253
UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION: MEASUREMENT, THEORY, AND CHANGE OVER TIME by Rebecca J. Collie B.Ed. (Hons.), University of Melbourne, 2006 M.A., University of British Columbia, 2010 A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in The Faculty of Graduate and Postdoctoral Studies (Human Development, Learning and Culture) THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA (Vancouver) February 2014 © Rebecca J. Collie, 2014

UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    6

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION:

MEASUREMENT, THEORY, AND CHANGE OVER TIME

by

Rebecca J. Collie

B.Ed. (Hons.), University of Melbourne, 2006

M.A., University of British Columbia, 2010

A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF

THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

in

The Faculty of Graduate and Postdoctoral Studies

(Human Development, Learning and Culture)

THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA

(Vancouver)

February 2014

© Rebecca J. Collie, 2014

Page 2: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

ABSTRACT

Teacher well-being and motivation play important roles in teacher and student experiences at

school. When teachers are faring well and feeling motivated to teach, they are more effective

in their teaching, leave the profession less often, and promote motivation and achievement

among their students. In this dissertation, three studies that investigated teacher well-being

and motivation were conducted with the aim of advancing our understanding of the two

constructs, as well as how they can be promoted among teachers. Study 1 involved

conceptualising, developing, and testing the Teacher Well-Being Scale, which measures

three factors of teacher well-being: workload well-being, organisational well-being, and

student interaction well-being. Among a sample of 603 practicing teachers, results revealed

that the new measure functioned similarly across the different demographic groups in the

sample and that the three factors of well-being related as expected with other constructs

(stress, job satisfaction, and flourishing). Study 2 involved elaborating and testing an

explanatory model of teacher well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and affective

organisational commitment that was based in self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985,

2002). Using the same sample as Study 1, structural equation modelling provided support for

the model’s main relationships. In addition, there were some unexpected findings that

provide directions for future research (e.g., a double-sided view of autonomy revealing that it

can be associated with positive and negative types of motivation). Study 3 involved

examining growth curve models of change in teacher well-being and self-efficacy for

teaching over two to three months. Among a sample of 71 practicing teachers, the findings

showed that teacher well-being was stable over time, whereas self-efficacy for classroom

management increased (the other two types of self-efficacy that were examined, self-efficacy

ii

Page 3: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

for student engagement and instructional strategies, did not change over time). Findings also

revealed the significance of the basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence, and

relatedness) in predicting teacher well-being and self-efficacy. Taken together, the three

studies help to improve our understanding of the highly important variables of teacher well-

being and motivation. Implications of the findings for both research and practice are

discussed.

iii

Page 4: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

PREFACE

This dissertation is original, unpublished, independent work by the author, R. Collie. The

three studies were approved by The University of British Columbia’s Behavioral Research

Ethics Board. Certificate number for Studies 1 and 2: H11-03417. Certificate number for Study

3: H10-02457.

iv

Page 5: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Abstract ..................................................................................................................................... ii Preface ..................................................................................................................................... iv

Table of Contents .......................................................................................................................v

List of Tables ......................................................................................................................... viii List of Figures ............................................................................................................................x

Acknowledgements.................................................................................................................. xi Dedication ............................................................................................................................... xii Chapter 1. Introduction ..............................................................................................................1

1.1. Well-being ...................................................................................................................... 1

1.2. Motivation ...................................................................................................................... 5

1.3. The Dissertation Research ............................................................................................. 7

Chapter 2. Study 1: Conceptualising and Measuring Teacher Well-being ...............................9

2.1. Literature Review ........................................................................................................... 9

2.1.1. Measurement Method .............................................................................................13

2.1.2. Overview of the Current Study ..............................................................................16

2.2. Methods ........................................................................................................................ 23

2.2.1. Participants .............................................................................................................23

2.2.2. Procedures ..............................................................................................................29

2.2.3. Measures .................................................................................................................34

2.3. Results .......................................................................................................................... 39

2.3.1. Factor Analyses ......................................................................................................39

2.3.2. Demographic Comparisons ....................................................................................49

2.3.3. Preliminary Evidence of Validity ...........................................................................53

2.4. Discussion .................................................................................................................... 56

2.4.1. Research Question 1: What is the Nature of Teacher Well-being? ........................57

2.4.2. Research Question 2: How do Scores on the New Measure Differ Across Demographic Groups? ......................................................................................................63

2.4.3. Research Question 3: Are Interpretations of Scores from the New Measure Supported by Evidence of Validity? ................................................................................65

2.5. Limitations and Future Directions ............................................................................... 67

2.6. Conclusions .................................................................................................................. 71

Chapter 3. Study 2: Developing and Testing a Framework .....................................................73

v

Page 6: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.1. Literature Review ......................................................................................................... 73

3.1.1. Self-Determination Theory ....................................................................................76

3.1.2. SDT, Job Satisfaction, and Organisational Commitment ......................................82

3.1.3. The Explanatory Model ..........................................................................................88

3.1.4. Overview of the Current Study ..............................................................................97

3.2. Methods ........................................................................................................................ 99

3.2.1. Participants and Procedures ...................................................................................99

3.2.2. Measures .................................................................................................................99

3.3. Results ........................................................................................................................ 104

3.3.1. Data Analyses .......................................................................................................104

3.3.2. Factor Analyses ....................................................................................................106

3.3.3. Structural Modelling ............................................................................................113

3.4. Discussion .................................................................................................................. 121

3.4.1. Basic Psychological Needs, Well-being, and Motivation ....................................122

3.4.2. Job Satisfaction ....................................................................................................126

3.4.3. Affective Organisational Commitment ................................................................128

3.5. Limitations and Future Directions ............................................................................. 130

3.6. Conclusions ................................................................................................................ 132

Chapter 4. Study 3: Teacher Well-Being and Motivation Over Time ...................................134

4.1. Literature Review ....................................................................................................... 134

4.1.1. Teachers’ Experiences Over Time .......................................................................135

4.1.2. Overview of the Current Study ............................................................................144

4.2. Methods ...................................................................................................................... 148

4.2.1. Participants ...........................................................................................................148

4.2.2. Procedures ............................................................................................................149

4.2.3. Measures ...............................................................................................................150

4.3. Results ........................................................................................................................ 158

4.3.1. Overview of Data Analysis ..................................................................................163

4.3.2. Results for Multilevel Findings ............................................................................165

4.4. Discussion .................................................................................................................. 188

4.4.1. Self-efficacy for Classroom Management ............................................................188

4.4.2. Teacher Well-Being .............................................................................................190

4.4.3. Need Satisfaction ..................................................................................................192

vi

Page 7: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.5. Limitations and Future Directions ............................................................................. 195

4.6. Conclusions ................................................................................................................ 196

Chapter 5. General Discussion ..............................................................................................198

5.1. Conclusions and Implications .................................................................................... 202

5.1.1. Contributions for Scientific Understanding .........................................................202

5.1.2. Contributions for Practice ....................................................................................206

5.1.3. Final Conclusions .................................................................................................207

References..............................................................................................................................209

Appendices ............................................................................................................................226

Appendix A: Invitation Letter for Studies 1 and 2 ............................................................ 226

Appendix B: Questionnaire for Studies 1 and 2 ............................................................... 228

Appendix C: Invitation Letter for Study 3 ........................................................................ 237

Appendix D: Questionnaire for Study 3 ........................................................................... 238

vii

Page 8: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

LIST OF TABLES

Table 2.1. Personal Demographic Characteristics .................................................................. 25

Table 2.2. Job-Related Demographic Characteristics............................................................. 27

Table 2.3. Perceived Socio-Economic Status and Academic Achievement Levels ............... 28

Table 2.4. Demographics of Study Sample and Population ................................................... 29

Table 2.5. Constructs Under Examination in Each of the Three Studies ............................... 36

Table 2.6. Descriptive Information for the Additional Measures ........................................... 39

Table 2.7. Factor Loadings for the Final Exploratory Factor Analysis .................................. 45

Table 2.8. Standardised Factor Loadings from the Confirmatory Factor Analysis ................ 46

Table 2.9. Latent Variable Correlations of the Well-being Factors ....................................... 47

Table 2.10. Descriptive Statistics for Items in the Teacher Well-being Scale ....................... 48

Table 2.11. Descriptive Statistics by Demographic Characteristics ....................................... 51

Table 2.12. Factor Loadings for the Flourishing and Job Satisfaction Items ......................... 54

Table 2.13. Correlations of the Well-being Factors with the Additional Measures ............... 55

Table 2.14. Partial Correlations Controlling for Social Desirability ...................................... 55

Table 3.1. Reliability Indexes, Means, and Standard Deviations of All Variables .............. 103

Table 3.2. Guidelines for Fit Indices .................................................................................... 106

Table 3.3. Factor Loadings from the Factor Analyses .......................................................... 108

Table 3.4. Correlations of Latent Variables .......................................................................... 112

Table 3.5. Standardised Effects on Each Outcome Variable ................................................ 115

Table 4.1. Frequency of Participants’ Ethnic Origins .......................................................... 149

Table 4.2. Actual and Potential Sample Size at Each Time Point ........................................ 150

viii

Page 9: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.3. Items Used to Assess the Constructs ................................................................... 153

Table 4.4. Mean, Standard Deviation, and Range for all Variables ..................................... 156

Table 4.5. Cronbach’s Alphas at the Three Time Points ...................................................... 157

Table 4.6. Differences in Participants Remaining In or Dropping Out of Study ................. 159

Table 4.7. Correlations Between Variables at Time 1 .......................................................... 160

Table 4.8. Correlations Between Variables at Time 2 .......................................................... 161

Table 4.9. Correlations Between Variables at Time 3 .......................................................... 162

Table 4.10. Multilevel Models for Workload Well-Being ................................................... 168

Table 4.11. Multilevel Models for Organisational Well-Being ............................................ 172

Table 4.12. Multilevel Models for Student Interaction Well-Being ..................................... 175

Table 4.13. Multilevel Models for Self-Efficacy for Student Engagement .......................... 178

Table 4.14. Multilevel Models for in Self-Efficacy for Classroom Management ................ 182

Table 4.15. Multilevel Models for Self-Efficacy for Instructional Strategies ...................... 185

Table 4.16. Hypothesised and Observed Need Satisfaction Effects ..................................... 187

ix

Page 10: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 3.1. The explanatory model of teacher well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and

affective organisational commitment. .................................................................................... 76

Figure 3.2. Summary of the hypotheses tested with predicted relationship polarity. ............. 98

Figure 3.3. Structural equation model of perceived autonomy support, need satisfaction,

well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment.. .......... 114

Figure 4.1. Predicted values of workload well-being for different levels of autonomy. ...... 169

Figure 4.2. Predicted values of organisational well-being for different levels of relatedness

with colleagues and autonomy. ............................................................................................ 173

Figure 4.3. Predicted values of student interaction well-being for different levels of

autonomy. ............................................................................................................................. 176

Figure 4.4. Predicted values of self-efficacy for student engagement for different levels of

relatedness with students. ..................................................................................................... 179

Figure 4.5. Predicted values of self-efficacy for classroom management for different levels of

relatedness with students and autonomy............................................................................... 183

Figure 4.6. Predicted values of self-efficacy for instructional strategies for different levels of

relatedness with students. ..................................................................................................... 186

x

Page 11: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to express my gratitude for the help and support provided by my

supervisor, Dr. Jennifer Shapka, and my committee members, Dr. Nancy Perry and Dr.

Kimberly Schonert-Reichl. Thank you for providing me with the perfect combination of

autonomy and support. I have learnt so much from you about the research process and

writing academically.

I would also like to gratefully acknowledge the funding support provided by the

Social Sciences and Humanities Council of Canada that enabled me to complete my doctoral

research. In addition, I must thank my fellow graduate students who shared their ideas and

feedback with me and helped this research take form and reach fruition. Also important are

the many teachers who participated in this research. Thank you.

Thank you Mum and Dad for a lifetime’s worth of support, and more recently, Dad

for reading over the manuscript so thoroughly and finding many of those little errors that

were all but invisible to me. Finally, thank you Hugues and Léo for supporting me

throughout this endeavour and providing me with not only ample time to work, but also lots

of enjoyable distractions.

xi

Page 12: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

DEDICATION

For my family and friends,

and for teachers everywhere

xii

Page 13: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

Effective teaching and learning are central goals of our educational system. Although

seeking to achieve these goals involves many complexities, there are certain factors that have

been shown to support them, two of which are teacher well-being and teacher motivation.

Research has shown that these two factors are linked to teaching effectiveness (Duckworth,

Quinn, & Seligman, 2009), resiliency in teaching (Klusmann, Kunter, Trautwein, Lüdtke, &

Baumert, 2008), quality instructional practices (Retelsdorf, Butler, Streblow, & Schiefele,

2010), and students’ motivation (Pakarinen et al., 2010; Taylor & Ntoumanis, 2007) and

achievement (e.g., Caprara, Barbaranelli, Steca, & Malone, 2006).

Despite knowing that the two variables are critical for effective teaching and learning,

there are several gaps in our understanding of teacher well-being and motivation including

(a) how teacher well-being can be measured, (b) how teacher well-being and motivation

interrelate with one another and other relevant constructs, and (c) how teacher well-being and

motivation change over time. The purpose of this dissertation research was to address each of

these gaps through three related studies to help improve our understanding of the two

variables and to foster optimal well-being and motivation among teachers, which will likely

lead to better learning and social-emotional outcomes among students.

1.1. Well-being

The construct of well-being is a complex one with several accepted definitions

existing in the literature. One of the most well-known definitions is subjective well-being,

which is defined as happiness, or more specifically, satisfaction with life and the experience

of positive emotions (Diener, 1984). Subjective well-being is a global measure of well-being

(i.e., it measures general life well-being and is not domain specific) and is also known as

1

Page 14: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

hedonic well-being (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2001; Vivoll Straume & Vittersø, 2012). It has been

examined extensively over the past three decades (e.g., Bowling, Eschleman, & Wang, 2010;

Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). While a great deal of interest in subjective well-being

remains, some researchers have begun to question the limitations of subjective well-being.

Seligman (2012) has even argued that life satisfaction essentially measures an individual’s

cheerful mood at the time of taking the questionnaire. As a result, in recent times researchers

have begun to consider a broader definition of well-being: human flourishing (e.g., Deci &

Ryan, 2008c; Huppert & So, 2013; Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryff, 1989; Seligman, 2012; Vivoll

Straume & Vittersø, 2012), which is related to eudaimonic well-being (Huppert & So, 2013)

and psychological well-being (Ryff, 1989). Like subjective well-being, human flourishing is

a global measure of well-being; however, it is defined more broadly.

Human flourishing is defined as “open, engaged, and healthy functioning” (Ryan &

Deci, 2011, p.47). It has also been defined as the “combination of feeling good and

functioning effectively” (Huppert & So, 2013, para. 1). Flourishing is broader than subjective

well-being in that it focuses on the wellness or way-of-living that underlies day-to-day

emotional experiences while also encompassing life satisfaction and positive affect, which

are measured in subjective well-being. For example, an individual may be unhappy about a

certain circumstance, while functioning well in their broader life (Ryan & Deci, 2011).

According to Ryan and Deci (2011), human flourishing “conduces to happiness, but does not

guarantee it… whereas happiness may be evident when people either are or are not living

well” (p. 48). As an example related to teaching, on any given day a teacher may be very

unhappy with her or his teaching, but use this as motivation for changing the method of their

teaching. After implementing the new strategy, the teacher may be quite happy. The

2

Page 15: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

teacher’s use of the negative experience to spur better teaching methods suggests positive

functioning, even though the experience involved a negative emotion (unhappiness).

In the interest of supporting robust research efforts, an operational definition of

flourishing is needed. One such definition has been provided by Diener and colleagues

(2010) that includes eight factors which are considered central to flourishing: purpose and

meaning, supportive relationships, engagement/interest, contributing to others, competence,

being a good person, optimism, and feeling respected. The current research utilised this

definition of human flourishing for the purpose of understanding teacher well-being. More

specifically, it examined flourishing in relation to teaching work as the definition for teacher

well-being.

The importance of well-being among the general population has been well

documented. Indeed, philosophers and researchers have been contemplating the issue of

well-being for many centuries (Linley, Maltby, Wood, Osborne, & Hurling, 2009). In

modern times, this research has generally involved consideration of a global measure of well-

being in reference to an individual’s life (including both subjective well-being and human

flourishing; e.g., Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). In a recent review, however,

Diener (2009) made a call for the advancement of research on well-being in different

domains, including the domain of work. This type of contextual well-being is known as

work-related well-being and refers to an individual’s positive evaluation of and healthy

functioning in their work environment (Van Horn, Taris, Schaufeli, & Schreurs, 2004).

Domain specific well-being can be defined using either subjective well-being or human

flourishing where these definitions are altered to tap into the context rather than global well-

being. As indicated above, the current research was concerned with human flourishing. In

3

Page 16: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

particular, it predominantly involved examination of work-related flourishing of teachers—

that is, teacher well-being. In addition, global assessments of well-being were also included

to further develop our understanding of well-being in relation to teachers.

The issue of teacher well-being has received much attention over the past century

(e.g., Borg & Riding, 1991; Kyriacou & Sutcliffe, 1977; Skaalvik & Skaalvik, 2011). This

likely stems from the belief that teachers who are functioning well make better teachers.

Research supports this belief—teacher well-being, measured through related constructs such

as stress and burnout, has been linked with teaching effectiveness (e.g., Duckworth et al.,

2009) and quality instructional practices (e.g., Retelsdorf et al., 2010).

Despite knowing that teacher well-being is critical for teachers and students, there are

still several gaps in the literature. To begin with, although we have global measures of well-

being, there is a need for measures that are dedicated to assessing teachers’ work-related

well-being based upon their experiences at work. This is a necessary next step so as to gain a

better understanding of what aspects of teaching work are aiding or thwarting teacher well-

being. Second, despite knowing that teacher well-being is significantly related to teacher job

satisfaction and organisational commitment (i.e., commitment to their school of employment;

e.g., Collie, Shapka, & Perry, 2012; Jackson, Rothmann, & van de Vijver, 2006; Klassen &

Chiu, 2011), we do not know how these variables interact when considered simultaneously

and we do not have explanatory frameworks for understanding their interrelationships. This

type of examination is important given that teachers’ experiences do not occur in isolation

(Collie et al., 2012). For example, when teachers feel a sense of job satisfaction, it not only

relates to their experiences of well-being (e.g., Klassen & Chiu, 2010), but also their

experiences of organisational commitment (e.g., Canrinus, Helms-Lorenz, Beijaard, Buitink,

4

Page 17: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

& Hofman, 2012). Finally, for the purpose of developing a comprehensive and more nuanced

understanding of teacher well-being, we need to employ diverse methodologies in our

research. More specifically, research that involves multiple time points is needed. This

dissertation research aimed to address these gaps through a systematic examination of

teacher well-being.

1.2. Motivation

A second major focus of this dissertation research is teacher motivation. Motivation

is the drive to act, think, and develop (Deci & Ryan, 2008a). Effective motivation requires

energy or drive, as well as affective and cognitive guidance (Deci & Ryan, 2012).

Individuals can be motivated to undertake an activity because they value it or because there

is a compelling external reason (Ryan & Deci, 2000). In this dissertation, reference is made

to motivational constructs based upon two different theories: self-determination theory (Deci

& Ryan, 1985, 2002) and social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1982, 1997). These constructs

are more thoroughly defined in the relevant studies throughout this dissertation.

When motivation results from an individual valuing an activity, the individual

experiences greater excitement, interest, and confidence, and, in turn, greater performance,

persistence, and creativity in relation to the activity (Ryan & Deci, 2000). Motivation,

therefore, is highly important in students’ learning (e.g., Fortier, Vallerand, & Guay, 1995;

Vallerand, Fortier, & Guay, 1997). Motivation is also important in teaching; however, as

Richardson and Watt (2010), along with Butler (2007) have established, research into teacher

motivation has been largely overlooked in favour of research on students’ motivation. In

spite of this, it is clear that teacher motivation is important for both teachers and students.

More specifically, research has shown that teacher motivation is associated with effective

5

Page 18: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

instructional practices (e.g., Butler & Shibaz, 2008), greater occupational commitment,

greater retention (e.g., Klassen & Chui, 2011), and reduced emotional exhaustion (e.g.,

Klassen, Perry, & Frenzel, 2012) among teachers. In addition, teachers who are motivated

encourage greater motivation (e.g., Patrick, Hisley, & Kemper, 2000) and achievement (e.g.,

Caprara et al., 2006) among their students.

Given that teacher motivation is so important, more research is needed to extend our

understanding of what influence teachers’ experiences of motivation at work. In particular,

more systematic and theory-driven research is needed to develop our conceptualisations of

teacher motivation (Richardson & Watt, 2010). The aim of the dissertation, therefore, was to

address three main gaps in the literature through systematic and theory-driven research. First,

although self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002) has been used extensively to

understand students’ motivation, only limited research has examined this theory among

teachers. In order to further our understanding of teacher motivation, there is a need for

research that examines the relevance of theories like self-determination theory for examining

teacher motivation. Second, although research has linked motivation with well-being, job

satisfaction, and organisational commitment among teachers, we do not have research or

explanatory frameworks for understanding how these variables interact simultaneously. As

indicated above, understanding this is necessary to obtain a full picture of teachers’

experiences of these variables. Finally, like well-being, there is a need for more research that

examines teacher motivation at multiple time points in order to understand how it functions

over time.

6

Page 19: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

1.3. The Dissertation Research

Although research has established that teacher well-being and motivation are critical

for teachers and students, further research is needed. The objective of this dissertation

research was to address the gaps in the literature, as described above, through three related

studies. The first study investigated the measurement of teacher well-being. A well-being

instrument based upon teachers’ perceptions of core aspects of their work was developed and

tested. This instrument is distinct from existing instruments that target global appraisals of

well-being (e.g., “I am satisfied with my life”; Diener et al., 1985), because it asks teachers

to rate the degree to which different aspects of their teaching work (e.g., managing the

classroom, marking assignments) influence their well-being. Although existing measures are

useful for providing understanding about individual functioning overall, we still do not

understand what aspects of teaching work are central to teacher well-being. The teacher well-

being instrument that I developed in Study 1 addresses this gap. It also builds understandings

about underlying dimensions of teacher well-being and how these are driven by aspects of

teachers’ work.

The second study examined reports of teacher well-being, motivation, job

satisfaction, and organisational commitment through an explanatory framework based in

self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002). As established above, research is

needed to ascertain the relevance of self-determination theory for understanding teacher

well-being and motivation, and to examine how these two variables interact simultaneously

with teacher job satisfaction and organisational commitment. Thus, an explanatory model

was described and tested in Study 2 to advance our understanding of teachers’ experiences at

work and how they interrelate. In addition, the model will help to provide critical information

7

Page 20: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

for understanding how efforts to improve teachers’ experiences of one outcome may affect

their experiences of the others. For example, if schools implement an initiative to improve

teacher well-being, the explanatory model will provide a better idea of how this will

simultaneously affect teachers’ experiences of motivation, job satisfaction, and

organisational commitment.

The final study in this dissertation examined how teachers’ perceptions of well-being

and motivation function over time. This type of examination is important for the purpose of

gaining understanding of the development or stability of constructs over time. Moreover,

given that teachers’ experiences at work are constantly changing within (and across) school

days, months, and years, this type of research will provide a better understanding of teachers’

experiences at work. In Study 3, therefore, I conducted analyses of data collected at three

time points with the aim of advancing our knowledge of teacher well-being and motivation,

including whether and how they change over time.

Taken together, the three studies address several gaps in the literature and provide a

comprehensive and systematic examination of teacher well-being and motivation. Moreover,

the three studies help to improve our understanding of the highly important variables of

teacher well-being and motivation not only independently, but also as they interact with one

another. Although the research focuses on teachers, the implications extend to students and

schools. After all, flourishing and motivated teachers encourage greater learning,

achievement, and positive social-emotional development among their students (e.g., Caprara

et al., 2006; Pakarinen et al., 2010).

8

Page 21: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

CHAPTER 2. STUDY 1: CONCEPTUALISING AND MEASURING

TEACHER WELL-BEING

2.1. Literature Review

Teaching is a rewarding profession that involves meaningful work fostering growth

and development among students, as well as the experience of joy and inspiration from

watching those individuals grow. At the same time, however, teaching can be challenging

due to the complex nature of the job and the myriad demands placed upon teachers. The

impact of teaching work on teachers has been a topic of research for many decades and, in

some cases, the findings have been quite concerning. Namely, research has shown that up to

one-third of teachers are stressed or extremely stressed (Geving, 2007; Collie, 2010; Thomas,

Clarke, & Lavery, 2003), and that between 25 and 30% of teachers leave the profession

within the first five years of teaching (Darling-Hammond, 2000; Ingersoll, 2002; Ingersoll &

May, 2012). As a result of these concerning trends, there has been increasing interest in the

well-being of teachers and how their working lives can be improved to promote optimal

well-being. This area of research is important not only for teachers, but also for students.

After all, well-being among teachers has been associated with effective teaching (Duckworth

et al., 2009; Klusmann et al., 2008), organisational commitment (Jackson et al., 2006), and

perceived teaching efficacy (Lent et al., 2011). It has also been associated with students’

attachment to school (Wei & Chen, 2010) and motivation for learning (e.g., Pakarinen et al.,

2010). Given that these outcomes are associated with significant financial, social-emotional,

and academic ramifications, improving our understanding of the well-being of teachers is

highly important not only for teachers, but for all members of our school communities.

9

Page 22: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Over the past 50 years, the well-being of teachers has been examined in research

through several related constructs. A prevalent construct featured in the literature is teacher

stress. As indicated above, teaching is considered a highly stressful career (Kyriacou, 2001).

Research has shown that high levels of stress are associated with negative outcomes for

teachers and students, whereas low levels of stress are associated with positive outcomes.

Collie, Shapka, and Perry (2012) found that higher levels of teacher stress due to student

behavioural issues were associated with lower self-efficacy for teaching. Moreover,

Pakarinen and colleagues (2010) showed that lower levels of teacher stress predicted greater

learning motivation among kindergarten students.

Another commonly examined construct is teacher burnout. Burnout is a

multidimensional construct composed of emotional exhaustion, depersonalisation, and

personal accomplishment (Maslach & Jackson, 1981). The literature is replete with evidence

of the negative effects of burnout on teachers (e.g., Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004; Steinhardt,

Smith Jaggers, Faulk, & Gloria, 2011; Wolpin, Burke, & Greenglass, 2002). In a model

looking at job demands and resources, Schaufeli and Bakker (2004) examined teachers’

perceptions of burnout, work engagement, and intentions to leave their job. They found that

burnout was negatively associated with work engagement and that it positively predicted

intention to leave. In other research, Steinhardt, Smith Jaggers, Faulk, and Gloria (2001)

showed that burnout had a mediating effect in the relationship between chronic teacher stress

and depressive symptoms. Teachers who felt stressed, reported higher burnout, and higher

depressive symptoms as a result. Finally, in a longitudinal study, Wolpin, Burke, and

Greenglass (2002) found that perceived burnout resulted in decreased in job satisfaction over

time among teachers.

10

Page 23: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Another variable that is more removed, but that has also been used as a proxy for

well-being is teacher job satisfaction. Klusmann and colleagues (2008) defined teacher well-

being as emotional exhaustion and job satisfaction. They found that teachers reporting the

lowest emotional exhaustion and highest job satisfaction had students who felt the most

competent and autonomous. In other research, Van Horn, Taris, Schaufeli, and Schreurs

(2004) used job satisfaction as part of their definition for affective well-being in a five

dimensional model of occupational well-being. Along with job satisfaction, their model

included measures of stress, burnout, and several other variables (e.g., organisational

commitment) to assess affective, professional, social, cognitive, and psychosomatic well-

being.

Taken together, the findings from these studies have provided us with important

information about teachers’ experiences at work. However, we still do not know much about

the construct of teacher well-being itself. That is to say, these variables relate highly to well-

being, but they are different from the construct of well-being, which in this dissertation is

defined as flourishing (i.e., “open, engaged, and healthy functioning;” Ryan & Deci, 2011, p.

47). In fact, very little research has been conducted on the construct of teacher well-being

itself (i.e., without defining it by using related constructs). This is surprising given the large

body of literature that has examined well-being among individuals more generally and

provided evidence highlighting its importance for individual and societal functioning (e.g.,

Tov & Diener, 2009).

