3
advantage. Any increase in benefit recovery (via kin selection) could move the cost/benefit ratio across the tipping point. Kin selection is very likely to be a strong force helping maintain genes encoding secreted factor even in the absence of gene transfer. Bacteria are not randomly mixed; E. coli’s neighbors in the intestinal lumen will be brethren produced from common (great) n -grandparents. Because cell division places daughter cells adjacent to one another, ‘public goods’ will be shared most frequently between members of a clone. The mechanistic question posed by Nogueira et al.’s [5] model is whether gene transfer can convert neighboring cells into ‘relatives’ at a rate sufficient to minimize their depletion of the public goods. It is not clear that individual- level selection is not sufficient to explain the data. First, we expect the genes under strong selection to be retained most readily; these would fail to show a history of loss and reacquisition. Genes under weak selection would be those detected by Nogueira et al. [5], and these may be biased towards those encoded secreted proteins since their cost is large relative to their diluted benefits. Alternatively, genes encoding external and secreted proteins are often subject to frequency-dependent and/or diversifying selection, both of which may lead to gene loss and reacquisition [9]. It is very difficult to determine if the benefits recovered by individual cells fail to exceed the costs, therefore requiring kin selection to make up the difference. There is an association between genes encoding secreted proteins and integrases and they are more often found in recombination ‘hotspots’. This would be expected if this were a mechanism for gene reacquisition, following from their first observation. Yet it does not address the question of relative cost and benefit. How is cooperation enforced? If kin selection were to provide sufficient benefit to maintain cooperation, then enforcement is not necessary. Yet Nogueira et al. [5] offer an addition route, whereby addiction modules weed out cheaters. These genetic elements are easily acquired but difficult to lose, as their deletion from the genome can be lethal (hence ‘addiction’ of the cell to their presence) [10]. A common example is bacterial restriction/modification genes: acquisition of the genes is easy, but sudden loss can be lethal as the longer-lived restriction enzyme begins to degrade genomic DNA that is left unprotected by its cognate modification enzyme. The authors find an association between genes encoding secreted proteins and those belonging to addiction modules. Therefore, sudden loss of genes encoding social traits could also remove the addiction module, resulting in cell death; thus, this association enforces cooperation by preventing recidivism (Figure 1). The association between social genes and addiction modules may simply reflect the transit of both elements on extrachromosomal elements; it remains to be seen if a social gene’s proximity to an addiction module has a meaningful impact on its frequency in the population. As a whole, Nogueira et al. [5] do not clearly demonstrate that horizontal gene transfer drives cooperation among bacteria. But that is also a very tall order. What they have done is provide an intriguing analysis that forces microbiologists both to examine forces leading to gene persistence in populations and to reconsider gene function solely in the context of individual cells. References 1. McFall-Nagai, M. (2008). Host-microbe symbiosis: the squid-Vibrio association — a naturally occurring, experimental model of animal/bacterial partnerships. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 635, 102–112. 2. Velicer, G.J., and Vos, M. (2009). Sociobiology of the myxobacteria. Annu. Rev. Microbiol. 63, 599–623. 3. Branda, S.S., Gonza ´ lez-Pastor, J.E., Ben- Yehuda, S., Losick, R., and Kolter, R. (2001). Fruiting body formation by Bacillus subtilis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 11621–11626. 4. Federla, M.J. (2009). Autoinducer-2-based chemical communication in bacteria: complexities of interspecies signaling. Contrib. Microbiol. 16, 18–32. 5. Nogueira, T., Rankin, D.J., Touchon, M., Taddei, F., Brown, S.P., and Rocha, E.P.C. (2009). Horizontal gene transfer of the secretome drives the evolution of bacterial cooperation and virulence. Curr. Biol. 19, 1683–1691. 6. Hamilton, W.D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 1–16. 7. Hamilton, W.D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 17–52. 8. MacLean, R.C. (2008). The tragedy of the commons in microbial populations: insights from theoretical, comparative and experimental studies. Heredity 100, 471–477. 9. Lipsitch, M., and O’Hagan, J.J. (2007). Patterns of antigenic diversity and the mechanisms that maintain them. J. R. Soc. Interface 4, 787–802. 10. Engelberg-Kulka, H., and Glaser, G. (1999). Addiction modules and programmed cell death and antideath in bacterial cultures. Annu. Rev. Microbiol. 53, 43–70. Department of Biological Sciences, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA. E-mail: [email protected] DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.045 Visual Maps: To Merge or Not To Merge A recent study of a child born with one cerebral hemisphere has revealed an extreme developmental reorganization of visual cortex. Self-organizing visual maps demonstrate a surprisingly flexible restructuring in response to cortical loss. Alyssa A. Brewer Primate visual cortex consists of multiple maps of visual space which follow the organization of the retina — hence these maps are termed retinotopic [1]. Under normal conditions, these retinotopic maps are stable throughout life; however, there are several examples of how these retinotopic cortical maps can be altered by genetic mutations and/or early visual experience [2–4]. A recent study by Muckli et al. [5] provides a striking illustration of the flexibility of the development of these retinotopic maps in human visual cortex. In their case study, a 10 year old girl presents with a congenital loss of her right cerebral hemisphere but with a visual field of view that Dispatch R945

