28
A production study of Romanian diphthongs and glide-vowel sequences This study compares two diphthongs ([ea], [oa]) and two glide-vowel sequences ([ja], [wa]) in Romanian. The diphthongs and the sequences are auditorily very similar, but they differ in their phonological patterning. An integrated production and perception experiment is conducted in search of perceptual and acoustic evidence for the different phonological representations proposed for the diphthongs and the sequences. Four acoustic parameters of the diphthongs and sequences are measured and compared in a production study. In addition, a perception experiment tests native speakers’ ability to correctly identify the two types of vocalic sequences. The results support the different representations proposed for [ea] vs. [ja], but not necessarily for [oa] vs. [wa]. This asymmetry is interpreted in the context of language-specific frequency differences, and of contrast maintenance. The study shows that detailed phonetic description is needed for a complete understanding of the phonological facts. 1 Introduction Glide-vowel sequences and rising diphthongs coexist in many languages. The interest of the Romanian data examined in this study lies in the striking auditory similarity between two

Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

A production study of Romaniandiphthongs and glide-vowel sequencesThis study compares two diphthongs ([ea], [oa]) and two glide-vowel sequences ([ja],[wa]) in Romanian. The diphthongs and the sequences are auditorily very similar, but theydiffer in their phonological patterning. An integrated production and perception experimentis conducted in search of perceptual and acoustic evidence for the different phonologicalrepresentations proposed for the diphthongs and the sequences. Four acoustic parameters ofthe diphthongs and sequences are measured and compared in a production study. In addition,a perception experiment tests native speakers’ ability to correctly identify the two types ofvocalic sequences. The results support the different representations proposed for [ea] vs.[ja], but not necessarily for [oa] vs. [wa]. This asymmetry is interpreted in the context oflanguage-specific frequency differences, and of contrast maintenance. The study shows thatdetailed phonetic description is needed for a complete understanding of the phonologicalfacts.1 IntroductionGlide-vowel sequences and rising diphthongs coexist in many languages. The interest of theRomanian data examined in this study lies in the striking auditory similarity between twosuch pairs: [ja] vs. [ea] and [wa] vs. [oa]. In all four cases the first element is pronounced asa glide. Although the language contains only one perfect minimal pair, the first one shown in(1), [ja]–[ea] and [wa]–[oa] occur in identical consonantal and prosodic environments.(1) a. bjátE‘poor’ fem.

Page 2: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

b. beátE‘drunk’ fem.kwártsu‘quartz’ def.skoártsE‘tree bark’polwáre‘pollution’paloáre‘pallor’From a purely descriptive standpoint, all the vocalic combinations illustrated in (1) could becalled diphthongs. However, since this paper argues for a representational difference between(1a) and (1b), a terminological distinction is also adopted, for the sake of clarity. Thus, the datain (1a) are said to contain glide-vowel sequences, and the data in (1b) diphthongs. The glide-vowel sequences differ in their phonological behavior from the diphthongs. The goal of thisstudy is to determine whether the glide-vowel sequences [ja] and [wa] are also phoneticallydifferent from the diphthongs [ea] and [oa], respectively. An integrated production andperception study is designed to answer this question.The paper is organized as follows. Section 2 presents the relevant data, focusing onthe phonological differences between glide-vowel sequences and diphthongs. Two differentphonological representations are proposed for sequences and for diphthongs, and the phoneticstudy undertaken here is designed to test their accuracy. Section 3 outlines earlier phoneticstudies of the Romanian glides and diphthongs. Section 4 describes the production experiment(4.1) and the perception experiment (4.2). Section 5 contains the discussion of the results, andsection 6 the conclusion.2 The distribution and phonological behavior of diphthongs

Page 3: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

and glide-vowel sequencesThe phonemic vowel inventory of Romanian is given in (2).(2) iÈueEoeaaoaglides:j wFollowing Chitoran (2001), the diphthongs are treated as the low counterparts of frontand back vowels, respectively. Some occurrences of the diphthongs are phonologically andmorphologically conditioned, but others are phonemic, as shown by (near) minimal pairs suchas the following:(3) témE‘theme’teámE‘fear’sérE‘greenhouse’seárE‘evening’flék‘shoe heel’fleák‘trifle’grótE‘cave’groápE‘ditch’póftE‘appetite’poártE