In order to extend our understanding of the well-being of teachers, there is a need for

research that directly examines the construct of teacher well-being. This type of direct

examination will not only broaden our knowledge of well-being, but also our knowledge of

11

Page 24: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

related variables like teacher stress, burnout, and job satisfaction. Furthermore, directly

examining the construct of teacher well-being has the potential to provide a different picture

of well-being than related constructs. For example, job satisfaction looks at how satisfied a

teacher is with his or her work, but this does not necessarily include an aspect of personal

flourishing that is associated with well-being as defined here. Moreover, by using stress as a

proxy for well-being, we may inadvertently disregard the positive aspects of teachers’ work

experiences. For example, Brenner, Perry, and Collie (2012) and Collie, Perry, and Brenner

(2013) found that student teachers reported moderately high levels of stress while still

reporting high levels of engagement in and commitment to the profession of teaching. This

suggests that these student teachers were faring well (as evidenced by the high levels of

engagement and commitment) in spite of experiencing relatively high stress levels.

Furthermore, it underscores the need for further research to better understand well-being and

how it differs from stress, which echoes Huppert and So’s (2013) suggestion that we need to

study well-being on its own and not simply as the absence of negative affect.

More recently, researchers have begun to examine the well-being of teachers directly

by using subjective well-being measures, such as life satisfaction, positive affect, and

negative affect (e.g., Albuquerque, Pedroso de Lima, Figueiredo, & Matos, 2011; Duckworth

et al., 2009). These studies have shown that teachers report average (Albuquerque et al.,

2011) to above average life satisfaction (Duckworth et al., 2009), but that experienced

teachers are less satisfied with life than beginning teachers (Duckworth et al., 2009). Taken

together, these studies reveal information about the subjective well-being of teachers. We

still do not have a thorough understanding, however, of well-being defined as flourishing,

nor of the well-being of teachers as it relates specifically to their work. In other words, global

12

Page 25: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

well-being—whether defined as life satisfaction or flourishing—tells us how a group of

individuals who happen to be teachers are faring in their life. However, it does not tell us

how the well-being that teachers experience is related to aspects of their work. This is the

focus of Study 1.

There has been an increasing call for the examination of well-being in different

contexts such as work (Diener, 2009). In addition, worldwide research conducted by Gallup

(Rath & Harter, 2010) revealed five domains of well-being, of which work-related well-

being was the most powerful at determining overall life well-being. This highlights the

importance of work-related well-being—not only is it important for individuals’ outcomes at

work, but it also relates strongly to their broader life experience. Therefore, research that

specifically focuses on the work context of teachers is needed to extend our understanding of

teacher well-being. Moreover, measures that tap directly into teachers’ experiences at work

will be helpful in understanding the different dimensions of teacher well-being and the

aspects of teachers’ working lives that affect their well-being, both positively and negatively.

2.1.1. Measurement Method

One method for measuring teacher well-being is by creating an instrument that taps

into the construct itself. This method focuses on individuals’ global appraisals of well-being

and is the format used in the well-known Satisfaction with Life scale (e.g., “In most ways my

life is close to my ideal;” Diener et al., 1985), and the more recently developed Flourishing

scale (e.g., “I am a good person and live a good life;” Diener et al., 2010). These instruments

provide researchers with an overall score of participants’ well-being that can be used to

understand how an individual is faring generally. However, they do not provide information

13

Page 26: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

about what specific aspects of the individual’s life influenced his or her score of well-being,

nor do they provide understanding about well-being specifically related to work.

A second method for measuring teacher well-being is by examining the impact of

teachers’ experiences at work on their well-being. This involves asking teachers to rate the

extent to which different aspects of their teaching work (e.g., attending meetings, student

behaviour) affect their well-being. In teacher research, this type of questioning has

commonly been used to examine teacher job satisfaction (e.g., Dinham & Scott, 1998) and

stress (e.g., Clunies-Ross, Little, & Kienhuis, 2008; Klassen & Chiu, 2011), and provides a

relevant method for gaining insight into the core aspects of teaching work that affect teacher

well-being. An important feature of this type of measure is that it highlights tangible factors

that administrators and schools can address to better support teacher well-being.

In Study 1, the second method for assessing well-being was used to create an

instrument to measure teacher well-being. The reason for using this method as opposed to the

first method is that it provides information about the aspects of teaching work that support or

thwart teacher well-being, while at the same time it has the ability to provide an overall well-

being score like the first method when the items are combined to form a composite variable.

A limitation of measures that have used the second method, however, is that they do

not often consider whether aspects of teaching work impact teachers negatively and/or

positively. Generally, these scales only allow teachers to indicate one polarity. For example,

a commonly used stress measure, the Teacher Stress Inventory (Boyle, Borg, Falzon, &

Baglioni, 1995), asks teachers to rate the level of stress that they experience from different

aspects of their work on a scale from no stress to extremely stressful. The total score from all

items provides an overall stress score for each teacher. Although this scale allows teachers to

14

Page 27: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

indicate that certain aspects of their work have no stress, it does not take into account the fact

that stress can be a motivating force (e.g., seen as a challenge rather than a threat; Hobfall,

1989) or that some of the items may actively reduce teacher stress. For example, by only

allowing teachers to report whether relationships with colleagues create no stress, the scale

does not allow us to determine if these relationships actually help to reduce teacher stress by

providing teachers an opportunity to express or share their experiences/feelings. Similarly,

by asking teachers to what extent various aspects of their work promote burnout, we neglect

to consider whether any of these aspects are energising and do the opposite. Although one-

dimensional views of stress and burnout have provided us with important information, the

consideration of positive and negative views is needed to extend our understanding.

Furthermore, although teacher well-being is defined differently from stress, it is also affected

positively and negatively by teachers’ experiences. Clearly, instruments that consider the full

range of influences on teacher well-being are necessary for the advancement of the field.

Emerging research from northern Europe is beginning to examine teacher well-being

through the second method while also allowing teachers to assess the different aspects of

their work as negatively or positively affecting their well-being. More specifically, Konu,

Viitanen, and Lintonen (2010) asked Finnish teachers about four categories of school

welfare: school conditions, social relationships, means of self-fulfillment, and health.

Teachers were asked to respond to the items (e.g., “I have friends in this school”) on a scale

that considered positive and negative influences—from totally agree to I completely

disagree. Konu and colleagues found that teachers rated means for self-fulfillment (e.g.,

teaching effectively, support from colleagues) most positively and school conditions (e.g.,

size of classrooms, safety of facilities) most negatively in relation to their welfare.

15

Page 28: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Although this research provides some understanding of teacher well-being, there are

several additional factors not included in Konu and Lintonen’s (2004) research that need to

be addressed. For example, attention needs to be paid to the impact of classroom

management on teacher well-being given that it has been shown to be a key cause of stress

among teachers (e.g., Collie et al., 2012; Klassen & Chiu, 2010). In addition, attention needs

to be paid to issues related to teachers’ workloads such as staying late after hours or the

overcrowded curriculum that are significant concerns for teachers (e.g., Naylor, 2001). These

are just a couple of examples that highlight the need for further research in this area so as to

develop our understanding of teacher well-being.

This type of examination will help to extend our understanding of what factors

influence the well-being of teachers and it will help place teacher education programs,

schools, school boards, and policy-makers in a better position to promote optimal well-being

among their teachers. Study 1, therefore, involved developing and testing a measure of

teacher well-being based upon teachers’ experiences at work. The measure was designed to

give teachers the option to report whether different aspects of their teaching work influence

their well-being positively or negatively so as to address the limitations discussed above.

2.1.2. Overview of the Current Study

Study 1 involved the development and testing of the Teacher Well-being Scale

(TWBS), which aims to provide teachers with the opportunity to report how key aspects of

their working lives impact their well-being in positive or negative ways. The study involved

measure development, testing, and refinement, as well as comparisons of scores on the new

measure across demographic groups and with related measures. Three research questions

were used to guide the investigations:

16

Page 29: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

1. What is the nature of teacher well-being?

2. How do scores on the new measure differ across demographic groups?

3. Are interpretations of scores from the new measure supported by evidence of

validity?

2.1.2.1. Measure development, testing, and refinement. Following, Miller,

McIntire, and Lovler’s (2011) steps for measure development (further details are provided

below), a large pool of items that covered key aspects of teaching work were collated to form

the TWBS, which was then administered via an online questionnaire to a sample of

practicing teachers. Items were chosen to cover a broad range of teachers’ experiences at

work. In addition, they were chosen based upon two theories to help determine if existing

theory is relevant for understanding teacher well-being.

The first theory used to inform item development was self-determination theory

(SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002). SDT posits that optimal motivation and well-being within

individuals is promoted through the satisfaction of three basic psychological needs:

autonomy, competence, and relatedness (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002). Autonomy refers to

people’s perception that they are the origin or source of their behaviour and have some

choice or input into the activities they undertake (Deci & Ryan, 2002; Filak & Sheldon,

2008). Competence refers to people’s sense that they are effective in undertaking and

mastering challenges and able to exercise and express their capacities (Deci & Ryan, 2002;

Filak & Sheldon, 2008). Finally, relatedness refers to people’s need to feel connected to

others and the community, and to care for and be cared for by those others (Deci & Ryan,

2002). In work-place research, relatedness generally refers to colleagues; however, for

teachers it also refers to relatedness with students (Klassen, Perry, et al., 2012). Research

17

Page 30: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

suggests that the three basic psychological needs do, in fact, relate to well-being and

motivation among students (e.g., Standage, Duda, & Ntoumanis, 2005) and employees in

business settings (e.g., Deci et al., 2001). Emerging research suggests that the same is true

for teachers (e.g., Klassen, Perry, et al., 2012). Given the relevance of SDT for examining

teacher well-being, items that tap into the three basic psychological needs were included in

the pool to see if they are relevant for teacher well-being as measured by teachers’

experiences at work.

The second theory was Bronfenbrenner’s (1978, 1994) socio-ecological theory,

which posits that individuals reciprocally influence and are influenced by the different

environments or systems of which they are a direct or indirect part (Bronfenbrenner, 1994).

These include micro-, meso-, exo-, macro-, and chronosystems (Bronfenbrenner, 1994).

Microsystems are the different environments in which the individual is an immediate part

(e.g., home, school). Mesosystems involve the processes or relations linking two or more

microsystems (e.g., relations between home and school). Exosystems involve the processes

or relations linking two or more microsystems where the individual only participates in one

of the systems—the other systems affect the individual indirectly (e.g., relations between

school and the district-level school board). Branching further out, macrosystems concern the

cultural characteristics that influence each of the smaller systems (e.g., belief systems,

values). Finally, chronosystems concern the changes in the individual and the environment

over time. As individuals go about their daily lives, there is reciprocal influence between

them and the different systems. In the current study, items that relate to relevant

microsystems (e.g., classroom, school, home), mesosystems (e.g., relations between school

and home), exosystems (e.g., students’ online social networking), and macrosystems (e.g.,

18

Page 31: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

the greater community) were included. The chronosystem was not considered given that this

measure examines teachers’ experiences at one point in time (however, this would be an

interesting consideration when attempting to understand how and why teachers’ reports may

change over time).

Following data collection, factor analyses were used to refine the measure and

ascertain its dimensionality (Research Question 1). Descriptive statistics for the final

instrument as a whole and as individual items were calculated, including the mean, standard

deviation, observed range, skewness, and kurtosis. In addition, Cronbach’s alphas were

calculated as an indicator of internal consistency.

2.1.2.2. Demographics comparisons. Group differences in scores on the TWBS

were examined based on several demographic variables (Research Question 2). These

variables were sex, years of teaching experience, school level (elementary, secondary),

school setting (urban, suburban, small town, rural), and teachers’ perceptions of students’

socio-economic status and academic achievement. The demographic comparisons were

conducted to provide preliminary understanding of how teacher well-being functions among

different groups. The presence of group differences would provide information about how

different groups of teachers experience well-being with the potential to facilitate targeted

attempts to improve teacher well-being.

2.1.2.3. Preliminary evidence of validity. The Standards for Educational and

Psychological Testing (Standards; American Psychological Association [APA], American

Educational Research Association [AERA], and National Council on Measurement in

Education [NCME], 1999) establish that “validity refers to the degree to which evidence and

theory support the interpretations of test scores” (p. 9). They also establish that validation is a

19

Page 32: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

continuing process involving the accumulation of evidence based on several sources to

provide a sound basis for the interpretation of scores. Study 1 begins the process of

accumulation of validity evidence for the new measure by providing evidence based on two

sources included in the Standards. The first is evidence of validity based on internal

structure, which indicates the extent to which the relationships among the instrument’s items

conform to the construct under examination (APA, AERA, & NCME, 1999). The second is

evidence of validity based on relations to other variables, which indicates the extent to which

relationships with other similar or different variables are consistent with the construct under

examination (APA, AERA, & NCME, 1999). For this second source, convergent evidence of

validity was provided by comparing scores on the new measure with other similar constructs

that were assumed to be related (both positively and negatively).

Evidence based on internal structure was provided through Cronbach’s alphas and

factor analyses. Cronbach’s alpha provides an understanding of the internal consistency of

the measure by ascertaining the extent to which items in a test are interrelated (Cortina,

1993). A higher coefficient alpha reflects a higher expectation of reliability of the measure.

In the case of internal consistency, this suggests that there is consistency in the results across

items in the test. Factor analyses provide an understanding of the underlying structure in a

measure by revealing subsets of items that are internally related, but relatively independent

of other subsets (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). These subsets, or factors, reflect underlying

processes (latent variables) that have prompted the correlations among the items within each

factor (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007).

Convergent evidence was provided by comparing scores on the TWBS with three

previously published measures: the Teacher Stress Inventory (Boyle et al., 1995), an

20

Page 33: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

unnamed 4-item job satisfaction scale (Caprara, Barbaranelli, Borgogni, & Steca, 2003), and

the Flourishing Scale (Diener et al., 2010; Research Question 3). In Study 1, stress was

defined as teachers’ experiences of negative emotions as a result of their teaching work

(Kyriacou, 2001). Job satisfaction was defined as the fulfilment gained from working as a

teacher (Locke, 1969). Finally, as previously described, human flourishing was defined as

healthy human functioning (Diener et al., 2010; Ryan & Deci, 2011). This was a global

measure of well-being (i.e., about the individual’s life as a whole and not specifically related

to teachers’ work).

In order to obtain convergent evidence of validity, several different hypotheses were

elaborated. First, it was expected that teacher well-being would have a moderately negative

relationship with teacher stress (Hypothesis 1). The rationale for this is that when employing

a one-dimensional definition as I have here, stress by its very nature of causing people to

experience negative emotions (e.g., Kyriacou, 2001) affects well-being negatively. The

rationale for the moderate strength of this relationship is that aspects of teaching work may

influence teacher stress and well-being differently. In other words, it is possible that some

aspects may associate positively with teacher stress, while at the same time, also associate

positively with teacher well-being. For example, teacher-student relationships can be

stressful for teachers, but also positive at the same time given the varying relationships that

teachers have with different students, at different times, and with respect to different parts of

the curriculum.

The second hypothesis was that teacher well-being would have a moderately positive

relationship with job satisfaction (Hypothesis 2a). The rationale for this is that teachers who

are experiencing well-being at work will tend to feel that their work-related needs are being

21

Page 34: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

met and have experiences with which they are happier about. This, in turn, will relate

positively to their sense of job satisfaction. This hypothesis is supported by previous

research. Bowling, Eschleman, and Wang (2010) conducted a meta-analysis of research

involving job satisfaction and subjective well-being among employees in a variety of settings

(e.g., university instructors, customer service employees, physicians) and found a moderately

positive relationship (r = .36) between the two variables. Given that subjective well-being

concerns an individual’s life in general rather than being specific to his or her work, it is

hypothesised that the relationship between job satisfaction and teacher well-being in the

current study will be slightly higher than Bowling and colleagues’ reported correlation

because job satisfaction and teacher well-being both refer to the working context (Hypothesis

2b).

The third hypothesis was that teacher well-being would have a moderately positive

relationship with flourishing (Hypothesis 3). The rationale for this hypothesis is that both

teacher well-being and flourishing are well-being constructs; however, teacher well-being is

a contextual construct specifically related to work (i.e., work-related well-being), whereas

flourishing is related to an individual’s life in general (i.e., it is a global measure). There will

not be a perfect relationship, therefore, between the two constructs. For example, an

individual may feel he or she leads a meaningful life (flourishing), while at the same time his

or her teacher well-being may be negatively impacted by workload.

In addition to the constructs measured to provide convergent evidence of validity,

social desirability was also measured. In psychological measurement, social desirability

refers to the tendency of individuals to endorse questionnaire items they perceive to be

socially acceptable and not endorse items they perceive to be socially unacceptable (Spector

22

Page 35: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

& Brannick, 2009). Social desirability can mean that responses tend to be more positive than

they are in actuality. Measuring this construct provides an understanding of the extent to

which participants’ responses are biased by the desire to appear in a positive light (Uziel,

2010). Put another way, controlling for this bias provides an understanding of the results

while excluding the influence of social desirability. Although controlling for social

desirability is not commonly done in teacher research, it is regularly mentioned as a

necessary inclusion in future research in the literature to add to the robustness of self-report

data collection (e.g., Retelsdorf et al., 2010). The handful of studies that have controlled for

social desirability have found small, but significant effects of social desirability on teachers’

responses (e.g., Brackett, Palomera, Mojsa-Kaja, Reyes, & Salovey, 2010; Roth, Assor,

Kanat-Maymon, & Kaplan, 2007). Other research, however, has suggested that social

desirability is less of an issue in anonymous online questionnaires than in paper

questionnaire (Joinson, 1999). Study 1 included a measure of social desirability to see

whether it played a role in teacher well-being. In addition, this inclusion helps to temper the

limitation of single-source bias that occurs in self-report research (Podsakoff, MacKenzie,

Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003).

2.2. Methods

2.2.1. Participants

Participants included 603 teachers from four different district-level teachers’

associations in British Columbia, Canada. The sample sizes for the four districts were 135,

138, 146, and 184 and ranged from 12% to 23% of the district populations (support for the

representativeness of these samples is provided below). Participants were from 218 different

schools (19% of participants chose not to identify their school) and had an average teaching

23

Page 36: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

experience of 15.8 (SD = 9.93) years and an average age of 44.2 (SD = 10.83) years. Table

2.1 shows the frequencies of personal demographic characteristics among the participants

including sex, birth country, ethnic origins, and previous education. It reveals that most of

the sample was female (76%), born in Canada (86%), had northern and western European

ethnic origins, and had completed a bachelor’s degree. Participants also reported having one

(86%), two (10%), or three (1%) ethnic backgrounds (3% chose not to answer this question).

Table 2.2 shows the frequencies of job-related demographic characteristics including

teaching level, role, time spent teaching, and school setting. Overall, it reveals that most of

the participants taught at the elementary level (53%), were classroom teachers (81%), spent

the majority of their working time teaching (89%), and worked in a suburban setting (48%).

Participants were also asked to rate the average socio-economic status (SES) of students and

their families and the average academic achievement of students at their schools compared to

most other people/schools in the province. Teachers indicated that they taught students from

the full range of SES and academic achievement levels. Table 2.3 shows the frequencies of

the different levels reported.

24

Page 37: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.1

Frequencies of Personal Demographic Characteristics of the Participants

Frequency

Sex

Female 76%

Male 21%

Birth country

Canada 86%

United Kingdom 4%

Mainland Europe 2%

United States 2%

Asia 1%

Othera 2%

Ethnic origins

Northern and western European 82%

Eastern and southern European 16%

Aboriginal 3%

East Asian 3%

South Asian 2%

Other 4%

25

Page 38: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.1 (Continued)

Frequency

Previous education

Bachelor 51%

Master 30%

Post-graduate diploma or extra credits 12%

Undertaking a master’s degree 4%

Doctorate 1%

Note. Where percentages do not add up to 100, the remaining participants chose not to respond. Northern and Western European origins refers to British, Scottish, German, Swedish, Danish, Norwegian, Dutch, etc. Eastern and Southern European origins refers to Polish, Russian, Ukrainian, Italian, Greek, Spanish, etc. Aboriginal origins refers to First Nations, Inuit, Metis, etc. East Asian origins refers to Chinese, Japanese, Korean, etc. South Asian origins refers to East Indian, Punjabi, Pakistani, etc. a The other category included Africa, South and Central America, the Middle East, and Oceania.

26

Page 39: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.2

Frequencies of Job-Related Demographic Characteristics of the Participants

Frequency

Teaching level

Elementary school 53%

Middle school 29%

Secondary school 10%

Multiple levels 8%

Teaching rolea

Classroom teacher 81%

Support teacherb 12%

Substitute teacher 3%

Teacher librarians 3%

Other rolesc 1%

Time spent teachingd

0-50% of working time 8%

51-75% of working time 13%

76-100% of working time 76%

27

Page 40: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.2 (Continued)

Frequency

School setting

Urban 34%

Suburban 48%

Small town 10%

Rural 7%

Note. Where percentages do not add up to 100, the remaining participants chose not to respond. a All participants were teachers or undertook teaching roles in addition to their other positions and were, therefore, classified as teachers in the results. b Support teachers refers to resource teachers, special education teachers, and counsellors. c Other roles included administrators (e.g., principals) and distance educators. d This refers to how much of the teachers’ work was spent teaching (as opposed to doing administrative tasks for example)

Table 2.3

Frequencies of Perceived Socio-Economic Status and Academic Achievement Levels

Low Low-average

Average High-average

High

SES of students 18% 21% 39% 15% 5%

Academic achievement

9% 20% 45% 19% 4%

Note. SES = socio-economic status.

28

Page 41: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

In order to gain a sense of the representativeness of the sample, the sample

characteristics were compared to the district populations from which they were drawn on

three variables: gender, age, and years of teaching experience. Table 2.4 provides the data

comparing individuals participating in Study 1 with the population (i.e., the four districts

involved in the study). As can be seen from Table 2.4, the sample data were very similar to

the population parameters, which offers support for the representativeness of the sample.

Details about the procedures for participant recruitment appear in the following section after

the description of measure development.

Table 2.4

Demographics of Study Sample and Population

n Female Average age Average years of experience

Study 603 78% 44.2 15.8

Population 3700 71% 44.8 12.7 Note. Population columns represent combined data/averages for the four districts involved in the study (Ministry of Education, 2012) 2.2.2. Procedures

2.2.2.1. Measure development. In order to develop psychological instruments, there

are several recommended steps that should be followed. In the current study, I followed steps

that Miller et al. (2011) have outlined. The first step involves defining the test universe (i.e.,

the content that the researcher wants to measure), target audience, and test purpose. The test

universe is a broad range of teachers’ daily work experiences, the target audience is

29

Page 42: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

practicing teachers, and the purpose of the test is to gain an understanding of how individual

factors affect teacher well-being as well as an overall score of well-being.

The second step involves creating an operational definition of the construct, the

format for the questions, and how the test will be scored (Miller et al., 2011). The operational

definition of teacher well-being is the definition noted in the Introduction (i.e., flourishing at

work involving the factors of purpose and meaning, supportive relationships, engagement,

contributing to others, competence, being a good person, optimism, and feeling respected).

Because I was interested in teachers’ own interpretations of how aspects of their work affect

their well-being, a simplified definition of this was included in the test instructions (details

below). The format for the questions was determined as a scale ranging from negatively to

positively such that teachers could indicate the degree to which different aspects of their

work affected their well-being in positive and negative ways. A 7-point Likert-type scale was

chosen to score the test.

The third step involves composing the test items (Miller et al., 2011). At this point, a

pool of items based on the many different aspects of teaching work (e.g., interpersonal

relationships, workload, leadership support) was created. The items included in the initial

pool were adapted from related instruments including the Teacher Stress Inventory (Boyle et

al., 1995) and the Job Satisfaction Scale (Spector, 1997), as well as additional experiences

not covered by existing measures (e.g., interacting with the parents of students, lesson

planning). These items were chosen to give a broad representation of teachers’ work

experiences. In addition, item selection was guided by SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002) and

socio-ecological theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1978) to provide an understanding of whether these

30

Page 43: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

two theories are relevant for the consideration of teacher well-being as measured by the new

instrument.

Because self-report questionnaires rely on participants’ accurate interpretations of the

items, several steps were taken to build the strength of the questionnaire. First, language that

is familiar to teachers in British Columbia was used. Karabenick and colleagues (2007)

identified this as an important consideration in the development of self-report items and

explained that unfamiliar language can negatively affect the participants’ understanding of

the item and, thus, reduce the ability of the item to accurately measure the phenomenon of

interest. Similarly, examples were provided in parentheses for items that may have been

ambiguous (e.g., ‘Relations with students not in my class [e.g., in hallways, etc.]’). This

consideration in item design can help to improve participant comprehension and reduce

measurement error (Podsakoff et al., 2003). Furthermore, items were described in straight-

forward, non-pejorative language to increase the likelihood of truthful responses (Simms,

2008).

The fourth step in measure development involves writing the test instructions (Miller

et al., 2011). For this, teachers were asked, “Currently, how do the following aspects of being

a teacher affect your well-being as a teacher?” As you will have noticed, the item stem asked

participants about their current experiences, rather than their experiences over a previous

time period. This is an important step in reducing the cognitive load required for

remembering (Winne, Jamieson-Noel, & Muis, 2002) and aids in the accuracy of responses.

However, there was an exception to this. At the time of data collection, teachers were

involved in industrial action (called job action in British Columbia) due to stalled

negotiations with the provincial government about a new work contract. This industrial

31

Page 44: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

action meant that there was a reduction in some administrative tasks normally completed by

teachers (further details are provided below). In this circumstance, if teachers were not

engaging in certain activities due to industrial action, they were asked about their previous

experiences.1

As described above, the instructions also included a brief definition of well-being:

“Well-being refers to open, engaged, and healthy functioning as a teacher.” In order to aid

understanding, an example relating to the first item about relations with teaching colleagues

was also provided: “For example, in your current working environment how do your

relations with fellow teachers influence your well-being? Do your relations have a positive or

negative influence on your well-being as a teacher?”

The fifth step of measurement development involves piloting the instrument (Miller

et al., 2011). Using the large pool of items, a small sample of practicing teachers (n = 5) was

invited to examine the items and suggest additional aspects of teaching work not covered.

This led to the sixth step, which involves analysing the items and making changes based on

results from pilot testing (Miller et al., 2011). Two additional items were suggested leading

to a final pool of 60 items that was used in data collection for the study. The seventh step

involves collecting evidence of validity for the test (Miller et al., 2011). This reflects the

investigations conducted for Research Question 3.

2.2.2.2. Measure testing. The TWBS was tested among practicing teachers who

were recruited from four district-level teachers’ associations in British Columbia. The four

districts enabled the collection of data from a variety of school settings and locations.

1 Although this contradicts the aim to focus on current experiences in the measure, it was necessary to refer to prior experiences in data collection given the constraints placed on teachers by the industrial action (i.e., some tasks were not being undertaken at the time of data collection).

32

Page 45: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Presidents of the four teachers’ associations were emailed details of the study including the

invitation letter for teachers (Appendix A shows the invitation letter). The research

instrument involved an online questionnaire. Presidents forwarded the study invitation

letter—including the URL to the online questionnaire—to teachers who then decided

whether or not they wanted to participate Teachers were given three weeks to complete the

questionnaire and were sent an email reminder after two weeks.

Data was collected during the 2011/2012 school year. Although the nature of the

recruitment process did not allow for the calculation of accurate response rates (i.e., it is not

possible to ascertain how many teachers actually viewed the study’s invitation email), there

are several factors, all of which have been used successfully in the past (e.g., Collie et al.,

2012; Mertler, 2002), that provide confidence in the representativeness of the findings. First,

the demographics of the study’s sample were very similar to the population (see Table 2.4).

Second, average scores found in the current study are comparable to those found using

similar measures in other studies (see below). Finally, Mertler (2003) conducted research

showing that teachers’ responses to paper and online questionnaires were not significantly

different, revealing that both methods obtain similar data and offering support for online

questionnaires as an appropriate method for data collection among teachers.

As noted earlier, during the time of data collection teachers were involved in

industrial action over contract negotiations with the provincial government. This industrial

action restricted their work to varying degrees (i.e., it involved work slowdown as opposed to

a full strike). All teachers were teaching their regular classes; however, most teachers were

not undertaking administrative duties such as writing report cards and attending meetings

throughout the data collection timeline (discussed in further detail in the limitations section).

33

Page 46: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Given this situation, teachers were provided with the following statement in the instructions

for the TWBS: “If the current job action means that you are not currently engaging in some

of the different items below, please answer based on your experiences prior to job action, or

if you are a beginning teacher, please leave these items blank.”

