16
VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITY CHILDREN ANDREA F ABER TAYLOR,FRANCES E. KUO AND WILLIAM C. SULLIVAN University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, U.S.A. Abstract Children growing up in the inner city are at risk of academic underachievement, juvenile delinquency, teenage pregnancy, and other important negative outcomes. Avoiding these outcomes requires self-discipline. Self-dis- cipline, in turn, may draw on directed attention, a limited resource that can be renewed through contact with nature. This study examined the relationship between near-home nature and three forms of self-discipline in 169 inner city girls and boys randomly assigned to 12 architecturally identical high-rise buildings with vary- ing levels of nearby nature. Parent ratings of the naturalness of the view from home were used to predict children’s performance on tests of concentration, impulse inhibition, and delay of grati¢cation. Regressions indicated that, on average, the more natural a girl’s view from home, the better her performance at each of these forms of self-discipline. For girls, view accounted for 20% of the variance in scores on the combined self- discipline index. For boys, who typically spend less time playing in and around their homes, view from home showed no relationship to performance on any measure. These ¢ndings suggest that, for girls, green space immediately outside the home can help them lead more e¡ective, self-disciplined lives. For boys, perhaps more distant green spaces are equally important. # 2002 Academic Press Introduction Children growing up in the inner city are at risk of academic underachievement (Brooks-Gunn, 1986), juvenile delinquency (Berrueta-Clement, 1984), teen- age pregnancy (Furstenberg, 1976), and other impor- tant negative outcomes, with profound consequences for themselves, those around them, and society. Outcomes such as these often re£ect failures of self-regulation, or self-discipline (Baume- ister et al., 1994). Could a feature of the physical environment a¡ect inner city children’s capacity for self-discipline, and as a consequence, play a role in these outcomes? This paper explores whether children’s self-disci- pline might be enhanced by contact with nature. Previous research suggests that natural settings and views can help renew the psychological re- source used in deliberately directing attention. It has been proposed that self-discipline draws on this same resource (Kuo, 2000); if so, we would expect self-discipline to decline when this resource is de- pleted or fatigued, and we would expect self-disci- pline to improve when this resource is renewed. Thus, regular contact with natural settings and views might be expected to enhance children’s capa- city for self-discipline on a day-to-day basis. To test this possibility, this study tested for links between the view from home and three forms of self- discipline in children. Speci¢cally, it examined whether, in an inner city neighborhood, children with ‘greener’ views from home were better able to concentrate, inhibit initial impulses, and delay grat- i¢cation. Three Forms of Self-Discipline Concentrating, inhibiting initial impulses, and de- laying grati¢cation are each distinct and important forms of self-discipline. They are distinct forms of self-discipline in that each involves overriding dif- ferent, unhelpful tendencies. And they are impor- tant in that each seems likely to play a pivotal role in the course of a young person’s life. More speci¢- cally, each seems likely to playan important role in negotiating the risks faced by inner city children: Journal of Environmental Psychology (2001) 21, 000^000 0272-4944/02/000000 + 00$35.00/0 r 2001 Academic Press doi:10.1006/jevp.2001.0241, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROMINNER CITY CHILDREN

ANDREA FABER TAYLOR, FRANCES E. KUO ANDWILLIAM C. SULLIVAN

University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, U.S.A.

Abstract

Children growing up in the inner city are at risk of academic underachievement, juvenile delinquency, teenagepregnancy, and other important negative outcomes. Avoiding these outcomes requires self-discipline. Self-dis-cipline, in turn, may draw on directed attention, a limited resource that can be renewed through contact withnature. This study examined the relationship between near-home nature and three forms of self-discipline in169 inner city girls and boys randomly assigned to 12 architecturally identical high-rise buildings with vary-ing levels of nearby nature. Parent ratings of the naturalness of the view from home were used to predictchildren’s performance on tests of concentration, impulse inhibition, and delay of grati¢cation. Regressionsindicated that, on average, the more natural a girl’s view from home, the better her performance at each ofthese forms of self-discipline. For girls, view accounted for 20% of the variance in scores on the combined self-discipline index. For boys, who typically spend less time playing in and around their homes, view from homeshowed no relationship to performance on any measure. These ¢ndings suggest that, for girls, green spaceimmediately outside the home can help them lead more e¡ective, self-disciplined lives. For boys, perhaps moredistant green spaces are equally important. # 2002 Academic Press

Introduction

Children growing up in the inner city are at risk ofacademic underachievement (Brooks-Gunn, 1986),juvenile delinquency (Berrueta-Clement, 1984), teen-age pregnancy (Furstenberg, 1976), and other impor-tant negative outcomes, with profoundconsequences for themselves, those around them,and society. Outcomes such as these often re£ectfailures of self-regulation, or self-discipline (Baume-ister et al., 1994). Could a feature of the physicalenvironment a¡ect inner city children’s capacity forself-discipline, and as a consequence, play a role inthese outcomes?This paper explores whether children’s self-disci-

pline might be enhanced by contact with nature.Previous research suggests that natural settingsand views can help renew the psychological re-source used in deliberately directing attention. Ithas been proposed that self-discipline draws on thissame resource (Kuo, 2000); if so, we would expectself-discipline to decline when this resource is de-pleted or fatigued, and we would expect self-disci-

pline to improve when this resource is renewed.Thus, regular contact with natural settings andviews might be expected to enhance children’s capa-city for self-discipline on a day-to-day basis.To test this possibility, this study tested for links

between the view from home and three forms of self-discipline in children. Speci¢cally, it examinedwhether, in an inner city neighborhood, childrenwith ‘greener’ views from home were better able toconcentrate, inhibit initial impulses, and delay grat-i¢cation.

Three Forms of Self-Discipline

Concentrating, inhibiting initial impulses, and de-laying grati¢cation are each distinct and importantforms of self-discipline. They are distinct forms ofself-discipline in that each involves overriding dif-ferent, unhelpful tendencies. And they are impor-tant in that each seems likely to play a pivotal rolein the course of a young person’s life. More speci¢-cally, each seems likely to play an important role innegotiating the risks faced by inner city children:

Journal of Environmental Psychology (2001) 21, 000^000 0272-4944/02/000000+ 00$35.00/0r 2001 Academic Pressdoi:10.1006/jevp.2001.0241, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 2: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

academic underachievement, juvenile delinquency,and teenage pregnancy.Concentrating requires overcoming the tendency

for the mind to wander, and sustaining attentionalfocus despite distractions, boredom, frustration, orfatigue. As it involves directing one’s thoughts tothe topic at hand, concentration is the form of self-discipline that most clearly draws on our capacityto deliberately direct attention. The ability to con-centrate is important because it enables an indivi-dual to mentally ‘buckle down’ and stay on a tasklong enough to make progress and be e¡ective. Italso seems to enable an individual to completetasks more quickly. In children, chronic or acutede¢cits in concentration could result in valuabletime spent in less-than-e¡ective ways. A child toomentally fatigued to concentrate might spend count-less hours in front of books and assignments, yetlearn very little due to their inability to focus onthe task at hand. Indeed, inattentiveness is a signif-icant predictor of academic underachievement (e.g.Mantzicopoulos, 1995; Rowe, 1992).Inhibiting initial impulses1 requires overcoming

the tendency to jump to conclusions or to act on im-pulse. It involves overriding one’s initial response toa problem or situation, in order to consider alterna-tives or consider the potential costs and bene¢ts ofa course of action. The ability to inhibit initial im-pulses is important because it gives rise to moreprudent and cautious choices, and consequently,more prudent and cautious actions. Chronic oracute de¢cits in a child’s ability to inhibit impulsescan have serious, negative long-term repercussions.For example, a child too mentally fatigued to inhibitimpulses is more likely to give in to repeated o¡ersof a lit cigarette or other dangerous substance. Adiminished capacity to inhibit impulses could alsocause a child to accept a dare to jump from one bal-cony to the next, or to snatch an elderly woman’spurse. Consistent with this, impulsivity is consis-tently linked with risky behavior (Donohew et al.,2000; McCoul, 2000), aggression and violence(e.g. Hynan & Grush, 1986; Markovitz, 1995), anddelinquency (Lynam, 2000; Rigby, 1989; White,1994).Delaying grati¢cation requires overcoming impa-

tience and the tendency to favor short-term rewardsover long-term goals. It involves internalized stan-dards and morals. The ability to delay grati¢cationis important because reaching future goals often re-quires postponing immediate rewards. It assists theindividual in persisting at goal-oriented behaviorsfor the good of their future. Even a temporary de¢-cit in the ability to delay grati¢cation can have ma-

jor repercussions. For example, a temporaryinability to delay grati¢cation might lead a youngcouple to give in to immediate desires and engagein unprotected sex, rather than wait until they arebetter prepared. Consistent with this, poor abilityto delay grati¢cation is a signi¢cant predictor of un-planned pregnancy (Donoghue, 1993; Sha¡er et al.,1978).In sum, concentration, impulse inhibition, and de-

lay of grati¢cation may play pivotal roles in thecourse of a young person’s life. How might these vi-tal forms of self-discipline be enhanced by the pre-sence of natural elements immediately outside thehome? We suggest that each of these forms of self-discipline draws on a resource which can be re-newed by contact with nature F the capacity fordeliberate or self-directed attention. In the next sec-tion, we review the literature on how natural set-tings and views can renew directed attention; wethen consider why self-discipline might draw on thisresource.

