43
Basic Assumption: Groups should be more likely to help in emergency situations (safety in numbers) What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most emergencies are, or at least begin) and others do not respond as if the situation is an emergency? Presence of others as a source of information (social cues) But, what if you expect others to help out, so there is no need for you to do anything?

What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

  • Upload
    cedric

  • View
    27

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Basic Assumption : Groups should be more likely to help in emergency situations (safety in numbers). But, what if you expect others to help out, so there is no need for you to do anything?. What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Citation preview

Page 1: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Basic Assumption: Groups should be more likely to help in emergency situations (safety in numbers)

What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most emergencies are, or at least begin) and others do not respond as if the situation is an emergency?

Presence of others as a source of information (social cues)

But, what if you expect others to help out, so there is no need for you to do anything?

Page 2: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

(From New York Times, March 27th, 1964)38 Who Saw Murder Didn’t Call the PoliceApathy at Stabbing of Queens Woman ShocksInspector By Martin Gansberg For more than half an hour 38 respectable, law-abiding citizens in Queens watched a killer stalk and stab a woman in three separate attacks in Kew Gardens.Twice the sound of their voices and the sudden glow of their bedroom lights interrupted him and frightened him off, Each time he returned, sought her out and stabbed her again. Not one person telephoned the police during the assault; one witness called after the woman was dead.That was two weeks ago today. But Assistant Chief Inspector Frederick M. Lussen, in charge of the bor ough’s detectives and a veteran of 25 years of homicide investigations, is still shocked.He can give a matter-of-fact recitation of many murders. But the Kew Gardens slaying baffles him — not because it is a murder, but because the ‘good people’ failed to call the police.‘As we have reconstructed the crime,’ he said, ‘the assailant had three chances to kill this woman during a 35-minute period. He returned twice to complete the job. If we had been called when he first attacked, the woman might not be dead now.’   

Kitty Genovese Story

Page 3: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

‘He Stabbed Me!’ She got as far as a street light in front of a bookstore before the man grabbed her. She screamed. Lights went on in the 10-storey apartment house at 82—67 Austin Street, which faces the bookstore. Windows slid open and voices punctured the early-morning stillness.Miss Genovese screamed: ‘Oh, my God, he stabbed me! Please help me! Please help me!’From one of the upper windows in the apartment house, a man called down: ‘Let that girl alone!’The assailant looked up at him, shrugged and walked down Austin Street toward a white sedan parked a short distance away. Miss Genovese struggled to her feet.Lights went out. The killer returned to Miss Genovese, now trying to make her way around the side of the building by the parking lot to get to her apart ment. The assailant grabbed her again.‘I’m dying!’ she shrieked.

Page 4: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

A City Bus Passed Windows were opened again, and lights went on in many apartments. The assailant got into his car and drove away. Miss Genovese staggered to her feet. A city bus, Q-10, the Lefferts Boulevard line to Kennedy International Airport, passed. It was 3.35 am.

The assailant returned. By then, Miss Genovese had crawled to the back of the building where the freshly painted brown doors to the apartment house held out hope of safety. The killer tried the first door; she wasn’t there. At the second door, 82—62 Austin Street, he saw her slumped on the floor at the foot of the stairs. He stabbed her a third time — fatally.

It was 3.50 by the time the police received their first call, from a man who was a neighbor of Miss Genovese. In two minutes they were at the scene. The neighbor, a 70-year-old woman and another woman were the only persons on the street. Nobody else came forward.

The man explained that he had called the police after much deliberation. He had phoned a friend in Nassau County for advice and then he had crossed the roof of the elderly woman to get her to make the call. ‘I didn’t want to get involved,’ he sheepishly told the police.

