8
EDITORIAL Vascular Dementia: Dead or Alive? RAHUL RAO 1 * AND ROBERT HOWARD 2 1 Senior Registrar in Old Age Psychiatry Maudsley Hospital, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AZ, UK 2 Senior Lecturer in Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK SUMMARY The term ‘Vascular Dementia’ remains popular as a diagnostic entity, since it encompasses a variety of vascular pathologies. This is in stark contrast to many clinical classificatory systems that weight their definitions strongly towards stroke alone. A diagnosis of vascular dementia is complicated by compounding factors that reduce both the validity and specificity of diagnostic systems. This review highlights some of the problems faced in epidemiological, clinical, neuropathological and radiological studies attempting to define a clear-cut syndrome of dementia associated with cerebrovascular disease. The role of non-stroke ischaemia is also discussed. It is concluded that the term ‘vascular dementia’ may have outlived its usefulness as a valid concept; alternative approaches are suggested. # 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriat. Psychiatry, 13: 277–284, 1998. KEY WORDS —dementia; cognitive impairment; stroke; cerebrovascular disease; diagnosis Opinion regarding the relationship between the cerebrovascular circulation and its eects on intellectual function has swung backwards and forwards in a pendular fashion over the past two centuries. The two aetiopathological mech- anisms debated have been those of thrombo- embolic disease and chronic ischaemia. The development of brain imaging techniques in the 1980s allowed the contribution of both pathologies to be assessed more comprehensively. As a result, the term ‘vascular dementia’ (VaD) emphasizes the importance of both ischaemic and thrombo- embolic/haemorrhagic correlates. Notwithstanding the influences that have shaped the current concept of VaD, the validity of a ‘pure’ dementia associated with cerebrovascular pathology remains open to debate. CARDIOVASCULAR RISK FACTORS AND DEMENTIA Cerebrovascular disease is known to be associated with certain risk factors, often referred to as vascular risk factors (VRFs). The relationship between VRFs and cognitive impairment has commonly been studied following stroke, as this is the commonest manifestation of cerebrovascular disease seen on clinical examination. The role of VRFs associated with cerebrovascular disease in the aetiology of cognitive impairment not accom- panied by clinical/radiological evidence of stroke is less clear (Drachman, 1993). This includes VRFs associated with ‘cerebral hypoperfusion’ as opposed to thromboembolism or haemorrhage (O’Brien, 1988; Frederiks, 1993). VRFs are also implicated in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Skoog et al., 1996; Kokmen et al., 1991; Prince et al., 1994; Mortel et al., 1993). CLINICAL VALIDITY OF VASCULAR DEMENTIA The commonest classifactory systems in use base their criteria for dementia on AD (World Health Organisation, 1993; American Psychiatric Associ- ation, 1994), although more recent diagnostic systems (Chui et al., 1992) allow a number of dierent areas of cognitive function to be aected. CCC 0885–6230/98/050277–08$17.50 Received 18 April 1997 # 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 15 December 1997 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF GERIATRIC PSYCHIATRY, VOL. 13: 277–284 (1998) * Correspondence to: Dr R. Rao, Consultant/Senior Lecturer in Old Age Psychiatry, Job Ward, Thomas Guy House, Guy’s Hospital, London SE1 9RT, UK.

Vascular dementia: dead or alive?

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Vascular dementia: dead or alive?

EDITORIAL

Vascular Dementia: Dead or Alive?RAHUL RAO1* AND ROBERT HOWARD2

1Senior Registrar in Old Age Psychiatry Maudsley Hospital, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AZ, UK2Senior Lecturer in Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK

SUMMARY

The term `Vascular Dementia' remains popular as a diagnostic entity, since it encompasses a variety of vascularpathologies. This is in stark contrast to many clinical classi®catory systems that weight their de®nitions stronglytowards stroke alone. A diagnosis of vascular dementia is complicated by compounding factors that reduce both thevalidity and speci®city of diagnostic systems. This review highlights some of the problems faced in epidemiological,clinical, neuropathological and radiological studies attempting to de®ne a clear-cut syndrome of dementia associatedwith cerebrovascular disease. The role of non-stroke ischaemia is also discussed. It is concluded that the term `vasculardementia' may have outlived its usefulness as a valid concept; alternative approaches are suggested. # 1998 JohnWiley & Sons, Ltd.

Int. J. Geriat. Psychiatry, 13: 277±284, 1998.

KEY WORDSÐdementia; cognitive impairment; stroke; cerebrovascular disease; diagnosis

Opinion regarding the relationship betweenthe cerebrovascular circulation and its e�ectson intellectual function has swung backwardsand forwards in a pendular fashion over the pasttwo centuries. The two aetiopathological mech-anisms debated have been those of thrombo-embolic disease and chronic ischaemia. Thedevelopment of brain imaging techniques in the1980s allowed the contribution of both pathologiesto be assessed more comprehensively. As a result,the term `vascular dementia' (VaD) emphasizesthe importance of both ischaemic and thrombo-embolic/haemorrhagic correlates. Notwithstandingthe in¯uences that have shaped the current conceptof VaD, the validity of a `pure' dementia associatedwith cerebrovascular pathology remains open todebate.