How Natural Settings and Views Restore DirectedAttention

Both theory and evidence suggest that the resourceunderlying our capacity to direct attention can berenewed by contact with nature. Attention Restora-tion Theory (Kaplan, 1995; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989)builds on William James’ description of attention toprovide an explanation for why natural settings andviews might be expected to renew this resource.James observed that certain elements in the envir-onment are e¡ortlessly engaging, and draw on whathe called involuntary attention: ‘strange things,moving things, wild animals, bright things. . .’(James, 1962, p. 231). For those stimuli and situa-tions that do not e¡ortlessly engage us, he proposed,we draw on a voluntary form of attention, or what S.Kaplan (1995) calls directed attention.The mechanism underlying directed attention ap-

pears to behave like a mental muscle. With pro-longed or intense use, the capacity to deliberatelydirect attention becomes fatigued and performancedeclines (Cohen & Spacapan, 1978; Glosser & Good-glass, 1990). In Attention Restoration Theory,S. Kaplan proposed that stimuli that draw primar-ily on involuntary attention give directed attentiona chance to rest. Further, he noted that natural set-tings and views appear to draw on involuntary at-tention; as a consequence, contact with natureshould assist in recovery from the fatigue of direc-ted attention.

2 A. F. Taylor et al.

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 3: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

Evidence in Adults. A number of studies in adultpopulations support Attention Restoration Theory.Several studies have shown that nature draws uponinvoluntary attention (e.g. Kaplan, 1973, 1983;Kaplan & Talbot, 1983, Ulrich, 1981). In addition, anumber of other studies have shown that exposureto natural environments can be e¡ective in restor-ing directed attention from fatigue (Canin, 1991,Cimprich, 1990, Hartig et al., 1991; R. Kaplan, 2001;Kuo, 2001; Lohr et al., 1996; Miles et al., 1998; Ovitt,1996, Tennessen & Cimprich, 1995).Of the previous empirical studies linking nature

and directed attention, three are particularly rele-vant to the study presented here. These studies fo-cus on residential nature and residential views ofnature. In one study, residents randomly assignedto relatively ‘green’ high-rise apartment buildingsscored signi¢cantly higher on an objective measureof attention than did residents assigned to relatively‘barren’ buildings (Kuo, 2001). In another study, uni-versity students with ‘all natural’ or ‘mostly natural’views from their dormitory room windows scoredsigni¢cantly higher on two objective measures of di-rected attention than did residents with ‘mostlybuilt’ or ‘all built’ views (Tennessen & Cimprich,1995). And in a third study, residents of low-riseapartment buildings with window views of naturalelements or settings rated themselves as function-ing better on several indices thought to berelated to attention restoration (Kaplan, 2001).Thus, there is some reason to think that residentialviews of nature might prove restorative in thisstudy.

Evidence in Children. Numerous studies havelinked directed attention to nature and near-homenature in adults; very little research has been con-ducted with children. Although Attention Restora-tion Theory does not exclude children and it hasbeen suggested nature might support directed at-tention in children (Trancik & Evans, 1995), onlytwo empirical studies have examined this possibi-lity.Wells (2000) examined children who moved frompoor quality housing to better quality housing inbetter neighborhoods. Among these children, thosewhose move involved the greatest increase in naturehad the highest rated levels of attentional function-ing post-move. Another study provides three addi-tional pieces of evidence about the link betweennature and directed attention in children. Thatstudy revealed that exposure to nature throughgreen activity settings was related to better atten-tional functioning (reduced attention de¢cit symp-toms) in a population of children with Attention

De¢cit Disorder (Faber Taylor et al., 2001). In thatstudy, parents rated a variety of leisure activitieswith respect to whether those activities left theirchild’s attention de¢cit symptoms better than usual,worse than usual, or the same as usual: results indi-cated that children function better than usual afteractivities in green settings. Moreover, ratings werehigher for those activities conducted in green set-tings than for those conducted in built outdoor orindoor settings. In addition, the greener a child’susual play setting, the less severe their attentionde¢cit symptoms were rated in general. And mostrelevant to the current study, several measures ofresidential greenness were signi¢cantly and nega-tively linked to overall severity of symptoms F butonly for girls and not for boys. Multiple potentialconfounds were evaluated; none could explain therelationships between green settings and better at-tentional functioning.In sum, not only do theory and evidence suggest

that nature supports directed attention in adults,but there is some evidence that it does so in chil-dren as well. Moreover, there is evidence to suggestthat near-home nature and residential views of nat-ure can help renew directed attention.

Does Self-discipline Draw on Directed Attention?

Might self-discipline draw on directed attention,and hence, be renewed by contact with nature?More than one investigator has proposed that thecapacity for self-discipline is a limited but renew-able resource (Kuo, 2000; Muraven & Baumeister,2000). Perhaps it is no coincidence that both whatpersonality psychologists call ‘self-control strength’(Muraven & Baumeister, 2000) and what environ-mental psychologists call ‘directed attention’(Kaplan, 1995) are subject to the same patterns ofdecline and restoration F decline with overuseand renewal with rest. Kuo (2000) has proposed thatthe mental mechanism that underlies self-disciplineand the mental mechanism that underlies directedattention are one and the same.Although directed attention has been operationa-

lized primarily in terms of e¡ective cognitive per-formance (e.g. maintaining focus or payingattention, resisting distractions, planning, decisionmaking, remembering things), it is clear fromKaplan’s description that the mechanism he pro-poses may be involved in much more (Kaplan &Kaplan, 1989; Kaplan, 1995). In essence, Kaplanproposes a general control mechanism for directingany of a variety of di¡erent forms of mental activity,including thoughts, images, sensations, and

Nature and self Discipline 3

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 4: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

impulses. Thus, the mechanism for directing atten-tion may be involved in the inhibition of anystrong-but-unhelpful mental activity in favor of anyweak-but-helpful mental activity.Each of the three forms of self-discipline exam-

ined here could plausibly draw on this proposed me-chanism. Concentration involves both inhibitingdistractions and other task-irrelevant thoughts, andsupporting on-task thoughts. Similarly, inhibition ofimpulses may involve inhibiting initial impulses,blocking out the stimuli that give rise to those im-pulses, and supporting the consideration of alterna-tives. And delay of grati¢cation may involveinhibiting impulses, inhibiting unhelpful thoughtsand sensations that fan one’s desire for immediategrati¢cation (e.g. warm chocolate cake), and sup-porting thoughts about long term goals (e.g. weightloss).Consistent with this conception, a number of stu-

dies and reviews have linked voluntary or controlledaspects of attention to forms of self-discipline andself-regulation. Mischel and colleagues have shownthat children’s ability to direct attention away fromimmediate rewards is pivotal in their ability to de-lay grati¢cation (Mischel et al., 1972), and that ado-lescents’ attentiveness and ability to concentrate ispredicted by their ability to delay grati¢cation aspre-schoolers (Shoda et al., 1990). Two studies haveindependently linked aspects of attention to moredisciplined ways of dealing with anger or con£ict(Eisenberg et al., 1994; Kuo & Sullivan, 2001). In fac-tor analyses of questionnaire data, Rothbart et al.,(in press) have found a broad e¡ortful control factor,in which attentional focusing clusters with inhibi-tory control. Posner & Rothbart (2000) review litera-ture suggesting that high-level attentional networksprovide the neural basis for self-regulation. And¢nally, in their review of over 500 books and articleson self-regulation failure, Baumeister et al., (1994)conclude that loss of control over attention is a keyfactor in self-regulation failure.

