation of distributed cortical sources spread out over the left

    Figure 1.   Spectrograms of acoustic Finnish-language stimuli used in the four experimental conditions. The standard and deviant stimuli in each

    pair are indiscriminable up to the divergence point (marked with white arrowheads) at their end. The standard–deviant contrasts are identical

    across all experimental conditions. Grammatically incorrect phrases are marked with an asterisk (*). See Table 1 for more details.

    Table 1.  Auditory Finnish-Language Stimuli Used in the Four Experimental Sessions

    Condition 1a Condition 1b Condition 2a Condition 2b

    Deviant   Mä tuo n   * Mä tuot    * Sä tuo n   Sä tuot 

    (‘‘I bring’’) (‘‘*I bring’’) (‘‘*you bring’’) (‘‘you bring’’)

    Standard * Mä tuot    Mä tuo n   Sä tuot    * Sä tuo n

    (‘‘*I bring’’) (‘‘I bring’’) (‘‘you bring’’) (‘‘*you bring’’)

    English translations are given in parentheses, ungrammatical phrases are marked with (*). The standard and deviant stimuli in each pair areindiscriminable up to the divergence point at their end. The standard–deviant contrasts are identical across all experimental conditions. Criticalcontrasts are in bold.

     Shtyrov et al. 1197 

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    5/13

    temporal cortex (see Figure 5). This activation was

    significantly stronger (Figure 4b) for the syntactically 

    incorrect than for correct deviant phrases,   F (1,11) =

    9.46,   p   < .011. Again, this difference was clearly 

    affected by the grammaticality, but neither by the pro-

    noun context (  p   > .25) nor by the suffix (  p   > .8).

    No differences between conditions were found in the

    right hemisphere.

     As one could notice in Figure 5 (upper left panel),

    some additional activation could be suggested to appear 

    also in the left inferior frontal cortex, which would be

    consistent with earlier results (Neville et al., 1991). We

    Figure 3.   Waveforms

    (magnetic field gradient) of 

    MMNm responses in the left

    hemisphere (grand average).

    Both ungrammatical deviant

    stimuli produced stronger 

    MMNm than the corresponding

    correct phrases regardless of 

    the acoustic, phonetic,

    phonological, or lexical

    properties of the stimuli.

    Temporal gradiometer with the

    most prominent responses

    (channel 0242) was used for 

    producing this diagram.

    Figure 2.   ECD models of MMNm activity in the left hemisphere (grand average). Both ungrammatical deviant stimuli produced stronger MMNm

    source than the corresponding correct phrases regardless of the acoustic, phonetic, phonological, or lexical properties of the stimuli. Red and blue

    contour lines indicate positive and negative values of the normal component of the magnetic field on the helmet-shaped MEG-array surface,respectively. The arrows show the dipole location and are proportional in relative size to the response magnitude. The view is from the left side of 

    the head.

    1198 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 8  

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    6/13

    analyzed activity in this area as well, but although we

    found that, similarly to the supratemporal source, it was

    somewhat increased for the ungrammatical deviant

    phrases, this effect was not significant.

    In the current study, the grammaticality affected theMMNm responses, but had no significant effect on the

    responses to the standard stimuli only (cf. Figures 3

    and 6). We also found no effects of grammaticality on

    the latency of the MMNm responses, the mean latency in

    the left hemisphere being 213 msec after the divergencepoint (mean SEM 19 msec). We also looked for significant

    differences between the conditions in later time intervals

    (>350 msec) but did not find any. Specifically, we would

    like to note that no late deflection of the magnetic

    response, which could be related to the P3- or P600-like

    components of the EEG signal (Osterhout, McLaughlin

    et al., 1997; Donchin, 1981), could be detected (seeFigures 3 and 6). We also noticed that the [n]-ending

    stimuli produced larger MMNm dipole moment than did

    the [t]-ending ones. However, this independent of the

    grammaticality effect (which could in principle be due

    to differences in attack and voicedness between the twophonemes) was only significant for comparison between

    Conditions 2a and 2b (  p   < .02; but   p   > .95 for 

    Figure 4.   Grand-average mean

    and standard error of mean of 

    the MMNm dipole-moment

    magnitude (nanoamperemeter,

    nAm) in the left hemisphere:

    (A) calculated for single

    equivalent current sources

    (ECD), (B) calculated for L1

    MCE (calculated as a sum of 

    dipole moments of all

    simultaneously activatedadjacent current sources). Both

    analyses indicated that the

    ungrammatical deviant stimuli

    produced a stronger MMNm

    source than the corresponding

    correct phrases regardless of 

    the acoustic, phonetic,

    phonological, or lexical

    properties of the stimuli.

    Figure 5.   Grand-average L1

    MCEs of MMNm in the left

    hemisphere. The MCE solutions

    are projected unto

    triangularized gray matter 

    surface. Both ungrammatical

    deviant stimuli produced

    stronger MMNm source than

    the correct phrases regardless

    of the acoustic, phonetic,

    phonological, or lexical

    properties of the stimuli.

     Shtyrov et al. 1199

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    7/13

    Conditions 1a and 1b) and only in ECD but not in MCE

    analysis, so it does not seem to present a solid finding.

    Finally, we found no topographical effects of any of the

    factors.

    DISCUSSION

    In the present study, mismatch responses were elicited

    by grammatically correct or incorrect pronoun–verb

    phrases. The choice of the single final phoneme in allphrases rendered the whole structure as syntactically 

    correct or as ungrammatical. To exclude confounding

    effects, the phonetic and lexical contrasts were counter-

    balanced over the experimental conditions using a fully orthogonal stimulus design. The subjects were instructed

    not to attend to the stimuli; they did not perform any stimulus-related language task.

