154
UNIVERSITY OF CINCINNATI Date: I, , hereby submit this original work as part of the requirements for the degree of: in It is entitled: Student Signature: This work and its defense approved by: Committee Chair: 8/18/2010 996 16-Aug-2010 Andrew James Myer Doctor of Philosophy Criminal Justice Hurricane Katrina, Citizen Displacement, and Social Control: A Test of the Threat and Benign Neglect Hypotheses and an Investigation of the Crime-Arrest Relationship Mitchell Chamlin, PhD Mitchell Chamlin, PhD Andrew James Myer

UNIVERSITY OF CINCINNATI · Hurricane Katrina, Citizen Displacement, and Social Control: A Test of the Threat and Benign Neglect Hypotheses and an Investigation of the Crime-Arrest

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • UNIVERSITY OF CINCINNATI

    Date:

    I, ,

    hereby submit this original work as part of the requirements for the degree of:

    in

    It is entitled:

    Student Signature:

    This work and its defense approved by:

    Committee Chair:

    8/18/2010 996

    16-Aug-2010

    Andrew James Myer

    Doctor of Philosophy

    Criminal Justice

    Hurricane Katrina, Citizen Displacement, and Social Control: A Test of the

    Threat and Benign Neglect Hypotheses and an Investigation of the

    Crime-Arrest Relationship

    Mitchell Chamlin, PhDMitchell Chamlin, PhD

    Andrew James Myer

  • Hurricane Katrina, Citizen Displacement, and Social Control: A Test of the Threat and BenignNeglect Hypotheses and an Investigation of the Crime-Arrest Relationship

    A dissertation submitted to the

    Graduate School

    of the University of Cincinnati

    in partial fulfillment of the

    requirements for the degree of

    Doctor of Philosophy

    In the School of Criminal Justiceof the College of Education, Criminal Justice, and Human Services

    by

    Andrew J. Myer

    B.A., Saint John’s University, 2004M.S., University of Cincinnati, 2005

    Dissertation Committee: Mitchell B. Chamlin, Ph.D. (Chair)Francis T. Cullen, Ph.D.James Frank, Ph.D.William Alex Pridemore, Ph.D.

  • ii

    ABSTRACT

    A guiding theory often used by criminologist to examine the application of social control

    is the conflict perspective. This perspective asserts that social control is used as a tool to protect

    elite segments of society. The conflict theory posits an increase in social control when elite

    groups are threatened, the threat hypothesis. While the benign neglect hypothesis predicts a

    decrease in crime control when the population under threat is a non-elite population. One of the

    main concerns of this paper is to address limitations in the oft studied threat hypothesis and the

    understudied benign neglect hypothesis.

    Two limitations of previous studies of conflict hypotheses are (1) lack of a direct

    measure of threat and (2) aggregation bias. This research will employ a more direct measure of a

    threatening event: the displacement of citizens to Houston, Texas, by Hurricane Katrina, moving

    beyond the reliance between the correlation of structural antecedents and social control. This

    research will also investigate aggregation bias by evaluating conflict hypotheses at a level below

    the city—the traditional unit of analysis of past research. To this end, this research will use time

    series analysis to directly account for social control pre- and post-displacement to determine if

    any changes in the administration of social control occurred as a result of the displacement.

    Finally, this paper also attempts to examine the role that social context plays in the

    reciprocal relationship between crimes and arrests. Past research on the crime arrest relationship

    has been mixed. Cross sectional designs often corroborate predictions of economic theory, while

    the more rigorous time series designs fail to provide similar results. Recent research suggests

    that the social context of an area may exert an influence on this relationship. To this end, this

    investigation will employ bivariate time series techniques to examine the role that social context

    plays in the reciprocal relationship between crimes and arrests.

  • iii

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    There are many people who deserve acknowledgement for their contributions in my

    completion of this dissertation and my doctoral degree. Many professors in the department of

    criminal justice influenced me greatly. Dr. Ed Latessa was instrumental in helping me feed my

    family, as well as feed my curiosity about practical applications in correctional settings. I

    learned a great deal from Dr. Smith on teaching and applying practical applications. I also wish

    to thank Dr. Jim Frank for not only serving on my committee, but also for the great

    conversations we had (some while golfing). I am in debt to Dr. Frank Cullen for showing me

    how to frame probing questions in an interesting and academic fashion, as well as serving on my

    committee. I am also grateful for the in-depth and thought-provoking comments to my

    dissertation provided by Dr. Bill Pridemore. I also wish to thank others for all of their

    contributions to my graduation: Dr. John Wooldredge, Dr. John Eck, Dr. Bonnie Fisher, Dr. Matt

    Makarios, Dr. Ben Steiner, John Schwartz, Janice Miller, and Jean Gary.

    Dr. Mitch Chamlin (maybe I will finally be able to call you Mitch now?) could not have

    been a bigger influence on me. I owe a great deal to my success because of what you taught me,

    the conversations we had, and the work we have done together. I truly cannot express my

    gratitude for you taking the time to guide me and not just show me the way. Thanks for being a

    great mentor and a wonderful friend.

    My mom and dad also deserve credit for my accomplishment. I would not be asking all

    of these questions if they had not fueled my curiosity and raised me to not fear asking questions.

    Thanks for all you have done for me.

  • iv

    To my wife, Leslie: I could not and would not be where I am if it wasn’t for you. Thanks

    for your support, your kindness, and warmth. Thanks for your generosity and sacrifice. I love

    you.

    To my son, Benet: I love you. What follows is proof that you can accomplish anything,

    as long as you listen to your mother.

  • v

    TABLE OF CONTENTS

    Abstract…………..………………………………………………………………………..... iAcknowledgements…………………………………………………………………………. iiiTable of Contents………..………………………………………………………………….. ivList of Tables…………..…………………………………………………………………… vi

    CHAPTER ONE: THE ISSUES OF DIRECT MEASUREMENT, AGGREGATION BIAS, AND SOCIAL CONTEXT UNDER THE CONFLICT PERSPECTIVE

    Introduction……………...………………………………………………………………….. 1The Present Research…………..…………………………………………………………… 7

    CHAPTER TWO: A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE CONFLICT PERSPECTIVE

    Introduction………..……………………………………………………………………….. 9The Conflict Perspective………..………………………………………………………….. 9

    The Threat and Benign Neglect Hypotheses…….……………………………………… 12Previous Research on the Threat and Benign Neglect Hypotheses……………………….. 14

    Summary..………………………………………………………………………………... 20Methodological Issues……...………………………………………………………………. 20

    Indirect Measurement….…............................................................................................... 20Aggregation Bias………..……..………………………………………………………… 22

    The Current Study…………..……………………………………………………………… 24

    CHAPTER 3: SOCIAL CONTEXT AND THE CRIME-ARREST RELATIONSHIP: EVALUATING THE PREDICTIONS OF THE CONFLICT PERSPECTIVE

    Introduction……………………………………………………………………………….. 26The Causal Mechanisms Underlying the Crime-Arrest Relationship…………………….. 27A Review of the Crime-Arrest Literature……………….…………………………………. 31Research on the Tipping Effect……………….…………………………………………… 37The Current Study………………..………………………………………………………… 38

    CHAPTER 4: METHODS

    Introduction…………..…………………………………………………………………….. 40Research Design………………..…………………………………………………………... 40

    Hurricane Katrina as Threat……………..………………………………………………. 42Data…………..……………………………………………………………………………... 44

    Crimes………..…………………………………………………………………………... 45Arrests…………..………………………………………………………………………... 47Police Divisions………..………………………………………………………………… 51

    Analytical Method…………..……………………………………………………………… 58Univariate Estimation………..…………………………………………………………... 59Interrupted Time Series……………………………………..…………………………… 62

  • vi

    Bivariate ARIMA Analysis.……………………..………………………………………. 65Conclusion………………………..…………..…………………………………………….. 66

    CHAPTER 5: RESULTS OF TIME SERIES ANALYSES OF CLEARANCES AND CRIMES

    Introduction…………..…………………………………………………………………….. 67Interrupted Time Series Results………………….……………………………………... 67Summary…………….…………………………………………………………………... 88

    Bivariate Analyses Results………………..………………………………………………... 89Conclusion……………..…………………………………………………………………… 111

    CHAPTER 6: DISCUSSION AND SUMMARY OF FINDINGS

    Introduction…………………………………………………………………………………. 116Interrupted Time Series Discussion………………………………………………………… 116Bivariate Analyses Discussion……………………………………………………………… 125Recommendations and Limitations…………………………………………………………. 128Conclusions…………………………………………………………………………………. 130