Visual Maps: To Merge or Not To Merge

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Visual Maps: To Merge or Not To Merge

advantage. Any increase in benefitrecovery (via kin selection) could movethe cost/benefit ratio across the tippingpoint.

Kin selection is very likely to bea strong force helping maintain genesencoding secreted factor even in theabsence of gene transfer. Bacteriaare not randomly mixed; E. coli’sneighbors in the intestinal lumen willbe brethren produced from common(great)n-grandparents. Because celldivision places daughter cells adjacentto one another, ‘public goods’ willbe shared most frequently betweenmembers of a clone. The mechanisticquestion posed by Nogueira et al.’s [5]model is whether gene transfercan convert neighboring cells into‘relatives’ at a rate sufficient tominimize their depletion of the publicgoods. It is not clear that individual-level selection is not sufficient toexplain the data. First, we expectthe genes under strong selection tobe retained most readily; these wouldfail to show a history of loss andreacquisition. Genes under weakselection would be those detectedby Nogueira et al. [5], and these maybe biased towards those encodedsecreted proteins since their cost islarge relative to their diluted benefits.Alternatively, genes encoding externaland secreted proteins are often subjectto frequency-dependent and/ordiversifying selection, both of whichmay lead to gene loss and reacquisition[9]. It is very difficult to determine if thebenefits recovered by individual cellsfail to exceed the costs, thereforerequiring kin selection to make upthe difference. There is an associationbetween genes encoding secretedproteins and integrases and they are

more often found in recombination‘hotspots’. This would be expectedif this were a mechanism for genereacquisition, following from theirfirst observation. Yet it does notaddress the question of relativecost and benefit.

How is cooperation enforced? If kinselection were to provide sufficientbenefit to maintain cooperation, thenenforcement is not necessary. YetNogueira et al. [5] offer an additionroute, whereby addiction modulesweed out cheaters. These geneticelements are easily acquired butdifficult to lose, as their deletion fromthe genome can be lethal (hence‘addiction’ of the cell to theirpresence) [10]. A common example isbacterial restriction/modificationgenes: acquisition of the genes is easy,but sudden loss can be lethal asthe longer-lived restriction enzymebegins to degrade genomic DNA thatis left unprotected by its cognatemodification enzyme. The authorsfind an association between genesencoding secreted proteins and thosebelonging to addiction modules.Therefore, sudden loss of genesencoding social traits could alsoremove the addiction module, resultingin cell death; thus, this associationenforces cooperation by preventingrecidivism (Figure 1). The associationbetween social genes and addictionmodules may simply reflect the transitof both elements on extrachromosomalelements; it remains to be seen ifa social gene’s proximity to anaddiction module has a meaningfulimpact on its frequency in thepopulation.

As a whole, Nogueira et al. [5] do notclearly demonstrate that horizontal

gene transfer drives cooperationamong bacteria. But that is alsoa very tall order. What they have doneis provide an intriguing analysisthat forces microbiologists bothto examine forces leading to genepersistence in populations andto reconsider gene function solelyin the context of individual cells.

References1. McFall-Nagai, M. (2008). Host-microbe

symbiosis: the squid-Vibrio association — anaturally occurring, experimental model ofanimal/bacterial partnerships. Adv. Exp. Med.Biol. 635, 102–112.