     We found that MMNm responses to the syntactically 

    incorrect phrases were always larger than those to the

    correct phrases ending in the same word regardless of thedirection of the acoustic contrast or of the preceding

    pronoun. That is, neither the choice of the final phoneme

    nor that of the pronoun in the sentence altered this effect,

     which was present in both conditions in which the

    deviant stimuli were grammatically inconsistent.

    The present data are consistent with earlier results onthe cerebral processing of syntactic violations (see In-

    troduction), which suggested negative-going compo-nents of brain responses at time delays of 125 msec or 

    more following syntactic errors. The present modifica-

    tion of the MMN may be related to this early negativity.There are, however, important differences between the

    present study and the earlier ones. Importantly, we were

    able to find syntactically related effects in the absence of 

    directed attention towards the stimuli as subjects were

    engaged in a different task and were instructed to ignorethe stimulation. The violations in this study modulated

    the MMN response, which is considered to reflect a pre-

    attentive automatic level of auditory processing in the

    cerebral cortex (Näätänen & Alho, 1995; Näätänen &Escera, 2000; Tiitinen et al., 1997). This suggests that the

    brain may be capable of automatic syntactic analysis of 

    the incoming language signals already at relatively early 

    stages of speech processing. This conclusion is support-ive of the earlier suggestion that early negativities, such

    as ELAN, may reflect automatic stages of syntactic pro-cessing (Friederici, 2002; Gunter et al., 2000; Hahne &

    Friederici, 1999).

    The ELAN/N125 component reported in the previous

    studies was found in response to phrase structure

     violations (examp le:   mathematician’s *of proof thetheorem ) and has been difficult to replicate (Takazawa

    et al., 2002; Neville et al., 1991), whereas in the present

    study the MMN modulation was elicited at a similar 

    latency by agreement violations ( mä   tuo*t, sä   tuo*n ),suggesting that a larger variety of grammatical violations,

    including agreement violations, could be reflected by early left-lateralized neurophysiological responses.

     An established view in psycholinguistics is that sen-

    tence comprehension is incremental, that is, a substantialamount of processing occurs immediately after a word in

    a sentence is perceived, prior to the perception of the

    following word (Pulvermüller, 2001; Traxler & Pickering,

    1996; Marslen-Wilson & Tyler, 1975; Marslen-Wilson,

    1987). This implies that by the time a violation occurs, a

    certain amount of analysis has been carried out by the

    parsing system, a position largely shared by severalsyntactic parsing models that may substantially differ 

    otherwise (e.g., Friederici, 2002; van Gompel & Pickering,2001; Spivey & Tanenhaus, 1998; MacDonald, Pearlmut-

    ter, & Seidenberg, 1994; Trueswell, Tanenhaus, & Kello,

    1993; Ferreira & Henderson, 1990; Frazier, 1987; Mitchell,1987). During such analysis, an expectation for syntac-

    tically possible subsequent morphemes may have built

    up. When this expectation fails to realize and the parser 

    therefore encounters a syntactic error (i.e., a mismatchbetween preceding and incoming morphemes), it sends

    an ‘‘error signal’’. Such an error signal may be due to a

    lack of neuronal links between lexical representations

     which results in a failure to provide priming between the

    representations of morphemes that do not match syntac-tically (Pulvermüller, 2002). In our case, the memory trace

    Figure 6.   Waveforms

    (magnetic field gradient)

    of auditory responses to

    standard stimuli in the left

    hemisphere (grand

    average). Although the

    MMNm responses were

    affected by grammaticality,

    there was no significant

    effect on the standard

    responses (cf. Figure 3).Temporal gradiometer 

     with the most prominent

    responses (channel 0242)

     was used for producing

    this diagram.

    1200 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 8  

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    8/13

    (neuronal assembly) representing the expected correct

    affix (Shtyrov & Pulvermüller, 2002a) would have been

    primed by the preceding context. Language-related

    priming effects are known to reduce negative-going

    components of the event-related potentials (e.g., Hol-comb & Neville, 1990; Bentin, McCarthy, & Wood, 1985).

    In ungrammatical conditions, this priming would be

    absent which should lead to an increased response as

    compared with grammatically consistent stimuli. Thus,the parser’s error signal, the unprimed activation of morpheme-related neuron ensembles, may be the basis

    of syntactically related early left-lateralized responses,

    including the larger MMN response to grammatical viola-

    tions observed here. Interestingly again, this putative

    process seems to occur outside the focus of attention

    (i.e., at least to some degree automatically).The strongest activation produced by the syntactic

    abnormalities in the current study was located in the left

    temporal cortex. We take this as an indication that there

    is a distributed neuronal system in the left superior 

    temporal lobe that contributes to grammar processing.This area has been repeatedly suggested as a neural

    substrate for a variety of language functions such as

    phonetic/phonological processing, lexical access, seman-

    tic and syntactic processing (e.g., Shtyrov et al., 2000;

    Shtyrov & Pulvermüller, 2002a, 2002b; Martin & Chao,2001; Pulvermüller et al., 2001; Friederici et al., 2000;

    Price, 2000; Helenius, Salmelin, Service, & Connolly,

    1998; Näätänen et al., 1997; Binder et al., 1995), which

    is in line with the current results. There probably could

    be an overlap between these functions with respect to

    underlying neuroanatomical structures, but it seemsreasonable to suggest that they are carried out by atleast partially distinct neuronal networks (Pulvermüller,

    2002). It also appears justified to suggest that the left-

    lateralized neural systems contributing to the grammat-

    ically related MMN in the superior temporal lobe may bedistinct from those responsible for classical bilateral

    acoustic-related MMNs.

