108
Vision in echolocating bats Johan Eklöf Dissertation Göteborg University Department of Zoology Box 463 SE-405 30 Göteborg Sweden Avhandling för filosofie doktorsexamen i zoomorfologi, som enligt Naturvetenskapliga fakultetens beslut kommer att offentligen försvaras onsdagen den 28 maj 2003, kl 10:00 i föreläsningssalen, Zoologiska institutionen, Medicinaregatan 18, Göteborg. Fakultetsopponent är Professor Paul Racey, University of Aberdeen.

Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

  • Upload
    vuhuong

  • View
    214

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

0

Vision in echolocating bats

Johan Eklöf

Dissertation

Göteborg University

Department of Zoology

Box 463 SE-405 30 Göteborg

Sweden

Avhandling för filosofie doktorsexamen i zoomorfologi, som enligt Naturvetenskapliga fakultetens beslut kommer att offentligen försvaras onsdagen den 28 maj 2003, kl 10:00 i föreläsningssalen, Zoologiska institutionen, Medicinaregatan 18, Göteborg. Fakultetsopponent är Professor Paul Racey, University of Aberdeen.

Page 2: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

1

Bat - Bats bat (b t) n. A binge; a spree

n. A stout wooden stick; a cudgel

n. Any of various nocturnal flying mammals of the order Chiroptera, having membranous wings that extend from the forelimbs to the hind limbs or tail and anatomical adaptations for echolocation, by which they navigate and hunt prey

v. To hit

v. To wander about aimlessly

v. To discuss or consider at length

bats adj. Crazy; insane

bat out To produce in a hurried or informal manner off the bat Without hesitation; immediately go to bat for To give assistance to; defend not bat an eye To show no emotion; appear unaffected have bats in (one's) belfry To behave in an eccentric, bizarre manner Göteborg University 2003 ISBN 91-628-5699-5

Page 3: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

2

Illustrations by Olof Helje

A doctoral thesis at a university in Sweden is produced either as a monograph or as a collection of papers. In the latter case, the introductory part constitutes the formal thesis, which summarises the accompanying papers. They have already been published or are manuscripts at various stages (in press, submitted or in ms).

Page 4: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

3

Page 5: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

4

Eklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University Key words: acoustic clutter, foraging tactics, Microchiroptera, perception, sensory ecology, ultrasound, visual acuity ABSTRACT The use of ultrasonic echolocation (sonar) in air is seriously constrained by the attenuation of high frequency sounds and unwanted echoes from the background (called clutter). Therefore, in many situations, echolocating bats have to rely on other sensory cues. The aim of this thesis is to investigate the use of vision by echolocating bats. Bat eyes are generally small, especially among aerial hawking insectivores, with the exception of members of the family Emballonuridae. In gleaning, and in frugivorous species, however, the eyes tend to be larger and more prominent. The eyes of all bats are well adapted to low illumination, having mainly rod-based retinas, large corneal surfaces and lenses, and generally large receptor fields. Bats can easily detect small differences in brightness on clear nights, and the visual acuity remains relatively good in dim illuminations. The visual resolving power (as obtained from counts of retinal ganglion cells or by optomotor response tests) varies considerably among the different species of bats, from less than 0.06° of arc in Macrotus californicus (Phyllostomidae) to almost 5° in aerial hawking Myotis species (Vespertilionidae). Generally, the visual acuity is similar to that of rats and mice, suggesting that cm-sized object can be discriminated at ranges less than a few metres. Studies on pattern discrimination have yielded highly variable results. Fruit and nectar eating species respond to patterns to a larger extent than aerial insectivores. One of the most fundamental roles of the eyes is to register the amount of ambient light, in order to establish photoperiodic cycles. Some tropical bats avoid too bright conditions, i.e. moonlit nights probably due to increased predation risk, a behaviour not found in high latitude species.

As sonar only works well at short ranges, vision is primarily used for detection of landmarks and to avoid objects when moving over long distances, for example during seasonal migration and when commuting between feeding sites. In these situations, there seems

Dayash
Highlight
Page 6: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

5

to be precedence of vision over sonar. At short range, within that of echolocation, bats may defer to visual cues in addition to sonar and spatial memory to solve different tasks of orientation, especially when there is conflicting information. Light conditions and time of the day may determine the behaviour of the bats and thus which sensory cues will be used.

There is an increasing amount of data suggesting that vision might be of importance in some situations and some aspects of foraging, especially for frugivorous and nectarivorous bats, which can make use of differences in brightness and spectral composition, to find different food items. But even in species traditionally considered to rely heavily on echolocation, such as most insectivorous bats, vision seems to play a more important role than has been recognised previously. The gleaning brown long-eared bat (Plecotus auritus, Vespertilionidae), known to forage mostly by using passive listening, detects prey more readily by using vision than by using sonar, and the aerial hawking northern bat (Eptesicus nilssonii, Vespertilionidae), use visual information in addition to sonar to find large stationary prey in clutter. Although echolocation is the key innovation that have made it possible for bats to fly at night, vision is retained as an important complement; and indeed bats use an array of different sensory inputs to solve the different tasks of life.

Page 7: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

6

Eklöf, J. Syn hos ekolokaliserande fladdermöss Zoologiska institutionen, Göteborgs universitet SAMMANFATTNING Fladdermöss av underordningen Microchiroptera använder sig av ekolokalisation (sonar; SOund Navigation And Ranging) för att orientera och för att finna byten i mörker. Sonar ersätter således till viss del den funktion som synen har hos många andra djur. På grund av uttunningen av ljudvågor i luft och så kallat ”klotter” är dock räckvidden vanligen begränsad till ett fåtal meter. Fladdermöss måste därför använda sig av andra sinnesintryck för att komplettera den ibland begränsade information som sonar ger. I denna avhandling belyser jag synens roll i fladdermössens liv. Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga, men variationen är stor. Hos arter som plockar byten från underlag (gleaners) och bland fruktätare finner man de största ögonen. Alla fladdermusögon är dock anpassade för svagt ljus, med stora linser och breda receptorfält. Fladdermöss är relativt bra på att upptäcka små skillnader i belysning och deras synskärpa försämras inte nämnvärt i ljusförhållanden vi skulle uppfatta som totalt mörker. Synskärpa eller upplösningsförmåga varierar dock mycket mellan olika arter. Man kan mäta upplösningsförmåga antingen teoretiskt genom att räkna ganglieceller i ögat, eller genom beteendestudier, i vilka fladdermössen presenteras med roterande linjemönster av olika storlek. Vissa av våra svenska Myotis-arter ser inte mycket bättre än att de kan separera objekt med 5° mellanrum, medan den amerikanska Macrotus californicus kan separera objekt med mindre än 0.06°, vilket ungefär motsvarar upplösningsförmågan hos en hund. Huruvida fladdermöss kan skilja ut olika former och mönster med hjälp av synen verkar också variera betydligt mellan olika arter, men generellt verkar frukt- och nektarätare vara bättre på detta än sina insektsätande släktingar. En av de mest grundläggande av ögats funktioner är att registrera mängden ljus i omgivningen och på så vis kalibrera den inre klockan. Vissa tropiska fladdermöss undviker att flyga ut om natten är för ljus, till exempel då det är fullmåne, ett beteende vi inte finner i någon högre utsträckning bland fladdermössen på våra breddgrader. Eftersom sonar endast fungerar tillfredsställande på korta avstånd, används synen främst på längre håll, för att till exempel finna landmärken

Page 8: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

7

och för att undvika hinder på väg till och från födoplatser, eller under migration. I sådana situationer verkar det som om synintryck är viktigare än information från sonar. Även inom räckvidden för sonar kan man ibland se att fladdermöss förlitar sig till synen, särskilt om sonar- och synintryck står i konflikt. Mängden ljus och tiden på dygnet kan också avgöra vilket av sinnena som har företräde.

Frukt- och nektarätande fladdermöss har generellt sett bättre syn än insektsätare och kan förmodas utnyttja synen i relativt stor utsträckning då de söker efter föda. Men även insektsätare tar hjälp av syninformation då det behövs. Långörad fladdermus Plecotus auritus plockar ofta stillasittande insekter från blad och använder då framför allt sin känsliga hörsel för att lokalisera ljud som bytena själva åstadkommer. Den använder dock synintryck hellre än ekolokalisation som komplement till den passiva hörseln. Nordisk fladdermus Eptesicus nilssonii använder sig delvis av syn för att finna stora stillastående byten bland växtlighet, byten som är svåra att urskilja med hjälp av sonar. Detta trots att de har en relativt begränsad visuell upplösningsförmåga, ca 1°, vilket är ungefär 60 gånger sämre än en människas. Ekolokalisationen är utan tvekan det som gjort fladdermössen till en av de mest framgångsrika och mångskiftande däggdjursgrupperna på jorden. De har dock behållit ett funktionellt synsinne som ett viktigt komplement. De, liksom vi använder sig av så många olika sinnesintryck som möjligt för att lösa livets uppgifter.

Page 9: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

8

CONTENTS

INTRODUCTION.……………………………………………………...10

VISION IN ECHOLOCATING BATS

The microchiropteran eye………………………………………….. 12

The brain and the retinal pathways………………………………… 15

What bats can see…………………………………………………... 17

Vision in orientation and navigation……………………………….. 24

Vision in foraging and prey detection…………………………….... 27

Predator surveillance and social behaviour……………………….... 33

Multimodality – vision and echolocation………………...…….… . 34

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS....…………….……………………….…... 38

REFERENCES…………………………….…………………………… 39

PAPER I. Eklöf, J. & Jones, G. 2003. Use of vision in prey detection by brown long-eared bats Plecotus auritus. - Animal Behaviour (In Press)..… 48

PAPER II. Eklöf, J., Svensson, A. M. & Rydell, J. 2002. Northern bats (Eptesicus nilssonii) use vision but not flutter-detection when searching for prey in clutter. - Oikos 99, 347-351….…………………………………. 62

PAPER III. Rydell, J. & Eklöf, J. 2003. Vision complements echolocation in the aerial hawking northern bat (Eptesicus nilssonii) - Submitted manuscript……………………………...…. 70

PAPER IV. Eklöf, J. 2003. Visual acuity and eye size in insectivorous bats. - Manuscript………………………...…………………... 80

PAPER V. Eklöf, J., Tranefors, T. & Vázquez, L-B. 2002. Precedence of visual cues in the emballonurid bat Balantiopteryx plicata. - Mammalian Biology 67, 42-46……………………………………………………. 92

PAPER VI. Karlsson, B-L., Eklöf, J. & Rydell, J. 2002. No lunar phobia in swarming insectivorous bats (family Vespertilionidae). - Journal of Zoology London 256, 473-477….……………………………….100

Dayash
Highlight
Page 10: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

9

Page 11: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

10

B

ats (Order: Chiroptera) are among the most diverse and abundant mammals on earth and the thousand or so species comprise about one fourth of all mammalians. Bats occur throughout the world, except the Polar Regions, and show a remarkable wide range of habitat use, behaviour, morphology, and diet. Most bats feed on insects but there are also bats that feed on fruit, nectar, fish, small vertebrates, and blood. Bats are the only mammals that have evolved active flight, and they can navigate through complete darkness by using echolocation or sonar (SOund Navigation And Ranging). Bats live almost everywhere, in tropical jungles as well as in cities; they inhabit caves, trees, houses, churches, bridges, coiled banana leafs, bamboo canes, and some species even build their own tents by using large leaves. Bats have a remarkable spatial memory and are quick learners. They can form colonies of up to 20 million individuals, eat hundreds or thousands of insects every night and migrate across continents. Many bats hibernate through a cold winter and some can reach more than 40 years of age. Despite this, bats are seldom people’s number one choice of favourite animal. Instead, bats have become symbols of darkness, doom and occultism in the western world. They often appear in not so flattering contexts, such as in myths, scary movies, heavy metal lyrics, and are often one of the most important ingredients in witches’ brews. Being called an old bat is not a compliment, and having a bat in one’s belfry is not very often socially accepted. In the eastern world, however, bats are considered as symbols of fortune and a long, prosperous life. Nevertheless, the bats’ leathery wings and their ability to navigate through the night are presumably two reasons behind their often somewhat scary reputation, as well as the two main reasons behind their success as a group. But how do they perceive the world; or as Thomas Nagel (1974) put it in his classic paper: what is it like to be a bat? All information about the surrounding world is filtered through our senses and processed in our brains in order to give us just the right kind and amount of information to help us make proper decisions. This is true for all animals, although the senses receiving the information and the brains that process it differ across the animal kingdom. The type and amount of information that is needed obviously varies considerably depending on life style. What humans cannot perceive tend to be called ultra-, infra-, or extra-something. We do not know what ultraviolet light looks like, only that it gives us a nice tan. We cannot hear infrasounds although elephants can, which is why we and not the elephants invented

Dayash
Highlight
Dayash
Highlight
Page 12: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

11

the telephone. Many animals live entirely in the world of ultra-, or infra-, making it hard for us to relate to their every day life, or as Thomas Carlyle (1837) elegantly put it: “In every object there is inexhaustible meaning; the eye sees in it what the eye brings means of seeing. To Newton and to Newton’s dog Diamond, what a different pair of universes.” Bat echolocation is different from any of the senses that we are familiar with, and therefore, we cannot know or even imagine how they experience the world; or as in the words of Thomas Nagel (1974): “Anyone who has spent some time in an enclosed space with an excited bat knows what it is to encounter a fundamentally alien form of life.” With this in mind, it may seem impossible to study sensory ecology, and still, we try. We accept that there is information outside our perception range, although we will never be able to fully understand those things. We may perhaps be able to understand how a bat collects and uses information from the environment, but never what this really is like for the bat. We may however, from a human point of view, describe behaviour and reactions of animals under defined conditions. For example, when studying bats flying, and recording and describing echolocation calls, we can tell that sonar is a high precision tool, as good as vision for perceiving and identifying objects, only entirely different. But we begin to understand that echolocation alone is not enough to fully experience the world as a bat. As high frequency sounds attenuate rapidly in air, the effective range of echolocation is limited to a few metres in practice. Background echoes, known as clutter, also impose severe constraints on the use of sonar, and for a bat to perceive distant objects or objects hidden in vegetation, other senses must be used. One of these senses is vision. However, looking at a typical bat eye gives little hope of any breathtaking visual adventures. The eyes are often small and inconspicuous, especially compared to the more fanciful ears and nose-leafs of many bats, and considering our own sensory limits and the fact that bats fly at night, it is not hard to imagine why an expression like “as blind as a bat” exists. But still, bats do have eyes and perhaps “as blind as we would be if we had bat eyes” would be a more suitable expression. As I will discuss in this thesis, bats do have eyes that function for bats. In the same way humans have a sense of smell that function for humans, although a dog, or a bat for that matter, probably would not be impressed! The aim of this introductory chapter is to put my work into perspective by summarizing current knowledge of the role of vision in the lives of the echolocating bats.

Dayash
Highlight
Dayash
Highlight
Page 13: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

12

VISION IN ECHOLOCATING BATS The microchiropteran eye The eyes of Microchiroptera1 rank among the smallest in mammals (Tab 1), although there are considerable differences in both eye size and morphology across species, reflecting a great ecological diversity (Chase 1972; Hope & Bhatnagar 1979a; b; Marks 1980; Suthers & Bradford 1980; Bell & Fenton 1986; Paper IV). In general, the eyes of frugivorous and nectarivorous Microchiroptera are larger than those of insectivorous species. Bats roosting in relatively exposed sites, and those that sometimes are active in dusk- and daylight conditions such as many members of the family Emballonuridae also have relatively large eyes. Hence eye size seems to reflect how much bats are exposed to light in their daily life. Footnote 1. The Microchiroptera includes ca 800 species of echolocating bats but excludes the generally non-echolocating Megachiroptera or flying foxes, which are not considered in this thesis.

Tab 1 - Eye size in Microchiroptera in relation to taxonomic affinity and general feeding behaviour. Family & Eye ball axial Lens axial Lens radial Eye- Mean body- Species length (mm) diameter (mm) diameter (mm) weight (mg) weight (g) Vespertilionidae gleaning insectivores Plecotus auritus --- --- 1.65 7 --- 7 6 Myotis myotis 3.1 2 1.3 2 1.6 2 --- 26 6

Vespertilionidae aerial-hawking insectivores Eptesicus fuscus --- 0.77 9 0.91 9 6 4 14 4 Myotis sodalis 1.68 1 0.6 1 0.94 1 --- 7.3 8 Myotis lucifugus --- --- --- 4.4 4 10 4 Nyctophilus gouldi 1.9 5 --- --- --- 10.5 11

Myotis mystacinus --- --- 0.95 7 --- 5 6 Myotis daubentonii --- --- 1.25 7 --- 10 10 Nyctalus noctula 1.7 2 1.03 2 1.43 2 --- 27 6

Emballonuridae aerial-hawking insectivores Saccopteryx bilineata 2.6 9 1.5 9 1.8 9 10.4 9 7 9 Saccopteryx leptura --- 1.1 9 1.4 9 7.4 9 4 9 Rhynconycteris naso --- --- --- 4.6 9 3 9

Dayash
Highlight
Page 14: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

13

Tab 1 - cont

Eye ball axial Lens axial Lens radial Eye- mean body- Species length (mm) diameter (mm) diameter (mm) weight (mg) weight (g) Molossidae aerial-hawking insectivores Molossus ater --- --- --- 3.44 9 26 9 Eumops perotis 3.3 6 --- --- --- 486 Natalidae aerial-hawking insectivores Natalus tumidirostris 0.66 9 --- --- 0.6 9 6 9 Rhinolophidae flutter-detecting insectivores Rhinolophus rouxi 1.8 5 --- --- --- --- Rhinolophus hipposideros --- 0.49 9 0.68 9 --- 7 12

Megadermatidae gleaning insectivores/carnivores Macroderma gigas 7.0 5 --- --- --- 120 6

Phyllostomidae frugivores and nactarivores Carollia perspicillata 2.62 1 1.28 1 1.75 1 8.5 4 16 4 Micronycteris megalotis 3.9 9 1.9 9 2.4 9 1.04 9 6 9 Phyllostomus hastatus 3.94 1 1.95 1 2.44 1 40 3 77 3 Glossophaga soricina 2.0 9 --- --- 6.22 9 9 9 Anoura geoffroyi --- --- --- 14 3 15 3 Sturnira lilium --- 2.0 9 2.3 9 11.75 9 18 9 Vampyrops helleri --- --- --- 24.2 9 12 9 Chiroderma villosum --- 1.9 9 2.2 9 70.0 9 40 9 Artibeus jamaicensis 4.0 6 --- --- 27.4 4 38 4 Artibeus lituratus --- --- --- 30.4 9 61 9 Phyllostomidae sanguivores Desmodus rotundus 2.5 7 --- --- 11 4 29 4 Diaemus youngi --- --- --- 14.1 9 40 9 Noctilionidae piscivores Noctilio leporinus 2.1 6 --- --- --- 58 6 Mormoopidae aerial-hawking insectivores Mormoops megalophylla --- --- --- 1.8 9 16 9 Pteronotus davyi --- 0.35 9 0.7 9 1.16 9 7 9 Pteronotus parnellii 0.9 9 0.42 9 0.7 9 0.90 9 18 9

1 Suthers & Wallis 1970, 2 Suthers 1970, 3 Suthers & Bradford 1980, 4 Marks 1980, 5 Pettigrew et al. 1988, 6 Baron et al. 1996a, 7 Paper IV, 8 Thomson 1982, 9 Chase 1972, 10 Bogdanowicz 1994, 11 Grant 1991, 12 Greenway & Hutson 1990

Page 15: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

14

The microchiropteran eyes are shaped for nocturnal conditions in that they have large corneal surfaces and lenses relative to the size of the eye. They also have relatively large receptor fields, which give them good light gathering power at the expense of acuity, i.e. the ability to resolve fine spatial details (Suthers 1970; Suthers & Wallis 1970). The bat retina, which is relatively thin (91-126 µm) compared to that of voles (178 µm) and rats (198 µm), for example, consists mainly of rods, which are arranged loosely in visual streaks (Chase 1972; Marks 1980; Pettigrew et al. 1998). However, cones or at least cone like structures (receptor cells with pedicles) are present at least in the fruit-eating bats Artibeus lituratus and Phyllostomus hastatus (Phyllostomidae) and the aerial hawking insectivorous Saccopteryx bilineata, Saccopteryx leptura and Rhynconycteris naso (Emballonuridae) (Suthers 1970; Chase 1972).

