Transcript
  • Spelling as a self-teaching mechanismin orthographic learningDaphna Shahar-Yames and David L. Share

    University of Haifa

    The present study examined the possibility that spelling fulfils a self-teachingfunction in the acquisition of orthographic knowledge because, like decoding, itrequires close attention to letter order and identity as well as to word-specificspellingsound mapping. We hypothesised that: (i) spelling would lead to significant(i.e. above-chance) levels of orthographic learning; (ii) spelling would actually resultin superior learning relative to reading owing to the additional processing demandsinvoked when spelling; (iii) there would be stronger outcomes for post-test spellingproduction compared with spelling recognition; and (iv) relative to reading, spellingwould produce superior orthographic learning in the case of later-occurringorthographic detail compared with information appearing earlier in the letter string.In a fully within-subjects design, third grade Hebrew readers were exposed to novelletter strings presented in three conditions: spelling, reading and an unseen controlcondition. With the exception of the position by condition interaction (our fourthhypothesis), which, although in the expected direction, failed to attain significance,all hypotheses were supported. These data highlight yet another dimension ofreadingwriting reciprocity by suggesting that spelling offers a powerful self-teaching tool in the compilation of word-specific orthographic representations.

    The acquisition of orthographic knowledge, whether represented in localist (e.g.

    Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon & Ziegler, 2001) or distributed architectures (Plaut,

    McClelland, Seidenberg & Patterson, 1996), is agreed to be one of the cornerstones of

    literacy. This knowledge consists of both word-specific orthographic representations

    critical for rapid automatised word recognition (and proficient spelling) and generalised

    (i.e. productive) knowledge of orthographic conventions (Siegel, Share & Geva, 1995).

    According to the self-teaching hypothesis (Jorm & Share, 1983; Share, 1995), this

    knowledge is accumulated largely via the process of phonologically recoding (i.e.

    decoding) novel letter strings all letter strings being unfamiliar at some point. Each

    successful decoding encounter with an unfamiliar letter string is assumed not only to

    enable the reader to identify unfamiliar words independently but also, perhaps more

    importantly, to provide an opportunity to acquire and consolidate word-specific (and

    general) orthographic information. This exhaustive (or near-exhaustive) decoding

    process1 is held to be critical in forming well-specified orthographic representations

    because it draws attention to the order and identity of letters and how they map

    phonological representations. In a sense then, the decoding process helps the reader see

    the logic of a words spelling, that is, the spellingsound mapping. Thus, phonological

    Journal of Research in Reading, ISSN 0141-0423 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00359.xVolume 31, Issue 1, 2008, pp 2239

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008. Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road,Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA

  • recoding is assumed to fulfil a self-teaching function, enabling the reader to inde-

    pendently acquire the orthographic representations crucial for skilled word recognition.

    The self-teaching notion has now received support from a modest number of empirical

    studies (Bowey & Miller, 2007; Bowey & Muller, 2005; Cunningham, 2006;

    Cunningham, Perry, Stanovich & Share, 2002; de Jong & Share, 2007; Kyte & Johnson,

    2006; Nation, Angell & Castles, 2007; Share, 1999, 2004; Share & Shalev, 2004).

    Collectively, these studies have demonstrated rapid and durable orthographic learning via

    self-teaching across orthographies of varying transparency as well as in both oral and

    silent reading. Furthermore, levels of orthographic learning have been shown to be

    closely tied to levels of target word decoding whether the latter are naturally occurring or

    created artificially via experimental manipulation (Bowey & Miller, 2007; Cunningham,

    2006; Cunningham et al., 2002; Kyte & Johnson, 2006; Share, 1999; Share & Shalev,

    2004; but see Nation et al., 2007).

    Decoding, however, is not the only process that has been hypothesised to have a self-

    teaching function. For example, the role of context as a supplementary (but not stand-

    alone) source of self-teaching has also been debated (see, e.g., Landi, Perfetti, Bolger,

    Dunlap & Foorman, 2006; Martin-Chang, Levy & ONeill, 2007; Share, 1995; Tunmer &

    Chapman, 1998). The present investigation considers another candidate process that

    might fulfil a self-teaching function in the growth of orthographic knowledge spelling.

    Our study was motivated by the idea that the process of spelling, like the process of

    decoding, requires the writer to attend to orthographic details (letter identity and order)

    and sub-lexical print-to-sound relationships in a comprehensive manner. Several lines of

    evidence are consistent with the idea that spelling influences reading.

    First, there are longitudinal studies of early (preschool/kindergarten) writing that have

    revealed significant predictive relationships with later first-grade reading (e.g. Mann,

    Tobin & Wilson, 1987; Morris & Perney, 1984; Shatil, Share & Levin, 2000). Second,

    longitudinal studies employing causal modelling techniques have reported a direct causal

    pathway from spelling to reading (e.g. Berninger, Abbot, Abbot, Graham & Richards,

    2002; Ellis & Cataldo, 1990; Shanahan, 1984). Finally, this causal link has been directly

    confirmed in experimental training studies. In their study of a group of kindergarten

    children, Ehri and Wilce (1987) taught their experimental group to segment and spell

    (with the aid of letter tiles) words and pseudowords printed in phonetically simplified

    spellings. The control group practised associating letters with isolated sounds. The

    experimental group was found to be more successful than the control group learning to

    read words comprising trained letters. Replicating and extending the Ehri and Wilce

    findings, Uhry and Shepherd (1993) found that training first graders in segmenting and

    spelling phonetically regular words (using lettered blocks) improved their pseudoword

    reading ability, real-word reading and even oral text reading speed and accuracy.