    Interestingly, although we found the strongest activa-

    tion in the temporal cortex, a left anterior-frontal re-

    sponse was reported earlier (Neville et al., 1991;Pulvermüller & Shtyrov, in press). As we found no

    significant grammatically dependent variation for theinferior frontal activity, we conclude that in the present

    data the frontal source did not seem to play a major role

    in syntactic processing. There could be several possible

    explanations for this: in principle, it could be that theadvanced source analysis algorithm used in this study,

    MCE, provides more accurate source localization than

    potential mappings used earlier. As a viable alternative

    explanation, one could suggest that the anterior source

    might not be detectable in MEG due to its spatialorientation (and/or, vice versa, that the posterior source

    may be difficult to image using EEG). Both of these

    explanations are supported by earlier MEG research, which has also demonstrated more temporal than frontal

    activity to syntactic violations when subjects were asked

    to judge grammaticality of auditory presented sentences

    (Friederici et al., 2000). However, a frontal ELAN gener-

    ator was reported in MEG, although this was also accom-

    panied by a temporal one (Gross et al., 1998). Finally, it ispossible that the absence of directed attention to the

    stimuli and of an active task may have reduced the frontal

    activity. This would imply that the inferior frontal source

    is of smaller importance for automatic syntactic parsing.To fully answer the question of the cortical generator(s)for the syntactic MMN modulation observed here and

    their relation to ELAN activation, further studies using

    similar paradigm would be needed.

    Finally, we could not find brain dynamics that might

    relate to the late centro-parietal positive shift (P600)

    reported earlier in ERP studies of grammatical violations. Again, one could suggest that cortical generators of late

    slow positive shifts are not optimal for MEG recordings,

    although, among late positivities, at least the P3 was

    successfully registered using MEG (Berg, Kakigi, Scherg,

    Dobel, & Zobel, 1999; Mecklinger et al., 1998; Tarkka,Stokic, Basile, & Papanicolaou, 1995). However, the latter 

    still does not rule out the possibility that the syntactically 

    related P600 response has no clear magnetic correlate.

     Another possible reason for the absence of a late gram-

    maticality effect in our present data set is the absence of an attention-demanding task. It has been proposed

    earlier that the positive shifts to grammatical violations

    reflect attention-dependent processes, for example, at-

    tempts at a structural reanalysis of a sentence fragment

    (Osterhout & Holcomb, 1992). In this view, the present

    absence of clear dynamics in P600 time range (alsoconfirmed in a separate EEG study, Pulvermüller &Shtyrov, in press) might suggest a role of attention in

    the generation of the late positivity. Because subjects did

    not focus their attention on language in this study, they 

    most likely did not engage in re-parsing the deviantstrings, and thus the centro-parietal positive shift did

    not occur. This would be consistent with notions that

    P600 reflects phrase reanalysis and repair (Friederici,

    1997; Osterhout, Holcomb, & Swinney, 1994) and may 

    not reflect specific syntactic processes per se (Münte,Heinze, Matzke, Wieringa, & Johannes, 1998).

    In conclusion, the present results suggest that the early syntax parsing of spoken language may not require

    focused attention and therefore could probably be auto-

    matic to a certain extent. The cortical structures in the left

    superior temporal lobe appear to play an important rolein carrying out such automatic grammar processing.

    METHODS

    Subjects

    Twelve healthy right-handed (handedness assessed

    according to Oldfield, 1971; no left-handed family mem-bers) native Finnish speakers (age 21–29, 7 women)

     Shtyrov et al. 1201

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    9/13

     wi th norm al hea ring an d no rec ord of neurol ogi-

    cal diseases were presented with four sets of spoken

    native-language stimuli in four separate experimental

    conditions.

    Stimuli

    The four short-phrase stimuli (see Figure 1 and Table 1)

     were prepared with requirements that the acoustic,phonetic, and phonological difference between thestandard and the deviant stimuli is identical in each

    condition and that the stimuli themselves are as similar 

    acoustically as possible. These Finnish stimuli were:

    (1a)   Mä tuon  (‘‘I bring,’’ syntactically correct),

    (1b) * Mä tuot  (‘‘*I bring,’’ syntactically incorrect: the verb   tuot   is in the second person inflection form, while

    the pronoun   mä, ‘‘I,’’ requires the first person); here

    and throughout we use linguistic convention of marking

    ungrammatical phrases with an asterisk (*),

    (2a) * Sä   tuon   (‘‘*you bring,’’ syntactically incorrect:the verb   tuon   is in the first person form, while the

    pronoun sä, ‘‘you,’’ requires the second person);

    (2b)   Sä tuot  (‘‘you bring,’’ syntactically correct).2

    Therefore, it is the single final phoneme, inflec-tional affix [n] or [t], which may render the whole phrase

    as syntactically correct or as ungrammatical. Remarkably,

    for ‘‘mä’’-stimuli (1a and 1b), the final [n] means that

    the phrase is grammatical, while for the ‘‘sä’’-stimuli

    (2a and 2b) the combination is exactly the opposite:

    [t] is needed in the end for the whole phrase to becorrect. This orthogonal design is implemented inorder to control for purely acoustic, phonetic, and pho-

    nological effects on mismatch responses as well as

    for putative lexical and semantic differences between

    the words. As seen in Table 1 presenting the complete stimulus

    design, the stimuli starting with the same pronoun were

    always presented in the same experimental condition,

    one of them serving as deviant and the other one as

    standard stimulus. Thus, the standard–deviant acoustic– phonetic contrast, the critical variable determining the

    MMN (Näätänen & Alho, 1997), was the same in allconditions. However, in each pair of conditions (1a, b

    and 2a, b) the responses were elicited either by correct

    or incorrect deviant forms. Furthermore, while the

    acoustic contrasts were identical in the corresponding

    conditions ( tuon/tuot   in 1a–2a and   tuot/tuon   in 1b–2b),the syntactic contrast (determined by the pronoun) was

    the opposite: stimuli ending in   tuon   were correct in

    conditions 1a and 1b, whereas  tuot -ending phrases were

    correct in Conditions 2a and 2b. This fully orthogonaldesign, therefore, allowed us to control not only for 

    possible acoustic/phonetic effects of the verb contrast,

    but even for an unlikely late differential effect of the twopreceding pronouns.