This study

If nature renews directed attention in children, andif directed attention is indeed involved in self-disci-pline, as we suggest, then children’s self-disciplineshould be strengthened by contact with nature. Thisstudy examined whether near-home nature is re-lated to three forms of self-discipline in both girlsand boys. Speci¢cally, we asked

K Do residential views of nature enhancechildren’s concentration?

K Do residential views of nature enhancechildren’s inhibition of initial impulses? and

K Do residential views of nature enhancechildren’s delay of grati¢cation?

This study breaks new ground in two respects.First, previous research has linked concentrationto nature empirically, but only in adults with nor-mal attentional functioning and in children withcompromised attentional functioning. This study isthe ¢rst to examine the relationship between natureand concentration in a sample of children with nor-mal attentional functioning. And second, althoughnature and concentration have been linked in somepopulations, neither impulse inhibition nor delay ofgrati¢cation have been linked to nature in any po-pulation. The ¢ndings of two studies (Kuo &Sullivan, 2001; Kuo, 2001) are consistent with a linkbetween nature and self-discipline, but neither ofthese studies directly examined impulse inhibitionor delay of grati¢cation.To examine the relationship between residential

views of nature and concentration, impulse inhibi-tion, and delay of grati¢cation in children, we con-ducted one-on-one tests and interviews with asample of inner city girls and boys and theirmothers. Objective performance measures were usedto assess children’s concentration, inhibition of initi-al impulses, and delay of grati¢cation. Mothers’ rat-ings were used to assess the naturalness of viewsfrom home.

Methods

Site & Design

The site was Robert Taylor Homes, a large publichousing development in Chicago, Illinois, USA. Atthe time of this study, Robert Taylor Homes (RTH)comprised 28 16-story buildings. It had over 12,000o⁄cial residents, of whom 31% were children be-tween 5 and 14 years old (CHA, 1995). Almost all ofthe heads of household (99?7%) were African-Ameri-can and most (75%) received Aid to Families withDependent Children (CHA, 1995).The physical characteristics of RTH help make it

an optimal site for studying the e¡ects of near-homenature. When the development was built in the1960s, trees and grass were planted in the commonspaces next to every building. Over the years, forreasons of reducing maintenance and dust, grassin most of the spaces was replaced with pavement,causing many of the trees to die and subsequentlybe removed. This attrition has left some buildings

4 A. F. Taylor et al.

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 5: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

barren and others with pockets of green. While theamount of nearby nature varies from building tobuilding, the buildings themselves are nearly identi-cal in architecture, layout, size, and number of resi-dential units. Thus, many would-be confounds areheld constant at RTH, allowing for clean compari-sons of the e¡ects of near-home nature.The social characteristics of RTH also help make

it an optimal site for studying the e¡ects of near-home nature. The housing assignment practices ofChicago Housing Authority result in de facto ran-dom assignment of residents to buildings, and resi-dents are not involved in landscaping decisions ormaintenance. Previous research at this site with adi¡erent sample of residents found no systematic re-lationships between levels of vegetation outsideapartment buildings and residents’ age, education,marital status, work status, income, Aid to Familieswith Dependent Children status, number of childrenat home, length of residence, or numerous other fac-tors (Kuo & Sullivan, 2001).

Participants & Procedures

To boost rapport between the participants and in-terviewers, we hired and trained residents of RTHas interviewers. The four interviewers were Afri-can-American women between 30 and 45 years old.Each had achieved at least a high school diploma.The interviewers received 40 hours of training in in-terviewing and administrating objective perfor-mance measures from our sta¡ and the NationalOpinion Research Center.In order to minimize distractions to interview

participants during the interview, we also hiredand trained residents to serve as child-care provi-ders. Child care providers accompanied the inter-viewers to the interviews and kept any children inthe apartment who were not being interviewed safeand entertained. All child care providers were atleast 18 years old and were completing or had com-pleted high school.Twelve apartment buildings with varying

amounts of vegetation were sampled; we excludedbuildings adjacent to parks, police stations or otherrelatively unique features.Within the selected build-ings, sampling was limited to the 2nd, 3rd, and 4th£oors because those £oors provide residents maxi-mal views of the trees and grass outside their build-ing; there are no residences on the ground £oor.To recruit participants, £yers were posted and in-

terviewers canvassed door-to-door. Interviewers didnot canvas or interview in the building in whichthey lived, and they were instructed not to inter-

view anyone with whom they were acquainted. Par-ent-child pairs were invited to participate in aUniversity of Illinois study about ‘the physical envir-onment of the neighborhood and how it a¡ectsmothers and children’. Any 7^12 year old child andtheir mother or primary caregiver was eligible toparticipate, so long as they had been residents ofRTH for at least a year. Potential participants weretold that they could refuse to answer any question,and could stop the interview at any time. Adults re-ceived >10 and children received a small gift at thecompletion of the interview.Of the eligible adult-child pairs approached, 169

of 174 agreed to participate F a 97% response rate.Ninety one of the child participants were boys; 78were girls. Both the boys’ and girls’ mean ages were9?6 years old (ranges 7?7^11?7 and 7?7 to 12?2 yearsold, respectively). All participants were African-American.Interviews and testing were conducted in partici-

pants’ apartments at the kitchen table. Adult inter-views and testing typically lasted a little morethan an hour. Child interviews and testing typicallylasted 45 minutes.

Measures

We measured near-home nature and three types ofself-discipline: concentration, inhibition of initialimpulses, and delay of grati¢cation.

Near-home nature. Near-home nature was assessedby asking the adult participants to rate the viewsfrom their apartment windows. Ratings in responseto two items were combined: ‘How much of the viewfrom your window is of nature (trees, plants,water)?’ and ‘How much of your view from your win-dow is man-made (buildings, street, pavement)?’ (re-verse-scored). Each item was rated on a ¢ve-pointscale, from 0 ‘not at all’ to 4 ‘very much’. Figure 1shows barren and green areas immediately outsideRTH apartment buildings.

Concentration. Concentration was assessed usingfour tasks. These tasks have previously been usedas measures of attention or concentration: SymbolDigit Modalities Test (Cimprich, 1992, Lezak, 1976;Smith, 1968), Digit Span Backwards (Cimprich1992; Wechsler, 1955), Alphabet Backwards(Cimprich, 1992), and Necker Cube Pattern Control(Cimprich, 1990; Schwartz, 1994; Tennessen &Cimprich, 1995). Phenomenologically, each of thesetasks is characterized by the e¡ortful use of atten-tion or paying attention.

Nature and self Discipline 5

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 6: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

In Symbol Digit Modalities (SDM), the partici-pant substitutes numbers for nine geometric sym-bols, including three mirror image pairs, as quicklyas possible (Smith, 1973). Scores on SDM were thenumber of correct substitutions in a 90-s period.One participant’s score was more than 2 S.D. higherthan the next highest score; this outlier was ex-cluded from further analysis.In Digit Span Backwards (DSB), the participant

listens to a sequence of numbers two to eight digitslong and then repeats the sequence aloud in reverseorder (Wechsler, 1955). Scores on DSB were the long-est number of digits repeated correctly before twoconsecutive failed trials.In Alphabet Backwards (ABK), the participant

recites the alphabet backwards beginning with aspeci¢ed letter (e.g. the letter u) (Cimprich, 1992).In this study, three trials were given; scores werethe average number of letters recited in correct (re-verse) sequence divided by the average time spentreciting them (i.e. the average speed with whichthe participant could recite the alphabet back-wards).In Necker Cube Pattern Control (NCPC), the par-

ticipant attempts to mentally ‘hold on to’ one inter-pretation of an ambiguous stimulus (Tennessen &Cimprich, 1995). First, the participant stares at athree-dimensional line drawing of a cube for 30 s,signaling each time the front and back faces ap-pears to reverse. Then, the participant tries to men-tally ‘hold the cube still’ or inhibit it from reversingfor 30 s, signaling each time the faces reverse. Scor-ing for this measure was the percent reduction inthe number of reversals from the ¢rst task F let-ting the cube reverse freely F to the second taskF holding the cube still. Scores were based on per-formance of the two tasks after a practice trial.Scores on SDM, DSB, ABK, and NCPC were stan-

dardized and averaged to create a summary index of

concentration. Z-scores were used because the fourtasks were scored on very di¡erent scales.