CARDIOVASCULAR RISK FACTORSAND DEMENTIA

Cerebrovascular disease is known to be associatedwith certain risk factors, often referred to as

vascular risk factors (VRFs). The relationshipbetween VRFs and cognitive impairment hascommonly been studied following stroke, as thisis the commonest manifestation of cerebrovasculardisease seen on clinical examination. The role ofVRFs associated with cerebrovascular disease inthe aetiology of cognitive impairment not accom-panied by clinical/radiological evidence of strokeis less clear (Drachman, 1993). This includes VRFsassociated with `cerebral hypoperfusion' asopposed to thromboembolism or haemorrhage(O'Brien, 1988; Frederiks, 1993). VRFs are alsoimplicated in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer'sdisease (AD) (Skoog et al., 1996; Kokmen et al.,1991; Prince et al., 1994; Mortel et al., 1993).

CLINICAL VALIDITY OFVASCULAR DEMENTIA

The commonest classifactory systems in use basetheir criteria for dementia on AD (World HealthOrganisation, 1993; American Psychiatric Associ-ation, 1994), although more recent diagnosticsystems (Chui et al., 1992) allow a number ofdi�erent areas of cognitive function to be a�ected.

CCC 0885±6230/98/050277±08$17.50 Received 18 April 1997# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 15 December 1997

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF GERIATRIC PSYCHIATRY, VOL. 13: 277±284 (1998)

*Correspondence to: Dr R. Rao, Consultant/Senior Lecturer inOld Age Psychiatry, Job Ward, Thomas Guy House, Guy'sHospital, London SE1 9RT, UK.

Page 2: Vascular dementia: dead or alive?

Using both diagnostic systems, criteria for thepresence of cerebrovascular disease are synony-mous with those for stroke (Hachinski, 1992).The Hachinski Ischaemic Scale (HIS) (Hachinskiet al., 1975) was developed as a method ofdistinguishing AD from `multi-infarct dementia',but the validity and reliability of this scale havebeen questioned. Firstly, it is based on the clinicalfeatures of reduced cerebral blood ¯ow, which areheavily weighted towards the clinical ®ndingsassociated with stroke. Thus, patients with cerebralhypoperfusion and clinically `silent' cerebral in-farcts would be classi®ed as having AD on the basisof the scale (Larson et al., 1989). The fact thatclinical features such as ¯uctuating course, abruptonset, stepwise progression and preservation ofpersonality are also shared with Lewy bodydementia also serves to weaken the validity of theHIS. Secondly, ambiguity in the interpretation ofcertain items has been shown to reduce interraterreliability (O'Neill et al., 1995).

Attributing causality from clinical manifesta-tions of cerebrovascular disease to dementia can beproblematic, as the temporal relationship betweenthe two may be uncertain (Lopez et al., 1994;Tatemichi et al., 1993); this is further complicatedby the presence of multiple pathologies in `mixeddementia' (Goto et al., 1981) where there isevidence for the coexistence of pathologies ofboth AD and cerebrovascular disease (Altermanand Avnon, 1992; Erkinjunnti, 1994).

CLINICOPATHOLOGICAL STUDIESOF VASCULAR DEMENTIA

Prospective clinicopathological studies examiningthe relationship between vascular risk factors anddementia have been hampered by an incompleteunderstanding of de®nitive pathological markersfor VaD as opposed to those found in normalageing (Crystal et al., 1993). The HIS uses a cuto�point of 7 to classify VaD. Using this scale,speci®cities of 70% or more have been found inthose studies designed to distinguish VaD fromAD,but these same studies have found low speci®citieswhen attempting to separate VaD from mixeddementia (Molsa et al., 1985; Chui, 1989; Wadeet al., 1987; Jellinger et al., 1990). The contributionof the HIS towards making a clinical diagnosis ofmixed dementia therefore remains problematic.Diagnoses of VaD according to NINCDS±AIRENcriteria await clinicopathological correlation.

NEUROPSYCHOLOGY ANDVASCULAR DEMENTIA

In comparison with AD, the neuropsychologicalassessment of VaD has been subject to compara-tively fewer studies (La Rue, 1992). A number ofstudies have shown that there is no pro®le ofgeneralized intellectual dysfunction (Erkinjunntiet al., 1986; Olafsson et al., 1989; Gandolfo et al.,1986; Mazzuchi et al., 1987), or impairment ofmemory (Carlesimo et al., 1994; Fischer et al.,1988a; Padovani et al., 1995), language (Lowensteinet al., 1991; Fischer et al., 1988b) or visuospatialfunction (Almkvist et al., 1993; Almkvist, 1994)that is speci®c to VaD. This is perhaps notsurprising, given the heterogeneity in pathophysio-logical processes involved in VaD (eg multiplecortical infarcts, strategic infarcts, lacunar infarctsand ischaemic white matter lesions), as well as thesize and number of such lesions.