    It should be noted, however, that both these studies of spelling training among

    beginning readers may be demonstrating general benefits of superior working knowledge

    of the alphabetic code among children still learning the rudiments of print-to-sound

    decoding rather than word-specific orthographic knowledge of the kind on which

    automatised skilled word recognition depends. In this regard, it is worth remarking that

    theoretical models of reading/spelling development (e.g. Frith, 1985) often place the

    locus of spelling-to-reading benefits at the initial code-breaking stage. However, a study

    by Cunningham and Stanovich (1990) provides some clues on the issue of acquiring

    word-specific spellings because, unlike the previous two studies, this study examined the

    contribution of spelling to first-graders reading and spelling for a set of words, most of

    SPELLING AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEARNING 23

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • which have highly unpredictable spellings, which depend heavily on word-specific

    orthographic knowledge (e.g. type, buy, calf, sign). This particular study focused

    specifically on the contribution of the motor component in handwriting to spelling and

    reading by comparing three modes of spelling, namely handwriting, computer keyboard

    typing and manipulating letter tiles. Cunningham and Stanovich (1990) found superior

    spelling outcomes among children who wrote words using pencil and paper (using the

    Simultaneous Oral Spelling technique), compared with typing them on a computer or

    manipulating letter tiles (experiment 1). In their second experiment, although handwriting

    once again was superior to the other two spelling conditions, there were no significant

    differences between the three conditions on reading. Nonetheless, it is important to note

    that after spelling training children in all three conditions successfully read approximately

    8 of the 30 training items compared with only 4 at pre-test. Unfortunately, spelling

    success was very low (averaging o1 in 10 words correct). Because at least six wordscould be spelled successfully on a purely phonetic basis (e.g. small, check, shelf, man,

    help, tell, car and possibly tribe), it is unclear to what extent word-specific orthographic

    knowledge had been acquired. This potential problem also applies to the reading

    outcomes. The data from these three experimental training studies among beginning

    readers, therefore, remain inconclusive. The present study sets out to address the issue of

    the contribution of spelling to the acquisition of word-specific orthographic knowledge

    among somewhat older children (Grade 3) who, in Hebrews highly regular orthography,

    are well past the initial code-learning stage.

    As a general prediction, we hypothesised that spelling would yield significant (i.e.

    above chance) orthographic learning in view of the fact that, like decoding, spelling

    requires letter-by-letter consideration of word-specific spellingsound (or more precisely

    soundspelling) relationships (Perfetti, 1997). Beyond this general prediction, however,

    we suspected that spelling might actually lead to superior orthographic learning

    compared with decoding in view of the greater processing demands involved in spelling

    (Bosman & van Orden, 1997). In the process of spelling unfamiliar words, the writer

    must segment the spoken form into its component phonemes. This same phoneme

    segmentation skill has been shown to be one of the most powerful predictors of early

    reading (see, e.g. Share, Jorm, Maclean & Matthews, 1984). Given the fact that most

    orthographies have multiple ways of representing many phonemes (Daniels & Bright,

    1996), the writer must select the correct word-specific grapheme associated with each

    phoneme. Even when spelling highly familiar words, the writer is obliged to retrieve the

    elements of the visual form of the word whereas reading only requires recognition

    (Perfetti, 1997). When spelling, furthermore, the writer must process each and every

    letter. In reading, on the other hand, the orthographic representation may be less than

    fully specified yet sufficient for word identification, particularly when encountered in

    meaningful context (Holmes & Carruthers, 1998).

    According to the self-teaching hypothesis, the initial encounter with an unfamiliar

    word is likely to be exhaustive or near-exhaustive but it seems likely that subsequent

    occurrences, especially in connected text, may be less exhaustive. In spelling, however,

    each and every spelling obliges the writer to process the complete letter array. In view of

    these differences, we hypothesised that, relative to reading, spelling would provide

    superior orthographic learning in the case of later-occurring orthographic detail compared

    with earlier-occurring information. Thus, we predicted an interaction between learning

    condition (reading versus spelling) and position (earlier-occurring versus later-occurring

    orthographic information).

    24 SHAHAR-YAMES and SHARE

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • Another unique dimension of spelling likely to enhance orthographic learning concerns

    the motor-kinesthetic aspects of spelling production (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990;

    Graham & Weintraub, 1996). In this regard, it is noteworthy that there exists a long-

    venerated tradition of multi-sensory teaching, emphasising the importance of tactile-

    kinesthetic information in the process of writing as a technique for remediating reading

    difficulties (Fernald, 1943; Gillingham & Stillman, 1956; Hulme, 1983; Montessori,

    1915; Orton, 1928). The stated rationale for these practices is that the movements made

    when writing (or tracing) the shapes and sequence of letters in a word are crucial in

    providing additional associative links between spelling and sound. Several empirical

    studies have now supported this assumption (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990; Hulme &

    Bradley, 1984; Hulme, Monk & Ives, 1987). Consequently, we anticipated that spelling

    would have an added advantage over reading in the case of orthographic learning tasks

    that require production of the target spelling compared with mere recognition that does

    not invoke motor-kinesthetic processes.

    Finally, it should be acknowledged that beyond the individual elements enumerated

    above, spelling demands the integration of multiple sources of information from several

    modalities including visual-perceptual, motor-kinesthetic and linguistic information

    (Abbot & Berninger, 1993; Graham & Weintraub, 1996). Understandably, the process of

    spelling typically requires more time than decoding.

    To recap, we predicted not only that spelling a novel word would lead to significant

    orthographic learning but also that spelling would actually be superior to reading (i.e.

    decoding). In addition, we expected this advantage to be most clearly evident in the case

    of spelling production compared with spelling recognition. Finally, we predicted that the

    advantage of spelling over reading would be more pronounced for later-occurring

    orthographic detail compared with earlier-occurring detail, that is, we expected an

    interaction between learning condition and target letter position.

    These hypotheses were tested in a sample of third grade (i.e. post-novice) Hebrew

    readers. In a fully within-subjects design, each child was exposed to novel letter strings

    (legal pseudowords) in three conditions: reading, spelling and a control (unseen)

    condition. A week later, orthographic learning was assessed using measures of spelling

    production and spelling recognition (orthographic choice).

    Method

    Sample

    This study took place in several third grade classrooms in two regular schools in an area

    of Haifa considered to be of average socioeconomic status. One of these schools is

    situated in the city of Haifa (n5 19) and the other in the Haifa periphery (n5 26). Fifty-three parents gave written consent for their children to participate in this study. Eight

    children failed to complete all three testing sessions (several of whom were very weak

    readers), leaving a final sample of 45 participants all native-born Israelis (19 boys and

    26 girls) with a mean age of 8.9 years (range, 8.410.0; SD, 3.80 months). All participants

    were native Hebrew speakers with the exception of one child whose native tongue was

    Russian and two other participants who spoke Hebrew and Russian at home (all three of

    them showed the same pattern of results).