Inhibition of initial impulses. Inhibition of initialimpulses was assessed by combining scores on threeestablished measures of impulsivity or impulse inhi-bition: Matching Familiar Figures Test (e.g.Welsh etal., 1991; Brown & Quay, 1977; Kagan, 1966), StroopColor-Word Test (Boucugnani & Jones, 1989; Davieset al., 1984; Dyer, 1973), and Category Matching(Melnyk & Das, 1992). Each of these tasks tends toevoke an initial response that is incorrect or verylikely to be incorrect. In each of these tasks, goodperformance requires avoiding the initial incorrectresponse in order to discern the correct response.In Matching Familiar Figures (MFF), the partici-

pant is presented with a target ¢gure and a set ofsix alternatives; the task is to select the single alter-native that exactly matches the target ¢gure(Kagan, 1966). Because all the alternatives all lookthe same at ¢rst glance, participants must be care-ful in evaluating them. For each trial, the number oferroneous choices a participant makes before select-ing the correct alternative is recorded. In this study,a participant’s score on the measure was the totalnumber of errors over 12 trials.MFF has been foundto be a reliable measure: reliability for total numberof errors ranges from 0?62 (Block et al., 1974) to 0?78(Cairns & Cammock, 1978). Matching FamiliarFigures has also been found to be a valid measureof impulsivity (Brown & Quay, 1977; although cf.Block et al., 1974).In the Stroop Color-Word Test (Stroop), the parti-

cipant is given a sheet of paper with 50 color namespresented in rows (Dodrill, 1978). Each color name isprinted in incongruent ink colors; e.g. the word redmight be printed in green ink. The participant is¢rst asked to read each of the words on the pagealoud, and then asked to name the ink color of each

FIGURE 1. Views of near-home nature vary from apartment to apartment at Robert Taylor Homes.

6 A. F. Taylor et al.

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 7: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

word on the page. The challenge of this task is toavoid the initial impulse to read the words ratherthan name the ink colors. In this study, a partici-pant’s score was the number of ink colors namedcorrectly on ¢rst attempt.In Category Matching (CM), the participant is

presented with a sheet containing 84 pairs oficons (Schwartz, 1994; adapted from Melnyk &Das, 1992). The participant evaluates pairs oficons, attempting to circle only those pairs in whichthe two icons belong to the same conceptual cate-gory. Twenty-one of the pairs are target pairs, whilethe remaining 63 are distractor pairs. The challengeof this task is in resisting the impulse to circlepairs in which the icons are similar in form butnot in conceptual category. A participant’s scorewas the number of pairs evaluated in 30 s less anyerrors.We created a summary index of inhibition of initi-

al impulses by averaging the z-scores of MFF(reverse-scored), Stroop, and CM.

Delay of grati¢cation. A version of Rodriguez et al.,(1989) task was used to assess children’s capacity todelay grati¢cation. In this task, the challenge is toresist an immediate, smaller reward in favor of adelayed but larger reward. The participant is ¢rstasked which of two kinds of candy they prefer. Then,they are shown a very large and a very small bag oftheir preferred candy, and told that if they can waitlong enough, they can have the larger bag; other-wise, they will receive the smaller bag. The test ad-ministrator then instructs the child to wait quietlywith their eyes closed and leaves the room, takingthe candy with her (cf. Rodriguez et al., 1989). Scoreson this task were the total time waited, with a max-imum score of 15min.

Results

Results are presented in four parts. We begin bypresenting preliminary analyses suggesting thatthe relationship between near-home nature andself-discipline should be examined separately bygender. We then examine relationships betweennear-home nature and self-discipline for girls andboys. Finally, we address the potential role of agedi¡erences in the relationship between nature andself-discipline.

Preliminary analyses: Should girls and boys beanalyzed separately?

Previous research has hinted at gender di¡erencesin the e¡ects of near-home nature on children(Faber Taylor et al., 2001). To determine whether thee¡ects of near-home nature on self-discipline wouldbest be analysed separately for girls versus boys, weconducted a number of preliminary analyses.First, we used independent t-tests to examine gen-

der di¡erences in self-discipline. Did the girls andboys in this study di¡er in their performance onthe three forms of self-discipline? As Table 1 shows,there are gender di¡erences on each of the threeforms of self-discipline tested, with girls outper-forming boys on two forms and boys outperforminggirls on the third. Girls’ scores are signi¢cantlyhigher on concentration and marginally signi¢-cantly higher on impulse inhibition (p=0?08); boys’scores are signi¢cantly higher on delay of grati¢ca-tion.These ¢ndings suggest that it would be prudent

to take gender into account in testing for links be-tween nature and self-discipline. To do so, we con-ducted 2� 2 factorial ANOVAs examining the

TABLE 1Means, standard deviations, and mean comparisions between girls and boys on measures of self-discipline

Means Standard Deviations

Girlsz Boys** Girls Boys t p

Concentrating* 0?15 �0?12 0?58 0?52 3?24 o0?01Inhibiting impulsesw 0?09 �0?09 0?69 0?62 1?79 0?08Delay of grati¢cationz 358 454 309 325 �1?95 0?05Self discipline} 0?03 �0?02 0?53 0?48 0?65 ns

*Concentration summary=average of z-scores on four constituent measureswInhibition of impulses summary=average of z-scores on 3 constituent measureszDelay of grati¢cation scores=total time waited in seconds}Self-discipline summary=average of three z-scores: concentration summary, inhibition summary, and delay of grati¢ca-tion.zn=78**n=91

Nature and self Discipline 7

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 8: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

e¡ects of gender and nature on self-discipline. Inparticular, we were interested in whether any ef-fects of nature might be moderated by gender. In-deed, consistent with previous research, gender bynature interactions emerged for each of the threeforms of self-discipline. Findings indicated that girlsdi¡ered from boys signi¢cantly in the e¡ect of near-home nature on concentration, F(1,165) = 5?7,po0?05, and delay of grati¢cation, F(1, 165) = 5.4,po0?05. Girls di¡ered from boys marginally signi¢-cantly in the e¡ect of nature on impulse inhibition,F(1,165) = 3?6, p=0?06.Accordingly, we examined the relationships

between near-home nature and each of the threeforms of self-discipline separately for girls and forboys.

Near-home nature and self-discipline in girls

Concentration. If near-home nature enhances thisform of self-discipline in girls, we might expect girlswith greener views to perform better, overall, atSymbol Digit Modalities, Alphabet Backwards,Necker Cube Pattern Control, and Digit Span Back-wards.We used a simple OLS regression to examinethe relationship between parent-rated naturalnessof apartment view and a summary index of thesefour measures of concentration.Do girls with greener views perform better at

tests of concentration? Yes. On average, the greenera girl’s view from home, the better she concentrates.As Figure 2 shows, there is a strong positive linearrelationship between naturalness of apartment viewand girls’ performance on the summary index of con-centration, F(1,76) = 10?9, po0?01, and each of theconstituent measures echo this pattern. For each

scale point di¡erence in rated greenness of view(for example, from 0 ‘not at all’ to 1 ‘a little’), perfor-mance increases by roughly a quarter of a standarddeviation, beta= 0?233. Greenness of view explainsapproximately one-eighth of the variance in concen-tration scores, R-squared= 0?126.

Inhibition of initial impulses. If near-home natureenhances this form of self-discipline in girls, wemight expect girls with greener views from hometo perform better, overall, at Matching FamiliarFigures Test, Stroop Color-Word Test, and CategoryMatching. We used a simple OLS regression to ex-amine the relationship between naturalness ofapartment view and a summary index combiningthese three measures of impulse inhibition.Do girls with greener views perform better at

tests of impulse inhibition? Yes. On average, thegreener a girl’s view from home, the e¡ective she isat inhibiting impulses. As Figure 3 shows, there is apositive relationship between naturalness of viewand girls’ performance on the summary index ofthese three measures; and again, the constituentmeasures echo this pattern. Naturalness of apart-ment view signi¢cantly and positively predicts im-pulse inhibition, F(1, 76) = 3?8, p=0?05. Greennessof view explains roughly 5% of the variance inimpulse inhibition scores, R-squared= 0?048, with abeta of 0?172.