Page 4: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

‘gate’The diphthongs, whether phonemic or derived, always occur under stress, a consequence oftheir historical development through the diphthongization of stressed mid vowels. As for themorphologically conditioned diphthongs, their distribution correlates with the category ofgender in nouns, and with person in verbs (see Chitoran (2001, to appear) for a completephonological and morphological analysis). The analysis presented in this section is somewhatsimplified for clarity of exposition.The distribution of the two diphthongs and of the two glide-vowel sequences in theRomanian lexicon is relevant for the setup of the production and perception experiments. Thefirst observation that can be made is that the front diphthong [ea] occurs in fewer environmentsand is subject to more phonological restrictions than [oa]. For example, the diphthong [ea]does not occur in absolute word-initial position, whereas [oa] does (oále ‘pot’, oáste ‘army’).While both diphthongs can be preceded by a consonant, as shown in (3), only [oa] can alsobe preceded by a vowel (vioáre ‘violin’). Only [ea], however, can occur in absolute word-final position (steá ‘star’, neá ‘snow’, kureá ‘belt’). Both diphthongs are subject to metaphony, the process by which the vowel of an inflectionalmarker affects the height of the stressed vowel of the stem. There are two contexts in whichmetaphony occurs in Romanian. When the inflectional marker contains the high vowel [i], thestressed vowel of the stem cannot be a diphthong. It surfaces instead as the corresponding midvowel [o] or [e], as shown in the singular/plural alternations in (4). The examples contain two—

Page 5: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

plural markers: /-i/ surfacing as a secondary articulation [-∆] for feminine nouns, and /-uri/surfacing as [-ur∆] for neuter and feminine nouns.(4) Metaphony before a high front vowelSINGULAR PLURALpoártE pórtsj ‘gate’seárE sérj ‘evening’treábE tréburj ‘task’The second type of metaphony is triggered by the vowel /e/, but it only affects the frontdiphthong, [ea]. In the following examples, in (5), [-e] is a plural marker. Before [-e], thediphthong [ea] surfaces as [e] instead (cf. (5a)), but [oa] may surface as the stressed vowelof the stem (cf. (5b)).(5) Metaphony before a mid front vowelSINGULAR PLURALa. beátE béte ‘drunk’ fem.mireásE mirése ‘bride’b. koástE koáste ‘rib’koroánE koroáne ‘crown’Examples (4) and (5) illustrate a crucial property of the diphthongs, namely, theirphonological and morphological alternation with the monophthongal vowels [e] and [o].This particular behavior is captured by the phonological representation in (6), proposed forthe diphthongs by Chitoran (2001).(6) The phonological representation of diphthongseaµ

Page 6: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

σIn (6), both elements of the diphthong are represented as sharing a syllable nucleus. Accordingto this representation, diphthongs are predicted to function as a single unit and a singlesegment.Further support for this representation comes from the comparison with Frenchdiphthongs. A well-formed syllable onset in both French and Romanian consists of twoelements or a maximum of three, where the first of the three is a sibilant. In addition, bothlanguages have a glide formation process, which turns a high vowel into a glide when followedby another vowel. Glide formation applies if the onset contains only one segment (cf. (7a)),but it is blocked if the onset already has two segments (cf. (7b)), such as an obstruent and aliquid (Morin 1976, Kaye & Lowenstamm 1984, among others).(7) Glide formation in French and RomanianFrenchRomaniana. lje∼lije‘to bind’pjan‘piano’fjakÂ∼fijak‘carriage’italjanE‘Italian’ fem.nwe∼nue‘to tie’lwa∼lua‘to take’b. plije∗plje‘to fold’klijent

Page 7: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

∗kljent‘client’klue∗klwe‘to nail down’triumf∗trjumf‘triumph’Based on the blocked glide formation in obstruent-liquid onsets, the syllables in (8), whichdo surface in French, have been explained by treating the glide-vowel portion as a diphthongfully contained in the nucleus. The following sequences thus contain diphthongal nuclei:(8) French diphthongs (complex onsets)[wa]tÂwa‘three’pÂwa‘prey’[Ái]plÁi‘rain’Similarly, in Romanian [ea] and [oa] can be preceded by obstruent-liquid clusters, unlike [ja]and [wa].(9) Romanian diphthongs (complex onsets)[oa]broaskE‘frog’gloatE‘crowd’[ea]treaz‘awake’zdreants´