Suthers and Wallis (1970) studied the eyes of two species of Vespertilionidae (Myotis sodalis and Pipistrellus subflavus) and four species of Phyllostomidae (the vampire bat Desmodus rotundus, and the fruit-eating Carollia perspicillata, Anoura geoffroyi and Phyllostomus hastatus), and concluded that the visual capabilities of all the species tested would allow the bats to see well at ranges beyond that of echolocation. Due to the more or less spherical lenses (small species tend to have more asymmetric lenses; Chase 1972), it also follows that most Microchiroptera have a short focal distance and hence a great depth of focus (Suthers & Wallis 1970). In fact, microchiropteran bats seem to be farsighted, indicating that vision is used predominantly at long ranges, which is where echolocation does not work so well.

The eye size and visual performance vary considerably between different species of Vespertilionidae. The northern bat Eptesicus nilssonii (left) has a visual acuity of ca 0.8° arc, the brown long eared-bat Plecotus auritus (middle), ca 0.5° arc, and Myotis spp. (right), 3 - 6° arc (Paper III, Paper IV).

Page 16: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

15

The brain and the retinal pathways The relative size of the internal brain structures of bats differs between insectivorous, sangivorous and carnivorous species on one hand and frugivorous and nectarivorous species on the other (Jolicoeur & Baron 1980; Barton et al. 1995; Barton & Harvey 2000). Whereas insect eating bats have enlarged echo-acoustic brain structures, fruit eating species have relatively large olfactory- and visual bulbs, clearly reflecting the different feeding strategies in the various species.

The size and lamination of the main targets of retinal projections in the brain: the superior colliculus (which transmits visual information and controls head- and eye movements) and the lateral geniculate body (a processing station on the way from the retina to the visual centre, which e.g. serves to enhance contrasts) have been studied in Artibeus, Eptesicus (Cotter 1985), Myotis (Cotter & Pentney 1979; Crowle 1980) and Pteronotus (Covey et al. 1987). Megadermatids and fruit eating phyllostomids show the thickest and most developed layers in the superior colliculus, at least in the superficial ones, which receive exclusively visual input. Also gleaning species tend to have relatively large superior colliculi. Open-air insectivorous species on the other hand, seem to have superior colliculi consisting almost entirely of the deeper layers, which receive a variety of different sensory inputs (including visual stimuli). However, some insectivorous bats, like the Emballonuridae (especially Saccopteryx and Cyttarops) have relatively large superior colliculi and resemble frugivores in this respect, although their total brain volume is smaller than in most other microchiropteran families (Baron et al. 1996b). This may perhaps reflect the fact that most emballonurid species roost in exposed sites and therefore live in bright light conditions. However, considering that the Emballonuridae form a basal clade in the phylogenetic tree, it may just as well suggest that bat ancestors had a well-developed visual system. (Simmons & Geisler 1998). The projections to the superior colliculus are similar to those of most mammals, in that they have no binocular overlap, and thus the left superior colliculus receives input only from the right eye and vice versa (Pettigrew 1986; Neuweiler 2000). In Megachiroptera and in primates, both superior colliculi receive input from both eyes, and hence these animals have better stereoscopic vision than Microchiroptera. (Interestingly the microchiropteran family Rhinolophidae, which contain highly specialized echolocators, show similarities to Megachiroptera in this respect (Reimer 1989). This may reflect phylogenetic relationship rather than visual adaptation, however (e.g. Springer et al. 2001)).

The lateral geniculate body consists of two parts, the ventral lateral geniculate, which has connections with several other brain

Page 17: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

16

Three examples of large-eyed bats: Species of the family Emballonuridae (left) have larger eyes than other insectivorous aerial-hawkers, probably reflecting an unusual visual capacity among bats. The large eyed Megaderma lyra (Megadermatidae) (middle) show a flexible hunting strategy and uses vision in combination with sonar and passive hearing. Macrotus californicus (Phyllostomidae) (right) is the only microchiropteran bat shown to be capable of catching insects using vision alone.

structures, and the dorsal lateral geniculate, which connects to the visual cortex. In most Microchiroptera, a larger proportion of the nerves are projected to the ventral side of the lateral geniculate body, suggesting that vision is important for orientation rather than for cognitive tasks (Neuweiler 2000). However, the sizes of the retinal pathways vary between genera. The nerves are generally larger in frugivores (Phyllostomus hastatus, Anoura geoffroyi, Suthers & Bradford 1980; and Artibeus jamaicensis, Cotter 1985) than in insectivores (Eptesicus fuscus, Cotter 1985; and Pteronotus parnellii, Covey et al. 1987), although, again, insectivorous Emballonuridae and Megadermatidae are exceptions. Both have relatively large visual pathways projecting through the dorsal lateral geniculate to the visual cortex. This suggests that vision is more important in these species, and they show similarities to the visually oriented Megachiroptera in this respect (Neuweiler 2000), and may reflect phylogenetic relationship (Springer et al. 2001). For a comparison of different brain structures between all groups of Microchiroptera, see Baron et al. (1996a; b; c).

Page 18: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

17

What bats can see Brightness discrimination and light sensitivity At the most basic level, vision is involved in the establishment of photoperiodic cycles, and serves to distinguish daylight from darkness. It was previously believed that this was the sole purpose of the microchiropteran eye (Eisentraut 1969 cited in Dietrich & Dodt 1970). The bat’s activity cycle is controlled by an endogenous circadian rhythm, which is synchronized with the daylight cycle by light sampling behaviour. This means that, before they emerge from the roost to feed, the bats move from the darker areas in their roosts to lighter areas near the entrance, in order to test the outdoor light level (Erkert 1982). Cloudiness and moonlight can thus affect the time of emergence. On moonlit nights, many tropical microchiropterans typically reduce their foraging activity, presumably due to increased predation risk (Morrison 1978; Usman et al. 1980; Fleming 1988) or perhaps lower availability of food (Lang et al. 2002). In contrast, bat activity at high latitudes is not influenced by moonlight to any high extent (Paper VI). On twelve nights in August-September 2000, the impact of moonlight on bat swarming activity (associated with mating season) was studied at an abandoned mine in southern Sweden. Bat activity at and near the mine entrance did not vary with moon phase, or cloud cover, suggesting that moonlight had no effect on the bats’ behaviour. It seems likely that insectivorous bats at high latitudes may not have been exposed to significant nocturnal predator pressure, leading to the evolution of lunar phobia, as many tropical bats. In contrast to high-latitude bats, the latter have to face specialized bat predators such as bat falcons (Falco rufigularis). Furthermore, high latitude bats are exposed to relatively bright light conditions throughout the summer. They do react to light, but not by decreasing their activity, instead, they fly closer to protective vegetation or sometimes high in the air (Rydell et al. 2002). This kind of behaviour is also seen in species that migrate during the day, such as the noctule, Nyctalus noctula (Ahlén 1997). Both types of behaviour may have the purpose of avoiding predatory birds (e.g. small hawks and falcons).

The ability of bats to detect small differences in brightness, i.e. brightness discrimination, was first studied by Eisentraut (1950), who found that Plecotus auritus and Eptesicus serotinus (Vespertilionidae) could easily distinguish black cards from white. Curtis (1952) trained the vespertilionids Eptesicus fuscus and Myotis lucifugus to search for food at the illuminated end of a box, and found that the bats’ ability of brightness discrimination is similar to that of rats and mice. Brightness discrimination performance in Eptesicus fuscus peaks around

Dayash
Highlight
Dayash
Highlight
Dayash
Highlight
Dayash
Highlight
Page 19: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

18

10 lux, which is equivalent to the light level prevailing at dusk and dawn, but remains good in illuminations as low as 0.001 lux, conditions which resembles darkness to a human eye adapted to low light intensity. As a comparison, a light level of 0.1 lux is equivalent to light levels at full moon, and on overcast nights the amount of light drops to 0.0001 lux (Ryer 1997). Based on focal distance and diameter of the dilated pupil, Dietrich and Dodt (1970) calculated that the light gathering power of Myotis myotis is 4-5 times that of man. This suggests that bats can readily use visual cues at dusk, when they normally emerge from their roosts, and probably also under nocturnal conditions (Ellins & Masterson 1974). As may be expected from a retina consisting predominantly of rods, the visual sensitivity generally declines as the ambient illumination increases towards daylight (Hope & Bhatnagar 1979b). This indicates that the bat eyes work better in dim light than in bright light. This has been verified behaviourally by Bradbury & Nottebohm (1969), who found that Myotis lucifugus avoids obstacles better under ambient illuminations resembling dusk, than they do in bright daylight. These findings may explain why early studies, which were made in room illumination, usually failed to prove any major visual capacity in microchiropteran bats (e.g. Eisentraut 1950; Curtis 1952).

Light tolerance has been estimated in three species of Vespertilionidae (Myotis myotis, Dietrich & Dodt 1970; Eptesicus serotinus, Bornschein 1961; and Eptesicus fuscus, Hope & Bhatnagar

Many tropical bats minimize their activity in moonlight, presumably due to predation risk. This behaviour is not found among high latitude bats (Paper VI)

Page 20: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

19

1979b) and three species of Phyllostomidae (Desmodus rotundus, Carollia perspicillata, and Artibeus jamaicensis, Hope & Bhatnagar 1979b) by measuring the luminance of light stimuli required to provoke electroretinogram responses. Among the vespertilionids, Eptesicus fuscus showed the highest light tolerance, and among the phyllostomids, which generally responded to lower luminance levels than the vespertilionids, Artibeus jamaicensis showed the highest tolerance. This presumably reflects the relative importance of vision in the different species, but perhaps more importantly the time at which these species normally emerge in the evening, and to what extent they are exposed to bright light (Hope & Bhatnagar 1979a; b). The Emballonuridae Emballonura spp. and Saccopteryx spp., some of which roost at exposed sites and often fly in daylight (Lekagul & McNeely 1977; Bradbury & Vehrencamp 1976; Kalko 1995), would thus be expected to be more light tolerant than other bats. Although, light tolerance levels have not been measured in these bats directly, the small receptive fields and the low receptor-to-ganglion ratio (ca 1:10) in Saccopteryx spp., compared to that of other microchiropteran species (ca 1:100), indicate a high light tolerance and good resolving power as expected. In fact they resemble diurnal mammals in this respect (Chase 1972). Nevertheless, the eyes of Microchiroptera work well under low ambient illumination, although the sensitivity to different light levels and the ability of brightness discrimination vary considerably between the different families and species. Spatial resolution The eyes of microchiropterans are primarily adapted to function in low light levels. This carries the disadvantage of a relative poor ability to resolve fine spatial details (acuity). The ability of spatial resolution of the bat eye can be estimated either anatomically, by calculating the density of retinal ganglion cells (Marks 1980; Pettigrew et al. 1998; Heffner et al. 2001) or behaviourally, by presenting the bats with striped patterns of different fineness (Suthers 1966; Bell & Fenton 1986; Paper IV). When the visual acuity is measured with the latter method, it is often referred to as grating acuity and is expressed as degrees of arc or as cycles per degree, where one cycle is one pair of black and white stripes. The two methods give indications of the minimum separable angles, i.e. the minimum distance between two points that an animal needs in order to separate them.

Page 21: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

20

Comparisons between the two methods should be treated carefully because the acuity values estimated by counting retinal ganglion cells tend to be higher than those estimated from behavioural studies. This suggests that the anatomical method gives a theoretical minimum, rather than an indication of what the bats actually respond to. Nevertheless, Table 2 should give an idea of the wide range of spatial resolution ability that has been documented in different species of microchiropteran bats, from the coarse vision of the small Myotis spp. (Vespertilionidae) (3-5º arc, Paper IV) to the relatively fine visual ability of Macrotus californicus (Phyllostomidae) (0.06° arc, Bell & Fenton 1986). Macrotus californicus has by far the best resolving power found in any microchiropteran bat studied so far, and is comparable to that of a dog in this respect (Heffner & Heffner 1992). It is also the only microchiropteran known to be capable of detecting insects, using vision alone (Bell 1985).

The device used for the optomotor response tests (Paper IV), in which a bat is presented with rotating, striped patterns of different fineness. The bat responds to the revolving patterns by moving its head in a stereotype manner. The thickness of the stripes corresponds to the bats visual resolving power (acuity), measured as degrees of arc.

Page 22: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

21

The visual resolving power is never a fixed value, but depends on the ambient light intensity. In the common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus, for example, the acuity drops from 0.8° arc at a light intensity of ca 310 lux to over 2° arc in ca 0.004 lux (Manske & Schmidt 1976). Other bats, such as Macrotus californicus (0.06° arc) and Antrozous pallidus (0.25° arc) retain their visual acuity down to light levels as low as ca 0.002 lux (Bell & Fenton 1986). In comparison, species of Megachiroptera, which do not echolocate, has been shown to respond to striped patterns of 0.8° in light levels of ca 0.0005 lux, whereas humans responds only to patterns of 1.3° arc under the same conditions (Neuweiler 1967). Hence, in very dim light, bats can see better than humans.

Tab 2 - Visual acuity in Microchiroptera (expressed as degrees of arc). Behavioural acuity values (b) come from optomotor response tests, and theoretical values (t) are calculated from the number of ganglion cells per unit area of the retina. Acuity is the minimum separable angle, i.e. the best values obtained for each species. Asterisks (*) indicate that the ambient light level was not measured (or that acuity was measured theoretically). For consistency, the values of visual acuity and light levels were sometimes converted from other units, used in the original paper. Light Visual Species (lux) acuity Reference Method Vespertilionidae; gleaning insectivores Antrozous pallidus 0.004 0.25° Bell & Fenton 1986 b Plecotus auritus 0.7 0.5° Paper IV b Vespertilionidae; aerial-hawking insectivores Eptesicus fuscus * 1° Bell & Fenton 1986 b Eptesicus fuscus * 0.7° Koay et al. 1998 t Eptesicus nilssonii 1-10 0.8° Paper III -

Eptesicus capensis 3600-4800 0.9° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Eptesicus zuluensis 4400 0.9° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Myotis lucifugus * 3-6° Suthers 1966 b Nyctophilus gouldi * 0.8° Pettigrew et al. 1988 t Myotis brandtii 0.1 5° Paper IV b Myotis mystacinus 0.1 5° Paper IV b Myotis daubentonii 0.1-0.3 5° Paper IV b Miniopterus screibersii 33 0.9° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Pipistrellus nanus 6400 0.9° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Pipistrellus rueppellii 3200 0.9° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Scotophilus borbonicus 40-5500 0.9° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Nycticeius schlieffeni 5000 1.5° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Emballonuridae aerial-hawking insectivores Saccopteryx bilineata * 0.5° Pettigrew et al. 1988 t Saccopteryx leptura * 0.7° Suthers 1966 b Taphozus georgianus * 0.4° Pettigrew et al. 1988 t

Page 23: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

22

Tab 2 – cont Light Visual Species (lux) acuity Reference Method Molossidae; aerial-hawking insectivores Molossus ater * 10° Chase 1972 b Tadarida pumila 81-5800 0.9° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Tadarida midas 20000 0.9° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Rhinolophidae flutter-detecting insectivores Rhinolophus rouxi * 1.4° Pettigrew et al. 1988 t Rhinolophus fumigatus 160-4800 0.9° Fenton & Portfors unpubl b Megadermatidae gleaning insectivores/carnivores Megaderma lyra * 0.3° Pettigrew et al. 1988 t Macroderma gigas * 0.3° Pettigrew et al. 1988 t Phyllostomidae frugivores and nectarivores Carollia perspicillata * 0.3° Suthers 1966 b Glossophaga soricina * 3° Chase 1972 b Anoura geoffroyi * 0.7° Suthers 1966 b Sturnira lilium * 0.3° Chase 1972 b Artibeus jamaicensis * 0.5° Heffner et al. 2001 t Artibeus cinereus * 0.4° Pettigrew et al. 1988 t Phyllostomidae sanguivores Desmodus rotundus * 0.7° Suthers 1966 b Desmodus rotundus 3.1 0.8° Manske & Schmidt 1976 b Desmodus rotundus 0.04 2.5° Manske & Schmidt 1976 b Diaemus youngi * 3° Chase 1972 b Phyllostomidae Gleaning insectivores Macrotus californicus 0.002 0.06° Bell & Fenton 1986 b Other mammals; Rattus (rat) * 0.3° Heffner & Heffner 1992 t Canis (dog) * 0.06° Heffner & Heffner 1992 t Felis (cat) * 0.045° Hughes 1977 t Macaca (macaque) * 0.01° Cowey & Ellis 1967 b Homo (man) * 0.009° Hughes 1977 t Homo (man) 0.0005 1.3° Neuweiler 1967 b

Page 24: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

23

Pattern discrimination Bats can visually distinguish patterns and shapes of objects. The nectarivorous Anoura geoffroyi (Phyllostomidae) distinguishes rectangles from solid discs of the same surface area, when trained to seek food at the discs (Suthers & Chase 1966; Suthers et al. 1969). This species is also able to distinguish outlines of erected triangles from inverted ones, as long as the baselines of the triangles are intact. However, when the bats were presented with two sides of a triangle, i.e. an outline of a triangle without a base, the shape was no longer distinguished from other shapes. This indicates that Anoura geoffroyi does not possess a concept of form, but rather perceive the relative position of horizontal lines. Similar conclusions were drawn from studies of common vampire bats Desmodus rotundus (Phyllostomidae). This species is able to separate vertical stripes but not horizontal stripes from circles of the same area (Schmidt & Manske 1978; Manske & Schmidt 1979). In contrast, the insectivorous species Vespertilio superans (Vespertilionidae) cannot distinguish objects of different shapes but equal size, and responds only to the size of the surface areas (Chung et al. 1990). The only bat that has been shown unambiguously to respond to shapes alone is the frugivorous phyllostomid Carollia perspicillata. This species can discriminate squares from circles, even if the squares are rotated (Suthers et al. 1969).

In conclusion, studies on pattern discrimination have yielded highly variable results, but in general it seems as if fruit- and nectar-eating microchiropterans respond to patterns and shapes more readily than insectivorous species. This may perhaps reflect that plants are more easily detected by vision, and less detectable by sonar than insects, and that frugivores therefore may use a different search image when foraging. Perception of colour Given that microchiropteran bats are all more or less nocturnal, true colour vision seems unlikely to occur in these animals, as it would probably be of minor importance. Nevertheless, cones occur in the retinas of some species, although most authors report only rods (reviewed by Suthers 1970; Chase 1972). Nevertheless, there is evidence that at least two different photo pigments occur in the eyes of Microchiroptera (Chase 1972; Hope & Bhatnagar 1979a). Electroretinogram response tests have shown sensitivity peaks around 500 nm and 570 nm in the vespertilionid species Myotis myotis (Dietrich & Dodt 1970) and Eptesicus fuscus (Hope & Bhatnagar 1979a) and the phyllostomid species Artibeus

Page 25: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

24

jamaicensis, Desmodus rotundus and Carollia perspicillata (Hope & Bhatnagar 1979a). There is also preliminary evidence that there is a spectral sensitivity peak in the near UV-range (around 390 nm) in the nectarivorous phyllostomid Glossophaga soricina (Lopez et al. 2001). It is thus possible that this species is able to perceive ultraviolet light reflected from fruits and plants. Vision in orientation and navigation Long distance navigation The fact that the eyes of most bats function better beyond than within the range of echolocation (Suthers & Wallis 1970) suggests that visual cues may preferably be used in preference to echolocation for navigation and orientation over longer distances.