Delay of grati¢cation. If near-home nature enhancesthis form of self-discipline in girls, we mightexpect girls with greener views from home to per-form better on the Mischel delay of grati¢cationtask.Are girls with greener views more able to resist

the temptation of an immediate-but-smaller reward?

−1.5

−1.0

−0.5

0

0.5

1.0

1.5

2.0

2.5

0 1 2 3 4

Naturalness of apartment view

2.5

0 1 2 3 4−1.5

2.5

AB

K

0 1 2 3 4

−1.5

−1.5

2.5

NC

PC

0 1 2 3 4−1.5

2.5

SDM

0 1 2 3 4

DSB

Con

cent

ratio

n

FIGURE 2. OLS regression of naturalness of view on the summary measure of girl’s concentration (left) and its four constituent mea-sures. All scores are standardized.

8 A. F. Taylor et al.

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 9: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

Yes. On average, the greener a girl’s view fromhome, the longer she is able to delay grati¢cation.As Figure 4 shows, there is a strong positive rela-tionship between naturalness of view and perfor-mance on this task. Naturalness of apartment viewsigni¢cantly and positively predicts delay of grati¢-cation, F(1, 76) = 12?7, po0?001. For each point di¡er-ence in rated greenness of view (for example, from 0‘not at all’ to 1 ‘a little’), performance increases byalmost half of a standard deviation, beta= 0?417.Greenness of view explains roughly one-seventh of

the variance in impulse inhibition scores, R-squared= 0?143.

Combined self-discipline measure. To further test therelationship between near-home nature and girls’self-discipline, we created a single index combiningscores on the three forms of self-discipline. Do girlswith greener views perform better, overall, on thesethree forms of self-discipline? Yes. As Figure 5shows, view from home strongly and positivelypredicts girls’ scores on this combined measure,

−1.5

−1.0

−0.5

0

0.5

1.0

1.5

2.0

2.5

0 1 2 3 4

Naturalness of apartment view

−1.5

−1.5

2.5

MFF

0 1 2 3 4

Naturalness

2.5

CM

0 1 2 3 4

Naturalness

2.5

Stro

op

0 1 2 3 4

Naturalness

Ιmpu

lse

inhi

bitio

n

FIGURE 3. OLS regression of naturalness of view on the summary measure of girl’s impulse inhibition (left) and its three constituentmeasures. All scores are standardized.

−1.50 1 2 3 4

−1.0

−0.5

0

0.5

1.0

1.5

2.0

2.5

Naturalness of apartment view

Del

ay o

f gr

atif

icat

ion

FIGURE 4. OLS regression of naturalness of view on girl’s delay ofgrati¢cation. Delay of grati¢cation scores are standardized.

−1.5

−1.0

−0.5

0

0.5

1.0

1.5

2.0

2.5

0 1 2 3 4

Naturalness of apartment view

Self

-dis

cipl

ine

FIGURE 5. OLS regression of naturalness of view on the summarymeasure of girl’s self-discipline. Self-discipline scores are standar-dized.

Nature and self Discipline 9

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 10: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

F(1, 76) = 19?4, po0?0001. On average, the greener agirl’s view from home, the better she scores overallon di¡erent forms of self-discipline; for each pointdi¡erence in greenness of view, scores increase byroughly a quarter of a standard deviation,beta= 0?274. Greenness of view explains roughlyone-¢fth of the variance in self-discipline scores,R-squared= 0?203.

Near-home nature and self-discipline in boys

Table 2 summarizes the ¢ndings for the relationshipbetween near-home nature and self-discipline bygender. As a comparison between the left and righthalves of the table shows, the ¢ndings for boys standin startling contrast to the ¢ndings for girls.Whereas girls show consistent and often stronglinks between near-home nature and various formsof self-discipline, boys show only the barest hint ofsuch a link. Beta coe⁄cients for boys hover aroundzero for concentration, delay of grati¢cation, andthe combined self-discipline measure. For impulseinhibition, boys’ scores show a slight tendency to in-crease with naturalness of the view from home,beta= 0?116, but this relationship is not signi¢cant,p=0?13.

Age, near-home nature, and self-discipline

To address the potential role of age in this study, weconducted 2� 2 factorial ANOVAs (age � nature)for concentration, impulse inhibition, and delay ofgrati¢cation. Girls’ scores and boys’ scores were ana-lysed separately. Findings for girls showed, not sur-prisingly, a main e¡ect for nature view for each ofthe three forms of self-discipline. Girls’ concentra-tion showed a main e¡ect of nature view, F(1,74) = 17?3, po0?0001, as did girls’ impulse inhibition,F(1,74) = 4?9, po0?05 and girls’ delay of grati¢cation,F(1,74) = 8?6, po0?01. There was no signi¢cant maine¡ect for age, nor was there a signi¢cant interac-tion between age and nature for any of the threeforms of self-discipline.

Findings for boys showed, again, no main e¡ectfor nature view for any of the three forms of self-dis-cipline. There was a hint of a main e¡ect of age onconcentration, F(1,74)=2?8, p=0?10, but there were noother signi¢cant e¡ects for age on other forms ofself-discipline, and no signi¢cant interactions be-tween age and nature for any of the measures.These results indicate that the basic ¢ndings of

the study do not change when age is taken into ac-count: for girls, near-home nature is consistentlylinked to self-discipline; for boys, near-home natureis not linked to self-discipline.

Discussion

This study tested for possible links between near-home nature and children’s self-discipline, more spe-ci¢cally their capacities for concentration, impulseinhibition, and delay of grati¢cation. Because preli-minary analyses indicated gender di¡erences Fand, more importantly, interactions between genderand nature F for each of these three forms of self-discipline, we examined the relationship betweennature and self-discipline separately for girls andboys.For girls, views of near-home nature were system-

atically related to each of these three forms of self-discipline. Girls’ performance on each of the follow-ing measures was signi¢cantly and positivelyrelated to nature: a summary measure of concentra-tion (based on Symbol Digit Modalities, AlphabetBackwards, Necker Cube Pattern Control, and DigitSpan Backwards); a summary measure of impulseinhibition (based on Matching Familiar Figures,Stroop Color-Word Test, and Category Matching);Mischel’s delay of grati¢cation measure; and an in-dex combining the three forms of self-discipline. Dif-ferences in girls’ near-home nature explained 20% ofthe variance in overall self-discipline scores.Findings for boys stood in striking contrast to

those for girls.Whereas girls showed signi¢cant, po-sitive relationships between near-home nature and

TABLE 2OLS regression summaries for naturalness of apartment view on measures of self-discipline for girls and boys.

Girls (78) Boys (91)

R2 beta F p R2 beta F p

Concentrating 0?13 0?23 10?9 0?001 0?01 0?07 1?2 nsInhibiting impulses 0?05 0?17 3?8 0?05 0?01 0?12 2?3 0?13Delay of grati¢cation 0?14 0?42 12?7 o0?001 0?00 �0?03 0?6 nsSelf discipline 0?20 0?27 19?4 o0?0001 0?01 0?05 0?7 ns

10 A. F. Taylor et al.

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 11: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

each of the outcome measures, boys showed no sig-ni¢cant relationships between near-home natureand any of the outcomes. What might account forthese gender di¡erences?One possibility seems promising at ¢rst, but be-

comes less plausible on further inspection ^ thatnature restores directed attention in girls but notboys. First, there is no a priori theoretical reasonto expect these e¡ects to be limited to girls. Atten-tion Restoration Theory (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989;Kaplan, 1995) would suggest that nature supportsdirected attention in any individual with an intactattentional system. And consistent with this, theempirical work with adults suggests that the nat-ure-directed attention relationship is true for bothmales and females (Canin, 1991; Cimprich, 1990;Hartig et al., 1991; Lohr et al., 1996; Miles et al.,1998; Ovitt, 1996; Tennessen & Cimprich, 1995). It isdi⁄cult to imagine why nature would a¡ect direc-ted attention in women, men, and girls, but notboys.Another possible explanation for the lack of rela-tionship between near-home nature and self-disci-pline in boys seems more promising. That is,perhaps boys are a¡ected by contact with nature injust the way that girls are, but boys have relativelyless contact than girls with the nature immediatelyoutside their homes. Studies that have geographi-cally mapped children’s play have found that boys ty-pically play farther from home than girls (Hart,1979; Sobel, 1993); for reviews see Moore & Young,(1978), Wohlwill and Heft (1987). Perhaps boys areuna¡ected by near-home nature simply because theyspend time elsewhere. Consistent with this, ¢ndingsfrom a previous study indicated that boys’ atten-tional functioning was not related to the level ofnature immediately around their home, but was re-lated to the level of nature in their usual play space(Faber Taylor et al., 2001). Future research should ex-amine the relationship between levels of nature inboys’ most typical play spaces and their self-disci-pline.The ¢ndings in boys notwithstanding, the overall

pattern of ¢ndings in this study strongly suggests alink between near-home nature and concentration,impulse inhibition, and delay of grati¢cation ingirls.