    SPELLING AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEARNING 25

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • The reading ability of this sample was slightly above national norms. Table 1 presents

    data on standardised tests of isolated word and pseudoword reading of the research

    sample and the national norms (Shany, Lachman, Shalem, Bahat & Zeiger, 2006). This

    sample scored 90% on word reading accuracy (national norm, 82%), read at a rate of 45

    words per minute (wpm) (norm, 40), scored 72% on pseudoword reading accuracy (there

    are no national norms for this measurement) and read at a rate of 24wpm (norm,

    19wpm). This instrument yields a percentile-rank-based classification divided into eight

    categories (ranging from the lowest 1st7th percentile to the highest 90th percentile

    and above). Our sample included children representing all but one (the lowest) of these

    eight percentile bands.

    Design

    This study was a fully within-subjects design with three conditions: reading, a reading-

    and-spelling composite and a control condition. Each participant was exposed to four

    target pseudowords in each of the three conditions 12 targets in all. Target items were

    rotated across conditions in order to neutralise any uncontrolled item differences. In

    addition, each target appeared in two alternate homophonic spellings with half of the

    sample seeing one spelling and half the other. Each target pseudoword was embedded in

    a pair of sentences, for example, Taguam is a very distant land. Taguam lies beyond the

    hills of darkness. Order of conditions was also rotated across subjects.

    The study was carried out in three consecutive sessions each separated by an interval of

    a week. The first two sessions included a learning phase (exposure to the targets and

    sentences) two targets in each of the two experimental conditions (reading and reading/

    spelling) and, in the second and third sessions, the post-testing of targets from the

    previous meeting. Post-testing also included the control condition in which targets which

    were not previously seen were post-tested. Thus, in the first session, the children were

    exposed to two targets in the reading condition and two in the reading/spelling condition.

    A week later, these four targets were post-tested together with another two (unseen)

    control items. Immediately following post-testing, another four targets were viewed

    two in the reading and two in the reading/spelling conditions. The third and final session

    was devoted to post-testing these four items and a further two control items.

    Learning phase

    Spelling. This condition consisted of reading followed by spelling because the learning

    of new orthographic forms via spelling alone has limited ecological validity.2 Furthermore,

    Table 1. Scores (means, standard deviations and percentiles) on standardised reading tests (Shany et al.,

    2006) of isolated word and pseudoword reading regarding accuracy (% errors) and fluency (wpm) for the

    research sample (n5 45) compared to national norms.

    Standardised tests Sample National norms

    M (SD) Percentiles M (SD)

    Reading isolated words accuracy (% errors) 10.08 (6.07) 6580 18.4 (15.67)

    Reading isolated words fluency (wpm) 45.48 (14.47) 6580 39.54 (16.87)

    Pseudoword reading (% errors) 27.75 (15.17) * *

    Pseudoword reading (wpm) 24.21 (7.51) 6580 19.32 (8.31)

    *There are no national norms for pseudoword reading accuracy for the third grade.

    26 SHAHAR-YAMES and SHARE

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • when spelling words, the writer is likely to read the production in order to check spelling.

    Thus, reading is often an inseparable part of spelling (Ehri, 2000; Perfetti, 1997).

    In the spelling condition, the participant first read a target pseudoword that appeared in

    each of two consecutive sentences (mean sentence length, 5.4 Hebrew words; range, 47

    words) and then was asked to write down the target from memory with the sentences

    removed from sight. For the first spelling production the target word was spoken aloud

    (dictated) by the experimenter. The childs spelling was then removed from sight and

    then he or she was requested to write it out again. On this occasion, the target was not

    named. In this condition, therefore, each participant was exposed to the target five times

    twice in reading, twice via spelling and once in spoken form.

    Reading. In this condition, the participant only read the targets. These were embedded in

    the same sentences as the spelling condition. However, in this condition, each pair of

    sentences was read twice instead of once, making a total of four target exposures.

    Immediately after sentence reading a comprehension question was posed orally in which

    the target word was specifically named. For example, after reading Akezet is a serious

    disease. You wouldnt want to get Akezet, the participant was asked Whats akezet? The

    dual aim here (apart from ensuring reading for meaning) was: (i) to provide an additional

    exposure to the spoken form of the target, thereby matching the spelling condition as

    described above; and (ii) to extend the overall processing time in the reading condition to

    make it more comparable to the spelling condition, which pilot work had indicated was a

    lengthier procedure.

    Control. The targets in this condition were neither seen nor heard, only post-tested.

    Target words

    The pool of target words was selected on the basis of suitability established in prior work

    at comparable ages (Share, 1999, 2004; Share & Shalev, 2004). Each of the target words

    used in the present study contained at least two homophonic letters comprising four of

    the six pairs of homophonic consonantal letters that exist in Hebrew (the words that were

    used as stimuli are presented in the Appendix). It is worth noting that items with two (or

    even more) homophonic letters are very common in Hebrew as approximately one-third

    of Hebrew phonemes can be transcribed by means of two alternate (homophonic) letters

    a feature that creates considerable challenges in the acquisition of spelling (Geva &

    Siegel, 2000; Ravid, 2002). Word length varied between four and six (consonantal) letters

    (M5 4.7 letters). Although a majority of these items were bisyllabic, this set also includedfour three-syllable and one four-syllable words. We chose to present our targets in a

    meaningful context to establish ecological validity and ensure that children read for meaning.

    Post-tests of orthographic learning

    Two measures of orthographic learning were administered 1 week after exposure to target

    words and sentences.

    Spelling production. In this dictation task, participants were requested to write a target

    word named by the experimenter. Productions were scored for both overall spelling

    accuracy (either as correct or incorrect) and accuracy of producing the two critical target

    letters.

    SPELLING AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEARNING 27

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • Orthographic choice. In this measure of orthographic learning, four fully homophonic

    spellings of the target were presented. This foursome included all four combinations of

    the two target letters, that is, the original spelling that appeared in the learning phase and

    three homophonic foils.

    Composite orthographic learning. Following Cunningham et al. (2002), we also

    combined the above two tasks into a single composite measure by averaging the

    (standardised) scores for spelling production and orthographic choice. The production

    component was derived by averaging standardised scores for overall (whole-word)

    spelling success and target letter spellings. An identical pattern of results was also

    obtained when we used only whole-word spelling or only target letter spelling as the

    spelling production measure.

    Assessment of general reading ability

    Two reading tasks were administered to gauge overall levels of reading ability in this

    sample. These were two recently standardised tests of word and pseudoword reading

    accuracy and fluency that supplied national norms (the Alef-to-Taf Battery for the

    Diagnosis of Reading and Writing Disabilities according to National Norms; Shany et al.,

    2006).

    Reading isolated words (Shany et al., 2006). This test of oral reading accuracy and

    fluency contains a list of 38 words varying in frequency, length and morphological

    structure.

