7
7/11/17 1 Race Ma*ers: Hearing Difference Session 2 2017 LSA Ins;tute Elaine Chun, University of South Carolina <[email protected]> Announcements/Reminders Submit 4 readings responses over the course of the month (See the first two excellent examples) You would have submi*ed yesterday on Baugh (2003) You can submit a response to Philips (1972) by Thursday, 12 pm Begin to think about Assignment 2: Data you might collect and preliminarily analyze Due July 21 (10 days from now) What counts? Spoken or wri*en discourses about racial issues, Metalinguis;c discourses about racialized language (e.g., racializing labels or descriptors); Spoken or wri*en discourses involving the indexing of racial posi4ons or iden44es (e.g., codeswitching, styleshi\ing). Where can I find my data? Recordings of people around you, online corpora (create your own), surveys, interviews, etc. What methods can I use? Any (ethnographic observa;ons, interac;onal analysis, varia;onist analysis, and other forms of discourse analysis) What if I’m at a loss? Talk to or email me! Race Ma*ers Race may be socially constructed, but it is deeply embedded in our material experiences Who we befriend, date, or marry Who receives housing and employment Who we define as intelligent, authorita;ve, or a*rac;ve So what can a linguist do? Understand how language keeps race relevant and how it keeps racial hierarchies in place Present solu;ons to racebased linguis;c discrimina;on What are we doing this week? Examining two ways in which race can come to ma*er because of (perceived) differences of language: Our (perceived) dis;nc;ve pa*erns of pronuncia;on (today) and our dis;nc;ve pa*erns of communica;on (Friday) Hearing race Examine how our prac;ces of racialized hearing can maintain racial hierarchies Answer some ques;ons: What is an accent? Can we tell a person’s race based on their accent? (Baugh 2003) Do our beliefs about the race of a speaker (based on what we see) shape what accent we hear? (Rubin 1992) What kinds of images and ideologies are we accessing when we hear a speaker who sounds “nonna;ve”? (Lindemann 2003) (When) is it wrong to hear an accent as “ethnic” or “racial”? What are the benefits and drawbacks of the (modified) matchguise technique used in these perceptual studies? Everyone has an accent” What do linguists mean by this? Everyone has a pa*erned way of pronouncing words (phonological habits) despite the percep;on of only some people as having accents Prosodic features (stress pa*ern: intona;on, rhythm, volume) Segmental features (consonants and vowels) Syllabic features: /ɛbʊriwən hɛbʊ ən aksentʊ/ Why do linguists assert this? This rela;vist statement decenters the linguis;c norma;vity of how we tend to hear accents; language “difference” is always relaIve to some (usually norma;ve) perspec;ve When do we hear an accent? Ideologies of that locate speakers as “other”(and some;mes “inferior”) rela;ve to some norm: ”I don’t have an accent but you do” or “I have a fobby accent” Will the real Barneys ad please speak up? Because you love Chinese accents From a collection of hand-painted ceramic ware from northern China, $1 to $5, ours alone.

W3-RaceMatters · 7/11/17 $hearing?$ university$ • $

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: W3-RaceMatters · 7/11/17 $hearing?$ university$ • $

7/11/17  

1  

Race  Ma*ers:  Hearing  Difference    

Session  2  2017  LSA  Ins;tute  

Elaine  Chun,  University  of  South  Carolina  <[email protected]>  

Announcements/Reminders  •  Submit  4  readings  responses  over  the  course  of  the  month  (See  the  first  

two  excellent  examples)  –  You  would  have  submi*ed  yesterday  on  Baugh  (2003)  –  You  can  submit  a  response  to  Philips  (1972)  by  Thursday,  12  pm  

•  Begin  to  think  about  Assignment  2:  Data  you  might  collect  and  preliminarily  analyze  –  Due  July  21  (10  days  from  now)  –  What  counts?  Spoken  or  wri*en  discourses  about  racial  issues,  Metalinguis;c  

discourses  about  racialized  language  (e.g.,  racializing  labels  or  descriptors);  Spoken  or  wri*en  discourses  involving  the  indexing  of  racial  posi4ons  or  iden44es  (e.g.,  code-­‐switching,  style-­‐shi\ing).  