    References…………………………………………………………………………………... 132

  • vii

    List of Tables

    Table 4.1: Crimes and Clearances by Year for the City of Houston, 1997-2008…..…..... 49Table 4.2: Crimes and Clearances by Police Division for the City of Houston, 1997-2008. 53Table 5.1: Final Univariate and Intervention Models for the City………………………… 68Table 5.2: Social Context of Westside Police Divisions in Houston Using Non-proportional Census Distribution…………………………………………………………… 71Table 5.3: Final Univariate Models for Westside Police Divisions……………………….. 72Table 5.4: Zero-Order Intervention Models for Westside Police Divisions………………... 73Table 5.5: Final Univariate Models for Crime in Westside Police Divisions……………… 76Table 5.6: Zero-Order Intervention Models on Crime for Westside Police Divisions…….. 77Table 5.7: Final Univariate Clearance Models for Eastside Police Divisions……………… 79Table 5.8: Social Context of Eastside Police Divisions in Houston Using Non-proportional Census Distribution…………………………………………………………………………. 81Table 5.9: Zero-Order Intervention Clearance Models for Eastside Police Divisions…….. 82Table 5.10: Final Univariate Crime Models for Eastside Police Divisions………………… 85Table 5.11: Zero-Order Intervention Crime Models for Eastside Police Divisions……….. 87Table 5.12: Final Univariate Models for Full, Pre-, and Post-displacement Series……….. 91Table 5.13: Estimated CCFs for the Full Series for All Aggregates……………………….. 96Table 5.14: Contextual Transfer Function Models, Full Series……………………………. 101Table 5.15: Estimated CCFs for the Pre-displacement Series for All Aggregates…………. 103Table 5.16: Contextual Transfer Function Models, Pre-displacement Series………………. 106Table 5.17: Estimated CCFs for the Post-displacement Series for All Aggregates……….. 107Table 5.18: Contextual Transfer Function Models, Post-displacement Series……………... 111Table 6.1: Social Context of Westside Police Divisions in Houston Using Non-proportional Census Distribution…………………………………………………………… 122

  • 1

    CHAPTER ONE

    THE ISSUES OF DIRECT MEASUREMENT, AGGREGATION BIAS, AND SOCIAL CONTEXT UNDER THE CONFLICT PERSPECTIVE

    INTRODUCTION

    The focus of the current research is to determine the indirect effects of Hurricane Katrina

    on crime, arrests, and the relationship between crime and arrests. More specifically, this research

    assesses the impact of the displacement of citizens to Houston, Texas on crime and arrests in

    Houston, a city not directly exposed to the natural disaster. This study occurs under the guidance

    of the conflict perspective and addresses the issues of: (1) direct measurement of the conflict

    perspective’s notion of threat, (2) the identification and selection of an appropriate unit of

    analysis, and (3) consideration of the social contexts when examining crimes, arrests, and the

    relationship between the two. The remainder of this chapter outlines the importance of

    addressing these issues when examining the conflict perspective.

    The conflict perspective states that crime control is a tool used to protect the interests of

    elite populations (Black 1976; Blalock 1967; Turk 1969; Chambliss and Seidman 1971; Spitzer

    1975; Quinney 1970, 1977; Jackson 1989; Liska 1992). Research on crime control under this

    perspective generally focuses on two hypotheses. The first is known as the threat hypothesis.

    This hypothesis states that symbolic or physical threats to elite populations will lead to an

    increase in crime control (Liska 1992). This increase in crime control is to protect the position of

    elites in society. The second hypothesis is known as the benign neglect hypothesis. The focus of

    this hypothesis is on symbolic or physical threats to non-elite populations. When threat is

    experienced by a non-elite population, the benign neglect hypothesis predicts a decrease in crime

    control (Liska 1992). This occurs because the non-elite population lacks the ability to legitimize

  • 2

    their complaints to police, or because the acts are committed intra-group and are viewed as a

    personal matter deemed unnecessary for formal intervention (Liska and Chamlin 1984). In

    general, past research finds support for the threat hypothesis (Brown and Warner 1992;

    Carmichael 2005; Corzine, Creech, and Corzine 1983; Eitle, D’Alessio, and Stolzenberg 2002;

    Greenberg, Kessler, and Loftin 1985; Holmes 2000; Jackson and Carroll 1981; Jacobs 1979;

    Jacobs and Britt 1979, Kane 2003; King and Wheelock 2007; Liska, Lawrence, and Benson

    1981; Liska, Chamlin, and Reed 1985; Nalla, Lynch, and Leiber 1997; Ruddell and Urbina 2004;

    Smith and Holmes 2003; Stults and Baumer 2007), but research on the benign neglect

    hypothesis, having far fewer tests, receives mixed support (Chamlin and Liska 1992; Liska and

    Chamlin 1984; Eitle, Stolzenberg, and D’Allesio 2005; Myer and Chamlin 2007; Parker et al.

    2005; Stolzenberg et al. 2004). Two topics that are common to the testing of both the threat

    hypothesis and the benign neglect hypothesis are (1) the measurement of the intervening variable

    (i.e., the concept of threat), and (2) the unit of analysis used to study the threat and benign

    neglect hypothesis (i.e., the city level of analysis).

    Previous research on the conflict perspective typically conceptualizes threat as the size of

    minority groups. The size of a non-elite population (e.g., percent black) is thought to be

    indicative of perceived threat, because an increase of this subordinate population poses a threat

    to the economic and political position of elite populations. As such, previous research has

    assessed the relationship between the size of minority groups and the size of police institutions

    (Chamlin, 1989; Greenburg, Kessler, and Loftin 1985; Jackson 1986, 1989; Jackson and Carroll

    1981; Jacobs 1979; Liska, Lawrence, and Benson 1981; Nalla, Lynch, and Leiber 1997; Stults

    and Baumer 2007) or the activity of police institutions (Brown and Warner 1992; Chamlin and

    Liska 1992; Eitle, D’Alessio, and Stolzenberg 2002; Liska and Chamlin 1984; Liska, Chamlin,

  • 3

    and Reed 1985; Parker, Stults, and Rice 2005; Stolzenburg, D’Alessio, and Eitle 2004). The

    emergence of a significant relationship between the size of a minority group and formal social

    control is often used to infer perceived threat by elites. The consequence of this approach is that

    the intervening variable of threat is not directly measured (Chamlin 1989; Chamlin 2009, Eitle et

    al. 2002; King and Wheelock 2007; Jacobs 1979; Parker, Stults, and Rice 2005). By not directly

    measuring the theoretical construct of threat, the statistical association between minority group

    size and some outcome (e.g., crime control) is susceptible to multiple interpretations. For

    example, the positive statistical relationship between minority size and arrests can also be

    thought of as a challenge to the social solidarity of the social system, a structural functionalist

    perspective (for example, see Inverarity 1976). It is the intention of the present research is to

    directly account for threat, thus eliminating multiple theoretical interpretations.

    In addition to the measurement of threat, this research also addresses the issue of

    aggregation bias. The examination of data at an improperly specified unit of analysis may lead

    to incorrect or null results. To avoid this, research must confront a theory with data collected at

    the appropriate unit of analysis in order to falsify it (Bailey 1984; Greenberg, Kessler, and Logan

    1981; Land, McCall, and Cohen 1990; Lieberson 1985). For example, Bailey (1984) states that

    cities are more appropriate than SMSAs when studying the poverty-crime relationship.

    Greenberg and colleagues (1981) demonstrate that state-level evaluations of general deterrence

    theory have little probative value because the causal mechanism operates at a smaller level of

    aggregation.

    Aggregation bias is relevant to the present research because previous research has

    evaluated threat and benign neglect across municipalities, when in fact the mechanisms

    underlying the threat and benign neglect hypotheses may occur at a level below the city (Myer

  • 4

    and Chamlin 2007, Stolzenberg, D’Allesio, and Eitle 2004). Recall that the benign neglect

    hypothesis predicts a decrease in crime control because of an inability of non-elite populations to

    legitimize their complaints or because of an increase in intragroup crime. This is significant to

    the conflict perspective because previous research has also evaluated the relationship between

    the distribution of minority groups (via a measure of segregation at the city level) and crime

    control (Brown and Warner 1992; Carmichael 2005; Chamlin and Liska 1992; Eitle et al. 2005;

    Liska and Chamlin 1984; Liska, Lawrence, and Benson 1981; Liska et al. 1985; Parker et al.

    2005; Parker, MacDonald, Jennings, and Alpert 2005; Stolzenberg et al. 2004).