Several species of Microchiroptera make long distance movements and some even perform seasonal migration (Griffin 1970). It seems unlikely that ultrasonic echolocation plays any major role in orientation over long distances, as it works only at short range. For example, insect sized targets can be detected a few metres away at best (Kick 1982), although trees, hillsides or the ground obviously may be detected much further away. However, even during the most favourable conditions, bats do not pay attention to echoes returning from more than 100 m or so away (Altringham 1996) and therefore, migration over long distances is almost certainly guided by other senses, including vision (Griffin 1970). Bats can use distant low frequency sounds for orientation over moderate distances, indicating that passive hearing may also be involved in navigation over longer distances (Griffin 1970; Buchler & Childs 1981). There is also some evidence that bats possess magnetic material (Buchler & Wasilewski 1985), but if they possess a magnetic sense like birds (Wiltschko & Wiltschko 1995) or not, is still unknown.

When migrating at night, it is possible that stars can serve as navigational cues for some species of bats. For example, Eptesicus fuscus can see point light sources, which simulate bright white and blue stars against the night sky, if these are located at > 6° angle (Childs & Buchler 1981). This species is also able to orient and navigate in relation to the post-sunset glow in the west (Buchler & Childs 1982).

In homing experiments with bats released within 10 km from their roost, the bats have been demonstrated to do well, using echolocation alone. This suggests that they are acoustically familiar with a relatively large home territory (Williams et al. 1966; Williams & Williams 1967; Davis & Barbour 1970). Nevertheless, blinded bats tend

Page 26: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

25

to fly slower and closer to the ground than non-treated bats (Mueller 1968), indicating that they change their orientation behaviour when they no longer are able to see. Bats also seem to rely heavily on their spatial memory, as they often follow the same paths night after night (Höller 1995). When commuting between roost- and feeding sites during dusk and dawn periods, bats often follow outlines in the landscape, such as river banks, forest edges, hedgerows and hillsides (Racey & Swift 1985; Limpens & Kapteyn 1991; Verboom & Huitema 1997). The reason may be to minimise predation risk (Swift 1998), or to use outlines as acoustic landmarks, which perhaps facilitate navigation by sonar (Verboom et al. 1999). More likely, however, landscape outlines and silhouettes provide the bats with visual cues, contrasting against the twilight sky, and such cues are probably essential for orientation and navigation along travelling routes (Davis 1966; Layne 1967; Griffin 1970; Manske & Schmidt 1979; Höller and Schmidt 1996). The frequent observation that bats have a tendency to crash into windows of buildings when released indoors (Fenton 1975), during migration (Timm 1988), or commuting (Test 1967), suggests that they predominantly rely on vision rather than on echolocation in situations when both acoustic and visual cues are available. The performance is greatly improved, i.e. there are fewer collisions, when the bats are blinded

Balantiopteryx plicata (Emballonuridae) relies on visual cues when presented with conflicting information from vision and sonar, for example in front of a window (Paper V).

Page 27: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

26

(Davis & Barbour 1965) or when they are flown under dark conditions, and hence are “forced” to rely on echolocation alone. The insectivorous Balantiopteryx plicata (Emballonuridae) was studied at different times of the day in an empty mesh greenhouse (Paper V). At night they flew smoothly and could easily avoid the ceiling and the walls of the greenhouse, but during the day and at dusk and dawn they often tried to fly through the mesh and thereby crashed into it. The bats used echolocation consistently and without any dramatic change in echolocation call structure that could be related to the prevailing light conditions. The study indicates that emballonurid bats trust their eyes over their ears when exposed to contradictory auditory and visual cues. Close range orientation and navigation When moving towards resting places and specific sites within roosts, bats sometimes face extremely unfavourable conditions for orientation, such as darkness, acoustic clutter from the walls of the roost, and simultaneous echolocation calls from many individuals. It is therefore likely that arrays of different sensory cues are used in such situations, and also that a good spatial memory is of great importance (Höller & Schmidt 1996). When introduced in a dark flight cage, Nyctophilus spp. (Vespertilionidae) ceased to echolocate after 6-8 hours of flight (Grant 1991), suggesting that they can learn to orient inside the cage, using spatial memory alone. In the same way, Megaderma lyra (Megadermatidae) remembers the positions of narrow openings with an accuracy of 2 cm, and if an obstacle is removed from the flight path, the bats may continue to avoid that position for days (Neuweiler & Möhres 1966). However, bats do not trust their spatial memory exclusively, but can compare stored data with new echo-acoustical and visual information (Joermann et al. 1988; Schmidt et al. 1988; Höller 1995). When flying in a room of subdued daylight, the two frugivores Carollia perspicillata and Phyllostomus hastatus (Phyllostomidae) are able to see and avoid obstacles consisting of 30 cm wide strips of cloth in their flight path (Chase & Suthers 1969). Those that were deafened with earplugs avoided the obstacles significantly better than those that were both deafened and blindfolded, showing that they could obtain visual information of features in the environment during flight. These results are consistent with those of Bradbury and Nottebohm (1969), who found that Myotis lucifugus (Vespertilionidae) avoided collisions in a string maze better in dim light than in total darkness. Rother and Schmidt (1982) noted that Phyllostomus discolor (Phyllostomidae) uses fewer sonar pulses in adequate illumination than in darkness. When flying the bats in a string maze, the same authors also

Dayash
Highlight
Dayash
Highlight
Dayash
Highlight
Page 28: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

27

showed that fewer pulses were used if the obstacles exceeded 0.25 mm in width. The results suggest that vision can shorten the bats’ reaction time for avoiding obstacles in a flight path, as long as there is enough ambient light and the obstacles are of sufficient size (given by the visual acuity threshold and the range).

Joermann et al. (1988) studied landing performance in two captive species of Phyllostomidae (Desmodus rotundus and Phyllostomus discolor). The bats were presented with visual illusions of landing grids, which thus gave them conflicting acoustic and visual information. Although the grids were not detectable by echolocation, the bats seemed to aim for them, and only ca 30 cm in front of the illusions the bats interrupted the approach and turned away. The authors concluded that bats rely mainly on echo-acoustical cues at close range, but in some situations they defer to visual cues in an early phase of detection, even within the range of echolocation.

To investigate what sense the Anoura geoffroyi (Phyllostomidae) (Chase 1981; 1983) and the Tadarida brasiliensis (Molossidae) (Mistry 1990) would defer to when escaping from a roost, the bats were flown in a Y-maze, in which one exit was blocked with Plexiglas and illuminated with a light source. The other exit was open but dark. When tested in daytime, nearly all bats chose the illuminated “exit”, thus indicating that they believed the light was an opening. However, when releasing bats at night, the escape behaviour was the opposite, the bats choosing the dark exit. It was suggested that the synchrony of light schedules to the bats’ circadian rhythm might determine the use of the appropriate sense (Mistry 1990).

Vision in foraging and prey detection At close range, echolocation usually gives more detailed information about the prey than vision (Suthers & Wallis 1970; Pettigrew 1980). However, in some situations, it may be favourable to change the modality with which to search for prey, and indeed, many bats use a variety of sensory cues, including smell (Hessel & Schmidt 1994; Kalko et al. 1996; Helversen et al. 2000), passive listening for prey generated sounds (Fiedler 1979; Ryan & Tuttle 1987; Arlettaz et al. 2001), tactile information (Baron et al. 1996c), visual cues (Bell 1985), and vampire bats possess the ability of thermo-perception (Kürten & Schmidt 1982).

Page 29: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

28

Insectivores and carnivores For bats that search for insects within or near vegetation, separation of prey echoes from the background clutter is usually a severe problem when using sonar alone (Jensen et al. 2001). In such situations bats have to rely on additional sensory cues to locate the prey. Nevertheless, few studies have addressed the obvious possibility that visual cues may be used for detection of prey in acoustically complex environments. However, when northern bats (Eptesicus nilssonii) search for stationary targets among high grass (clutter), this seems indeed to be the case (Paper II, Paper III). During early summer in Sweden, ghost swift moths Hepialus humuli (Lepidoptera: Hepialidae) swarm in stationary display flight over and among grass at dusk. These moths are large (ca 6 cm wingspan) and conspicuously silvery white (Andersson et al. 1998), and in contrast to most other moths, they lack ultrasonic hearing (Rydell 1998), and are intensively exploited by northern bats patrolling in the air over the field (Andersson et al. 1998; Rydell 1998; Jensen et al. 2001). In an experimental set-up, making use of this natural foraging situation, Hepialus humuli were presented to the bats, either with their white dorsal side up or with their dark ventral side up. It was found that the white moths were attacked more frequently than the dark ones, indicating that the bats were guided by visual cues (Paper II).

The aerial hawking northern bat, Eptesicus nilssonii (Vespertilionidae), uses visual cues as a complement to echolocation when searching for moths in acoustically complex environments (Paper II, III).

Page 30: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

29

The brown long-eared bat Plecotus auritus (Vespertilionidae) is a gleaning insectivore, which usually uses its large and sensitive ears to passively locate its prey by the noise they make (Anderson & Racey 1991). However Plecotus auritus also has relatively big eyes (Cranbrook 1963, Tab 1), suggesting that they have relatively good vision. We investigated if brown long-eared bats exploit visual cues when searching for prey (Paper I). By using petri dishes, containing mealworms that either were available to the bats or presented under glass, and presenting these in different levels of illumination, we provided the bats with visual cues, sonar cues or both. The bats did best in situations where both sonar cues and visual cues were available, but the visual information seemed to be more important than sonar. The California leaf-nosed bat Macrotus californicus (Phyllostomidae), a gleaner that normally searches for prey on the ground, has been shown to locate prey by using auditory- and visual cues as well as by sonar. Indeed this bat shows a particularly flexible hunting behaviour. In moonlight Macrotus californicus can see well enough to hunt using vision alone (Bell 1985). This allows the bat to hunt without alerting the prey with ultrasound (Fullard 1987; Rydell 1992a), and also to detect stationary targets, which otherwise would be hard to detect (Arlettaz et al. 2001; Jensen et al. 2001; Paper II). In visual acuity tests Macrotus californicus responded to stripes subtending 0.06° arc, (Tab 2), which is the best visual acuity found in any microchiropteran bat (Bell & Fenton 1986).

Gleaning brown long-eared bats, Plecotus auritus (Vespertilionidae), feeding from bowls presenting different sensory cues, seem to prefer visual information to sonar cues. (Paper I).

Page 31: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

30

Moreover, the eyes of Macrotus californicus are relatively large and have a much higher degree of binocular overlap (50°) than in other bats (for example Antrozous pallidus 25° and Eptesicus fuscus 19°, Bell & Fenton 1986). This suggests that Macrotus californicus has good stereoscopic vision and that the near field distance perception is of great importance (McIlwain 1996), as would be expected in a species that forage visually. Macrotus californicus exploits diurnal prey, that are stationary at night and therefore unavailable to other bats (e.g. Howell 1920 cited in Bell & Fenton 1986).

The African yellow-winged bat Lavia frons (Megadermatidae) employs feeding tactics that involve both gleaning and aerial hawking. This species is a sit-and-wait predator, which scans the vicinity while hanging from a branch, waiting for insects to pass by. Lavia frons is active in relative bright ambient illumination, at dusk as well as late mornings, and is often seen catching prey against the sky. It has large eyes and may be able to see insects against the bright sky (Vaughan & Vaughan 1986). Nyctophilus gouldi and Nyctophilus geoffroyi (Vespertilionidae), also combine aerial hawking with gleaning, and have been shown to use different sensory cues according to circumstances. As in Lavia frons, visual cues are preferentially used to detect prey in the air, whereas auditory cues are used to detect prey on the ground (Grant 1991). The visual acuity of Nyctophilus gouldi is nowhere near that of Macrotus californicus and Antrozous pallidus, but rather similar to that of other aerial hawking Vespertilionidae (Tab 2), which explains why they cannot find prey on the ground visually.

Eklöf & Anderson (unpublished) observed northern bats (Eptesicus nilssonii, Vespertilionidae) feeding under midnight sun conditions in northern Norway. The bats caught prey against the bright sky and sometimes without detectable sonar signals. However, based on the performance of Eptesicus fuscus (Tab 2) it seems unlikely that Eptesicus nilssonii has sufficient resolving power to detect small airborne prey visually. A 2 cm insect is first detected at a distance of ca 1 m using vision (considering a visual acuity of 0.7° -1° arc, Tab 2), but the same object is first detected at ca 5 m using echolocation (Kick 1982), which thus suggests that echolocation would be the preferred sense. On the other hand, when northern bats search for ghost swift moths (described above), vision increases the chance of detection of the prey, only because they exceed 5 cm in wingspan and are detected at rather close range (3.5 m) (Paper III). Smaller targets are detected using echolocation alone.

Little brown bats (Myotis lucifugus) have been observed to catch prey apparently without using echolocation (D. R. Griffin personal comm.) This species’ visual resolving power is even poorer than that of the northern bat, and in addition, its prey items are even smaller,

Page 32: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

31

so it is thus highly unlikely that vision is involved in prey catching. In this species the apparent absence of echolocation calls must have another explanation. In fact, earlier observations of northern bats (Rydell 1992b) and little brown bats (Rydell et al. 2002) have suggested that attempted insect captures are always associated with echolocation calls, even in bright light conditions at high latitudes.

Under conditions that appear to us to be completely dark (0 lux), bats may still be able to see conspicuous insects. For example, it has been reported that bat activity is high where fireflies occur (Lloyd 1989), and it has been shown that some fireflies stop flashing when approached by bats (Farnworth 1973). This suggests that the light emitted by fireflies may guide the bats or at least evoke their curiosity. More interestingly, fireflies are not eaten by bats and were rejected by Eptesicus fuscus in feeding experiments (Vernon 1981). In the same study, the bats were presented with flashing fireflies as well as with artificial flashes. The bats responded to the flashes, although it was not clear if they associated the flashes with food or with unpalatability. It seems possible that firefly flashes may function as a visual aposematic signal to bats.

Frugivores and nectarivores In general, fruit- and nectar feeding bats have larger eyes (Tab 1), better visual resolving power (Tab 2) and enlarged visual and olfactory bulbs, compared to insectivorous species (Jolicoeur & Baron 1980; Barton et al. 1995; Barton & Harvey 2000). They also perceive and respond to different patterns more readily than insectivorous species (Suthers & Chase 1966; Suthers et al. 1969), suggesting that vision may perhaps play a more important role in these bats than in most insectivores.

Hessel and Schmidt (1994) investigated which sensory cues Carollia perspicillata (Phyllostomidae) uses when orienting toward a food source. The bats were presented with a triple choice of passive acoustic-, olfactory-, and visual cues. At least initially, the visual cue was the most frequently preferred stimulus. But after training the bats changed their behaviour and responded more to the olfactory stimulus. The experiment suggests that Carollia perspicillata can detect new sources of food using visual cues, and that they subsequently rely more on olfaction as the food source becomes known. Indeed these bats seem to possess a remarkable sense of olfaction (Fleming 1988; Laska 1990). Kalko et al. (1996) showed that fig eating Microchiroptera do not use vision when foraging, presumably because figs eaten by these bats are visually inconspicuous. Instead, they rely mainly on olfactory cues,

Page 33: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

32

combined with broadband echolocation. In fact, most bat-pollinated plants are greenish, pink and white, which presumably reflect the fact that bats are most likely colour-blind (Suthers 1970; Faegri & van der Pijl 1979). On the contrary, many species of bat pollinated Parkia (Leguminosae: Mimosoideae) have bright red and yellow colours (Hopkins 1984). It is also suggested that dark flowers can be seen as silhouettes, against the sky and that pale flowers appear conspicuous against dark foliage (Start 1974, cited in Hopkins 1984). If the bats make use of such differences in contrast, one would expect to find that the position of differently coloured flowers vary accordingly in relation to the foliage, i.e., red flowers far from foliage and yellow flowers closer, which in fact, seems to be the case.

The capitula of Parkia are also highly reflective under moonlight and starlight conditions, and are therefore presumably visible to pollinating bats (Hopkins 1984). Many pollinators make use of a broad spectrum reflected from flowers, fruits or seeds, including ultraviolet (UV) light (for example insects, Kevan et al. 2001; and birds, Church et al. 2001). Ultraviolet vision seems, however, to be absent in most mammals, although some rodents have been shown to have UV sensitive retinas (Jacobs et al. 1991). Recently, it was suggested that bats might perceive UV-light, as there is evidence for a spectral sensitivity peak around 390 nm (i.e. in the near UV-range) in the nectarivorous Glossophaga soricina (Lopez et al. 2001). However, if the bats actually use UV reflecting surfaces as orienting cues is still uncertain, although Willson and Whelan (1989) have shown that UV-reflectance is indeed relatively common throughout the plant kingdom. The Passiflora species, Passiflora galbana and Passiflora mucronata, two plants which flowers are exploited by the nectarivorous glossophaginae bats, reflect light down to ca 400 nm and 370 nm (upper UV range), respectively. This should be compared to the hummingbird pollinated Passiflora speciosa, which has its main reflection above 570 nm (Varassin et al. 2001), perhaps reflecting the spectral sensitivity of the pollinators. Furthermore, 80% of nocturnal Lepidoptera have wing patterns that reflect UV, compared to ca 30% in diurnal species (Lyytinen 2001 cited in Honkavaara et al. 2002). On the other hand, this may imply that bats cannot make use of the ultraviolet light, in contrast to birds, which usually forage in daylight.

Page 34: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

33

Predator surveillance and social behaviour As discussed earlier, vision seems to be important in escape behaviour (Chase 1981; Chase 1983; Mistry 1990). Presumably it is also important in detection of predators; it is much easier to approach a blindfolded bat than a non-blindfolded individual (Chase 1972). Species of the family Emballonuridae often fly earlier in the evening than most other bats, and sometimes even in the afternoon and they often roost on exposed and well lit sites such as tree trunks (e.g. Bradbury & Vehrencamp 1976). A Saccopteryx sp. will quite easily detect an approaching person, and take flight without emitting any echolocation calls (Suthers 1970), and Rhynconycteris naso seems to be disturbed more easily by seeing an approaching figure at a distance, than by sudden sounds or vibrations at close range (Dalquest 1957). Vaughan and Vaughan (1986) noted that Lavia frons (Megadermatidae), which also roosts exposed, seems to be constantly alert during the day, scanning its surroundings for predators. In fact, the authors almost never saw a bat with its eyes closed, and were never able to approach one undetected.

The evidence for the use of vision in social behaviour is mainly anecdotal. Social grooming occurs in the vampire Desmodus rotundus (Phyllostomidae) and may serve to identify individuals (Wilkinson 1986), although it is generally rare (Fleming 1988). Goodwin and Greenhall (1961) noted that avian vampire bats (Diaemus youngi) show grooming behaviour when seeing a mirror reflection, indicating that vision might be involved in this behaviour. Sometimes bats are also observed to imitate other individuals grooming themselves (Vaughan & Vaughan 1986).

Some bat species have distinct fur patterns, which may serve as visual recognition signals (Fenton 2001), in addition to scents and sound, although fur patterns may also serve as camouflage (Neuweiler 2000). Threat displays are common in for example Carollia perspicillata (Phyllostomidae), and includes wing shaking, harsh sounds, and aggressive looks such as extension of the tongue (Fleming 1988). Sexual displays are also common. The monogamous Lavia frons and Cardioderma cor (Megadermatidae), perform stereotypical circular flights, described as aerial ballets (McWilliam 1987; Vaughan & Vaughan 1986). Among Saccopteryx bilineata (Emballonuridae), the males defend territories where they maintain harems. In front of the females of the harem, they perform sexual displays, which include stereotyped singing, and also shaking of wings and hovering. The wing shaking presumably enhances the effect of olfactory glands by spreading pheromones, but it may also function as a visual signal to draw the females’ attention (Chase 1972).