The concept of `subcortical dementia' (Rogers,1986) speci®cally associated with deep small vesseldisease (Marie, 1901) has gained momentumduring the past 50 years. Among a broad rangeof clinical features that characterize subcorticaldementia and serve to distinguish it from AD,frontal lobe dysfunction is listed as one suchfeature. However, no speci®c pattern of frontalde®cit has been consistently demonstrated inVaD (Caplan and Schoene, 1978; Babikian et al.,1990; Wolfe et al., 1990; Villardita, 1993). Closerinspection studies ®nding frontal lobe impairmentto be commoner in VaD reveal methodological¯aws such as selection bias. Moreover, somestudies exclude patients with stroke-related demen-tia, which would result in other vascular lesions(such as lacunar infarcts) becoming more prevalent(Kertesz and Clysedale, 1994). Impaired frontallobe function is also observed in early AD (Miller,1984; Pendleton et al., 1982; Storandt et al., 1984).

NEUROIMAGING AND VASCULARDEMENTIA

Neuroimaging has revolutionized the detectionof lesions associated with vascular pathology(Erkinjunnti et al., 1988). Current imagingmethods have advanced from structural imaging(such as the use of computerized tomography (CT)and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)) to func-tional imaging of cerebral blood ¯ow and regional

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL. 13: 277±284 (1998)

278 R. RAO AND R. HOWARD

Page 3: Vascular dementia: dead or alive?

metabolism using transcranial Doppler, radio-labelled xenon, single-photon emission computer-ized tomography (SPECT) and positron emissiontomography (PET). The two main areas of interestin VaD are the detection of cortical infarctsinvolving major cerebral arteries and lesionsre¯ecting small vessel disease. The evidence for anassociation between dementia and the presence ofstrategic cortical infarcts appears consistent,particularly those located in the medial temporallobe, medial frontal lobe and the dominant angulargyrus (Ott and Saver, 1993; Alexander and Freed-man, 1984; Benson et al., 1990). When dementia isaccompanied by multiple cortical infarcts, theproblem of attributing causality becomes morecomplex. However, in cases of VaD where suchmultiple infarcts are present, temporoparietallesions show a more consistent association withdementia (Erkinjunnti et al., 1987; Liu et al., 1992;Tatemichi et al., 1990; Gorelick et al., 1992).

The association between VaD and deep smallartery disease a�ecting subcortical structures ismore contentious. In the case of single lacunarinfarcts, there is some limited evidence for strategicinfarcts in certain thalamic regions to be associatedwith VaD (Bougousslavasky et al., 1988; Castaigneet al., 1981; Gra�-Radford et al., 1985; Tatemichiet al., 1992). Infarcts in the head of the caudate(Caplan et al., 1990) and putamen (Pullicino et al.,1994) have similarly been associated with VaD.Dementia associated with lacunar rather thancortical infarcts has been hotly disputed by someresearchers (Tatemichi et al., 1994) and has been asource of ongoing debate for more than 30 years(Fisher, 1965). The lack of replicable data showinga clear association between lacunar infarctionand dementia leaves this debate open (Loeb et al.,1992).

White matter disease seen on structural neuro-imaging has attracted a considerable degree ofinterest since the more widespread use of MRI(Awad et al., 1986a,b). The most consistentassociations have been with hypertension (Bolleret al., 1977; Brott et al., 1986; Schmidt et al., 1990)and stroke (Gerrard and Weisberg, 1986; Sullivanet al., 1990; Zimmerman et al., 1988; Hijdra et al.,1990). These ®ndings are confounded by the sharprise in the presence of white matter lesions with age(Bradley et al., 1984; Hunt et al., 1989; Fazekas,1989; Diaz et al., 1991). Another ®nding that mayreduce the speci®city of VaD based on structuralimaging is that of white matter lesions in AD(Skoog et al., 1993), which some investigators have

found to be commoner than in healthy age-matched controls (Rezek et al., 1987; Lechneret al., 1991). It should be noted that white matterlesions are not necessarily synonymous withischaemic changes, and pathological processessuch as spongiosis (Munoz et al., 1993) and eÂtatcrible (widening of the extravascular spaces)(van Sweiten et al., 1991) are known to have anassociation with cerebrovascular disease.

Functional imaging has allowed a better under-standing of metabolic and haemodynamic corre-lates of cognitive function, but has (as mentionedabove) also questioned the di�erentiation of VaDfrom AD on the basis of vascular pathology. Of thefunctional imaging techniques mentioned earlier,the commonly employed methods in studyingcerebrovascular disease are cerebral blood ¯ow,oxygen utilization and glucose consumption.

Using functional imaging, the diversity in lesionlocation has hampered the search for focal patternsof low cerebral blood ¯ow. However, neitherbaseline perfusion (Duara et al., 1989) nor a patchyfocal pattern of cortical perfusion (Hamada et al.,1993) have been useful in di�erentiating VaDfrom AD. Studies of brain glucose metabolismusing PET have been similarly inconclusive(Schapiro et al., 1993; Mielke et al., 1994). Adi�erent approach has been to measure haemo-dynamic abnormalities as a measure of vascularreserve; in this respect, a higher pulsatility index(a measure of vascular resistance) has been foundin VaD than in AD (Biedert et al., 1993). A similarprinciple involving cerebrovascular `reactivity'(vasodilation) after hypercapnic challenge hasproduced similar results (Kuwabara et al., 1992).In common with other areas of study, interpretingthe results of functional imaging research sharesthe potential biases inherent in any study design,which include both technical considerationsrelated to the type of imaging and epidemiologicalconsiderations such as matching groups for age,education, VRFs and severity of cognitive impair-ment. Certainly, the most important limitation is inpatient numbers, owing to the considerable costinvolved in using methods such as PET andSPECT.