In addition, comparative measures were used to help ascertain the representativeness

of the data and these suggest that the results are similar to findings in previous studies despite

the industrial action that was taking place. For example, the mean for stress in the current

study was 3.53 (see Table 2.6). This falls within the range of average stress scores among

Canadian teachers in other studies. Teachers in Klassen and Chiu’s study (2011) reported

stress levels that corresponded to means ranging from 3.45 to 3.78 out of 5. Furthermore,

Collie (2010) found a similar mean (M = 3.24, not significantly different) among a sample

from the same population as the current study (i.e., British Columbia teachers) in data

collected when industrial action was not taking place. Similarities also exist for teacher job

satisfaction (Klassen, Usher, & Bong, 2010), and flourishing (Diener et al., 2010). These

comparisons provide support for the fact that the industrial action did not significantly affect

teachers’ responses.

2.2.3. Measures

Participants completed an online, self-report questionnaire that included the new

measure, along with the measures of stress, job satisfaction, flourishing, and social

desirability to provide evidence for representativeness and validity. In addition, there were

questions pertaining to demographics. Table 2.5 shows the constructs that were examined in

Study 1, along with those examined in Studies 2 and 3. It also shows the location in the

Appendices in which the items for the different scales can be found.

34

Page 47: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

2.2.3.1. Teacher well-being. The TWBS consists of items relating to teachers’

experiences at work. Originally, 60 items were included in the questionnaire (see Appendix

B, Section 6, Questions 1 through 60). Teachers were asked to indicate the degree to which

different aspects of teaching work affect their well-being as a teacher. A brief definition of

the construct of teacher well-being was included in the opening question: “Currently, how do

the following aspects of being a teacher affect your well-being as a teacher? Well-being

refers to open, engaged, and healthy functioning as a teacher.” The items were scored on a

seven-point Likert-type scale ranging from Negatively (1), Mostly negatively (2), More

negatively than positively (3), Neither positively not negatively (4), More positively than

negatively (5), Mostly positively (6), to Positively (7). These anchor points were chosen to

allow teachers to report the positive or negative influence of work experiences on their well-

being. Given that the scale was being developed, no evidence of validity or reliability

previously existed for this scale. Evidence for reliability and validity are provided below in

the results section.

2.2.3.2. Additional constructs. For the purpose of providing some support for the

representativeness of the data and preliminary convergent evidence of validity, three

additional constructs were assessed: teacher stress, job satisfaction, and flourishing. In

addition, a social desirability measure was administered to ascertain whether teachers’

responses to the well-being items were subject to biases. Table 2.6 shows the reliability

indexes, means, standard deviations, and ranges for these variables. Composite variables

were created by taking the mean of all the items for each construct.

35

Page 48: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.5

Constructs Under Examination in Each of the Three Studies and Item Location in the Appendices

Appendix B Appendix D

Study 1 Study 2 Study 3

Teacher well-being Yes (see Section 6) Yes (see Table 2.7) Yes (see Section 2)

Stress Yes (see Section 5, Q. 5) — —

Job satisfaction Yes (see Section 5, Q. 1-4) Yes (see Section 5, Q. 1-4) —

Flourishing Yes (see Section 2, Q. 7-14) Yes (see Section 2, Q. 7-14) —

Social desirability Yes (see Section 5, Q. 6-12) — —

Perceived autonomy support — Yes (see Section 2, Q. 1-6) —

Need satisfaction — Yes (see Section 3) Yes (see Section 3)

Autonomous and controlled motivation — Yes (see Section 4) —

Organisational commitment — Yes (see Section 2, Q. 15-20) —

Self-efficacy for teaching — — Yes (see Section 4)

36

Page 49: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

The first construct to provide convergent evidence of validity was a single-item

teacher stress measure: “In general, how stressful do you find being a teacher?” (Boyle et al.,

1995; see Appendix B, Section 5, Question 5). Teachers responded to this on a scale ranging

from Not Stressful (1) to Extremely Stressful (7). This approach to measuring stress has been

used in previous research involving teachers (e.g., Chaplain, 2008).

The second construct was job satisfaction and this was assessed using four items that

Caprara, Barbaranelli, Borgogni, and Steca (2003) developed (see Appendix B, Section 5,

Questions 1 through 4). The items (e.g., “I am satisfied with my job”) were measured on a

scale ranging from Never (1) to Almost Always (7). Previous studies have found evidence for

reliability (with Cronbach’s alphas between .83 and .85) and validity (Caprara et al., 2003;

Klassen et al., 2010) of scores from this scale. In the current study, a similar level of

reliability was found (α = .87).

The third construct was the Flourishing Scale (Diener et al., 2010; see Appendix B,

Section 2, Questions 7 through 14). The scale consists of eight items covering purpose and

meaning, supportive relationships, engagement/interest, contributing to others, competence,

being a good person, optimism, and feeling respected (e.g., “I am engaged and interested in

my daily activities”). The items are measured on a scale ranging from Strongly Disagree (1)

to Strongly Agree (7). In several samples of college students (Diener et al., 2010), high

convergence with conceptually similar scales and a robust factor structure were found, as

was evidence of reliability through internal consistency (α = .87) and temporally (α = .71). In

the current study, a Cronbach’s alpha of .86 was found.

The fourth construct was the Revised Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale

(Form X1; Fischer & Fick, 1993; see Appendix B, Section 5, Questions 6 through 12). This

37

Page 50: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

scale consists of seven items used to assess the degree to which participants’ answers are

influenced by social desirability (e.g., “I sometimes try to get even rather than forgive and

forget”). Participants indicated whether the items were true or false for them. In the current

study, the Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was low (α = .52). Unfortunately, this low alpha is

common for this scale (e.g., Beretvas, Meyers, & Leite, 2002; Brackett, et al., 2010) and may

reflect several minor dimensions (cf. Loo & Thorpe, 2000). The presence of multiple, albeit

minor, dimensions can result in a lower Cronbach’a alpha because there is less

interrelatedness between the items (Cortina, 1993). Despite the low internal consistency,

support for the use of this scale was provided by a confirmatory factor analysis that revealed

good fit (details below). Nevertheless, the results involving this scale should be interpreted

with caution.

2.2.3.3. Demographic information. Teachers were asked to supply demographic

information (see Appendix B, Section 1, Questions 1 to 14) including age, sex, subjects

taught, highest level of education, school name, ethnic/cultural heritage, country of birth,

average SES of students in their school, average academic achievement of students in their

school, years of teaching experience, roles and responsibilities, including school level and

teaching position (e.g., general classroom teachers, special education teachers), time spent

teaching per week, and school setting (urban, suburban, small town, or rural).

38

Page 51: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.6

Reliability Indexes, Means, and Standard Deviations of the Additional Measures

Range

Variable α M SD Potential Observed

Stress ― 3.53 0.834 1.00 – 5.00 1.00 – 5.00

Job Satisfaction .87 5.35 0.838 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Flourishing .86 6.09 0.686 1.00 – 7.00 1.75 – 7.00

Social desirability .52 0.63 0.211 0.00 – 1.00 0.14 – 1.00

2.3. Results

There was very little missing data for this study, ranging from 0 to 8% for all the

variables. The original pool of items contained 60 items. Before conducting factor analyses,

two items were removed because they were highly similar to other items in the pool.

Specifically, questions 39 and 60 were mistakenly duplicates of one another (see Section 6,

Appendix B) and questions 4 and 5 were highly similar (r = .88; see Section 6, Appendix B),

resulting in the removal of questions 4 and 60.

2.3.1. Factor Analyses

Factor analyses were used to ascertain the dimensionality of the TWBS (Research

Question 1). Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was first conducted, followed by

confirmation factor analysis (CFA). EFA allows researchers to examine patterns of

interrelatedness among items in a measure without being constrained by a priori hypotheses

about how the items should load on different factors or the number of factors that might be

present in the data (Kline, 2011). In other words, it allows the researcher to discover whether

39

Page 52: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

there are any coherent subsets of items and the nature of these subsets (Tabachnick & Fidell,

2007). EFA does this by summarising patterns of correlations and shared variance between

items (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). When items correlate and share variance, they load on a

factor (or latent variable) that gives an idea of the underlying dimension that ‘causes’ these

items (i.e., the latent variable that produces scores on the items; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007).

The decision was made to utilise EFA instead of principal components analysis,

another tool suitable for preliminary investigations of patterns of interrelatedness, due to the

presumed underlying dimensions. Principal components analysis is appropriate when the

relationships between items are merely empirical, whereas EFA is appropriate when some

underlying dimension is driving the relationship between items (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007).

Given that I believe one or more underlying dimensions of teacher well-being are driving the

relationships between items, factor analysis was chosen as the more appropriate method.

Once several EFAs were run and an understanding of the factors was obtained, CFA

was conducted. Where EFA aims to identify the underlying processes that produced

correlations between items, CFA aims to test whether the correlations among items are

consistent with the hypothesised factor structure provided by the exploration conducted

through the EFA (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). CFA does this by restricting items such that

they only load on the factors specified by the researcher (Brown, 2006). For both EFA and

CFA, the acceptability of the model is determined by assessing goodness of fit,

interpretability of the items, and the strength of the parameter estimates (Brown, 2006).

Three indices were used to assess the goodness of fit of the models: Root Mean

Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and Standardised

Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR). RMSEA measures goodness-of-fit by assessing fit of

40

Page 53: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

the model compared to a perfect model, where a lower score represents better fit (Tabachnick

& Fidell, 2007). CFI measures relative improvements to the fit of the final model compared

to an independence model (i.e., a model involving completely unrelated variables; Kline,

2011; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). A higher CFI corresponds with better fit. Finally, SRMR

examines differences between the observed and predicted correlations in the data and model,

respectively. For SRMR, a lower statistic represents better fit. These three indices of fit were

chosen because they offer three different methods for calculating fit and combined they

provide a more comprehensive argument for the fit of the models than one index may

provide. In regard to the statistical power involved in these factor analyses, ‘rules of thumb’

suggested by Tabachnick and Fidell (2007) indicate that the sample size was adequate.

In order to determine an acceptable degree of fit for these indices, cut-offs established

in Hu and Bentler’s (1999) work were used. RMSEA values less than .10 were assessed as

evidence of adequate fit and values less than .06 were assessed as evidence of a good fit.

CFI values greater than .90 and .95 were assessed as evidence of adequate and good fit,

respectively. SRMR values less than .10 and .08 were assessed as evidence of adequate and

good fit, respectively. The chi-square model fit test is also reported.

When using factor analysis, it is important to take into account the hierarchical nature

of the data. Given that the data came from teachers who were nested within the schools at

which they worked, the data were hierarchically nested. Accordingly, steps were taken to

reduce artificial inflation of the parameter estimates, which can occur because teachers

within the same school tend to be more similar to one another in their responses than teachers

from different schools. The issue of hierarchically nested data was addressed using several

relevant features of Mplus version 7.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2012). These included

41

Page 54: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

using robust (sandwich estimator) standard errors, a robust chi-square test, and the

“TYPE=COMPLEX” option (with school as a cluster variable) as per Asparouhov and

Muthén’s (2005) recommendations. In order to perform the factor analyses, the robust

weighted least squares (WLSMV) estimator and geomin oblique rotation were used. The

WLSMV estimator is appropriate for categorical data and is robust to missing and non-

normal data. Oblique rather than orthogonal rotation was chosen as it allows the factors to be

correlated (correlation of the factors was expected given that they were all related to well-

being); however, this was only used for the EFA as CFA does not require rotation (the

factors are defined a priori and do not require rotation to find best fit).

2.3.1.1. Exploratory factor analyses. As described, data from the TWBS items

were subjected to several EFAs. The EFAs were conducted on a randomly selected half of

the sample to promote robustness of the results and so that the CFA could be conducted (and

the factors confirmed) on a different set of responses. Because EFA is an exploratory

procedure (Brown, 2006), steps must be taken to determine the most appropriate number of

factors to be retained. In order to do this, parallel analysis was utilised. Parallel analysis is

considered a highly accurate method of determining the number of factors to retain (Velicer,

Eaton, & Fava, 2000). It estimates eigenvalues based on a random dataset that contains the

same number of variables and cases as the real data. For factors to be retained from the real

data, their eigenvalues must be greater than those created from the random data (Hayton,

Allen, & Scarpello, 2004). This method was used to determine the best number of factors for

each of the EFA models that were run. For the initial EFA, there were five factors. By the

final EFA, there were three factors.

42

Page 55: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Through the running of several EFAs, 35 items were removed based on the strength

of the factor loading (i.e., low loading or cross-loading with other factors) and/or

interpretability (i.e., they did not make conceptual sense in relation to the factor on which

they loaded; Brown, 2006). Through this process, items that loaded under .400 on all factors,

items that cross-loaded as well as loaded under .500 on all factors, and items that cross-

loaded within .100 were removed. This removal left 23 items in the measure organised under

four factors. At this point, the items were examined to ascertain the underlying factors. Three

of the items fell under the fourth factor: (a) “Using technology for administrative work” (see

Section 6, Appendix B, Question 14), (b) “Using technology for teaching” (Question 15),

and (c) “The challenge of teaching work” (Question 16). Conceptually, grouping these items

together under a fourth factor did not make sense. Furthermore, the factor only explained 8%

of the variance and so for these two reasons it was removed. This step reduced the scale

down to three factors. In the final step, an additional four items were removed because they

did not fit well conceptually and/or load well.

The eigenvalues for the final three-factor model were 5.871, 2.254, and 1.693.

Together they explained 61% of the total variance. Fit indices for the final EFA involving 16

items showed the following: χ2 (75, N = 239) = 160.511, p < .001, RMSEA = .069, CFI =

.972, and SRMR = .037. These indices suggest that the three-factor solution has adequate to

good fit. Table 2.7 shows the factors, the corresponding items, and the factor loadings. The

items in each factor were examined to identify names that accurately describe the latent

variable underlying the items. Factor 1 was given the name, workload well-being, as it relates

to issues associated with workload and associated pressure. Factor 2 was given the name,

organisational well-being, as it relates to teachers’ perceptions of the school-as-an-

43

Page 56: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

organisation including perceptions of school leadership and the culture towards teachers and

teaching at the school. Factor 3 was given the name, student interaction well-being, as it

relates to teachers’ interactions with students.

2.3.1.2. Confirmatory factor analyses. A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was

conducted on the other half of the sample to confirm the factor structure revealed in the EFA.

Fit indices revealed adequate to good fit of the confirmatory measurement model: χ2 (101, N

= 237) = 222.763, p < .001, RMSEA = .071, and CFI = .97 (SRMR was not available for this

estimation method). Standardised factor loadings from the CFA are shown in Table 2.8.

Using the whole sample, the latent variable correlations between the three factors were

calculated. These are shown in Table 2.9 and suggest that these factors are tapping into

related, albeit different dimensions of teacher well-being. The moderate strength of these

correlations also suggests that the three factors can be combined to obtain an overall score of

teacher well-being.

44

Page 57: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.7

Factor Loadings for the Final Exploratory Factor Analysis

Item Loading

Factor 1: Workload well-being

1. Marking work .638

2. Fitting everything in to the allotted time .653

3. Administrative work related to teaching .790

4. Work I complete outside of school hours for teaching .748

5. Working to finish my teaching tasks .685

6. Staying late after work for meetings and activities .702

Factor 2: Organisational well-being

7. Relations with administrators at my school .775

8. Support offered by school leadership .809

9. Recognition for my teaching .526

10. School rules and procedures that are in place .700

11. Communication between members of the school .706

12. Participation in school-level decision making .724

Factor 3: Student interaction well-being

13. Relations with students in my class .620

14. Student behaviour .933

15. Student motivation .823

16. Classroom management .657

45

Page 58: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.8

Standardised Factor Loadings from the Well-being Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Item Loading

Factor 1: Workload well-being

1. Marking work .638

2. Fitting everything in to the allotted time .748

3. Administrative work related to teaching .715

4. Work I complete outside of school hours for teaching .766

5. Working to finish my teaching tasks .773

6. Staying late after work for meetings and activities .742

Factor 2: Organisational well-being

7. Relations with administrators at my school .674

8. Support offered by school leadership .790

9. Recognition for my teaching .671

10. School rules and procedures that are in place .773

11. Communication between members of the school .668

12. Participation in school-level decision making .681

Factor 3: Student interaction well-being

13. Relations with students in my class .493

14. Student behaviour .927

15. Student motivation .832

16. Classroom management .792

46

Page 59: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.9

Latent Variable Correlations of the Well-being Factors

Workload well-being Organisational well-being

Organisational well-being .478

Student interaction well-being .555 .483

2.3.1.3. Descriptive statistics. Descriptive statistics for the final 16 items and the

three factors (created by taking the average of all items for each factor) are shown in Table

2.10. The observed range among items matched the potential range in all bar one item,

suggesting a substantial amount of dispersion of scores. For workload well-being, the means

of the items and the composite variable fell around or below the midpoint of the scale,

whereas for organisational and student interaction well-being, the means of all items and the

composite variables fell above the midpoint. The item that teachers reported affected their

well-being most positively was item 13, “Relations with students in my class.” In contrast,

the item that affected teacher well-being most negatively was item 6, “Staying late after work

for meetings and activities.” The items associated with workload well-being generally

showed positive skewness, revealing a concentration of scores below the mean. In contrast,

the items associated with organisational and student interaction well-being all showed

negative skewness, revealing a concentration of scores above the mean. Kurtosis was varied

with some items showing leptokurtic distributions (positive kurtosis), others showing

platykurtic distributions (negative kurtosis), and some showing almost no kurtosis.

Cronbach’s alphas for the three factors were .85, .89, and, .82, respectively.

47

Page 60: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.10

Descriptive Statistics for Items in the Teacher Well-being Scale

Skewness Kurtosis

Item M SD Statistic Std. error

Statistic Std. error

Observed range

Workload 3.43 1.058 0.31 0.102 0.02 0.203 1.00 – 6.83

1. 4.06 1.372 -0.03 0.102 -0.11 0.203 1.00 – 7.00

2. 3.11 1.375 0.40 0.102 -0.03 0.203 1.00 – 7.00

3. 3.17 1.285 0.42 0.102 0.18 0.204 1.00 – 7.00

4. 3.27 1.523 0.32 0.102 -0.65 0.204 1.00 – 7.00

5. 3.91 1.367 -0.02 0.103 -0.25 0.205 1.00 – 7.00

6. 3.06 1.460 0.45 0.102 -0.32 0.204 1.00 – 7.00

Organisational 4.52 1.050 -0.45 0.102 0.18 0.203 1.00 – 7.00

7. 5.20 1.433 -1.03 0.101 0.71 0.202 1.00 – 7.00

8. 4.50 1.385 -0.55 0.103 -0.05 0.205 1.00 – 7.00

9. 4.05 1.579 -0.21 0.102 -0.61 0.204 1.00 – 7.00

10. 4.35 1.332 -0.48 0.102 0.03 0.204 1.00 – 7.00

11. 4.52 1.384 -0.47 0.102 -0.33 0.204 1.00 – 7.00

12. 4.48 1.312 -0.47 0.103 0.11 0.205 1.00 – 7.00

Student interaction 4.72 1.092 -0.27 0.101 -0.42 0.202 1.00 – 7.00

13. 5.90 0.923 -0.97 0.101 1.38 0.202 2.00 – 7.00

14. 4.30 1.505 -0.40 0.101 -0.57 0.202 1.00 – 7.00

15. 4.27 1.473 -0.28 0.101 -0.70 0.202 1.00 – 7.00

16. 4.42 1.418 -0.21 0.102 -0.58 0.203 1.00 – 7.00

48

Page 61: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Teachers’ responses on the three factors were examined in relation to the variable of

social desirability. First, the social desirability measure was subjected to a CFA to ensure the

items loaded as expected. The CFA revealed good fit: χ2 (14, N = 485) = 20.86, p = .12,

RMSEA = .032, and CFI = .95. Social desirability correlated with workload well-being at

.103, with organisational well-being at .135, and with student interaction well-being at .098.

These three correlations were significant (p < .05); however, their effect sizes were small

(i.e., R2 < 2% of variance). This suggests that social desirability played a significant, but

small role in teachers’ ratings of teacher well-being.

2.3.2. Demographic Comparisons

Comparisons of TWBS score differences across groups were conducted based on the

following demographic variables: sex, teaching experience, school level, school setting, and

teachers’ perceptions of students’ SES and academic achievement (Research Question 2).2

Table 2.11 shows the descriptive statistics for each group on the different demographic

variables for each of the three factors.

2.3.2.1. Sex and teaching experience. A t-test revealed no significant differences

between females and males on any of the three well-being factors (i.e., female and male

teachers reported similar levels of well-being). In addition, Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)

was run to ascertain if there were any significant differences in well-being between different

career stages (see Table 2.11 for career stages examined). Once again, there were no

significant differences between groups on any of the factors.

2 Comparisons involving different positions were not conducted given that the vast majority of participants were classroom teachers (81%).

49

Page 62: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

2.3.2.2. School level. ANOVA revealed significant differences between teachers at

different school levels in their reports of workload well-being, F (3, 572) = 4.826, p = .005,

η2 = .02, and organisational well-being, F (3, 571) = 5.571, p = .002, η2 = .03. Participants

whose job involved teaching at more than one school level concurrently (e.g., middle and

secondary as a learning support teacher) reported significantly higher workload well-being

scores than teachers who taught at only one school level (e.g., elementary only). For

organisational well-being, elementary teachers reported significantly higher scores than

secondary teachers. In both cases, the effect sizes were small suggesting that minimal

variance in scores was accounted for by school level (only 2-3% of the variance).

2.3.2.3. School setting. ANOVA revealed a significant difference in reports of

workload well-being for school setting, F (3, 563) = 5.808, p = .001, η2 = .03. Participants

who taught in rural school settings reported significantly higher scores than teachers in all

other settings. Again, the effect size was small suggesting little practical difference. There

were no significant differences in organisational well-being and student interactional well-

being for school setting.

2.3.2.4. Socio-economic status and academic achievement. Teachers’ reports of

workload, organisational, and student interaction well-being did not differ as a function of

teachers’ perceptions of their students’ SES. For teachers’ perceptions of students’ academic

achievement, there were no significant differences in reports of workload and organisational

well-being; however, there were significant differences in reports of student interaction well-

being, F (4, 565) = 3.226, p = .025, η2 = .02. Teachers who reported low-average academic

achievement among their students had significantly lower student interaction well-being than

teachers who reported high-average academic achievement. Once again, the effect size was

50

Page 63: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

small suggesting that minimal variance in student interaction well-being was accounted for

by academic achievement of students.

Table 2.11

Descriptive Statistics for the Three Well-being Factors by Demographic Characteristics

Workload well-being

M (SD)

Organisational well-being

M (SD)

Student interaction well-being

M (SD)

Sex

Female 3.39 (1.080) 4.54 (1.047) 4.70 (1.102)

Male 3.55 (0.980) 4.41 (1.015) 4.85 (1.001)

Significant difference ns ns ns

Years’ experience

< 6 years 3.44 (0.944) 4.55 (0.884) 4.53 (1.038)

6-10 years 3.27 (0.963) 4.69 (0.843) 4.59 (1.056)

11-20 years 3.39 (1.068) 4.40 (1.074) 4.72 (1.055)

20-30 years 3.52 (1.104) 4.53 (1.086) 4.90 (1.138)

> 30 years 3.77 (1.181) 4.51 (1.302) 4.89 (1.244)

Significant difference ns ns ns

School level

Elementary 3.38 (1.044) 4.65 (1.025) 4.67 (1.124)

Middle 3.27 (1.015) 4.39 (0.981) 4.63 (1.087)

Secondary 3.43 (1.092) 4.27 (1.124) 4.83 (1.063)

All levels 3.96 (0.967) 4.63 (0.855) 4.74 (0.975)

Significant difference p = .005 p = .002 ns

51

Page 64: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.11 (Continued)

Workload well-being

M (SD)

Organisational well-being

M (SD)

Student interaction well-being

M (SD)

School setting

Urban 3.41 (1.042) 4.53 (1.111) 4.70 (1.100)

Suburban 3.36 (1.049) 4.52 (0.993) 4.71 (1.071)

Small Town 3.42 (0.919) 4.26 (1.087) 4.66 (1.060)

Rural 4.11 (1.108) 4.82 (0.895) 5.06 (1.120)

Significant difference p = .001 ns ns

SES of students’ families

Low 3.42 (1.004) 4.53 (1.141) 4.70 (1.021)

Low-average 3.35 (0.956) 4.49 (0.988) 4.59 (1.093)

Average 3.53 (1.102) 4.56 (0.988) 4.72 (1.129)

High-average 3.33 (1.125) 4.50 (1.152) 4.87 (1.054)

High 3.38 (1.000) 4.30 (0.997) 4.93 (0.998)

Significant difference ns ns ns

Academic achievement of students

Low 3.43 (0.979) 4.49 (1.028) 4.70 (1.037)

Low-average 3.45 (0.925) 4.70 (0.937) 4.56 (1.052)

Average 3.48 (1.088) 4.48 (1.040) 4.68 (1.122)

High-average 3.34 (1.119) 4.50 (1.086) 5.01 (1.012)

High 3.31 (1.141) 4.21 (1.282) 4.88 (0.984)

Significant difference ns ns p = .025

Note. ns = not significant.

52

Page 65: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

2.3.3. Preliminary Evidence of Validity

Evidence of validity based on internal structure was reported through the Cronbach’s

alphas and factor analyses detailed previously. Namely, the Cronbach’s alphas provided

adequate support for internal consistency, and the EFAs and CFA showed adequate to good

fit for the factor structure of the three-factor solution. Taken together, these findings provide

evidence for the validity of TWBS score interpretations based on internal structure.

Below, convergent evidence of validity is presented by comparing scores on the new

measure with three related constructs. Before doing that, however, a CFA using the whole

sample was conducted for the two multiple item comparative measures (i.e., the flourishing

and job satisfaction scales) to ensure that they loaded as expected. The CFA revealed

adequate to good fit, χ2 (53, N = 485) = 264.44, p < .001, RMSEA = .091, CFI = .98, with

items loadings on two factors as expected: flourishing and job satisfaction (Table 2.12 shows

the items and factor loadings).

2.3.3.1. Preliminary convergent evidence of validity. Table 2.13 shows the

correlations of the three factors in the new measure with the constructs of stress, job

satisfaction, and flourishing (Research Question 3). Given that the three factors of well-being

correlated significantly with social desirability, partial correlations were also calculated to

assess whether the inclusion of social desirability diminished the findings. Table 2.14 shows

the partial correlations that controlled for social desirability. In all cases, the correlations

between the well-being factors and the comparison measures remained significant and

changed very little as a consequence of controlling for social desirability, indicating that

social desirability did not significantly affect the findings. Taken together, both Table 2.13

and Table 2.14 show that reports of stress had moderately negative relationships with the

53

Page 66: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

three factors of well-being, and that reports of both job satisfaction and flourishing had

moderately positive relationships with the three factors of well-being.

Table 2.12

Standardised Factor Loadings from the Flourishing and Job Satisfaction Confirmatory

Factor Analysis

Item Loading

Factor 1: Flourishing

1. I lead a purposeful and meaningful life .862

2. My social relationships are supportive and rewarding .761

3. I am engaged and interested in my daily activities .829

4. I actively contribute to the happiness and well-being of others .741

5. I am competent and capable in the activities that are important to me .772

6. I am a good person and live a good life .789

7. I am optimistic about my future .629

8. People respect me .715

Factor 2: Job satisfaction

1. I am satisfied with my job .881

2. I am happy with the way my colleagues and superiors treat me .639

3. I am satisfied with what I achieve at work .896

4. I feel good at work .932

54

Page 67: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 2.13

Correlations of the Three Well-being Factors with the Additional Measures

Workload well-being

Organisational well-being

Student interaction well-

being

Stress -.393 -.264 -.312

Job satisfaction .339 .515 .515

Flourishing .271 .347 .375

Note. All correlations are significant at p < .01.

Table 2.14

Partial Correlations of the Three Well-being Factors with the Additional Measures

Controlling for Social Desirability

Workload well-being

Organisational well-being

Student interaction well-

being

Stress -.402 -.273 -.321

Job satisfaction .335 .520 .518

Flourishing .267 .345 .373

Note. All correlations are significant at p < .01.

55

Page 68: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

2.4. Discussion

Teacher well-being is an issue that is receiving increasing attention given its link to

important outcomes for teachers, students, and schools (e.g., Duckworth et al., 2009,

Retelsdorf et al., 2010). Study 1 aimed to extend our understanding in this area by

developing a dedicated measure for assessing teacher well-being. The majority of extant

research in this area has assessed teacher well-being through related constructs (e.g., stress,

Van Horn, Schaufeli, & Taris, 2001; burnout, McCarthy, Lambert, O’Donnell, & Melendres,

2009) or through measures of global well-being (e.g., life satisfaction; Albuquerque et al.,

2011). More recently, researchers are beginning to consider how teacher well-being is shaped

by experiences at work (e.g., Konu et al., 2010). In order to further extend our knowledge,

more examinations like this are needed so that we can develop our understanding of what

and how work-related factors influence teacher well-being. This type of research also has the

potential to inform our understanding of other important ramifications like teacher attrition

and retention. Study 1 addressed these issues by developing a measure of teacher well-being

based on teachers’ experiences at work.