    The segregation of less powerful populations increases the likelihood of intraracial crime

    on populations that already have limited sociopolitical power to legitimize their complaints to

    police (Blauner 1972; Spitzer 1975). Past research typically measures segregation at the city

    level through the use of the dissimilarity index, which compares the racial composition of city

    blocks to the racial composition of the city as a whole. This is done for all blocks in order to

    measure the percentage of one racial group that would have to move to a different neighborhood

    to be distributed similarly as another racial group (See Massey and Denton, 1988). The resulting

    index score provides a measure of segregation for the entire city. If results produce a negative

    relationship between segregation and social control, support for benign neglect is revealed. The

    issue paramount to this approach is that the city level, the typical level of aggregation when

    examining the benign neglect (and threat) hypothesis, may be masking observable effects that

    occur at a level beneath the city. As an example, imagine a sample of cities, each containing

    only one or two neighborhoods that are segregated. If such were the case, there is likely to be

    little variation in the level of segregation across the sample of cities. Necessarily, measures of

    segregation would have little, if any, impact on crime control outcomes. The effects of

  • 5

    segregation that operate across neighborhoods within cities are then being masked by the use of

    city-level data (Blauner 1972; Spitzer 1975). If such were the case, previous research has

    neglected to use the appropriate unit of analysis. An objective of this study is to examine

    whether the predictions of the threat and benign neglect hypotheses are evidenced at sub-city

    level by taking into account contextual characteristics of neighborhoods within the city.

    Accounting for the social context is important because previous research has demonstrated that

    social control can be a function of the social context within which police officers operate

    (Klinger 1997; Klinger 2004; Smith 1986; Smith, Visher, and Davidson 1984; Terrill and Reisig

    2003).

    The final intent of this research is to examine the relationship between crime and arrests.

    While theory posits a relationship between crimes and arrests (see Becker 1968; Gibbs 1975;

    Tittle 1980), different hypotheses predict different interactions between the two variables. The

    overload hypothesis predicts that increases in crime will lead to a decrease in arrests because the

    resources of the police are taxed (Fisher and Nagin 1978; Geerken and Gove 1977), whereas the

    public choice hypothesis predicts that an increase in crimes will lead to an increase in arrests

    because there is more pressure put on police to address the up-rise in crime (Borcherding and

    Deacon 1972; Deacon 1978; Tridimas 2001). Alternatively, the deterrence hypothesis predicts

    that an increase in arrests will lead to a decrease in crime because the costs of committing crimes

    exceed the benefits (Becker 1968; Geerken and Gove 1977; Gibbs 1975).

    Initial tests of the above hypotheses were undertaken using cross sectional designs.

    While many of these tests produced significant findings, there was growing concern about the

    limitations of cross sectional designs in the evaluation of the reciprocal relationship between

    crimes and arrests. Accordingly, researchers began evaluating the predictions of these

  • 6

    hypotheses through time series analyses in an attempt to examine the temporal order and lag

    structure of the relationship between crimes and arrests—both limitations of cross-sectional

    designs. Generally, time series analyses did not replicate the findings of cross sectional designs

    (Chamlin 1988; Greenberg et al. 1979; Greenberg and Kessler 1982; Logan 1975). This has led

    some to argue that findings from the cross-sectional analyses are corrupted by model-

    misspecification error (Greenberg et al. 1979). Recently, it has been argued that the inability of

    time-series analyses to replicate findings from cross-sectional analyses on the crime-arrest

    relationship may in fact be due to the inability to account for the social contexts of cities

    (Chamlin and Myer 2009). This research will build off this new line of research to determine if

    social context does in fact play a role in the reciprocal relationship between crimes and arrests.

    The threat hypothesis and the benign neglect hypothesis vary in their predictions of crime

    control based on the status of the group experiencing threat, but the effects of their predictions

    may not be mutually exclusive. As such, the predictions of these hypotheses may have utility in

    the investigation of the reciprocal relationship between crimes and arrests. If there is an increase

    in arrests to protect the interests of social elites (the threat hypothesis), it would be expected to

    simultaneously reduce law enforcement in areas dominated by non-elites. This is then likely to

    generate a reduction in the amount of offenses cleared by arrest. Accordingly, the benign neglect

    hypothesis predicts that after the experience of symbolic or physical threat, the level of crime

    will begin to negatively affect the level of arrests in predominately black (non-elite) areas of the

    city. For the purposes of the current research, this means that changes in the level of arrest will

    affect changes in the level of crimes post-displacement, but not before. These changes will also

    be a function of the social contexts of Houston, Texas.

  • 7

    THE PRESENT RESEARCH

    The intent of this study is to determine the indirect effects of Hurricane Katrina on crime,

    arrests, and the relationship between crimes and arrests. The current research utilizes a quasi-

    experimental, time series design to account for the occurrence of Hurricane Katrina and the

    resulting displacement of citizens to Houston. Previous research has evaluated the effects of

    disasters such as hurricanes (Brezina and Kaufman 2008; Davila, Marquart, and Mullings 2005;

    Faust and Kauzlarich 2008; Kirk 2009; Kulkarni, Bell, Beausoleil, Lein, Angel, and Mason

    2008; Lanza-Kaduce, Dunham, Akers, and Cromwell 1998; LeBeau 2002; Rojek and Smith

    2007; Thomas 2008), riots (Bergesen and Herman 1998; Chamlin 2009; Chamlin and Cochran

    2000; Chamlin and Myer 2009; King and Waddington 2004; Myer and Chamlin 2007),

    earthquakes (Green 2005), maritime disasters (Rothe, Muzzatti, and Mullins 2006), and terrorism

    (Bornstein 2005; Frank, Dewart, Schmeidler, and Demirjian 2006; Pandiani, Knisley, Banks,

    Simon, and Blackburn 2005; Pridemore, Chamlin, and Trahan 2008) on various outcomes. Most

    of the research on Hurricane Katrina has evaluated the direct effects on New Orleans; however,

    the indirect effects of Katrina also merit inspection. Assessing the effects of Hurricane Katrina

    on Houston, Texas is relevant because of the amount of citizens that were affected, directly

    (displaced New Orleans residents) and indirectly (Houston residents)—Houston eventually

    housed approximately 250,000 evacuees (Berger 2006). The displacement of citizens to

    Houston, Texas constitutes a threatening event for both the displaced citizens of Louisiana, as

    well as the residents of Houston, due to the abrupt nature of the event.

    This research will assess the predictions of the threat hypothesis and the benign neglect

    hypothesis by providing a more direct measure of threat, the displacement of citizens. The

    predictions of these two hypotheses will also be assessed at a level below the city. Houston

  • 8

    Police divisions and their social contexts will be examined. Examination of social context is

    important because it can influence the administration of social control (Klinger 1997; Klinger

    2004; Smith 1986; Smith, Visher, and Davidson 1984; Terrill and Reisig 2003). Finally, this

    paper will also explore the predictions of these two hypotheses in the context of the crime-arrest

    literature to determine if the reciprocal relationship between crimes and arrests is affected in

    different social contexts of the city, post-displacement.

    To this end, a number of objectives must be accomplished. A review of the research on

    the conflict perspective’s threat and benign neglect hypotheses will be presented in Chapter 2.

    This discussion will highlight how previous research measures threat and segregation and the

    level at which these analyses have been conducted. The crime-arrest relationship literature will

    be discussed in Chapter 3. Because one of the aims of the present research is to examine if the

    relationship between crimes and arrests was influenced by the displacement of citizens to

    Houston, it is important to review how previous research attempts to examine the association

    between crimes and arrests. Chapter 4 provides the research strategy and methodology used to

    examine the threat hypothesis, the benign neglect hypothesis, and the relationship between

    crimes and arrests. The results of the time series analyses on crimes, arrests, and the relationship

    between the two will be presented in Chapter 5. Finally, Chapter 6 provides a discussion on the

    results, as well as the implications of the findings.

  • 9

    CHAPTER TWO

    A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE CONFLICT PERSPECTIVE

    INTRODUCTION

    The conflict perspective has a rich tradition in criminological research. In order to better

    understand the foundations of the current research, this chapter offers a discussion on the origins

    of the criminological conflict perspective’s views on crime and its predictions on the

    administration of social control. This includes a review of the threat hypothesis and the benign

    neglect hypothesis. A second priority of this chapter is to present past research on the threat and

    benign neglect hypothesis. For each study reviewed, the measurement of threat and unit of

    analysis will be offered. Third, because it is the intent of the current research to provide a more

    direct measure of threat and identify the proper unit of analysis to inspect the two hypotheses, a

    brief review of the methodological concerns of indirect measurement and aggregation bias will

    be provided. Finally, an outline of how the present research will address the issues of threat and

    aggregation bias will be offered.

    THE CONFLICT PERSPECTIVE

    The conflict perspective stems from the early works of the sociologists Karl Marx and

    Friederich Engels. Their writings focused on the conflict between the working class and the

    property owners. In short, they argue that it is in the best interest of property owners to maintain

    their elite status through the exploitation of the working class and by determining the ruling ideas

    of society. While much of the writings of Marx nor Engles did not place an emphasis on crime

    and the criminal justice system, modern day scholars have produced a theoretical work on crime

    and the criminal justice system based on Marx and Engle’s scholarship. This line of reasoning is

    the focus of this research.