Page 8: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

‘rag’These data therefore support the representation in (6), according to which [ea] and [oa]constitute diphthongal syllable nuclei.Another important aspect of the proposed representation is the presence of a single mora.Although it is true that the Romanian diphthongs occur only in stressed syllables, thesesyllables can be either open or closed, and there is no evidence that diphthongal nuclei haveextra weight compared to monophthongal ones.Turning now to the glides [j] and [w], they are rendered in the orthography as ⟨i⟩ and ⟨u⟩,respectively. The orthography therefore encodes a distinction between ⟨ia⟩, the glide-vowelsequence, and ⟨ea⟩, which corresponds to the diphthong. Similarly, orthography distinguishesthe sequence ⟨ua⟩ and the diphthong ⟨oa⟩. The distinction made in the orthography issignificant to the extent to which it reflects a phonological contrast. The question asked in thisstudy is whether native speakers actually produce and perceive these contrasts ([ja] vs. [ea]and [wa] vs. [oa]). Impressionistically, [ja] and [ea] have very similar pronunciations, and[wa] sounds even more similar to [oa], if not identical.The distribution of the glides [j] and [w] in the language is assymetrical. The front glidehas a wider distribution than the back one. This asymmetry is not surprising, given that thepalatal glide is also typologically more common than the labio-velar one. In Romanian, [j] canbe found in a syllable onset in absolute word-initial position (járne ‘winter’, jépure ‘rabbit’,júte ‘quick’), after a consonant (pjátre ‘stone’, fjér ‘iron’) and word-internally after anothervowel (beját ‘boy’). It can also surface in a syllable coda both word-internally (hájne ‘coat’)

Page 9: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

and word-finally (púj ‘chicken’). The glide [w], however, never occurs in a word onset (thereare no words beginning orthographically with the sequence #⟨ua⟩). As a syllable onset, [w] isalways followed by the vowel [a], whereas [j] can also be followed by other vowels, as seen inmany of the preceding examples. As a syllable coda, [w] only surfaces in word-final position(bów ‘ox’), never word-internally, except for exactly two words (áwgust ‘August’ and awgúr‘augur’). A crucial difference in the phonological behavior of the glide-vowel sequences is the factthat [ja] and [wa] do not alternate with monophthongal vowels, the way the diphthongs do.The singular–plural alternations in (10), for example, still involve metaphony, but this timeonly the nucleus vowel is affected.(10) SINGULARPLURALpjátrEpjétre‘stone’bjátEbjéte‘poor’ fem.polwárepolwE´rj‘pollution’The representation in (11) has therefore been proposed as one that best captures this behaviorof glide-vowel sequences (Chitoran 2001).(11) The phonological representation of glide-vowel sequencesj aµσThis representation differs from that proposed for the diphthongs in the fact that only the vowel

Page 10: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

belongs to the nucleus, while the glide belongs to the onset. This syllable structure predictsthe resistance to glide formation following an obstruent-liquid cluster, illustrated earlier in (7)for French and Romanian.The distribution and the phonological behavior of the Romanian diphthongs comparedto glide-vowel sequences reveal several important observations. First, we see that the backdiphthong [oa] has fewer phonological restrictions than [ea], and thus occurs in morephonological environments. Second, the back glide [w] occurs less frequently in a syllableonset than [j]. Third, throughout the Romanian lexicon, the sequence [wa] is also much lessfrequent than the diphthong [oa]. At the same time, the distribution of [ja] and [ea] in thevocabulary is much more balanced. Frequency differences must be taken into considerationin a phonetic study, since they may affect native speakers’ production and perception of acontrast between glide-vowel sequences and diphthongs.In addition to the low frequency of [wa], it should be noted that this sequence occurspredominantly in loanwords, mostly from French. This explains the ⟨ua⟩ orthography, which isbased on the French spelling. A list of native and non-native words containing the orthographicsequence ⟨ua⟩ is given in (12a, b). The native Romanian examples in (12b) constitute anexhaustive list. A dash (–) indicates a morpheme boundary.(12) a. NON-NATIVEb. NATIVEkwarts‘quartz’alwat‘dough’kwantE/kwantum‘quantum’