Page 5: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Suspect is Arrested Six days later, the police arrested Winston Moseley, a 29-year-old business-machine operator, and charged him with the homicide. Mosely had no previous record. He is married, has two children and owns a home at 133—19 Sutter Avenue, South Ozone Park, Queens. On Wednesday, a court committed him to Kings County Hospital for psychiatric observation.The police stressed how simple it would have been to get in touch with them. ‘A phone call,’ said one of the detectives, ‘would have done it.’Today witnesses from the neighborhood, which is made up of one-family homes in the $35,000 to $60,000 range with the exception of the two apartment houses near the railroad station, find it difficult to explain why they didn’t call the police.Lieut. Bernard Jacobs, who handled the investiga tion by the detectives, said:‘It is one of the better neighborhoods. There are few reports of crimes. You only get the usual complaints about boys playing or garbage cans being turned over.’The police said most persons had told them they had been afraid to call, but had given meaningless answers when asked what they had feared.‘We can understand the reticence of people to become involved in an area of violence,’ Lieutenant Jacobs said, ‘but where they are in their homes, near phones, why should they be afraid to call the police?’

Page 6: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

He said that his men were able to piece together what happened — and capture the suspect — because the residents furnished all the information when detectives rang doorbells during the days following the slaying.‘But why didn’t someone call us that night?’ he asked unbelievingly.Witnesses — some of them unable to believe what they had allowed to happen — told a reporter why.

A housewife, knowingly if quite casual, said, ‘We thought it was a lovers’ quarrel’. A husband and wife both said, ‘Frankly, we were afraid’. They seemed aware of the fact that events might have been different. A distraught woman, wiping her hands in her apron, said, ‘I didn’t want my husband to get involved’.

One couple, now willing to talk about that night, said they heard the first screams. The husband looked thoughtfully at the bookstore where the killer first grabbed Miss Genovese. ‘We went to the window to see what was happening,’ he said, ‘but the light from our bedroom made it difficult to see the street’. The wife, still apprehensive, added: ‘I put out the light and we were able to see better’. Asked why they hadn’t called the police, she shrugged and replied, ‘I don’t know’.

Page 7: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most
Page 8: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Types of Helping

1) Long vs. short-term helping2) Direct vs. indirect (costs)3) Behavioral, emotional, or informational

Examples:

A) Short-term, direct, behavioral, danger present

• Prevent other’s drowning

• Stopping a shoplifter

B) Short-term, indirect, behavioral

• Call 911

C) Direct response without danger• Giving up seats on a bus• Picking up dropped goods

Page 9: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Examples cont…D) Response to a direct request

• Give some spare change• Let someone use your phone• Give directions

E) Returning lost articles• Letters• Wallet• Money

F) Long-term helping• Listen to a friend in need• Letting an elderly parent live with you• Reading to a child• Working on a help/hot line• Care for someone with a terminal disease• Donations (e.g., money, clothes, food, blood, organs, time)

Page 10: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Latane and Darley’s Model of Emergency Intervention (1970)What are the cognitive steps an individual must progress through before

offering help in and emergency?

1. Notice the emergency

2. Define as emergency?

HELP

3. Take responsibility?

4. Have the knowledge, ability to help?

5. Decide to help?

Don’t Help

Yes

No

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

No

No

No

Page 11: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

100

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

1 2 3 4 5 6Time from Start of Smoke Infusion (minutes)

Cu

mu

lative Prop

ortion

Rep

orting S

mok

e (%)

Alone

Three Naïve Subjects

Two Passive Subjects

“Pluralistic Ignorance” – Social Comparison

Page 12: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

The presence of others also affected how quickly participants noticed the smoke in the room!

What if a condition existed where a confederate signaled the need to help?

Alone = Less than 5 seconds (63% noticed within 5 sec.)

Group = About 20 seconds (26% noticed within 5 sec.)

Page 13: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Cumulative Percentages of Subjects Responding in Different Conditions to Smoke Pouring Into the Room

What effect does the presence of other people have on our response to a possible emergency???