    Pseudoword reading (Shany et al., 2006). The test of pseudoword reading accuracy and

    fluency includes 33 words, of which 24 are structured according to the conventions of

    Hebrew morphology and an additional nine are morphologically novel items.

    Procedure

    The experiment was conducted with individual children over three separate testing

    sessions. These sessions took place during regular school hours in a separate room

    allocated to the researchers.

    As already indicated, session 1 included a learning phase in which children read two

    targets (embedded in meaningful sentences) and spelled another two targets (with the

    order counterbalanced across participants). At the beginning of the first session, the

    Shany et al. (2006) word reading test was administered to establish minimal reading

    competence. Although this test revealed several very weak readers, two of whom failed to

    complete all the sessions, no child was dropped from the sample purely on the basis of

    their standardised test result. A week later, session 2 began with the tests of orthographic

    learning for the four targets learned in session 1 as well as two unseen control words. This

    was followed by exposure to two more targets in reading and two more in spelling. The

    final session, session 3, took place 7 days later and included the post-tests for the session

    2 words together with the remaining two control items. Immediately afterwards,

    participants completed the standardised tests of general reading ability. Finally, the Shany

    et al. (2006) pseudoword reading test was administered.

    The session began by showing the child a card with two printed sentences. The child

    was asked to read the sentences aloud as accurately as possible. In the case of the spelling

    28 SHAHAR-YAMES and SHARE

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • condition, the child was also told that he or she would be asked to write something. The

    child was also told that the experimenter would be measuring time with a stopwatch and

    would be recording the childs reading with an audiotape recorder. The researcher hand-

    recorded the total time elapsing between the commencement of reading the first word and

    the response given to the comprehension question (in the reading condition) or the final

    letter spelled (in the spelling condition). Errors committed while reading and spelling the

    target words were also recorded.

    Reading condition. Instructions were as follows: Read aloud the sentence on the card,

    then turn the card over and read the next sentence. After the child finished reading both

    sentences, he or she was asked to repeat this procedure. After completing this (thereby

    having read the target words four times), the comprehension question was given. No

    assistance was given with either reading the sentences or the comprehension question.

    Only general non-contingent encouragement was given during reading and questioning.

    Spelling. In this condition, children were first asked to read aloud the two sentences as in

    the reading condition. After they had read each of the two sentences once, they were

    given a piece of paper and asked to write the target word named by the experimenter.

    This piece of paper was then removed and the child was asked to write the word once

    again (this time without any mention of the pronunciation). If the child was unsure about

    the spelling, he or she was told to try to write the word as best he or she could. No

    additional help was given.

    Session 2

    The second session contained two parts post-testing and exposure to new targets. The

    first part tested orthographic learning of the targets seen a week earlier and included both

    spelling production and orthographic choice. These two measures were administered in a

    fixed order spelling production and then orthographic choice. (No order effects have

    been found with these measures, see Share, 2004.)

    Post-test spelling (dictation). Each participant was asked to write a spoken target. If the

    child seemed unsure, he or she was told to write it as best he or she could.

    Orthographic choice. Following spelling, the child was presented with a page with the

    four homophonic spellings of the target and requested to indicate the correct target. This

    procedure was repeated for all six of the targets. The order of the six targets as well as the

    location of the correct spelling among the four alternatives were rotated from subject to

    subject.

    The second part of this session was devoted to reading and spelling the remaining four

    targets.

    Session 3

    This final meeting involved only the post-testing of the four targets (and two controls)

    from the second session a week earlier. The standardised test of pseudoword reading was

    then administered. This list was left until last in order to avoid any possible interference

    with the target pseudowords.

    SPELLING AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEARNING 29

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • Results

    Target word decoding in the exposure/learning phase

    Table 2 presents the data on reading and writing success during initial exposure to the

    targets read in the two experimental conditions.

    It can be seen from Table 2 that in both conditions performance levels were high the

    vast majority of targets were accurately decoded and spelled (unassisted). It is also

    evident that the level of spelling success matched decoding success. Fortuitously, there

    were no significant differences between any of these figures (all t values were less than

    unity). Thus, not only was decoding accuracy in the two conditions similar but also

    spelling success was comparable to reading success both within the reading/spelling

    condition and across the conditions; that is, spelling success matched the reading success

    in the reading-only condition.

    The fact that similar levels of success were achieved in both the reading and spelling

    conditions is gratifying in that it ensures that comparisons of orthographic learning across

    the two conditions are not confounded by initial differences in learning at the time of

    exposure.

    Post-test orthographic learning

    In this study overall post-test orthographic learning was first evaluated by comparing

    target spelling reproduction (spelling) and identification (orthographic choice) to chance

    levels. In the case of spelling the complete letter string in which a range of errors are

    possible (e.g. letter omission, interpolation, transposition, etc.), performance was

    compared with the control condition alone.

    If no orthographic learning had taken place, it would be expected that correct

    reproduction of the critical target letters would be 50% per letter, or 25% for the correct

    production of both letters. For three of the four homophonic letters, there were only two

    phonologically plausible letters.3

    Table 3 presents the post-test orthographic learning data (means and standard

    deviations in percentages) across the three conditions (reading, reading/spelling and

    control).

    Above-chance levels of orthographic learning in reading and spelling conditions

    First, it can be seen that levels of performance were very close to chance in the control

    condition in which targets were only post-tested without any prior exposure. The

    Table 2. Mean accuracy (in percentages) and standard deviations for target reading/writing in reading and

    spelling conditions, length of time spent reading (or reading-and-spelling) in the two conditions (in seconds)

    and percent response accuracy for comprehension questions (reading condition only) (n5 45).

    Condition Target

    decoding/spelling

    (% accuracy)

    Duration of

    time spent in

    condition in seconds

    Reading

    comprehension

    (% accuracy)

    M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

    1. Reading 82.9 (22.90) 23.4 (3.23) 93.9 (12.10)

    2. Reading/spelling 85.3 (15.94) 27.5 (4.10)

    Reading 86.1 (17.52)

    Spelling 84.4 (22.63)

    30 SHAHAR-YAMES and SHARE

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • Table3.Post-testorthographiclearning(m

    eansandstandarddeviationsin

    percentages)acrossthreeconditions(control,reading,reading/spelling)with

    tvalues

    andeffect

    size

    (Eta

    Squared)(n545).