Page 11: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

lw–a‘to take’kwartet‘quartet’lw–at‘taken’kwaternar‘quaternary’akwarelE‘watercolor’gwaSe‘gouache’nwantsE‘nuance’swav‘suave’an–w–al‘yearly’an–w–ar‘yearbook’akt–w–al‘current’tekst–w–al‘textual’kontSept–w–al‘conceptual’spirit–w–al‘spiritual’polw–are‘pollution’polw–at‘polluted’dilw–at‘diluted’eventwal‘possibly’The shorter words, such as swav ‘suave’ and the forms of the verb lwa ‘to take’, show

Page 12: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

some speaker variation, possibly related to differences in speech rate. The pronunciationssuav and lua are also attested. In fact, Academia Român˘a (1995), the Handbook of Ortho-graphy of the Romanian Academy, which is also a pronunciation guide, prescribes thedisyllabic pronunciation [u.a] for all the words in (12), with the exception of the first sixwords in (12a), which should be pronounced with monosyllabic [wa]. It is also interestingto notice that in all of these six words the glide-vowel sequence is preceded by a velarstop.The difference in orthography and the prescribed pronunciation have importantconsequences for a phonetic study. The data in (12) show that it is virtually impossibleto construct a well-balanced wordlist, with lexical items contrasting [wa] and [oa]. Anumber of predictions can also be made based on these two factors. If native speakers’pronunciation is affected by the ⟨ua⟩ spelling and by the prescribed pronunciation of thisvocalic sequence ([u.a]), then we expect to find a significant difference in the productionof the glide-vowel sequence relative to the diphthong. Specifically, we expect the glide-vowel sequence to be significantly longer than the diphthong, simply because the sequencemay count as two syllables (two vowels with hiatus), while the diphthong always counts asone.If, on the contrary, native speakers are not sensitive to the spelling difference and do notfollow the prescribed pronunciation, then it is less likely that a difference will be found in theproduction and perception of [wa] and [oa], since the wider phonological distribution of thediphthong [oa] may in some sense eclipse the sequence [wa].

Page 13: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

The question of a possible contrast between diphthongs and similar glide-vowel sequenceshas been addressed previously by Romanian phoneticians.

3 Previous phonetic studies of the [ja]--[ea], [wa]--[oa] distinctionThe question of the acoustic distinction between the two types of vocalic sequences, [ja]–[ea]and [wa]–[oa] in Romanian was addressed early on by Rosetti (1955, 1959), in an acoustic,articulatory and perception study. The acoustic study consists of duration measurements madeon kymograph recordings of (near) minimal pairs. Rosetti finds that [ja] and [wa] are distinctfrom [ea] and [oa], although they have a similar acoustic structure. In all four sequences,two elements can be clearly distinguished, the first of which is always a glide, whether highor mid.A first set of comparisons involves the front diphthong and glide. The duration of [ea] iscompared to that of [ja], then [ea] and [ja] are each compared to the vowel [a]. Rosetti foundthat the total durations of [ea] and [ja] are comparable, although the first element in [ea]is shorter than the [j] in [ja]. The duration of [a] also varies, making up for the difference.It should be noted that the target words were spoken in isolation, not embedded in carrierphrases, and that the results were not subjected to a statistical analysis.In the second set of measurements, the back diphthong [oa] is compared to themonophthongs [a], [u] and [o]. Here the first element of the diphthong can be clearly delimitedacoustically and identified as corresponding in height to the back mid vowel [o].More convincing evidence for the difference between [ea] and [ja] comes from articulatorydata, from comparisons of palatograms of [Cea] and [Cja] sequences. More contact is visible