    For stimulus preparation, we recorded multiple rep-

    etitions of each word uttered by a female native speaker 

    of Finnish. With great care we selected a combination of 

    recordings, whose vowels matched in their fundamental

    frequency (F0) and whose overall length and maximalsound energy were identical both for  sä  and  mä  and for tuon  and tuot . We then combined the four words in the

    four abovementioned phrases with a 50-msec gap be-

    tween the pronoun and the verb; all phrases were766 msec in duration. Thus, the two stimuli in eachcondition were perceptually identical up to a time point

    (referred to as divergence point) shortly before their 

    end (see Figure 1). In order to determine this diver-

    gence point, a gating experiment was performed, in

     which initial fragments of increasing duration of bothtuon  and   tuot  were presented to the subjects who wereasked whether they could perceive any difference be-

    tween the two. In average, they could identify the

    difference only when the first 336 msec (  SEM   9 msec)

    of the words were presented (which equals to 636 msec

    after the onset of the complete phrase). This perceiveddivergence point is therefore considered as the onset

    of the perceptual standard/deviant contrast and a zero

    time reference in the current study. Importantly, it is

    the same in all pairs of stimuli due to their design.

     All stimuli were normalized to have the same peaksound energy. For the analysis and production of the

    stimuli, we used the Cool Edit 96 program (Syntrillium

    Software, AZ).

     Acoustic Stimulation

     All four experimental conditions (Table 1) were per-formed with every subject, their order being counter-

    balanced across the subject group. The stimuli were

    binaurally presented at 50 dB above the individual

    hearing threshold (determined using the experimentalstimuli) via headphones connected to an STIM setup

    (Neuroscan Labs, VA). The interstimulus (stimulus

    onset asynchrony, SOA) interval was 1450 msec. In each

    condition, the deviant stimulus was presented with a

    probability of 16.7% among the repetitive standardstimuli: A pseudorandom sequence of stimuli was cre-

    ated so that there were always at least two standardstimuli between any two deviants. During the stimula-

    tion, the subjects were seated in a magnetically shielded

    chamber and instructed to watch a silent video film

    of their own choice and to pay no attention to theauditory stimulation.

    Magnetoencephalographic Recording

    The evoked magnetic field was recorded (passband0.03–200 Hz, sampling rate 600 Hz) with 204 planar 

    gradiometer channels of a whole-head Neuromag Vec-

    torview MEG system (Elekta Neuromag, Helsinki) duringthe auditory stimulation (Ahonen et al., 1993). The

    1202 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 8  

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    10/13

    recordings started 100 msec before stimulus onset

    and ended 1450 msec thereafter. The responses were

    on-line averaged separately for the standard and devi-

    ant stimuli in each condition. Epochs with voltage

     variation exceeding 150   m V at either of two bipolar eye-movement electrodes or magnetic-field gradient

     variation exceeding 3000 femtotesla per centimeter (fT/ 

    cm) at any gradiometer channel were excluded from

    averaging. The recordings for each condition containedat least 120 accepted responses to deviant stimuli (cor-responding to   ¹600 standard responses) in order to

    achieve satisfactory averaged signal quality. This require-

    ment led to slight variations in overall recording dura-

    tion, which in average constituted 2 hr per subject.

    MEG Data Processing

    The averaged responses were filtered off-line (passband

    1–20 Hz). The period of 50 msec before the divergence

    point was used as the baseline. The MMNm was ob-

    tained by subtracting the averaged response to thestandard stimuli from that to the deviant ones. The

    responses were evaluated separately for each subject

    for all experimental conditions. Two approaches were

    used for analyzing the neuromagnetic data.

     Single-Dipole Fit 

    Fifty-four gradiometer channels on each side of the

    magnetometer helmet were used for assessing cortical

    responses in the left and right cerebral hemispheres. By 

    means of Neuromag sequential single-dipole fittingsoftware (Elekta Neuromag), the generator sources(equivalent current dipoles [ECDs]) of the MMNm were

    estimated (Ilmoniemi, 1993). Only dipole models ex-

    plaining more than 65% of the field gradients were

    selected for statistical analysis; otherwise, the dipole-moment value was set to 0 nAm. The range of latencies

    (as determined from peak dipole moment) accepted for 

    the MMNm responses was 100–350 msec after the

    divergence point and the best fit (maximal goodness

    of fit/dipole moment) was entered into analysis. Thedipole moments of the MMNm generators were calcu-

    lated and compared between the experimental condi-tions. ECD fits were successfully constructed only for 

    nine subjects, suggesting that the single-dipole model

    may not be optimal for explaining the syntactically 

    related activity. We therefore performed minimum-norm current analysis, which uses a distribution of 

    multiple current sources for modeling the neuromag-

    netic signal.

     L1 Minimum-Norm Current Estimate

    The estimation of the cortical sources of the measured

    neuromagnetic activity was performed using L1 MCEson the basis of recordings from all 204 gradiometer 

    channels equally distributed over the entire scalp. The

    minimum-norm method provides a solution to the in-

     ver se probl em of localizing neu ral activ ity in the

    brain from its external recordings by revealing the

    unique constellation of active neuronal current ele-ments that models the recorded magnetic field distri-

    bution with the smallest amount of overall activity 

    (Uutela, Hämälä inen, & Somersalo, 1999; Ilmoniemi,

    1993; Hämäläinen & Ilmoniemi, 1984, 1994). Among theinfinitely many current source combinations that canexplain a given scalp topography of the magnetic

    field, this technique finds one that does so with the

    least amount of overall activity. This minimal solution

    should be preferred for the sake of parsimoniousness.