    Post-testmeasure

    Condition

    Control

    Reading

    Spelling

    Reading/Control

    Reading/Spelling

    Spelling/Control

    M(S

    D)

    M(S

    D)

    M(S

    D)

    tZ2

    tZ2

    tZ2

    Spellingproduction

    Whole

    word

    22.2a(19.38)

    26.1a(19.91)

    37.8b(25.35)

    0.93

    .19

    2.36

    *.11

    3.39

    **

    .21

    Target

    letters(n52)

    47.8a(14.91)

    54.7b(14.67)

    61.9c(19.02)

    2.05

    *.09

    2.04

    *.09

    4.00

    **

    .27

    Letter1

    46.7a(23.60)

    55.0ab(18.92)

    60.0b(29.39)

    1.80

    .07

    .99

    .02

    2.51

    *.13

    Letter2

    48.9a(20.61)

    54.4ab(25.16)

    63.9b(22.96)

    1.08

    .03

    1.68

    .06

    3.48

    **

    .22

    Orthographic

    choice(4-choice)

    23.9a(21.29)

    37.4b(20.61)

    41.9b(26.02)

    2.82

    **

    .15

    .86

    .02

    3.46

    **

    .21

    Composite

    orthographic

    learning(z)

    0.79a(1.39)

    0.07b(1.68)

    0.67c(1.36)

    2.67

    **

    .14

    2.12

    *.09

    4.81

    **

    .35

    No

    te:Superscriptsindicatedifferencesbetweenconditions;conditionswithdifferentsuperscriptsaresignificantlydifferent.

    *Indicates

    significance

    at0.05level.

    **Indicates

    significance

    at0.01level.

    SPELLING AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEARNING 31

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • outcomes for the two experimental conditions, as anticipated, contrasted sharply with the

    control data. Spelling of the individual target letters (treated separately) in the reading

    condition did not exceed the designated 50% chance level; first letter, t(44)5 1.77,p5 .083, second letter, t(44)5 1.19, ns (whole word spelling, as already noted, was notevaluated against chance performance). On the other hand, performance on the combined

    target letter spelling and on the orthographic choice measure, as well as the composite

    measure of orthographic learning, exceeded chance in both experimental conditions

    according to one-sample t-tests, indicating significant orthographic learning. These data

    replicate a number of prior studies indicating robust orthographic learning among young

    non-novice (Grade 2 and 3) Hebrew readers following unassisted reading of targets

    embedded in text (Share, 1999, 2004; Share & Shalev, 2004).

    The data from the reading/spelling condition confirm the central prediction that

    spelling would also result in significant orthographic learning. However, we expected that

    spelling would not only yield reliable orthographic learning but also actually be superior

    to reading owing to the unique processing demands involved in reproducing (from

    memory) the complete array of letters. To examine this hypothesis we turn to the

    comparisons between the conditions.

    Comparisons across reading, spelling and control conditions

    Planned comparisons in the form of paired-sample t-tests were used to examine pair-wise

    differences between (i) the control condition and reading, (ii) the control condition and

    spelling and (iii) reading versus spelling.

    The reading condition led to significant orthographic learning (relative to the control

    condition) in orthographic choice, t(44)5 2.82, po.01, and on production of the targetletters (combining both letters), t(44)5 2.05, po.05, although not on the target lettersconsidered individually first letter, t(44)5 1.80, p5 .079, or second (non-initial) targetletter, t(44)5 1.08, ns or the spelling of the entire string of letters (whole word),t(44)o1.0, ns. Performance in the reading condition also exceeded control levels on thecomposite measure of orthographic learning, t(44)5 2.70, po.05.Spelling, on the other hand, led to more powerful and more consistent learning

    outcomes. Spelling surpassed control levels significantly on all six measures: whole word

    spelling, t(44)5 3.39, po.005; combined target letter spelling, t(44)5 4.00, po.000;first target letter spelling, t(44)5 2.51, po.05; second letter spelling, t(44)5 3.48,po.005; orthographic choice, t(44)5 3.46, po.005; and the composite, t(44)5 4.74,po.000. Absolute scores in this reading/spelling condition also exceeded performance inthe reading condition (at least numerically) on each of the six measures,4 reaching

    statistical significance in three instances: whole word spelling, t(44)5 2.36, po.05;combined target letter spelling, t(44)5 2.04, po.05; and the composite spelling/orthographic choice measure, t(44)5 2.06, po.05. There were non-significant differ-ences on the first letter, t(44)o1.0, ns; second letter, t(44)5 1.68, p5 .079; and inorthographic choice, t(44)o1.0, ns. By and large, these findings are consistent with ourhypothesis that spelling (reading/spelling) would lead to greater orthographic learning

    than reading alone.

    Does spelling provide unique benefits for non-initial letters?

    We predicted that because spelling obliges the reader/writer to process the letter string

    exhaustively, with no possibility of earlier closure as in reading, spelling production

    32 SHAHAR-YAMES and SHARE

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • might be expected to show unique benefits for orthographic information appearing later

    in the printed word relative to initial letters. Thus, we hypothesised that later-appearing

    letters would enjoy an added advantage compared with word-initial target letters when

    spelled. Consistent with this expectation, in the reading condition, spelling accuracy for

    initial and non-initial target letters considered individually (55.0% and 54.4%) was very

    similar and, as already noted, neither was significantly above either chance or control

    levels. In spelling, the second letter was actually superior (numerically) to the initial letter

    although not significantly so. The difference between reading and spelling in the case of

    the second letter was almost twice the difference in the case of the first letter (9.5% versus

    5.0%). This interaction between target letter position and experimental condition was

    submitted to a repeated measures analysis of variance, but failed to attain significance,

    F(1, 44)o1.0, ns. It should be noted, however, that the present data do not provide thestrongest test of this hypothesis because, in a large number of target words, most non-

    initial letters were not word-final. We return to this outcome in the discussion.

    Does spelling provide added benefits for spelling production compared with spelling

    recognition?

    Owing to the fact that the act of writing requires memory retrieval as well as motor-

    kinesthetic processing, we predicted an advantage for the reading/spelling condition in

    spelling production relative to spelling recognition. Summing across the two target letters

    and across the entire string (whole-word spelling accuracy), this expectation was

    confirmed. Furthermore, in both cases, the spelling contribution was substantial the

    spelling contribution was substantially stronger than the learning effect observed for

    reading. Also, consistent with our prediction, spelling recognition (orthographic choice)

    revealed only a small non-significant advantage for the spelling condition.