Page 35: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

34

Multimodality – vision and echolocation The echolocation detection range of a 19 mm insect is around 5 m for Eptesicus fuscus (Kick 1982), and the visual acuity of this species is 0.7°-1° arc, Tab 2). This allows visual detection of the 19 mm object only when it is closer than ca 1 m. This simple calculation strongly suggests that echolocation is the more accurate sense at close range and for small objects. However, larger objects can be detected visually at distances of hundreds of meters, far beyond the range of echolocation. For example, an object of 5 m diameter can potentially be detected visually by Eptesicus fuscus at a distance of ca 300 m. Using echolocation; the same object is detected at a distance of only 25-30 m at most (depending on call strength, attenuation etc., Lawrence and Simmons 1982; M. B. Fenton personal comm.). This supports the general view that vision is used primarily for detection of large objects and landmarks and for navigating over longer distances (Davis 1966; Layne 1967; Griffin 1970; Höller and Schmidt 1996). Nevertheless, for bats with better visual resolving power, vision can be used and even replace echolocation, at short distances. The California leaf nosed bat Macrotus californicus, referred to above, can visually detect a 19 mm insect at a distance of ca 18 m. This presumably gives this bat a longer range of operation if they use vision instead of echolocation, at least under conditions of moonlight or bright starlight (Bell & Fenton 1986). Other bats, such as some Emballonuridae, which have visual acuities below 0.4° arc (Tab 2), can visually detect insect sized objects (1 cm) at distances less than 1 m, suggesting a range of operation roughly similar for vision as for sonar. One could therefore assume that emballonurid bats could use either vision or echolocation to detect prey, as suggested by Pettigrew (1980). He observed one species of Emballonuridae (Craseonycteris thonglongyai) catching prey against a bright sky apparently without using echolocation and suggested that the bats could see the insects as silhouettes against the sky.

The Australian ghost bat Macroderma gigas (Megadermatidae) also has a similar prey detection range for vision as for echolocation. Since this species also has good auditory sensitivity in the sonic range (Fiedler 1979; Kulzer et al. 1984), it switches between vision, echolocation and passive listening (Pettigrew et al. 1986; Pettigrew et al. 1983). For frequencies below 20 kHz, the acoustic axis (as defined from the directionality of the pinna and noseleaf) of Macroderma gigas is aligned with the visual axis (defined by areas of highest ganglion cell density), indicating that auditory cues help the bats to visually detect the source of the sound (Pettigrew 1988). In fact, a major function of sound localisation in animals is to direct the eyes toward the sound-source

Page 36: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

35

(Heffner & Heffner 1992; Heffner et al. 1999). This reflex is even quicker than the reaction to a flashlight (Whittington et al. 1981), and hence suggests that hearing is closely co-ordinated with vision (Heffner 1997).

Sound localisation acuity is related to retinal organisation and the width of fields of best vision (defined as the portion of the retina with at least 75% of maximum ganglion cell density, Heffner et al. 2001). Animals with narrow fields of best vision (foveae) have generally better localisation acuity than animals with broad or elongated fields of best vision (visual streaks). The retinas of microchiropteran bats are loosely arranged in visual streaks and the density of ganglion cells falls irregularly toward the periphery. The field of best vision is concentrated in the temporal part of the retina and seems to be broader in frugivores than in insectivorous species (Heffner et al. 2001). Overall, there is a higher ganglion cell density in the inferior part of the retina than in the superior (Marks 1980; Pettigrew et al. 1988; Koay et al. 1998; Heffner et al. 2001; Eklöf unpublished). This means that the sharpest image on the retina results from light reaching the eye from above, and consequently, the bat eyes focus slightly upwards. Without moving their heads, bats are looking up (Pettigrew 1988). The functional significance of this can be difficult to establish, but it seems likely that vision and echolocation have evolved to provide the bat with as little information overlap as possible. While echolocation call emission and hearing is most effective in the flight direction and downwards (Schnitzler & Grinnell 1977; b), vision serves as a complement by being most effective upwards it thus gives additional information of obstacles and landmarks further away. In Megachiroptera, which do not echolocate, one would thus expect the fields of best vision to be above rather than below the optic disk, which in fact seems to be the case (Pettigrew 1986).

All bats have well developed retinofugal projections (pathways of information from retina to visual cortex) to the lateral geniculate nuclei as well as to the superior colliculus (see above), which are the main targets for retinal projections in mammals (Pentney & Cotter 1976; Suthers & Bradford 1980; Reimer 1989). In the superior colliculus, different sensory modalities are integrated and transformed, and the output may be perceived as a “new product” (Stein & Meredith 1993). The capacity to deal with multisensory information is however developed first after experience of multimodal inputs (Wallace & Stein 2001). The superior colliculus controls for example eye movements, which serves to keep objects of interest in the focal field. Auditory projections to the superior colliculus are generally sparse in mammals. However, in the mustache bat Pteronotus parnellii (Mormoopidae), at least three areas in the brain stem contribute with well-developed auditory projections to the

Page 37: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

36

superior colliculus. It has been shown that pinna movements can be controlled in the same way as eye movements in other mammals (Covey et al. 1987), and thus that orienting behaviour can be influenced through auditory as well as through visual feedback. It is also known that auditory stimuli can trigger visuomotor neurones and hence that the eyes can respond to sounds (Stein & Meredith 1993). Combined sensory inputs can enhance perception and detection, but also cause behavioural depression, for example when the cues are contradictory, as in the case with bats and windows (discussed above). Cats have been shown to respond “half way” between contradicting sounds and images (Stein & Meredith 1993), but in most cases when animals have multiple cues to choose from, one can see a clear sensory hierarchy (e.g. Dyer & Gould 1981), so also in bats (Chase 1983). However, the hierarchy can change depending on the behavioural context. Visual cues have been shown to have precedence over auditory cues in for example escape behaviour and when commuting (Chase 1981; Chase 1983; Mistry 1990; Paper V). In cases where echolocation and visual cues are complementary rather than contradictory, the bats may still rely on vision over sonar. In a study on brown long-eared bats (Plecotus auritus), feeding from bowls presenting different sensory cues (Paper I), the bats scored best in situations where both visual and sonar cues were present. The visual information seemed however to be the more important.

It has been suggested that there sometimes can be interference between the two senses. For example, Simmons (cited in Chase 1981) has noted that some bats have a problem learning acoustic discrimination when visual cues are present, but can easily perform the same task in darkness. When trained to respond to black or white triangles of different size, Myotis lucifugus (Vespertilionidae) responded to brightness cues rather than the size of the triangles, although these bats are capable of size discrimination by echolocation (Ellins 1970; Masterson & Ellins 1974). This suggests that interference may have occurred, or at least that the bats had a preference for visual cues in this case.

It is not yet known if bats can perform cross-modal recognition, i.e. learning an object using one sense and then immediately recognising the same object by using another sense, which is the case with for example bottle nose dolphins Tursiops truncatus. These animals can integrate information from vision to echolocation just as well as from echolocation to vision. Hence, what the dolphins perceive from one sense is functionally similar of what it perceives from the other (Pack & Herman 1995). Although the question of how sensory inputs are combined in bats remains unsolved, several authors have shown the importance of

Page 38: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

37

multimodality (Pettigrew et al. 1983; Schmidt 1988; Hessel & Schmidt 1994). In a two choice test, two phyllostomid bats (Desmodus rotundus and Phyllostomus discolor) were trained to respond to a combination of visual, olfactory and acoustic stimuli, and were then presented with one of the three modalities separately (Schmidt et al. 1988). It was found that Phyllostomus discolor chose the visual stimuli to a higher degree, whereas Desmodus rotundus preferred the passive acoustic stimuli. However, both bats were able to respond to all three modalities, although responses to the olfactory stimuli needed additional training, as also noted by Hessel and Schmidt (1994), when studying Carollia perspicillata. However when the Carollia had learned to respond to the olfactory cue, this became the preferred stimuli, which was not the case with Desmodus or Phyllostomus, which both used two other senses. This clearly shows that bats use an array of different senses in the field, and that ecology, feeding strategies and behavioural context all influence the use of different modalities.

Echolocation may be the most important innovation throughout bat evolution, allowing these animals to explore a niche of their own. But there is more to the sensory ecology of microchiropteran bats, where vision is an important piece of the puzzle and certainly needs further attention in the future.

Page 39: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

38

Acknowledgements First of all I would like to acknowledge all the co-authors of this thesis for obvious reasons, and I wish to thank Olof Helje for making the splendid bat illustrations. Then I wish to thank my supervisor and mentor -Jens Rydell – and also, many thanks to the rest of the Rydell family for your great hospitality. There are several people having answered several more or less stupid questions on bats, vision, statistics, experimental design, the meaning of life and other various topics throughout the years. Especially I would like to thank Donald R Griffin and Brock Fenton, but also Eric Warrant, Dan E Nilsson, Tom, Gareth Jones, Susan Swift, Kristina Mieziewska, Winston Lancaster; and of course, many thanks to Cajsa, John G, Christoffer, Gim, Jen, Christin, John R, Marc, Jenny, Kalle, Kristina, Cess, Bomull, Tobias, Annika, Christoffer and Staffan, just to mention a few of you. Just as many people have helped me to make the every day work possible; at the department, by joining me in the field, on conferences, courses and work shops and to some extent even in the lab (although some may think I do not know what that is); having helped me arrange field work and experiments, being guides, eye suppliers, hosts, or just good company during batting. Especially I wish to thank Monica, Tompa and Cajsa, thanks also to Maria, Héctor, Henke, Luis-Bernardo, Hans, Marie, Gabriela, Stefan, Åsa, Per, Britt-Louise, Anna, Lee, Dr. F-Jo, Blomman, Karl-Johan, Stefan, Sean, Jorge, Jenny, Andreas, Eric, Lars-Erik, Cess, Annemarie, Magnus, Berndt, Dave, Anne-Sofie, Bengt, Mia, Lilioth, the “NASBR and Chamela-students”, the Lövhaugs, all other department employees not mentioned, and of course Mexican hospitality and British humour. For unknown reasons, I have been deeply involved, not only in research and teaching, but also in the work of the faculty board, the Swedish Association of Scientists and the Students’ Union. I wish to thank the various members of all the different working groups and committees, not at least Cajsa, Marie, Stefan Henrik, and Andreas. There are other things but science, like having almost normal conversations, sharing stupid ideas, trying to do music, lying on beaches, e-mailing, having coffee and drinking beer, and presumably some other stuff as well. For those things, I wish to thank Per and Andreas for helping me to create “PSL” which for a while brought order to my life, much in the same way as “Johnny” did, only different. I wish to thank the e-mailers, the floorballers, the chatters, the Herb Boys crew and fan club, the coffee drinkers and the travellers. There is no doubt that the Friday after work sessions have been almost as important as the actual research for being able to finish this thesis. I wish to acknowledge the most frequent ones: Christoffer, Anna, Viktoria, Ågot and lately Jenny P. But of course, Sara, Erik, Fredrik, Jenny T, Linda, Tove, Goran and a whole bunch of other you-know-who-you-are. There are two persons having shared my biology- as well as my non biology-time, to a larger extent than perhaps any others: first of all, thank you Cajsa for not bringing your calculator; and for numerous moments thereafter, and second, Tompa, for making everyday April fools day. Thank you all on the second floor: Per, Bengt, Stig, Gunnar, Anders, Inger, Lena, Åke, Jan, Sebbe, Björn, Jenny, Malin, Urban, Anna H, Anna Z, Monica, Susanne, Ulla, Mare, Christoffer, Arne, Marcus, other hangarounds, not mentioned, past and present. Thank you mom, dad, and Kristian, and thanks to all friends in the real world. My work has been funded by Kungliga och Hvitfeldtska Stiftelsen, Lunds Djurskyddsfond, Knut och Alice Wallenbergs Stiftelse, Folke Eklöf, Adlerbertska Forskningsstiftelsen, Wilhelm och Martina Lundgrens Vetenskapsfond, Kungliga vetenskaps- och Vitterhetssamhället i Göteborg, Stiftelserna Paul och Marie Berghaus Donationsfond, J A Ahlstrands Testamentsfond, Lars Hiertas Minne, and Rådman & Fru Ernst Collianders Stiftelse FVÄ, and of course CSN. Finally I wish to acknowledge (please fill out your name); believe me you are not really forgotten.

Page 40: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

39

References Ahlén, I. 1997. Migratory behaviour of bats at south Swedish coasts. Z. Säugetierk. 62, 375-380 Altringham, J. D. 1996. Bats: biology and behaviour. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK Anderson, M. E. & Racey, P. A. 1991. Feeding behaviour of captive long eared-bats, Plecotus auritus. Anim. Behav. 42, 489-493 Andersson, S., Rydell, J. & Svensson, M. G. E. 1998. Light, predation and the lekking behaviour of the ghost swift Hepialus humuli (L.) (Lepidoptera: Hepialidae). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 1345-1351 Arlettaz, R., Jones, G. & Racey, P. A. 2001. Effect of acoustic clutter on prey detection by bats. Nature 414, 742-745 Baron, G., Stephan, H. & Frahm, H. D. 1996a. Comparative neurobiology in Chiroptera vol. I Macromorphology, brain structures, tables and atlases. Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel, Switzerland Baron, G., Stephan, H. & Frahm, H. D. 1996b. Comparative neurobiology in Chiroptera vol. II Brain characteristics in taxonomic units. Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel, Switzerland Baron, G., Stephan, H. & Frahm, H. D. 1996c. Comparative neurobiology in Chiroptera vol. III Brain characteristics in functional systems, ecoethological adaptation, adaptive radiation and evolution. Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel, Switzerland Barton, R. A., Purvis, A. & Harvey, P. H. 1995. Evolutionary radiation of visual and olfactory brain systemes in primates, bats and insectivores. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 348, 381-392 Barton, R. A. & Harvey, P. H. 2000. Mosaic evolution of brain structure in mammals. Nature 405, 1055-1058 Bell, G. P. 1985. The sensory basis of prey location by the California leaf-nosed bat Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 16, 343-347 Bell, G. P. & Fenton, M. B. 1986. Visual acuity, sensitivity and binocularity in a gleaning insectivorous bat, Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Anim. Behav. 34, 409-414 Bogdanowicz, W. 1994. Myotis daubentonii. Mammalian Species 475, 1-9 Bornschein, H. 1961. Vergleichende Elektrophysiologie der Retina. In: Das Visuelle System. Neurophysiologie und Psychophysik (Jung, R. & Kornhuber, H. eds.). Berlin, Springer-Verlag pp 74-79 Bradbury, J. & Nottebohm, F. 1969. The use of vision by the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus, under controlled conditions. Anim. Behav. 17, 480-485 Bradbury, J. W. & Vehrencamp, S. L. 1976. Social Organization and Foraging in Emballonurid Bats I. Field studies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1, 337-381 Buchler, E. R. & Childs, S. B. 1981. Orientation to distant sounds by foraging big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus). Anim. Behav. 29, 428-432 Buchler, E. R. & Childs, S. B. 1982. Use of the post-sunset glow as an orientation cue by big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus). J. Mammal. 63, 243-247

Page 41: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

40

Buchler, E. R. & Wasilewski, P. J. 1985. Magnetic remanence in bats. In: Magnetite Biomineralization and Magnetoreception in Organisms: A New Biomagnetism. (Kirschvink, J. L., Jones, D. S. and MacFadden, B. J. eds.). New York: Plenum press, pp 483-487 Chase, J. 1972. The role of vision in echolocating bats. Ph.D thesis, Indiana University Chase, J. 1981.Visually guided escape responses of microchiropteran bats. Anim. Behav. 29, 708- 713 Chase, J. 1983. Differential responses to visual and acoustic cues during escape in the bat Anoura geoffroyi: cue preferences and behaviour. Anim. Behav. 31, 526-531 Chase, J. & Suthers, R. A. 1969. Visual obstacle avoidance by echolocating bats. Anim. Behav. 17, 201-207 Childs, S. B. & Buchler, E. R. 1981. Perception of simulated stars by Eptesicus fuscus (Vespertilionidae): A potential navigational mechanism. Anim. Behav. 29, 1028-1035 Chung, K. S., Lee, J. H. & Park, S. R. 1990. Visual pattern discrimination in the Oriental discoloured bat, Vespertilio superans. Korean J. Zool. 33, 127-132 Church, S. C., Merrison, A. S. L. & Chamberlain, T. M. M. 2001. Avian ultraviolet vision and frequency dependent seed preferences. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 2491-2498 Cotter, J. R. 1985. Retinofugal Projections of the Big Brown Bat, Eptesicus fuscus and the neotropical Fruit Bat, Artibeus jamaicensis. Am. J. Anat. 172, 105-124 Cotter, J. R. & Pentney, R. P. 1979. Retinofugal projections of nonecholocating (Pteropus giganteus) and echolocating (Myotis lucifugus) bats. J. Comp. Neurol. 184, 381-399 Covey, E., Hall, W. C. & Kobler, J. B. 1987. Subcortical connections of the Superior colliculus in the Mustache bat, Pteronotus parnellii. J. Comp. Neurology 263, 179-197 Cowey, A. & Ellis, C. M. 1967. Visual acuity of rhesus and squirrel monkeys. J. Comp. Physiol.Psychol. 64, 80-84 Cranbrook, The Earl of. 1963. Notes on the feeding habits of the long-eared bat. Trans. Suff. Nat. Hist. Soc. 11, 1-3 Crowle, P. K. 1980. Retinofugal projections to subcortical visual centers in the microchiropteran bat, Myotis sodalis. Proc. Mont. Acad. Sci. 30, 1-11 Curtis, W. E. 1952. Quantative studies on echolocation in bats (Myotis lucifugus); studies on vision in bats (Myotis l.and Eptesicus fuscus); and quantative studies on vision in owls (Tyto alba practincola). PhD-thesis, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY Dalquest, W. W. 1957. Observations on the sharp-nosed bat, Rhynconycteris naso (Maximilian). Texas. J. Sci. 9, 219-226 Davis, R. 1966. Homing performance and homing ability in bats. Ecol. Monogr. 36, 201-237 Davis, W. H.. & Barbour, R. W. 1965. The use of vision in flight by the bat Myotis sodalis. Am. Midl. Nat. 74, 497-499 Davis, W. H. & Barbour, R. W. 1970. Homing in blinded bats (Myotis sodalis). J. Mammal. 51, 182-184 Dietrich, C. E. & Dodt, E. 1970. Structural and some physiological findings on the retina of the bat Myotis myotis. Symp. Electroretinography (ed. A. Wirth). Pacini, Pisa, 120-132

Page 42: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

41

Dyer, F. C. & Gould, J. L. 1981. Honey bee orientation: a backup system for cloudy days. Science 214, 1041-1042 Eisentraut, M. 1950. Dressurvessuche zur festellung eines optischen orientierungsvermögens der fledermmäuse. Verein. Vaterl. Naturk. Im Vurttemberg, Stuttgart. 106, 34-45 Eklöf, J. & Jones, G. 2003 (Paper I). Use of vision in prey detection by brown long-eared bats, Plecotus auritus. Anim. Behav. (In Press) Eklöf, J., Tranefors, T. and Vazquez, L. B. 2002 (Paper V). Precedence of visual cues in the emballonurid bat Balantiopteryx plicata. Mamm. Biol. 67, 42-46 Eklöf, J., Svensson, A. M. & Rydell. J. 2002 (Paper II). Northern bats (Eptesicus nilssonii) use vision but not flutter-detection when searching for prey in clutter. Oikos 99, 347-351 Ellins, S. R. 1970. The role of vision in the sensory orientation of the echolocating bat, Myotis lucifugus. Thesis, Newark, Del. Ellins, S. R. & Masterson, F. A. 1974. Brightness discrimination thresholds in the bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Brain, Behav. Evol. 9, 248-263 Erkert, H. G. 1982. Ecological Aspects of Bat Activity Rythms. In: Ecology of Bats (Kunz, T. H. ed.). Plenum Press, New York, pp 201-242 Faegri, K. & Pijl, L. van der 1979. The principles of pollination ecology. Pergamon, Oxford Farnworth, E. G. 1973. Flashing behaviour, ecology and systematics of the Jamaican lampyrid fireflies. Ph.D. Dissertation, Univ. Of Florida, Gainesville Fenton, M. B. 1975. Observations on the biology of some Rhodesian bats, including a key to the Chiroptera of Rhodesia. Life Sci. Contr. R. Ont. Mus. 104, 1-27 Fenton, M. B. 2001. Bats. Checkmark Books, NY. Fenton, M. B., Audet, D., Obrist, M. K. & Rydell, J. 1995. Signal strength, timing, and self-deafening: the evolution of echolocation in bats. Paleobiology 21, 229-242 Fiedler, J. 1979. Prey catching with and without echolocation in the Indian false vampire bat (Megaderma lyra). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 6, 155-160 Fleming, T. H. 1988. The short-tailed fruit bat, a study in plant-animal interactions. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago and London, pp 365 Fullard, J. H. 1987. Sensory ecology and neuroethology of moths and bats: interactions in a global perspective. In: Recent advances in the study of bats (Fenton, M. B, Racey, P. A. & Rayner, J. M. V. eds.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 244-272 Goodwin, G. G. & Greenhal, A. M. 1961. A review of the bats of Trinidad and Tobago. Bull. Amer. Mus. Nat. Hist. 122, 3 Grant, J. D. A. 1991. Prey location by two Australian long-eared bats, Nyctophilus gouldi and N. geoffroyi. - Australian J. Zool. 39, 45-56 Greenway, F. & Hutson, A. M. 1990. A field guide to British bats. Bruce Coleman Books, Oyster books Ltd, Somerset Griffin, D. R. 1970. Migration and homing of bats. In: Biology of Bats, Vol. II. (Wimsatt, W. A ed.), Academic Press, NY pp 233-264