2. Velicer, G.J., and Vos, M. (2009). Sociobiologyof the myxobacteria. Annu. Rev. Microbiol. 63,599–623.

3. Branda, S.S., Gonzalez-Pastor, J.E., Ben-Yehuda, S., Losick, R., and Kolter, R. (2001).Fruiting body formation by Bacillus subtilis.Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 11621–11626.

4. Federla, M.J. (2009). Autoinducer-2-basedchemical communication in bacteria:complexities of interspecies signaling. Contrib.Microbiol. 16, 18–32.

5. Nogueira, T., Rankin, D.J., Touchon, M.,Taddei, F., Brown, S.P., and Rocha, E.P.C.(2009). Horizontal gene transfer of thesecretome drives the evolution of bacterialcooperation and virulence.Curr. Biol. 19, 1683–1691.

6. Hamilton, W.D. (1964). The genetical evolutionof social behaviour. I. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 1–16.

7. Hamilton, W.D. (1964). The genetical evolutionof social behaviour. II. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 17–52.

8. MacLean, R.C. (2008). The tragedy of thecommons in microbial populations: insightsfrom theoretical, comparative and experimentalstudies. Heredity 100, 471–477.

9. Lipsitch, M., and O’Hagan, J.J. (2007). Patternsof antigenic diversity and the mechanisms thatmaintain them. J. R. Soc. Interface 4, 787–802.

10. Engelberg-Kulka, H., and Glaser, G. (1999).Addiction modules and programmed cell deathand antideath in bacterial cultures. Annu. Rev.Microbiol. 53, 43–70.

Department of Biological Sciences,University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh,PA 15260, USA.E-mail: [email protected]

DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.045

DispatchR945

Visual Maps: To Merge or Not ToMerge

A recent study of a child born with one cerebral hemisphere has revealed anextreme developmental reorganization of visual cortex. Self-organizing visualmaps demonstrate a surprisingly flexible restructuring in response to corticalloss.

Alyssa A. Brewer

Primate visual cortex consists ofmultiple maps of visual space

which follow the organization of theretina — hence these maps aretermed retinotopic [1]. Under normalconditions, these retinotopic maps

are stable throughout life; however,there are several examples ofhow these retinotopic corticalmaps can be altered by geneticmutations and/or early visualexperience [2–4]. A recent study byMuckli et al. [5] provides a strikingillustration of the flexibility of thedevelopment of these retinotopicmaps in human visual cortex. Intheir case study, a 10 year old girlpresents with a congenital lossof her right cerebral hemispherebut with a visual field of view that

Page 2: Visual Maps: To Merge or Not To Merge

Current Biology Vol 19 No 20R946

unexpectedly spans both hemifieldsof visual space. Such a drasticchange in early in utero corticaldevelopment has rarely beeninvestigated.

To understand why the preservationof her bilateral vision is so surprising,one must examine the typical patternof spatial organization in visualcortex. Normally, the retinotopicmaps of primate visual cortex spanthe occipital lobes of both corticalhemispheres (Figure 1A) [6]. Theinformation from the left hemifieldof visual space is represented in theprimary visual cortex (V1) of theright occipital lobe, and theinformation from the right hemifieldis represented in V1 of the leftoccipital lobe. A coherentrepresentation of visual space isvery carefully conserved in each V1,with the retinotopic map smoothlyrepresenting visual space from theupper to the lower vertical meridian

of each hemifield. At therepresentations of the dorsal andventral vertical meridia in V1, thespatial representations reverse backtowards the horizontal meridianto form the dorsal and ventralquarterfield representations thatcomprise area V2 (Figure 1B).This repetition of distinct maps ofvisual space is repeated throughoutlarge regions of visual cortex.Within a single visual field map,each position in visual space is onlyrepresented once, and each mapis assumed to perform a set ofunique computations across visualspace [7].

In accordance with this division ofvisual image processing into distincthemispheres, image informationfrom each hemifield normally reachesvisual cortex along very separatepathways [8]. An image falling on theretinae is separated into nasal andtemporal components in each eye.