    These data were submitted to a multivariate analysis of variance. Although these

    interactions did not reach significance in the initial analyses for either the whole word,

    F(1, 88)5 1.28, ns, or the target letter measures, F(1, 88)5 1.49, ns, after we partialledout condition duration (i.e. the non-equivalent times recorded for the two conditions), the

    interaction for the whole-word spelling measure attained significance, F(2, 86)5 5.30,po.05, and fell marginally short of significance, F(2, 87)5 2.95, p5 .058, (two-tailed),in the case of the combined target letters. These outcomes suggest, as anticipated, that the

    unique processing demands of spelling (retrieval and motor-kinesthetic processing) create

    added benefits for measures of spelling production compared with spelling recognition.

    The influence of exposure/learning time on orthographic learning

    As discussed above, the act of spelling, in many ways, appears to be more demanding

    than reading requiring several component activities that are not involved in reading

    including picking up the pencil and gaining the right grip, getting oriented on the paper,

    retrieving phonemes, selecting the orthographically correct graphemes and forming the

    letters one after another while maintaining good spacing and horizontal alignment.

    Among young children for whom writing is not yet as automatic as it is among skilled

    writers, these are non-trivial demands that take up more time than reading alone even

    letter-by-letter sounding out. The non-trivial demands of this process were borne out by

    the fact that three children actually dropped their pencils onto the floor during spelling. It

    came as no surprise that even after stretching reading condition times by adding a

    comprehension question focused on the target word, the spelling condition still took

    SPELLING AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEARNING 33

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • significantly more time (27.5 versus 23.4 seconds), t(44)5 6.04, po.05. Our concern wasthat the longer time-on-task in the spelling condition may explain away the differences

    between the two experimental conditions. Analysis of covariance, however, showed that

    the same pattern of findings emerged after partialling out the time variable. The

    significant advantage in the spelling condition for whole word spelling and target letter

    spelling remained significant, F(44)5 9.61, po.005, and F(44)5 5.66, po.05,respectively, while the non-significant differences for the first letter, for the second

    target letter and for orthographic choice, all Fs(44)o1.0, remained non-significant.In short, the stronger orthographic learning outcomes in spelling compared with

    reading do not appear to be attributable to overall differences in processing times.

    Discussion

    The self-teaching hypothesis was founded on the idea that the process of recoding a

    printed word to sound obliges the reader to attend to orthographic detail and to word-

    specific print-to-sound mapping in a way that facilitates the establishment of the well-

    specified and well-unitised representations that are crucial for rapid automatised word

    recognition. The present study was motivated by similar considerations regarding the

    process of spelling, namely, that the process of producing a spelling might also serve a

    self-teaching function because it too requires the writer to attend to orthographic detail

    (letter identity and order) and sub-lexical print-to-sound relationships in a systematic

    manner. Our findings confirmed these expectations significant orthographic learning

    was obtained in both spelling and reading.

    However, we predicted not only significant orthographic learning for spelling but also

    that spelling would actually produce superior learning outcomes compared with reading

    owing to the additional demands placed on the writer specifically the more taxing

    retrieval and motor-kinesthetic processes involved in spelling. In broad-brush terms, our

    results were consistent with this prediction performance levels for spelling at the post-

    test exceeded levels for spelling in all six measures and, in three cases, these differences

    attained significance (composite recognition/production measure, whole word spelling

    and combined target letter spelling). As already noted, it was only on measures requiring

    spelling production that these differences were statistically reliable exactly as predicted

    by our hypothesis that spelling would be especially beneficial for production as compared

    with recognition. Once differences in overall processing time were partialled out, the

    interaction between spelling production and spelling recognition (orthographic choice)

    was significant in the case of whole word spelling and on the border of significance (using

    the more conservative two-tailed test) when target letter spelling was considered. This

    result is unsurprising because in the spelling condition at the learning phase and post-test,

    the same method (activity) was used.

    We also hypothesised that spelling would lead to extra advantages for orthographic

    detail appearing later in a spelling. We reasoned that although the initial encounter

    decoding a novel word (especially in oral reading) is likely to invoke an exhaustive letter-

    to-sound translation procedure, it seems reasonable to assume that on subsequent

    occasions this process may be partly curtailed, particularly if the target is embedded in

    meaningful text. Spelling, in contrast, does not admit of any abbreviated processing.

    Every production obliges the writer to process each and every letter of the string, giving

    full and equal consideration to each letter.

    34 SHAHAR-YAMES and SHARE

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • Consistent with these expectations, the advantage for spelling compared with reading

    on the second target letter (9.5%) was almost twice the advantage for the first letter

    (5.0%). This interaction, however, did not attain statistical significance. Nonetheless, we

    think it premature to reject the hypothesis of position-sensitive orthographic learning for

    two reasons. First, the position of our first and second letters did not maximise the

    distance between the two letters. Although 10 of the 12 first letters were indeed initial

    letters, a majority of post-initial letters were not word-final letters. In fact, in only four

    instances was the non-initial letter the word-final letter. In three cases, the post-initial

    letter was actually in the first half of the word. In the remaining cases, the target letter was

    either second last or third last. The second factor, which we think makes it premature to

    dismiss the position-sensitive hypothesis, is that our test words were all rather short

    averaging only 4.7 (consonantal) letters. A systematic comparison between word-initial

    and word-final graphemes in words of varying length would provide a more incisive test

    of this hypothesis. More generally, however, there may be important differences between

    orthographies in orthographic redundancy. In consonantal alphabets (or abjads) such as

    Hebrew, there is relatively little redundancy (Share & Levin, 1999), necessitating a

    relatively comprehensive processing of the letter string. In plene alphabets such as

    English, which consist of fully-fledged letters representing both consonants and vowels,

    there exists a much larger degree of redundancy (see Adams, 1990), which affords greater

    leeway for foreshortening the decoding process, thereby creating the possibility of

    stronger position-sensitive learning effects for reading compared with spelling.