NON-STROKE CEREBROVASCULARISCHAEMIA AND DEMENTIA

There is growing interest in the role of cerebro-vascular hypoperfusion in the pathogenesis of

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL. 13: 277±284 (1998)

VASCULAR DEMENTIA 279

Page 4: Vascular dementia: dead or alive?

cognitive impairment, although this still remains acontentious issue. It has been proposed that acute/chronic ischaemia (in the absence of arrestedcardiac function or stroke) may be associatedwith the development of cognitive impairment.The putative correlates of such ischaemic episodesare diverse and include heart failure (Raiha et al.,1993), atrial ®brillation (DePedis et al., 1987),carotid artery stenosis (Wodarz, 1980) and hyper-tension. Hypertension is the most widely studiedvascular risk factor in relation to chronic (non-stroke) cerebral ischaemia. Both cross-sectionaland longitudinal epidemiological studies haveshown hypertensive subjects to perform less wellthan their case-matched normotensive counter-parts on speci®c neuropsychological tests(Waldstein et al., 1991; Elias et al., 1993; Wilkieand Eisdorfer, 1971; Sands and Meredith, 1992).

The group most widely studied in exploring thee�ects of acute/transient ischaemic on cognitivefunction is patients with symptomatic transientischaemic attacks (TIAs) related to thromboem-bolic cardiac disease or carotid stenosis. In the caseof the latter, cognitive function has commonly beenexamined in patients prior to carotid endart-erectomy or intracranial/extracranial (ICA/ECA)bypass operations. Very few of these studies havebeen restricted to TIAs in the absence of completedstroke. One study restricted to this subgroup foundevidence of intellectual impairment preoperatively(Nielson et al., 1985). However, this study failed toinclude a control group. Preoperative studies ofcarotid endarterectomy patients compared to con-trol groups have not been conclusive (Kelly et al.,1980; Casey et al., 1989; Sinatra et al., 1984; Vanden Burgh, 1985; Taghavey and Hamer, 1995).

The mechanism by which cerebral hypoper-fusion is related to cognitive impairment in humansis largely speculative, as neuropathological studieshave been con®ned to animal models of ischaemia.Such studies have demonstrated disruption ofcentral cholinergic function following bilateralocclusion of the internal carotid arteries (Kondoet al., 1996; Tanaka et al., 1996). It is known thatwatershed areas in the cerebral cortex and deepwhite matter are most sensitive to hypoxia, butbecause the cortex has a lower threshold forischaemia and a higher metabolic demand, severetransient ischaemia commonly results in infarc-tion in cortical areas more readily than in deepwhite matter. Thus, the study of patients withtransient ischaemic attacks may not be useful inthis respect.

VASCULAR DEMENTIA: A VALIDDIAGNOSTIC ENTITY?

The existence of a clear-cut syndrome of globalcognitive impairment associated with speci®c riskfactors for cardiovascular disease and characteristicfeatures on clinical examination and on neuro-imaging that can distinguish it reliably from otherdementias (particularly AD) remains dubious. Theway forward in examining the relationship betweencerebrovascular disease and cognition requires thetraditional boundaries previously applied to VaDto be transcended. This would require the adoptionof a dimensional approach to cognitive testing, aswell as correlating domains of cognitive functionwith in vivo functional neuroimaging. The develop-ment of cognitive impairment following stroke doesnot appear to be in dispute; as such, investigatingthe relationship between VRFs and cognitiveimpairment may be best examined by excludingpatients with evidence of stroke.

Both the above issues surrounding conceptrede®nition are encompassed by the term `vascularcognitive impairment' (VCI) (Hachinski andBowler, 1993). The core elements of this conceptinvolve the dimensional nature of cognitiveimpairment as well as the search for patterns ofneuropsychological de®cit and neuroimagingabnormalities associated with particular VRFs(Devasenapathy and Hachinski, 1997). Given thatthe progression of cerebrovascular disease ispotentially treatable, VCI also encompasses theconcept of studying patients with known VRFs andno evidence of cognitive impairment/abnormalitieson neuroimaging (so-called `brain-at-risk' patients)(Hachinski, 1994) through to those with focal andglobal neuropsychological de®cit. The transitionfrom VaD to vascular cognitive impairment is notsimply a matter of degree, but a means ofexamining the range of cognitive impairmentacross individual domains of cognitive function inrelation to cerebrovascular pathology, therebyproviding a more valid methodology to a newconcept that has been clouded by the historicalmisconception of de®ning cognitive impairmentbased on AD.

The millennium will undoubtedly bring with itthe development of a radical rede®nition of thecurrent concept of VaD, which is now outdated.This will also require close collaboration betweendi�erent disciplines if a new concept of `vascularcognitive impairment' is to be achieved. As VaDnow appears to have outlived its usefulness as a

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL. 13: 277±284 (1998)

280 R. RAO AND R. HOWARD

Page 5: Vascular dementia: dead or alive?

valid concept, it would seem timely for it to be laidto rest.

REFERENCES

Alexander, M. P. and Freedman, M. (1984) Amnesiaafter anterior communicating artery aneurysmrupture. Neurology 34, 752±757.