In order to develop the new measure, a pool of items was administered to teachers.

Following this, exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were conducted to identify and

confirm three factors of teacher well-being that tap into three distinct areas of teachers’ work

experiences (Research Question 1): workload well-being, organisational well-being, and

student interaction well-being. Following this, demographic comparisons were conducted to

ascertain how the new measure functions among different groups of teachers (Research

Question 2). Finally, scores on the new measure, the TWBS, were correlated with other

56

Page 69: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

measures to provide preliminary evidence of validity (Research Question 3). Key findings

are discussed below.

2.4.1. Research Question 1: What is the Nature of Teacher Well-being?

The factor analyses revealed three factors of teacher well-being: workload,

organisational, and student interaction well-being. Descriptive statistics and factor analyses

revealed a psychometrically acceptable instrument for measuring these factors. The scores on

the items retained for the three factors fell only slightly above or below the mid-point and the

observed ranges matched the potential ranges in all but one item. These properties suggest

that the items adequately differentiate between teachers. Furthermore, the Cronbach’s alphas

were high for the three factors indicating adequate internal consistency.

2.4.1.1. Factor one: Workload well-being. The first factor of the new measure is

workload well-being. The six items associated with this factor relate to tasks that teachers are

required to do as part of their teaching duties such as marking assignments, attending

meetings, and working after hours. All of the items associated with this factor had means that

were below the mid-point of the scale. In other words, the aspects of teaching that relate to

workload influenced teacher well-being negatively on average. This finding confirms

previous studies showing that these types of teaching tasks are associated with stress (Collie

et al., 2012; Klassen & Chiu, 2010) and burnout (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004).

The finding is important for administrators and policy-makers as it underscores the

importance of providing balanced workloads for teachers. Ample research has shown that

teachers’ workloads are associated with stress (e.g., Collie et al., 2012; Klassen & Chiu,

2010). Furthermore, Milkie and Warner (2011) found that teachers who indicated that

administrative tasks interfered with their teaching were more likely to report greater

57

Page 70: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

externalising behavioural problems among their students (e.g., fighting, disturbing ongoing

activities). Administrators and policy-makers, therefore, may want to pay more attention to

the increasing workloads that are being placed upon teachers. Although the aim of increasing

workloads is to encourage better learning and achievement among students, this may not

occur if teacher well-being is threatened. In fact, the opposite may occur given that teacher

well-being is associated with important outcomes such as effective teaching (Duckworth et

al., 2009). In other words, teaching effectiveness and students’ learning and achievement

may suffer from the very workloads that are attempting to increase teaching effectiveness

and student achievement.

Future research that examines why some teachers reported that these items positively

influence their well-being has the potential to shed more light on what school leadership and

policy-makers can do to minimise the negative effects of workloads on teachers. Perhaps it is

the case that those teachers who reported positive influences worked in environments where

workloads were appropriately balanced or more support was offered for meeting workload

demands.

2.4.1.2. Factor two: Organisational well-being. The second factor of the new

measure is organisational well-being. The six items associated with this factor refer to

organisational-level issues relating to teachers and teaching such as relations and

communications between teachers and administrators, support and recognition offered by

administrators, participation in decision-making by teachers, and the school rules and

procedures in place. All of the items associated with this factor had means that were above

the mid-point of the scale. In other words, teachers generally felt that the organisational-level

aspects of their work influenced their well-being positively. This is a positive finding for

58

Page 71: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

school administrators as it suggests they are succeeding in creating working environments

that are healthy for teachers. In particular, “relations with administrators in my school” was

rated most highly among these items. This finding is supported by research on school climate

that has linked positive perceptions of these types of items with well-being related constructs

(e.g., lower burnout, Grayson & Alvarez, 2008; lower stress, Collie et al., 2012).

This finding is important as it highlights the significance of organisational-level

issues for teacher well-being. It extends our understanding of well-being by showing how

and what organisational-level issues play a role in teachers’ reports of their work-related

well-being. It also underscores the critical role that administrators play in teacher well-

being—all of the items tap into organisational-level issues that are overseen or governed by

administrators. Furthermore, this finding identifies several different ways administrators can

improve teacher well-being outcomes. Namely, they can help to foster teacher well-being by

promoting positive relationships and dialogue across the school, ensuring teachers have input

in decision-making, ensuring appropriate and constructive rules and procedures are in place,

and recognising teachers’ efforts.

The ramifications of this factor of well-being, however, extend further. These

organisational-level issues are all related to school climate. Given there are many positive

outcomes associated with positive school climates for teachers, students, and schools (e.g.,

student learning, Bryk & Schneider, 2003; student achievement, MacNeil, Prater, & Busch,

2009), targeting this factor for improvement has the potential for far-reaching and positive

implications for all members of the school community, not just teachers.

59

Page 72: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

2.4.1.3. Factor three: Student interaction well-being. The third factor of the new

measure is student interaction well-being. The four items associated with this factor refer to

teachers’ relations with their students, student behaviour, student motivation, and classroom

management. Like factor two, all of the items associated with this factor had means that were

above the mid-point of the scale. Teachers generally felt that their interactions with students

influenced their well-being positively. This finding corroborates Klassen, Perry, and

Frenzel’s (2012) research showing that relatedness with students was positively associated

with enjoyment for teaching and negatively associated with anxiety, anger, and emotional

exhaustion among teachers. Furthermore, given that working with students is a primary

reason that many teachers cite for entering the profession (e.g., Watt & Richardson, 2007;

Sinclair, 2008) and finding it satisfying (e.g., Shann, 1998), it is not surprising that student

interactions influenced teacher well-being positively. The fact that these items formed their

own factor underscores the importance of teacher-student relations.

Despite findings from the current study highlighting the importance of student

interactions for teacher well-being, teacher-student interactions can also be stressful (e.g.,

Collie et al., 2012; Klassen & Chiu, 2010). In other words, it appears that student interactions

can be stressful for teachers, while also influencing teacher well-being positively. Taken

together, these links between teachers’ reports of well-being/stress and teacher-student

relationships further underscore the importance that teachers place on their relationships with

students. This finding also highlights the complex nature of teaching and has important

implications for researchers. In particular, it highlights the importance of considering a broad

picture when attempting to understand how teachers are influenced by their experiences at

work. Focusing on one variable may only provide a partial understanding of teachers’

60

Page 73: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

experiences at work. This finding also extends our understanding by suggesting that stress

and well-being can both be present in relation to the same aspect of teaching. This may be

because of the multifaceted nature of stress (Selye, 1974)—at moderate levels, stress can be

positive for teachers, whereas at high levels it is often detrimental (Collie, Perry, & Brenner,

2013). Alternatively, it may relate to teachers’ relationships with different students in the

class (e.g., relationship quality may vary with different students) at different times (e.g.,

relationship quality may change over the course of the school year). Future research should

attempt to untangle when and how teachers experience stress and well-being at the same time

and what this means for teacher effectiveness.

2.4.1.4. Summary. Taken together, the three factors highlight that teacher well-

being as measured by the TWBS is a multi-dimensional construct. This has important

implications for research and practice. In particular, given that workload well-being was

rated quite differently from organisational and student interaction well-being, utilising a one-

dimensional view of teacher well-being in research may not provide a full picture of how

teachers experience their work. Instead, researchers should consider a multi-dimensional

view so as to obtain a more complete understanding of this construct. For practice, a multi-

dimensional view of teacher well-being promises to give administrators and policy-makers a

deeper understanding of how teachers are faring in three critical areas, along with an

indication of what specific issues should be targeted to improve teachers’ experiences at

work and their well-being.

Although not directly examined, the results also provide an understanding of how

existing theory is relevant to understanding teacher well-being. As described earlier, items

derived from components of SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002) and socio-ecological theory

61

Page 74: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

(Bronfenbrenner, 1978) were included in the pool of items. The final list of items provides

insights into how these theories are relevant for understanding teacher well-being. Many of

the items appeared to relate to the basic psychological needs of SDT (autonomy,

competence, and relatedness). For workload well-being, if teachers are struggling with their

marking workload, they may lack a sense of competence (if they find it hard to mark) or

autonomy (if they were not responsible for designing the assignment). For student interaction

well-being, the items appear to connect with the need for relatedness with students (e.g.,

“Student motivation”). This supports Klassen, Perry, and colleagues’ (2012) finding that

relatedness with students was an important predictor of teachers’ outcomes.

Socio-ecological theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1978) also appears to provide an

understanding of the new measure. For organisational and student interaction well-being, all

the items appear to relate to macrosystems—that is, the beliefs and values of a particular

group (Bronfenbrenner, 1994). For example, the item, “recognition offered for teaching

work,” relates to belief systems that dictate how teachers are viewed and treated at the

school-level, as well as at the district and broader societal levels. In contrast, for workload

well-being, the items mostly relate to microsystems (e.g., the classroom microsystem for

completing marking work). In addition, there were two items that related to mesosystems.

“Work I complete outside of school hours for teaching” and “staying late after work for

meetings and activities” appear to relate to the linkages between school and teachers’ homes.

It is interesting to note that staying late after work was rated the most negatively of all the

items in the instrument, suggesting that the linkage between home and school may not be

positive for teacher well-being. This is understandable given that staying late after school

takes away teachers’ time with their families.

62

Page 75: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

In combination, these links to theory suggest a need for research that directly

examines relationships that the three factors of well-being have with components from SDT

(Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002) and socio-ecological theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1978). Such

research promises to help us understand how and why certain aspects of teaching work are

implicated in teacher well-being. Studies 2 and 3 take first steps in addressing this in relation

to SDT by examining empirical relations between the three factors of well-being and the

three basic psychological needs from two different perspectives. Study 2 examines this cross-

sectionally as part of an explanatory model based in SDT that also investigates relationships

between and among well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and organisational commitment.

Study 3 examines relationships between the basic psychological needs and teacher well-

being over time.

2.4.2. Research Question 2: How do Scores on the New Measure Differ Across

Demographic Groups?

For the second research question, an examination of differences in reports of teacher

well-being was conducted across demographic groups that were formed based on sex, years

of teaching experience, school level, school setting, and teachers’ perceptions of students’

SES and academic achievement. These comparisons were undertaken to examine whether or

not these demographic characteristics play a substantial role in teacher well-being. There

were no significant effects obtained for teachers’ reports of sex, years of teaching experience,

and SES of students, indicating these three demographic variables did not play a statistically

significant role in differences observed in scores on the TWBS for participants in this study.

Significant effects were obtained for reports of workload well-being and

organisational well-being in relation to school level, for workload well-being in relation to

63

Page 76: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

school setting, and for student interaction well-being in relation to teachers’ perceptions of

students’ academic achievement. In all cases, the effect sizes were small and explained less

than 3% of the variance. Although statistically significant, these small effect sizes suggest

that the examined demographic variables do not play a substantial role in differences in

teacher well-being. For the personal demographic characteristics (e.g., sex, ethnicity), it is

possible that this finding reflects the relative homogeneity of teachers (e.g., Caucasian,

female). However, the fact that these job-related demographic characteristics (e.g., teaching

experience, school level) also did not play a major role provides support for the instrument’s

relevance among teachers in different school settings and teaching situations.

This finding has several implications for research and practice. First, it extends our

understanding of teacher well-being by suggesting that we should focus our attention towards

other demographic variables. For instance, given that special education teachers experience

particularly high levels of stress, burnout, and attrition (e.g., Lavian, 2012; Thornton, Peltier,

& Medina, 2007), comparing differences across teaching positions may reveal more

differences than the examined demographic variables. The findings also highlight the

importance of examining differences in well-being based on other factors such as school

psychosocial factors (e.g., school climate) and individual beliefs.

Second, this finding reduces concerns about variance in scores being due to

demographic characteristics given that teachers from the different groups reported similar

scores. It is important to note, however, that in more diverse samples this may not be the case

(e.g., with more male teachers, greater ethnic diversity). There is a need, therefore, for

research among more diverse teaching samples on whether and how these and other

demographic variables play a role in teacher well-being. Notwithstanding this, the finding

64

Page 77: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

suggests that efforts to improve teacher well-being are likely to affect teachers among

demographically comparable samples in similar ways.

Finally, and more broadly, the fact that the measure functioned similarly across these

demographic groups poses larger questions about the types of individuals that enter teaching

in the first place and why they are so similar demographically. Research that endeavours to

examine why individuals from broader demographic groups are not choosing teaching will

have implications for understanding how to make teacher education programs more

accessible to and representative of the larger population.

2.4.3. Research Question 3: Are Interpretations of Scores from the New Measure

Supported by Evidence of Validity?

The final research question involved conducting analyses to provide preliminary

evidence of validity. Factor analyses and reliability analyses provided preliminary evidence

of validity based on internal structure. In addition, correlations provided preliminary

evidence of validity based on relations to three other variables: stress, job satisfaction, and

flourishing (i.e., the measure of well-being relating to an individual’s life as a whole).

Three of the four hypotheses about convergent evidence of validity were supported.

Namely, the moderately negative correlations between stress and the well-being factors

(Hypothesis 1), along with the moderately positive correlations between job satisfaction and

the well-being factors (Hypothesis 2a) and between flourishing and the well-being factors

(Hypothesis 3), were supported and provide evidence of validity. Taken together these

correlations conceptually situate the well-being factors where they were expected to exist and

provide support for the interpretations of scores on the TWBS. Future research should

65

Page 78: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

examine scores on the TWBS in relation to additional variables so as to provide further

evidence of validity and greater understanding about the three factors of teacher well-being.

The fourth and final hypothesis (Hypothesis 2b) concerned a comparison with

Bowling and colleagues’ (2010) research. It was hypothesised that the relationship between

reports of job satisfaction and teacher well-being in the current study would be stronger than

that found between reports of job satisfaction and subjective well-being by Bowling and

colleagues (i.e., r = .36). Support for this hypothesis was only found for two factors:

organisational (r = .52) and student interaction well-being (r = .52). The correlation for

workload well-being (r = .34) was similar to Bowling and colleagues’ correlation. Although

the hypothesis was not supported for workload well-being, upon reflection the finding makes

sense given that teachers rated the workload well-being factor as negatively affecting their

well-being. In other words, negatively-rated workload well-being should relate less strongly

with job satisfaction than positively-rated subjective well-being. Although unexpected, this

finding does, in fact, provide evidence of validity for the meaning of the latent variable.

Taken together, the relationships with other variables extend our understanding of

teacher well-being by situating it in relation to other relevant constructs. In addition, the

findings indicate that teacher well-being is distinct from the other constructs. The findings

warrant concluding, therefore, that teacher well-being should be examined separately.

Finally, the findings provide important evidence of validity to support interpretations of the

test scores of the TWBS. In order to further support the robustness of the measure, future

research is needed to test the measure with different samples to see whether the three factors

remain relevant and/or whether other factors are important.

66

Page 79: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

2.5. Limitations and Future Directions

There are several limitations to this study that must be highlighted. First, due to the

nature of the online data collection it was not possible to ascertain exact response rates. More

specifically, because details of the study were sent to presidents of the teachers’ association

who then forwarded them onto teachers, I could not determine how many teachers received

and/or read the study email and chose not to participate. It is possible that some teachers did

not check their email, ignored the email, or had filters that deleted the email about the study

(e.g., the email was marked as spam). Given this, I was only able to ascertain the number of

teachers who read the details and chose to participate.

Despite this limitation, online data collection does have several strengths including its

broad reach (data can be collected from anywhere there is an Internet connection) and lower

cost (it does not require expensive mail outs). Furthermore, given the increasing time

individuals are spending online (on laptops, tablets, and mobile phones), online

questionnaires are a method of data collection that are readily accessible by participants.

Taking all this into account, online data collection is a method that will likely increase in

popularity. Steps are needed, therefore, to support the robustness of this data collection

method.

In the current study, several steps were taken to temper the limitation of the unknown

response rates and to support the representativeness of the data. In particular, the

demographic characteristics of the sample were compared with the population and found to

be very similar. In addition, average scores on the constructs were compared with other

studies using related measures among similar populations and were found to be comparable.

In the future, efforts that attempt to collect response rates will be important in providing

67

Page 80: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

support for the robustness of the current study’s findings and also to provide further support

for the TWBS as an appropriate measure of teacher well-being. One method for doing this

would involve sending personalised email invitations to participants. By emailing individual

teachers directly, researchers would gain a clearer idea of how many teachers read the email

(e.g., by a follow-up email or phone call). Another method would involve face-to-face

recruiting in schools. Collecting consent forms at the time of introducing the study to

teachers would provide researchers with an idea of response rates. Given that online survey

research is becoming more prevalent, these types of steps are needed more globally to

support the robustness of this data collection method.

A second limitation of the study concerns the sample size. Rules of thumb in the

literature (e.g., Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007) suggest that the number of participants was

adequate for the analyses undertaken. However, rules of thumb have been criticised for being

overly simplistic (Klein, 2011). Power analyses conducted prior to data collection in future

research will help to temper this limitation and provide a more robust understanding of the

appropriateness of the sample size.

The third limitation of Study 1 is that the teachers were involved in industrial action

at the time of data collection. It is possible, therefore, that teachers’ responses were affected

by the industrial action. In order to mitigate this limitation, comparisons with teachers not

involved in such action from previous studies were provided and they suggested that

teachers’ responses were not significantly different. Of particular note, average stress levels

in the current study were not significantly different from those measured among the same

population when there was no industrial action taking place (Collie, 2010). Nevertheless,

68

Page 81: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

future research among different samples of teachers is important to ascertain whether the

measure behaves similarly among teachers who are not involved in industrial action.

At the same time, it is also important to note that at any given time, individual

teachers or groups of teachers may be experiencing other specific, time-limited stressors that

may influence their well-being (family issues, students’ personal issues, etc.). Collecting data

about contextual variables (e.g., leadership stability in the school) and/or personal

experiences (e.g., health issues of teachers or their families) will help to provide researchers

with an understanding of what factors teachers consider when selecting their responses on

the TWBS and what other issues affect their responses. In addition, research involving other

samples will help to provide support for the relevance of these three factors of teacher well-

being among different teachers.

The limitations of self-report data have been argued elsewhere (e.g., Perry & Winne,

2006; Winne, Jamieson-Noel, & Muis, 2002). A well-documented limitation of self-report

data is that participants’ responses may not be accurate representations of reality. Given that

I am interested in how teachers experience their workplace, self-reports are a relevant

method of obtaining their perceptions. However, it is important when reporting this type of

data that representative language is used (e.g., teachers perceived, reported, experienced) to

clarify that the data refer to self-reports rather than other means of measurement (e.g.,

biological measures).

Another limitation of self-report data is that it is assumed participants interpret

questions in the way the researcher intended, but this may not be the case. There are several

methods that can help to address this limitation. The use of factor analyses provided evidence

that teachers’ interpretations were as expected. That is, the factors made theoretical sense and

69

Page 82: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

the items interrelated in expected ways, thus providing support for the participants’

interpretations of the items’ meanings. Furthermore, the correlations with other variables

provided further evidence of the construct meaning. Notwithstanding this, investigations of

participants’ thought processes as they respond to each question are a highly important area

of future research and will provide understanding about teachers’ interpretations and

decision-making processes for score selection. Specifically, it would be helpful to ask

participants to indicate their thoughts as they come to a decision about each item and to ask

about their reasoning when choosing between the anchor points on the scale. This would also

provide evidence of validity of TWBS score interpretations based on response processes

(APA et al., 1999).

The final limitation to be discussed is that the data were collected from one

administration of a self-report questionnaire. This means that there was only one source of

data and that the findings are threatened by single source bias. There are several factors that

influence single source bias, one of which is social desirability. Specifically, items that are

more (or less) socially desirable may relate more (or less) strongly with each other in spite of

differences in the underlying construct (i.e., social desirability causes some of the

relatedness; Podsakoff et al., 2003). Controlling for social desirability, therefore, is one

method of reducing single source bias and this method was employed in the current study.

Given that the social desirability measure had low internal consistency, future research

utilising more robust measures is important. Moreover, investigations involving multiple

measures are an important next step as these will reduce the biases that are associated with

single-source methods (e.g., higher measurement error) and allow for greater robustness

though triangulation. Examples of this include comparing results on the TWBS with other

70

Page 83: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

indicators of teacher well-being (e.g., physical well-being, behaviour at work) through

multiple measure investigations. The limitations notwithstanding, the findings reported here

suggest that the TWBS is an appropriate measure for ascertaining teacher well-being that is

based on teachers’ own experiences.

2.6. Conclusions

In summary, Study 1 has involved developing and testing a measure of teacher work-

related well-being that is based on teachers’ own experiences. As such, the TWBS is a

practical measure that holds promise for helping teachers, school administrators, and policy

makers to understand and, potentially, guide efforts to improve teacher well-being. In

addition, it is hoped that the findings of Study 1 will help to spur greater attention to the

construct of teacher well-being in the literature. Indeed, the need for more attention to be

paid to work-related well-being has been highlighted by several experts on the matter

(Diener 2009; Rath & Harter, 2010).

EFA and CFA revealed three factors of teacher well-being—well-being related to

workload, organisational-level issues, and student interactions. These findings indicate that

teacher well-being, as measured here, has a multidimensional nature. Other findings provided

evidence of validity for interpretations of scores on the new measure, along with evidence

that variance in scores was minimally impacted by the examined demographic variables.

For the purpose of further developing our understanding of teacher well-being, two

key types of examinations are needed. First, there is a need for examinations that attempt to

situate the well-being construct in relation to other relevant constructs, such as motivation

and job satisfaction. This will help build a greater understanding of teacher well-being, how

it interacts with other constructs, and whether it actually measures what we want it to

71

Page 84: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

measure. Examinations that attempt to understand how the construct functions over time are

also needed. This will promote our understanding of teachers’ experiences of well-being,

how well-being develops over time and, ultimately, guide research that aims to promote

teacher well-being. Studies 2 and 3 of this dissertation begin the process of addressing these

needs. Specifically, Study 2 examined teacher well-being as measured by the TWBS

alongside other important constructs (motivation, job satisfaction, etc.) within the framework

of SDT, and Study 3 examined reports of teacher well-being and self-efficacy for teaching at

three time points across several months.

72

Page 85: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

CHAPTER 3. STUDY 2: DEVELOPING AND TESTING A FRAMEWORK

3.1. Literature Review

What helps teachers do their best teaching? How can teachers be supported to teach

effectively? How can teachers inspire learning and achievement among their students? The

answers to these questions have long been the focus of educational theorists and researchers.

Although definitive answers are complex, we do know that there are certain factors that can

help to promote effective teaching and learning, two of which are a teacher’s well-being and

motivation. As described in Study 1, research has shown that these two factors have been

positively associated with teaching effectiveness (e.g., Duckworth et al., 2009), resiliency in

teaching (e.g., Klusmann et al., 2008), quality instructional practices (e.g., Retelsdorf et al.,

2010), and students’ achievement (e.g., Caprara et al., 2006). Also relevant for effective

teaching and learning are teachers’ job satisfaction and organisational commitment (e.g.,

Bogler & Somech, 2004; Weiqi, 2007). In fact, research has established that teachers’

experiences of these two work-related variables are not only interrelated (e.g., Feather &

Rauter, 2004), but they are also associated with well-being and motivation among teachers

(e.g., Collie et al., 2012; Hakanen, Bakker, & Schaufeli, 2006; Jackson et al., 2006; Klassen

& Chiu, 2011; Moè, Pazzaglia, Ronconi, 2010; Weiqi, 2007). These teacher-related

variables, therefore, have far-reaching ramifications for teachers, students, and schools.

Despite knowing that teachers’ experiences of these four variables (well-being,

motivation, job satisfaction, and organisational commitment) are interrelated, we still do not

know how they interact when considered simultaneously. To date, no research in this area

has examined these four variables in combination. In order to advance our understanding of

teachers’ experiences at work, therefore, it is necessary to examine how these four variables

73

Page 86: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

interact with one another in concert. This is important given that these variables are

associated with key outcomes for teachers and students (e.g., teaching effectiveness,

Duckworth et al., 2009; students’ motivation, Pakarinen et al., 2010). It is also important

given that the variables may function differently when considered in combination.

Furthermore, it will help to provide critical information in understanding how efforts to

improve one of the variables may affect teachers’ experiences of the other three variables.

For example, if schools implement an initiative to increase teachers’ motivation, it would be

very helpful to understand how this may simultaneously affect teachers’ well-being, job

satisfaction, and organisational commitment.

A relevant framework through which to examine the interrelationships between these

variables is self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002). Briefly, SDT

establishes that an individual’s well-being and motivation in a certain context, such as work,

are influenced by the autonomy support provided/experienced within that context, as well as

the satisfaction of three basic psychological needs—autonomy, competence, and

relatedness—in that context. For teachers, this means that their motivation and well-being are

affected by the autonomy support provided in their working environment, along with their

need satisfaction for autonomy, competence, and relatedness.

SDT research has been applied in a variety of settings, including classroom settings

(e.g., Filak & Sheldon, 2008; Roth et al., 2007); however, most of this research has involved

students, with only a limited amount of research examining teachers’ self-determination and

factors associated with it (e.g., Klassen, Perry, et al., 2012; Taylor, Ntoumanis & Standage,

2008). Roth, Assor, Kanat-Maymon, and Kaplan (2007) have called this surprising and I

agree given that SDT provides a relevant and empirically supported framework for

74

Page 87: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

understanding well-being and motivation among teachers. Moreover, SDT provides a strong

foundation for considering how well-being and motivation are related to teachers’ job

satisfaction and organisational commitment.

The aim of Study 2 was to present and test an explanatory model of teacher well-

being, motivation, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment that is based in

SDT. The explanatory model is shown in Figure 3.1. The first part of the model utilises SDT

to show that autonomy-supportive work climates (shown as perceived autonomy support)

predict need satisfaction and, in turn, need satisfaction predicts well-being and motivation.

The second part of the model shows how well-being and motivation are associated with job

satisfaction and organisational commitment (shown as affective organisation commitment).

Although some conceptual support exists for these relationships (e.g., Caprara et al., 2006),

more research is needed to extend our understanding of how these constructs interact

simultaneously. Study 2, therefore, involved testing a model of these relationships. Before

describing the explanatory model, SDT and the core variables are briefly outlined.

75

Page 88: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Figure 3.1. The explanatory model of teacher well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and

affective organisational commitment.

3.1.1. Self-Determination Theory

SDT posits that an autonomy-supportive work climate has a positive influence on

employees’ well-being and motivation (Deci & Ryan, 2002). One way to examine autonomy

support is to look at the support provided by people who supervise in each context (e.g.,

managers, principals). In business settings, autonomy-supportive work climates involve

managers providing employees with a meaningful rationale for the work tasks that they are

required to do, acknowledging employee’s feelings regarding uninteresting tasks, and

providing employees with choices regarding what work tasks they do, when they do them,

and how they complete them (Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, & Leone, 1994). In educational

settings, many studies have shown that teachers who provide an autonomy-supportive

76

Page 89: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

learning environment for their students (i.e., through taking the students’ perspective,

acknowledging the students’ feelings, providing choice, and minimising demands; Black &

Deci, 2000) tend to have students who experience positive outcomes (e.g., Deci, Schwarz,

Sheinman, & Ryan, 1981; Filak & Sheldon, 2008; Jang, Reeve, & Deci, 2010; Reeve, Jang,

Carrell, Jeon, & Barch, 2004; Roth et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2008). Emerging research is

showing that a similar relationship may occur between teachers and principals. Namely,

when teachers perceive that their principal is supporting their autonomy, they are more likely

to report positive outcomes (e.g., Klassen, Perry, et al., 2012).

One relevant premise of SDT is that autonomy-supportive work climates allow

individuals to experience satisfaction of three basic psychological needs: autonomy,

competence, and relatedness (see Path 1 in Figure 3.1; Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002). As defined

in Study 1, autonomy involves the perception that we are the origin or source of our

behaviour and that we have a choice in the types of actions we undertake (Deci & Ryan,

1985, 2002; Skinner & Edge, 2002). Competence involves the perception that we are

effective in our interactions (Deci & Ryan, 2002). Relatedness refers to our need to feel

connected to important others and the community (Deci & Ryan, 2002; Taylor & Ntoumanis,

2007).