    The L1 MCE minimizes the integral of the current

    amplitude and reveals a realistic and robust constellationof localized generators. It is applicable when it can be

    assumed a priori that the source distribution consists of 

    discrete areas of neuronal activity (in contrast with a

    situation when no essential a priori information is

    available, in which case L2 minimum norm estimate would be preferable). This assumption of local ized

    sources is justified in the investigation of language

    because neuroimaging results obtained with different

    methodologies proved the activation of discrete cortical

    areas during a variety of language tasks (Pulvermüller,2001; Price, 2000). A triangularized gray matter surface

     was used for projecting MCE solutions for the each

    subject individually and for the grand average magnetic

    field. The dipole moments of the MMNm generators

     were calculated (as a sum of dipole moments of all

    simultaneously activated adjacent current sources) andcompared between the experimental conditions. TheMCE source analysis was successfully carried out for all

    12 subjects, which suggests that it could be a better 

    method for analyzing distributed neural activity (as in

    case of language-related phenomena) than ECD.

    Statistical Analysis

    The data were subjected to analyses of variance (AN-

    OVA). Parameters of current sources constructed for each hemisphere were analyzed separately for ECD

    and MCE. We compared them between conditions usingthe factors grammaticality (syntactically correct vs. in-

    correct deviant stimulus), pronoun (  sä   vs.   mä ), and

    suffix ([t] vs. [n]).

    Ethical Considerations

     All subjects gave their written informed consent to

    participate in the experiments and were paid for 

    their participation. The experiments were performedin accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. Ethical

    permission for the experiments was issued by the Re-

    search Ethics Committee of Helsinki University CentralHospital.

     Shtyrov et al. 1203

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    11/13

     Acknowledgments

     We thank Olaf Hauk, Vadim Nikulin, Maritta Maltio-Laine, SimoMonto, Tuomas Murdoch, Christopher Bailey, Jussi Nurminen,

     Johanna Salonen, Anthea Hills, Seppo Kähkönen, and WilliamMarslen-Wilson for their contribution at different stages of this

     work. We would also like to thank three anonymous refereesfor their helpful comments and constructive critique.

    Reprint requests should be sent to Dr. Yury Shtyrov, Medical

    Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, 15Chaucer Road, CB2 2EF Cambridge, UK, or via e-mail: [email protected].

    Notes

    1. Function words are grammatical words, such as articles,conjunctions, auxiliary verbs, and so on and so forth, whichhave no concrete meaning by themselves and are utilized for grammatical purposes.2. These spoken-language stimuli represent the most usualcolloquial pronunciation used in the Helsinki area; therefore,

     we only used Finnish subjects who had lived in the Helsinkiarea for a minimum of 6 years before the experiment.

    REFERENCES

     Ahonen, A. I., Hämäläinen, M., Kajola, M., Knuutila, J., Laine, P.,Lounasmaa, O., Parkkonen, L., Simola, J., & Tesche, C.(1993). 122-channel SQUID instrument for investigating themagnetic signals from the human brain. Physica Scripta,T49, 198–205.

     Alho, K. (1992). Selective attention in auditory processingas reflected by event-related brain potentials.

     Psychophysiology, 29,  247–263. Alho, K. (1995). Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity 

    (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes. Ear and Hearing, 16,  38–51.

     Alho, K., Connolly, J. F., Cheour, M., Lehtokoski, A.,Huotilainen, M., Virtanen, J., Aulanko, R., & Ilmoniemi,R. J. (1998). Hemispheric lateralization in preattentiveprocessing of speech sounds.   Neuroscience Letters, 258,9–12.

     Assadollahi, R., & Pulvermüller, F. (2003). Early influences of  word length and frequency: a group study using MEG. Neuroreport, 14,  1183–1187.

    Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event-relatedpotentials, lexical decision and semantic priming.

     Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60,343–355.

    Berg, P., Kakigi, R., Scherg, M., Dobel, C., & Zobel, E. (1999).Source modelling of the EEG and MEG oddball response i n asubject with a large P300.  Electroencephalography and Clinical  Neurophysi ology Supplement, 49,  189–193.

    Binder, J. R., Rao, S. M., Hammeke, T. A., Frost, J. A.,Bandettini, P. A., Jesmanowicz, A., & Hyde, J. S. (1995).Lateralized human brain language systems demonstrated by task subtraction functional magnetic resonance imaging.

     Archives of Neurology, 52,  593–601.Donchin, E. (1981). Surprise! ... Surprise?  Psychophysiology,

    18,  493–513.Escera, C., Alho, K., Winkler, I., & Näätänen, R. (1998). Neural

    mechanisms of involuntary attention to acoustic novelty and change.   J  ournal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10,

    590–604.Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (1990). Use of verb information

    in syntactic parsing: Evidence from eye movements and word-by-word self-paced reading.  Journal of Experimental  Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16,555–568.

    Frazier, L. (1987). Sentence processing: A tutorial review. In M.Coltheart (Ed.), The psychology of reading  (Vol. 12,pp. 559–586). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Friederici, A. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing.   Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6,78–84.

    Friederici, A. D. (1997). Neurophysiological aspects of languageprocessing.  Clinical Neuroscience, 4,  64–72.

    Friederici, A. D., Wang, Y., Herrmann, C. S., Maess, B., & Oertel,U. (2000). Localization of early syntactic processes in frontaland temporal cortical areas: A magnetoencephalographicstudy. Human Brain Mapping, 11,  1–11.