In this study by Latane and Darley (1970) subjects sat in a room either alone with two other subjects, or with two passive

confederates. As they completed questionnaires, smoke began pouring into the room through an air vent. The

researchers measured how quickly subjects sought help or reported the emergency.

*** As the previous graph shows, single subjects were much more likely to seek help, and they responded to the

possible emergency more quickly.

Page 14: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Epileptic Seizure Information

Page 15: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

100

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

60 120 180 240

Cu

mu

lative Prop

ortion H

elpin

g (%

)

Time from Beginning of Fit (Seconds)

Subject & Victim

Subject, Victim & Stranger

Subject, Victim & 4 Strangers

“Diffusion of Responsibility”(Others Can Help)

Page 16: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Cumulative Percentages of Subjects Responding to an Epileptic Fit Under Different Conditions

Does the bystander effect occur in an unambiguous emergency involving a suffering human victim?

Latane and Daley (1970) had subjects communicate via a microphone with another student in a nearby room. Subjects

believed there were no, one, or four other people listening in on the conversation. Partway through the experiment, the other

student seemed to experience an epileptic seizure. The researchers observed how quickly subjects helped the victim

***As the previous graph shows, subjects were more likely to help the victim of the seizure when they were the only person participating in the conversation. All subjects who believed that they were alone when they heard the seizure aided the victim within three minutes; however, not all subjects in the

other two situations aided the victims.

Page 17: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

• Kin Altruism (e.g., survival of the species)

More likely assist closely related genetic relatives (Bressan, 2009)

More willing to give costly help (e.g., emergencies) to healthy relatives than non-healthy ones (Burnstein et al., 1994)

• Direct Reciprocity/Reciprocal Altruism [Likely to get help in return for helping] Tit-for-Tat Strategy (e.g., respond based on the actions of others – most effective strategy across time): Cooperation = Cooperation; Competition = Competition

• Modeling behavior of others [Car on roadside study]

• Ability/Expertise

• Genetics -- A range of genetic factors can account for 40% - 60% of prosocial behavior (e.g., Gregory, Light-Hauserman, Rijsdijk, & Eley, 2009)

• Moods [[NEGATIVE‑STATE RELIEF MODELNEGATIVE‑STATE RELIEF MODEL]] • Broken camera study, Confession study

Some Factors Related to Helping Behavior

Page 18: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

No Fragrance

Fragrance

Males 22.2 45.5

Females 16.7 60.9

* From Baron, R. A. (1997). The sweet smell of … helping: Effects of pleasant ambient fragrance on prosocial behavior in shopping malls. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 498-503.

The findings were mediated by positive affect

Positive Affect, Smells and Helping

Page 19: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Male Female

100

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

0

90

30

70

35

Homosexual making request

Heterosexual making request

Wrong phone number study

From Shaw, Borough, & Fink, 1994

Role of similarity?

Participants received an apparently wrong number telephone call from a male caller who portrayed himself as either a heterosexual or a homosexual. The caller said his car broke down and that he was at a pay phone and out of change. He asked the participant to call his girlfriend/boyfriend for him.

Page 20: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Number of warnings [Gender bias in helping behavior?] Similarity?

Type of crimes: Speeding, DUI, others

Driver gender Officer Gender

Male Female

Male

Female

Page 21: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

In a local grocery store you notice a small child in a shopping cart. A woman, likely the mother, slaps him in

the face and yells for the child to shut up or be hit again. You fell bad for the child but you wonder if you’d make things worse if you say something. What would

you do?

Page 22: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Piliavin and Piliavin’s Cost Analysis of Emergency Intervention

How do perceived costs for helping and not helping affect our willingness to intervene in an emergency?

Piliavin and Piliavin (1972) proposed that a moderately aroused bystander to an emergency assesses the costs of helping and not helping before deciding whether to intervene. The table below predicts what a bystander is most likely to do in an emergency when the costs for helping are low or high and the costs for not helping are low or high.