One notable piece of research examining this premise of SDT among teachers is

Klassen, Perry, et al.’s (2012) work. In two related studies conducted among Canadian

teachers, they utilised structural equation modelling to examine the impact of teachers’

perceptions of principal’s autonomy support on need satisfaction, and the subsequent

relationships that need satisfaction has with work engagement, emotional exhaustion, anger,

anxiety, and enjoyment. The findings revealed that teachers’ perceptions of principal’s

77

Page 90: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

autonomy support positively predicted their reports of basic psychological need satisfaction,

which in turn predicted positive outcomes. Of particular importance, was their finding of a

two-component model of the basic psychological need of relatedness: relatedness with

colleagues and relatedness with students. Like research in business settings, they found that

relatedness with colleagues was an important predictor of teachers’ outcomes; however, they

found that relatedness with students was more important. Thus, the findings of the Klassen,

Perry, et al. study extend our understanding of relatedness among teachers by showing that it

is different from the more typical conceptualisations of this need in business and other

settings.

A second premise of SDT is that when an individual experiences need satisfaction of

autonomy, competence, and relatedness, his or her well-being (see Path 2 in Figure 3.1) and

motivation (see Path 3 and 4 in Figure 3.1) are positively affected (Deci & Ryan, 1985,

2002). In the current study, well-being is defined as flourishing in relation to teaching work.

As defined in the Introduction, it includes eight factors: purpose and meaning, supportive

relationships, engagement, contributing to others, competence, being a good person,

optimism, and feeling respected (Diener et al., 2010). Research has supported the premise

that need satisfaction positively influences well-being (defined in different ways; e.g.,

emotional exhaustion, vitality) in a variety of settings. Klassen, Perry, et al.’s (2012) study

introduced above found that teachers’ need satisfaction of relatedness with students was

negatively associated with emotional exhaustion—a construct negatively related to well-

being. Among college students, Reis, Sheldon, Gable, Roscoe, and Ryan (2000) found that

the degree of need satisfaction reported in daily activities predicted fluctuations in individual

well-being (i.e., positive and negative affect, vitality). In another study, Gagné, Ryan, and

78

Page 91: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Bargmann (2003) found that athletes’ perceptions of basic psychological need support from

coaches during a training session predicted their reports of well-being (i.e., positive and

negative affect, vitality) after the training.

Motivation (see Path 3 and 4 in Figure 3.1), as explained in the Introduction, is

defined as the drive to act, think, and develop (Deci & Ryan, 2008a) in relation to teaching

work. Although motivation is both an individual characteristic (i.e., a result of our innate

personality) and a contextual characteristic (i.e., a result of our surroundings and

experiences), this study focuses on how the experiences within a context can influence

motivation. SDT highlights two types of motivation: autonomous and controlled motivations,

each of which are influenced by intrinsic and/or extrinsic regulatory mechanisms (see Deci &

Ryan, 2012 for a review).

Autonomous motivation is highly self-determined and involves identifying with

behaviours and activities, integrating them into one’s sense of self, and undertaking them

with full volition and choice (Deci & Ryan, 2008a; Deci & Ryan, 2008b). There are three

types of behaviour regulation that are associated with autonomous motivation. The first is

intrinsic regulation, which is internally or ‘self’ motivated and refers to engaging in a

behaviour because it is inherently interesting and satisfying (e.g., a teacher reads new topic

material because it is enjoyable; Deci & Ryan, 2008a; Ryan & Connell, 1989). The second is

integrated regulation, which is extrinsically motivated but involves a behaviour that is

brought into line with the individual’s overarching beliefs, values, and goals (e.g., a teacher

reads new topic material because it means she will be a better teacher; Deci & Ryan, 2002).

The third type of regulation, identified regulation, is also extrinsically motivated and

involves consciously valuing the behaviour, but it may not represent an individual’s

79

Page 92: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

overarching beliefs (e.g., a teacher reads new topic material because he wants to understand

the topic or recognises that the topic is important for teaching; Deci & Ryan, 2002; Ryan &

Connell, 1989).

In contrast, controlled motivation is not self-determined and involves undertaking

behaviours and activities due to pressure or demands that are perceived to be externally

controlled (Deci & Ryan, 2008a). Controlled motivation encompasses two types of

behaviour regulation that are extrinsically motivated. The first is external regulation, where

an individual engages in a behaviour to satisfy an external demand, to gain a reward, or to

avoid a punishment (e.g., a teacher reads new topic materials because that is what she is

supposed to do; Deci & Ryan, 2002; Ryan & Connell, 1989). This involves no self-

determination and no internalisation—it is fully externally determined. The second type of

regulation, introjected regulation, reflects behaviour that is partially internalised (i.e.,

incorporated with the individual’s sense of self), but undertaken to avoid guilt/shame (e.g., a

teacher reads new topic material because he will feel guilty if he does not know it well

enough) or obtain contingent self-worth (e.g., a teacher reads new topic material so that

others think she is a good teacher; Deci & Ryan, 2002; Ryan & Connell, 1989). Although

this is partially internalised, it is not self-determined, but self-controlled (Pelletier, Fortier,

Vallerand, & Brière, 2001).

Another notable study involving teachers is Roth and colleagues (2007) research,

which examined whether the types of motivation described by SDT are relevant for

understanding teachers’ experiences. They investigated whether teachers discriminate

between the types of regulation (e.g., identified, introjected) as expected and whether their

experiences of these types of regulation fall along the continuum of self-determination as

80

Page 93: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

described by SDT. The study, conducted among Israeli teachers, revealed that four types of

behaviour regulation—external, introjected, identified, and intrinsic (integrated regulation

was excluded because it is difficult to differentiate it from intrinsic regulation

psychometrically; Roth et al., 2007)—were distinguished by teachers and fell along the

expected continuum of self-determination. They also examined whether reports of

autonomous motivation for teaching were associated with positive outcomes for teachers and

students. Their results revealed that autonomous motivation for teaching was negatively

associated with teachers’ emotional exhaustion and positively associated with teachers’ sense

of personal accomplishment, autonomy-supportive teaching, and students’ own autonomous

motivation for learning. Combined, the findings of this study provide important foundational

understanding of how the types of regulation associated with SDT are relevant for explaining

teacher motivation. In order to further advance our understanding, there is a need for research

that examines whether need satisfaction predicts these different types of regulation.

According to SDT, when individuals’ basic psychological needs are fulfilled, they are

more likely to internalise or identify more closely with certain behaviours (i.e., act with self-

determination), which results in more autonomous forms of motivation (Deci, Vallerand,

Pelletier, & Ryan, 1991). Research has shown that autonomous motivation is associated with

positive outcomes at work (e.g., Fernet, Guay, & Senécal, 2004). This also occurs among

teachers.

A third notable piece of research involving teachers is Taylor, Ntoumanis, and

Standage’s (2008) study, which tested a model linking perceptions of job pressure with need

satisfaction (i.e., autonomy, competence, relatedness with colleagues), self-determined

motivation (calculated using a weighted average of responses to the different types of

81

Page 94: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

regulation), and teachers’ use of autonomy-supportive teaching strategies (e.g., providing a

meaningful rationale to students) among physical education teachers in the UK. They found

that perceived job pressure negatively predicted need satisfaction, which positively predicted

self-determined motivation for teaching, and in turn the use of autonomy-supportive teaching

strategies. Although this study provides support for the relevance of several key components

of SDT among teachers, the use of the weighted average for self-determination does not

allow us to understand the relationships between need satisfaction and each different type of

regulation. Research examining this will provide a more nuanced understanding of the three

needs and how they interact with the different types of regulation.

Taken together, these constructs and the premises of SDT establish that autonomy-

supportive work climates predict need satisfaction, which in turn predicts well-being and

motivation. These relationships are shown in the explanatory model in Figure 3.1 (see paths

1 through 6). The next section elaborates the relationships that well-being and motivation

have with job satisfaction and organisational commitment among teachers (see paths 7

through 12 in Figure 3.1).

3.1.2. SDT, Job Satisfaction, and Organisational Commitment

SDT provides an ideal framework through which to explore the relationships between

teachers’ experiences of well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and organisational

commitment. As noted earlier, research has linked job satisfaction and organisational

commitment with well-being and motivation (e.g., Collie et al., 2012; Hakanen et al., 2006;

Lent et al., 2011). In addition, the use of SDT to theorise about organisational commitment

and job satisfaction among employees in general is not unprecedented (e.g., Gagné,

Chemolli, Forest, & Koestner, 2008; Gagné & Deci, 2005; Meyer & Maltin, 2010).

82

Page 95: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

However, the consideration of teacher well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and

organisational commitment simultaneously with the SDT variables of perceived autonomy

support and need satisfaction has not been elaborated.

SDT provides a relevant and conceptually-sound framework for examining this given

that the components of need satisfaction and self-determination form the underlying

mechanisms for the relationships between well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and

organisational commitment. For example, when teachers experience self-determined forms

of motivation, it is the accompanying internalisation of the value of work tasks that promotes

a sense of job satisfaction and organisational commitment (Gagné et al., 2008; Gagné &

Deci, 2005). For well-being, flourishing reflects positive feelings about one’s activities,

relationships, and self-perceptions (e.g., purpose and meaning, supportive relationships,

competence; Diener et al., 2010). In the context of work, I suggest that these positive feelings

are associated with one’s work tasks and the place of work, leading to greater job satisfaction

and organisational commitment.

I believe a model that considers all these constructs simultaneously is needed given

that SDT establishes relationships between several of these constructs and empirical

evidence suggests relationships between the remaining constructs. In addition, previous

models have been concerned with employees more generally. A model that specifically

focuses on teachers is important given that teachers’ work is unique in many ways (e.g., their

‘clients’ are students and they are physically isolated from their colleagues for much of the

work day; Barnabé & Burns, 1994). Furthermore, this type of examination is necessary in

order for us to extend our understanding of SDT in relation to teachers and to obtain a more

complete picture of teachers’ experiences. As described above, several important studies

83

Page 96: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

have examined the components of SDT among teachers (e.g., Klassen, Perry, et al., 2012;

Roth et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2008). In order to advance our understanding of teachers’

experiences and the relevance of SDT, research is needed to examine the components of

SDT simultaneously and to consider how these components relate to other important

outcomes like job satisfaction and organisational commitment. The current study, therefore,

aims to extend the literature by exploring these variables simultaneously and in direct

relation to teachers.

3.1.2.1. Job satisfaction. Job satisfaction can be defined as a sense of fulfilment that

is gained from working in a job (Locke, 1969). Teachers’ sense of job satisfaction is

important for teachers, students, and schools. For teachers themselves, research has shown

that job satisfaction is positively associated with their experiences of positive affect (Moè et

al., 2010), life satisfaction (Lent et al., 2011), and emotion regulation ability (Brackett et al.,

2010). In outcomes that extend to students’ learning and achievement, and school

effectiveness, teachers’ job satisfaction is positively associated with teachers’ work

enthusiasm (Weiqi, 2007), teaching efficacy (Collie et al., 2012), and organisational

commitment (Feather & Rauter, 2004).

A significant amount of the research on teachers’ job satisfaction is framed within

Herzberg, Mausner, and Snyderman’s (1959) two-factor theory, which establishes that job

satisfaction is influenced by two different factors. Intrinsic factors, which relate to work tasks

(e.g., work performance, professional growth), are responsible for job satisfaction, whereas

extrinsic factors of job satisfaction, which relate to peripheral issues surrounding work tasks

(e.g., administrative practices, job security), are responsible for job dissatisfaction. Research

on the two-factor theory has shown that teachers are generally satisfied with the work-related

84

Page 97: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

aspects of their job (e.g., professional growth), but dissatisfied with the working conditions

(e.g., physical environment; Butt et al., 2005; Dinham & Scott, 1998). The two-factor theory

has been criticised on several grounds, however, one being that it only considers what

promotes motivated action, rather than also how motivated action is directed (Gagné & Deci,

2005). Furthermore, the two-factor theory does not consider the potentially positive impact

on employees’ job satisfaction of interpersonal relations with supervisors—in the two-factor

theory, relations with supervisors are considered a dissatisfying extrinsic factor. Given that

research has highlighted the importance of supervisors on individual functioning (e.g., Deci

et al., 2001), this is an important factor to consider.

In order to extend our understanding, we need to examine teacher job satisfaction

utilising robust theoretical frameworks. SDT provides a more robust framework than the

two-factor theory. It addresses the two limitations of the two-factor theory. Namely, it

considers both what promotes motivated action (e.g., need satisfaction) and how it is directed

(e.g., autonomous motivation; Gagné & Deci, 2005). In addition, it considers that supervisors

may have a positive or negative influence on employee functioning. Perhaps most

importantly, support for SDT has been established among a variety of samples and in

multiple contexts.

Another important consideration about SDT is that it allows us to examine how job

satisfaction simultaneously relates to teacher well-being, motivation, and organisational

commitment. This is important given that these four variables are empirically linked. In other

words, examining them simultaneously will allow us to extend our understanding of how

they influence and are influenced by one another. The explanatory model, therefore, utilises

85

Page 98: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

SDT to examine job satisfaction and how it is related to teacher well-being, motivation, and

organisational commitment.

3.1.2.2. Organisational commitment. Another aspect of teachers’ working lives

that can be examined as part of SDT is organisational commitment. Although organisational

commitment is related to job satisfaction, research has shown that these two variables are

distinct constructs: Job satisfaction refers to feelings of contentment, while work

commitment refers to a perceived link or attachment to the organisation (Schleicher, Hansen,

& Fox, 2011). In Study 2, organisational commitment is defined more specifically as

“identification with, involvement in, and emotional attachment to the organisation” (Allen &

Meyer, 1996, p. 253). This is known as affective organisational commitment (Allen &

Meyer, 1996) because it includes an emotional component and is considered a robust

measure of organisational commitment (Solinger, van Olffen, & Roe, 2008).

Interest in teachers’ organisational commitment has been largely fuelled by concern

regarding high rates of turnover and attrition, especially among beginning teachers

(Ingersoll, 2002). Affective organisational commitment has been associated with teaching

efficacy, and behaviours that are supportive to students, colleagues, and the school (Bogler &

Somech, 2004). In addition, organisational commitment more broadly defined has been

associated with job satisfaction (Feather & Rauter, 2004), work engagement, and burnout

(Hakanen et al., 2006). These outcomes affect teaching performance and, as a result,

students’ learning and achievement (e.g., Caprara et al., 2006).

SDT provides a framework through which to extend our understanding of teacher

organisational commitment. In fact, the use of SDT to examine organisational commitment

has been established for employees in general (e.g., Meyer, Becker, & Vandenberghe, 2004;

86

Page 99: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Meyer & Maltin, 2010). The literature has not examined this construct, however, within an

SDT framework among teachers. Moreover, the literature has not examined teacher

organisational commitment alongside well-being, motivation, and job satisfaction. This is

important given that organisational commitment does not occur in isolation—it affects and is

affected by teachers’ experiences of well-being, motivation, and job satisfaction (e.g.,

Hakanen et al., 2006; Jackson et al., 2006; Feather & Rauter, 2004). Furthermore, it may

function differently when examined concurrently with the other three variables. Thus, the

explanatory model will extend the literature by considering all four variables together in

relation to teachers and by using the robust definition of affective organisational

commitment.

At this point, I should mention another type of work commitment that has been

commonly examined in the literature: professional or occupational commitment. Professional

commitment refers to attachment to the profession of teaching, rather than the school of

employment (Klassen & Chiu, 2011). Although professional commitment is likely implicated

in the variables I am considering, it is beyond the scope of the model. I have chosen to focus

on organisational commitment because it is contextually based and refers directly to a

teachers’ experiences at his or her current place of work. Professional commitment refers

more broadly to experiences in the profession of teaching and may relate to experiences

teachers have had at previous schools. Given that the model is focused on current

experiences, organisational commitment was deemed more relevant.

87

Page 100: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.1.3. The Explanatory Model

The model elaborated in this study aims to provide understanding of how four

important teacher constructs interrelate using the empirically-supported framework of SDT.

In addition, it aims to support a program of research that will enable studies of teachers’

experiences at work and how these experiences relate to other outcomes relevant for

teachers, students, and schools. The hypothesised relationships in the model are described

below in relation to theoretical and empirical support.

3.1.3.1. Perceived autonomy support and need satisfaction. As noted earlier, SDT

establishes that autonomy-supportive work climates predict satisfaction of the three basic

psychological needs among employees. Research in a variety of settings supports this

relationship. Of most relevance, Klassen, Perry, et al.’s (2012) study described earlier found

that teachers’ perceptions of the autonomy support offered by their principals was a positive

predictor of their reports of need satisfaction of autonomy, competence, relatedness with

colleagues, and relatedness with students. The model incorporates this central view of SDT

showing that teachers who perceive autonomy support, in this case from their principals, will

report greater need satisfaction for autonomy, competence, and relatedness with colleagues

and students as a result.

• Hypothesis 1. Perceived autonomy support will positively predict need satisfaction (Path

1).

3.1.3.2. Need satisfaction, well-being, and motivation. As previously established,

SDT refers to both well-being and motivation indicating that these two constructs are

influenced by need satisfaction. Among teachers, two studies that were introduced above

have shown support for this relationship. Klassen, Perry, and colleagues (2012) showed that

88

Page 101: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

when teachers’ needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness with students were met,

they experienced greater work engagement and lower emotional exhaustion. In addition,

Taylor and colleagues (2008) found that teachers’ reports of a composite need satisfaction

variable positively predicted self-determined motivation (a weighted average of the different

types of regulation). Given that work engagement is considered an indicator of intrinsic

motivation (Klassen, Aldhafri et al., 2012; Leiter & Bakker, 2010) and emotional exhaustion

is inversely related to well-being, coupled with Taylor et al.’s findings about teachers’ self-

determined motivation, these previous studies provide a starting point for understanding how

we might expect these relationships to function using the definitions of well-being and

motivation used here. Specifically, the model uses the results from these previous studies to

provide the basis for the paths from basic psychological needs to both well-being defined as

flourishing and autonomous/controlled motivation.

• Hypothesis 2. Satisfaction of autonomy, competence, and relatedness will positively

predict well-being (Path 2).

• Hypothesis 3. Satisfaction of autonomy, competence, and relatedness will positively

predict autonomous motivation (Path 3).

• Hypothesis 4. Satisfaction of autonomy, competence, and relatedness will negatively

predict controlled motivation (Path 4).

3.1.3.3. Well-being and motivation. Although not a main focus of the model, it is

important to consider how well-being and motivation interrelate. Research has shown that a

relationship exists between well-being and motivation among teachers. Lent and colleagues

(2011) found that self-efficacy for teaching (i.e., motivation) was associated with positive

affectivity and life satisfaction (i.e., well-being). Furthermore, Jackson et al. (2006) found

89

Page 102: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

that teachers’ work engagement (motivation) was negatively associated with emotional

exhaustion and stress-induced psychological ill-health (well-being).

Up to this point in time, research examining the relationship between

autonomous/controlled motivation and well-being has not been conducted among teachers.

However, research in other settings indicates that such relationships are relevant. For

example, Lynch, Plant, and Ryan (2005) showed that well-being (vitality and stress) was

related to autonomous motivation among hospital employees. While this study provides

support for similar relationships among teachers, the work environments at hospitals are very

different from schools highlighting the need for research to examine this relationship

specifically among teachers.

Although there is evidence to support the relationship between motivation and well-

being, the causality remains unknown (e.g., Brouwers & Tomic, 2000; Hakanen, Schaufeli,

& Ahola, 2008). Hence, a bi-directional relationship between these two variables is shown in

the model. This incorporates the view that motivation and well-being influence one another

reciprocally. Teachers who are feeling autonomously motivated by their work may

experience greater well-being because they identify with and value their work tasks leading

to greater flourishing at work. At the same time, teachers who experience well-being will

tend to experience autonomous motivation because their flourishing or healthy functioning

will provide fertile ground for internalisation of work tasks. For controlled motivation, it is

expected that the non-existent (i.e., external regulation) or limited (i.e., introjected

regulation) levels of internalisation that occur will mean a weak, but positive, relationship

between it and well-being (i.e., when individuals are not self-determined regarding their

work tasks this will relate to less flourishing at work).

90

Page 103: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

• Hypothesis 5. Autonomous motivation and well-being will be positively associated (Path

5).

• Hypothesis 6. Controlled motivation will be positively, but weakly associated with well-

being (Path 6).

3.1.3.4. Well-being and job satisfaction. The relationship between well-being and

job satisfaction is the first relationship in the explanatory model that goes beyond the main

components of SDT. As yet, research has not examined this relationship considering well-

being as flourishing, highlighting the need for more research in this area. Using other

definitions, research has shown that well-being (e.g., positive affect) is positively associated

with job satisfaction among teachers (e.g., Brackett et al., 2010). In terms of the directional

nature of this relationship, research suggests that well-being predicts job satisfaction. For

example, Wolpin and colleagues (2002) used longitudinal research methods to show that

perceptions of burnout predicted lower job satisfaction among teachers, not the other way

around.

Research among employees and individuals in other settings also supports this with

subjective well-being. Specifically, Bowling and colleagues (2010) conducted a meta-

analysis and found overall support for a causal link from subjective well-being to job

satisfaction. Although burnout and subjective well-being are defined differently, a similar

relationship between flourishing and job satisfaction may occur. This is based on the idea

that when teachers are flourishing at work, among other positive outcomes, they experience a

sense of meaning and purpose, supportive relationships, and interest in their work activities

(Diener et al., 2010). In turn, these experiences mean that teachers are likely to place more

value on their work (because they feel a sense of purpose) and enjoy it more (because they

91

Page 104: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

have supportive relationships and are interested in what they do), leading to greater job

satisfaction. In the model, this reasoning is used to show that well-being positively predicts

job satisfaction.

• Hypothesis 7. Well-being will positively predict job satisfaction (Path 7).

3.1.3.5. Well-being and affective organisational commitment. The second

relationship involving well-being is that with affective organisational commitment. Empirical

support for this relationship among teachers has generally involved other assessments of

organisational commitment (i.e., not affective organisational commitment; e.g., Hakanen et

al., 2006). One exception, however, is Van Horn, Taris, Schaufeli, and Schreurs’ (2004)

study conducted among Dutch teachers. Through factor analyses, they found a relationship

between self-reported positive affect and affective organisational commitment. However,

details about the polarity were not provided. Although we assume this was a positive

relationship, it highlights the need for more research involving these constructs.

For further evidence, we turn to research in other settings. Thoresen, Kaplan, Barsky,

Warren, and de Chermont (2003) conducted a review of research among employees in a

variety of work places (e.g., hospitals, businesses) and showed that reports of positive affect

were positively associated, whereas reports of negative affect were negatively associated

with affective organisational commitment. These other settings suggest that a similar

relationship may occur among teachers, but given the aforementioned differences in work

environments between teachers and hospital or office workers, there is a need for research

specifically involving teachers.

In terms of the direction of this relationship, the causal nature has not been

established (e.g., Jamal, 1990; Jepson & Forrest, 2006). Consequently, longitudinal research

92

Page 105: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

is needed in this area. In the model, well-being is shown to predict affective organisational

commitment. This is derived from research that has shown stress is a determinant of

employee commitment (e.g., Klassen & Chiu, 2011). I believe a similar relationship occurs

with well-being. Specifically, when an individual is flourishing he or she experiences

meaning and purpose, supportive relationships, and interest in the activities undertaken

(Diener et al., 2010). When this occurs at work, I believe it reflects and promotes positive

emotions and associations in the individual about the work tasks and place of work. This, in

turn, positively influences the emotional connection and affective commitment that an

individual has to his or her work (e.g., emotional attachment, identification; Solinger et al.,

2008). Study 2 aims to provide an understanding of whether well-being predicts affective

organisational commitment; however, future research is needed to examine the direction of

this relationship longitudinally in order to test the causality.

• Hypothesis 8. Well-being will positively predict affective organisational commitment

(Path 8).

3.1.3.6. Motivation and job satisfaction. The next relationship in the model

involves motivation and job satisfaction. Empirical research has provided support for a

relationship between these two variables, albeit with narrower definitions of motivation. For

example, Barnabé and Burns (1994) showed that self-reported intrinsic work motivation

among teachers was related to general satisfaction with work. Research in other settings has

corroborated this finding by showing that perceptions of intrinsic motivation were positively

associated with reports of job satisfaction among factory employees (Ilardi, Leone, Kasser, &

Ryan, 1993) and that perceptions of external regulation were negatively related to reports of

job satisfaction among hospital employees (Lynch et al., 2005). Taken together, these studies

93

Page 106: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

provide support for a relationship between motivation and job satisfaction. Given that these

previous studies used narrower definitions of motivation and/or were conducted among

different populations, however, there is a need for research using autonomous/controlled

motivation and involving teachers.

In order to understand the direction of this relationship, we must turn to research

using different definitions of motivation. Caprara and colleagues (2003, 2006) examined the

relationship between motivation (defined as self-efficacy for teaching) and job satisfaction

and found that motivation was a determinant of job satisfaction as reported by Italian

teachers. This provides some support for situating autonomous/controlled motivation as a

predictor of job satisfaction (and also highlights the need for more research in this area).

Further support is derived from SDT’s premise of internalisation.

Specifically, SDT states that autonomous motivation involves internalising

behaviours such that they become more self-determined (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002). If

behaviours or tasks are internalised then it is more likely that the individual will identify with

and value those behaviours/tasks and experience greater job satisfaction as a result (i.e.,

because the individual perceives he or she is doing meaningful tasks; Gagné & Deci, 2005).

As a result of this understanding, the model shows that autonomous motivation positively

predicts job satisfaction. In addition, controlled motivation is shown to negatively predict job

satisfaction. This is based on the reverse reasoning. If individuals are acting with controlled

motivation, they will have less identification with and place less value on their work tasks

(Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002), likely predicting lower job satisfaction as a result (i.e., if an

individual does not value what he or she is doing, then it is less likely to leave that individual

feeling satisfied).

94

Page 107: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

• Hypothesis 9. Autonomous motivation will positively predict job satisfaction (Path 9).

• Hypothesis 10. Controlled motivation will negatively predict job satisfaction (Path 10).

3.1.3.7. Motivation and affective organisational commitment. The second

relationship involving motivation includes affective organisational commitment. Although

research has examined this relationship with other definitions of commitment among

teachers (e.g., occupational commitment; Canrinus et al., 2012), little attention has been paid

to this relationship using affective organisational commitment. One exception is Bogler and

Somech’s (2004) study. They examined motivation (defined as self-efficacy) reported by

Israeli teachers and found that it positively predicted affective organisational commitment. In

order to further develop our understanding, there is a need for research that examines this

relationship using autonomous/controlled motivation.

In order to provide understanding of the directional nature of this relationship,

research conducted in other work settings is more forthcoming. Gagné, Chemolli, Forest, and

Koestner (2008) used cross-lagged analysis to examine the causality of the relationship

between reports of autonomous/controlled motivation and affective organisational

commitment among employees at a large Canadian telecommunications company. Their

results showed that autonomous motivation positively predicted affective organisational

commitment, and not the other way around. They explained that it is the internalisation that

occurs with autonomous motivation that promotes affective organisational commitment—

when individuals identify with and value their work tasks, they become attached to their

work place as a result. Their results also showed that the two forms of controlled

motivation—introjected and external regulation—had weaker positive relationships with

95

Page 108: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

affective organisational commitment. This was likely due to the lower levels of

internalisation that occurred among these individuals.

Using this previous research as a starting point, I suggest that a similar relationship

occurs among teachers. Although office workers’ and teachers’ working environments are

different, internalisation is an underlying mechanism that is relevant in both working

contexts (i.e., internalisation can occur in any context depending on the supports for self-

determination). Consequently, the model shows that autonomous and controlled motivation

predict affective organisational commitment. The nature of these two relationships differ

though. Autonomous motivation is shown to positively predict affective organisational

commitment. This is based on reasoning described above by Gagné et al. (2008), which

suggests that individuals (in this case, teachers) who are autonomously motivated will

experience greater affective organisational commitment due to the internalisation that occurs

as part of autonomous motivation. In contrast, controlled motivation is shown to negatively

predict affective organisational commitment. This is different from Gagné et al.’s findings

and is based on the idea that teachers who are acting with controlled motivation will

experience lower affective organisational commitment due to the non-existent or limited

levels of internalisation that occur regarding work tasks and the organisation.

• Hypothesis 11. Autonomous motivation will positively predict affective organisational

commitment (Path 11).

• Hypothesis 12. Controlled motivation will negatively predict affective organisational

commitment (Path 12).

96

Page 109: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.1.3.8. Job satisfaction and affective organisational commitment. The final

relationship in the model is that between teacher job satisfaction and affective organisational

commitment. Very little research has examined this relationship among teachers using the

definitions chosen in this dissertation—research has examined this relationship, however,

using other definitions of commitment (e.g., occupational commitment; Canrinus et al.,

2012). A notable exception is Feather and Rauter’s (2004) study conducted among

Australian teachers. They found that perceptions of job satisfaction were positively related to

affective organisational commitment. Given that the directional nature between these two

variables remains unclear (e.g., Billingsley & Cross, 1992), the model incorporates a bi-

directional view. Job satisfaction is shown to be positively associated with affective

organisational commitment. This is based on the idea that teachers who are satisfied with

their work will enjoy their work more, resulting in positive emotions and associations and

higher affective organisational commitment as a result. At the same time, teachers who are

highly committed may experience greater job satisfaction because they feel their time is

spent on a worthwhile cause. This interpretation is supported by Billingsley and Cross (1992)

who proposed that the two variables evolve simultaneously.