–  Where  can  I  find  my  data?  Recordings  of  people  around  you,  online  corpora  (create  your  own),  surveys,  interviews,  etc.  

–  What  methods  can  I  use?  Any  (ethnographic  observa;ons,  interac;onal  analysis,  varia;onist  analysis,  and  other  forms  of  discourse  analysis)  

–  What  if  I’m  at  a  loss?  Talk  to  or  email  me!  

Race  Ma*ers  •  Race  may  be  socially  constructed,  but  it  is  deeply  embedded  

in  our  material  experiences  –  Who  we  befriend,  date,  or  marry  –  Who  receives  housing  and  employment  –  Who  we  define  as  intelligent,  authorita;ve,  or  a*rac;ve  

•  So  what  can  a  linguist  do?  –  Understand  how  language  keeps  race  relevant  and  how  it  keeps  racial  

hierarchies  in  place  –  Present  solu;ons  to  race-­‐based  linguis;c  discrimina;on  

•  What  are  we  doing  this  week?  –  Examining  two  ways  in  which  race  can  come  to  ma*er  because  of  

(perceived)  differences  of  language:  Our  (perceived)  dis;nc;ve  pa*erns  of  pronuncia;on  (today)  and  our  dis;nc;ve  pa*erns  of  communica;on  (Friday)  

Hearing  race  •  Examine  how  our  prac;ces  of  racialized  hearing  can  maintain  

racial  hierarchies  •  Answer  some  ques;ons:  

–  What  is  an  accent?  –  Can  we  tell  a  person’s  race  based  on  their  accent?  (Baugh  2003)  –  Do  our  beliefs  about  the  race  of  a  speaker  (based  on  what  we  see)  

shape  what  accent  we  hear?  (Rubin  1992)  –  What  kinds  of  images  and  ideologies  are  we  accessing  when  we  hear  a  

speaker  who  sounds  “non-­‐na;ve”?  (Lindemann    2003)  –  (When)  is  it  wrong  to  hear  an  accent  as  “ethnic”  or  “racial”?  –  What  are  the  benefits  and  drawbacks  of  the  (modified)  match-­‐guise  

technique  used  in  these  perceptual  studies?  

“Everyone  has  an  accent”  •  What  do  linguists  mean  by  this?  

–  Everyone  has  a  pa*erned  way  of  pronouncing  words  (phonological  habits)  despite  the  percep;on  of  only  some  people  as  having  accents  •  Prosodic  features  (stress  pa*ern:  intona;on,  rhythm,  volume)  •  Segmental  features  (consonants  and  vowels)  •  Syllabic  features:  /ɛbʊriwən    hɛbʊ    ən    aksentʊ/  

•  Why  do  linguists  assert  this?  –  This  rela;vist  statement  decenters  the  linguis;c  norma;vity  of  how  

we  tend  to  hear  accents;  language  “difference”  is  always  relaIve  to  some  (usually  norma;ve)  perspec;ve  

•  When  do  we  hear  an  accent?  –  Ideologies  of  that  locate  speakers  as  “other”(and  some;mes  

“inferior”)  rela;ve  to  some  norm:  ”I  don’t  have  an  accent  but  you  do”  or  “I  have  a  fobby  accent”  

Will  the  real  Barneys  ad  please  speak  up?  

Because you love Chinese accents

From a collection of hand-painted ceramic ware from northern China, $1 to $5, ours alone.

Page 2: W3-RaceMatters · 7/11/17 $hearing?$ university$ • $

7/11/17  

2  

The  “Oriental”  accent  

•  Breakfast at Tiffany’s (1961)  –  Starring Mickey Rooney as. . . Mr. Yunioshi

Group  discussion  ques;ons  •  Why  do  people  perceive  some  accents  as  beau9ful,  friendly,  or  intelligent  

and  some  as  ugly,  evil,  and  ignorant?  Which  of  these  proposed  answers  do  you  buy?  Reject?  –  “Some  dialects  and  accents  are  characterized  by  inherently  inferior  sounds.“  

Counter  examples?  –  “Individuals  carry  personal  prejudices  against  certain  kinds  of  people;  

nega9ve  evalua9ons  of  accents  reflects  an  personal  prejudicial  sen9ments.”  –  “Everyday  discourses,  including  media  imagery,  reproduce  links  between  

nega9ve  quali9es  with  groups  that  have  been  historically  marginalized”  –  “It  is  not  all  foreign  accents,  but  only  accents  linked  to  skin  that  isn’t  white,  or  

which  signals  a  third-­‐world  homeland,  which  evokes  such  nega9ve  reac9ons”  (Lippi  Green,  1997,  English  with  an  Accent:  253).  