Almkvist, O. (1994) Neuropsychological de®cits invascular dementia in relation to Alzheimer's disease:Reviewing evidence for functional similarity or diverg-ence. Dementia 5, 202±209.

Almkvist, O., Backman, L. and Basun, H. (1993)Patterns of neuropsychological performance inAlzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Cortex29, 669±673.

Alterman, A. and Avnon, M. (1992) Mixed types ofdementia: Co-existence of vascular and degenerativetypes. Neurol. Res. 14, 109±111.

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic andStatistical Manual of Mental Disorders. AmericanPsychiatric Association, Washington, DC.

Awad, I. A., Johnson, P. C. and Spetzler, R. F. (1986a)Incidental subcortical lesions on magnetic resonanceimaging in the elderly. II. Postmortem pathologicalcorrelates. Stroke 17, 1090±1097.

Awad, I. A., Spetzler, R. F. and Hodak, J. A. (1986b)Incidental subcortical lesions on magnetic resonanceimaging in the elderly. I. Correlation with age andcerebrovascular risk factors. Stroke 17, 1084±1089.

Babikian, V. L., Wolfe, N. and Linn, R. (1990) Cognitivechanges in patients with multiple cerebral infarcts.Stroke 21, 1013±1018.

Benson, D. F., Cummings, J. L. and Tsai, S. Y. (1990)Angular gyrus syndrome simulating Alzheimer'sdisease. Arch. Neurol. 47, 133±143.

Biedert, S., Forstl, H. and Hewer, W. (1993) The value oftranscranial Doppler sonography in the di�erentialdiagnosis of vascular dementia of the Binswanger-type. Mol. Chem. Neuropathol. 19, 15±23.

Bogousslavasky, J., Regli, F. and Uske, A. (1988)Thalamic infarcts: Clinical syndromes, etiology andprognosis. Neurology 38, 837±848.

Boller, F., Vrtounski, B. and Mack, J. L. (1977)Neuropsychological correlates of hypertension. Arch.Neurol. 34, 701±705.

Bradley, W. G., Waluch, V. and Brandt-Zawadski, M.(1984) Patchy, periventricular white matter lesions inthe elderly: A common observation during MRIimaging. Noninv. Med. Im. 1, 35±41.

Brott, T. L., Mouradian, M. and Uhtman, B. (1986)Dementia, ventricular enlargement and hypertensionassociated with periventricular hypodensity by com-peted tomography. Ann. Neurol. 20, 129±130.

Caplan, L. R. and Schoene, W. C. (1978) Clinicalfeatures of subcortical arteriosclerotic encephalopathy(Binswanger's disease). Neurology 28, 1206±1215.

Caplan, L. R., Schmahmann, J. D., Kase, C. S.,Feldmann, E., Baquis, G., Greenberg, J. P., Gorelick,P. B., Helgason, C. and Hier, D. B. (1990) Caudateinfarcts. Arch. Neurol. 47, 133±143.

Carlesimo, G. A., Fadda, I. and Lorusso, S. (1994)Verbal and spatial memory spans in Alzheimer'sdisease and multi-infarct dementia. Acta Neurol.Scand. 89, 132±138.

Casey, J. E., Ferguson, G. G., Kimura, D. andHachinski, V. C. (1989) Neuropsychological improve-ment versus practice e�ect following unilateral end-arterectomy in patients without stroke. J. Clin. Exp.Neuropsychol. 11, 461±470.

Castaigne, P., Lhermitte, E. and Buge, A (1981) Para-median thalamic and midbrain infarcts: Clinical andneuropathological study. Arch. Neurol. 10, 127±148.

Chui, H. C. (1989) Dementia: A review emphasisingclinicopathological correlation and brain±behaviourrelationships. Arch. Neurol. 46, 806±814.

Chui, H. C., Victoro�, J. I. and Margolin, D. (1992)Criteria for the diagnosis of ischemic vasculardementia proposed by the State of California Alzhei-mer's Disease Diagnostic and Treatment Centres.Neurology 42, 473±480.

Crystal, H. A., Dickinson, D. W. and Silwinski, M. J.(1993) Pathology markers for normal ageing anddementia in the elderly. Ann. Neurol. 34, 566±573.

DePedis, G., Hedner, K., Johansson, B. W. and Steen, B.(1987) Cardiac arrhythmia in geriatric patients withorganic dementia. Compr. Gerontol. 1, 115±117.

Devasenapathy, A. and Hachinski, V. (1997) Vascularcognitive impairment: A new approach. In Advancesin Old Age Psychiatry: Chromosomes to CommunityCare (C. Holmes. and R. Howard, Eds). WrightsonBiomedical, London.

Diaz, J. F., Merskey, H. and Hachinski, V. C. (1991)Improved recognition of leukoaraiosis and cognitiveimpairment in Alzheimer's disease. Arch. Neurol. 48,1022±1025.

Drachman, D. A. (1993) New criteria for the diagnosisof vascular dementia: Do we know enough yet?Neurology 43, 243±245.

Duara, R., Barker, W. W. and Lowenstein, D. A. (1989)Sensitivity and speci®city of positron emission tom-ography and magnetic resonance imaging in Alzhei-mer's disease and multi-infarct dementia. Eur. Neurol.29, 9±15.