Alternative Interpretations

To what extent do the links between near-home nat-ure and these forms of self-discipline re£ect a cau-sal relationship between nature and self-discipline?While de¢nitively showing a cause and e¡ect rela-

tionship requires a true experimental design, wecan begin to address some possible alternative inter-pretations here.One possible alternative interpretation for the

current ¢ndings might be that self-discipline islinked to near-home nature, but not because natureenhances self-discipline. That is, perhaps some formof self-selection is operating: perhaps more e¡ective,more self-disciplined parents ¢nd ways to be as-signed to greener apartments, or they ¢nd ways tocreate greener surroundings, or the Chicago Hous-ing Authority assigns ‘better’ prospective tenants togreener buildings. Chicago Housing Authority poli-cies work against each of these possibilities. Apart-ment assignment policies result in de facto randomassignment of residents with respect to levels ofnearby nature at RTH. Furthermore, on-going land-scape maintenance at RTH is handled by a smalllandscaping crew; residents are not involved inmaintenance and funds are inadequate to ful¢ll spe-cial requests from residents. Thus it seems unlikelythat any of these forms of self-selection are takingplace. Moreover, it is not clear why, if ‘better’ par-ents self-select into, or create, or are assigned togreener apartments, their superior qualities wouldbe re£ected only in their daughters.Another possible interpretation might be that

more self-disciplined children actually have thesame levels of near-home nature as their less self-disciplined counterparts, and the link between self-discipline and high greenness ratings is an artifact.For example, perhaps more self-disciplined, more ef-fective parents tend to have better lives and be inmore positive moods than their less e¡ective coun-terparts, and these positive moods lead them to bemore agreeable, thus leading them to endorse itemsmore highly ^ including their greenness ratings.Consistent with this, previous research has foundlinks between mood and suggestibility (Tata & Gud-jonsson, 1990). However, two considerations renderthis possibility implausible. First, the measure ofnaturalness of view in this study was composed oftwo items, one of which was reverse-scored. To theextent that positive moods induced residents ofgreener buildings to endorse all items more highly,the in£ation in the reverse-scored item should bal-ance the in£ation of the positively scored item.And second, again, it is not clear how this explana-tion could account for the mothers of girls, but notboys, giving higher greenness ratings.A third possible alternative interpretation might

involve some form of experimenter demand. Mightthe interviewers have somehow in£uenced motherswith high-performing children to give greener

Nature and self Discipline 11

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 12: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

ratings? Alternatively, might they have in£uencedchildren from greener buildings to score higher?Although these possibilities cannot be ruled out en-tirely, neither seems likely. The test administratorsdid not know the hypothesis of the study and thuswould not know which mothers or children to in£u-ence, or in what direction to in£uence them. Andyet again, it is not clear how this interpretationcould account for the lack of relationship betweennature and self-discipline for boys.In sum, the links between nature and self-disci-

pline found here do not appear to be simple arti-facts of self-selection, systematic biases inassignment of participants to conditions, mood-ele-vated nature ratings, or experimenter demand.Nonetheless, a causal relationship between natureand enhanced self-discipline F even for girls F re-mains to be substantiated.

Contributions to the Literature

By documenting a systematic, positive link betweennear-home nature and three forms of self-disciplinein girls, this work contributes to the research on thebene¢ts of nature in three ways.First, the results underscore the potential impor-

tance of views of nature. Previous research hasshown that a variety of positive outcomes are asso-ciated with views of nature in adults in a variety ofsettings. In residential settings, views of naturehave been linked to residential satisfaction, en-hanced well-being, more e¡ective patterns of coping,and greater day-to-day e¡ectiveness (Kaplan, 1985,2001; Kuo, 2001; Tennessen & Cimprich, 1995) re-spectively. In workplaces, views of nature have beenlinked to job satisfaction and well-being (Kaplan,1993); in prisons, to decreased demand for healthcare services (Moore, 1981); and in hospitals, tofaster recovery from surgery (Ulrich, 1984). The¢ndings here add to a growing body of evidence sug-gesting that views of nature are no mere amenity.Second, this work contributes to our understand-

ing of the bene¢ts of nature for children. Speci¢-cally, the ¢ndings from this study combine with the¢ndings from a previous study to suggest that atten-tional restoration may be an important and univer-sal bene¢t of nature for children. The current studylinks nature and superior attentional functioning ina sample of extremely low-income, attentionally nor-mal African American children. The previous studylinked nature and better attentional functioning ina primarily middle and upper-income, predomi-nately European American sample of children withAttention De¢cit Disorder (Faber Taylor et al., 2001).

Together, the two sets of ¢ndings suggest the possi-bility of a nature-attention link that generalizesacross socioeconomic status, race, and attentionalstatus, as well as di¡erent levels of residentialgreenness F from the most barren of public hous-ing grounds to the lushest of backyards in wealthyneighborhoods.Perhaps the most important contribution of this

work is to identify two new bene¢ts of nature. Pre-vious research on a nature-directed attention rela-tionship has focused primarily on cognitiveoutcomes, especially the capacity to pay attentionor concentrate. Although previous ¢ndings linkingnature and reduced aggression are certainly consis-tent with the hypothesis that nature enhances self-discipline (Kuo & Sullivan, 2001), to our knowledge,this is the ¢rst study to systematically document alink between nature and less cognitive forms of self-discipline, speci¢cally impulse inhibition and delayof grati¢cation. Failure to inhibit impulses can haveboth immediate consequences and important long-term implications for an individual; similarly, a pat-tern of failure in the delay of grati¢cation may sub-stantially alter the course of an individual’s life andtheir chances of success in a variety of domains. Forexample, previous research has indicated that chil-dren’s ability to delay grati¢cation predicts theiracademic achievement, social competency, and abil-ity to cope with frustration and stress in adoles-cence (Mischel et al., 1988). If near-home naturecan provide a daily, easily accessible means of sup-porting impulse inhibition and delay of grati¢cationin a setting where individuals are likely to bechronically mentally fatigued (Kuo, 1992), the impli-cations for individuals, families, and society may beenormous.This study underscores the potential importance

of views of nature, extends previous research on at-tentional restoration in children to a very di¡erentpopulation and setting, and introduces two poten-tial new bene¢ts of nature: enhanced impulse inhi-bition and delay of grati¢cation. The ¢ndings have anumber of implications for practice.