Page 14: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

for the sequence [Cja], whereas [Cea] shows contact only on the edges of the palate.Finally, in a perception experiment, subjects were asked to listen to words containing[ja] and [ea], played backwards. Rosetti reports that in most cases the listeners were able toidentify the two elements in reverse order ([ae] or [aj]), and to distinguish between [j] and[e] as the final portion of the sequence. The perception experiment for [oa] and [wa] wasdifferent, and involved splicing. When the [o] portion was replaced by the vowel [u], subjectsreported perceiving a different diphthong rather than [oa] (Rosetti 1959: 41).A later acoustic study by Ulivi (1975) considers only the first element of the diphthongs,[e] and [o], respectively, and compares it to [j] and [w]. The comparisons are based onwideband spectrograms of the recordings of three speakers. The data consisted of words readin isolation, with the target segments in different positions in the word. The author reports adifference in duration, with the [e] and [o] portions being shorter than [j] and [w]. This resultis consistent with Rosetti’s, but Ulivi’s study also does not include a statistical analysis. Theacoustic descriptions are nevertheless valuable, since they do suggest a difference between [j],[w] and [e], [o]. For example, the author interprets [j] and [w] as being consonantal, based onthe larger amount of frication seen on the spectrogram, compared to [e] and [o]. She concludesthat [j], [w], [e] and [o] are all glides, but the first two are consonantal (‘semi-consonants’),while the diphthongs contain vocalic glides (‘semi-vowels’).The two studies do not provide a definitive answer, but they do support the phoneticdistinction between diphthongs and similar glide-vowel sequences in Romanian. The present

Page 15: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

experiment was designed to further clarify this question: do native speakers distinguishbetween [ja]–[ea] and [wa]–[oa], or not? If they do, how many and what particular acousticcues do they rely on? Does the phonetic realization support the phonological analysis whichargues for a distinction between [ja]–[ea] and [wa]–[oa]? The following section describesthe production and perception experiments undertaken to answer these questions.4 ExperimentSection 2 presented evidence that the diphthongs and glide-vowel sequences behave differentlyin the phonology. Diphthongs alternate with monophthongal vowels in singular-plural pairs(be ´ate –b ´ete ‘drunk’ fem.), while the glide portion of a glide-vowel sequence remainsunaffected in the same context, and only the nucleus vowel changes (bj ´ate –bj ´ete ‘poor’ fem.).Consequently, the diphthongs are analyzed as single segments, the low counterparts of frontand back vowels, respectively, whereas glide-vowel sequences are analyzed as a sequence oftwo segments, an onset glide and a nucleus vowel. Romanian orthography also maintains adistinction between all four: ⟨ea, oa, ia, ua⟩.Based on the differences outlined above, and on the results of previous studies by Rosettiand Ulivi, the following hypothesis can be formulated regarding the phonetics of diphthongsand glide-vowel sequences: [ja] and [wa] are produced differently from [ea] and [oa]. The firstportion is in all cases a glide, but they differ in height, and this difference should be reflectedin the acoustic structure. At the same time, if frequency and prescribed pronunciation play arole, then it is possible that [ja] and [wa] are phonetically identical to [ea] and [oa], in spite

Page 16: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

of the phonological difference. In this case we do not expect native speakers to be able toidentify four different sequences [ja], [wa], [ea] and [oa].If the first hypothesis is correct, native listeners should be able to identify four differentvocalic sequences, based on differences in several acoustic parameters. The first parameter isacoustic duration. The assumption here is that the phonology-to-phonetics mapping assignsindividual duration to individual segments. Therefore, if diphthongs constitute one segment,and glide-vowel sequences two segments, then, phonological weight being equal, the totalduration of diphthongs is expected to be shorter than that of glide-vowel sequences.A second acoustic parameter expected to differ is the transition portion characteristic of aglide, the change in frequency between the onset of the glide and the onset of the vowel portionfollowing it. The duration of this transition can also be affected by the one- vs. two-segmentdifference. Glide-vowel sequences are expected to have a longer transition duration than thediphthongs. If [j] and [w] differ from [e] and [o] in height, this difference is also expected toaffect the second formant transition, from the first vocalic element to the following vowel [a].The perception experiment is described in the following subsection,and the results are presented.4. Experiment : Perception4.1.1 MethodologyThe goal of the perception experiment is to determine whether native speakers can perceivethe difference between glide-vowel sequences and diphthongs..Listeners should be able to correctly identify [ja] and [ea], whose phoneticrealization was found to be different. Given that no difference was found in the production