Elias, M. F., Wolf, P. A., D'Agostino, R. B., Cobb, J.and White, L. R. (1993) Untreated blood pressure isinversely related to cognitive functioning: TheFramingham study. Am. J. Epidem. 138, 353±364.

Erkinjunnti, T. (1994) Clinical criteria for vasculardementia: the NINCDS±AIREN criteria. Dementia5, 189±192.

Erkinjunnti, T., Haltia, M. and Palo, J. (1988) Accuracyof the clinical diagnosis of vascular dementia: A pro-spective clinical and post-mortem neuropathologicalstudy. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat. 51, 1037±1044.

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL. 13: 277±284 (1998)

VASCULAR DEMENTIA 281

Page 6: Vascular dementia: dead or alive?

Erkinjunnti, T., Ketonen, L. and Sulkava, R. (1987)CT in the di�erential diagnosis between Alzheimer'sdisease and vascular dementia. Acta Neurol. Scand. 75,262±270.

Erkinjunnti, T., Laaksonen, R. and Sulkava, R. (1986)Neuropsychological di�erentiation between normalaging, Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia.Acta Neurol. Scand. 74, 393±403.

Fazekas, F. (1989) Magnetic resonance signal abnorm-alities in asymptomatic individuals: Their incidenceand functional correlates. Eur. Neurol. 29, 164±168.

Fischer, P., Gatterer, G. and Danielczyk, W. (1988a)Semantic memory in DAT, MID and Parkinsonism.Func. Neurol. 3, 301±307.

Fischer, P., Gatterer, G. and Marterer, A. (1988b)Nonspeci®city of semantic impairment in dementiaof the Alzheimer type. Arch. Neurol. 45, 1341±1343.

Fisher, C. M. (1965) Lacunes: Small, deep cerebralinfarcts. Neurology 15, 774±784.

Frederiks, J. A. (1993) Transient global amnesia. J. Clin.Neurol. Neurosurg. 95, 265±283.

Gandolfo, C., Vecchia, R., Moretti, C., Brusa, G. andScotto, P. A. (1986). WAIS testing in degenerative andmulti-infarct dementia. Acta Neurol. 8, 45±60.

Gerrard, G. and Weisberg, L. A. (1986) MRI peri-ventricular lesions in adults. Neurology 36, 998±1001.

Gorelick, P. B., Chatterjee, A. andPatel, D. (1992)Cranialcomputed tomographic observations in multi-infarctdementia: A controlled study. Stroke 23, 804±811.

Goto, K., Ishii, N. and Fukasawa, H. (1981) Di�usewhite matter disease in geriatric populations. Radi-ology 141, 39±48.

Gra�-Radford, N. R., Damasio, H. and Yamada, T.(1984) Nonhaemorrhagic thalamic infarction. Brain108, 485±516.

Hachinski, V. (1994) Vascular dementia: A radicalrede®nition. Dementia 5, 130±132.

Hachinski, V. C. (1992) Preventable senility: A call foraction against the vascular dementias. Lancet 340,645±648.

Hachinski, V. C. and Bowler, J. V. (1993) Vasculardementia. Neurology 43, 2159±2160.

Hachinski, V. C., Illif, L. D. and Zilkha, E. (1975) Cere-bral blood ¯ow in dementia.Arch. Neurol. 32, 632±637.

Hamada, T., Takita, M. and Kawano, H. (1993)Di�erence in blood ¯ow volume of the commoncarotid artery between vascular and non-vasculardementia detected by colour duplex sonography.J. Neurol. 240, 191±194.

Hijdra, A., Verbeten, B. and Verhulst, J. A. M. P. (1990)Relationship of leukoaraiosis to lesion type in strokepatients. Stroke 21, 890±894.

Hunt, A. L., Orrison, W. W. and Yeo, R. A. (1989)Clinical signi®cance ofMRI white matter lesions in theelderly. Neurology 39, 1470±1474.

Jellinger, K., Danielczyk, W., Fischer, P. and Gabriel, E.(1990) Clinicopathological analysis of dementia dis-orders in the elderly. J. Neurol. Sci. 95, 239±258.

Kelly, M. P., Garron, D. C. and Javid, H. (1980) Carotidartery disease, carotid endarterectomy and behaviour.Arch. Neurol. 37, 743±748.

Kertesz, A. and Clysedale, S. (1994) Neuropsychologicalde®cits in vascular dementia versus Alzheimer'sdisease. Frontal lobe de®cits prominent in vasculardementia. Arch. Neurol. 51, 1226±1231.

Kokmen, E., Beard, C. M. and Chandra, V. (1991) Clini-cal risk factors for Alzheimer's disease: A population-based case-control study. Neurology 41, 1393±1397.

Kondon, Y., Ogawa, N., Asanuma, M., Matsuura, K.,Nishibayashi, K. and Iwata, E. (1996) Preventivee�ects of bifemalen hydrochloride on decreased levelsof muscarinic acetylcholine receptor and its mRNAin a rat model of chronic cerebral hypoperfusion.Neurosci. Res. 24, 409±414.