Consider in Figure 2A how theimage information from just the lefthemifield is transmitted from theretinae to visual cortex. The part ofthe image within the left hemifieldfalls on the nasal retina in the lefteye and the temporal retina in theright eye. The information fromthese two sections of retinaerepresenting the left hemifield istransmitted through the optic nerveto the optic chiasm. To keep thecorrect hemifield informationtogether for collective corticalprocessing, the axons fromthe nasal retina of the left eye crossover to the right optic tract at theoptic chiasm, while the axons fromthe temporal retina of the righteye remain on the right side.Together, these sets of fiberscarrying left hemifield imageinformation then project first tothe right lateral geniculate nucleus(LGN) of the thalamus and thenthrough the optic radiations toV1 at the occipital pole of theright cortical hemisphere.Retinotopic organization ispreserved along these pathwaysand then integrated into specificmaps within visual cortex underthe guidance of molecular cuesand patterns of coordinatedactivity [9].

So how does the child in the casestudy from Muckli et al. [5] havevision that spans both hemifields,when she is missing her entire rightvisual cortex? Muckli et al. [5]suggest that all of the axons of theintact left eye in their case studyproject to the left hemisphere,with the few axons of theunderdeveloped right eye also allcrossing at the chiasm to the lefthemisphere (Figure 2B). Therepresentations of visual space inthis subject show a massiverestructuring of the normalretinotopic maps to represent bothhemifields in a single corticalhemisphere. Alternative patternsof map organization forthe coherent merging of therepresentations of the left and righthemifields into a single hemisphereinclude: directly overlappingcontralateral and ipsilateral mapsand/or the insertion of additional,discrete ipsilateral spatialrepresentations [10]. In the first case,the same region of cortex wouldrepresent both hemifields of space

B

A

V1

V2vV3v

V2d

FuG

ColS

CalS

POS

20°

FuGColS

CalS

POS CC

STS

CS

IPS

V1

V2vV3v

V2d

FuG

ColS

CalS

POS

FuGColS

CalS

POS CC

STS

CS

IPS

Current Biology

Figure 1. Visual field maps in human cortex.

(A) The span of cortical responses in posterior visual cortex to an expanding ring stimulus isshown across the left hemisphere of a single example subject. The lateral view is shown on theleft, medial on the right. Eccentricity measurements were made using a 3� radius stimulus. Thecolor overlay depicts the region of visual space (see inset) that most effectively stimulates thatregion of cortex. (B) A view of the posterior occipital lobe of a single subject’s left hemisphereillustrates the position of the first three posterior visual field maps: V1, V2, V3. The color over-lay indicates the visual field angle (left) or eccentricity (right) that produces the most powerfulresponse at each cortical location (see the colored legends on the right). For clarity,only responses within these visual field maps are colored, and only locations with a powerfulresponse are shown (statistical coherence >0.20). CalS, calcarine sulcus; POS, parieto-occipital sulcus; ColS, collateral sulcus; FuG, fusiform gyrus; IPS, intraparietal sulcus; STS,superior temporal sulcus; CS, central sulcus; CC, corpus callosum.

Page 3: Visual Maps: To Merge or Not To Merge

DispatchR947

smoothly across the corticalrepresentation, much like theinterspersed input to oculardominance columns. In the second,distinct islands of ipsilateralmaps would be present within thedistorted representation of thecontralateral hemifield. FunctionalMRI (fMRI) measurements by Muckliet al. [5] suggest that both occur intheir case study across areas V1, V2and V3.

What might be the driving forcesbehind this unusual integration ofboth hemifields of visual space intoone hemisphere? The axons ofthe optic nerve originating fromthe retinal ganglion cells usuallyfind their way through this mazeof pathways by following precisemolecular guidance cues.These guidance cues directspecific axons to one hemisphereor the other, depending on theparticular receptor expressed byeach retinal cell interacting withreceptor cues at the optic chiasm(for example, Eph-B1, Ephrin-B2 [11]).The visual pathways are further

refined by synchronizedneuronal activity, both spontaneouswaves and experience-drivenresponses [9].