  • 10

    Building on the thinking of Marx and Engles, the conflict perspective takes a unique

    perspective on crime in the sense that it does not view crime as an inherent condition. For

    example, other theories explain crime in terms of an individual’s attachment to institutions of

    society (Hirschi 1969), the level of personal self-control (Gottfredson and Hirschi 1990), the

    ability to learn crime (Burgess and Akers 1966), or the amount of strain an individual has

    (Agnew 1985, 1992). Instead, the conflict perspective asserts that crime results from the conflict

    between powerful, elite groups of society and less powerful, non-elite groups of society. It is

    this conflict between the groups that produces crime.

    One of the first criminologists to build off of Marx and Engles was Willem Bonger

    (1916). Bonger follows the original underpinnings of the conflict perspective and argues that a

    consequence of capitalism is crime (see also Currie 1997). More specifically, Bonger argues that

    capitalism stresses focus on individual level success, what Bonger labels “egoism.” Produced by

    capitalism, egoism stresses individual profits through the exploitation of others at the benefit of

    individual success and the exclusion of the larger social good. This leads to an unequal

    distribution of resources and moves the cultural towards individualism. Crime emerges from the

    increase in self-interest, as the powerless commit crimes out of a sense of need or injustice.

    Turk (1969) agrees that the ongoing struggle between elite, powerful groups and non-

    elite, powerless groups produces crime. But crime is not necessarily the result of differences in

    economic standing so much as it is the social construction of law. According to Turk (1969),

    perceptions of group behavior are relative—the behavior of any one group is going to be

    perceived as negative by the other group. Turk argues that elite groups of society label the

    behavior of non-elite groups of society as threatening and devote resources to curtail these

    “threatening” behaviors. The labeling of non-elite’s behaviors as threatening allows elite groups

  • 11

    to protect their development of the cultural order and maintain their status and control as an elite

    group (Chambliss 1975; Chambliss and Seidman 1971). This idea was further embraced by

    Richard Quinney (1969; 1970), who argues that social segments of society conceptualize and

    develop their own normative systems that define acceptable behavior. Each segment of society

    then views their own system’s definitions as normal and the other’s as abnormal. These clashing

    definitions lead to crime, with the end result being that the powerless have their definitions

    labeled as criminal.

    Imbedded in the above discussion is the notion that laws are created to protect the

    interests of the powerful. In order to protect interests, laws are created that function as a

    mechanism to control the powerless. Thus, a main feature of the conflict perspective is

    explaining the administration of social control. It is also this aspect of the conflict perspective

    emphasizes the notion of threat. One of the leading discussions of threat comes from the work of

    Hubert Blalock.

    Blalock (1967) argues that increases in social control will result when the political and

    economic status of elite groups are threatened by non-elite groups. Up to this point, there had

    been no clear articulation of what constitutes elite and non-elite groups. Addressing this, Blalock

    couches his theory in terms of minority-group relations. Succinctly stated, Blalock argues that a

    nonlinear relationship between discrimination and the size of minority groups is a result of threat

    felt by non-minority groups (pp. 144-154). Accordingly, when increases in minority groups

    threaten the socioeconomic position, competition, and power of non-minority groups, increases

    in discrimination (or motivation to discriminate) will result (see also Black 1976).

    The idea that laws are created to benefit some and not all has been amalgamated with the

    theoretical underpinnings of Blalock’s theory on minority-group relations. This marriage of

  • 12

    ideas has led to the conflict perspective’s rationale on the administration of social control. One

    of the first attempts to apply Blalock's theory of majority group discrimination to the relationship

    between minority group size and macro social control is Jackson and Carroll’s (1981)

    examination of variations in policing resources among 90 non-southern cities. Drawing on

    Blalock’s power threat hypothesis, they contend that as minority group size increases, so too

    does the level of policing resources. This relationship holds until a point at which racial

    minorities achieve a numerical superiority (around 50%) and thereby begin to resist the use of

    policing resources to suppress them. The current body of research uses two main hypotheses to

    examine social control as a function of racial tensions. These two hypotheses are known as the

    threat hypothesis and the benign neglect hypothesis.

    The Threat and Benign Neglect Hypotheses

    Both the threat and the benign neglect hypotheses make predictions on the use of social

    control based on group exposure to threat. Key to both the threat and the benign neglect

    hypotheses is the concept of threat. The difference between the two hypotheses is the group

    (elite or non-elite) who experiences the threat.

    The threat hypothesis predicts an increase in social control when elite groups experience

    threat to their social status. In order to protect their social standing and position in society, elite

    groups utilize the formal mechanisms of crime control to protect their interests from groups that

    are viewed as threatening their status quo. The ancillary benign neglect hypothesis predicts a

    decrease in crime control. This is because the groups experiencing the threat are powerless, non-

    elite groups. There are two mechanisms underlying the benign neglect hypothesis (Liska and

    Chamlin, 1984). First, non-elite groups may not be able to legitimize their complaints because of

  • 13

    their lack of socio-political power. Second, the threat may lead to increases in intra-group crime

    which may be viewed by the police as a personal matter undeserving of formal intervention.

    While there remains debate on the constitution of elite groups (Bierne 1979; Liska 1992;

    Spitzer 1975), the operationalization of key variables involved in the testing of the threat and

    benign neglect hypotheses has been established in the literature. Namely, tests of the threat

    hypothesis look at the relationship between the size of minority groups and crime control, while

    tests of the benign neglect hypothesis evaluate the relationship between the distribution of

    minority groups and crime control. Past tests of the threat and benign neglect hypotheses have

    evaluated the relationship between perceived threat and the size of police institutions (Chamlin,

    1989a; Greenburg, Kessler, and Loftin 1985; Jackson 1986, 1989; Jackson and Carroll 1981;

    Jacobs 1979; Liska, Lawrence, and Benson 1981; Nalla, Lynch, and Leiber 1997; Stults and

    Baumer 2007), police use of force (Chamlin 1989b; Holmes 2000; Liska and Yu 1992; Parker,

    MacDonald, Jennings, and Alpert 2005; Smith and Holmes 2003;), incarceration (Carmichael,

    2005; Myers 1990; Tittle and Curran 1988), executions and lynchings (Phillips 1986; Tolnay,

    Beck, and Massey 1989, 1992) or the use of arrests by police (Brown and Warner 1992;

    Chamlin 2009, Chamlin and Liska 1992; D’Alessio, Stolzenberg, and Eitle 2002; Eitle,

    D’Alessio, and Stolzenberg 2002; Eitle, Stolzenberg, and D’Alessio 2005; Liska and Chamlin

    1984; Liska, Chamlin, and Reed 1985; Parker, Stults, and Rice 2005; Stolzenburg, D’Alessio,

    and Eitle 2004). The focus of the present research is on the relationship between the use of arrest

    as social control. As such, the following review of the literature on the threat and benign neglect

    hypotheses will focus on the related research on the relationship between threat and police

    aggressiveness. Special attention will be paid to the measurement of threat, segregation, and the

    unit of analysis at which a study is conducted.

  • 14

    PREVIOUS RESEARCH ON THE THREAT AND BENIGN NEGLECT HYPOTHESES

    Williams and Drake (1980) produced the first study to evaluate the relationship between

    threat and police use of arrest. Investigating a sample of SMSAs with a population above

    500,000 in 1970, Williams and Drake inspect the role of economic inequality, percent black,

    percent unemployed, population size, and victimization rate on arrest rates for each of the Type I

    index crimes. Results showed no relationship between the structural variables—including their

    measure of percent black—and any of the seven index crimes.

    One of the drawbacks of Williams and Drake’s (1980) research was the inability to

    control for the crime rate. To address this, Liska and Chamlin evaluated a sample of 76 cities

    with a population over 100,000. The authors used regression analyses and controlled for the

    crime rate, and also the city population, police force size, percent poor, income inequality,

    segregation, and percent nonwhite. Liska and Chamlin also took the unique step of disaggregated

    property and personal arrest rates by race. This allowed for the creation of race specific models

    of arrests. Analyses for the aggregate models (i.e., models that did not disaggregate arrests by

    race) demonstrate a positive, significant relationship between percent nonwhite and personal and

    property arrests. Arrests were then disaggregated by race, revealing no significant effect of

    percent nonwhite and white personal or property arrests. This is contrary to the predictions of

    the threat hypothesis. However, percent non-white was significantly and negatively related to

    black arrest rates for both personal and property crimes, being the least important predictor in the

    white arrest model and the most important predictor in nonwhite arrest model. Inspection of the

    role of segregation revealed a significant, negative relationship to arrest rates for both white and

    nonwhite arrest rate models. The results of Liska and Chamlin’s (1984) analyses provide

    support for the benign neglect hypothesis, but not the threat hypothesis.