• Hypothesis 13. Job satisfaction and affective organisational commitment will be

positively associated (Path 13).

3.1.4. Overview of the Current Study

Research has shown that teachers’ perceptions of principal autonomy support predict

need satisfaction (Klassen, Perry, et al., 2012), which in turn, predicts emotional exhaustion

(negatively) and engagement (positively; Klassen, Perry, et al., 2012), as well as self-

determined motivation (Taylor, et al., 2008). Research has also suggested that the different

97

Page 110: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

types of regulation proposed by SDT are appropriate for examining teacher motivation (Roth

et al., 2007). There remain, however, several aspects of SDT untested among teachers

including the relationships that need satisfaction has with well-being defined as flourishing

and the different types of regulation (e.g., identified, introjected). Additionally, relationships

that well-being (defined as flourishing) and autonomous/controlled motivation have with job

satisfaction and affective organisational commitment have been largely untested among

teachers. In order to address these gaps, Study 2 involved hypothesising and testing the

explanatory model of teacher well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and affective

organisational commitment shown in Figure 3.1, and more specifically, the hypotheses

detailed above. Figure 3.2 shows a summary of the hypotheses tested in the current study.

Figure 3.2. Summary of the hypotheses tested with predicted relationship polarity.

98

Page 111: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.2. Methods

Data for this study were drawn from the same data that were collected for Study 1.

Specifically, data were examined from the Teacher Well-Being Scale (TWBS) developed in

Study 1, as well as from the more established flourishing and job satisfaction measures used

in Study 1. Additional measures included in Study 2 are described below. These measures

were included to examine relationships theorised in the explanatory model.

3.2.1. Participants and Procedures

The participants and procedures for data collection in Study 2 are identical to Study

1. To recap, participants included 603 teachers recruited through four different district-level

teachers’ associations in British Columbia, Canada. The sample statistics were identical to

Study 1. In an attempt to further extend our understanding of well-being among teachers, two

types of well-being were examined in Study 2. The first type, teacher well-being, was used to

measure teachers’ work-related well-being. The second type, a measure of flourishing was

used to assess global (i.e., general life) well-being of the participants. Together, these two

types of well-being provide an idea of how work-related and global well-being function in

relation to the other variables under examination in the model.

3.2.2. Measures

Participants completed an online, self-report questionnaire that included measures of

perceived autonomy support, need satisfaction, flourishing, teacher well-being, motivation at

work, job satisfaction, and organisational commitment. Table 3.1 shows the descriptive

statistics and Cronbach’s alphas for the measures used in the current study. As introduced in

Study 1, Table 2.5 shows the constructs (and item location in Appendices) that were

examined in Study 2, along with those examined in the other two studies.

99

Page 112: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.2.2.1. Perceived autonomy support. An adapted version of the Work Climate

Questionnaire (WCQ; Baard et al., 2004) was used to measure perceived autonomy support

from principals (see Appendix B, Section 2, Questions 1 through 6). The original 6-item

WCQ contains statements about the employee’s manager (e.g., “I feel understood by my

manager”) and the participants are asked to respond on a scale ranging from Strongly

Disagree (1) to Strongly Agree (7). In the adapted version, Klassen, Perry, and colleagues

(2012) replaced the word manager with principal and found high internal consistency for the

adapted measure (α = .95). Using this adapted measure, the Cronbach’s alpha in the current

study was .96.

3.2.2.2. Need satisfaction. The Work-related Basic Need Satisfaction scale (Van den

Broeck, Vansteenkiste, De Witte, Soenens, & Lens, 2010) was used to measure teachers’

sense of need satisfaction for autonomy, competence, and relatedness with colleagues (see

Appendix B, Section 3, Questions 1 through 18). The scale consists of 18 statements (e.g., “I

really master my tasks at my job”) and participants respond to these on a scale ranging from

Strongly Disagree (1) to Strongly Agree (7). The measure has been used with a large sample

of Dutch-speaking workers in the Netherlands (Van den Broeck et al., 2010), as well as with

a large sample of Canadian teachers (Klassen, Perry, et al., 2012). In both examples, support

was found for the factor structure, validity, and reliability of the scores from the measure

(Cronbach’s alphas in the two studies ranged from .77 to .85 for the three subscales). In the

current study, Cronbach’s alphas ranged from .84 to .88 (see Table 3.1).

In educational settings, teachers not only have relationships with colleagues, but also

students. Four additional items were included, therefore, to measure relatedness with students

(e.g., “I am very committed to my students”; see Appendix B, Section 3, Questions 19

100

Page 113: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

through 22). Klassen, Perry, et al. (2012) adapted these items from Deci and colleagues’

(2001) relatedness scale and they were measured on the same 7-point scale as the other need

satisfaction items. High internal consistency was found for the items in Klassen, Perry, et

al.’s study (α = .80), as well as in the current study (α = .88).

3.2.2.3. Well-being and job satisfaction. Two measures were used to measure well-

being and one was used to measure job satisfaction. These three measures—the Flourishing

Scale (Diener et al., 2010), the TWBS (developed in Study 1), and the job satisfaction items

Caprara and colleagues (2003) developed—were described in Study 1. The Flourishing Scale

was used to measure global well-being and the TWBS was used to measure work-related

well-being.

3.2.2.4. Motivation. The Motivation at Work Scale (Gagné et al., 2010) was used to

assess teachers’ levels of autonomous and controlled motivation (see Appendix B, Section 4,

Questions 1 through 12). Items in this measure provide 12 reasons for doing a certain job

(e.g., “Because this job fulfils my career plans”) and participants indicate the extent to which

each reason is accurate for them on a scale ranging from Not At All (1) to Exactly (7). The

measure contains four subscales that refer to different types of behaviour regulation: intrinsic

regulation, identified regulation, introjected regulation, and external regulation. The scale

does not include integrated regulation because it is psychometrically difficult to differentiate

from intrinsic motivation (Gagné et al., 2010). Autonomous motivation, therefore, is

represented by intrinsic and identified regulation and controlled motivation is represented by

introjected and external regulation. Gagné and colleagues (2010) found that this measure had

adequate reliability (with Cronbach’s alphas between .69 and .89) and a good factor

structure. Evidence of validity was also provided through comparison with hypothesised

101

Page 114: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

antecedents and outcomes, and through a simplex pattern (Gagné et al., 2010). In the current

study, the factors had Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .73 to .91 (see Table 3.1).

3.2.2.5. Affective organisational commitment. An adapted version of the

Vandenberghe and Bentein (2009) six-item scale was used to measure affective

organisational commitment (see Appendix B, Section 2, Questions 15 through 20). I adapted

the original scale by replacing the word organisation with school or school community. This

measure consists of statements about the organisation (e.g., “I am proud to belong to this

school community”) and participants respond on a scale ranging from Strongly Disagree (1)

to Strongly Agree (7). Previous studies have found adequate reliability for the scale

(Cronbach’s alphas between .69 and .78; Vandenberghe et al., 2007; Vandenberghe &

Bentein, 2009) and good factor structure (Vandenberghe et al., 2007). The Cronbach’s alpha

for the current study with the adapted items was .86.

3.2.2.6. Demographic information. The same demographic information was

collected as for Study 1 (see Appendix B, Section 1, Questions 1 through 14).

102

Page 115: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 3.1

Reliability Indexes, Means, and Standard Deviations of All Variables

Range

Variable α M SD Potential Observed

Perceived autonomy support

.96 5.05 1.401 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Relatedness with colleagues

.85 4.33 1.276 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Relatedness with students

.88 6.54 0.602 1.00 – 7.00 2.25 – 7.00

Competence .86 5.73 0.979 1.00 – 7.00 1.17 – 7.00

Autonomy .84 4.65 1.203 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Flourishing .83 6.54 0.602 1.00 – 7.00 2.25 – 7.00

Workload well-being .85 3.43 1.055 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Organisational well-being

.82 4.38 1.065 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Student interaction well-being

.84 4.33 1.276 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Intrinsic regulation .91 5.52 1.112 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Identified regulation .84 5.13 1.156 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Introjected regulation .83 2.73 1.434 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

External regulation .73 2.74 1.189 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Job satisfaction .90 5.36 0.881 1.00 – 7.00 1.00 – 7.00

Organisational commitment

.86 5.55 1.276 1.00 – 7.00 1.33 – 7.00

103

Page 116: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.3. Results

3.3.1. Data Analyses

The factor structure of the constructs was examined using exploratory and

confirmatory factor analyses. Following this, structural equation models (SEM) were used to

simultaneously examine the relationships among all factors. As described in Study 1, factor

analysis involves exploring and then confirming the factor structure in the data. This

provides the opportunity to test through EFA and confirm through CFA whether the items

are associated with the expected constructs. Furthermore, it is an important aspect of

determining internal structure and is a necessary step before conducting SEM to ensure that

the structural model is based on a sound measurement model or CFA (Kline, 2011).

SEM allows researchers to examine relationships among multiple predictor and

outcome variables. In that sense, it examines paths between variables like regression

analysis, but instead of using observed variables, it involves the factors or latent variables

determined through the CFA measurement model (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). SEM is an

appropriate tool for research that is based on a priori specifications of relationships among

variables and their directionalities (Kline, 2011). In other words, the researcher determines

the structure of the model and the direction of each relationship a priori using existing

research and theory (Martin, 2007). Once the model is specified, SEM is used to determine

whether the solution is well-defined and fits the data, as well as whether the a priori

predictions are supported (Kline, 2011; Martin, 2007). Although directional relationships are

included in the integrated model, the cross-sectional design of the current study does not

allow for causal claims to be made about the relationships. In other words, the findings

indicate the plausibility of the directional relationships in the explanatory model (i.e., using

104

Page 117: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

SEM we test the hypothesised causal model using sample data; Kline, 2011). Conclusive

claims about directionality, however, cannot be made as they require

longitudinal/experimental examination. This is particularly true in the case of the paths

where previous longitudinal research does not exist.

Similar to factor analysis, it is important to consider the hierarchical nature of the

data before conducting SEM. As in Study 1, the data examined in the current study were

hierarchical in nature (i.e., teachers were nested in schools). Accordingly, the same

procedures used in Study 1 were employed in Study 2 to address this. Namely, robust

(sandwich estimator) standard errors and a robust chi-square test were used (Asparouhov &

Muthén, 2005; Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2012). In addition, the “TYPE=COMPLEX” option

was used with the cluster variable set as participants’ school of employment. All analyses

were conducted using Mplus Version 7.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2012). The same fit

indices and guidelines stipulated in Study 1 were used in the current study and are

summarised in Table 3.2. In addition, the chi-square model fit test is reported. Missing data

for this study ranged from 0-6% for all the variables. Typically, cases with missing data are

excluded from analyses, which can lead to reduced power and loss of important information

(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). In order to avoid this, multiple imputation was conducted for

missing values on the predictor variables using Bayesian estimation through Mplus. For the

outcome variables, methods robust against missing data were utilised for data analysis.

105

Page 118: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 3.2

Guidelines for Fit Indices

Fit index Evidence of adequate fit Evidence of good fit

RMSEA < .10 < .06

CFI > .90 > .95

SRMR < .10 <. 08

3.3.2. Factor Analyses

Data from the measures were subjected to several exploratory factor analyses (EFAs)

on a randomly selected half of the dataset. The EFAs were performed with the robust

weighted least squares (WLSMV) estimator and geomin oblique rotation. This method of

estimation is appropriate for categorical data and is robust to missing and non-normal data.

For the EFAs, the items all loaded well. Given the highly related nature of the constructs,

however, cross-loading was an issue with some items. The decision was made to exclude

these items. In addition, all the items associated with two constructs obtained high cross-

loadings. The two constructs were intrinsic regulation and job satisfaction. This is

understandable given that intrinsic regulation is driven by enjoyment or interest in the

behaviour. In other words, if teachers’ motivation is intrinsically regulated, they teach

because it is enjoyable, and by logical extension it is highly likely they would find it

satisfying. Although the relationship between the constructs is understandable, it is

problematic psychometrically. It suggests that these two constructs cannot be differentiated

106

Page 119: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

by the measures used in the current study. By including both of them in the statistical

analyses, it would increase the threat of multicollinearity and reduce the robustness of

results. Given this, the intrinsic regulation items were removed from analyses; however,

autonomous motivation was still represented by the items relating to identified regulation

that loaded well and did not cross-load.

For the final EFA, the intrinsic regulation items were excluded along with any other

items that cross-loaded within .100 leaving between three and six items per construct. The

model showed good fit: χ2 (1021, N = 241) = 1213.095, p < .001, RMSEA = .028, CFI

= .989, and SRMR = .023. Table 3.3 shows the items that were included in the final model

for each construct and their factor loadings.

Following the EFAs, a CFA was run on the other half of the dataset to confirm the

factor structure. Once again, the robust WLSMV estimator was used. Given the highly

related nature of the constructs involved in the model, the decision was made to combine the

teacher well-being subscales into a higher-order factor to reduce the possibility of

multicollinearity, which is problematic in statistical analyses as it can inflate the error terms,

thus reducing the power of the analyses (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Multicollinearity

occurs when there is high correlation among factors. Using this second-order factor for

teacher well-being, the fit indices revealed good fit of the confirmatory measurement model:

χ2 (1641, N = 244) = 2377.067, p < .001, RMSEA = .043, and CFI = .959 (SRMR was not

available for this estimation method). Table 3.3 shows the standardised factor loadings for

the items from the CFA. The latent variable correlations among the factors using the whole

dataset are shown in Table 3.4 and provide evidence that the factors are distinct constructs.

107

Page 120: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 3.3

Factor Loadings from the Factor Analyses

Items Rotated factor

loadings from EFA

Standardised loadings

from CFA

Perceived autonomy support

My principal listens to how I would like to do things .897 .951

I feel that my principal provides me with choices and options .930 .944

I feel understood by my principal .960 .962

My principal conveys confidence in my ability to do well at my job .877 .864

My principal encourages me to ask questions .909 .883

My principal tries to understand how I see things before suggesting a new way of doing things

.905 .903

Relatedness with colleagues

I don’t really feel connected with other people at work (R) .855 .841

I don’t really mix with colleagues at work (R) .806 .845

At work, I can talk with people about things that really matter to me .722 .678

I often feel alone when I am with my colleagues (R) .899 .793

Relatedness with students

I am very committed to my students .828 .863

Connecting with my students is an essential part of the job .934 .908

I value the relationships I build with my students .921 .960

I feel connected to my students .906 .889

108

Page 121: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 3.3 (Continued)

Items Rotated factor

loadings from EFA

Standardised loadings

from CFA

Competence

I don’t really feel competent in my job (R) .871 .844

I really master the things I have to do at work .827 .815

I feel competent at my job .924 .905

I have doubts about whether I am able to do my job properly (R) .740 .700

I am good at the things I do in my job .887 .924

I have the feeling that I can accomplish even the most difficult tasks at work

.724 .819

Autonomy

At work, I often feel like I have to follow other people’s commands (R)

.679 .636

If I could choose, I would do things at work differently (R) .782 .675

The things I have to do at work are in line with what I really want to do

.778 .876

I feel free to do my job the way I think it should be done .859 .876

In my job, I feel forced to do things I don’t want to do (R) .819 .629

Flourishing

I lead a purposeful and meaningful life .924 .833

My social relationships are supportive and rewarding .767 .748

I am engaged and interested in my daily activities .814 .849

I actively contribute to the happiness and well-being of others .685 .796

I am a good person and live a good life .755 .717

I am optimistic about my future .571 .707

109

Page 122: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 3.3 (Continued)

Items Rotated factor

loadings from EFA

Standardised loadings

from CFA

Workload well-being

Marking work .671 .694

Fitting everything in to the allotted time .654 .638

Administrative work related to teaching .708 .692

Work I complete outside of school hours for teaching .814 .686

Working to finish my teaching tasks .703 .827

Staying late after work for meetings and activities .691 .732

Organisational well-being

Support offered by school leadership .675 .828

Recognition for my teaching .522 .680

School rules and procedures that are in place .770 .694

Communication between members of the school .734 .683

Participation in school-level decision making .730 .678

Student interaction well-being

Student behaviour .948 .921

Student motivation .739 .886

Classroom management .633 .782

Identified regulation

I chose this job because it allows me to reach my life goals .952 .885

Because this job fulfils my career plans .852 .862

Because this job fits my personal values .678 .778

110

Page 123: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 3.3 (Continued)

Items Rotated factor

loadings from EFA

Standardised loadings

from CFA

Introjected regulation

Because I have to be the best in my job, I have to be a “winner” .784 .743

Because my work is my life and I don’t want to fail .915 .813

Because my reputation depends on it .761 .887

External regulation

Because this job affords me a certain standard of living .848 .662

Because it allows me to make a lot of money .818 .722

I do this job for the paycheque .611 .935

Job satisfaction

I am satisfied with my job .822 .916

I am satisfied with what I achieve at work .880 .887

I feel good at work .848 .909

Affective organisational commitment

This school has a great deal of personal meaning for me .865 .894

I am proud to belong to this school community .876 .922

I do not feel emotionally attached to my school (R) .829 .732

Note. (R) refers to reverse coded items.

111

Page 124: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 3.4

Correlations of Latent Variables from the Confirmatory Factor Analysis

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14.

1. PAS

2. Rel. with colleagues .381

3. Rel. with students .183 .283

4. Competence .146 .253 .432

5. Autonomy .546 .476 .274 .418

6. Flourishing .312 .460 .471 .644 .504

7. Workload WB .321 .250 .148 .188 .360 .275

8. Organisational WB .494 .384 .228 .289 .554 .423 .484

9. Student interaction WB .412 .321 .191 .241 .463 .353 .404 .622

10. Teacher well-being .572 .445 .264 .335 .642 .490 .561 .863 .720

11. Identified regulation .188 .318 .458 .386 .497 .521 .242 .373 .311 .432

12. Introjected regulation -.070 -.091 .051 -.059 -.095 -.085 -.045 -.069 -.057 -.079 .192

13. External regulation -.009 -.113 -.371 -.139 -.081 -.205 -.005 -.007 -.006 -.008 -.057 .372

14. Job satisfaction .350 .440 .420 .636 .637 .648 .359 .552 .461 .640 .595 -.038 -.174

15. Aff. org. commitment .426 .653 .494 .366 .457 .556 .272 .419 .349 .485 .473 .113 -.184 .539

Note. Correlations with an absolute value equal to or greater than r = .90 are significant at p < .05, those with an absolute value equal to or greater than r = .117 are significant at p < .01, and those with an absolute value equal to or greater than r = .150 are significant at p = .001. All other correlations are not significant. PAS = perceived autonomy support. Rel. = relatedness. WB = well-being. Aff. org. commitment = affective organisational commitment.

112

Page 125: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.3.3. Structural Modelling

Following the factor analyses, the relationships between the variables in Figure 3.1

were analysed with SEM on the full dataset in order investigate whether they related to one

another as predicted. A model was specified in Mplus 7.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2012)

that corresponded with paths elucidated in the explanatory model. The constructs specified in

this model referred to the items that were confirmed through the factor analyses (i.e., the

measurement model provided by the CFA was used). By running the SEM, coefficients were

estimated for the relationships specified in the explanatory model. When a parameter

estimate was significant, it indicated that there was a reliable relationship between the two

constructs as expected. In contrast, non-significant parameter estimates indicated that there

was no significant path between those variables in the model. Non-significant paths, starting

with the lowest standardised coefficients, were deleted for reasons of parsimony one at a

time until only significant paths remained in the model. This provided the final model, which

was then examined for goodness of fit. The fit indices suggested adequate to good fit of the

final model: χ2 (1680, N = 485) = 4036.828, p < .001, RMSEA = .054, and CFI = .933

(SRMR was not available for this estimation method).

Figure 3.3 shows the final model. Given that non-significant paths were removed

from the model, all of the path coefficients are statistically significant at p < .05.

Standardised direct, total indirect, and total effects are shown in Table 3.5. These effects

indicate the parameter estimates that occur from one variable directly to another (direct

effects), the parameter estimates between two variables when they are mediated by one or

more other variables (indirect effects), and the total strength of the relationship considering

both direct and indirect paths (total effects).

113

Page 126: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Figure 3.3. Structural equation model of perceived autonomy support, need satisfaction, well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment. Only significant paths are shown. All coefficients are significant (p < .05). Standardised coefficients are reported. Significant correlations also examined but not shown in the figure included identified regulation and flourishing (r = .48, p < .001), identified regulation and teacher well-being (r = .39, p < .001), external regulation and flourishing (r = -.196, p < .001), and job satisfaction and affective organisational commitment (r = .54, p < .001).

114

Page 127: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 3.5

Standardised Effects for the Predictor Variables on Each Outcome Variable

Direct Total indirect Total

Predictor: Perceived autonomy support

Relatedness with colleagues .508 — .508

Relatedness with students .313 — .313

Competence .284 — .284

Autonomy .629 — .629

Flourishing — .473 .473

Teacher well-being — .581 .581

Identified regulation — .384 .384

External regulation — -.111 -.111

Job satisfaction — .513 .513

Organisational commitment — .464 .464

Predictor: Relatedness with colleagues

Flourishing .224 — .224

Teacher well-being .330 — .330

External regulation -.204 — -.204

Job satisfaction — .209 .209

Organisational commitment — .270 .270

115

Page 128: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 3.5 (Continued)

Direct Total indirect Total

Predictor: Relatedness with students

Flourishing .336 — .336

Identified regulation .322 — .322

External regulation -.417 — -.417

Job satisfaction — .239 .239

Organisational commitment — .259 .259

Predictor: Competence

Flourishing .574 — .574

Teacher well-being .221 — .221

Identified regulation .331 — .331

Job satisfaction — .419 .419

Organisational commitment — .297 .297

Predictor: Autonomy

Flourishing .145 — .145

Teacher well-being .557 — .557

Identified regulation .300 — .300

External regulation .195 — .195

Job satisfaction — .339 .339

Organisational commitment — .257 .257

116

Page 129: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.3.3.1. Basic psychological needs. The first findings refer to the relationships

between perceived principal’s autonomy support and teachers’ sense of autonomy,

competence, and relatedness—their basic psychological needs. It was hypothesised that

perceived autonomy support would positively predict need satisfaction (Hypothesis 1). The

findings revealed that participants’ perceptions of principals’ autonomy support were reliably

related to their sense of need satisfaction of relatedness with colleagues (β = .508, p < .001),

relatedness with students (β = .313, p < .001), competence (β = .284, p < .001), and

autonomy (β = .629, p < .001). All relationships were positive revealing that as perceptions

of principals’ autonomy support increased, so did teachers’ need satisfaction. These findings

provide support for Hypothesis 1. In terms of the effect sizes, the model explained different

amounts of variance in the basic psychological needs. It explained the most variance in

autonomy (39.6%), followed by relatedness with colleagues (25.8%), relatedness with

students (9.8%), and competence (8.1%).

3.3.3.2. Well-being. The second set of findings refers to the relationship between the

basic psychological needs and the two types of well-being. It was hypothesised that need

satisfaction would positively predict flourishing (i.e., the global measure of well-being) and

teacher well-being (i.e., work-related well-being; Hypothesis 2). The findings revealed that

participants’ need satisfaction of relatedness with colleagues (β = .224, p < .001), relatedness

with students (β = .336, p < .001), competence (β = .574, p < .001), and autonomy (β = .145,

p < .001) were reliably related to their reports of flourishing. In addition, need satisfaction of

relatedness with colleagues (β = .330, p < .001), competence (β = .221, p < .001), and

autonomy (β = .557, p < .001) were reliably related to reports of teacher well-being. These

findings indicate that as teachers’ need satisfaction increased, so did their reports of

117

Page 130: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

flourishing and teacher well-being (providing support for Hypothesis 2). The model

explained a large amount of the variance in well-being—67.8% of the variance in flourishing

and 65.0% of the variance in teacher well-being—suggesting quite a large effect size for both

types of well-being.

3.3.3.3. Motivation. The relationships between the basic psychological needs and

the different types of motivation are reported next. It was hypothesised that need satisfaction

would positively predict autonomous motivation (Hypothesis 3). As described above,

autonomous motivation was measured with one type of regulation, identified regulation.

Participants’ need satisfaction of relatedness with students (β = .322, p < .001), competence

(β = .331, p < .001), and autonomy (β = .300, p < .001) were reliably related to their reports

of identified regulation. This finding indicates that as teachers’ perceptions of relatedness

with students, competence, and autonomy increased, so did their reports of identified

regulation (providing support for Hypothesis 3).

It was also hypothesised that need satisfaction would negatively predict controlled

motivation (Hypothesis 4). For controlled motivation, two types of regulation were

examined: external and introjected regulation. The findings showed that relatedness with

colleagues (β = -.204, p < .001), relatedness with students (β = -.417, p < .001), and

autonomy (β = .195, p < .001) were reliably related to teachers’ reports of external

regulation, yet they had different polarities. Teachers who perceived need satisfaction with

respect to relatedness with colleagues and students, reported reduced external motivation (as

per Hypothesis 4). Contrary to expectations, however, teachers who perceived need

satisfaction for autonomy, reported higher levels of external regulation. In addition, none of

the basic psychological needs were reliably related to introjected regulation. Turning to the

118

Page 131: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

effect sizes, the model explained 39.6% of the variance in identified regulation and 22.3% of

the variance in external regulation. It did not explain any of the variance in introjected

regulation because that only acted as an exogenous variable in the model (i.e., it did not act

as an outcome variable).

3.3.3.4. Job satisfaction. The next findings concern the relationships that well-being

and motivation had with job satisfaction. It was hypothesised that flourishing and teacher

well-being would positively predict job satisfaction (Hypothesis 7), as would autonomous

motivation (Hypothesis 9). The findings revealed that participants’ reports of flourishing (β =

.433, p < .001), teacher well-being (β = .339, p < .001), and identified regulation (i.e., the

measure of autonomous motivation; β = .291, p < .001) were reliably and positively related

to their reports of job satisfaction. As reports of flourishing, teacher well-being, and

identified regulation increased, so did reports of job satisfaction, providing support for

Hypothesis 7 and 9.

It was also hypothesised that controlled motivation would negatively predict job

satisfaction (Hypothesis 10). However, controlled motivation, measured as external and

introjected regulation, did not predict lower job satisfaction. In fact, external and introjected

regulation were not significantly related to job satisfaction at all. In terms of the effect size,

the model explained 73.0% of the variance in job satisfaction.

3.3.3.5. Affective organisational commitment. The relationships that well-being

and motivation had with affective organisational commitment are reported next. It was

hypothesised that flourishing and teacher well-being (Hypothesis 8), along with autonomous

motivation (i.e., identified regulation; Hypothesis 11), would positively predict affective

organisational commitment. The findings revealed that participants’ sense of flourishing (β =

119

Page 132: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

.285, p < .001), teacher well-being (β = .435, p < .001), and identified regulation (β = .112, p

< .001) predicted their reports of affective organisational commitment. As teachers’ reports

of flourishing, teacher well-being, and identified regulation increased, so did their reports of

affective organisational commitment providing support for Hypotheses 8 and 11.

It was also hypothesised that controlled motivation (i.e., introjected and external

regulation) would negatively predict affective organisational commitment (Hypothesis 12).

The findings showed that introjected regulation (β = .147, p < .001) and external regulation

(β = -.306, p < .001) predicted teachers’ reports of affective organisational commitment,

albeit in different ways. External regulation was negatively associated with affective

organisational commitment as hypothesised; however, contrary to expectations introjected

regulation was positively associated. This means that participants who reported teaching with

external regulation experienced lower affective organisational commitment, whereas

participants who reported teaching with introjected regulation experienced greater affective

organisational commitment. Together, the findings provided only partial support for

Hypothesis 12. In terms of the effect size, the model explained a large amount of variance in

affective organisational commitment (63.4%).

3.3.3.6. Correlational hypotheses. In addition to the directional paths shown in

Figure 3.3, there were three additional hypotheses concerning correlational relationships

(Hypotheses 5, 6, and 13). These were not shown in Figure 3.3 given its existing complexity

with the directional paths. As described earlier, Hypothesis 5 proposed that autonomous

motivation would be positively associated with flourishing and teacher well-being.

Consistent with these expectations, latent variable correlations estimated from the SEM

120

Page 133: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

revealed that autonomous motivation (measured as identified regulation) was positively

associated with flourishing (r = .48, p < .001) and teacher well-being (r = .39, p < .001).