Kuwabara, Y., Ichaya, Y. and Otsuka, M. (1992)Cerebrovascular responsiveness to hypercapnia inAlzheimer's disease and vascular dementia of theBinswanger-type. Stroke 23, 594±598.

La Rue, A. (1992) Aging and Neuropsychological Assess-ment. Plenum, New York.

Larson, D. B., Lyons, J. S. and Bareta, J. C. (1989) Theconstruct validity of the ischaemic score of Hachinskifor the detection of dementias. J. Clin. Neuropsychiat.Clin. Neurosci. 1, 181±187.

Lechner, H., Niederkorn, K. and Schmidt, R. (1991)Does cerebrovascular insu�ciency contribute toAlzheimer's disease? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 640, 74±79.

Liu, C. K., Miller, B. L. and Cummings, J. L. (1992)A quantitative MRI study of vascular dementia.Neurology 42, 138±143.

Loeb, C., Gandolfo, C. and Croce, R. (1992) Dementiaassociated with lacunar infarction. Stroke 23, 1225±1229.

Lopez, O. L., Larumbe, M. R. and Becker, J. T. (1994)Reliability of the NINCDS±AIREN clinical criteriafor the diagnosis of vascular dementia. Neurology 42,1240±1245.

Lowenstein, D. A., D'Elia, L. and Guterman, A. (1991)The occurrence of di�erent intrusive errors in patientswith Alzheimer's disease, multiple cerebral infarctionsand major depression. Brain Cog. 16, 104±117.

Marie, P. (1901) Des foyers lacunaires de de sinte grationet de di�e rents autres e tats carrtaires du cerveau. Rev.Med. (Paris) 21, 281.

Mazzucchi, A., Capitani, E. and Poletti, A. (1987)Discriminant analysis of WAIS in di�erent types ofdementia and depressed patients. Func. Neurol. 2,155±163.

Mielke, R., Pietrzyk, U. and Jacobs, A. (1994) HMPAOSPET and FDG PET in Alzheimer's disease andvascular dementia: Comparison of perfusion andmetabolic patterns. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. 21, 1052±1060.

Miller, E. (1984) Verbal ¯uency as a function of ameasure of verbal intelligence and in relation todi�erent types of cerebral pathology. J. Clin. Psychol.24, 211±212.

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL. 13: 277±284 (1998)

282 R. RAO AND R. HOWARD

Page 7: Vascular dementia: dead or alive?

Molsa, P. K., Paljarvi, L. and Rinne, O. (1985) Validityof clinical diagnosis in dementia: A prospectiveclinicopathological study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psy-chiat. 48, 1085±1090.

Mortel, K. F., Wood, S., Pavol, M. A., Meyer, J. S. andRexer, J. L. (1993) Analysis of familial and individualrisk factors among patients with ischemic vasculardementia. Angiology 44, 599±605.

Munoz, D. G., Hastak, S. M. and Harper, B. (1993)Pathological correlates of increased signals of thecentrum ovale on magnetic resonance imaging. Arch.Neurol. 50, 492±497.

Nielson, H., Hojer-Pederson, E. and Gulliksen, G.(1985) A neuropsychological study of 12 patientswith transient ischaemic attacks before and after EC/IC bypass surgery. Acta Neurol. Scand. 71, 317±320.

O'Brien, M. D. (1988) Vascular dementia is under-diagnosed. Arch. Neurol. 45, 797±798.

O'Neill, D., Gerrard, J., Surman, D. and Wilcox, G. W.(1995) Variability in scoring the Hachinski Ischaemicscore. Age Ageing 24, 242±246.

Olafsson, K., Korner, A. and Bille, A. (1997) TheGBS scale in multi-infarct dementia. Acta Psychiatr.Scand. 7, 94±97.

Olafsson, K., Korner, A., Bille, A., Jensen, H. V.,Thiesen, S. and Andersen, J. (1989) The GBS scalein multi-infarct dementia and senile dementia ofAlzheimer type. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 79, 94±97.

Ott, B. R. and Saver, J. L. (1993) Unilateral amnesicstroke: Six new cases and a review of the literature.Stroke 24, 1033±1042.

Padovani, A., DiPiero, V. and Bragoni, M. (1995)Patterns of neuropsychological impairment in milddementia: A comparison between Alzheimer's diseaseand multiinfarct dementia. Acta Neurol. Scand. 92,433±442.

Pendleton, M. G., Heaton, R. K. and Lehman, R. A. W.(1982) Diagnostic utility of the Thurstone WordFluency Test in neuropsychological evaluation.J. Clin. Neuropsychol. 4, 307±317.

Prince, M., Cullen, M. and Mann, A. (1994) Risk factorsfor Alzheimer's disease and dementia: A case controlstudy based on the MRC elderly hypertension trial.Neurology 44, 97±104.

Pullicino, P. M., Lichter, D. and Benedict, R. H. B.(1994) Micrographia with cognitive dysfunction:`Minimal' sequelae of a putaminal infarct. MovementDis. 9, 371±373.

Raiha, I., Tarvonin, S. and Kurki, T. (1993) Relationshipbetween vascular factors and white matter lowattenuation of the brain. Acta Neurol. Scand. 87,286±289.

Rezek, D. L., Morris, J. C. and Fuling, K. H.(1987) Periventricular white matter lucencies in seniledementia of the Alzheimer type and normal aging.Radiology 37, 1365±1368.