  • 15

    Liska, Chamlin, and Reed (1985) pit the predictions of the conflict perspective against

    the predictions of an economic approach in explaining the certainty of punishment. To do this,

    Liska and colleagues evaluate the clearance rates (ratio of arrests to crimes) for 77 cities with a

    population over 100,000. Regression analysis is used to control for the effects of workload,

    resources, population, percent poor, income inequality, segregation, and percent nonwhite on

    each of the seven index crimes (i.e., homicide, rape, assault, robbery, burglary, larceny, and auto

    theft). Results of the analyses indicate that after controlling for economic and conflict

    exogenous variables, percent nonwhite—the indicator of threat—was positively and significantly

    related to the certainty of arrest for all index crimes but assault, lending support to the threat

    hypothesis. Moreover, segregation was significantly and negatively related to arrests for all

    crimes except homicide, supporting the benign neglect hypothesis.

    To account for the possibility of period effects of their earlier research (Liska and

    Chamlin 1984), Chamlin and Liska (1992) examine similar data 10 years later. As such,

    regression analyses were conducted on race specific arrest rates for property and personal crimes

    for 97 cities. Analyses of the effects of percent nonwhite on white personal and property arrests

    rates again revealed no support for the threat hypothesis. In fact, for property arrests, whites

    were now significantly less likely to be arrested as the percentage of nonwhites in a city

    increased. Percent nonwhite was not significantly related to the arrest rate for white personal

    crimes. The relationship between segregation and arrest rates for both the white and nonwhite

    arrest models was non-significant, unlike in previous findings. In short, this analysis now

    revealed no support for benign neglect.

    Brown and Warner (1992) argue that the previous tests of the threat hypothesis may be

    subject to a historical effect—the turbulent social unrest of the 1960s. More specifically, Brown

  • 16

    and Warner argue that the reliance of the measure of percent minority may be affected by the

    social movements of the period under investigation. To determine if the findings are relevant to

    a different historical time period, Brown and Warner examine data from the 50 largest cities in

    the United States in 1900. Rather than employ percent minority as their measure of threat, the

    authors institute a measure percent foreign born to capture threat felt by native born Americans

    in predicting arrests for drunkeness. Results demonstrated that percent foreign born was a

    significant, positive predictor of arrests for drunkenness despite controls for alcohol consumption

    and police force size. In later models, this significant effect was not observed, as it appears to be

    mediated by the political and social structure of a city. With regards to segregation, there was no

    support for the benign neglect hypothesis as this variable demonstrated no statistically significant

    relationship to arrests.

    Eitle, D’Alessio, and Stolzenberg (2002) take issue with the operationalization of threat

    as the relative size of minority population. To address this, the authors utilize three distinct

    measures of threat: (1) the ratio of black-to-white voting; (2) the ratio of black-to-white

    unemployment; and (3) the black-on-white crime rate. Eitle and colleagues use NIBRS data

    from 46 counties in South Carolina to estimate pooled cross-section time series equations.

    Analysis on the effects of black-on-white crime on black arrest rates revealed a positive,

    significant relationship. Specifically, as interracial crime increases, the probability of a black

    individual being arrested also increases, which is consistent with the threat hypothesis.

    However, there was no statistical relationship between the other two measures of threat, the ratio

    of black-to-white voting or the ratio of black-to-white unemployment, and arrest rates. Eitle and

    colleagues did not include a measure of segregation in any of their analyses.

  • 17

    In a subsequent study, D’Alessio, Stolzenberg, and Eitle (2002) evaluate the effects of

    both the ratio of black-to-white voting and the ratio of black-to-white unemployment, as well as

    the effect of percent nonwhite, on four different dependent variables. These four endogenous

    variables are: (1) white-on-black crime rate; (2) black-on-white crime rate; (3) white-on-white

    crime rate; and (4) black-on-black crime rate. Again, data were from the NIBRS on 46 counties

    in South Carolina. Analyses from the pooled cross-section time series equations revealed no

    significant effects for the percent nonwhite or the ratio of black-to-white voting across any of the

    four dependent variables. The ratio of black-to-white unemployment was a significant, negative

    relationship with white-on-black crime. This variable has no significant impact on any of the

    other victim-offender dyads. Segregation was not included as a variable in any of the models.

    Kane (2003) explores a different unit of analysis than past studies. Specifically, Kane

    argues that threat hypothesis research is better suited for a smaller unit of analysis. Kane

    suggests that police districts are the most appropriate units because this aggregate level is where

    police develop work groups and norms of behavior. To this end, Kane hypothesizes that

    increases in nonwhite population, disaggregated into percent black and percent Latino, will lead

    to increases in police deployment. A panel design is used on three years of data from the New

    York City Police Department. Results from the analyses showed no relationship between police

    deployment and percent black. However, once the Latino population of a police district reached

    23 percent, there was a significant, positive relationship with police deployment. Thus, Kane’s

    police district analysis provided mixed support for the threat hypothesis.

    While Kane (2002) did not include a specific measure of segregation in his models, his

    data were able to capture the percent black and percent Latino of each district under

    investigation. In this sense, he did succeed in capturing the relative segregation of each police

  • 18

    district. Accordingly, Kane found no support for the benign neglect hypothesis. Increases in

    black population (i.e., increases in district segregation) were unrelated to arrests, and increases in

    Latino population in each district (i.e., increases in district segregation) were in the opposite

    direction predicted by the benign neglect hypothesis.

    Stolzenburg, D’Alessio, and Eitle (2004) combine microlevel NIBRS data with macro

    level census data to develop a multilevel test of the threat hypothesis. In short, Stolzenberg and

    colleagues wanted to control for individual factors (e.g., race) as well as structural factors (e.g.,

    segregation) to determine the important influences on the probability of arrest for a violent

    crime. The results of the multilevel analysis failed to support the threat hypothesis. Instead, the

    authors found that the percent population black was significantly and negatively related to the

    probability of arrest. That is, as a city’s black population increased, the likelihood of arrests

    decreased, which is not predicted by the threat hypothesis. While this finding is in disagreement

    with the threat hypothesis, it appears to be consistent with the benign neglect hypothesis.

    However, further analysis revealed that percent black was not significantly related to the

    likelihood that black offenders will be arrested or the probability that intraracial crimes will

    result in an arrest. Analyses of segregation revealed that there was a significant and negative

    relationship between a city’s level of segregation and (1) the overall probability of arrest (black

    or white offender), (2) the likelihood of black offenders being arrested, and (3) the probability

    that intraracial crimes (black-on-white) resulted in an arrest. Taken as a whole, these findings

    provided no support for the threat hypothesis, and mixed support for the benign neglect

    hypothesis.

    Parker, Stults, and Rice (2005) offer different operationalizations of threat by

    incorporating three different measures of threat in their analyses. Specifically, Parker and

  • 19

    colleagues measure threat through the traditional percent black population, but also include the

    measures of percent black immigration and a racial inequality index. The racial inequality index

    was developed using principal components factor analysis with the following factors: percent

    persons living below poverty, income inequality, percent males jobless aged 16 and older,

    percent children not living with both parents, white-black unemployment ratio, white-black

    bachelor degree ratio, and white-black high school diploma ratio. Data were from 245 cities

    with a population over 100,000 in 2000. The authors estimated separate models for arrests

    disaggregated by race. Results demonstrated no relationship for the racial inequality index for

    either white or black arrests. Moreover, the measures of percent black and percent black

    immigration were not related to white arrests, but were significantly and negatively related to

    black arrests. Specifically, increases in the size of the black population and increases in the

    number of black immigrants were associated with a decrease in the number of black arrests.

    These findings are inconsistent with the threat hypothesis, but consistent with the benign neglect

    hypothesis. Nevertheless, analysis of segregation produced no statistical relationship with

    arrests, leading to mixed findings regarding the benign neglect hypothesis.

    Eitle, Stolzenberg, and D’Alessio (2005) employed hierarchal linear modeling to examine

    the impact of individual, city, and police organization variables on probabilities of arrest for

    aggravated assaults and simple assault separately in 105 small cities. There was no relationship

    between a city’s percent black population and simple or aggravated assaults. Thus, Eitle and

    colleagues did not find support for the threat hypothesis. However, with regards to the benign

    neglect hypothesis, there was a significant negative relationship between a city’s level of

    segregation and probability of arrest for aggravated assault and simple assault. In fact, the

  • 20

    authors noted that the difference between arrest probabilities for whites and blacks was relatively

    large. This finding supports the predictions of benign neglect.

    Chamlin (2009) also attempts to provide a more direct measure of threat. Rather than

    rely on a compositional measure of minority population, Chamlin uses the occurrence of a race

    riot as a measure threat. Using time series data on robbery arrests in the city, Chamlin finds an

    increase in robbery arrests directly after the threatening event of the race riot, as predicted by the

    threat hypothesis. To control for the possibility of a history effect, similar data were examined

    for the cities of Dayton and Columbus, Ohio, where no riots occurred, and no changes were

    observed in the level of robbery arrests. There were no controls for segregation in Chamlin’s

    analysis.