Costs (to helper) for Directly Helping Victim

Costs (to victim

) if No

Direct H

elp G

iven

High

High

Low

Low

LowLow HighHigh

Direct Intervention

Intervention or Nonintervention largely a function of perceived norms in situation

Indirect intervention or

Redefinition of the situation, disparagement of victim, etc., which lowers costs for no help, allowing

Leaving the scene, ignoring, denial

Page 23: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Blood on Victim

No Blood on Victim

Perceived Costs & Helping

Strangers Arguing

Couples Arguing

Greater levels of helping

Page 24: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

You are driving along a city street and you notice that a car has slid across the shoulder and into a ditch. A middle-aged woman is standing next to the car,

bewildered. You are running a a few minutes late to meet to meet someone in town, but it’s obvious that the

woman needs help. What would you do?

Page 25: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

% helping70

60

50

40

30

20

10

0

Ahead of schedule

On schedule Behind schedule

Time Pressure and Helping

Page 26: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Tim

e Elap

sed B

efore Interven

tion

(Secon

ds)

260

240

220

200

180

160

140

120

100

100

80

60

40

Non-Arousal Placebo Arousal Placebo

Unambiguous Emergency

(with screams)

Ambiguous emergency (without screams)

Page 27: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Misattribution of arousal and speed of helping in ambiguous or unambiguous emergencies

How do attributions of arousal affect our behavior in emergency situations?

In Gaertner and Dovidio’s (1997) experiement, subjects sat in a room and heard what sounded like a stack of chairs falling on a woman in the next room. Sometimes the emergency was unambiguous (the woman screamed), and sometimes it was ambiguous (no scream). Subjects had earlier taken a pill (a placebo); some were told that it might increase their heart rate, and some were not. Gaertner and Dovidio measured how quickly subjects responded to the possible emergency in the next room.

*** As the previous graph shows, subjects responded more slowly to the ambiguous emergency. Furthermore, subjects exposed to the ambiguous emergency responded even less quickly when they could misattribute their arousal to the pill.

Page 28: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Helping request (e.g., stranger

asking for spare change)

Physiological arousal

External attribution (e.g., poor economy

is at fault)

Analysis of the situation

Internal attribution (e.g., stranger is

lazy)

Positive emotions

Helping

Negative emotions

No helping

Attributions & Helping

What are the “big picture” implications of such a finding, especially for social programs

(e.g., unemployment insurance, health care, food stamps)?

Page 29: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Who helps more, men or women?

• Overall, males provide more help than females (e.g., emergency interventions, giving $ to strangers, helping individuals who have dropped an item)

BUT:

• Females provide more emotional help than males (e.g., social support, caregiving)

• Females:

• 13% more likely to donate kidneys

• 16% more likely to serve in Peace Corps

• 2 times greater chance of serving as physicians with Doctors of the World

Who Receives Help?

• Females (meta-analysis: 1.69 standard deviations more help for females) • Similar others• Attractive individuals

Page 30: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

You a watching the TV news about a large-scale national disaster across the world. Men, women and children are shown blankly starring at the TV screen.

It’s easy to contribute; you can donate money by calling a number on your cell phone. How would this make you

feel? What would you do?

Page 31: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Ask for directions

Give help

Thanked for helping

“Punished” for helping (“I cannot understand what you’re saying.

Never mind, I’ll ask someone else”

Less likely to provide

assistance in future

Impact of Past Experience on Helping

Page 32: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

The United Nations Special Envoy to Haiti has reported that only 40 percent of money

raised for Haiti in 2010 has been distributed, and only 15 percent of needed

temporary housing has been built.