•  Can  you  (never,  rarely,  some9mes,  usually,  always)  tell  a  person’s  race  by  how  they  speak?  Can  you  tell  a  person’s  race  by  how  they  look?  

•  We  o\en  hear  accents  as  clues  about  a  person’s  social  iden;ty.  Is  it  wrong  to  hear  an  accent?  To  guess  a  person’s  race  based  on  her  speech?  Can  you  think  of  an  example  when  it  might  be  produc;ve?  

Can  you  hear  their  race?  What  else  can  you  guess  about  them?  

Kim  Pegula  Co-­‐owner  of  Buffalo  Bills  

Gordon  Chang  Professor  at  Stanford  

John  Rennie  Short  Professor  at  UMBC  

John  McWhorter  Professor  at  Columbia  U  

John  Baugh  Professor  at  Wash  U  

Baugh’s  (2003)  opening  •  What  does  Baugh  find  problema;c  about  courts  accep;ng  listeners’  claims  

that  they  can  “tell”  a  person’s  race  based  on  speech  alone?  Doesn’t  he  believe  that  we  can  o\en  “tell”?  –  OJ  Simpson  trial  (California  v.  Simpson)  and  NY  Superior  Court  (People  vs.  Sanchez  1985)  

Cri;ques  of  linguis;c  profiling  •  A  listener  may  incorrectly  ‘hear’  a  person’s  race  and  

subsequent  ac;on  that  is  taken  based  on  this  false  iden;fica;on  may  be  detrimental  –  Heterogeneity:  Not  all  members  of  a  racial  group  speak  the  same  –  Style-­‐shi\ing:  Speakers  do  not  speak  the  same  way  all  the  ;me  

 

Cri;ques  of  linguis;c  profiling  •  A  listener  may  correctly  ‘hear’  a  person’s  race  and  

discriminate  on  this  basis  (e.g.,  housing  discrimina;on)  –  The  listener  may  claim  she  isn’t  discrimina;ng  based  on  race,  but  she  

may  very  well  be  doing  so  

•  LinguisIc  profiling  (Baugh  2003):  Racial  idenIficaIon  (whether  correct  or  incorrect)  and  discriminaIon  based  on  linguis;c  (auditory)  cues  

Page 3: W3-RaceMatters · 7/11/17 $hearing?$ university$ • $

7/11/17  

3  

Housing  discrimina;on  study  •  “Perceptual  and  Phone;c  Experiments  on  American  English  

Dialect  Iden;fica;on”  (Purnell,  Idsardi,  and  Baugh  1999)  in  the  Journal  of  Language  and  Social  Psychology  

•  Racial  discrimina;on  is  visual  and  auditory  •  Four  experiments  and  findings  

1.  Dialect  discrimina;on  takes  place  2.  Ethnic  group  affilia;on  is  iden;fiable  in  speech  3.  Very  li*le  speech  (hello)  needed  to  dis;nguish  dialects  4.  There  are  phone;c  correlates  (syllable  dura;on  of  the  first  syllable  

of  hello)  of  dialects  

Experiment  1  •  Ques;on  

–  Do  landlords  discriminate  in  housing  interviews  over  the  phone?  

•  Method  –  Baugh  (tridialectal  speaker)  conducted  telephone  interviews  with  

prospec;ve  landlords  

–  Called  each  3  ;mes  (at  least  30  minutes  in  between),  once  with  each  accent  

•  Results  –  White  areas  had  strongest  biases  against  non-­‐standard  dialect—i.e.,  

Dialect  discrimina;on  takes  place  in  certain  neighborhoods  

Lowest  rate  of  appt  confirma;on  for  non-­‐standard  speakers  

Highest  concentra;on  of  white  residents  

Lowest  rate  of  appt  confirma;on  

Experiment  2  •  Ques;on  

–  Is  ethnic  group  affilia;on  be  iden;fiable  in  speech?  