    Summary

    The above discussions have demonstrated that results of tests of the threat hypothesis and

    benign neglect hypothesis are mixed. Two themes emerged in the above discussion: (1) tests of

    the threat hypothesis either lacked direct measures of threat or the exploration of more direct

    measures was a focal concern; and (2) the vast majority of tests were conducted at the city level

    of analysis. Because findings for both hypotheses were clearly mixed, this dissertation will

    address these two themes. As such, the next section provides a discussion surrounding the

    concerns of the two methodological issues: indirect measurement and aggregation bias.

    METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES

    Indirect Measurement: The Threat Hypothesis and the Size of Minority Groups

    The size of minority group is the most frequently used operationalization of threat. The

    use of this measure stems from Blalock’s (1967) assertion that increases in black minority groups

    are viewed as threatening to the status of the white majority. Tests of the threat hypothesis

  • 21

    evaluate the correlation between percent black and the size of a police force or the use of arrest

    by police. A significant positive association between the size of minority group and crime

    control is taken as an overall indication that there is a response to perceived threat to elites.

    Therefore, the indirect measurement of threat has become a staple in previous research on the

    conflict perspective’s hypotheses on the administration of crime control.

    The inability to directly measure threat presents an immediate obstacle for theory testing,

    thus leaving the interpretation of the correlation between the size of a minority group and crime

    control susceptible to multiple interpretations. For example, a structural functionalist argument

    would say that increases to the size of minority groups represent a remonstrance to the

    universalistic values of the public good (Durkheim 1933; Inverarity 1976; Lauderdale 1976).

    This argument was made by Inverarity (1976:278) who argues that the observed positive

    relationship between percent black and lynchings across Louisiana Parishes is a function of the

    detrimental effects of racial heterogeneity on the stable operation of societal order. A similar

    example can be provided using social disorganization theory (Shaw and McKay 1972).

    One of the key exogenous variables in social disorganization theory is population

    heterogeneity. According to social disorganization theory, racial heterogeneity impedes

    communication and centralizing community interaction. This in turn weakens informal social

    control and increases the need for formal social control (Bursik 1988; Sampson and Groves

    1989; Shaw and McKay 1972). But this relationship between heterogeneity and crime is

    inherently nonlinear. As population heterogeneity lessens (increasing minority group

    homogeneity), reliance on formal mechanisms of social control (such as arrest) should also

    decrease. As noted above, Blalock (1967) argued for a nonlinear relationship between

    discrimination and percent minority. In his power-threat hypothesis, increases in percent

  • 22

    minority threaten non-minority populations. However, once the minority groups numbers are

    large enough, they are able to defy the discrimination by non-minority groups. The presence of

    this curvilinear relationship has been supported by research on the conflict perspective. Jackson

    and Carroll (1981) identified this concave relationship between percent black and police

    expenditures, while Greenberg, Kessler, and Loftin (1985) found the same form in the

    relationship between percent non-white and police force size.

    The same relationships predicted by conflict theory can also be explained by competing

    theories’ hypotheses, which fails to falsify the predictions of any theory. In order to avoid the

    problem of multiple interpretations and failing to falsify conflict theory’s threat hypothesis, it

    stands to reason that a more direct measure of threat is needed. This dissertation offers a more

    direct measure of threat—the displacement of citizens by Hurricane Katrina—which allows for

    the falsification of competing theoretical explanations.

    The Proper Unit of Analysis: The Distribution of Minority Groups and the Benign Neglect

    Hypothesis

    The notion of group distribution within society has a place in the conflict perspective.

    For example, Quinney (1974; 1977) argues that the powerful utilize class fragmentation to get

    the powerless class to fight within themselves. Blauner (1972) suggests that segregation can

    influence the administration of social control by affecting the relative probabilities of the

    offender-victim dyad. Specifically, the segregation of blacks (the less powerful) lessens the

    likelihood of interracial victimizations and simultaneously increases the probability of intraracial

    victimizations. Thus, based on the benign neglect hypothesis, one would predict that the

    segregation of racial minorities would reduce reliance on more overt and formal mechanisms of

    social control (Blauner 1972; Spitzer 1975).

  • 23

    Evaluation of the benign neglect hypothesis looks at a city’s level of segregation and its

    relationship to crime control. If a significant, negative relationship is observed, this is taken as

    evidence of benign neglect. Measurement of segregation is typically done through the use of the

    dissimilarity index, which compares the racial composition of city blocks to the racial

    composition of the city as a whole. This is done for all blocks in order to measure the

    percentage of one racial group that would have to move to a different neighborhood to be

    distributed similarly as another racial group, and produces an index score that measures

    segregation for the entire city (See Massey and Denton, 1988). Thus the benign neglect

    hypothesis is studied at the city level of analysis. This research contends that this level of

    analysis may be inappropriate for the study of the benign neglect hypothesis given the

    mechanisms underlying benign neglect.

    Picture a sample of cities, each containing only one or two neighborhoods that are

    segregated. If this is the circumstance, there would likely be little variation in the level of

    segregation across a sample of cities. Hence, measures of segregation probably would have

    little, if any, impact on crime control outcomes. Therefore, the segregation on crime for the one

    neighborhood of a city would go unnoticed (Blauner 1972; Spitzer 1975). In addition to the

    mechanisms of benign neglect warranting further investigations of the proper unit of analysis,

    there is another reason to suspect a more appropriate unit of analysis—police use of social

    control is a function of the social context within which they operate (Klinger 1997; Klinger

    2004; Smith 1986; Smith, Visher, and Davidson 1984; Terrill and Reisig 2003). Because the

    conflict perspective is interested in explaining social control, it stands to reason to evaluate social

    control at a level where effects may differ.

  • 24

    Research has demonstrated that social control is a function of social context. Smith,

    Visher, and Davidson (1984) included a measurement of neighborhood poverty in a multivariate

    analysis exploring the antecedents of arrest across sixty residential neighborhoods. Their

    analysis shows that the probability of arrests by police increased as the poverty level of the

    neighborhood increased. Smith (1986) analysis on the effects of 11 neighborhood characteristics

    (e.g., poverty, residential mobility, racial heterogeneity) identified that: police are more likely to

    stop suspicious people in racially heterogeneous neighborhoods; in neighborhoods of lower

    socioeconomic status, the odds of arrest were higher; and there was an inverse relationship

    between neighborhood crime rates and the likelihood of police officers taking a crime report.

    Smith (1986: 337) concludes that “officers act differently in different neighborhood contexts.”

    Terrill and Reisig (2003) found that police use of force was a function of the

    neighborhood context. More specifically, Terril and Reisig’s analysis included a factor of

    neighborhood poverty consisting of percent in poverty, percent unemployed, percent of female-

    headed families, and percent black and found a significant, positive relationship between

    neighborhood poverty and police use of force, as well as neighborhood crime level and police

    use of force. The totality of these findings has spawned the advance of a theory that

    acknowledges community characteristics when explaining police action (Klinger 1997).

    Moreover, this research supports the arguments made in this dissertation that past conflict

    perspective studies using the city as a unit of analysis may have masked observable effects.

    THE CURRENT STUDY

    Clearly the inability to provide a direct measure of threat and addressing aggregation bias

    are important considerations when testing the threat and benign neglect hypotheses. The current

    research utilizes a quasi-experimental, time series design to account for the occurrence of

  • 25

    Hurricane Katrina and the resulting displacement of citizens to Houston and determine whether

    or not this event impacted arrest levels of various police districts. I believe the use of citizen

    displacement by Hurricane Katrina constitutes a more direct measure of threat, and inspection of

    police districts rather than just the city addresses the issue of aggregation bias.

    Citizens were ordered to evacuate New Orleans on short notice, and eventually Houston

    housed approximately 250,000 evacuees (Berger 2006). I believe that the abrupt nature and

    magnitude of citizen displacement constitutes a threatening event for both the displaced citizens

    as well as the citizens that make up communities that now must house a larger population. By

    accounting for the displacement of citizens by Hurricane Katrina, this research provides a more

    direct measure of threat than the traditional size of minority population.

    Furthermore, data on clearance levels will be inspected by police division, not just at the

    city level of analysis. The use of police divisions allows for the characteristics of the each

    division to be accounted for. As police behavior can be influenced by the divisions within which

    they operate (Klinger 1997; Klinger 2004; Smith 1986; Smith, Visher, and Davidson 1984;

    Terrill and Reisig 2003), it is important to account for changes in clearances within a city and

    across divisions. The time series design of this study allows for this to occur. Moreover,

    because displaced citizens were housed within specific police divisions, this allows for specific

    predictions on which areas of Houston should experience threat or benign neglect.