    Gross, J., Ioannides, A. A., Dammers, J., Maess, B., Friederici, A. D., & Muller-Gartner, H. W. (1998). Magnetic fieldtomography analysis of continuous speech. BrainTopography, 10, 273–281.

    Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Hahne, A. (1999). Brainresponses during sentence reading: visual input affectscentral processes.  NeuroReport, 10, 3175–3178.

    Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Schriefers, H. (2000).

    Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive

     Neuroscience, 12,  556–568.Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic

    positive shift (SPS) as an ERP-measure of syntacticprocessing.  Language and Cognitive Processes, 8,  439–483.

    Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Electrophysiologicalevidence for two steps in syntactic analysis. Early automaticand late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive

     Neuroscience, 11,  194–205.Hämäläinen, M. S., & Ilmoniemi, R. J. (1984). Interpreting

    measured magnetic fields of the brain: minimum normestimates of current distributions. Helsinki University of Technology, Technical Report TKK-F-A559.

    Hämäläinen, M. S., & Ilmoniemi, R. J. (1994). Interpretingmagnetic fields of the brain: minimum norm estimates.

     Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing, 32,35–42.

    Helenius, P., Salmelin, R., Service, E., & Connolly, J. F. (1998).Distinct time courses of word and context comprehension inthe left temporal cortex.  Brain, 121,  1133–1142.

    Holcomb, P. J., & Neville, H. J. (1990). Auditory and visualsemantic priming in lexical decision: A comparison usingevent-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive

     Processes, 5,  281–312.Ilmoniemi, R. J. (1993). Models of source currents in the brain.

     Brain Topography, 5,  331–336.Korpilahti, P., Krause, C. M., Holopainen, I., & Lang, A. H.

    (2001). Early and late mismatch negativity elicited by wordsand speech-like stimuli in children. Brain and Language,76,  332–339.

    Korth, M., & Nguyen, N. X. (1997). The effect of stimulus sizeon human cortical potentials evoked by chromatic patterns.Vision Research, 37, 649–657.

    Kraus, N., McGee, T., Carrell, T. D., & Sharma, A. (1995).Neurophysiologic bases of speech discrimination. Ear and 

     Hearing, 16,  19–37.MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S.

    (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101,   676–703.

    Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. K. (1975). Processing structureof sentence perception. Nature, 257,  784–786.

    Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition.  Cognition, 25, 71–102.

    1204 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 8  

    http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2916L.555[aid=146258]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1065-6766^28^294L.64[aid=3031346]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1065-9471^28^2911L.1[aid=5256052]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0896-0267^28^2910L.273[aid=5416571]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0048-5772^28^2929L.247[aid=3155408]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0140-0118^28^2932L.35[aid=5416574]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0006-8950^28^29121L.1133[aid=211903]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0169-0965^28^295L.281[aid=124475]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2916L.555[aid=146258]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0196-0202^28^2916L.19[aid=4348304]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0093-934X^28^2976L.332[aid=4986532]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0169-0965^28^295L.281[aid=124475]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0140-0118^28^2932L.35[aid=5416574]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0898-929X^28^2911L.194[aid=213072]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0898-929X^28^2912L.556[aid=5416573]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0896-0267^28^2910L.273[aid=5416571]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1364-6613^28^296L.78[aid=5256051]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2916L.555[aid=146258]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0898-929X^28^2910L.590[aid=212628]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0048-5772^28^2918L.493[aid=212626]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0424-8155^28^2949L.189[aid=5416570]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0013-4694^28^2960L.343[aid=847369]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0304-3940^28^29258L.9[aid=5416568]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0010-0277^28^2925L.71[aid=212355]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0028-0836^28^29257L.784[aid=212357]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0033-295X^28^29101L.676[aid=214495]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0196-0202^28^2916L.19[aid=4348304]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0042-6989^28^2937L.649[aid=5416576]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0093-934X^28^2976L.332[aid=4986532]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0896-0267^28^295L.331[aid=5416575]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0169-0965^28^295L.281[aid=124475]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0006-8950^28^29121L.1133[aid=211903]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0140-0118^28^2932L.35[aid=5416574]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0898-929X^28^2911L.194[aid=213072]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0169-0965^28^298L.439[aid=212527]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0898-929X^28^2912L.556[aid=5416573]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0959-4965^28^2910L.3175[aid=5416572]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0896-0267^28^2910L.273[aid=5416571]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1065-9471^28^2911L.1[aid=5256052]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1065-6766^28^294L.64[aid=3031346]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1364-6613^28^296L.78[aid=5256051]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2916L.555[aid=146258]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0898-929X^28^2910L.590[aid=212628]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0048-5772^28^2918L.493[aid=212626]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0003-9942^28^2952L.593[aid=847233]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0424-8155^28^2949L.189[aid=5416570]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0013-4694^28^2960L.343[aid=847369]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0959-4965^28^2914L.1183[aid=5416569]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0304-3940^28^29258L.9[aid=5416568]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0196-0202^28^2916L.38[aid=846887]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0048-5772^28^2929L.247[aid=3155408]

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    12/13

    Martin, A., & Chao, L. L. (2001). Semantic memory and thebrain: Structure and processes. Current Opinion in

     Neurobiology, 11,  194–201.Mecklinger, A., Maess, B., Opitz, B., Pfeifer, E., Cheyne, D., &

     Weinberg, H. (1998). A MEG analysis of the P300 in visualdiscrimination tasks.  Electroencephalography and Clinical 

     Neurophysiology, 108,  45–56.Mitchell, D. C. (1987). Lexical guidance in human parsing:

    Locus and processing characteristics. In M. Coltheart (Ed.),The psychology of reading. (Vol. 12, pp. 601–618). Hillsdale,

    NJ: Erlbaum.Münte, T. F., Heinze, H. -J., Matzke, M., Wieringa, B. M., &

     Johannes, S. (1998). Brain potentials and syntactic violationsrevisited: No evidence for specificity of the syntactic positiveshift.   Neuropsychologia, 36,  217–226.