The next correlational hypothesis, however, was not supported. Hypothesis 6

indicated that controlled motivation (i.e., introjected and external regulation) would have a

weak, positive relationship with well-being. External regulation, however, was negatively

related with flourishing (r = -.196, p < .001) and not significantly related with teacher well-

being (r = -.049, ns). Furthermore, introjected regulation, was not significantly related to

flourishing (r = -.041, ns) or teacher well-being (r = -.050, ns). The final hypothesis was that

job satisfaction and affective organisational commitment would be positively related

(Hypothesis 13). The findings were consistent with this hypothesis: Job satisfaction was

positively associated with affective organisational commitment (r = .54, p < .001).

3.4. Discussion

Improving teachers’ experiences at work is a crucial component of efforts to promote

a whole host of positive outcomes in schools. Teachers’ who are faring well, feeling

motivated to teach, experiencing satisfaction with their work, and feeling committed to their

school of employment tend to be more effective teachers (e.g., Duckworth et al., 2009) and

better at promoting students’ motivation (e.g., Pakarinen et al., 2010) and achievement (e.g.,

Caprara et al., 2006). However, we still have a great deal to learn about how teachers’

experiences of well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and affective organisational

commitment interact with one another simultaneously. Furthermore, we do not have

explanatory models to enable us to study these important variables concurrently. Study 2

aimed to address these gaps. Grounded in self-determination theory (SDT), an explanatory

model of relationships between teachers’ experiences of well-being, motivation, job

121

Page 134: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment was elaborated and tested. After

confirming the factor pattern, SEM was conducted and provided support for the explanatory

model. Not all specified paths were consistent, however, with expectations. Key findings in

relation to the hypotheses are discussed below.

3.4.1. Basic Psychological Needs, Well-being, and Motivation

The findings revealed that the relationships in the model that were taken from SDT

were generally supported. Namely, teachers’ perceptions of principal’s autonomy support

predicted satisfaction of the needs for relatedness with colleagues and students, competence,

and autonomy (Hypothesis 1). Furthermore, need satisfaction positively predicted well-being

and identified regulation (i.e., autonomous motivation), but negatively predicted external

regulation (i.e., controlled motivation; Hypotheses 2, 3, and 5). The relationship found

between perceived autonomy support and need satisfaction corroborates Klassen, Perry, et

al.’s (2012) research. For the relationships involving need satisfaction, well-being, and

motivation, however, the findings extend the literature given that these relationships have not

been examined previously among teachers.

In combination, these findings extend our understanding of teachers’ experiences at

work and how they interrelate. This is particularly important given that well-being and

motivation have not only been linked with important teacher outcomes, such as teacher

effectiveness (e.g., Duckworth et al., 2009), but also student achievement (e.g., Caprara et

al., 2006). The findings also confirm the relevance of SDT for guiding research among

teachers. Previous research has provided support for the use of SDT in examining well-being

and motivation among students (e.g., Filak & Sheldon, 2008) and employees in other work-

related settings (e.g., Deci et al., 2001). The current study corroborates and extends the

122

Page 135: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

emerging literature on teachers by showing that SDT is relevant for examining well-being,

autonomous motivation, and controlled motivation among teachers.

The implications of these findings are relevant for school administrators and policy-

makers for several reasons. In particular, they highlight the importance of principals’

autonomy support—or more accurately, teachers’ perceptions of this. By ensuring that

principals promote an autonomy-supportive working environment, teachers are more likely

to perceive this to be the case. In turn, teachers will likely experience greater need

satisfaction, well-being, and motivation. In order to extend our understanding, future

research should examine whether and how teachers’ perceptions of autonomy support

compare with actual efforts by principals to be autonomy-supportive. In addition, although

research has shown the types of managerial behaviours that employees in factory settings

find autonomy-supportive (e.g., Deci et al., 1994), no research has examined this among

teachers. This type of research would be helpful for guiding efforts to improve teachers’

perceived autonomy support and the many important outcomes related with it (e.g., teacher

motivation).

3.4.1.1. Autonomy and external regulation. Although support was found for the

main premises of SDT, one finding that was contrary to expectations was the positive

relationship between the need for autonomy and external regulation. I hypothesised that there

would be a negative relationship between these two variables (Hypothesis 4). The negative

latent variable correlation from the CFA supported my original hypothesis. The relationship

between these two variables became positive, however, in the structural modelling,

suggesting that there may be suppression effects occurring due to unobserved variables.

123

Page 136: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

One possible reason for the changing sign is that high autonomy may have been

reported by two different groups of teachers. According to Pearson and Moomaw (2005),

autonomy can be viewed differently by teachers. Where one teacher may view autonomy as

the freedom to adjust his or her teaching, another teacher may “view autonomy as a means to

gain substantial freedom from interference or supervision” (Pearson & Moomaw, 2005, p.

42). In this sense, perhaps one group of teachers reported high autonomy with the

understanding that it allows them greater freedom to adjust their teaching methods and the

content to best meet their students’ needs. In contrast, perhaps a second group reported high

autonomy because it means there is less monitoring of their teaching by supervisors and, as a

result, less pressure to put additional effort into their teaching (e.g., in the case of teachers

who are experiencing disengagement due to burnout; Demerouti, Bakker, Nachreiner, &

Schaufeli, 2001).

Although the data do not allow us to determine if this was the case, this interpretation

is reflected in the model by the fact that autonomy was positively associated with both

identified regulation and external regulation. If these two groups do exist, teachers from the

first group may have reported high identified regulation, whereas teachers from the second

group may have reported high external regulation. This interpretation is also supported by

Gagné et al. (2010), who found that need satisfaction of autonomy was positively associated

with identified and external regulation (along with other types too). Their results suggest that

while autonomy predicts autonomous motivation, it is also related—psychometrically at

least—to controlled motivation. Given all this, it is conceivable that the swinging beta was

caused by this double-sided view of autonomy. Unfortunately, we are not able to determine if

this is the case from the current data, but future research should examine this relationship in

124

Page 137: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

greater depth to understand why satisfaction of the need for autonomy predicts both external

and identified regulation.

3.4.1.2. Introjected regulation. Another finding that was contrary to expectations

was that need satisfaction did not predict introjected regulation (Hypothesis 4). Perhaps for

teachers who perceive they need to avoid shame/failure or who rely on contingent self-

worth—beliefs associated with introjected regulation (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 2002)—need

satisfaction is not a primary concern. In other words, for these individuals, relatedness with

colleagues and students, competence, and autonomy may be secondary to their need to meet

the goals of introjected regulation (e.g., avoid failure or be seen positively in the eyes of

others). In a related finding, reports of introjected regulation were not reliably related to

flourishing or teacher well-being, which was contrary to my expectations of a weak, but

positive correlation between these constructs (Hypothesis 6). One possible explanation for

this is that perhaps introjected regulation for teaching has a differential relationship with

well-being that depends on whether the goals of introjected regulation are met. For example,

if a teacher works with the aim of avoiding failure or shame, perhaps he or she experiences a

sense of well-being when this goal is met, but not otherwise.

Taken together, these findings raise concerns about how teachers come to adopt

introjected regulation for teaching, and perhaps more importantly, how we can help teachers

who are acting with introjected regulation for teaching move into more autonomous forms of

motivation. Research that examines the characteristics of teachers who teach with introjected

regulation and investigates whether there are other needs that drive their motivation (e.g.,

perfectionism, fear of failure) will help to advance our knowledge of this type of regulation.

At the same time, introjected regulation represents the ‘first step’ in the process of

125

Page 138: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

internalisation of behaviour (Gillison, Osborn, Standage, & Skevington, 2009). That is, it

involves some degree of internalisation—although this relates to internal pressure and is not

self-determined, but rather self-controlled (Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, & Brière, 2001).

Nonetheless, it is not fully extrinsically motivated and so may act as a first step towards

greater internalisation (and more self-determined motivation) suggesting the need for efforts

to focus on how self-determination can be promoted among these teachers. Clearly, more

research is needed to provide a greater understanding of this type of regulation.

3.4.2. Job Satisfaction

Job satisfaction was the first of two variables in the model that move beyond the

theoretical framework of SDT. It was hypothesised that well-being and autonomous

motivation (i.e., identified regulation) would positively predict job satisfaction, whereas

controlled motivation (i.e., introjected and external regulation) would negatively predict job

satisfaction. The findings supported two of the hypotheses: teachers’ reports of flourishing,

teacher well-being, and identified regulation were positive predictors of job satisfaction

(Hypotheses 7 and 9). However, the third hypothesis involving controlled motivation and job

satisfaction (Hypothesis 10) was not supported. Contrary to expectations, experiences of

external regulation and introjected regulation did not predict job satisfaction.

One possible reason for this non-significance is that job satisfaction may be a

secondary concern for teachers who teach with introjected or external regulation. That is, the

goals associated with introjected regulation (e.g., avoiding failure) and external regulation

(e.g., obtaining a reward) may override a focus on job satisfaction. For example, if a teacher

is motivated to teach for the paycheque (i.e., external regulation), it may not be important

whether or not the job is satisfying in relation to the work tasks. This finding and the finding

126

Page 139: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

involving identified regulation are important as they provide insight into the differential

relationships that occur between the types of regulation and job satisfaction.

This result is important for practice. It suggests that school administrators, policy-

makers, and even educators themselves may be wise to look at work motivation alongside

job satisfaction. Efforts that focus solely on understanding and improving teacher job

satisfaction may fail to address the underlying issue of work motivation. If teachers’

underlying motivation is not considered simultaneously, these efforts may be ineffective

among teachers with controlled work motivation. Instead, by engaging in efforts to increase

teachers’ autonomous motivation through steps outlined in the model (i.e., through

principals’ autonomy support and teachers’ need satisfaction), schools will be in a better

position to address teacher job satisfaction while also improving teacher well-being,

motivation, and affective organisational commitment.

Building on this, the finding also has implications for research. Namely, it suggests

that rather than focusing on job satisfaction by itself, researchers may gain a more complete

picture by considering it alongside work motivation. Another important area of future

research is the use of profile or cluster analysis techniques to examine different groups of

teachers and how their experiences are explained by the explanatory model. This would

advance our understanding of how teachers come to teach with the different types of

regulation, how regulation influences other experiences like job satisfaction, and ultimately,

how we can help them become more self-determined.

127

Page 140: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.4.3. Affective Organisational Commitment

Affective organisational commitment was the second variable in the model that goes

beyond the premises of SDT. As hypothesised, experiences of flourishing, teacher well-

being, and identified regulation (autonomous motivation) had positive relationships with

affective organisational commitment (Hypotheses 8 and 11). In addition, job satisfaction and

affective organisational commitment were positively correlated as expected (Hypothesis 13).

The hypothesised relationship involving controlled motivation, however, was not fully

supported (Hypothesis 12). Consistent with expectations, external regulation was negatively

associated with affective organisational commitment. However, introjected regulation was

positively associated with affective organisational commitment.

For the first part of Hypothesis 12, the negative relationship between external

regulation and affective organisational commitment has important implications for school

administrators, policy-makers, students, and researchers. Given the positive outcomes

associated with affective organisational commitment (e.g., teachers’ sense of efficacy;

Bogler & Somech, 2004) and organisational commitment more broadly defined (e.g., work

engagement; Hakanen et al., 2006), it is important that teachers are given the best

opportunity to form affective organisational commitment. The importance of this is made

even more evident when one considers the fact that organisational commitment is negatively

associated with turnover and attrition among teachers (Day, Elliot, & Kington, 2005)—two

issues that have severe financial and educational implications for students, schools, and

societies (Collie et al., 2011; Ronfeldt, Loeb, & Wyckoff, 2013). The model described here

provides school administrators and policy-makers with methods through which to support

positive experiences of affective organisational commitment among teachers (e.g., by

128

Page 141: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

supporting teachers’ need satisfaction). Future research should examine the relationship

between motivation and affective organisational commitment in greater detail, along with

other forms of organisational commitment such as commitment driven by perceived costs of

quitting a job (i.e., continuance commitment; Meyer & Herscovitch, 2001) to see how they

relate to motivation.

For the second part of Hypothesis 12, introjected regulation was positively associated

with affective organisational commitment. Although this was unexpected, a plausible

explanation for why it occurred relates to the emotional dimensions of both introjected

regulation and affective organisational commitment. By looking at the items used to assess

introjected regulation, we can gain a better understanding of this. Specifically, if individuals

are teaching “because [their] work is [their] life and [they don’t] want to fail” or “because

[their] reputation depends on it” (Gagné et al., 2010), they are driven by ‘internal pressure’ to

avoid shame/guilt or gain contingent self-worth (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002). It is

conceivable, therefore, that they could report affective organisational commitment because it

is their job that provides an opportunity to meet these two different types of internal pressure

(e.g., if individuals teach because their reputation depends on it, they may feel emotionally

attached to their school given that it helps to ensure their reputation is kept intact). This

finding has important implications for researchers. Although it links introjected regulation to

a positive outcome (i.e., higher affective organisational commitment), earlier findings

showed that introjected regulation was not associated with job satisfaction or well-being, nor

was it predicted by need satisfaction. Taken together, these findings underscore the need for

research that examines introjected regulation among teachers more closely to understand

what types of teachers work with this regulation, along with its causes and correlates.

129

Page 142: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.5. Limitations and Future Directions

Study 2 has several limitations that must be discussed. Given that the same methods

of data collection were used as those described in Study 1, the limitations discussed in Study

1 are also relevant for Study 2. In particular, response rates were not able to be calculated

during data collection raising questions about the representativeness of the data; teachers

were involved in industrial action at the time of data collection, which may have affected

their responses; it was assumed that participants’ interpretations of questions matched the

researchers’ raising questions about the accuracy of responses; and the study involved a

single self-report survey meaning the results are threatened by single source bias.

As mentioned in Study 1, efforts were taken to temper these limitations (e.g.,

comparing data to other studies as an indication of representativeness, conducting factor

analysis, etc.)3. Notwithstanding this, a necessary development in future research involves

finding more robust ways to address these limitations, including finding ways to collect

response rates in online data collection (e.g., through personalised email invitations or face-

to-face recruiting), conducting ‘think-alouds’ with teachers as they complete questionnaires

to ascertain their thought processes and understand how they interpret the questions, and

using multiple data collection methods to reduce issues of single source bias (e.g.,

conducting interviews or focus groups with teachers).

In addition, there were two further limitations unique to this study. First, I was not

able to include intrinsic regulation in the model due to high correlation with job satisfaction.

Consequently, the results here do not extend to intrinsic regulation. Combined with the

3 One exception to this is that social desirability was not controlled for in Study 2. This decision was based on the fact that social desirability had only a small influence on the results of Study 1 (explaining variance less than 2%) and because there were concerns with the reliability of the social desirability data.

130

Page 143: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

psychometric issues associated with measuring integrated regulation, this raises questions

about how we can come to understand the most self-determined types of regulation among

teachers (i.e., integrated and intrinsic). Moving forward, there is a need to create more

sensitive measures that differentiate or consider the overlap between job satisfaction and the

inherent interest of intrinsic regulation among teachers. Further attention should also be

placed on integrated regulation to ascertain whether there are other methods for assessing

this type of regulation (e.g., observation, interviews). This is important for further testing of

the model, as well as for developing and improving our measurement of these constructs in

general.

The second limitation is that I was not able to test causal relationships. SEM is used

to test a hypothesised explanatory model using sample data (Kline, 2011). It cannot provide

proof of causality, but rather indicates whether the model is consistent with the data. In the

current study, the findings revealed that the model was consistent with the data suggesting

the plausibility of the directional relationships that were decided a priori. However, there is a

need to test these relationships directly using longitudinal and/or experimental designs to

provide support for the causality and to accept or rule out other plausible directions. A likely

first step for completing this type of examination would be to investigate smaller sections of

the explanatory model over time. For example, cross-lagged analysis involving a path that

does not have existing longitudinal support (e.g., the relationship between well-being and

affective organisational commitment) would be a fruitful first step for examining causality in

the model.

131

Page 144: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

3.6. Conclusions

In summary, Study 2 has involved elaborating and testing an explanatory model of

teacher motivation, well-being, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment

based in SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002). Models such as this are an important step forward

in the literature on teachers as they help to explain why teachers come to experience certain

constructs, how efforts can be designed to promote positive work experiences among

teachers, and ultimately, how we can help teachers to create the most effective learning

environments and experiences for their students. Through the use of factor analysis and

SEM, the findings revealed that the core relationships established through SDT were

generally supported: teachers’ reports of principal’s autonomy support predicted need

satisfaction, which, in turn, predicted well-being and motivation. These findings corroborate

the existing literature in several ways, while also extending it in relation to the examination

of well-being as flourishing and the different types regulation.

Also important were the findings that showed significant relationships involving

motivation, well-being, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment. In

particular, experiences of autonomous motivation (measured as identified regulation) and

well-being were positively associated with job satisfaction and affective organisational

commitment. There were some unexpected relationships, however, involving autonomy and

controlled motivation (measured as introjected regulation and external regulation), as well as

between controlled motivation, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment.

These findings extend our understanding of how teachers’ perceptions of motivation, well-

being, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment are interrelated and

132

Page 145: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

highlight the need for more research in this area that examines these outcomes

simultaneously.

Another area of research that requires greater attention is examination of teachers’

experiences of constructs like well-being and motivation over time. This is an important

future step for developing our understanding of the explanatory model tested here and will

provide evidence for understanding whether and how these variables have causal influence

on one another (e.g., whether well-being does cause affective organisational commitment).

Another advantage of research over multiple time points is that it can reveal understanding

about how these variables change over time and whether there are trends in, say, teacher

well-being and motivation over the course of a school term, year, or career in teaching. This

type of examination can provide much-needed understanding of how these constructs

develop among early career teachers and how these constructs continue to develop across the

career span. Furthermore, they are a needed addition in the literature to support a

comprehensive understanding of teacher well-being and motivation. The final study in this

dissertation, Study 3, aims to begin this process by conducting an examination over time.

That is, it involved examining teachers’ experiences of well-being and motivation over

multiple time points to ascertain whether and how they changed over several months and

whether there were discernible trends in these constructs over time.

133

Page 146: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

CHAPTER 4. STUDY 3: TEACHER WELL-BEING AND

MOTIVATION OVER TIME

4.1. Literature Review

The importance of teacher well-being and motivation for a whole host of positive

outcomes relating to teachers, students, and schools has been established in previous research

(e.g., Duckworth et al., 2009; Pakarinen et al., 2010; Retelsdorf et al., 2010) and further

supported in Studies 1 and 2 of this dissertation. In the past, research in this area has largely

involved cross-sectional examinations. Although these have advanced our understanding in

many ways, there is a need for examinations that are conducted over time. After all, there is

knowledge that cannot be ascertained by cross-sectional research such as understanding of

the causal ordering between constructs and understanding of the development and stability of

constructs over time.

Given our broadening understanding of teacher well-being and motivation through

cross-sectional research, a necessary next step in the development of the literature is to

examine these constructs over time. Indeed, this is a call that has been made frequently in the

literature on teachers (e.g., Brackett et al., 2010; Chan, 1998; Klassen et al., 2010, 2011;

Klusmann, et al., 2008; Skaalvik & Skaalvik, 2009) and in the more general well-being

literature (e.g., Diener, 2009). Only a handful of studies, however, have explored teacher

outcomes over time (e.g., Brouwers & Tomic, 2000; Burke & Greenglass, 1995). Clearly,

more research is needed to better understand how teacher outcomes change over time. This is

necessary for advancing our understanding of the causes of these constructs, along with a

need to understand whether and how they develop and change over time. This second point is

especially important given that teachers’ experiences vary regularly throughout a school year

134

Page 147: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

including changes in relationships with students, colleagues, and parents, changes in

requirements made by administrators and the government, and changes in students’ social,

emotional, and academic needs.

The aim of Study 3, therefore, was to examine whether and how teachers’

experiences of work-related well-being and motivation change over time among a new

sample of teachers. As part of the investigation, I also examined whether differences in well-

being and motivation among participants were predicted by their perceived satisfaction of the

basic psychological needs for relatedness, competence, and autonomy. The rationale for

examining this was to provide further support for the relationship between need satisfaction

and well-being as examined in Study 2, and to extend our understanding of self-efficacy for

teaching by analysing its relationship with need satisfaction.

4.1.1. Teachers’ Experiences Over Time

The examination of teacher outcomes over time has been recognised as a great need

in the literature (e.g., Klassen, Tze, Betts, & Gordon, 2011). In Study 3, teachers’

experiences of work-related well-being and motivation were examined over time. As

highlighted in Studies 1 and 2, the definition of well-being used in this dissertation is

flourishing (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2011). For teacher well-being, this refers to flourishing at

work. For the examination of motivation, the construct of self-efficacy for teaching was used

in Study 3. This construct stems from social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1982, 1997) and

involves a forward looking belief about one’s capabilities in specific contexts (Klassen et al.,

2011). For teachers, it refers to perceptions of their “capabilities to bring about desired

outcomes of student engagement and learning, even among those students who may be

difficult or unmotivated” (Tschannen-Moran & Woolfolk Hoy, 2001, p. 783). Self-efficacy

135

Page 148: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

for teaching is considered a key motivational belief that influences teachers’ actions. Like the

motivational constructs examined in Study 2 (i.e., self-determination), self-efficacy for

teaching has been associated with important outcomes for students, teachers, and schools

(e.g., students’ achievement, Caprara et al., 2006; teacher work engagement, Schaufeli &

Bakker, 2004; commitment to the profession, Canrinus et al., 2012; mastery goals for

teaching; Cho & Shim, 2013), thus justifying its inclusion in Study 3. In order to inform our

understanding of how teacher well-being and self-efficacy function over time, I turn to the

literature in these respective areas.

4.1.1.1. Changes in well-being over time. Although educational psychology

research has focused on well-being related constructs over time (e.g., stress), there is a

paucity of research looking at teacher well-being as its own construct over time.

Accordingly, we have to turn to research on stress and burnout to help inform our

understanding of how teacher well-being might function over time. Looking at changes in

stress and mental health over time, Manthei, Gilmore, Tuck, and Adair (1996) collected data

among New Zealand teachers at five waves over four years (at the beginning and end of the

school year in two consecutive years, and again two years later). Using instruments that

assessed sources of stress, overall stress, and experiences of symptoms related to mental

health (e.g., crying), ANOVA revealed only two significant differences between consecutive

time points. Two of the sources of stress—poor remuneration and community antagonism

(e.g., public criticism of teachers)—rose significantly from Time 1 to Time 2. Although this

method of comparing significant change between consecutive time points reveals whether

there were significant increases or decreases in teacher stress and mental health at the group

level, it does not allow us to determine if there were trends over time at the individual level.

136

Page 149: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Examination of trends would allow us to understand the continuing impact that the school

system has on teachers’ experiences.

In order to examine trends, multilevel modelling is needed. Multilevel modelling is a

data analysis technique that allows researchers to estimate individual trajectories of change in

a certain outcome variable along with the impact of predictor variables on changes to the

starting point and growth of those trajectories (Laursen & Hoff, 2006). Multilevel modelling

provides a more robust framework than repeated measures ANOVA or t-tests for examining

changes over time as it takes into account the hierarchical nature of data. This is necessary to

ensure interpretational and statistical errors do not occur. In addition, addressing the nested

nature of data prevents the loss of power that can occur when hierarchical data are not treated

as such (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Another strength of multilevel modelling is that it does

not require strict adherence to the assumption of independence of errors (Tabachnick &

Fidell, 2007) and is more flexible in terms of missing data, non-normality, and other data

complexities (Curran, Obeidat, & Losardo, 2010).

A notable example using multilevel modelling is Pas, Bradshaw, and Hershfeldt’s

(2012) study that examined changes in self-reported burnout and self-efficacy for handling

students with behavioural problems across two school years at three time points (six and

twelve months apart; findings involving self-efficacy are discussed in the next section). The

sample included practicing teachers in the US who were at various stages in their teaching

careers. Using emotional exhaustion—one of Maslach and Jackson’s (1981) three

components of burnout—they found a significant increase in teachers’ reports of burnout

over time. They also found that several other variables negatively predicted changes in both

the intercept and growth of burnout (e.g., teacher preparedness for handling classroom

137

Page 150: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

management; positive perceptions of school leadership). Taken together, the findings of this

study inform our understanding of burnout by suggesting that it tends to increase across

school years. A question that remains is whether it can change over shorter time frames.

In a recent study that examined this, Roeser et al. (2013) investigated the effects of

mindfulness training on teachers from Canada and the US. The training involved 8-weeks of

teaching activities on mindfulness and self-compassion (e.g., guided mindfulness and yoga).

Measures of self-reported burnout and stress were taken at the beginning of the program

(Time 1), immediately after the program finished (Time 2), and three months later (Time 3).

After controlling for baseline levels, the findings revealed that teachers who completed the

training reported significantly lower levels of stress and burnout than a control group at Time

2 and Time 3. The authors also collected data on physiological measures of stress (e.g.,

cortisol, blood pressure); however, there were no significant differences in these measures

between the two groups. Combined, the findings of Roeser et al.’s study advance our

understanding of the ability of stress and burnout to be impacted by interventions and to

change over relatively short periods of time (eight weeks). A follow-up question that needs

to be examined is whether these constructs can change over similarly short time frames

without teachers’ being engaged in regular exercises to promote their well-being. In other

words, can we expect teacher stress and burnout to remain stable or increase/decrease in the

face of normal teaching activities and no regular mindfulness exercises? This is important for

understanding the stability of teacher well-being and whether we can realistically expect any

immediate changes as a result of efforts by schools to promote well-being among teachers

and students.

138

Page 151: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Broadly, Roeser et al.’s (2013) findings along with those from Manthei et al. (1996)

and Pas et al. (2012) inform our understanding of how well-being related constructs function

over time. More attention needs to be devoted to teacher well-being as its own construct,

however, given that it is more than the absence (or opposite) of stress or burnout (Huppert &

So, 2013). In order to provide some understanding of how teacher well-being may function

over time, it is necessary to turn to the wider psychological literature involving subjective

well-being—the relative newness of the flourishing construct means that longitudinal

examinations are not yet available. This literature (e.g., Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, Boehm, &

Sheldon, 2011; Schimmack & Lucas, 2010; Suh, Diener, & Fujita, 1996) indicates that

subjective well-being remains relatively constant over time and returns to baseline levels

rather quickly after changes in life circumstances—unless individuals are regularly engaged

in activities to boost their well-being (e.g., mindfulness, Roeser et al., 2013). It is possible

that a similar phenomenon occurs with flourishing; however, the differences between these

two constructs and the differences between global measures of well-being (like subjective

well-being) and teacher well-being mean that research is needed to determine if teacher well-

being is generally stable over time or whether it can change, as well as whether any changes

can be expected over short or longer time frames.

Taken together, previous research indicates that stress and burnout among teachers

can change over time, whereas subjective well-being among individuals does not tend to vary

much over time. Accordingly, these findings underscore the need for the examination of

teacher well-being directly. In addition to examining that, Study 3 also examined the impact

of need satisfaction on teacher well-being over time. This provides a further opportunity to

examine the core relationship of self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2002)

139

Page 152: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

that was examined in Study 2. Furthermore, it will provide an understanding of the impact of

contextually-related constructs on teacher well-being over time.

4.1.1.2. Changes in self-efficacy over time. The literature on self-efficacy for

teaching is quite vast, and fortunately, has involved several studies examining the construct

over time. Looking at changes in efficacy over approximately two years, Woolfolk Hoy and

Spero’s (2005) conducted a study among pre-service teachers and continued to collect data

from these teachers as they completed their first year of teaching in the profession. They

examined self-efficacy for teaching using two previously published measures (Bandura,

1997; Gibson & Dembo, 1984) and one they created for their study. The participants were

asked to complete these measures at the beginning (Time 1) and end (Time 2) of their teacher

education program, and a year after they began teaching (Time 3). The researchers conducted

repeated measures ANOVA to examine whether there were significant differences in self-

efficacy between the different time points. For all measures, self-efficacy increased

significantly from Time 1 to Time 2. The findings from Time 2 to Time 3 were mixed with

some measures showing a significant decrease in self-efficacy for teachers and others

showing no change.

The findings of Woolfolk Hoy and Spero’s (2005) research provide a revealing

insight into the development of self-efficacy at the very early career stage. At the same time,

concerns over the conceptual accuracy of one of the measures used—the Gibson and Dembo

(1984) instrument—point to a larger issue in the literature. Woolfolk Hoy and Spero, along

with Tschannen-Moran, Woolfolk Hoy, and Hoy (1998), and more recently Klassen et al.