    By directly accounting for the occurrence of a threatening event to all populations and

    evaluating the effects of this event within a city, this paper is positioned to provide a more direct

    test of the threat hypothesis and benign neglect hypothesis.

  • 26

    CHAPTER THREE

    SOCIAL CONTEXT AND THE CRIME-ARREST RELATIONSHIP: EVALUATING THE PREDICTIONS OF THE CONFLICT PERSPECTIVE

    INTRODUCTION

    The previous chapter was devoted to the predictions of the conflict perspective’s threat

    and benign neglect hypotheses, and presented the argument that issues of measurement and

    aggregation bias may play a role in the proper identification of the level at which threat and

    benign neglect occur. Central to the argument was the role of social context. Succinctly stated,

    Chapter Two argued that it may be important to take into account characteristics of the social

    context when evaluating conflict theory hypotheses. This chapter extends the argument that

    social context is an important component of the conflict perspective to research on the crime-

    arrest relationship.

    It has long been posited that crimes and arrests are interlinked (Becker 1968; Gibbs 1975;

    Tittle 1980). Three findings from the research on the crime-arrest relationship are paramount to

    the present discussion: (1) most studies rely on the predictions of hypotheses derived from

    economic theories and fail to take into account the predictions of the conflict perspective; (2)

    results from previous research appear to be a function of the methodology used to examine the

    reciprocal relationship between crimes and arrests; and (3) the role of social context appears to

    be an important part of the crime-arrest relationship.

    There are many hypotheses surrounding this relationship. Because the current study is

    concerned with the macro level crime-arrest relationship, attention will be paid to the relevant

    macro-social literature. This chapter is presented accordingly: first, a review of the theoretical

    predictions regarding the crime and arrest relationship is discussed. This is provided so in order

  • 27

    to demonstrate how the predictions of the conflict perspective can be incorporated into the

    research on the crime-arrest relationship. Second, a review of the research on the reciprocal

    relationship between crimes and arrests is presented. This section will organize the relevant

    literature around the methodology used to examine the crime-arrest relationship (i.e., cross

    sectional or longitudinal). This vital section demonstrates how past research has relied on

    economic theories of crime, although, as demonstrated in the first section of this chapter, the

    conflict perspective may have utility in explaining the crime-arrest relationship. This section

    also demonstrates how the methodology of past research on the crime-arrest relationship has

    driven findings. Finally, an argument is offered on how threat may influence the social context

    of an area and may be useful in examining the relationship between crime and arrests.

    THE CAUSAL MECHANISMS UNDERLYING THE CRIME-ARREST

    RELATIONSHIP

    The attempt to identify the causal linkage between crimes and arrest has a long history

    (see Geerken and Gove 1977). Much of this history surrounds an attempt to unearth the true

    causal ordering of the relationship, as well as the direction of this relationship. For example, one

    can imagine how crimes would affect arrests (e.g., as crimes go up, police respond with more

    arrests). One could also picture how arrests affect crimes (e.g., as arrests increase, criminals

    become more aware and crime decreases). In the case of the direction of the relationship, it is

    viable to picture a situation in which crimes exert a positive influence on arrests, but it is also

    likely that crimes could exert a negative impact on arrests, for example. The discussions below

    will illustrate hypotheses dedicated to explaining the relationship between arrests and crimes.

    Economic theory has been at the forefront of reasoning behind research into this

    relationship (Becker 1968; Ehrlich 1973; Schmidt and Witte 1984). Economic theory

  • 28

    approaches the relationship between crime and arrests using a precise model based on a general

    economic perspective. Namely, economic theory assumes that individuals have relatively stable

    interests. Behavior exhibited by individuals is a result of this stability and the weighing of costs

    versus benefits to alternative behaviors. Individuals choose to exhibit behavior that maximizes

    the ratio of benefits to costs. Therefore, individuals will not exhibit particular behaviors when

    the costs of that behavior outweigh the benefits of displaying that behavior (see, Becker 1968).

    The role of crime control (e.g., arrests), from an economic standpoint, is to raise the costs

    of criminal behavior so that individuals will choose to exhibit non-criminal behavior because the

    ratio of benefits to costs is reduced. Failure to increase the costs of crime will result in more

    criminal behavior. Thus, the crime arrest relationship can be viewed as part of an overall general

    model (see, Liska 1987). Further, crime control can be affected by workload and resources (see

    Phillips and Votey 1981; Liska et al. 1985). The following section details predictions of an

    economic approach to the crime arrest research.

    The first hypothesis is termed the public choice hypothesis. It predicts that increases in

    crime lead to increases in arrests. This hypothesis makes the assumptions that public citizens are

    knowledgeable about the costs and consequences of municipal functioning (Borcherding and

    Deacon 1972: 892) and the polity respond to voter outcry (Tridimas 2001: 300). Therefore, this

    hypothesis states that increases in crime lead to citizens’ placing more pressure on the

    government (i.e., police, politicians) to crack down on crime in their area. As an example,

    politicians, serving at the pleasure of voters, may cry out for more crime control because of

    pressure from the public. This may then result in more arrests as crime increases (Borcherding

    and Deacon 1972; Deacon 1978; Tridimas 2001).

  • 29

    Another hypothesis put forth by economic theory predicts that increases in crime lead to

    decreases in clearances. This hypothesis is known as the overload hypothesis (Chamlin and

    Brandl 1998; Fisher and Nagin 1978; Geerken and Gove 1977; Schulenberg 2003). The

    overload hypothesis predicts that increases in crime can overwhelm the capacities of law

    enforcement. When police capacities have reached their limit, the ability to respond to other

    crimes is greatly reduced. Therefore, the lessening of police resources leads to fewer aggregate

    arrests (Fisher and Nagin 1978; Geerken and Gove 1977).

    Economic theory also hypothesizes how arrests can influence crime, the deterrence

    hypothesis (in general, see Becker 1968; Geerken and Gove 1977; Gibbs 1975; Lynch 1999;

    Nagin 1998; Paternoster 1987; Petersilia 1998; Pratt, Cullen, Blevins, Daigle, and Madensen

    2006). The deterrence hypothesis also operates under a utilitarianism approach by arguing that

    humans are rational actors who base their decisions on the maximization of profit. Because

    rational actors weigh the costs of exhibiting a behavior against the benefits of exhibiting the

    same behavior, an increase in arrest (i.e., the certainty of punishment) should sway rational

    actors away from performing criminal behaviors. The deterrence hypothesis argues that

    increases in arrests (cost) make crime a less profitable option and, therefore, leads to a decrease

    in crime (Becker 1968; Geerken and Gove 1977; Gibbs 1975).

    Another hypothesis, the incapacitation hypothesis, makes a similar prediction of the

    affect of punishment (e.g., arrests) on crimes (in general, see DiIulio and Piehl 1991; Geerken

    and Gove 1977; Gottfredson and Gottfredson 1994; Marvel and Moody 1994; Petersilia 1992;

    Piehl and DiIulio 1995; Sabol and Lynch 2000; Spelman 2000; Visher 1987; Zimring and

    Hawkins 1988). Increases in arrests lead to the removal of offenders from the social order,

    thereby reducing the amount of crime that could have been committed had these individuals not

  • 30

    been apprehended. To the extent that an increase in arrests removes individuals from society

    and they cannot return to the general public while awaiting criminal justice proceedings, the

    aggregate level of crime should decrease (Geerken and Gove 1977).

    The hypotheses outlined above provide predictions on the reciprocal relationship between

    arrests and crimes under the umbrella of economic theory. As will be discussed in the

    subsequent section, the vast majority (all but one) of the macro-social studies on the crime-arrest

    relationship evaluate the crime-arrest relationship using one or more of these hypotheses. Far

    fewer tests have used the predictions of the conflict perspective to inform research on the

    reciprocal relationship between arrests and crimes. The following paragraphs will illustrate how

    the predictions of the threat hypothesis and benign neglect hypothesis can be utilized to better

    understand the crime-arrest relationship.

    As discussed in Chapter Two, the conflict perspective conceptualizes crime control as a

    tool used by elites to advance their own interests (Black 1976; Blalock 1967; Quinney 1970,

    1977; Turk 1969). This view-point argues that culturally dissimilar groups are viewed as threats

    to the social order by elites and law enforcement practices are used as a mechanism to control

    these threatening populations (Turk 1969). The threat and benign neglect hypotheses are two

    predictions used to explain the use of social control. The threat hypothesis predicts an increase

    in arrests when the status of the social elites is threatened. The benign neglect hypothesis

    predicts a decrease in arrests when the non-elite are threatened. Therefore, the difference

    between an increase or a decrease in social control is the group that is experiencing threat.