    Münte, T. F., Schiltz, K., & Kutas, M. (1998). When temporalterms belie conceptual order. Nature, 395, 71–73.

    Näätänen, R. (1995). The mismatch negativity: A powerfultool for cognitive neuroscience.   Ear and Hearing, 16,6–18.

    Näätänen, R. (1999). Phoneme representations of the humanbrain as reflected by event-related potentials. Functional 

     Neuroscience: Evoked Potentials and Magnetic Fields, EEG Supplement, 170–173.

    Näätänen, R. (2001). The perception of speech sounds by thehuman brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN)and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). Psychophysiology, 38,1–21.

    Näätänen, R., & Alho, K. (1995). Mismatch negativity—A unique measure of sensory processing in audition.

     International Journal of Neuroscience, 80,  317–337.Näätänen, R., & Alho, K. (1997). Mismatch negativity—The

    measure for central sound representation accuracy. Audiology and Neurootology, 2,  341–353.

    Näätänen, R., & Escera, C. (2000). Mismatch negativity: Clinicaland other applications. Audiology and Neurootology, 5,105–110.

    Näätänen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., Cheour, M.,

    Huotilainen, M., Iivonen, A., Valnio, A., Alku, P., Ilmoniemi,R. J., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Sinkkonen, J., & Alho, K. (1997).Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses.   Nature, 385,432–434.

    Näätänen, R., & Picton, T. (1987). The N1 wave of the humanelectric and magnetic response to sound: A review and ananalysis of the component structure.  Psychophysiology, 24,375–425.

    Näätänen, R., & Winkler, I. (1999). The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience.

     Psychological Bulletin, 12,  826–859.Neville, H., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I., & Garrett, M. F.

    (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes:

    Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive N euroscience, 3, 151–165.

    Opitz, B., Rinne, T., Mecklinger, A., von Cramon, D. Y., &Schröger, E. (2002). Differential contribution of frontal andtemporal cortices to auditory change detection: fMRI andERP results.   Neuroimage, 15,  167–174.

    Osterhout, L., Bersick, M., & McKonnon, R. (1997). Brainpotentials elicited by words: Word length and frequency predict the latency  of an early negativity.   Biological 

     Psychology, 46,  143–168.Osterhout, L., & Hagoort, P. (1999). A superficial resemblance

    does not necessarily mean you are part of the family:Counter ar guments to Coulson, King and Kutas (1998) in theP600/SPS-P300 debate.   Language and Cognitive Processes,

    14,  1–14.Oster hout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain

    potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31,  785–806.

    Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1994). Brainpotentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of theapplication of verb information during parsing.  Journal of 

     Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 786–803.

    Osterhout, L., McLaughlin, J., & Bersick, M. (1997).Event-related brain potentials and human language. Trendsin Cognitive Sciences, 1,  203–209.

    Osterhout, L., & Swinney, D. A. (1993). On the temporal courseof gap-filling during comprehension of verbal passives.

      Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22,  273–286.Picton, T., & Hillyard, S. (1974). Human auditory evoked

    potentials: II. Effects of attention.   Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 36,  191–200.

    Picton, T. W., Alain, C., Otten, L., Ritter, W., & Achim, A. (2000).Mismatch negativity: Different water in the same river.

     Audiology and Neurootology, 5,  111–139.Price, C. J. (2000). The anatomy of language: Contributions

    from functional neuroimaging. Journal of Anatomy, 197,335–359.

    Pulvermüller, F. (1999). Words in the brain’s language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22,  253–336.

    Pulvermüller, F. (2001). Brain reflections of words and their meaning.  Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5,  517–524.

    Pulvermüller, F. (2002). A brain perspective on languagemechanisms: From discrete neuronal ensembles to serialorder. Progress in Neurobiology, 67,  85–111.

    Pulvermüller, F., Kujala, T., Shtyrov, Y., Simola, J., Tiitinen, H., Alku, P., Alho, K., Martinkauppi, S., Ilmoniemi, R. J., &Näätänen, R. (2001). Memory traces for words as revealed by the mismatch negativity.  Neuroimage, 14,  607–616.

    Pulvermüller, F., & Shtyrov, Y. (in press). Automatic processingof grammar in the human brain as revealed by the mismatchnegativity.  Neuroimage.

    Shtyrov, Y., Kujala, T., Palva, S., Ilmoniemi, R. J., & Nä ätänen,R. (2000). Discrimination of speech and of complex 

    nonspeech sounds of different temporal structure in theleft and right cerebral hemispheres.   Neuroimage, 12,657–663.

    Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermüller, F. (2002a). Memory traces for inflectional affixes as shown by mismatch negativity.

     European Journal of Neuroscience, 15,  1085–1091.Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermüller, F. (2002b). Neurophysiological

    evidence of memory traces for words in the human brain. NeuroReport, 13,  521–525.

    Spivey, M. J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Syntactic ambiguity resolution in discourse: Modeling the effects of referentialcontext and lexical frequency.  Journal of Experimental 

     Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24,1521–1543.

    Takazawa, S., Takahashi, N., Nakagome, K., Kanno, O.,Hagiwara, H., Nakajima, H., Itoh, K., & Koshida, I. (2002).Early components of event-related potentials related tosemantic and syntactic processes in the Japanese language.