Rogers, D. (1986) Bradyphrenia in Parkinsonism:A historical review. Psychol. Med. 16, 257±265.

Sands, L. P. and Meredith, W. (1992) Blood pressure andintellectual functioning in late midlife. J. Gerontol. 47,81±84.

Schapiro, M. B., Pietrini, P. and Grady, C. L. (1993)Reductions in parietal and temporal cerebralmetabolic rates for glucose are not speci®c forAlzheimer's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat.56, 859±864.

Schmidt, R., Fazekas, F. and O�enbacher, H. (1990)Magnetic resonance imaging white matter lesions andcognitive impairment in hypertensive individuals.Arch. Neurol. 48, 417±420.

Sinatra, M. G., Boeri, R., DelTon, F. and Fornari, M.(1984) Neuropsychological evaluation in transientischaemic attack and minor stroke. J. Neurol. 231,194±197.

Skoog, I., Lernfel, B., Landahl, S., Palmertz, B.,Andreasson, L. A., Nilsson, L., Persson, G., Oden, A.and Svanborg, A. (1996) 15-year longitudinal study ofblood pressure and dementia. Lancet 347, 1141±1145.

Skoog, I., Palmertz, B. and Andreasson, L. A. (1993) Theprevalence of white matter lesions on computed tomo-graphy of the brain in demented and nondemented85 year olds. J. Geriatr. Psychiat. Neurol. 7, 169±175.

Storandt, M., Botwinik, J. and Danziger, W. L. (1984)Psychometric di�erentiations of mild senile dementiaof the Alzheimer type. Arch. Neurol. 41, 497±499.

Sullivan, P., Pary, R. and Teland, R. (1990) Risk factorsfor white matter changes detected by magnetic reson-ance imaging in the elderly. Stroke 21, 1424±1428.

Taghavey, A. and Hamer, H. (1995) Symptomatic andasymptomatic high-grade unilateral internal carotidartery stenosis: Scalp topography of event-relatedpotentials (p300) and psychometric testing. Electro-enceph. Clin. Neurophys. 94, 163±165.

Tanaka, K., Ogawa, N., Asanuma, M., Kondo, Y. andNomura, M. (1996) Relationship between cholinergicdysfunction and discrimination learning disabilities inWistar rats following chronic cerebral hypoperfusion.Brain Res. 729, 55±65.

Tatemichi, T. K., Desmond, D. W. and Paik, M. (1993)Clinical determinants of dementia related to stroke.Ann. Neurol. 33, 568±575.

Tatemichi, T. K., Desmond, D. W., Prohovnik, I., Cross,D. T., Gropen, T. I., Mohr, J. P. and Stern, Y. (1992)Confusion and memory loss from capsular genuinfarction: a thalamocortical disconnection syndrome?Neurology 42, 1966±1979.

Tatemichi, T. K., Desmond, D. W. and Stern, Y. (1994)Cognitive impairment after stroke: Frequency, pat-terns and relationships to functional abilities. J.Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat. 57, 202±207.

Tatemichi, T. K., Foulkes, M. A. and Mohr, J. P. (1990)Dementia in stroke survivors in the stroke data bankcohort. Stroke 19, 858±866.

Van den Burgh, W. (1985) Carotid endarterectomy: Doesit improve cognitive or motor functioning? Psychol.Med. 15, 341±346.

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL. 13: 277±284 (1998)

VASCULAR DEMENTIA 283

Page 8: Vascular dementia: dead or alive?

van Sweiten, J. C., van den Hout, J. H. and van Ketel,B. A. (1991) Periventricular lesions in the whitematter on magnetic resonance imaging in the elderly:A morphometric correlation with arteriosclerosis anddilated perivascular spaces. Brain 114, 761±774.

Villardita, C. (1993) Alzheimer's disease comparedwith cerebrovascular dementia: Neuropsychologicalsimilarities and di�erences. Acta Neurol. Scand. 87,299±308.

Wade, J. P. H., Mirsen, T. R. and Hachinski, V. C. (1987)The clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. Arch.Neurol. 44, 24±29.

Waldstein, S. R., Manuck, S. B., Ryan, S. B. andMuldoon, M. F. (1991) Neuropsychological correlatesof hypertension: Review and methodological con-siderations. Psychol. Bull. 110, 451±468.

Wilkie, F. and Eisdorfer, C. (1971) Intelligence andblood pressure in the aged. Science 172, 959±962.

Wodarz, R. (1980) Watershed infarctions and computedtomography: A topographical study in cases withstenosis/occlusion of the carotid artery. Neuro-radiology 19, 245±254.

Wolfe, N., Linn, R. and Babikian, V. L. (1990) Frontalsystems impairment following multiple lacunarinfarcts. Arch. Neurol. 47, 129±132.

World Health Organisation (1993) International Classi-®cation of Diseases. World Health Organisation,Geneva.

Zimmerman, R. D., Fleming, C. A. and Lee, B. C. P.(1988) Periventricular hyperintensity as seen bymagnetic resonance imaging: Prevalence and signi®-cance. AJNR 9, 425±429.

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL. 13: 277±284 (1998)

284 R. RAO AND R. HOWARD