•  Method  –  S;mulus  tokens  recorded  by  20  total  speakers  of  3  dialects,  

including  Baugh  –  421  undergraduate  and  graduate  student  judges  –  Forced  choice  of  race  or  ethnicity:  “African  American,  Hispanic  

American,  and  European  American”  

•  Results  –  Baugh’s  dialect  in  each  of  the  three  guises  was  correctly  iden;fied  –  e.g.,  Ethnic  group  affilia;on  is  iden;fiable  in  speech  

Experiment  3  •  Ques;on  

–  Can  just  a  short  hello  be  enough  to  iden;fy  the  race  of  the  speaker?  •  Method  

–  50  undergraduates  (Caucasian  na;ve  SAE  speakers)  guessed  dialect  of  tokens  –  hello  extracted  from  s;mulus  (10  tokens  x  3  dialects  x  twice    =  120  tokens  

•  Results  –  Very  li*le  speech  (hello)  needed  to  dis;nguish  dialects  

33%  =  perfect  

Page 4: W3-RaceMatters · 7/11/17 $hearing?$ university$ • $

7/11/17  

4  

Experiment  4  •  Ques;on  

–  What  acous;c  cues  dis;nguish  the  three  dialects  in  the  word  hello?  •  Method  

–  30  tokens  (10  for  each  dialect)  –  Measurements  

•  Segment,  syllable,  word  dura;ons  •  F1  and  F2:  Midpoint  formant  frequencies  •  H1:H2:  Ra;o  of  amplitude  of  first  two  harmonics  •  F0:  Midpoint  pitch  and  pitch  peak  

•  Results  –  4  significant  measurements  

•  F2  in  /ɛ/:  ChE  has  highest  F2  (frontest  /ɛ/)  (see  next  slide)  •  Pitch  peak  ra;o  •  Dura;on  of  first  syllable  •  HNR  (harmonic  to  noise  ra;o)  

SAE  backest  vowel  =  lowest  F2  

ChE  had  rela;ve  late  pitch  peak  

AAVE  had  rela;vely  long  first  syllable  

AAVE  had  lowest  HNR  ra;o  (ar;culatory  implica;on  unclear;  also,  unclear  if  the  measurement  taken  

was  noise-­‐to-­‐harmonic  or  vice  versa)  

Race  discrimina;on  h*p://www.eeoc.gov/laws/types/race_color.cfm  

         •  Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964 (Title VII): This law

makes it illegal to discriminate against someone on the basis of race, color, religion, national origin, or sex. . .!

•  Race discrimination involves treating someone (an applicant or employee) unfavorably because he/she is of a certain race or because of personal characteristics associated with race (such as hair texture, skin color, or certain facial features). Color discrimination involves treating someone unfavorably because of skin color complexion. . .!

!

•  Do  you  believe  that  linguis;c  profiling,  as  defined  by  Baugh,  can  be  legally  classified  as  “race  discrimina;on”  as  defined  in  Title  VII  of  the  Civil  Rights  Act?    

Race/class  discrimina;on  •  Note  that  SAE  callers  did  not  fare  well  in  East  Palo  Alto  

(Experiment  1).  Is  this  linguis;c  profiling/racial  discrimina;on?  •  If  one  of  the  guises  sounded  like  a  “working  class  white”  

Californian,  would  this  speaker  have  been  linguis;cally  profiled?  •  Baugh’s  (2015)  follow  up  study:  When  black  speakers  use  

Mainstream  American  Standard  English  (MASE),  they  are  o\en  granted  appointments  (see  also  Massey  &  Lundy  2001).  Which  is  most  likely  explana;on?  –  Explana;on  1:  Class  is  more  important  than  race  –  Explana;on  2:  It’s  s;ll  about  race:  MASE  speakers  are  heard  as  white  –  Explana;on  3:  Race  and  class  are  o\en  intertwined:  Racial  language  cues  

also  indicate  class  (‘sounding  white’  means  ‘sounding  privileged’)  

•  How  would  you  design  a  study  to  tease  out  the  effects  of  race  and  class?  Should  they  need  to  be  teased  apart?  