     Brain Topography, 14,  169–177.Tarkka, I. M., Stokic, D. S., Basile, L. F., & Papanicolaou,

     A. C. (1995). Electric source localization of the auditory P300 agrees with magnetic source localization.

     Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 96,538–545.

    Tiitinen, H., May, P., & Näätänen, R. (1997). The transient40-Hz response, mismatch negativity, and attentionalprocesses in humans. Progress in

     Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 21,

    751–771.Traxler, M. J., & Pickering, M. J. (1996). Case-marking in the

     Shtyrov et al. 1205

    http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2920L.786[aid=213083]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0013-4694^28^2936L.191[aid=1474081]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0140-525X^28^2922L.253[aid=888946]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1420-3030^28^295L.105[aid=5416579]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1053-8119^28^2912L.657[aid=5256118]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0898-929X^28^293L.151[aid=211911]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-5846^28^2921L.751[aid=5416587]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2924L.1521[aid=2265757]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2920L.786[aid=213083]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-5846^28^2921L.751[aid=5416587]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0013-4694^28^2996L.538[aid=846559]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2924L.1521[aid=2265757]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1053-8119^28^2912L.657[aid=5256118]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0021-8782^28^29197L.335[aid=1426606]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0013-4694^28^2936L.191[aid=1474081]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1364-6613^28^291L.203[aid=1510089]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2920L.786[aid=213083]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0749-596X^28^2931L.785[aid=212364]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0169-0965^28^2914L.1[aid=3516254]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0301-0511^28^2946L.143[aid=303374]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0898-929X^28^293L.151[aid=211911]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0048-5772^28^2924L.375[aid=213559]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0028-0836^28^29385L.432[aid=211910]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1420-3030^28^295L.105[aid=5416579]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0048-5772^28^2938L.1[aid=5256112]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0196-0202^28^2916L.6[aid=872174]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0013-4694^28^29108L.45[aid=5416577]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0959-4388^28^2911L.194[aid=1893160]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-5846^28^2921L.751[aid=5416587]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0013-4694^28^2996L.538[aid=846559]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0896-0267^28^2914L.169[aid=5416586]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2924L.1521[aid=2265757]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0959-4965^28^2913L.521[aid=5416585]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0953-816X^28^2915L.1085[aid=5416584]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1053-8119^28^2912L.657[aid=5256118]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1053-8119^28^2914L.607[aid=5256116]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0301-0082^28^2967L.85[aid=5416583]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1364-6613^28^295L.517[aid=2727046]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0140-525X^28^2922L.253[aid=888946]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0021-8782^28^29197L.335[aid=1426606]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1420-3030^28^295L.111[aid=2288908]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0013-4694^28^2936L.191[aid=1474081]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0090-6905^28^2922L.273[aid=5416582]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1364-6613^28^291L.203[aid=1510089]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2920L.786[aid=213083]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0749-596X^28^2931L.785[aid=212364]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0169-0965^28^2914L.1[aid=3516254]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0301-0511^28^2946L.143[aid=303374]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1053-8119^28^2915L.167[aid=5416581]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0898-929X^28^293L.151[aid=211911]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0048-5772^28^2924L.375[aid=213559]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0028-0836^28^29385L.432[aid=211910]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1420-3030^28^295L.105[aid=5416579]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1420-3030^28^292L.341[aid=1488543]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0020-7454^28^2980L.317[aid=318582]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0048-5772^28^2938L.1[aid=5256112]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0196-0202^28^2916L.6[aid=872174]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0028-0836^28^29395L.71[aid=5416578]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0028-3932^28^2936L.217[aid=213078]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0013-4694^28^29108L.45[aid=5416577]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0959-4388^28^2911L.194[aid=1893160]

  • 8/19/2019 2003 Shtyrov Grammar Processing Outside the Focus of Attention

    13/13

    parsing of complement sentences: Evidence from eyemovements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,

     49,  991–1004.Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993).

     Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separatingeffects of lexical preference from garden-paths.   Journal of 

     Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 528–553.

    Uutela, K., Hämäläinen, M., & Somersalo, E. (1999). Visualization of magnetoencephalographic data using

    minimum current estimates.  Neuroimage, 10,  173–180. van Gomp el, R. P., & Pickering, M. J. (2001). Lexical guidance in

    sentence processing: A note on Adams, Clifton, and Mitchell(1998). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 851–857.

     Woods, D. L., Alho, K., & Algazi, A. (1993). Intermodal selective

    attention: evidence for processing in tonotopic auditory fields.   Psychophysiology, 30,  287–295.

     Woods, D. L., Knight, R. T., & Scabini, D. (1993). Anatomicalsubstrates of auditory selective attention: Behavioral andelectrophysiological effects of posterior association cortex lesions.  Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 1,227–240.

     Yamasaki, H., LaBar, K. S., & McCarthy, G. (2002). Dissociableprefrontal brain systems for attention and emotion.

     Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 99,

    11447–11451. Yantis, S., Schwarzbach, J., Serences, J. T., Carlson, R. L.,

    Steinmetz, M. A., Pekar, J. J., & Courtney, S. M. (2002).Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex duringspatial attention shifts.  Nature Neuroscience, 5,  995–1002.

    http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2919L.528[aid=302834]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0027-8424^28^2999L.11447[aid=5416590]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0926-6410^28^291L.227[aid=4681819]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2919L.528[aid=302834]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1097-6256^28^295L.995[aid=5416591]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0027-8424^28^2999L.11447[aid=5416590]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0926-6410^28^291L.227[aid=4681819]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0048-5772^28^2930L.287[aid=2288915]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1069-9384^28^298L.851[aid=5416589]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/1053-8119^28^2910L.173[aid=5256087]http://zerlina.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2919L.528[aid=302834]