From: The Oakland Press, Wednesday, January 26, 2011

Page 33: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Rank Charity Expenses Fundraising 1 Disabled Veterans Associations 4.6% 94.3%2 Children's Charitable Foundation 10.3% 87.3%3 Firefighters Charitable Foundation 8.3% 86.4%4 Disabled Police Officers Center 11.8% 85.7%5 Law Enforcement Education Prog. 2.2% 84.1%6 Operation Lookout 2.6% 80.8%7 Wishing Well Foundation USA 10.3% 78.3%8 Children's Charity Fund, Inc. 5.7% 78.1%9 Coalition Against Breast Cancer 18.3% 78.1%10 Children With Hairloss 24.5% 72.3%

Page 34: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

60

50

40

30

20

10

“High” Energetic

Warm Calm Self-worth

Less aches and

pains

Source: Luks, 1988.

Personal benefits for helping others

“He who helps others helps himself”

Page 35: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Country # Helpful Acts

Philippines 280

Kenya 156

Mexico 148

Japan 97

U.S. 86

India 60

Culture and Helping

*Source: Whiting & Whiting, 1975

Page 36: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Does Altruism Exist?

Egoism: Behaving in one’s own self interest

Altruism: Unselfish concern for others

• Helping is a behavior

• Egoism and altruism are motivational forces

How can we know?

One assumption:

“Kindness is it’s own reward.”

If altruism exists, the level of costs involved should not impact the

behavior of those helping out

Page 37: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Easy Escape Difficult Escape

Low Empathy

High Empathy% Helping

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

[Can leave study] [Have to stay and watch]

(Dissimilar Victim)

(Similar Victim)

No difference in helping rates between these 2 conditions

Page 38: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Personality Characteristics of People Choosing to Help

[Assistance to driver after a traffic accident]

• High empathy scores

• Strong belief in a just world (“people get what they deserve”)

(Other research: greater helping when people believe others deserve help but less helping when other’s problems are perceived as self-inflicted)

• Greater levels of social responsibility

• Internal locus of control scores

• Less egocentric (selfish)

Page 39: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

• I have given directions to a stranger.

• I have given money to a charity.

• I have donated blood.

• I have delayed an elevator and held the door open for a stranger.

• I have allowed someone to go ahead of me in a lineup (at Xerox machine, in the supermarket).

• l have pointed out a clerk's error (in a bank, at the supermarket) in undercharging me for an item.

• I have helped a classmate who 1 did not know that well with a homework assignment when my knowledge was greater than his or hers.

• I have voluntarily looked after a neighbor's pets or children without being paid for it.

• I have helped an acquaintance to move households.

--- From Rushton, Chrisjohn, & Cynthiafekken

Sample Altruism Items

Page 40: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Do you believe that most problems will solve themselves if you just don't fool with them?

Do you feel that you have a lot of choice in deciding who your friends are?

Most of the time, do you feel that you can change what might happen tomorrow by what you do today?

Do you think that people can get their own way if they just keep trying?

Are some people just born lucky?

Do you believe that if somebody studies hard enough he or she can pass any subject?

Are you often blamed for things that just aren't your fault?

Sample Locus of Control Items

Page 41: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

• I am confident that justice always prevails over injustice.

• I think basically the world is a just place

• I am convinced that, in the long run, people will be compensated for injustices.

• I firmly believe that injustices in all areas of life (e.g. professional, family, politics) are the exception rather than the rule.

• I believe that, by and large, people get what they deserve

• I think that people try to be fair when making important decisions.

Sample Just World items

From: Dalbert, Montada, & Schmitt, 1987)

Page 42: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Helping Models

Moral Individuals are responsible Individuals are responsible

Medical Individuals not responsible Experts responsible

Compensatory Individuals not responsible Individuals are responsible

Enlightenment Individuals are socialized to Higher Power be responsible

Model Problems Solutions

Page 43: What if the situation is relatively ambiguous (as most

Some Issues With Receiving Help

• Recipient feeling indebted to the giver (e.g., reciprocity)

• Perception of being controlled by the giver

• Feeling of inadequacy (lowered self-esteem)

“There are different ways of assassinating a man --- by sword, poison, or moral assassination. They are the same in their results except that the last is the more cruel.” Napolean I, Maxims (1804-1815)