Page 43: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

42

Heffner, R. S. 1997. Comparative study of sound localization and its anatomical correlates in mammals. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl. 532, 46-53 Heffner, R. S. & Heffner, H. E. 1992. Visual Factors in Sound Localization in Mammals. J. Comp. Neurobiology 317, 219-232 Heffner, R. S., Koay, G. & Heffner, H. E. 1999. Sound localization in an old-world fruit bat, (Rousettus aegyptiacus): Acuity, use of binaural cues, and relationship to vision. J. Comp. Psych.113, 297-306 Heffner, R. S., Koay, G. & Heffner, H. E. 2001. Sound localization in a new-world frugivorous bat, Artibeus jamaicensis: Acuity, use of binaural cues, and relationship to vision. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109, 412-421 Helversen, O. von., Winkler, L. & Bestmann, H. J. 2000. Sulphur-containing "perfumes" attract flower-visiting bats. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 186, 143-153 Hessel, K. & Schmidt, U. 1994. Multimodal orientation in Carollia perspicillata (Phyllostomidae). Folia Zoologica 43, 339-346 Honkavaara, J., Koivula, M., Korpimäki, E., Siitari, H. & Viitala, J. 2002. Ultraviolet vision and foraging in terrestrial vertebrates. Oikos 98, 505-511 Hope, G. M. & Bhatnagar, K. P. 1979a. Electrical responses of bat retinas to spectral stimulation: comparisons of four microchiropteran species. Experentia 35, 1189-1191 Hope, G. M. & Bhatnagar, K. P. 1979a. Effect on light adaptation on electrical responses on the retina of four species of bats. Experentia 35, 1191-1192 Hopkins, H. C. 1984. Floral biology and pollination ecology of the neotropical species of Parkia. J. Ecology 72, 1-23 Hughes, A. 1977. The topography of vision in mammals of contrasting life style: Comparative optics and retinal organisation. In: Handbook of sensory physiology vol VII/5. The visual system in vertebrates (Crescitelli, F. Ed.). Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 613-756. Höller, P. 1995. Orientation by the Bat Phyllostomus discolor (Phyllostomidae) on the Return Flight to its Resting Place. Ethology 100, 72-83 Höller, P. & Schmidt, U. 1996. The orientation behaviour of the lesser spearnosed bat, Phyllostomus discolor (Chiroptera) in a model roost. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 179, 245-254 Jacobs, G. H., Neitz, J. & deegan, ll. 1991. Retinal receptors in rodents maximally sensitive to ultraviolet light. Nature 353, 544-554 Jensen, M. E., Miller, L. A. and Rydell, J. 2001. Detection of prey in clutter by the northern bat, Eptesicus nilssonii. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 199-208 Joermann, G., Schmidt, U. and Schmidt, C. 1988. The mode of orientation during flight and approach to landing in two Phyllostomid bats. Ethology 78, 332-340 Jolicoeur, P. & Baron, G. 1980. Brain Center Correlations among Chiroptera. Brain Behav. Evol. 17, 419-431 Kalko, E. K. V. 1995. Echolocation signal design, foraging habitats and guild structure in six Neotropical sheath-tailed bats (Emballonuridae). In: Ecology, evolution and behaviour of bats (Racey, P. A. & Swift, S. M. eds.). Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 67, 259-273

Page 44: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

43

Kalko, E. K. V & Schnitzler, H. U. 1993. The echolocation and hunting behavior of Daubenton’s bat, Myotis daubentoni. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 24, 225-238 Kalko, E. K. V., Herre, E. A. & Handley Jr, C. O. 1996. Relation of fig fruit characteristics to fruit-eating bats in the New and Old world tropics. J. Biogeography 23, 565-576 Karlsson, B-L., Eklöf, J. & Rydell, J. 2001 (Paper VI). No lunar phobia in swarming insectivorous bats (family Vespertilionidae). J. Zool. Lond. 256, 473-477 Kevan, P. G., Chittka, L. & Dyer, A. G. 2001. Limits to the salience of ultraviolet: lessons from colour vision in bees and birds. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 2571-2580 Kick, S. 1982. Target-detection by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Comp. Physiol. A 145, 432-435 Koay, G., Kearns, D., Heffner, H. E. & Heffner, R. S. 1998. Passive sound-localization ability of the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus). Hearing Research 119, 37-48 Kulzer, E., Nelson, J. E., McKean, J. L. & Möhres, F. P. 1984. Prey catching behaviour and echolocation in the Australian ghost bat, Macroderma gigas (Microchiroptera: Megadermatidae). Aust. Mammal. 7: 37-50 Kürten, L. & Schmidt, U. 1982. Thermo-perception in the common vampire bat (Desmodus rotundus). J. Comp. Physiol. A 146, 223-228 Lang, A. B., Kalko, E. K. V., Dechmann, D. K. N. & Bockholdt, C. 2002. Associations of lunar-correlated activity rythms of Neotropical Katydids with activity patterns of the gleaning insectivorous Round-eared bat, Tonatia silvicola. Abstract 32nd NASBR, p 58 Laska, M. 1990. Olfactory sensitivity to food odor components in the short-tailed fruit bat, Carollia perspicillata (Phyllostomatidae, Chiroptera). J. Comp. Physiol. A 166, 395-399 Lawrence, B. D. & Simmons, J. A. 1982. Measurements of atmospheric attenuation at ultrasonic frequencies and the significance for echolocation by bats. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 71, 585-590 Layne, J. N. 1967. Evidence for the use of vision in diurnal orientation of the bat Myotis austroriparius. Anim. Behav. 15, 409-415 Lekagul, B. & McNeely, J. A. 1977. Mammals of Thailand. Assoc. Consev. Wildlife, Bangkok Limpens, H. J. G. A. & Kapteyn, K. 1991. Bats, their behaviour and linear landscape elements. Myotis 29, 39-48 Lloyd, J. E. 1989. Bat (Chiroptera) connections with firefly (Coleoptera: Lampyridae) luminescence, I: Potential significance, historical evidence, and opportunity. The Coleopterists Bulletin, 83-91 Lopez, J. Winter, Y. & Helversen, O. von 2001. Behavioural measurements of spectral sensitivity in a flower visiting bat (Glossophaga soricina). Abstracts 12th International Bat research Conference, Bangi, Selangor, Malaysia, pp 58 Manske, U. & Schmidt, U. 1976. Visual acuity of the vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus, and its dependence upon light intensity. Z. Tierpsychol. 42, 215-221 Manske, U. & Schmidt, U. 1979. Untersuchungen zur optischen Musterunterscheidung bei der Vampirfledermaus, Desmodus rotundus. Z. Tierpsychol. 49, 120 Marks, J. M. 1980. Retinal ganglion cell topography in bats. MA thesis. Bloomington, IN: Indiana Univ.

Page 45: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

44

Masterson, F. A. & Ellins, S. R. 1974. The role of vision in the orientation of the echolocating bat, Myotis lucifugus. Behaviour LI, 1-2, 88-98 McIlwain, J. T. 1996. An introduction to the biology of vision. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge McWilliam, A. N. 1987. Territorial and pair behaviour of the African false vampire bat, Cardioderma cor (Chiroptera: Megadermatidae), in coastal Kenya. J. Zool. Lond. 213, 243-252 Mistry, S. 1990. Characteristics of the visually guided escape response of the Mexican free-tailed bat, Tadarida brasiliensis mexicana. Anim. Behav. 39, 314-320 Morrison, D. W. 1978. Lunar phobia in a neotropical fruit bat, Artibeus jamaicensis (Chiroptera, Phyllostomidae). Anim. Behav. 26, 852-855 Mueller, H. C. 1968. The role of vision in vespertilionid bats. Am. Midl. Nat. 79, 524-525 Nagel, T. 1974. What is it like to be a bat? Phil. Rev. 83, 535-450 Neuweiler, G. 1967. Interaction of other sensory systems with the sonar system. In: Les Systemes Sonars Animaux Biologie et Bionique (ed. Busnel, R. G.). Nato Adv. Study Inst. Frascati, pp 509-533 Neuweiler, G. 2000. The biology of bats. Oxford university Press, NY Neuweiler, G. & Möhres, F. P. 1966. The role of spatial memory in the orientation. In: Les Systemes Sonars Animaux Biologie et Bionique (ed. Busnel, R. G.). Nato Adv. Study Inst. Frascati, pp 129-140 Pack, A. A. & Herman, L. M. 1995. Sensory integration in the bottlenosed dolphin: Immediate recognition of complex shapes across the senses of echolocation and vision. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 98, 722-733 Pentney, R. P. & Cotter, J. R. 1976. Retinofugal projections in an echolocating bat. Brain Research 115, 479-484 Pettigrew, J. D. 1980. Microbat vision and echolocation in an evolutionary context. In: Nachtigall, P. E. and Moore, P. W. B. (eds.), Animal Sonar. Processes and Performance. New York, Plenum Press, pp 645-650 Pettigrew, J. D. 1986. Flying primates? Mega-bats have the advanced pathway from eye to midbrain. Science 231, 1304-1306 Pettigrew, J. D. 1988. Microbat vision and echolocation in an evolutionary context. NATO ASI Series A Life Sciences 156, 645-650 Pettigrew, J. D., Coles, R. B., Guppy, A., Brown, M. & Nelson, J. 1983. Sensory abilities of the Australian ghost bat, Macroderma gigas. Neurosci. Letts. Suppl. 11, 568 Pettigrew, J. D. Baker, G. B., Baker-Gabb, D., Baverstock, G., Coles, R., Conole, L., Churchill, S., Fitzherbert, K., Guppy, A., Hall, L., Helman, P., Nelson, J., Priddel, D., Pulsford, I., Richards, G., Schulz, M. & Tidemann, C. R. 1986. The Australian ghost bat, Macroderma gigas, at Pine Creek, Northern territory. Macroderma 2, 10-19 Pettigrew, J. D., Dreher, B., Hopkins, C. S. McCall, M. J. & Brown, M. 1988. Peak density and distribution of ganglion cells in the retinae of microchiropteran bats: Implications for visual acuity. Brain Behav. Evol. 32, 39-56

Page 46: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

45

Racey, P. A. & Swift, S. M. 1985. Feeding ecology of Pipistrellus pipistrellus during pregnancy and lactation. I. Foraging behaviour. J. Anim. Ecol. 54, 205-215 Reimer, K. 1989. Retinofugal projections in the rufous horseshoe bat, Rhinolophus rouxi. Anat. Embryol. 180, 89-98 Rother, G. & Schmidt, U. 1982. Der einfluss visueller information auf die Echoortung bei Phyllostomus discolor (Chiroptera). Z. Zäugertierkunde 47, 324-334 Ryan, J. & Tuttle, M. D. 1987. The role of prey-generated sounds, vision, and echolocation in prey localization by the African bat Cardioderma cor (Megadermatidae). J. Comp. Physiol. A 161, 59-66 Rydell, J. 1992a. Exploitation of insects around street lamps by bats in Sweden. Funct. Ecol. 6, 744-750 Rydell, J. 1992b. Occurrence of bats in northernmost Sweden (65°N) and their feeding ecology in summer. J. Zool. Lond. 227, 517-529 Rydell, J. 1998. Bat defence in lekking ghost swift (Hepialus humuli), a moth without ultrasonic hearing. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 1373-1376 Rydell, J., Parker McNeill, D. & Eklöf, J. 2002. Capture success of little brown bats feeding on mosquitoes. J. Zool. Lond. 256, 379-381 Ryer, A. 1997. Light measurement handbook. International Light, Newburyport, MA. Schmidt, U. 1988. Orientation and sensory functions in Desmodus rotundus. In: Natural history of vampire bats (Greenhall, A. M. & Schmidt, U. eds.) CRC Press, Inc. Boca Raton Florida, pp 143-166 Schmidt, U. & Manske, U. 1978. Visual pattern discrimination in the vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus. Congressus Theriologicus Internationalis 2, 59 Schmidt, U., Joermann, G. & Rother, G. 1988. Acoustical vs. visual orientation in neotropical bats. In: Animal Sonar (Nachtigall, P. E. & Moore, P. W. B. eds.), Plenum Publishing Corporation, pp 589-593 Schnitzler, H. U. & Grinnell, A. D. 1977a. Directional sensitivity of echolocation in the horse shoe bat, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum. I. Directionality of sound emission. J. Comp. Physiol. 116, 51-61 Schnitzler, H. U. & Grinnell, A. D. 1977b. Directional sensitivity of echolocation in the horse shoe bat, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum. II. Behavioral directionality of hearing. J. Comp. Physiol. 116, 63-76 Simmons, N. B. & Geisler, J. H. 1998. Phylogenetic relationships of Icaronycteris, Archaeonycteris, Hassianycteris, and Palaeochiropteryx to extant bat lineages, with comments on the evolution and foraging strategies in Microchiroptera. Bull. Amer. Mus. Nat. Hist. 235 Springer, M. S., Teeling, E. & Stanhope, M. J. 2001. External nasal cartilages in bats: Evidence for Microchiropteran monophyly? J. Mamm. Evol. 8, 231-236 Stein, B. E. & Meredith, A. 1993. The merging of the senses. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA Suthers, R. A. 1966. Optomotor responses by echolocating bats. Science 152, 1102-1104 Suthers, R. A. 1970. Vision, olfaction and taste. In: Biology of Bats Vol. II (Wimsatt, W. A. ed.). Academic Press, New York, pp 265-281 Suthers, R. A. & Bradford, M. R. 1980. Visual systems and the evolutionary relationships of the Chiroptera. Proc. 5th Int. Bat. Res. Conf. 331-346

Page 47: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

46

Suthers, R. A. & Chase, J. 1966. Visual pattern discrimination by an echolocating bat. Amer. Zool. 6, 573 Suthers, R. A. & Wallis, N. E. 1970. Optics of the eyes of echolocating bats. J. Vision Res. 10, 1165-1173 Suthers, R. A., Chase, J. & Bradford, B. 1969. Visual form discrimination by echolocating bats. Biol. Bull. 137, 535-546 Swift, S. M. 1998. Long-eared bats. T & AD Poyser Natural History. London Test, F. H. 1967. Indicated use of sight in navigation by molossid bats. J. Mamm. 48, 482-483 Thomson, C. E. 1982. Myotis sodalis. Mammalian species 163, 1-5 Timm, R. M. 1989. Migration and molt patterns of red bats, Lasiurus borealis (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae). Illinois. Bull. Chic. Acad. Sci. 14, 1-7 Usman, K., Habersetzer, R., Subbaraj, R., Gopalkrishnaswamy, G. & Paramandam, K. 1980. Behaviour of bats during a lunar eclipse. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 7, 79-80 Varassin, I. G., Trigo, J. R. & Sazima, M. 2001. The role of nectar production, flower pigments and odour in the pollination of four species of Passiflora (Passifloraceae) in south-eastern Brazil. Bot. J. Linn. Soc. 136, 139-152 Vaughan, T. A. & Vaughan, R. P. 1986. Seasonality and the behaviour of the African yellow-winged bat. J. Mamm. 67, 91-102 Verboom, B. & Huitema, H. 1997. The importance of linear landscape elements for the pipistrelle Pipistrellus pipistrellus and the serotine bat Eptesicus serotinus. Landscape Ecology 12 (2), 117-125 Verboom, B., Boonman, A. M. & Limpens, H. J. G. A. 1999. Acoustic perception of landscape elements by the pond bat (Myotis dasycneme) J. Zool. Lond. 248, 59-66 Vernon, C. L. 1981. The use of vision in prey selection by the Big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Master thesis, the University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Wallace, M. T. & Stein, B. E. 2001. Sensory and multisensory responses in the newborn monkey superior colliculus. J. Neurosci. 21, 8886-8898 Whittington, D. A., Hepp-Raymond, M. C. & Flood, W. 1981. Eye and head movements to auditory targets. Exp. Brain Res. 41, 358-363 Wilkinson, G. S. 1986. Social grooming in the vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus. Anim. Behav. 34, 1880-1889 Williams, T. C. & Williams, J. M. 1967. Radiotracking of homing bats. Science 155, 1435-1436 Williams, T. C., Williams, J. M. & Griffin, D. R. 1966. Homing ability of the neotropical bat Phyllostomus hastatus. Anim. Behav. 14, 468-473 Willson, M. F. & Whelan, C. J. 1989. Ultraviolet reflectance of fruits of vertebrate-dispersed plants. Oikos 55, 341-348 Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. 1995. Magnetic orientation in animals. Springer-Verlag, Berlin

Page 48: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

47

Page 49: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

48

”Love looks not with the eyes, but with the mind; and therefore is winged Cupid painted blind” - William Shakespeare

I

Page 50: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

49

Page 51: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

50

Use of vision in prey detection by brown long-eared bats Plecotus auritus JOHAN EKLÖF1 & GARETH JONES2

1Zoology Department, Göteborg University, Sweden 2 School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, UK Eklöf & Jones, Use of vision in Plecotus auritus Correspondence Johan Eklöf, Zoology Department, Göteborg University, Box 463, SE-405 30 Göteborg, Sweden. E-mail: [email protected] Gareth Jones, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Woodland Road, Bristol BS8 1UG, UK

ABSTRACT We investigated the ability of brown long-eared bats (Plecotus auritus) to make use of visual cues when searching for food. By using petri dishes containing mealworms that were subjected to different levels of illumination, we presented four bats with different sensory cues: visual cues, sonar cues and a combination of these. The bats preferred situations where both sonar cues and visual cues were available, and the visual information was more important than the sonar cues. The bats did, however, emit echolocation calls throughout the experiments.

Animal Behaviour – In Press

Page 52: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

51

Microchiropteran bats use echolocation for orientation, and often for prey detection, and can thus operate in darkness and under unpredictable lighting (Griffin 1958). However, as high frequency sounds attenuate quickly in air, and limit the echolocation range (Kick 1982; Kalko & Schnitzler 1993; Fenton et al. 1995), sonar cannot be effectively used for detection of small targets over long distances. Also, for echolocating bats foraging close to vegetation, separation of prey from the background clutter (echoes from objects other than the target of interest) is usually problematic (Schnitzler & Kalko1998; Arlettaz et al. 2001; Jensen et al. 2001). Therefore, some bats use for example prey-generated sounds (Ryan & Tuttle 1987; Arlettaz et al. 2001) and smell (Thies et al. 1998) as additional cues when searching for prey. Few studies have addressed the possibility that visual cues may also be important for detection of prey. The aerial hawking northern bat (Eptesicus nilssonii: Vespertilionidae), is guided by visual cues when searching for the large and conspicuously white ghost swifts Hepialus humuli (Lepidoptera), hovering among high grass (clutter) at dusk (Eklöf et al. 2002). Other aerial hawking bats, such as Craseonycteris thonglongyai (Emballonuridae) might potentially use visual cues by making use of the bright sky, against which insects are seen as silhouettes (Pettigrew 1980). The California leaf-nosed bat (Macrotus californicus) uses a gleaning foraging tactic, and catches prey from the ground. This species is the only bat so far shown to be capable of catching prey by using vision alone (Bell 1985).