Complexi;es  of  linguis;c  profiling  •  Baugh  men;ons  two  other  related  processes:  

•  Preferen;al  linguis;c  profiling  –  Racial  iden;fica;on  can  be  used  to  connect  with  (rather  than  

discriminate  against)  others  –  Though  Baugh  concedes  that,  “one  person’s  linguis;c  pride  can  easily  

serve  as  another  person’s  source  of  linguis;c  displeasure”  (164)  

•  Linguis;c  adora;on  –  Favorable  linguis;c  stereotypes  of  Bri;sh  and  French  accents  –  Though  Baugh  notes  the  example  of  the  black  Bri;sh  colleague  who  

was  mocked  for  her  Bri;sh  accent.  

Baugh’s  conclusion  •  Baugh  (2003)  writes,  

–  Each  example  confirms  global  linguis9c  profiling  in  human  an9quity.  On  the  basis  of  our  keen  auditory  skills  as  a  species,  I  believe  that  linguis4c  profiling  will  exist  as  long  as  human  language  exists.  The  challenge  to  Americans  and  our  fellow  ci9zens  elsewhere  is  to  have  wisdom,  pa4ence,  and  sufficient  tolerance  of  others  whose  linguis9c  backgrounds  differ  substan9ally  from  our  own.  To  do  so  would  accentuate  the  benefits  of  preferen4al  linguis4c  profiling,  while  discarding  the  tradi9on  of  discriminatory  linguis9c  profiling  that  fans  the  embers  of  racial  discord.  

•  Do  you  agree  that  linguis;c  profiling  will  always  exist?  •  What  kind  of  wisdom,  pa;ence,  and  tolerance  is  Baugh  referring  to?  Does  

he  presuppose  that  those  who  discriminate  are  not  wise,  pa;ent,  or  tolerant.  Do  you  agree  that  this  is  the  case?  

•  What  would  an  accentua;on  of  preferen;al  linguis;c  profiling  without  discriminatory  profiling  look  like?  

Sight  and  sound  •  What  is  the  rela;onship  between  visual  or  auditory  in  

processes  of  racializa;on?  Does  visible  race  always  take  priority  over  audible  race?  

•  Does  a  person’s  looks  like  affect  how  you  hear  them?  

•  Is  there  a  “racial”  parallel  for  the  McGurk  Effect?  

PERCEIVED  LANGUAGE  

PERCEIVED  RACE  

PERCEIVED  RACE  

PERCEIVED  LANGUAGE  

or  

Page 5: W3-RaceMatters · 7/11/17 $hearing?$ university$ • $

7/11/17  

5  

The  McGurk  Effect   Imagined  accent  

•  Philip  Rubin’s  (1992)  study  of  percep;ons  of  NNSTAs  –  “Nonlanguage  Factors  Affec;ng  Undergraduates'  Judgments  of  Nonna;ve  

English-­‐Speaking  Teaching  Assistants”  in  Research  in  Higher  Educa9on  •  Study  1:  Does  instructor  ethnicity  and  lecture  subject  affect  percep9ons  of  

her  ethnicity,  accent,  and  quality  as  well  as  student  comprehension?  –  62  undergraduates  listened  to  a  4-­‐minute  lecture  on  tape.  –  2  projected  slide  photographs:  white  vs.  Asian  –  2  lectures:  science  vs.  humani;es  –  Both  recordings  made  by  same  speaker  (na;ve  speaker  of  English  

from  central  Ohio)  –  Test  of  listening  comprehension  +  ra;ngs  of  accent,  ethnicity,  quality  

of  teaching.  1 2 3 4 5 6 7!

American accent ! : : : : : : : ! Foreign accent !

Caucasian/European ! : : : : : : : ! Oriental/Asian !

Poor teacher ! : : : : : : : ! Effective teacher !