The disruption of normal guidancecues has been studied in severalgenetic models that demonstratethe possibility of the integrationof contra- and ipsilateral visualfields into a single hemisphere(for example, albino mutationswith complete chiasmatic crossing[3]). One outstanding question hasbeen the importance of potentialcontralateral and ipsilateralguidance cues in the properdevelopment of visual pathways. Inthe case study by Muckli et al. [5],the normal guidance cues fromone entire hemisphere are lost. Theredirection of the left nasal axonsto the ipsilateral left hemispheredemonstrates that there are norepressor cues from the ipsilateralhemisphere preventing thisrestructuring of the visualpathways. With no changes inmolecular expression thought toexist in the intact hemisphere in

this subject, changes in thepatterns of input activity are thelikely organizing forces for theflexible restructuring of theinternal representations ofvisual space in visual field mapslike V1.

The interplay of changes inmolecular guidance cues andself-organizing cortical activityproduce a merged corticalrepresentation of visual space inthis unique subject. Her ability tosee across both hemifieldsof visual space demonstratesthe astonishing range of flexiblerestructuring of the visualpathways possible underextreme changes of corticaldevelopment.

References1. Van Essen, D.C. (2003). Organization of visual

areas in macaque and human cerebral cortex.In The Visual Neurosciences, L.M. Chalupa andJ.S. Werner, eds. (Boston: Bradford Books),pp. 507–521.

2. Baseler, H.A., Brewer, A.A., Sharpe, L.T.,Morland, A.B., Jagle, H., and Wandell, B.A.(2002). Reorganization of human corticalmaps caused by inherited photoreceptorabnormalities. Nat. Neurosci. 5,364–370.

3. Hoffmann, M.B., Tolhurst, D.J., Moore, A.T.,and Morland, A.B. (2003). Organization of thevisual cortex in human albinism. J. Neurosci.23, 8921–8930.

4. Fine, I., Wade, A.R., Brewer, A.A., May, M.G.,Goodman, D.F., Boynton, G.M., Wandell, B.A.,and MacLeod, D.I. (2003). Long-termdeprivation affects visual perception andcortex. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 915–916.

5. Muckli, L., Naumer, M.J., and Singer, W. (2009).Bilateral visual field maps in a patient with onlyone hemisphere. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA106, 13034–13039.

6. Wandell, B.A., Dumoulin, S.O., andBrewer, A.A. (2007). Visual field maps in humancortex. Neuron 56, 366–383.

7. Wandell, B.A., Brewer, A.A., andDougherty, R.F. (2005). Visual field mapclusters in human cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc.Lond. B. 360, 693–707.

8. Horton, J.C., and Hoyt, W.F. (1991). Therepresentation of the visual field in humanstriate cortex. A revision of the classicHolmes map. Arch. Ophthalmol. 109,816–824.

9. Katz, L.C., and Shatz, C.J. (1996).Synaptic activity and the constructionof cortical circuits. Science 274,1133–1138.

10. Krubitzer, L. (2007). The magnificentcompromise: cortical field evolution inmammals. Neuron 56, 201–208.

11. Polleux, F., Ince-Dunn, G., and Ghosh, A.(2007). Transcriptional regulation ofvertebrate axon guidance and synapseformation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8,331–340.

Department of Cognitive Sciences, 3151Social Sciences Plaza, University ofCalifornia, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92617, USA.E-mail: [email protected]

DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.016

LGN

V1 V1

LGNLGNLGN

V1 V1

A B

LGN

V1 V1

LGN

VVVVVVVV

LGGGGGGGGGGGGGG

Current Biology

Figure 2. Visual pathways from retina to V1.

(A) The pathway of visual information from retina to primary visual cortex is shown overlaid onan axial T1 image. Note that the information from the left hemifield of visual space (green) ineach eye travels along pathways in the right hemifield, and vice versa for the right hemifield(red). ‘V1’ denotes the approximate locations of primary visual cortex in each hemisphere.Similarly, ‘LGN’ denotes the approximate locations of the lateral geniculate nucleus of the thal-amus. (B) The visual pathways of the case study from Muckli et al. [5] are estimated. Note thatall information in the left eye remains in the left hemisphere, with no crossing at the opticchiasm. Residual fibers from the subject’s underdeveloped right eye are shown as the dottedline crossing completely at the chiasm to the left hemisphere. The right cortical hemisphere isabsent in this subject.