The brown long-eared bat Plecotus auritus is also a gleaning bat, that sometimes takes insects from leaves (Swift 1998). This means that it faces the problems of detecting prey in a cluttered environment. Its echolocation calls are faint and short FM (frequency modulated) sweeps (Ahlén 1981; Parsons & Jones 2000) which may be an adaptation for foraging close to vegetation or, alternatively, may be used only for spatial orientation (Arlettaz et al. 2001). Passive listening plays a major role for detecting the prey in the long-eared bat (Anderson & Racey 1991). The ears are large and the hearing is exceptionally sensitive to sounds around 15 kHz, close to the frequencies emitted by insects moving in clutter (Coles et al. 1989).

Plecotus auritus also has relatively big eyes compared to many other insectivorous bats (Cranbrook 1963), suggesting that these bats also have relatively good vision. Eisentraut (1950) was able to train brown long-eared bats to discriminate between black and white 9-cm square shaped cards, but when he presented the bats with a circle and a cross they failed to make the right choice. This indicates that P. auritus can discriminate between different targets by using vision, but not different patterns. This is in contrast to some phyllostomid bats (Suthers & Chase 1966; Suthers et al. 1969), which show more sophisticated discrimination of patterns. However, Eisentraut’s experiments were carried out in bright light, and as subsequently shown by several authors, microchiropteran vision works better in dim ambient light (i.e. dusk and dawn illumination) than in bright daylight (Bradbury & Nottebohm 1969; Ellins & Masterson 1974; Hope & Bhatnagar 1979).

The aim of this study is to investigate if brown long eared bats use visual cues in addition to sonar cues when searching for prey. Its large eyes and gleaning foraging behaviour suggest that this may be the case. We quantified

Page 53: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

52

the ability of brown long eared bats to find prey by using vision alone; i.e. to find prey on dark and dimly illuminated backgrounds and also behind a transparent surface.

METHODS The study was conducted in the School of Biological Sciences at Bristol University, UK. Four female Plecotus auritus were captured at their roost (Ilminster, Somerset) 23 April, and released at capture site 12 May 2002. They were kept in a 2.2m x 3m x 3m flight room, where also the experiments took place. The room had ventilation installed and the bats could move around freely and they had several places to hide, including boxes and pieces of cloths on the walls. The bats were fed on mealworms with vitamin supplements. They were fed by hand the first day, presented with bowls containing mealworms the second day, and could feed by themselves from the bowls from day three. Water was given in the same kind of bowls and was available to the bats on the flight room floor all the time (and changed twice every day). The temperature of the room was 13-16° C, except during the experiments, when it was 20° C. The daylight period was partly reversed (lights on at 03:00 and off at 16:00) with experiments starting at ca. 18:00. All the bats lost a little weight (0.5 - 1g) during the first two days, but on the day of release, they all had their original weight except one that had gained 1g. Experiment 1 Two halogen light sources (Schott) with two fibre-optic goose necks each were placed on the floor in the flight room. At the end of each goose neck, we attached plastic tubes, two of them with neutral density light filters (reducing light intensity with 50%), and the other two with dark covers (letting no light through). This set-up provided us with two circular 25-cm diameter areas of dim light (4 lux; measured with a Gossen Mavolux digital light meter) and two similar “areas of darkness” (0.2 lux), 30-40cm apart on the flight room floor. The overall illumination in the room never exceeded 0.1 lux. The plastic tubes on the fibre-optic goose necks could be switched and, hence, the arrangement of the lit and dark areas could be changed. On the floor; under each goose neck we put a petri dish (9 cm diameter, 1.8 cm deep), placed inside the lid belonging to it, either with mealworms in the petri dish itself or in the lid (hence between lid and dish). This provided four different combinations of sensory cues. 1: lit area with mealworms in the dish (visual and sonar cues). 2: lit area with mealworms placed in the lid (visual cues only) 3: dark area with mealworms in the dish (sonar cues only). 4: dark area with mealworms in the lid (no cues except from the dish itself). We assume that acoustical cues arising from the mealworms moving in the dishes or the lids were the same in all cases.

During the experiment the bats were flying individually in the flight room for 30-40 minutes, feeding from the petri dishes. Each landing at a

Page 54: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

53

dish was recorded as a foraging attempt, and as soon as a landing was recorded the arrangement of the feeding situation was changed. To prevent the bats from using spatial memory the experimental area was divided in two parts (A & B), which were used alternately, so that a foraging attempt in area A was followed by an experiments in area B. The light sources could be placed at four different positions within each area, and the positions were changed at random after each feeding attempt. Also the arrangement of the four petri dishes (presenting the different sensory cues) was changed at random after each feeding attempt. Hence, in total there were 32 potential positions for each petri dish.

An infra red sensitive video camera (Sony Mini DV TRV9E Handycam) with a wide-angle lens was placed above the experimental area. It was connected to a monitor placed outside the flight room and thus making it possible to survey the experiments without disturbing the bats.

In order to test if the probability of a feeding attempt at a certain dish was the same for all bats, i.e. if we could treat the bats as one group, we applied a Chi square test of homogeneity (testing if a specific distribution is the same in different situations, in this case for the different bat individuals). Then we made pair-wise comparisons between dish preferences, i.e. we compared the probability that one dish was preferred over another. We did so by using 95% confidence intervals, i.e. intervals having 95% probability of covering our estimated preferences (number of captures in each category divided by total number of captures) for the different feeding dishes. As we calculated several intervals using the same data set, we applied the Bonferroni method for multiple comparisons.

Experiment 2 To exclude the possibility that the bats considered the light or the petri dishes rather than the actual prey items as potential food sources, we presented the bats with two lit areas, one with a petri dish containing mealworms, and one with an empty dish. The set-up was similar to the one in experiment 1, although only one light source (with two goose necks) was used, and this time all four bats were flying simultaneously. We also placed a bat detector (Ultra Sound Advice S-25 set in frequency-division mode) next to one of the petri dishes, and connected it to a speaker outside the flight room. The different positions of the light source and the arrangement of the two petri dishes together with the position of the bat detector, were changed after each feeding attempt and randomised in a similar manner as in experiment 1. This experiment was also surveyed using an IR sensitive video camera. The number of feeding attempts at the two petri dishes was compared using Chi square statistics, and echolocation calls from feeding bats were noted.

Page 55: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

54

RESULTS

General observations When released in the flight room, the bats typically flew across the feeding area a few times before deciding from which dish to feed. They usually hovered just above one of the dishes (10 – 15 cm) for a short period and then landed next to it. The bats then crawled into the dish to feed. On a few occasions, bats crawled around the feeding area on the floor to investigate the different dishes. After capture of a mealworm, the bats consumed it while hanging on one of the walls in the flight room. Bats moving around consistently emitted detectable echolocation calls. Experiment 1 The number of feeding attempts and estimates of preference at the different dishes are presented in table 1. We found no significant differences between bat individuals in their probability of feeding at a certain dish (?2

9 = 7.94, p>0.05).

This means that we do not gain any statistically significant information by treating the individuals separately, and we therefore pooled the results.

When comparing the number of feeding attempts between each of the four foraging situations (data and statistics in tab. 1 and 2), we found that in lit situations, it made no difference if sonar cues were added or not. However, there were more feeding attempts when sonar cues and visual cues were present, compared to when only sonar cues were available. This suggests that the bats predominantly used vision when they searched for prey.

When comparing the frequency of foraging attempts at situations providing visual or sonar cues only, the bats scored better at the visual cues, and when comparing sonar cues only to the no cue situation, we found no significant difference. This analysis too suggests that the bats predominantly relied on vision and it also suggests that they could not find prey items using sonar alone. Experiment 2 There were significantly more feeding attempts at lit petri dishes containing mealworms than at lit empty ones (?2

1 = 7.7, p <0.01, n =39 & 18 respectively). This suggests that the bats were able to detect mealworms either acoustically or visually, and that they did not associate structural features of the food dish with reward. Hence, the bats were not only heading for the light or the dishes alone. Echolocation calls were detected both from bats approaching the dish with mealworms, as well as from bats investigating the empty dish.

Page 56: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

55

DISCUSSION

We found that long-eared bats are capable of visual detection of prey, at least under the light intensity of 4 lux, and that they prefer visual cues to sonar cues if given a choice. The results also indicate that the bats were unable to detect the prey items using sonar alone.

There is both experimental and observational evidence that echolocating bats make use of vision and even give precedence to visual stimuli in some situations, including long distance orientation (Griffin 1970), detection of landmarks (Davis 1966), obstacle avoidance (Bradbury & Nottebohm 1969) and prey detection (Bell 1985). When selecting an escape route in an experimental set-up, the phyllostomid Anoura geoffroyi used visual cues alone and disregarded acoustical cues that also were provided (Chase 1981, 1983). The observation that bats have a tendency to crash into windows of buildings when released indoors (Fenton 1975) or during natural migration (Timm 1989) also suggests that they predominantly rely on vision rather than on echolocation in situations when both acoustic and visual cues are available. The performance is greatly improved (i.e. fewer crashes) when the bats are blinded (Davis & Barbour 1965) or when flying under natural dark conditions, and hence are “forced” to rely on echolocation (Eklöf et al. 2002). Suthers and Wallis (1970) studied the eyes of two species of Vespertilionidae (Myotis sodalis and Pipistrellus subflavus) and four different phyllostomids (Desmodus rotundus, Carollia perspicillata, Anoura geoffroyi and Phyllostomus hastatus), and concluded that the visual capabilities of all the species tested would allow the bats to see well beyond the range of echolocation. Due to the more or less spherical lenses, it also follows that Microchiroptera has a short focal distance and hence a great depth of focus (Suthers & Wallis 1970). In fact, microchiropteran bats seem to be farsighted, indicating that vision is used preferably over longer distances, where it may not overlap with echolocation, which is a short-range operation (Kick 1982; Fenton et al. 1995). Nevertheless there are some bats, such as Phyllostomus hastatus, which also use vision within the range covered by their echolocation system, i.e. when approaching a landing spot (Joermann et al. 1988).

We cannot exclude the possibility that the bats used passive listening to detect the mealworms, as some other gleaning bats do (Arlettaz et al. 2001). Sounds coming from the prey could have been detected from any of the dishes, including the dishes with visual cues only and the no cue situation, as live mealworms were crawling under the petri dishes (in the lids). Also olfactory cues from the mealworms may have been available to the bats in all four feeding situations. Hence, olfactory cues and prey-generated sounds may explain some of the feeding attempts at petri dishes with prey in the lids.

During the experiments, bats were sometimes seen hovering at positions where no petri dish was available, but where dishes had been available previously, presumably relying on spatial memory (Neuweiler & Möhres 1966; Grant 1991; Höller & Schmidt 1996). Hence, the bats tended to revisit positions where they previously had been rewarded and it seems possible that the results

Page 57: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

56

are influenced to some extent by this. However, we believe that our experimental design, where the position of the dishes were changed after every feeding attempt, would minimise these effects, and that the results show preference for feeding dishes, rather than spatial memory.

The results of Experiment 2 allowed rejection of the hypothesis that bats associated structural features of the food dish with reward, and were therefore attracted to features of the dishes by associative learning (Siemers 2001). However, as all the bats flew simultaneously in this experiment, we cannot exclude the possibility that some individuals sometimes explored the dishes without getting a reliable indication of reward. In the first experiment there were equally many feeding attempts at the petri dishes providing sonar cues as at the no cue dishes, which means that we found no indication that the bats could detect the prey items by using sonar alone. One can therefore hypothesize that bats having to rely exclusively on sonar may learn to recognize structures rather than prey.

There were more feeding attempts at the petri dishes providing visual cues only compared to those that provided sonar cues only. This suggests that the long-eared bats were capable of finding prey visually and that they even preferred using vision when possible. Long-eared bats emerge from their roosts late in the evening (15-55 minutes after sunset depending on the latitude; Swift 1998), which means that they normally operate in very low light levels (<1 lux). Their technique of gleaning insects off leaves also means that they do not use the bright sky to backlight prey, which has been suggested to be the case for some aerial hawking bats (Pettigrew 1980; Vaughan & Vaughan 1986). However, Eptesicus fuscus (Vespertilionidae) can discriminate differences in brightness in ambient illumination as low as 0.001 lux, conditions which resemble darkness to a human dark adapted eye, although the optimal performance is around 10 lux, which is equivalent to dusk and dawn conditions (Ellins & Masterson 1974). This suggests that bats can use visual cues under nocturnal conditions. The resolving power of vespertilionid bats typically ranges between 0.7 and 1 degree of arc (Marks 1980; Bell & Fenton 1986; Pettigrew et al. 1988), which means that an object of mealworm size (ca. 2 cm) can be detected from a distance of ca. 1 m. The eyes of long-eared bats are larger than those of most other Vespertilionidae (Cranbrook 1963), suggesting that they presumably also have better visual acuity as well as light sensitivity (Land & Nilsson 2002). As the bats in this study usually hovered above the petri dishes on a height of 10-15 cm, the mealworms were most likely visible to the bats at least on the lit surfaces. Cranbrook (1963) noted that long-eared bats, despite their big eyes, did not seem to look for the food when feeding from bowls, but rather felt about until a prey item was captured. Studies on northern bats (Eptesicus nilssonii, Vespertilionidae) catching ghost swifts (Hepialis humulii) suggest that vision is used primarily in an initial search phase to detect prey, rather than in the terminal attack (Eklöf et al. 2002). In a similar manner, phyllostomid bats detect landing grids visually, but use echolocation when performing the actual landing operation (Joermann et al. 1988). Hence, it seems possible that the long-eared bats used visual cues for detection, but exploited additional information, such as echolocation and passive listening during the final pursuit.

Page 58: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

57

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We wish to acknowledge Marc Holderied and Julian Partridge for comments on the experimental design and Jens Rydell for comments on the manuscript. We also wish to thank John Gustafsson and Catrin Bergqvist for statistical advice. The study was supported by ”Stiftelsen Paul och Marie Berghaus donationsfond”, and ”Adlerbertska forskningsstiftelsen” (JE). Research was performed under licence from English Nature.

REFERENCES Ahlén, I. 1981. Identification of Scandinavian bats by their sounds. Uppsala: Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Department of Wildlife Ecology. Anderson, M. E. & Racey, P. A. 1991. Feeding behaviour of captive long eared-bats, Plecotus auritus. Animal Behaviour, 42, 489-493 Arlettaz, R., Jones, G. & Racey, P. A. 2001. Effect of acoustic clutter on prey detection by bats. Nature, 414, 742-745. Bell, G. P. 1985. The sensory basis of prey location by the California leaf-nosed bat Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 16, 343-347. Bell, G. P. & Fenton, M. B. 1986. Visual acuity, sensitivity and binocularity in a gleaning insectivorous bat, Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Animal Behaviour, 34, 409-414. Bradbury, J. & Nottebohm, F. 1969. The use of vision by the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus, under controlled conditions. Animal Behaviour, 17, 480-485. Chase, J. 1981.Visually guided escape responses of microchiropteran bats. Animal Behaviour, 29, 708- 713. Chase, J. 1983. Differential responses to visual and acoustic cues during escape in the bat Anoura geoffroyi: cue preferences and behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 31, 526-531. Coles, R. B., Guppy, A., Anderson, M. E. & Schlegel, P. 1989. Frequency sensitivity and directional hearing in the gleaning bat, Plecotus auritus (Linnaeus 1758). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 165, 269-280. Cranbrook, The Earl of. 1963. Notes on the feeding habits of the long-eared bat. Transaction of Suffolk Natural History Society, 11, 1-3. Davis, R. 1966. Homing performance and homing ability in bats. Ecological Monographs, 36, 201-237. Davis, R. & Barbour, R. W. 1965. The use of vision in flight by the bat Myotis sodalis. The American Midland Naturalist, 74, 497-499. Eisentraut, M. 1950. Dressurvessuche zur festellung eines optischen orientierungsvermögens der fledermmäuse. Verein für Vaterländische Naturkunde In Vürttemberg, Stuttgart, 106, 34-45.

Page 59: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

58

Eklöf, J., Tranefors, T. & Vazquez, L. B. 2002. Precedence of visual cues in the emballonurid bat Balantiopteryx plicata. Mammalian Biology, 67, 42-46. Eklöf, J., Svensson, A. M. & Rydell, J. 2002. Northern bats (Eptesicus nilssonii) use vision but not flutter-detection when searching for prey in clutter. Oikos, 99, 347-351. Ellins, S. R. & Masterson, F. A. 1974. Brightness discrimination thresholds in the bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Brain, Behaviour and Evolution, 9, 248-263. Fenton, M. B. 1975. Observations on the biology of some Rhodesian bats, including a key to the Chiroptera of Rhodesia. Life Science Contributions of the Royal Ontario Museum, 104, 1-27. Fenton, M. B., Audet, D., Obrist, M. K. & Rydell, J. 1995. Signal strength, timing, and self-deafening: the evolution of echolocation in bats. Paleobiology, 21 (2), 229-242. Grant, J. D. A. 1991. Prey location by two Australian long-eared bats, Nyctophilus gouldi and N. geoffroyi. - Australian Journal of Zoology, 39, 45-56. Griffin, D. R. 1958. Listening in the Dark. New haven: Yale University Press. Griffin, D. R. 1970. Migration and homing of bats. In: Biology of Bats, Vol. II. (Ed. By W. A. Wimsatt), pp. 233-264. New York, Academic Press. Hope, G. M. & Bhatnagar, K. P. 1979. Effect on light adaptation on electrical responses on the retina of four species of bats. Experentia, 35, 1191-1192. Höller, P. & Schmidt, U. 1996. The orientation behaviour of the lesser spearnosed bat, Phyllostomus discolor (Chiroptera) in a model roost. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 179, 245-254. Jensen, M. E., Miller, L. A. and Rydell, J. 2001. Detection of prey in clutter by the northern bat, Eptesicus nilssonii. Journal of Experimental Biology, 204, 199-208. Joermann, G., Schmidt, U. and Schmidt, C. 1988. The mode of orientation during flight and approach to landing in two Phyllostomid bats. Ethology, 78, 332-340. Kalko, E. K. V. & Schnitzler, H.-U. 1993. Plasticity of echolocation signals of European pipistrelle bats in search flight: implications for habitat use and prey detection. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 33, 415-428. Kick, S. 1982. Target-detection by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 145, 432-435 Land, M. F. & Nilsson, D. E. 2002. Animal Eyes. Oxford Animal Biology Series. Oxford University Press. Marks, J. M. 1980. Retinal ganglion cell topography in bats. MA thesis, Indiana University, Bloomington. Neuweiler, G. & Möhres, F. P. 1966. The role of spatial memory in the orientation. In: Les Systemes Sonars Animaux Biologie et Bionique (Ed. by R. G. Busnel), pp. 129-140. Frascati: NATO Advanced Study Institute. Parsons, S. & Jones, G. 2000. Acoustic identification of twelve species of echolocating bat by discriminant function analysis and artificial neural networks. Journal of Experimental Biology, 203, 2641-2656. Pettigrew, J. D. 1980. Microbat vision and echolocation in an evolutionary context. In: Animal Sonar. Processes and Performance (Ed. by P. E. Nachtigall & P. W. B. Moore), pp. 645-650. New York: Plenum Press.