Page 17: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

of [wa] and [oa], the prediction is that listeners will not be able to correctly identify theglide-vowel sequence and the diphthong.The tokens of one male speaker, speaker 1, were used in the perception experiment.Two separate tests were set up, one containing the [ja] and [ea] tokens, the other containingthe [wa] and [oa] tokens. Two repetitions of each token, chosen randomly, were includedin a practice test (24 [ja] sequences, 24 [ea] diphthongs, 10 [wa] sequences and 10 [oa]diphthongs). All the tokens recorded, with additional three repetitions of each, were includedin the actual perception test (108 [ja] sequences, 108 [ea] diphthongs, 45 [wa] sequences,45 [oa] diphthongs). The subjects were fourteen nativespeakers of Romanian, five male and nine female. Two of them were teenagers (aged 13 and15 years), and the rest were aged between 25 and 60 years. Some speakers took the testsindividually, others took it in small groups, on different days. The two tests ([ja]–[ea] and[wa]–[oa]) were given in different order to different groups of listeners. The subjects were asked to listen to the practice test first. For the actual tests they were askedto perform an identification task. This particular task was chosen rather than a discriminationtask precisely in order to make the test more difficult.. Each listener was given a sheet on which two different orthographieswere marked for each token: ⟨ia⟩ and ⟨ea⟩ for one test, ⟨ua⟩ and ⟨oa⟩ for the other. The listenerswere asked to circle the spelling they considered appropriate for each token they heard.4.2.2 ResultsThe answers to the two perception tests were analyzed by a binomial sampling distribution test.The two predictions based on the production study were both borne out.

Page 18: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

The sequence [ja] was correctly identified 89% of the time, and the diphthong [ea]78% of the time.The [wa]–[oa] test, as expected, presented more difficulty. The sequence [wa] was correctly identified 46%of the time, and the diphthong [oa] 48% of the time. The glide-vowel sequenceand the diphthong are produced differently and perceived as different. The phonologicaldifference between [wa] and [oa], however, is not reflected in their acoustic realization, andthus the sequence and the diphthong cannot be reliably identified.5 DiscussionThe first piece of information revealed by the results presented above concerns the use ofprescribed pronunciation. The findings show that native speakers do not follow the prescribedpronunciation, and the reliable evidence comes from the study of the [wa]–[oa] pair. Threeof the five ⟨ua⟩ words in the wordlist contain [u.a] prescribed pronunciations, and if thepronunciation had been observed, there should have been a statistically significant differencein all four acoustic parameters, primarily in the total duration and the transition duration. Thefact that no such difference was found confirms that all the orthographic ⟨ua⟩ sequences werepronounced as monosyllables, [wa]. The results of the currentstudy do not, therefore, support the presence of [u.a] sequences in the data.The repeated test, using a subset of the [ja]–[ea] list, also confirmed that the prescribed[i.a] pronunciation was not observed either. This strengthens the validity of the results for[ja] and [ea] and confirms that the statistically significant duration differences are not dueto prescribed pronunciation, but to structural differences between glide-vowel sequences and

Page 19: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

diphthongs.It is, therefore, safe to conclude that different phonological representations for [ja] and [ea]are supported by the results of the production-perception study. The phonological behavior of[ja] as a sequence of two segments and of [ea] as one segment is supported by the durationdifferences. The total duration and transition duration of glide-vowel sequences is significantlylonger than that of diphthongs. The first elements of [ja] and [ea] are not identical, but differin height, as shown by significant differences found in the F2 onset and in transition rate. Thesequence [ja] has a higher F2 onset, corresponding to a high glide, and a faster transitionrate, whereas the diphthong [ea] has a lower F2 onset, corresponding to a mid glide, and alower transition rate. Moreover, native speakers of Romanian can reliably identify [ja] and[ea]. These results are also consistent with the acoustic and articulatory descriptions of [ja]and [ea] by Rosetti (1955, 1959) and Ulivi (1975).The phonological difference between [wa] and [oa], however, is not directly encodedin the phonetics. The glide-vowel sequence and the diphthong are comparable in all fouracoustic parameters measured, and native speakers could not identify them in the perceptionexperiment. These results go against the earlier interpretation of the acoustic data by Rosettiand Ulivi. Does this mean that Romanian does not actually distinguish between a back glide-vowel sequence and a back diphthong although the phonology and the orthography suggestthat they both exist?The existence of only one phonetic realization for both [wa] and [oa] can be explainedby two factors, one language-specific and one universal. It has already been shown that in