Implications for Practice

These ¢ndings help reinforce the importance of in-corporating trees and grass in spaces for children.One implication of this research concerns the de-sign of public housing developments. As a large pro-portion of urban public housing residents arechildren (in Chicago family housing in 1995, for ex-ample, roughly 60% of residents were 19 years oldor younger; roughly 50% were 14 or younger, CHA,

12 A. F. Taylor et al.

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 13: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

1995), these ¢ndings argue for the potential impor-tance of incorporating trees and grass aroundpublic housing apartment buildings. Moreover,these ¢ndings suggest that designers of publichousing should consider more than just ground-levelviews of common spaces when placing trees andgrass; it may be helpful to place trees and grassstrategically within view from the surroundingapartments. Along the same lines, the ¢ndingshere suggest that, in suburban areas and on the ur-ban-rural fringe, the practice of constructing tree-less residential developments may have importantunintended costs. Previous work has suggested thatthe urban forest may be a vital part of children’s liv-ing environments (Faber Taylor et al., 2001; FaberTaylor et al., 1998); the work here reinforces thatnotion.Another implication of this research concerns the

design of schoolyards. These ¢ndings raise the possi-bility that incorporating trees and grass in school-yards could play an important role in theclassroom. Perhaps after spending breaks in greenschoolyards, children return to their classrooms bet-ter prepared to pay attention, to suppress disruptiveimpulses, and to wait patiently for future breaks.Again, strategic placement may be important here.It may be that an occasional long glance out a class-room window helps support a child’s capacity forself-discipline throughout the school day. Perhapsgreater bene¢ts from a given investment in land-scaping can be obtained by placing vegetation tomaximize views of trees and grass through class-room windows.We close by noting the implications of this study

for helping inner city children negotiate the manyrisks of urban poverty. The ¢ndings here suggestthat the barrenness of inner city neighborhoodsmay contribute to lower levels of self-disciplineand, potentially, to higher rates of negative out-comes in inner city children. In this study, thegreener a girl’s view from home, the better her per-formance on measures of concentration, inhibitionof impulses, and delay of grati¢cation. Thesethree forms of self-discipline may play key roles inthe likelihood of such negative outcomes as aca-demic underachievement, juvenile delinquency, andteenage pregnancy. Perhaps when housing man-agers and city o⁄cials decide to cut budgets forlandscaping in inner city areas, they deprive chil-dren of more than just an attractive view. Neglect-ing landscaping may deprive inner city children ofa much needed resource for self-discipline ^ for thepsychological capacities that lead to a brighter fu-ture.

Notes

This work was funded through a grant from the NationalUrban and Community Forestry Advisory Council, grant#NA-95-0333 USDA, and by the Cooperative State Re-search, Education and Extension Service, U.S. Depart-ment of Agriculture under project #65-370NRES. Thedata presented here were collected as part of the GrowingHope archive, a multi-study research e¡ort examining thee¡ects of the physical environment on the functioning ofmothers and children living in urban public housing. Thisresearch was conducted in partial ful¢llment of the re-quirements for a doctoral degree in Natural Resourcesand Environmental Sciences at the University of Illinois,Urbana-Champaign. We are grateful for the work done byDr. Angela Wiley in hiring, training, and supervising in-terviewers and child care providers, and coordinatingand supervising the data collection. We thank the inter-viewers, child care providers, and the residents of RobertTaylor Homes for their participation, and Chicago Hous-ing Authority for their assistance in the data collectionfor this research. We are also grateful to Dr. StephenKaplan for his helpful suggestions regarding terminology.Correspondence concerning this article should beaddressed to Andrea Faber Taylor, Human Environ-ment Research Laboratory, University of Illinois, 1103 S.Dorner Dr., Urbana, IL 61801, U.S.A. E-mail: [email protected]‘Inhibiting initial impulses’ has also been labeled ‘inhi-biting prepotent responses’ (Logan et al., 1997).

References

Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F. & Tice, D. M. (1994).Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regu-lation. San Diego: Academic Press.

Berrueta-Clement, J. R., Schweinhart, L. J., Barnett,W. S.,Epstein, A. S. & Weikart, D. P. (1984). Changed Lives:The e¡ects of the Perry Preschool Program on YouthsThrough Age 19. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.

Block, J., Block, J. H. & Harrington, D. M. (1974). Somemisgivings about the Matching Familiar Figures Testas a measure of re£ection-impulsivity. DevelopmentalPsychology, 10, 611^632.

Boucugnani, L. L. & Jones, R. W. (1989). Behaviors analo-gous to frontal lobe dysfunction in children with at-tention de¢cit hyperactivity disorder. Archives ofClinical Neuropsychology, 4, 161^173.

Brooks-Gunn, J. & Furstenburg, F.F., Jr. (1986). The chil-dren of adolescent mothers: physical, academic, andpsychological outcomes. Developmental Review, 6,224^251.

Brown, R. T. & Quay, L. C. (1977). Re£ection-Impulsivity innormal and behavior-disordered children. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 5, 457^462.

Cairns, E. & Cammock, T. (1978). Development of a morereliable version of the Matching Familiar Figurestest. Developmental Psychology, 14, 555^560.

Canin, L. H. (1991). Psychological restoration among AIDScaregivers: Maintaining self-care. Unpublished Disserta-tion, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

Chicago Housing Authority (CHA) (1995). Statistical Pro-¢le . Chicago: CHA.

Nature and self Discipline 13

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 14: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

Cimprich, B. (1990). Attentional fatigue and restoration inindividuals with cancer. Dissertation Abstracts Inter-national, 51B, 1740.

Cimprich, B. (1992). Attentional fatigue following breastcancer surgery. Research in Nursing & Health, 115,199^207.

Cohen, S. & Spacapan, S. (1978). The aftere¡ects of stress:An attentional interpretation. Environmental Psychol-ogy and Nonverbal Behavior, 3, 43^57.

Davies, D. R., Jones, D. M. & Taylor, A. (1984). Selective-and sustained-attention tasks: Individual and groupdi¡erences. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds),Varieties of Attention . NewYork: Academic Press, Inc.pp. 000^000.

Dodrill, c. B. (1978). A neuropsychological battery for epi-lepsy. Epilepsia, 19, 611^623.

Donoghue, E. (1993). Sociopsychological correlates ofteen-age pregnancy in the United States VirginIslands. International Journal of Mental Health, 21,39^49.

Donohew, L., Zimmerman, R., Cupp, P., Novak, S., Colon,S. & Abell, R. (2000). Sensation seeking, impulsive de-cision-making, and risky sex: implication for risk-tak-ing and design of interventions. Personality &Individual Di¡erences, 28, 1079^1091.

Dyer, F. (1973). The Stroop phenomenon and its use in thestudy of perceptual, cognitive, and response pro-cesses. Memory & Cognition, 1, 106^120.

Faber Taylor, A., Kuo, F. & Sullivan, W. C. (2001). Copingwith ADD: The Surprising Connection to Green PlaySettings. Environment & Behavior, 33, 54^77.

Faber Taylor, A., Wiley, A., Kuo, F. E. & Sullivan, W. C.(1998). Growing up in the inner city: Green spaces asplaces to grow. Environment & Behavior, 30, 3^27.

Furstenberg, F. F., Jr. (1976). Unplanned Parenthood: TheSocial Consequences of Teenage Child-bearing. NewYork: Free Press.

Glosser, G. & Goodglass, H. (1990). Disorders in executivecontrol functions among aphasic and other brain da-maged patients. Journal of Clinical and ExperimentalNeuropsychology, 12, 485^501.

Hart, R. (1979). Children’s Experience of Place. New York:Irvington Publishers, Inc.

Hartig, T., Mang, M. & Evans, G.W. (1991). Restorative Ef-fects of Natural Environment Experiences. Environ-ment and Behavior, 23, 3^26.

Hynan, D.J. & Grush, J.E. (1986). E¡ects of impulsivity,depression, provocation, and time on aggressivebehavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 20,158^171.

James,W. (1962). Psychology: The Briefer Course. NewYork:Collier Books. (original work published 1892).

Kagan, J. (1966). Re£ection^Impulsivity: the generalityand dynamics of conceptual tempo. Journal of Abnor-mal Psychology, 71, 17^24.

Kaplan, R. (1973). Some psychological bene¢ts of garden-ing. Environment & Behavior, 5, 145^161.

Kaplan, R. (1983). The role of nature in the urban context.In I. Altman & J. Wohlwill (Eds), Behavior and theNatural Environment, Vol. 6. New York: Plenum Press.pp. 127^162.

Kaplan, R. (1985). Nature at the doorstep: residential sa-tisfaction and the nearby environment. Journal of Ar-chitectural and Planning Research, 2, 115^127.

Kaplan, R. (1993). The role of nature in the context ofthe workplace. Landscape and Urban Planning, 26,193^201.

Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative bene¢ts of nature: To-ward an integrative framework. Journal of Environ-mental Psychology, 15, 169^182.

Kaplan, R. (2001). The nature of the view from home:psychological bene¢ts. Environment & Behavior 33,507^542.

Kaplan, R. & Kaplan, S. (1989). The Experience of Nature:A Psychological Perspective. New York: Cambridge.