    The precise nature of the processes critical for the establishment of orthographic

    representations remains to be investigated. There is clearly much in common between

    reading and spelling (Ehri, 1980; Gough, Juel & Griffith, 1992; Holmes & Carruthers,

    1998; Perfetti, 1997) but there are also unique processes. Word identification via both

    decoding and spelling obliges the reader to examine each of (nearly all) the letters in a

    letter string. Selective non-exhaustive processing of letter information may well enable

    the novice reader (or even pre-reader, see Treiman & Kessler, 2003) to make initial

    partial or skeletal representations between print and speech (see, e.g., Ehri, 1995; Rack,

    Hulme, Snowling & Wightman, 1994; Share, 1995; Stuart & Coltheart, 1988) but will

    ultimately fail the reader in the long run owing to the fact that many words, particularly

    shorter monosyllables, are often distinguished only by a single letter (house/horse) or

    even only by letter ordering (salt/slat). However, mere visual inspection of the items in a

    letter array seems unlikely to explain orthographic learning. In an experiment in which

    children were shown non-alphabetic (hence non-recodable) strings of common keyboard

    characters (e.g. #@*?) and required to attend to each of the constituent elements as well

    as to the string as a whole (by means of a dual-task procedure requiring both symbol

    search (YES/NO decision) and string length report (how many characters appear in this

    string?), Share (1999, experiment 4) found extremely low levels of visual/orthographic

    learning compared with orthographic learning of alphabetic (i.e. recodable) words of

    similar length. It appears that something about the process of print-to-sound translation

    may be a critical ingredient in the decoding process (Share, 1999, experiments 2 and 3;

    see also Kyte & Johnson, 2006). Both these studies found that viewing a novel letter

    string while articulating an unrelated syllable resulted in significantly poorer orthographic

    learning compared with viewing the same string while articulating the relevant (i.e.

    correct) pronunciation. Both decoding and spelling share print-to-sound conversion

    (although in opposite directions), which can be conceived as a process of seeing the

    logic of a words spelling and, in another sense too, coming to hear the pronunciation in a

    SPELLING AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEARNING 35

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • new way (Olson, 1994; Share, 1995). This, perhaps, is the fundamental difference

    between mere visual/logographic or pre-alphabetic reading and so-called orthographic

    recognition (Ehri, 1995; Frith, 1985).

    Although both spelling and decoding share a mode of processing especially conducive

    to orthographic learning, there are unique elements in spelling that led us to hypothesise

    additional benefits for spelling production. In the case of novel words, aspects of spelling

    production include identification of the successive phonemes in the spoken form,

    selection of the corresponding grapheme and the motor-kinesthetic component (as well as

    the visual-spatial component). The evidence reviewed in the introduction regarding the

    unique role for motor-kinesthetic information in both letter learning and spelling, at least

    among struggling readers (Fernald, 1943; Hulme, 1983; Hulme et al., 1987), may provide

    an additional layer or network of connections in the traditional orthography

    phonologymeaning triangle, thereby creating additional redundancies because the

    motor-kinesthetic activity constitutes another layer of mnemonic information concerning

    the orthographic form of a word.

    Pinpointing those elements in the multidimensional spelling process that contribute

    most to orthographic learning is the obvious next step in this line of research. Is the

    phoneme-by-phoneme segmentation of the spoken form the critical element in

    establishing new word-level spellingsound mappings, the comparatively onerous

    retrieval processes involved in selecting the appropriate grapheme corresponding to a

    given phoneme, or possibly the motor-kinesthetic dimension that has been emphasised in

    the remedial reading literature? Or is it a combination of some or all of these ingredients

    that holds the key? Future work would do well to tease out these various components.

    More generally, the present research confirms the important reciprocal ties between reading

    and spelling by pointing to yet another way in which spelling and reading collaborate.

    Notes

    1. The self-teaching hypothesis does not imply that the process of phonological recoding is necessarily a

    purely overt letter-by-letter sounding out and blending process. In orthographies with digraphs or even larger

    orthographic units (see Ziegler & Goswami, 2005), multi-letter sequences may be processed as integral units

    in the process of sequentially decoding larger sub-lexical supra-graphemic units. Furthermore, this assembly

    process may be either overt or covert (i.e. internalised).

    2. The only real-life situation which comes to mind is the case of oral spelling of proper names. In most

    everyday literacy contexts, especially in school, the learner would normally be writing/spelling words he or

    she has previously read.

    3. In the case of the glottal stop, however, in addition to the two standard letters (ALEF and AYIN), it has

    become increasingly common for Hebrew speakers to write a third letter the letter HEY, which in certain

    contexts (mostly word-final) has the same phonemic value. Because there were only nine instances of the

    HEY in the entire sample representing fewer than 2% of the ALEF/AYIN/HEY productions, it was decided

    to leave the designated chance levels unchanged (i.e. 25% and 50%).

    4. Although an unconventional way of examining statistical reliability, it is worth noting that six consecutive

    differences favouring spelling is well beyond chance according to the binomial distribution (265 .016).

    References

    Abbot, R.D. & Berninger, V.W. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental

    skills and writing skills in primary- and intermediate-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85,

    478508.

    36 SHAHAR-YAMES and SHARE

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read. Cambridge, MA: Bradford.

    Berninger, V.W., Abbot, R.D., Abbot, S.P., Graham, S. & Richards, T. (2002). Writing and reading:

    Connections between language by hand and language by eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35,

    3956.

    Bosman, A.M. & Van Orden, G.C. (1997). Why spelling is more difficult than reading. In C.A. Perfetti, L.

    Rieben & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: research, theory and practice. (pp. 173194). Mahwah, NJ:

    Erlbaum.

    Bowey, J.A. & Miller, R. (2007). Correlates of orthographic learning in third-grade childrens silent reading.

    Journal of Research in Reading, 30, 115128.

    Bowey, J.A. & Muller, D. (2005). Phonological recoding and rapid orthographic learning in third-graders silent

    reading: A critical test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92,

    203219.

    Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R. & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual-route cascaded model of

    visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204256.

    Cunningham, A.E. (2006). Accounting for childrens orthographic learning while reading: Do children self-

    teach? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 5677.

    Cunningham, A.E., Perry, K.E., Stanovich, K.E. & Share, D.L. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading:

    Examining the role of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 185199.

    Cunningham, A.E. & Stanovich, K.E. (1990). Early spelling acquisition: Writing beats the computer. Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 82, 159162.

    Daniels, P.T. & Bright, W. (1996). The worlds writing systems. New York: Oxford University Press.

    De Jong, P.F. & Share, D.L. (2007). Orthographic learning during oral and silent reading. Scientific Studies of

    Reading, 11, 5571.

    Ehri, L. (1980). The development of orthographic images. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling.

    (pp. 311338). London: Academic Press.

    Ehri, L. (2000). Learning to read and learning to spell: Two sides of a coin. Topics in Language Disorders, 20,

    1936.