    The utility of the threat and benign neglect hypotheses role in the crime-arrest

    relationship is clear. If the displacement of citizens by Hurricane Katrina to Houston, Texas,

    increased fear of crime among elite populations (i.e., the group experiencing threat), this would

  • 31

    be reflected in a positive association between crimes and arrests (the threat hypothesis). If the

    group experiencing threat were the non-elite populations, then the benign neglect hypothesis

    would be reflected by a negative association between crime and arrests (see Chamlin and Myer

    2009).

    The relationship and direction between crime and arrests has received significant

    empirical examination. The following section reviews macro-level research on this relationship.

    Two issues are relevant—methodology used and social context. As such, the section that follows

    will begin with a discussion on cross-sectional research, followed by discussions on longitudinal

    research. Finally, research on the role of social context in the examination of the crime-arrest

    relationship is presented.

    A REVIEW OF THE CRIME-ARREST LITERATURE

    Examination of the crime-arrest relationship has typically employed two different designs

    in an attempt to investigate the reciprocal nature of crimes and arrests: cross sectional designs

    and longitudinal designs. Research on the reciprocal relationship between crimes and arrests

    began under examination of cross-sectional data. This section begins with a review of research

    on the crime-arrest association with cross-sectional data.

    Sjoquist (1973) used data from 53 municipalities with populations between 25,000 and

    200,000 in 1960 to examine 1968 property crimes (i.e., burglary, larceny, and robbery) and

    arrests and convictions reported to the FBI. Multiple regression was used to estimate the effects

    of arrests on crimes. Results demonstrated that an increase in the probability of arrest and

    conviction was significantly related to a decrease in property crime. This result offers support

    for the deterrence hypothesis. Tittle and Rowe (1974) also revealed support for the deterrence

    hypothesis. Using data from cities and counties with populations over 2,500, the authors found a

  • 32

    significant negative correlation between certainty of arrest and crime rates for counties (-.65) and

    cities (-.19).

    Geerken and Gove (1977) used data on 65 SMSAs with populations over 500,000. For

    each of these units, the authors combined the reported number of crimes (i.e., homicide and non-

    negligent manslaughter, aggravated assault, rape, robbery, burglary, larceny, and auto theft) for

    the years 1970, 1971, and 1973. For each of these indices, Geerken and Gove then created a

    certainty of arrest measure by dividing the number of crimes cleared by the number of crimes

    reported. These two measures were then correlated to determine if any significant relationship

    existed. Geerken and Gove found a significant negative association for all crimes (r = -.25), auto

    theft (r = -.65), larceny (r = -.41), robbery (r = -.40), burglary (r = -.22), and rape (r = -.26).

    There were no significant findings for either assault or homicide. Thus, the authors argue their

    findings, at least in regards to economic crimes, support the deterrence hypothesis.

    Brown (1978) analyzed crime and arrest data in California cities and counties. In regards

    to the California counties (n=58), Brown found a negative association between the arrest

    clearance rate and the crime rate (r = -.39). When cities in California were the unit of analysis (n

    = 114), a negative association between arrest probability and crime rate also emerged (r = -.17),

    although smaller than the counties measure of association.

    Using data from 35 large American cities in 1975, Wilson and Boland (1978) used two-

    stage least squares regression to examine the crime arrest relationship. Their analysis revealed a

    significant negative relationship between the robbery crime rate and the robbery arrest ratio

    while controlling for age, sex, race, unemployment, and population. The same findings could

    not be replicated for burglary or auto theft.

  • 33

    While the above studies demonstrate that the crimes and arrests are in fact related to one

    another, the above cross-sectional research fails to do is explain the mechanisms underlying this

    relationship. The above research cannot articulate whether crimes influence arrest, or whether

    arrest influence crimes. While one could employ simultaneous equations to unravel the

    reciprocal effects between crimes and arrests, it would involve the ability to identify and employ

    instrumental variables that allow one to uniquely identify the two equations (Hanushek and

    Jackson 1977). However, there has yet to be an identifiable instrumental macro-level predictor

    that affects one of these variables and not the other (for example, see Fisher and Nagin 1978;

    Greenberg and Kessler 1982; Nagin 1978). In short, cross-sectional designs investigating this

    relationship are unable to deal with simultaneous equation bias (Greenberg and Kessler 1982).

    As Logan (1975) laments, cross-sectional designs fail to divulge the direction of the crime-arrest

    relationship.

    In an attempt to unearth the proper temporal order of the crime-arrest relationship, many

    studies began to use longitudinal designs. Specifically, researchers employed the technique of

    multi-wave panel designs to examine the reciprocal relationship between arrests and crimes.

    Logan (1975) used 1964 to 1968 state level data to examine the effect of arrest clearances on

    crimes. Arrest clearance was calculated as the number of crimes cleared by arrest divided by the

    number of crimes known to police. Seven indices were created for each of the FBI Type I

    crimes (i.e., homicide, rape, robbery, aggravated assault, burglary, larceny, and auto theft).

    Logan employed a two-wave panel design for successive years to examine a lagged and

    immediate relationship between yearly measures of crime and arrest. Findings from Logan’s

    analyses reveal no evidence of any relationship between crimes and arrests or arrests and crimes.

  • 34

    Greenberg, Kessler, and Logan (1979) used a three-wave panel design on yearly arrest

    clearance and offense rates for 98 U.S. cities for the years 1964 to 1970. As above, the panel

    design was used to examine possible lagged and instantaneous effects of the reciprocal

    relationship. Of the 24 estimated coefficients examining the effect of crime on arrest, only two

    were significant (assault and homicide). However, the assault coefficient was small and unstable

    and the homicide coefficient was not in the predictive direction (i.e., positively related). For the

    24 coefficients in their model predicting the effect of arrest on crime, no coefficients emerged as

    significant predictors. In an extension of this analysis, Greenberg and Kessler (1982) again used

    a three-wave panel design on the same data, this time controlling for 12 structural predictors.

    The results of these analyses provided few estimates reaching statistical significance, and were

    thus not noticeably different from the above study.

    The above panel designs were an attempt to address the troubles cross-sectional designs

    had in determining the reciprocal relationship between crimes and arrest, and these studies were

    unable to replicate the findings of cross-sectional studies. However, panel studies are not

    without their own limitations. Namely, the panel studies and the data used place restrictions on

    the estimation of the lag structure between crime and arrests. The studies by Logan (1975),

    Greenberg et al. (1979), and Greenberg and Kessler (1982) use yearly data. If the relationship

    between arrests and crimes is distinguished by yearly lags, then the techniques used are

    satisfactory. However, there is reason to believe that the reciprocal relationship between crimes

    and arrests occurs at a smaller lag (see, for example, Bursik, Grasmick, and Chamlin 1990;

    Chamlin 1988; Chamlin, Grasmick, Bursik, and Cochran 1992). Thus, the inability of panel

    designs to provide evidence of a reciprocal relationship may be due to model misspecification.

  • 35

    Chamlin (1988) addressed the identification of a lagged relationship through the use of

    auto regressive integrated moving average (ARIMA) techniques. The use of ARIMA techniques

    does not require one to make rigid assumptions about the potential lag structure between arrests

    and crimes. Rather, bivariate ARIMA analyses allow the data to determine the lag structure.

    This is beneficial because no theory specifies precisely the lag at which crimes affect arrests or

    vice versa (Greenberg and Kessler 1982; Loftin and McDowall 1982). To this end, Chamlin

    (1988) uses monthly data for both Oklahoma City and Tulsa spanning 1967 to 1980 for the

    offense categories of robbery, burglary, larceny, and auto theft. Chamlin found that arrests have

    a negative effect (with a 1 month lag) on crime for robbery, but not for the remaining crimes.

    This is in partial support of the deterrence hypothesis. However, there was no lagged

    relationship evidenced for the effect of crime on arrests for either city on any of the offense

    types.

    In a further examination of the lag structure between arrests and crimes, Chamlin and

    colleagues (1992) evaluated the reciprocal relationship between arrests and crimes for the

    individual offenses of robbery, burglary, larceny, and auto theft in Oklahoma City. Each of the

    series was examined at the monthly, quarterly, and semiannual time aggregation. Bivariate

    ARIMA analyses indicated: (1) crime had no significant lagged effects on arrests; (2) monthly

    levels of arrest had significant negative effects on robbery crimes and auto theft at the one month

    lag; and (3) quarterly arrests had a negative effect on larceny crimes at the 1 quarter lag. Thus,

    the results support a short term deterrent effect.

    Bursik, Grasmick, and Chamlin (1990) argue that prior examinations of the reciprocal

    relationship between crimes and arrests are not only hindered by the inflexibility in examining

    the proper lag structure, but they are also confounded with aggregation bias. Greenberg and

  • 36

    colleagues (1981) and Green