Kaplan, S. & Talbot, J. F. (1983). Psychological bene¢ts ofa wilderness experience. In I. Altman & J. Wohlwill(Eds), Behavior and Natural Environment, Vol. 6. NewYork: Plenum Press. pp. 163^203.

Kuo, F. (1992). Inner Cities and Chronic Mental Fatigue: De-sign for a Fighting Chance. Paper presented at the En-vironmental Design Research Association.

Kuo, F.E. (2000). ‘Environment and Healthy Human Func-tioning: Towards a Mechanism.’ Presentation given aspart of symposium on ‘Environment and HealthyHuman Functioning’ at the 31st International Confer-ence of the Environmental Design Research Associa-tion. San Francisco, May, 2000.

Kuo, F. E. (2001). Coping with Poverty: Impacts of Envir-onment and Attention in the Inner City. Environment& Behavior, 33, 5^34.

Kuo, F. E. & Sullivan,W.C. (under review). Aggression andviolence in the inner city: impacts of environment viamental fatigue. Environment & Behavior.

Kuo, F. E. & Sullivan,W. C. (2001). Environment and crimein the inner city: does vegetation reduce crime? En-vironment & Behavior, 33, 343^367.

Lezak, M. (1983). Neuropsychological Assessment. NewYork: Oxford University Press.

Logan, G., Schachar, R. & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivityand inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8,60^64.

Lohr, V., Pearson-Mims, C. & Goodwin, G. (1996). Interiorplants may improve worker productivity and reducestress in a windowless environment. Journal of Envir-onmental Horticulture, 14, 97^100.

Lynam, D., Caspi, A., Mo⁄t, T., Wikstroem, P., Loeber, R.& Novak, S. (2000). The interaction between impulsiv-ity and neighborhood context on o¡ending:The e¡ects of impulsivity are stronger in poorerneighborhoods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109,563^574.

McCoul, M. D. (2000) Personality traits of impulsivity andsensation seeking and their relation to high-risk sex-ual behavior in males. Dissertation Abstracts Interna-tional, 60(8-B), 4237.

Mantzicopoulos, P. Y. M., D. (1995). A comparison of boysand girls with attention problems: Kindergartenthrough second grade. American Journal of Orthopsy-chiatry, 64, 522^533.

Markovitz, P. (1995). Pharmacotherapy of impulsivity, ag-gression, and related disorders. In E. Hollander (Ed.),Impulsivity and Aggression. Chichester, England UK:John Wiley & Sons. pp. 263^287

Melnyk, L. & Das, J. P. (1992). Measurement of attentionde¢cit: correspondence between rating scales andtests of sustained and selective attention. AmericanJournal on Mental Retardation, 96(6), 599^606.

14 A. F. Taylor et al.

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 15: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

Miles, I., Sullivan, W. & Kuo, F. (1998). Ecological restora-tion volunteers: the bene¢ts of participation. UrbanEcosystems, 2, 27^41.

Mischel,W., Ebbesen, E. B. & Zeiss, A. R. (1972). Cognitiveand attentional mechanisms in delay of grati¢cation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21,204^218.

Mischel, W., Shoda, Y. & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature ofadolescent competencies predicted by preschool delayof grati¢cation. Journal of Personality & Social Psy-chology, 54, 687^696.

Moore, E. O. (1981). A prison environments’ e¡ect onhealth care service demands. Journal of Environmen-tal Systems, 11, 17^34.

Moore, R. & Young, D. (1978). Childhood outdoors: towarda social ecology of the landscape. In I. Altman & J. F.Wohlwill (Eds.), Human Behavior and Environment,Vol. 3. New York: Plenum. pp. 83^130.

Muraven, M. & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulationand depletion of limited resources: does self-controlresemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247^259.

Ovitt, M. (1996). The e¡ect of a view of nature on perfor-mance and stress reduction of ICU nurses. UnpublishedMaster’s Thesis, University of Illinois, Urbana-Cham-paign.

Posner, M. I. & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing me-chanisms of self-regulation. Developmental and Psy-chopathology, 12, 427^441.

Rigby, K., Mak, A.S. & Slee, P.T. (1989). Impulsiveness, or-ientation to institutional authority, and gender as fac-tors in self-reported delinquency among Australianadolescents. Personality & Individual Di¡erences, 10,689^692.

Rodriguez, M. L., Mischel,W. & Shoda,Y. (1989). CognitivePerson Variables in the Delay of Grati¢cation of OlderChildren at Risk. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 57, 358^367.

Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S., Hershey, K. & Fisher, P. (inpress). Investigations of temperament at 3-7 years:the children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Develop-ment.

Rowe, K. J. & Rowe, K.S. (1992). The relationship betweeninattentiveness in the classroom and reading achieve-ment: Part B: An explanatory study. Journal of theAmerican Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,31, 357^368.

Schwartz, D. A. (1994). The measurement of inhibitory atten-tion and psychological e¡ectiveness among adolescents.Unpublished Master’s thesis, University of Michigan,Ann Arbor.

Sha¡er, D., Pettigrew, A., Wolkind, S. & Zajicek, E. (1978).Psychiatric aspects of pregnancy in schoolgirls: a re-view. Psychological Medicine, 8, 119^130.

Shoda, Y., Mischel, W. & Peake, P. K. (1990). PredictingAdolescent Cognitive and Self-Regulatory Competen-cies From Preschool Delay of Grati¢cation: Identify-ing Diagnostic Conditions. Developmental Psychology,26, 978^986.

Smith, A. (1968). The Symbol Digit Modalities Test: a neu-ropsychologic test for economic screening of learningand other cerebral disorders. Learning Disorders, 3,83^91.

Smith, A. (1973). Symbol Digit Modalities Test Manual. LosAngeles: Western Psychological Services.

Sobel, D. (1993). Children’s Special Places: Exploring the roleof forts, dens, and bush houses in middle childhood.Tucson, AZ: Zephyr.

Tata, P. R. & Gudjonsson, G. H. (1990). The e¡ects ofmood and verbal feedback on interrogative suggest-ibility. Personality & Individual Di¡erences, 11,1079^1085.

Tennessen, C. M. & Cimprich, B. (1995). Views to nature:E¡ects on attention. Journal of Environmental Psychol-ogy, 15, 77^85.

Trancik, A. M., & Evans, G. W. (1995). Spaces ¢t for chil-dren: Competency in design of daycare center envir-onments. Children’s Environments, 12, 311^319.

Ulrich, R. S. (1981). Natural versus urban scenes: Somepsychophysiological e¡ects. Environment & Behavior.13. 523^556.

Ulrich, R. S. (1984). View through a window may in£uencerecovery from surgery. Science, 224, 420^421.

Wechsler, D. (1955).Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Man-ual. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Wells, N. (2000). At home with nature: e¡ects of ‘green-ness’ on children’s cognitive functioning. Environment& Behavior, 32, 775^795.

Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F. & Groisser, D. B. (1991). Anormative-developmental study of executive function:A window on prefrontal function in children. Develop-mental Neuropsychology, 7, 131^149.

White, J. L., Mo⁄tt, T.E., Caspi, A., Bartusch, D.J., et. al.(1994). Measuring impulsivity and examining its rela-tionship to delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-ogy, 103, 192^205.

Wohlwill, J. F., & Heft, H. (1987). The physical environmentand the development of the child. In D. Stokols & I.Altman (Eds.), Handbook of Environmental Psychology,Vol. 1. New York: Wiley and Sons, Inc. pp. 281^328.

Nature and self Discipline 15

JEVP: 20010241 7.51c/W (Jun 11 2001) GRACY USHAMANI

Page 16: VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE: EVIDENCE FROM INNER CITYCHILDRENlhhl.illinois.edu/documents/5_article_Viewsofnatureand... · 2009-07-02 · VIEWS OF NATURE AND SELF-DISCIPLINE:

AUTHOR QUERY FORM

HARCOURTPUBLISHERS

JOURNAL TITLE: JEVP DATE: 2/1/2002ARTICLE NO. : 20010241

Queries and / or remarks

ManuscriptPage/line

Details required Author’s response

P. 9 Gsenberg et al. 1994 - pl. supplyreference

P. 9 Rothbart et al. (in press) - pl update

P. 13 Lezan 1976 - supply ref.

P. 28 Davies et al. 1984 - supply page range

P. 30 Kuo & Dullivan (under review) -pl update