Page 60: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

59

Pettigrew, J. D., Dreher, B., Hopkins, C. S. McCall, M. J. & Brown, M. 1988. Peak density and distribution of ganglion cells in the retinae of microchiropteran bats: Implications for visual acuity. Brain, Behaviour and Evolution, 32, 39-56. Ryan, J. & Tuttle, M. D. 1987. The role of prey-generated sounds, vision, and echolocation in prey localization by the African bat Cardioderma cor (Megadermatidae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 161, 59-66 Schnitzler, H.-U. & Kalko, E.K.V. 1998. How echolocating bats search and find food. In: Bat Biology and Conservation (Ed. by T. H. Kunz & P. A. Racey), pp. 183-204. Washington D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press. Siemers, B.M. 2001. Finding prey by associative learning in gleaning bats: experiments with a Natterer’s bat Myotis nattereri. Acta Chiropterologica, 3, 211-215. Suthers, R. A. & Chase, J. 1966. Visual pattern discrimination by an echolocating bat. American Zoologist, 6, 573 Suthers, R. A., Chase, J. & Bradford, B. 1969. Visual form discrimination by echolocating bats. Biological Bulletin, 137, 535-546. Suthers, R. A. & Wallis, N. E. 1970. Optics of the eyes of echolocating bats. Journal of Vision Research, 10, 1165-1173 Swift, S. M. 1998. Long-eared bats. London: T & AD Poyser Natural History. Thies, W., Kalko, E. K. V & Schnitzler, H-U. 1998. The roles of echolocation and olfaction in two neotropical fruit-eating bats, Carollia perspicillata and C. castanea feeding on piper. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 42, 397-409 Timm, R. M. 1989. Migration and molt patterns of red bats, Lasiurus borealis (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae). Illinois Bulletin of the Chicago Academy of Sciences, 14, 1-7. Vaughan, T. A. & Vaughan, R. P. 1986. Seasonality and the behaviour of the African yellow-winged bat. Journal of Mammalogy, 67 (1), 91-102

Page 61: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

60

Table 1. – The number of feeding attempts and mean proportions of attempts at petri dishes providing four individuals of Plecotus auritus with different sensory cues. sensory cue bat # 1 bat # 2 bat # 3 bat # 4 total mean proportion visual only 12 27 11 17 67 0.34 visual/sonar 16 21 14 21 72 0.38 sonar only 13 10 5 7 35 0.18 none 3 8 3 6 20 0.10 total 44 66 33 51 194 1.0

Page 62: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

61

Table 2. – Pair-wise differences between mean proportions of attempts made to dishes providing different sensory cues, with 95% Bonferroni-corrected confidence intervals for each difference. If a confidence interval includes zero, the bats were equally likely to make feeding attempts at the two types of dish. Sensory cue comparison Confidence interval preferred dish p(visual cues) – p(visual/sonar cues) -0.04±0.17 no preference

p(visual cues) – p(sonar cues) 0.16±0.14 visual

p(visual/sonar cues) – p(sonar cues) 0.20±0.14 visual/sonar

p(visual cues) – p(no cue) 0.24±0.12 visual

p(visual/sonar cues) – p(no cue) 0.27±0.13 visual/sonar

p(sonar cues) – p(no cue) 0.08±0.10 no preference

Page 63: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

62

“The Eocene brought mammals mean And bats began to sing; Their food they found by ultrasound And chased it on the wing.” - John D Pye

II

Page 64: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

63

Page 65: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

64

Page 66: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

65

Page 67: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

66

Page 68: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

67

Page 69: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

68

Page 70: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

69

Page 71: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

70

“You can't depend on your eyes when your imagination is out of focus” - Mark Twain

III

Page 72: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

71

Page 73: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

72

Vision complements echolocation in an aerial-hawking bat

Jens Rydell and Johan Eklöf

Zoology Department, Göteborg University, Box 463, SE 405 30 Göteborg, Sweden.

Corresponding author:

Johan Eklöf, address as above

Tel. +46-317733666

Fax. +46-31416729

E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract The northern bat Eptesicus nilssonii hawks flying insects in the air using frequency-modulated echolocation calls. It is known to detect and catch visually conspicuous prey (white moths) hovering low among grass stalks. To overcome the problem with acoustic clutter from the grass that interferes with target echo detection, the bats made use of visual cues in addition to those of echolocation. However, vision increased the chance of detection only when the moths were at least 5 cm in wingspan. Smaller targets were detected using echolocation alone. The mean detection range was 3.5 m, which suggests a visual acuity of 49´ of arc. This is consistent with results of optomotor response tests and counts of retinal ganglion cells in closely related species. The results suggest that vision in Eptesicus bats is not sufficiently sharp for prey detection under normal conditions but only when the prey is unusually large and conspicuous. Nevertheless, the northern bat shows flexibility in prey-detection techniques not previously recognised among aerial-hawking bats. Key words: acoustic clutter, Hepialidae, insectivorous bats, nocturnality, ultrasound, visual acuity.

Submitted manuscript

Page 74: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

73

Introduction Functions normally served by vision in most vertebrates have been taken over by ultrasonic echolocation in insectivorous bats. In particular, the detection and tracking of flying insects is usually believed to be entirely acoustic (Kalko & Schnitzler 1993). Ultrasonic echolocation allows detection of very small targets, but its practical range is normally limited to a few metres, which is due to severe atmospheric attenuation and spreading loss of high-frequency sound and the poor reflective power of targets as small as insects (Lawrence & Simmons 1982, Kick 1982). On the other hand, although the eyes of insectivorous bats are small, they generally have good light-gathering capacity and good focal depth (Suthers 1970, Suthers & Wallis 1970). Vision can therefore be assumed to provide important cues particularly at ranges beyond that of echolocation, and is presumably useful for orientation and navigation at night. However, adaptation of the visual system for nocturnal conditions occurs partly at the expense of acuity, the ability to resolve details, and this presumably limits the use of vision for some short-range purposes such as finding prey

(Suthers 1970). Nevertheless, at least some bat species, particularly those that glean prey from surfaces and for which acoustic clutter (background echoes) makes echolocation less useful (Arlettaz et al. 2001), have a visual acuity that is good enough for detection of insects and other objects at close range (Bell 1985, Joermann et al. 1988). We recently discovered that vision also plays a role in prey detection by the northern bat Eptesicus nilssonii (Family Vespertilionidae), an aerial-hawking species (Eklöf et al. 2002). However, because aerial-hawking bats generally seem to have poor visual acuity (Suthers 1970), this can be expected to set a lower limit to the size of insects that can be detected by vision. Experimental setting and methods To determine the visual acuity for E. nilssonii foraging under practical conditions in the field, we took advantage of a natural situation where bats regularly exploit groups of male ghost swift moths Hepialus humuli (Hepialidae) displaying over hayfields during midsummer evenings in southern Sweden (57°N). These moths are silvery white and highly reflective on the dorsal side and display visually in hovering flight among the grass panicles to attract females (Andersson et al. 1998). Hepialids are unusual among larger moths in that they are earless and do not show any evasive response to bat echolocation calls, whether these are natural or synthetic (Rydell 1998). At two different moth display sites, each regularly patrolled simultaneously by up to 10 northern bats (which were not marked), we added dead and spread individuals to the naturally displaying moth population. The dead moths were glued on top of steel wires and presented pair wise about 2 m apart and 0.5-0.7 m above the grass in various parts of the fields. One moth in a pair had its white

Page 75: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

74

(dorsal) side up and the other had its dark grey (ventral) side up towards the patrolling bats. We assumed that the two were equally detectable by echolocation but that the white moth was more detectable by vision. This assumption was based on a previous experiment, showing that the moths´ silvery white coloration, which also contains a UV-component, is particularly contrasting during their natural display time just after sunset and against a background of green grass (Andersson et al. 1998). We thus expected the white and the dark moth to be attacked with equal frequency if bats use echolocation alone but with unequal frequency if they also use visual cues. To determine the minimum size of moths detectable by vision, we presented pairs of moths (one white and one dark) which were either intact (ca. 6 cm wingspan; Eklöf et al. 2002) or where both had the wingtips cut to give a total wingspan of either 5, 4 or 3 cm. Hence, size differed between the pairs of moths but the white and the dark moth that formed a pair were always of the same size. The moths were replaced when destroyed by the bats, but otherwise reused as long as possible. To prevent the bats from learning the exact positions of the moths, the pairs were moved at least a few metres following each attack by a bat. Hence each pair of moths was attacked only once while in each position. We deliberately presented the moths at a height where the bats´ echolocation would be complicated by clutter from the grass, overlapping with echoes from the moths, so that the bats were encouraged to use visual cues to find the moths. The extent of the ”clutter overlap zone”, which depends on the duration of the echolocation calls (7-8 ms), was 0.6-1.2 m above the grass (Jensen et al. 2001). Moths and bats were observed visually and also acoustically with a Pettersson D-940 bat detector from a distance of 2-10 m. The visual observations were facilitated by the relatively good light conditions prevailing at 57°N around midsummer (June 2002), which always made it possible to see what happened in sufficient detail. The experiments were performed only as long as moths were displaying naturally nearby, which occurred for about 30 minutes each evening (Andersson et al. 1998). Results Neither bats nor moths showed any obvious response to our presence. The bats seemed to forage normally, perhaps because they had become habituated to our presence over several seasons. The bats typically patrolled in large circles over the field at a height of 3-4 m (mean 3.5 m). The height was determined by using a measured and marked lamppost at the edge of the field as a reference. The bats always emitted echolocation calls during the search as well as throughout the attacks on the moths. An attacking bat typically performed a rapid and more or less vertical dive towards the grass while switching from search phase echolocation calls to a typical “feeding-buzz”, i.e. short pulses and high pulse repetition rate (Jensen et al. 2001). This behaviour strongly suggests that the attacks consistently were guided by echolocation. We counted the number of attacks on white and dark moths and compared the results for each moth size using one-tailed chi square statistics. Attacks on white moths were more frequent than on dark moths when the moths were 5 cm or larger (Fig. 1), suggesting that the detection was facilitated by vision in these

Page 76: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

75

cases. The detection of 4 cm and 3 cm moths were apparently not facilitated by vision and therefore must have been guided entirely by echolocation. We expected the total number of attacks on large moths to be more frequent than on smaller moths, because the larger size presumably increased the chance of detection. Although this appeared to be the case, the absolute attack frequency (the number of attacks per bat) was difficult to measure because the number of bats searching for moths over the field changed constantly. Discussion Because 5 cm moths were detected visually at a range of 3.5 m, the distance between the wing tips of the moths represents 49´ of arc. This agrees very well with theoretical estimates of visual acuity based on counts of retinal ganglion cells, suggesting 40´ of arc (Pettigrew et al. 1998, Koay et al. 1988) and behavioural tests of the optomotor response, suggesting at least 1° of arc, in the closely related species Eptesicus fuscus from North America (Bell & Fenton 1986). Unpublished optomotor response tests of other Eptesicus species, namely E. capensis and E. zuluensis from southern Africa, suggest that these species have a visual acuity of at least 54´of arc (M. B. Fenton & C. V. Portfors, personal communication). Our experiment is the first estimate of the visual acuity of E. nilssonii. The visual acuity of Eptesicus spp. appears to be intermediate among bats. It is much better than in many other aerial-hawking insectivores, e.g. Myotis spp. (3-6°) (Suthers 1966) but not as good as that of some gleaning insectivores, e.g. Macrotus californicus and Antrozous pallidus (4´ and 15´, respectively) (Bell & Fenton 1986). It is comparable to that of vampires and frugivores of the family Phyllostomidae (42´-16´) and insectivores of the family Emballonuridae (42´-23´) (Pettigrew et al. 1998, Suthers 1966, Manske & Schmidt 1976). Why the visual acuity differs so much among species and genera of bats is not clear. The repertoire of detection techniques used by northern bats searching for insects is wide. E. nilssonii usually feeds on swarming insects in open air (Rydell 1989), where echolocation is relatively straightforward and insects or swarms of insects can be detected through single echoes, so called “glints”. Insects that move rapidly near vegetation, so that acoustic clutter masks the echoes from the insects, are detected through their shift in position relative to the background. This technique obviously requires comparison of the echoes containing both the target and the background between several successive pulses (Jensen et al. 2001). When the insects stay among clutter and do not move relative to the background, as in the present case, few echolocation cues are available and the bats apparently employ vision to enhance the detection. We have shown previously that E. nilssonii do not make use of the Doppler-effects induced by the wing movements of the hovering moths (Eklöf et al. 2002).

However, if vision is useful or not in a particular foraging situation depends not only on the size of the target and the range, but presumably also on the contrast between the target and the background and the prevailing light conditions (Andersson et al. 1998, Ellins & Masterson 1974). In our case, the prey insects were much larger than most other prey eaten by this species (Rydell 1989) and they also displayed an unusually high contrast against the background

Page 77: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

76

(Anderson et al. 1998). Hence, the use of vision for prey detection is probably unusual in this species, and we can therefore assume that it normally relies on echolocation alone for this purpose. Nevertheless, our study shows that echolocating bats are flexible and ready to use whatever information is available to find food, and, assuming that the visual acuity of E. nilssonii is similar to that of E. fuscus, we find that these bats are able to use their full visual capacity in the field.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the landowners whose hay fields we partly devastated and T. Tranefors for practical help and M. B. Fenton and C. V. Portfors for giving access to unpublished data. The work was funded by the Science Research Council of Sweden. References Andersson S, Rydell J, Svensson MGE (1998) Light, predation and the lekking

behaviour of the ghost swift Hepialus humuli (L.) (Lepidoptera: Hepialidae). Proc R Soc Lond B 265: 1345-1351

Arlettaz R, Jones G & Racey PA (2001) Effect of acoustic clutter on prey detection by bats. Nature 414: 742-745 Bell GP (1985) The sensory basis of prey location by the California leaf-nosed

bat Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16: 343-348

Bell GP, Fenton MB (1986) Visual acuity, sensitivity and binocularity in a gleaning insectivorous bat, Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Anim Behav 34: 409-414

Eklöf J, Svensson AM, Rydell J (2002) Northern bats (Eptesicus nilssonii) use vision but not flutter-detection when searching for prey in clutter. Oikos 99: 347-351

Ellins SR, Masterson FA (1974) Brightness discrimination thresholds in the bat Eptesicus fuscus. Brain Behav Evol 9: 248-263 Jensen ME, Miller LA, Rydell J (2001) Detection of prey in clutter by the northern bat, Eptesicus nilssonii. J Exp Bio 204: 199-208 Joermann G Schmidt U, Schmidt C (1988) The mode of orientation during flight and approach to landing in two Phyllostomid bats. Ethology 78: 332-340 Kalko EKV, Schnitzler HU (1993) Plasticity of echolocation signals of

European pipistrelle bats in search flight: implications for habitat use and prey detection. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 33: 415-428

Kick S (1982) Target-detection by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. JComp Physiol A 145: 432-435 Koay G, Kearns D, Heffner HE, Heffner RS (1998) Passive sound-localization ability of the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus). Hearing Res 119: 37-48

Page 78: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

77

Lawrence BD, Simmons JA (1982) Measurements of atmospheric attenuation at ultrasonic frequencies and the significance for echolocation by bats. J Acoust Soc Am 71: 585-590

Manske U, Schmidt U (1976) Untersuchungen zur optischen Musterunterscheidung bei der Vampirfledermaus, Desmodus rotundus. Z Tierpsychol 49: 120.

Pettigrew JD, Dreher B, Hopkins CS, McCall MJ, Brown M (1988) Peak density and distribution of ganglion cells in the retinae of microchiropteran bats: Implications for visual acuity. Brain Behav Evol 32: 39-56

Rydell J (1989) Food habits of northern (Eptesicus nilssoni) and brown long- eared (Plecotus auritus) bats in Sweden. Holarct Ecol 12: 16-20 Rydell J (1998) Bat defence in lekking ghost swift (Hepialus humuli), a moth without ultrasonic hearing. Proc R Soc Lond B 265: 1373-1376 Suthers RA (1966) Optomotor responses by echolocating bats. Science 152, 1102-1104 Suthers RA (1970) Vision, olfaction and taste. In: Wimsatt WA (ed) Biology of Bats vol. II. Academic Press, New York, pp 265-281 Suthers RA, Wallis NE (1970) Optics of the eyes of echolocating bats. J Vision Res 10: 1165-1173.

Page 79: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

78

Figure 1. - Frequency of attacks by northern bats Eptesicus nilssonii on dead and spread moths Hepialus humuli mounted on top of wires and presented to foraging bats in a field among naturally occurring moths. Moths were cut to different sizes (wingspans) and displayed pair wise, one showing its white dorsal side up and the other the dark grey ventral side. Higher attack frequency on white than on dark moths indicates that the bats detected the moths using visual cues. The asterisk indicates that the 6 cm moths were not cut, but presented at their natural size (mean 6 cm).

10

50

3cm4cm6cm* 5cm

Atta

cks

on m

oths

(%

)

Moth size

100

n=45

n.s

n=51

n.s

n=62

P<0.001n=56

P<0.01

Page 80: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

79

Page 81: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

80

“And Bats flew round in fragrant skies and wheel'd or lit the flimsy shapes that haunt the dusk; with ermine capes and woolly breasts, and beaded eyes.” ?- Alfred Tennyson

IV

Page 82: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

81

Page 83: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

82

Visual acuity and eye size in four species of insectivorous bats Johan Eklöf Zoology Department, Göteborg University, Box 463, SE-405 30 Göteborg, Sweden, E-mail: [email protected] Abstract

Behavioural tests on optomotor responses establish a visual acuity threshold in four species of bats of the family Vespertilionidae. Three species of Myotis spp., which are aerial-hawking bats, responded only to a stripe pattern equivalent to 5 degrees of arc, whereas Plecotus auritus, which is a gleaner, responded down to 0.5 degrees of arc. Eye diameter was positively correlated with visual acuity, and varied from 0.9 mm in Myotis mystacinus to 1.8 mm in Plecotus auritus. These results are consistent with earlier findings. The variation in eye size and visual acuity presumably reflects differences in foraging technique (aerial-hawking vs. gleaning) and, in particular, how vision is used as a complement to sonar. Key words: Chiroptera, grating, optomotor response, resolving power, spatial resolution, vision Introduction

The microchiropteran eyes are generally adapted for nocturnal conditions in that they have large corneal surfaces and lenses relative to the size of the eye, and generally large receptor fields, which give them good light gathering power at the expense of acuity, i.e. the ability to resolve fine spatial details (Suthers 1970; Suthers & Wallis 1970). Bat eyes are generally better suited for long- than short distance operation, and due to the short effective range of sonar, vision is probably of major importance in guidance over longer distances (Griffin 1958, 1970). Loss of vision drastically reduces the homing performance in many bats (Williams et al. 1966, Hassell 1963, 1966, Davis & Barbour 1970). At least some bats are able to use vision over short distances as well, for example during escape and obstacle avoidance (Chase 1981, 1983, Chase & Suthers 1969, Bradbury & Nottebohm 1969). There is also evidence that some species of bats use visual cues to find prey (Bell 1985, Grant 1991, Vaughan & Vaughan 1996, Eklöf et al. 2002), a task which presumably requires relatively fine detail discrimination. Visual acuity has been estimated theoretically, based on counts of the number of retinal ganglion cells, in several species of bats (Marks 1980, Pettigrew et al. 1988, Heffner et al. 2001), and has shown a large range of variation; from 16’ of arc in the gleaning carnivorous species Macroderma gigas (Megadermatidae) to 1.4° of arc in Rhinolophus rouxi (Rhinolophidae), an insectivorous flutter-detector (Pettigrew et al. 1988). Optomotor response tests have also shown that

Manuscript

Page 84: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

83

the visual acuity varies considerably between species of bats (Suthers 1966, Manske & Schmidt 1976, Bell & Fenton 1986). The evidence thus suggests that visual acuity may be correlated with the food searching technique among bats. In particular, gleaners seem to have better visual acuity than those that catch insects in the air. The purpose of this study was to test this hypothesis by examining the optomotor response in some sympatric insectivorous vespertilionid bats that use different foraging techniques (gleaning and aerial-hawking), in order to establish a behavioural visual acuity threshold for these particular species. We also tested the assumption that visual acuity is positively related to the size of the eyes among insectivorous bats. Materials and methods