Study  1:  Results  TOPIC:    HumaniIes     Science  INSTRUCTOR  PHOTO:    Caucasian      Asian      Caucasian      Asian    

N=   16   17   16   13    MEAN      SD      MEAN      SD      MEAN      SD      MEAN      SD  

Perceived  accent   3.44   2.56   4.94   1.98   2.75   2.44   3.77   1.88  Perceived  ethnicity   2.75   1.98   5.53   1.97   2.06   1.39   6.23   0.93  Comprehension   11.94   4.34   9.93   5.70   12.50   5.90   7.31   4.70  Teaching  qualifica;ons   8.69   3.61   8.18   1.81   7.25   2.46   9.15   2.73  A�tude  homophily   5.56   5.51   6.00   4.95   2.38   4.24   3.38   4.37  Background  homophily   12.75   4.12   12.18   4.03   13.25   4.48   10.31   3.54  Values  homophily   15.88   4.53   14.94   2.88   14.81   3.21   14.46   3.86  Appearance  homophily   11.13   5.04   10.71   5.77   10.13   4.77   7.69   3.22  

Asian  photo   perceived  as   more  Oriental/Asian  

Caucasian  photo     perceived  as   less  

Asian  photo   perceived  as   more  foreign  

Caucasian  photo     perceived  as   less  

Asian  photo   resulted  in   worse  comprehension  

Caucasian  photo     resulted  in   be*er  

more  or  less?  

more  or  less?  

be*er  or  worse?  

Study  2  •  Study  2:  What  factors  predict  undergraduates'  ra9ngs  of  NNSTAs  and  their  

comprehension  of  NNSTA  speech?  •  Par;cipants:  148  students  •  S;muli  variables  

–  Accent:  high  Chinese,  moderate  Chinese,  or  SAE  –  Subject:  science  or  humani;es  –  Photo:  Asian,  Caucasian/European,  or  no  photo  

•  Ra;ngs  performed  just  as  in  Study  1  •  Background  ques;onnaire  

–  Number  of  classes  in  which  they  had  been  instructed  by  NNSTAs  –  Number  of  weeks  they  had  traveled  outside  the  United  States    –  A�tudes  toward  nonna;ve  speakers  in  general  (e.g.,  "Our  campus  benefits  by  having  

nonna;ve  English  speakers  a*ending").  

Study  2:  Results  

Perceived  more  similar  a�tude   correlated  with   higher  teacher  ra;ng  

Perceived  greater  foreign  accent   correlated  with   lower  teacher  ra;ng  

A*ended  more  NNSTA  classes   resulted  in   beZer  listening  comprehension  

Study  3:  Pilot  interven;on  effort  in  which  undergraduates  served  as  teaching  coaches  for  NNSTAs.  No  detectable  effect  on  undergraduates'  a�tudes.  

Ques;ons  •  What  are  the  prac;cal  implica;ons  of  this  study?  Do  changes  

need  to  be  made  in  universi;es?  What  kind?  •  Rubin  uses  a  “matched-­‐guise”  approach  for  part  of  his  study  

(the  two  Chinese  accents).  What  are  the  benefits  and  disadvantages  of  this  approach?  

Page 6: W3-RaceMatters · 7/11/17 $hearing?$ university$ • $

7/11/17  

6  

What  does  a  non-­‐na;ve  accent  really  mean  for  hearers?  

•  We  know  that  consequences  may  arise  from  hearing  an  accent,  but  what  are  hearers  really  hearing?  

•  Stephanie  Lindemann.  2003.  Koreans,  Chinese  or  Indians?  A�tudes  and  ideologies  about  non-­‐na;ve  English  speakers  in  the  United  States.  Journal  of  Sociolinguis9cs.  

•  Local  ideologies  vary,  so  a  “na;ve  speaker”  accents  can  evoke  different  kinds  of  images  –  Not  all  non-­‐na;ve  accents  are  s;gma;zed:  Cargile  &  Giles  (1998)  

showed  that  a  “moderate”  Japanese  accent  was  highly  rated  on  status  (but  not  a*rac;veness)  traits  compared  to  a  ‘Stand.  American’  guise  

–  Hearers  may  be  a*uned  to  other  social  dimensions  (e.g.,  class)  

•  Hearers  some;mes  misiden;fy  the  race  of  the  speaker  

Lindemann’s  study  •  Mo;va;ons:  Researchers  haven’t  inves;gated  misiden;fied  

ethnicity  or  how  local  ideologies  may  shape  percep;ons  •  Ques;on:  How  do  Michiganders  view  (unfamiliar)  Koreans?  