Page 20: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

Romanian the sequence ⟨ua⟩ occurs in considerably fewer lexical items than the diphthong⟨oa⟩, and primarily in loanwords. The relatively limited distribution of ⟨ua⟩ may be responsiblefor the phonetic neutralization between the glide-vowel sequence and the diphthong. At thesame time, the acoustic difference between two back rounded glides is harder to maintainthan one between two front glides. Back vowels and glides are characterized by a lowsecond formant, and the effect of pronounced lip rounding characteristic of back glidesfurther reduces the distance between the first two formants. This means that the possibilityof maintaining a qualitative difference between back rounded glides is limited relative to adistinction between front glides. We may conclude that the phonological analysis of [wa]and [oa] can be maintained, but in the case of back glide-vowel sequences and diphthongs aprocess of phonetic neutralization takes place, motivated by the relatively low frequency of[wa] sequences, and by the difficulty of maintaining a contrast between two back roundedglides [w] and [o].6 ConclusionThe integrated perception study presented in this paper illustrates theimportance of considering phonological information in testing a proposedphonological analysis and providing an accurate phonological description of a language. Thedistinct phonological representations proposed for front glide-vowel sequences and diphthongsin Romanian are supported by acoustic data and by the results of a perception experiment. Atthe same time, the study revealed a phonetic neutralization that takes place between the backglide-vowel sequence [wa] and the back diphthong [oa]. Acoustically there is no difference

Page 21: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

between the sequence and the diphthong, and listeners cannot reliably identify either one.This neutralization can be explained by a language specific difference in frequency, andby the difficulty of maintaining a contrast between two back rounded glides. It is thereforeproposed that underlying /ua/ sequences undergoing glide formation have the same phoneticrealization as the diphthong [oa].ReferencesACADEMIA ROM ˆAN ˘A (1995). Îndreptar ortografic, ortoepic si de punctuatie [Handbook of orthography,orthoepy, and punctuation]. Bucharest: Institutul de lingvistica ‘Iorgu Iordan’, Univers enciclopedic.CHITORAN, I. (2001). The Phonology of Romanian: A Constraint-based Approach. Berlin & New York:Mouton de Gruyter.CHITORAN, I. . The phonology and morphology of Romanian diphthongization. Probus.HAM, W. (1998). Phonetic and Phonological Aspects of Geminate Timing. Ph.D dissertation, CornellUniversity. (Published by Routledge Outstanding Dissertations in Linguistics, 2001.)KAYE, J. D. & LOWENSTAMM, J. (1984). De la syllabicité. In Dell, F., Hirst., D. & Vergnaud, J-R. (eds.),La forme sonore du langage, 123–159. Paris: Herman.LIBERMAN, A., DELATTRE, P., GERSTMAN,L.&COOPER, F. (1956). Tempo of frequency change as a cuefor distinguishing classes of speech sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology 52, 127–137.MORIN, Y-C. (1976). Phonological tensions in French. In Hensey, F. & Luj`an, M. (eds.), Current Studiesin Romance Linguistics. 37–49. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.REN, H. (1986). On the Acoustic Structure of Diphthongal Syllables. Ph.D. dissertation, UCLA. (Publishedas UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 65.)ROSETTI, A. (1955). Cercetari experimentale asupra diftongilor românesti [Experimental studies of the

Page 22: Документ Microsoft Office Word (3)

Romanian diphthongs]. Studii si cercetari lingvistice 5, 7–27.ROSETTI, A. (1959). Recherches sur les diphtongues roumaines. Bucarest & Kopenhagen: EdituraAcademiei RPR & Munksgard.ULIVI, A. (1975). Observatii asupra structurii acustice a semiconsoanelor românesti [y] si [w] [Remarkson the acoustic structure of the Romanian semi-consonants [y] and [w]]. Fonetica si dialectologie 9