The experiments were performed at the old magnetite mine of Taberg, located south of Jönköping (57ºN) in southern Sweden. The bats were caught in a mist net set outside the mine entrance. They were tested for optomotor responses immediately after capture or as soon they had come to rest. The tests were made outdoors in the evening between August and November 2002, and between March and April 2003. To achieve optomotor responses, we used a device similar to that employed by Suthers (1966) and Bell & Fenton (1986). A bat was placed in a 20 cm high, 10 cm diameter Plexiglas cylinder surrounded by a 30 cm high and 60 cm diameter, revolving drum (Fig. 1). The natural light was insufficient for direct observation of the response in most cases, so the study area was lit up by a 40 W light bulb placed ca. 2 m above and 5 m away from the set-up. This provided a light intensity of 0.1-0.7 lux inside the drum (Photometer IL1400A, International Light Inc.). The drum could be rotated freely and independently of the cylinder containing the bat. Sinusoidal grating patterns, i.e. stripes with continuously changing luminance from black to white, of different fineness was attached to the inside of the drum. The drum was then rotated around the bat by hand at ca. 5 rpm randomly in both directions, and the behaviour of the bat was observed from above. Using sinusoidal patterns instead of black and white stripes reduces the risk of optical illusions, which could otherwise elicit responses from the bats and thus make the results hard to interpret (D. Nilsson & E. Warrant personal comm.). We used six gratings with different stripe width (distance from white to white): 2.84 cm, 1.42 cm, 0.57 cm, 0.43 cm, 0.28 cm and 0.14 cm. From the bats´ point of view this is equivalent to subtending angles of 5°, 2.5°, 1°, 0.75° (45’), 0.5° (30’) and 0.25° (15’) of arc. When a response was achieved the grating was switched to a finer pattern until no response could be recorded, indicating that the bats no longer could resolve the pattern. At this point a wider pattern was reintroduced to make sure that the bats still responded to moving stripes. This also served as a control for responses to stimuli other than the stripes, such as noise originating from the drum. After testing optomotor responses, we photographed the bats, using a Pentax 645 camera, on 50 ASA medium format slide film. We held the bats by hand so that the face of the bat was perpendicular to the camera. A ruler was held next to the bats, providing us with a cm-scale. The photos were scanned and magnified 17x – 33x, and the eye size of the individual bats were measured from the

Page 85: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

84

computer screen. The bats were released outside the mine immediately after the photographs had been taken. Results

Sixteen individual bats belonging to four different species were caught and tested: Plecotus auritus, Myotis mystacinus, M. brandtii and M daubentonii. When tested, the bats typically moved about in the Plexiglas cylinder for a while before coming to rest, and they sometimes continued to move around during the tests. However, most bats unambiguously responded to the rotating stripes by moving their heads in a snappy, stereotyped manner, either following the rotational direction or the opposite direction, as described earlier by other authors (Suthers 1966; Bell & Fenton 1986). The results were relatively consistent within a species and genus but differed considerably between the two genera. The species of Myotis responded only to the largest pattern (5° of arc), while all the Plecotus auritus individuals except one responded down to the pattern equivalent to 1-0.5° of arc (Table 1). The eye size varied with visual acuity as expected (Table 1). The Myotis species had smaller eyes (ca. 1 mm diameter) than Plecotus auritus (ca. 1.7 mm). Discussion Visual acuity is highly variable among vespertilionid bats, which presumably reflects the extent to which bats of the different genera make use of vision and what they do with it. As might have been expected, the relatively big-eyed gleaner Plecotus auritus did much better than the aerial-hawking and trawling Myotis spp., which also had much smaller eyes. The reaction to the 5° but not to the 2.5° pattern by the Myotis species used in this study is consistent with an earlier investigation of another Myotis species, the little brown bat (M. lucifugus), which responded down to 3-6° (Suthers 1966). A visual acuity in this range suggests that these bats can only detect 5-9 cm objects at a distance of 1 m, and hence it seems unlikely that they can use vision to detect the insects that they eat. Prey items captured by any of these species are much smaller than this and they are presumably detected using sonar cues alone (Swift & Racey 1983, Kalko & Schnitzler 1989). However, vision could well be used to detect large objects at distances beyond the range of echolocation, i.e. objects important for orientation and navigation. Indeed, it has been shown that loss of vision drastically reduces the homing performance by other Myotis species, such as M. sodalis (Hassell 1963, 1966, Davis & Barbour 1970) and M. austroriparius (Layne 1967). Nevertheless, Bradbury and Nottebohm (1969) showed that hearing impaired M. lucifugus could avoid 2 mm wide strings in dim light, when the strings contrasted sharply against the background. Considering their visual acuity, it is unlikely that the bats could have seen the strings more than 5 cm away. Nevertheless, the results from this and other studies (Mueller 1966, 1968) suggest that vision may be important for normal flight behaviour in these bats, although contrast sensitivity might perhaps be more important that visual acuity in some cases.

Page 86: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

85

The brown long-eared bat Plecotus auritus responded to patterns equivalent to 30’ of arc, which means that this species should be able to detect objects as small as 0.9 cm at a distance of 1 m. Among the Vespertilionidae, only Antrozous pallidus has been shown to have a better resolving power (15’; Bell & Fenton 1986). These results and the fact that P. auritus has larger eyes than most Vespertilionids (Cranbrook 1963; Tab 1.) suggest that it should be possible for long-eared bats to detect prey sized objects visually. It typically feeds on relatively large insects including many moths and beetles (Swift & Racey 1983, Rydell 1989). As P. auritus is a gleaner and sometimes takes insects from leaves (Swift 1998), it faces potential problems with clutter and therefore use other sensory cues in addition to sonar. In fact, passive listening plays a major role in prey detection by P. auritus (Anderson & Racey 1991). These bats are exceptionally sensitive to sounds around 15 kHz, which is close to the frequencies emitted by insects moving in clutter (Coles et al. 1989). However, the long-eared bats may also use visual information when searching for prey. In a recent study on feeding behaviour, it was shown that P. auritus preferred to use visual cues to sonar when possible, and that they could detect ca. 2 cm long mealworms visually (Eklöf & Jones, in press). Visual acuity has previously been tested in a number of species (Table 2), both behaviourally by optomotor response tests (Suthers 1966, Bell & Fenton 1986), and theoretically by counting the number of retinal ganglion cells (Koay et al. 1998, Heffner et al. 2001). Both methods give indications of the minimum separable angles, i.e. the minimum distance between two points that an animal need in order to be able to separate them. The acuity values estimated by counting retinal ganglion cells tend to be higher than those estimated from behavioural studies, suggesting that the anatomical method gives a theoretical threshold, rather than what the bats actually respond to. Nevertheless, although the acuity values obtained from the different methods are roughly in the same order, comparisons across the two methods should be made with care. As shown by the literature data presented in Table 2, frugivorous and nectarivorous bats seem to have better spatial resolution than most insectivorous species. Nevertheless, the finest spatial resolution in any bat (3’38’’) is found in the gleaning insectivore Macrotus californicus (Phyllostomidae), and this happens to be the only bat known to find prey, using vision alone (Bell 1985, Bell & Fenton 1986). Indeed, gleaning insectivores may have better visual acuity than aerial-insectivores in general, and this suggests that the aerial-hawking insectivores rely mostly on echolocation rather than vision for detection of small targets, while the opposite may be true in gleaners. At the same time it seems as if, among aerial-hawking insectivores, Emballonuridae have better resolution than Vespertilionidae. The visual resolving power may depend on ambient light intensity. In the common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus, for example, the acuity drops from 48’ at a light intensity of 31 mL (ca. 310 lux) to over 2° in 4*10-4 mL (ca. 0.004 lux) (Manske & Schmidt 1976). Other bats, such as Macrotus californicus and Antrozous pallidus retain their visual acuity down to light levels as low as 2*10-4 mL (ca. 0.002 lux) (Bell & Fenton 1986). As a comparison, a light level of 0.1 lux is equivalent to light levels at full moon, and similar to the conditions in this

Page 87: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

86

study. On overcast nights the amount of light drops to 0.0001 lux (Ryer1997). Eptesicus fuscus responds optimally to brightness discrimination in ambient light levels around 10 lux (conditions equivalent to dusk or dawn) but performs well down to levels of 0.001 lux (Ellins & Masterson 1974). As the ambient illumination increases towards daylight conditions the visual sensitivity generally declines, although the light tolerance varies between species (Hope & Bhatnagar 1979). Bradbury & Nottebohm (1969) found that Myotis lucifugus avoids obstacles better under ambient illumination resembling dusk than in daylight, which also indicates that the eyes of microchiropteran bats work better in dim light than in bright light. Nevertheless, in a study on optomotor response (Fenton et al. unpublished), several bats responded to striped patterns of 0.9° (the narrowest available in the study) even in bright daylight. Ambient light levels and the way it is measured, if reported at all, varies between different optomotor response studies, which make the results somewhat hard to compare. Acknowledgements I wish to acknowledge Bengt Svensson for building the optomotor device, Lars-Erik Appelquist for making it possible to work at Taberg, Åsa Norén-Klingberg, Jens Rydell, Stefan Pettersson and Karl-Johan Börjesson for help in the field and comments on the manuscript. References Anderson, M. E. & Racey, P. A. 1991. Feeding behaviour of captive long eared-bats, Plecotus auritus. Animal Behaviour 42, 489-493. Baker, A. G. & Emerson, V. F. 1983. Grating acuity of the mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). Behaviour and Brain Research 8, 195-209. Bell, G. P. 1985. The sensory basis of prey location by the California leaf-nosed bat Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 16, 343-347. Bell, G. P. & Fenton, M. B. 1986. Visual acuity, sensitivity and binocularity in a gleaning insectivorous bat, Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Animal Behaviour 34, 409-414. Bradbury, J. & Nottebohm, F. 1969. The use of vision by the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus, under controlled conditions. Animal Behaviour 17, 480-485. Chase, J. 1972. The role of vision in echolocating bats. PhD Thesis, University of Indiana,Bloomington. Chase, J. 1981.Visually guided escape responses of microchiropteran bats. Animal Behaviour 29, 708-713. Chase, J. 1983. Differential responses to visual and acoustic cues during escape in the bat Anoura geoffroyi: cue preferences and behaviour. Animal Behaviour 31, 526-531. Chase, J. & Suthers, R. A. 1969. Visual obstacle avoidance by echolocating bats. Animal Behaviour 17, 201-207.

Page 88: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

87

Coles, R. B., Guppy, A., Anderson, M. E. & Schlegel, P. 1989. Frequency sensitivity and directional hearing in the gleaning bat, Plecotus auritus (Linnaeus 1758). Journal of Comparative Physiology A 165, 269-280. Cowey, A. & Ellis, C. M. 1967. Visual acuity of rhesus and squirrel monkeys. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology 64, 80-84 Cranbrook, The Earl of. 1963. Notes on the feeding habits of the long-eared bat. Transaction of Suffolk Natural History Society 11, 1-3. Davis, W. H. & Barbour, R. W. 1970. Homing in blinded bats (Myotis sodalis). Journal of Mammalogy 51, 182-184 Davis, R. & Barbour, R. W. 1965. The use of vision in flight by the bat Myotis sodalis. The American Midland Naturalist 74, 497-499 Eklöf, J., Svensson, A. M. & Rydell. J. 2002. Northern bats Eptesicus nilssonii use vision but not flutter-detection when searching for prey in clutter. Oikos 99, 347-351. Eklöf, J. & Jones, G. 2003. Use of vision in prey detection by brown long-eared bats Plecotus auritus. Animal Behaviour (in press). Ellins, S. R. and Masterson, F. A. 1974. Brightness discrimination thresholds in the bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Brain, Behaviour and Evolution 9, 248-263. Grant, J. D. A. 1991. Prey location by two Australian long-eared bats, Nyctophilus gouldi and N. geoffroyi. Australian Journal of Zoology 39, 45-56. Griffin, D. R. 1958. Listening in the dark. Yale University Press, New Haven. Griffin, D. R. 1970. Migration and homing of bats. In: Biology of Bats Vol. II (Wimsatt, W. A ed.). Academic Press, New York, pp. 233-264. Hassell, M. D. 1963. A study of homing in the Indiana bat, Myotis sodalis. Transactions of the Kentucky Academy of Science 24, 1-4. Hassell, M. D. 1966. The need of vision in homing by Myotis sodalis. Journal of Mammalogy 47, 356-357. Heffner, R. S., Koay, G. & Heffner, H. E. 2001. Sound localization in a new-world frugivorous bat, Artibeus jamaicensis: Acuity, use of binaural cues, and relationship to vision. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 109, 412-421. Hope, G. M. & Bhatnagar, K. P. 1979. Effect on light adaptation on electrical responses on the retina of four species of bats. Experentia 35, 1191-1192. Hughes, A. 1977. The topography of vision in mammals of contrasting life style: Comparative optics and retinal organisation. In: Handbook of sensory physiology vol VII/5. The visual system in vertebrates (Crescitelli, F. Ed.). Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 613-756. Kalko, E. K. V. & Schnitzler, H.-U. 1989. The echolocation and hunting behavior of Daubenton’s bat, Myotis daubentoni. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 24, 225-238.

Page 89: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

88

Koay, G., Kearns, D., Heffner, H. E. & Heffner, R. S. 1998. Passive sound-localization ability of the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus). Hearing Research 119, 37-48. Layne, J. N. 1967. Evidence for the use of vision in diurnal orientation of the bat Myotis austroriparius. Animal Behaviour 15, 409-415. Manske, U. & Schmidt, U. 1976. Untersuchungen zur optischen Musterunterscheidung bei der Vampirfledermaus, Desmodus rotundus. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 49, 120. Marks, J. M. 1980. Retinal ganglion cell topography in bats. MA thesis, Indiana Univ., Bloomington, IN. Mueller, H. C. 1966. Homing and distance-orientation in bats. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 23, 403-421. Mueller, H. C. 1968. The role of vision in vespertilionid bats. The American Midland Naturalist 79, 524-525. Pettigrew, J. D., Dreher, B., Hopkins, C. S. McCall, M. J. & Brown, M. 1988. Peak density and distribution of ganglion cells in the retinae of microchiropteran bats: Implications for visual acuity. Brain, Behaviour and Evolution 32, 39-56. Rydell, J. 1989. Food habits of northern (Eptesicus nilssoni) and brown long- eared (Plecotus auritus) bats in Sweden. Holarctic Ecology 12, 16-20. Ryer, A. 1997. Light measurement handbook. International Light, Newburyport, MA. Suthers, R. A. 1966. Optomotor responses by echolocating bats. Science 152, 1102-1104. Suthers, R. A. 1970. Vision, olfaction and taste. In: Biology of Bats Vol II (Wimsatt, W. A. ed.). Academic Press, New York, pp. 265-281. Suthers, R. A. & Wallis, N. E. (1970) Optics of the eyes of echolocating bats. Journal of Vision Research 10, 1165-1173 Swift, S. M. 1998. Long-eared bats. Poyser Natural History. London. Swift, S. M. & Racey, P. A. 1983. Resource partitioning in two species of vespertilionid bats (Chiroptera) occupying the same roost. Journal of Zoology London 200, 249-259. Vaughan, T. A. & Vaughan, R. P. 1986. Seasonality and the behaviour of the African yellow-winged bat. Journal of Mammalogy 67, 91-102. Williams, T. C., Williams, J. M. & Griffin, D. R. 1966. Homing ability of the neotropical bat Phyllostomus hastatus. Animal Behaviour 14, 468-473.

Page 90: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

89

Fig. 1. – The device used for the optomotor response tests, in which a bat is presented with rotating, striped patterns of different fineness. The bat responds to the revolving patterns by moving its head in a stereotype manner. The thickness of the stripes corresponds to the bats visual resolving power (acuity), measured as degrees (or minutes) of arc (illustrated by Olof Helje).

Page 91: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

90

Tab. 1. – Eye diameter and optomotor responses to patterns of different fineness in four species of insectivorous bats. The asterisks indicate that the 5o pattern was not tested and that the bat did not respond to finer patterns. Ambient Eye diameter Minimum Species Ind. light (lux) (mm) separable angle Plecotus auritus 1 0.6 1.6 45’

2 0.6 1.8 2.5° 3 0.6 not measured 1° 4 0.1 1.7 1° 5 0.1 1.7 45’ 6 0.7 not measured 30’

7 0.2 not measured 45’ 8 0.3 not measured 1° Myotis mystacinus 1 0.6 1.0 no response*

2 0.6 0.9 no response* 3 0.1 not measured 5°

Myotis brandtii 1 0.1 not measured 5°

2 0.1 not measured 5° Myotis daubentoni 1 0.1 1.2 5°

2 0.1 1.3 no response 3 0.3 not measured 5°

Page 92: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

91

Tab. 2. – Visual acuity expressed as degrees of arc in Microchiroptera obtained from previous studies. Behavioural acuity values come from optomotor responses, and theoretical values are calculated from the number of retinal ganglion cells. Acuity is the minimum separable angle, i.e. the best values for each species. Asterisks indicate that the ambient light level was not measured (or acuity was measured theoretically). For consistency, the values of visual acuity were sometimes converted from other units, used in the original paper. Light Visual Method Species (lux) acuity Author (behav/theor) a) Vespertilionidae; gleaning insectivores Macrotus californicus 0.002 3.6’ Bell & Fenton 1986 b Antrozous pallidus 0.004 15’ Bell & Fenton 1986 b b) Vespertilionidae; aerial-hawking and trawling insectivores Eptesicus fuscus * 1° Bell & Fenton 1986 b Eptesicus fuscus 40’-43’ Koay et al. 1998, Marks 1980 t Myotis lucifugus * 3-6° Suthers 1966 b Nyctophilus gouldi 50’ Pettigrew et al. 1988 t c) Emballonuridae; aerial-hawking insectivores Saccopteryx bilineata 29’ Pettigrew et al. 1988 t Saccopteryx leptura * 42’ Suthers 1966 b Taphozus georgianus 23’ Pettigrew et al. 1988 t d) Molossidae; aerial-hawking insectivores Molossus ater * 10° Chase 1972 b e) Rhinolophidae; flutter-detecting insectivores Rhinolophus rouxi 1.4° Pettigrew et al. 1988 t f) Megadermatidae; gleaning insectivores/carnivores Megaderma lyra 20’ Pettigrew et al. 1988 t Macroderma gigas 16’ Pettigrew et al. 1988 t g) Phyllostomatidae; frugivores and sanguivores Carollia perspicillata * 16’ Suthers 1966 b Anoura geoffroyi * 42’ Suthers 1966 b Artibeus jamaicensis 27’ Heffner et al. 2000 t Artibeus cinereus 22’ Pettigrew et al. 1988 t Desmodus rotundus * 42’ Suthers 1966 b Desmodus rotundus 3.1 48’ Manske & Schmidt 1976 b Desmodus rotundus 0.04 2.5° Manske & Schmidt 1976 b h) Other mammals Rattus (rat) * 20’ Heffner & Heffner 1992 t Canis (dog) * 3.6’ Heffner & Heffner 1992 t Felis (cat) * 2.7’ Hughes 1977 t Macaca (macaque) * 38’’ Cowey & Ellis 1967 b Homo (man) * 32’’ Hughes 1977 t

Page 93: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

92

“The bat that flits at close of Eve Has left the brain that won't believe.” - William Blake

V

Page 94: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

93

Page 95: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

94

Page 96: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

95

Page 97: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

96

Page 98: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

97

Page 99: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

98

Page 100: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

99

Page 101: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

100

“I'm waiting for the night to fall I know that it will save us all When everything's dark Keeps us from the stark reality” - Martin L Gore

VI

Page 102: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

101

Page 103: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

102

Page 104: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

103

Page 105: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

104

Page 106: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

105

Page 107: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

106

Page 108: Vision in echolocating bats - · PDF fileEklöf, J. Vision in echolocating bats Zoology Department, Göteborg University ... Fladdermössens ögon är vanligen små och kan verka obetydliga,

107