–  Background  facts:  Asians  as  a  “model  minority”  and  Koreans  o\en  mistaken  for  Chinese  

•  Par;cipants:  39  na;ve  speakers  of  English  at  a  Midwestern  university  

•  S;mulus:  4  na;ve  Midwestern  English  and  4  na;ve  Korean  speakers  reading  a  101-­‐word  answering  machine  outgoing  message;  2  female  and  2  male  for  each  na;ve  language  

•  Task:  Listen  to  first  sentence  of  10  passages;  then  listen  to  each  message  twice;  Iden;fy  non-­‐na;ve  speaker  ethnicity;  Rate  speakers  on  6  status    +    6  solidarity  scales  (7-­‐point  scale)  

Result  1  •  RESULT  1:  Koreans  iden;fied  as  (in  this  order)  Asian  (23%),  Chinese,  

Indian,  Japanese,  La;no,  Korean  (8%),  Other.  •  BUT  doesn’t  mean  judgments  were  of  Indians/La;nos;  judgments  at  

second  (higher,  subconscious)  order;  l/r  alterna;on  may  be  “Korean”  or  “foreign/strange”    

Results  2a  •  Na;ve  Korean  speakers  judged  lower  on  status  scales  

–  Ra;ngs  of  posi;ve  quali;es  (intelligent,  successful,  ambi;ous);  nega;ve  quali;es  (lazy,  uneducated,  incompetent)  

Results  2a  •  Na;ve  Korean  speakers  judged  lower  on  status  scales  

–  Ra;ngs  of  posi;ve  quali;es  (intelligent,  successful,  ambi;ous);  nega;ve  quali;es  (lazy,  uneducated,  incompetent)  

Results  2b  •  Na;ve  Korean  speakers  judged  equal  on  solidarity  scales  

–  Ra;ngs  of  posi;ve  quali;es  (friendly,  likeable,  helpful);  nega;ve  quali;es  (unkind,  insincere,  aloof)  

–  English  NSs  as  ‘stuck  up’  

Page 7: W3-RaceMatters · 7/11/17 $hearing?$ university$ • $

7/11/17  

7  

Results  2c  •  Na;ve  Korean  speakers  judged  lower  on  language-­‐focused  

traits  –  Ra;ngs  of  posi;ve  (appropriate  to  hire  to  do  message,  nice  to  listen  to);  

nega;ve  (speaks  poorly)  –  Raters  felt  it  was  okay  to  rate  language  poorly  even  if  not  the  speaker  

Lindemann’s  discussion  •  Results  may  be  shaped  by  specific  site  where  listeners  are  not  

threatened  by  Koreans  (What  about  LA  or  NYC?)  •  Unsurprising  result:  Koreans  judged  as  an  outgroup  and  as  equally  

friendly  (equal  solidarity),  less  intelligent  (low  status)  –  Aligned  with  previous  studies:  –  Subordinate/outgroup  received  lower  status  ra;ngs  –  High  solidarity  can  be  assigned  to  ingroup,  dominant,  or  subordinate  group  

•  Surprising  results:  Judged  as  less  intelligent  despite  local  stereotype  of  Asians  as  hardworking,  intelligent,  reserved  –  Suggests  that  listeners  primarily  heard  them  as  “non  na;ve”  as  opposed  to  

“Asian”  or  “Korean”  

–  Possible  explana;on:  Koreans  are  not  a  salient  category  (as  seen  by  misiden;fica;ons)  

•  Korean  accent  interpreted  as  ‘foreign  faul;ness’  rather  than  ‘Koreanness’  per  se  

Conclusions  •  Is  it  wrong  to  hear  an  accent?  

–  Problems  arise  not  because  we  can  (or  think  we  can)  iden;fy  race  but  because  we  assign  moral  qualiIes  based  on  these  racialized  accents  (‘Friendly’  La9no;  ‘Annoying’  Chinese;  ‘Sexy’  blonde)  as  well  as  take  acIons  that  reproduce  racial  hierarchies.  

•  If  racial  iden;fica;on  is  an  inevitable  part  of  our  social  reality,  where  do  we  go  from  here?