    Ehri, L. & Wilce, L.S. (1987). Does learning to spell help beginners to read words? Reading Research

    Quarterly, 22, 4765.

    Ehri, L.C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18,

    116125.

    Ellis, N.C. & Cataldo, S. (1990). The role of spelling in learning to read. Language and Education, 4, 128.

    Fernald, G.M. (1943). Remedial techniques in basic school subject. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, M. Coltheart & J. Marshall

    (Eds.), Surface dyslexia. (pp. 301330). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Geva, E. & Siegel, L.S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic

    reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 130.

    Gillingham, A.M. & Stillman, B.U. (1956). Remedial training for children with specific disability in reading,

    spelling and penmanship. New York: Sackett & Wilhelms.

    Graham, S. & Weintraub, N. (1996). A review of handwriting research: Progress and prospects from 1980 to

    1994. Educational Psychology Review, 8, 787.

    Gough, P.B., Juel, C. & Griffith, P.L. (1992). Reading, spelling, and the orthographic cipher. In P.B. Gough,

    L.C. Ehri & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition. (pp. 3548). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Associates.

    Holmes, V.M. & Carruthers, J. (1998). The relation between reading and spelling in skilled adult readers.

    Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 264289.

    Hulme, C. (1983). Multi-sensory teaching and specific reading retardation. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Hulme, C. & Bradley, L. (1984). An experimental study of multi-sensory teaching with normal and retarded

    readers. In R. Maltesha & H. Whitaker (Eds.), Dyslexia: A global issue. (pp. 431443). The Hague: Martinus

    Nijhoff.

    Hulme, C., Monk, A. & Ives, S. (1987). Some experimental studies of multi-sensory teaching: The effects of

    the manual tracing on childrens paired-associate learning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5,

    299307.

    Jorm, A.F. & Share, D.L. (1983). Phonological recoding and reading acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4,

    103147.

    Kyte, C.S. & Johnson, C.J. (2006). The role of phonological recoding in orthographic learning. Journal of

    Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 166185.

    SPELLING AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEARNING 37

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • Landi, N., Perfetti, C.A., Bolger, D., Dunlap, S. & Foorman, B. (2006). The role of discourse context in

    developing word form representations: A paradoxical relation between reading and learning. Journal of

    Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 114133.

    Mann, V.A., Tobin, P. & Wilson, R. (1987). Measuring phonological awareness through the invented spellings

    of kindergarten children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 365391.

    Martin-Chang, S.L., Levy, B.A. & ONeill, S. (2007). Word acquisition, retention, and transfer: Findings from

    contextual and isolated word training. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 3756.

    Montessori, M. (1915). The Montessori method. London: Heinemann.

    Morris, D. & Perney, J. (1984). Developmental spelling as a predictor of first-grade reading achievement. The

    Elementary School Journal, 84, 441457.

    Nation, K., Angell, P. & Castles, A. (2007). Orthographic learning via self-teaching in children learning to read

    English: Effects of exposure, durability and context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 7184.

    Olson, D.R. (1994). The world on paper. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Orton, S.T. (1928). Specific reading disability Strephosymbolia. Journal of the American Medical Association,

    90, 10951099.

    Perfetti, C.A. (1997). The psycholinguistics of spelling and reading. In C.A. Perfetti, L. Rieben & M. Fayol

    (Eds.), Learning to spell: research, theory and practice. (pp. 2138). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Plaut, D.C., McClelland, J.L, Seidenberg, M.S. & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and im-

    paired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103,

    56115.

    Rack, J.P., Hulme, C., Snowling, M.J. & Wightman, J. (1994). The role of phonology in young childrens

    learning of sight words: The direct-mapping hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57,

    4271.

    Ravid, D. (2002). Tfisat haktiv haivri etsel heyeled beyisrael [The perception of Hebrew spelling by the child in

    Israel: A developmental view]. [In Hebrew]. Megamot, 2957.

    Shanahan, T. (1984). Nature of the readingwriting relationship: An exploratory multivariate analysis. Journal

    of Educational Psychology, 76, 466477.

    Shany, M., Lachman, D., Shalem, Z., Bahat, A. & Zeiger, T. (2006). Alef-Taf. Evaluation system for the

    diagnosis of disability in the reading and writing process according to national norms. Holon: Yesod

    Publishing.

    Share, D.L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55,

    151218.

    Share, D.L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching

    hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 95129.

    Share, D.L. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self-

    teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 267298.

    Share, D.L., Jorm, A.F., Maclean, F. & Matthews, R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in reading

    acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 13091324.

    Share, D.L. & Levin, I. (1999). Learning to read and write in Hebrew. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.),

    Learning to read and write. (pp. 89111). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Share, D.L. & Shalev, C. (2004). Self-teaching and disabled readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary

    Journal, 16, 131.

    Shatil, E., Share, D.L. & Levin, I. (2000). On the contribution of Kindergarten writing to Grade 1 literacy:

    A longitudinal study in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 125.

    Siegel, L.S., Share, D.L. & Geva, E. (1995). Dyslexics have orthographic skills that are superior to normal

    readers. Psychological Science, 6, 250254.

    Stuart, M. & Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages? Cognition, 30, 139181.

    Treiman, R. & Kessler, B. (2003). The role of letter names in the acquisition of literacy. In R. Kail (Ed.),

    Advances in child development and behavior. (Vol. 31, pp. 105135). San Diego, CA: Academic

    Press.

    Tunmer, W.E. & Chapman, J.W. (1998). Language prediction skill, phonological recoding ability,

    and beginning reading. In C. Hulme & R. Joshi (Eds.), Reading and writing: Development and disorders.

    (pp. 3367). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Uhry, J.K. & Shepherd, M.J. (1993). Segmentation/spelling instruction as part of a first-grade reading program:

    Effects on several measures of reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 219233.

    Ziegler, J.C. & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia and skilled reading across

    languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 329.

    38 SHAHAR-YAMES and SHARE

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008

  • Appendix. Target words (in alternative

    homophonic spelling) appearing

    in test sentences

    Received 8 May 2007; revised version received 19 August 2007.

    Address for correspondence: David Share, Department of Learning Disabilities,University of Haifa Mount Carmel, 31905, Haifa, Israel.

    E-mail: [email protected]

    The symbol x represents the laryngeal fricative heard at the end ofthe word Bach.

    SPELLING AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEARNING 39

    r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2008