(2011) have cautioned that the Gibson and Dembo instrument focuses on locus of control

rather than self-efficacy. Consequently, they question the accuracy of interpretations about

140

Page 153: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

self-efficacy from the measure. This highlights the need for research using what Klassen et

al. (2011) argue are conceptually clearer measures. Specifically, Klassen et al. (2011) have

proposed that in order to be congruent with self-efficacy theory, instruments must attend to

judgments related to specific outcomes (e.g., engaging students) and focus on forward-

looking capabilities. Tschannen-Moran and Woolfolk Hoy’s (2001) Teacher Sense of

Efficacy Scale (TSES) is one such measure that enables us to examine three factors of

teacher self-efficacy—self-efficacy for student engagement, classroom management, and

instructional strategies—using a conceptually and theoretically sound construction.

Notwithstanding the conceptual issues with one of the scales, the Woolfolk Hoy and

Spero (2005) study provides important understanding of the malleability of self-efficacy at

the pre-service level and indicates that it tends to reduce or remain constant in the first year

of teaching. A question that remains is how self-efficacy functions over time among more

experienced teachers. In particular, there is a need for research to determine whether it

becomes relatively stable once teachers gain more experience or whether it continues to be

flexible and/or malleable. One study that examined changes in self-efficacy for teaching

among more experienced teachers is Carleton, Fitch, and Krockover’s (2012) research

examining the impact of a two-week science professional development program on self-

efficacy for science teaching. The authors examined self-efficacy at four waves: before and

after the two-week summer workshop on science concepts, once in autumn when teachers

returned for a one day workshop, and in spring when teachers gave a presentation at a

science teachers’ association meeting. Using paired t-tests, they found that participants

showed a significant increase in self-efficacy for science teaching from before to after the

141

Page 154: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

workshop. In addition, the autumn and spring waves were also significantly higher than the

original measurement.

Taken together, the findings from Carleton and colleagues’ (2008) study indicate that

self-efficacy for teaching can change among teachers at varying career stages as a result of

professional development. These changes also occurred over a short time frame (i.e., two

weeks). Once again, however, concerns with the robustness of the instrument they used

necessitate caution in interpreting the results. The items Carleton and colleagues (2008) used

to assess self-efficacy for science teaching were based on an instrument (i.e., Friedman and

Kass, 2002), which Klassen et al. (2011) argue drifts from ‘theory-based understanding’ of

self-efficacy. For example, Carleton and colleagues used the item, “I see myself as an

interesting and motivating science and/or math teacher” (Carleton et al., 2008, p. 52), which

appears to focus on something akin to self-concept as a science teacher rather than self-

efficacy. Although the authors need to be commended on their efforts to inform our

understanding of changes in self-efficacy as a consequence of professional development, the

measurement issues highlight the need for more research in this area. Given that teachers are

regularly involved in various short-term initiatives/professional development programs such

as that examined by Carleton and colleagues, research that endeavours to understand whether

changes in self-efficacy over short periods of time are feasible will advance our

understanding of the realistic impact of these programs on self-efficacy for teaching.

Other longitudinal studies are beset by similar measurement issues (e.g., Ross, 1994).

In fact, Klassen et al. (2011) claim that the research on self-efficacy for teaching has been

plagued by measurement concerns. Another methodological issue that deserves attention is

the data analytic techniques used to assess self-efficacy over time. Like well-being, more

142

Page 155: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

examination of self-efficacy over time using robust multilevel modelling techniques is

needed. A notable example that addresses both of these concerns is Pas and colleagues’

(2012) study, which was introduced earlier. In addition to examining changes in burnout over

time, they also examined changes in self-efficacy for handling students with behavioural

problems. As noted, their sample included practicing teachers at various stages in their

teaching careers and the authors found a significant increase in self-efficacy over time among

the teachers. They also found that several other variables positively predicted changes in the

intercept (e.g., positive perceptions of school leadership) and/or growth (i.e., teacher

preparedness for handling classroom management) of self-efficacy. This study informs our

understanding of how self-efficacy for managing disruptive behaviour can change over time

and also suggests that changes do not appear to be limited to teachers who are at the

beginning of their career. Research examining other types of self-efficacy (e.g., for engaging

students) will further develop our understanding of this construct.

Another study supporting the flexibility of self-efficacy for teaching across the career

is Klassen and Chiu’s (2010) study examining the relationship between self-efficacy for

teaching and years of experience. The authors used the conceptually sound TSES

(Tschannen-Moran & Woolfolk Hoy, 2001) to examine three factors of self-efficacy— self-

efficacy for student engagement, classroom management, and instructional strategies—

among a large sample of Canadian teachers. The study was cross-sectional, but the authors

examined relationships between self-efficacy and years of experience to understand how

self-efficacy was different at various career stages. The findings revealed that the three

factors of self-efficacy increased along with teaching experience up to mid-career, but that

they decreased as teachers moved into late career. This research provides support for the idea

143

Page 156: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

that self-efficacy for teaching can continue to change as teachers gain experience. Moving

forward, research needs to examine these three types of self-efficacy using data collected at

multiple time points.

Taken together, previous research highlights the need for greater examination of self-

efficacy for teaching over time using robust measures and data analytic techniques. It also

provides evidence that self-efficacy is flexible over both short and long time periods. Given

the measurement concerns with some previous studies (e.g., Carleton et al., 2008), however,

research using more conceptually sound measures is needed to confirm whether changes in

self-efficacy are realistic over shorter time periods. As noted above, this will help us

understand whether short-term interventions that are commonly applied in schools are likely

to have an immediate effect or whether slower, more gradual changes in self-efficacy can be

expected. To that end, Study 3 examined changes in self-efficacy for three factors of efficacy

during a relatively short time period of two to three months. Like well-being, it also

investigated the influence of need satisfaction on changes in self-efficacy to better

understand how contextually-related experiences affect teachers.

4.1.2. Overview of the Current Study

Cross-sectional research provides us with an understanding of constructs at one point

in time, but it does not tell us how teachers’ experiences change over time. For Study 3, I

examined how teacher well-being and self-efficacy for teaching function over a relatively

short time frame of two to three months. One particular commonality about the participants’

experiences is that they all participated in a professional development program on anti-

oppression. In the interest of transparency, it is noted that the program’s focus on anti-

oppression may have affected teachers’ perceptions of well-being and self-efficacy for

144

Page 157: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

teaching. However, given that professional development is a typical experience for teachers

in British Columbia every two to three months (Ministry of Education, 2012), any influence

was treated as a feature of the environment within which the participants worked. In addition,

anti-oppression is a topic with which teachers in British Columbia are familiar given the

province’s performance standards on social responsibility (Ministry of Education, 2001). The

performance standards, created for voluntary use in schools, refer to students’ skills and

awareness in contributing to the classroom and school community, solving problems in

peaceful ways, valuing diversity and defending human rights, and exercising democratic

rights and responsibilities (Ministry of Education, 2001). Taken together, professional

development on topics like anti-oppression is a feature of the schooling system within which

the participants worked and so was treated as such in Study 3.

Taking this setting into account, Study 3 involved examining changes in teacher well-

being and self-efficacy for teaching over time. Moreover, the study investigated how basic

psychological need satisfaction influenced these outcomes over time. Two research questions

guided this study:

• How do teacher well-being and self-efficacy for teaching function over time?

(Research Question 1)

• How does need satisfaction affect teachers’ initial experiences of well-being and

motivation? (Research Question 2)

For Research Question 1, I hypothesised that well-being would show little change over time.

This is based on research showing that subjective well-being is generally stable over time

and returns to baseline levels relatively quickly after changes in life circumstances

(Lyubomirsky et al., 2011; Schimmack & Lucas, 2010; Suh et al., 1996; Hypothesis 1a).

145

Page 158: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

• Hypothesis 1a. Well-being will remain stable over the two- to three-month period of

examination.

For self-efficacy, however, I hypothesised that there may be some change given that previous

research has provided evidence of the variability of teacher self-efficacy over relatively short

amounts of time (e.g., three, six, twelve months; Carleton et al., 2009; Klassen & Chiu, 2010;

Woolfolk Hoy & Spero, 2005; however, as noted earlier there are concerns about the

conceptual accuracy of Carleton and colleagues’ findings).

• Hypothesis 1b. Self-efficacy will show variation over the two- to three-month period.

For Research Question 2, the hypothesised relationships between need satisfaction

and well-being are based on relationships postulated in Study 1.

• Hypothesis 2a. Satisfaction of the needs for autonomy and competence will positively

predict workload well-being.

• Hypothesis 2b. Satisfaction of the needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness

with colleagues will positively predict organisational well-being.

• Hypothesis 2c. Satisfaction of the needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness

with colleagues will positively predict student interaction well-being.

The relationships between need satisfaction and self-efficacy are drawn from

understanding of both social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1982, 1997) and SDT (Deci & Ryan,

1985, 2002). Although motivation is defined differently in these two theories, I suggest that

the relationship between need satisfaction and motivation in SDT is also relevant when

defining motivation according to social cognitive theory (i.e., as self-efficacy). In essence, I

posit that satisfaction of psychological needs is positively associated with confidence in

future abilities (i.e., self-efficacy). Previous research supports this reasoning by highlighting

146

Page 159: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

that self-efficacy is related to the need for competence (e.g., Lynch et al., 2005). Further,

self-efficacy relates to agency (i.e., the belief that we shape our own life circumstances;

Bandura, 1989), which is similar to autonomy. Together, these findings warrant further

examination of the relationship between need satisfaction and self-efficacy.

Rationales for the specific hypotheses between need satisfaction and self-efficacy

were based on the meaning of the constructs. First, I hypothesised that competence and

relatedness with students would be significant predictors of self-efficacy for student

engagement because teachers are more likely to feel confident in their ability to engage their

students if they have a connection with their students (i.e., because they better understand

their students’ learning needs) and if they feel competent in their teaching ability (i.e.,

confidence reflects a sense of competence).

• Hypothesis 2d. Satisfaction of the needs for competence and relatedness with students

will positively predict self-efficacy for student engagement.

Second, I hypothesised that autonomy, competence, and relatedness with students

would be significant predictors of self-efficacy for classroom management given that

teachers who feel connected to their students, competent, and in control are more likely to

understand the best ways to successfully manage their students, feel more confident about

their ability to manage the classroom, and choose classroom management strategies that

work for them.

• Hypothesis 2e. Satisfaction of the needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness

with students will positively predict self-efficacy for classroom management.

For the final outcome variable, it was hypothesised that competence and autonomy

would predict self-efficacy for instructional strategies because a sense of competence likely

147

Page 160: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

relates to greater confidence in one’s abilities and because autonomy likely means that

teachers are free to choose instructional strategies that they feel confident in using.

• Hypothesis 2f. Satisfaction of the needs for autonomy and competence will positively

predict self-efficacy for instructional strategies.

4.2. Methods

4.2.1. Participants

Participants included 71 teachers from a large, suburban school board in British

Columbia, Canada. These teachers represent a different sample from the first two studies.

Moreover, they worked in a different school district that was not involved in Studies 1 and 2

data collection. Approximately 69% of the sample was female and 49% taught at the

elementary level (37% at the secondary level, 10% at both levels, and 1% taught at an

alternative education centre). The average teaching experience was 11.6 (SD = 8.14) years

and the average age of participants was 40.4 (SD = 10.28) years. Participants were asked

about their ethnic origins. Among the sample, 75% reported having one, 18% reported

having two, and 1% reported having three ethnic backgrounds (6% chose not to answer). The

frequency of the reported ethnic backgrounds are shown in Table 4.1. Participants were

asked whether they considered themselves to be a minority based on race, gender, ethnicity,

class, ability and/or sexual orientation. Thirty-nine percent (39%) of participants considered

themselves to be a minority, 12% were unsure, and the remainder did not consider

themselves to be a minority (45%).

The majority of participants were classroom teachers (58%). Other positions included

working as support teachers (e.g., resource teachers, special education teachers, or

counsellors, 14%), youth and family support workers (13%), multicultural liaison worker

148

Page 161: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

(7%), administrators (e.g., principals; 3%), teacher librarians (1%), and other positions (9%;

e.g., settlement workers in schools, district support staff, special education assistant). All

participants worked with students and so were classified as teachers in the results.

Table 4.1

Frequency of Participants’ Ethnic Origins

Northern and western

European

Eastern and southern European

East Asian

South Asian

Aboriginal Other

Frequency 62% 14% 14% 11% 4% 10% Note. Northern and Western European origins refers to British, Scottish, German, Swedish, Danish, Norwegian, Dutch, etc. Eastern and Southern European origins refers to Polish, Russian, Ukrainian, Italian, Greek, Spanish, etc. Aboriginal origins refers to First Nations, Inuit, Metis, etc. East Asian origins refers to Chinese, Japanese, Korean, etc. South Asian origins refers to East Indian, Punjabi, Pakistani, etc. Other included southeast Asian, self-identified Canadian, Latin American, self-identified Jewish, and unspecified.

4.2.2. Procedures

Participants for Study 3 were recruited through the booking process of a professional

development program that aimed to promote anti-oppression in classrooms and schools

(Appendix C shows the invitation letter provided to teachers). When teachers booked the

program for their classroom or participated in it at district-level professional development

days, they were invited to participate in the research. Most of the data collection occurred via

online questionnaire; however, teachers who participated in the professional development

days completed a paper copy of the questionnaire at Time 1 and online copies at Time 2 and

3. Data were collected over two school years. Table 4.2 shows the sample size for the three

waves of data collection broken down by potential sample size (i.e., participants who

consented to the research) and actual sample size (i.e., participants who actually filled out the

149

Page 162: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

questionnaires).4 It also shows the response rates for the three time points. Details about

attrition are discussed below. Teachers completed the questionnaire at three time points.

Time 2 occurred around six weeks after Time 1 (M = 40.14 days, SD = 20.84 days; time

varied depending on teacher availability and school vacations) and Time 3 occurred around

nine weeks after Time 1 (M = 64.31 days, SD = 27.57 days; this means that Time 3 occurred

about four weeks after Time 2). Data were collected over a two- to three-month period.

Table 4.2

Actual and Potential Sample Size at Each Time Point

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Potential 71 50a 50

Actual 65 28 22

Response rate 92% 56% 44% Note. Pro-d = professional development. a The drop in potential sample size between Time 1 and 2 was because 21 teachers in Year 2 were not included in data collection at Time 2 and 3. See Footnote 4.

4.2.3. Measures

As noted above, three self-report questionnaires were used to collect data at three

times points. The questionnaires included measures of well-being, need satisfaction, self-

efficacy, and questions pertaining to demographics. Table 4.3 shows the items that were used

for each construct. The choice of items was based on factor analyses conducted in previous

4 The drop in potential sample size between Time 1 and Time 2 reflects a change in school district protocol which meant we were unable to collect email addresses of teachers who only participated in the program at the professional development days (i.e., they did not make a subsequent booking to have the program in their classroom).

150

Page 163: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

research among different samples from the same British Columbian population of teachers

(e.g., Study 1 and 2; Collie et al., 2012). Table 4.4 shows the descriptive statistics for the

different constructs and Table 4.5 shows the Cronbach’s alphas. As described in Studies 1

and 2, Table 2.5 shows the constructs that were examined in Study 3, along with those

examined in the previous two studies.

4.2.3.1. Well-being. Well-being was measured with the Teacher Well-Being Scale

(TWBS) developed in Study 1 (see Appendix D, Section 2). As established in Study 1 and

shown in Table 4.3, six items were used to measure workload well-being (e.g., “Staying late

after work for meetings and activities”), six items were used to measure organisational well-

being (e.g., “Support offered by school leadership”), and four items were used to measure

student interaction well-being (e.g., “Relations with students in my class”). All items were

scored on a 7-point scale ranging from Negatively (1) to Positively (7). The Cronbach’s

alphas at all three time points provide evidence for validity based on internal structure (see

Table 4.5).

4.2.3.2. Self-efficacy for teaching. The Teachers’ Sense of Efficacy Scale (TSES,

Tschannen-Moran & Woolfolk Hoy, 2001) was used to measure self-efficacy for teaching

(see Appendix D, Section 4). Based on Collie et al.’s (2012) factor analyses, three items were

used to measure self-efficacy for student engagement (e.g., “How much can you do to get

students to believe they can do well in school work?”), another three items were used to

measure self-efficacy for classroom management (e.g., “How much can you do to get

children to follow classroom rules?”), and four items were used to measure self-efficacy for

instructional strategies (e.g., “To what extent can you use a variety of assessment

strategies?”). Items were scored on a scale ranging from Not At All (1) to A Great Deal (9).

151

Page 164: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Evidence of validity, including reliability, of the scores on this measure have been supported

in a variety of samples (e.g., α ≥ .64; Klassen et al., 2009; Tschannen-Moran & Woolfolk

Hoy, 2001), as well as in Study 3 (see Table 4.5).

4.2.3.3. Need satisfaction. Need satisfaction was measured with the same instrument

used in Study 2, the Work-related Basic Need Satisfaction scale (Van den Broeck et al.,

2010; see Appendix D, Section 3). Four items were used to measure relatedness with

colleagues (e.g., “At work, I can talk with people about things that really matter to me”),

three items were used to measure relatedness with students (e.g., “I am very committed to my

students”), six items were used to measure competence, (e.g., “I feel competent at my job”),

and five items were used to measure autonomy (e.g., “I feel free to do my job the way I think

it should be done”). Participants responded to these statements on a scale ranging from

Strongly Disagree (1) to Strongly Agree (7). Once again, Cronbach’s alphas at all three time

points were adequate.

4.2.3.4. Demographic information. Teachers were asked to supply demographic

information including age, sex, ethnic/cultural heritage, whether or not they considered

themselves a member of a minority group, school grades currently teaching, current teaching

position, and years of teaching experience (see Appendix D, Section 1).

152

Page 165: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.3

Items Used to Assess the Well-Being, Self-Efficacy, and Need Satisfaction Constructs

Items Item number

Workload well-being

Marking work 3

Fitting everything in to the allotted time 4

Administrative work related to teaching 7

Work I complete outside of school hours for teaching 13

Working to finish my teaching tasks 14

Staying late after work for meetings and activities 16

Organisational well-being

Relations with administrators at my school 2

Support offered by school leadership 9

Recognition for my teaching 10

School rules and procedures that are in place 11

Communication between members of the school 12

Participation in school-level decision making 15

Student interaction well-being

Relations with students in my class 1

Student behaviour 5

Student motivation 6

Classroom management 8

153

Page 166: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.3 (Continued)

Items Item number

Self-Efficacy for Student Engagement

How much can you do to motivate students who show low interest in school work?

2

How much can you do to help your students value learning? 4

How much can you do to get students to believe they can do well in school work?

7

Self-Efficacy for Classroom Management

How much can you do to control disruptive behaviour in the classroom?

1

How much can you do to calm a student who is disruptive or noisy? 3

How much can you do to get children to follow classroom rules? 6

Self-Efficacy for Instructional Strategies

To what extent can you craft good questions for your students? 5

To what extent can you use a variety of assessment strategies? 9

To what extent can you provide an alternative explanation or example when students are confused? 10

How well can you implement alternative teaching strategies in your classroom? 12

Relatedness with colleagues

I don’t really feel connected with other people at work (R) 1

I don’t really mix with colleagues at work (R) 3

At work, I can talk with people about things that really matter to me 4

I often feel alone when I am with my colleagues (R) 5

154

Page 167: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.3 (Continued)

Items Item number

Relatedness with students

I am very committed to my students 19

Connecting with my students is an essential part of the job 20

I feel connected to my students 22

Competence

I don’t really feel competent in my job (R) 7

I really master the things I have to do at work 8

I feel competent at my job 9

I have doubts about whether I am able to do my job properly (R) 10

I am good at the things I do in my job 11

I have the feeling that I can accomplish even the most difficult tasks at work 12

Autonomy

At work, I often feel like I have to follow other people’s commands (R) 14

If I could choose, I would do things at work differently (R) 15

The things I have to do at work are in line with what I really want to do

16

I feel free to do my job the way I think it should be done 17

In my job, I feel forced to do things I don’t want to do (R) 18

155

Page 168: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.4

Means, Standard Deviations, and Ranges for all Variables

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

M SD Observed range

M SD Observed range

M SD Observed range

Workload well-being 3.92 0.936 1.20 – 6.17 4.15 1.139 1.50 – 6.50 4.30 1.149 1.67 – 7.00

Organisational well-being 5.05 1.070 2.67 – 6.83 5.01 1.066 3.00 – 7.00 4.94 1.117 3.00 – 7.00

Student interaction well-being 5.23 1.028 3.00 – 6.75 5.36 0.817 2.50 – 6.75 5.48 0.862 4.00 – 7.00

Self-efficacy for student engagement 6.77 0.984 4.33 – 9.00

7.11 1.050 6.00 – 9.00

6.90 1.028 5.33 – 9.00

Self-efficacy for classroom management 7.03 1.044 4.33 – 9.00

7.53 0.944 6.00 – 9.00

7.46 0.963 5.67 – 9.00

Self-efficacy for instructional strategies 7.25 1.059 5.00 – 9.00

7.17 0.969 5.75 – 9.00

7.13 1.130 4.75 – 9.00

Relatedness with colleagues 3.83 0.830 1.00 – 5.00 3.82 0.703 2.00 – 5.00 3.80 0.892 1.00 – 5.00

Relatedness with students 4.53 0.443 3.33 – 5.00 4.49 0.555 3.00 – 5.00 4.45 0.540 3.00 – 5.00

Competence 4.10 0.571 2.33 – 5.00 3.96 0.650 2.17 – 5.00 3.95 0.467 3.00 – 3.00

Autonomy 3.32 0.749 1.80 – 5.00 3.47 0.854 1.40 – 5.00 3.31 0.722 2.00 – 5.00

156

Page 169: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.5

Cronbach’s Alphas for the Different Variables at the Three Time Points

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Workload well-being .84 .84 .92

Organisational well-being .85 .95 .94

Student interaction well-being .84 .87 .84

Self-efficacy for student engagement .75 .90 .87

Self-efficacy for classroom management .83 .84 .86

Self-efficacy for instructional strategies .86 .81 .90

Relatedness with colleagues .87 .71 .95

Relatedness with students .71 .90 .85

Competence .87 .92 .82

Autonomy .87 .92 .91

157

Page 170: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.3. Results

Before conducting the main analyses to answer the research questions, preliminary

analyses were used to examine the attrition that occurred and what differences existed

between participants who remained in the study and those who dropped out. Time 1 data was

used to run several independent t-tests to compare these two groups. The results are shown in

Table 4.6. The findings revealed no significant differences in well-being and self-efficacy at

Time 1 between those who dropped out after Time 1 and those who remained in the study.

Although it is possible that this non-significance reflects the small sample size, it does

provide some confidence that the groups were similar in relation to their experiences with the

outcome variables at Time 1. Unfortunately, this does not allow us to conclude that there

were no differences in unmeasured variables and/or differences that occurred after Time 1

(e.g., if participants became very busy and could not fill out the questionnaire, which may

have had implications for their motivation and well-being). Further details of this are

discussed in the limitations section below. Correlations between all variables at each time

point are shown in Table 4.7, Table 4.8, and Table 4.9.

158

Page 171: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.6

Differences in Time 1 Scores Among Participants Remaining In or Dropping Out of Study After Time 1

Teachers who only participated at Time 1

M (SD)

Teachers who remained in study after Time 1

M (SD)

t df p

Workload well-being 3.87 (0.900) 3.97 (0.988) -0.418 57 .677

Organisational well-being 4.98 (1.076) 5.13 (1.078) -0.523 60 .603

Student interaction well-being 5.09 (1.040) 5.39 (1.007) -1.173 61 .245

Self-efficacy for student engagement 6.68 (1.096) 6.87 (0.864) -0.709 51 .481

Self-efficacy for classroom management 6.88 (1.075) 7.18 (1.008) -1.058 51 .295

Self-efficacy for instructional strategies 7.17 (1.201) 7.33 (0.897) -0.526 51 .601

159

Page 172: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.7

Correlations Between Variables at Time 1

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.

1. Workload WB

2. Organisational WB .47*

3. Student interaction WB .45* .50*

4. SE for student engagement .26 .38* .59*

5. SE for classroom management .25 .41* .49* .65*

6. SE for instructional strategies -.04 .31* .28* .54* .46*

7. Rel. with teachers .15 .59* .13 .06 .09 .18

8. Rel. with students .04 .20 .26 .31* .40* .42* .15

9. Competence .23 .42* .41* .36* .37* .25 .22 .40*

10. Autonomy .47* .74* .50* .35* .48* .22 .43* .32* .48* Note. WB = well-being. SE = self-efficacy. Rel. = relatedness. * p < .05.

160

Page 173: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.8

Correlations Between Variables at Time 2

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.

1. Workload WB

2. Organisational WB .60*

3. Student interaction WB .68* .44*

4. SE for student engagement .35 .26 .37

5. SE for classroom management .30 .21 .39* .52*

6. SE for instructional strategies .58* .31 .57* .54* .54*

7. Rel. with teachers .47* .66* .42* .04 .17 .02

8. Rel. with students .07 .10 .37 .35 .41* .22 .38*

9. Competence .42* .52* .79* .28 .37 .26 .61* .51*

10. Autonomy .69* .78* .68* .38* .33 .19 .69* .28 .66* Note. WB = well-being. SE = self-efficacy. Rel. = relatedness. * p < .05.

161

Page 174: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.9

Correlations Between Variables at Time 3

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.

1. Workload WB

2. Organisational WB .51*

3. Student interaction WB .56* .42

4. SE for student engagement .40 .51* .61*

5. SE for classroom management .28 .40 .60* .69*

6. SE for instructional strategies .47* .66* .62* .76* .79*

7. Rel. with teachers .26 .59* .46* .08 .43 .41

8. Rel. with students .23 .20 .57* .33 .57* .58* .47*

9. Competence .49* .76* .72* .58* .62* .74* .72* .60*

10. Autonomy .70* .73* .50* .32 .43 .60* .65* .29 .67* Note. WB = well-being. SE = self-efficacy. Rel. = relatedness. * p < .05.

162

Page 175: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.3.1. Overview of Data Analysis

In order to examine how teacher well-being and motivation function over time,

individual trajectories of growth were fitted using univariate multilevel modelling. This

involves fitting increasingly complex models to see if the addition of variables provides a

better fitting model that explains changes in the outcome variables over time.

4.3.1.1. Unconditional models. The first model that was fitted was an unconditional

means model with no predictors (Model 1) to check whether there was significant variation

in the outcome variables between participants. This model included a fixed component

representing the average of the outcome variable across participants and the three time points

(i.e., the grand mean; π0i). In addition, there were two random components representing

variation in the grand mean across individuals (between-level; u0i) and variation across the

three time points within individuals (within-level; rti). The following equation represents the

unconditional means model:

Yti = π0i + [u0i + rti]

If the grand mean was significantly different from zero this revealed that the outcome

variable, Yti, differed significantly across individuals. Furthermore, if the variance

components for the two random effects were statistically significant this revealed that there

was both between-person variance (variance associated with the intercept, τ00) and within-

person variance (σ2) to be explained. The reasons for fitting the following models was to

reduce the between-person and within-person variance by adding factors into the model. By

reducing these variance components, it means that the model more accurately explains

individual trajectories.

163

Page 176: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

The second set of models were unconditional growth models where time was entered

as an effect. The first of these, Model 2a, involved entering time as a fixed effect to ascertain

whether there was significant change over time in participants’ scores on the outcome

variable. The following equation represents the unconditional growth model with time as a

fixed effect:

Yti = [π 0i + π 1iTimeti] + [u0i + rti]

where [π 0i + π 1iTimeti] represented the fixed effects of intercept and slope, respectively, and

[u0i + rti] represents the random effects of intercept and the growth rate (rti).

For the second unconditional growth model, Model 2b, time was allowed to be a

random effect (Model 2b). This captured any differences in the slope of the trajectories

across participants. The following equation represents the unconditional growth model with

time as a random effect:

Yti = [π 0i + π 1iTimeti] + [u0i + u1i Timeti + rti]

where the addition of u1i Timeti represents the random effect of slope. There were two

additional between-person variance components in this model: the variance associated with

the slope (τ11) and the covariance between the slope and intercept (τ01).

The third unconditional model, Model 2c, involved testing the curvature of the

trajectory by including a fixed factor of Time2. The following equation represents this model:

Yti = [π 0i + π 1iTimeti + π 2iTimeti2] + [u0i + u1i Timeti + rti]

where the addition of π 2iTimeti2 represents the fixed effect of curvature. Following this, the

final unconditional model, Model 2d, involved allowing Time2 to vary randomly. This

captured any differences in the curvature of the trajectories across participants. The equation

for this model is as follows:

164

Page 177: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Yti = [π 0i + π 1iTimeti + π 2iTimeti2] + [u0i + u1i Timeti + u1i Timeti2 + rti]

where the addition of u1i Timeti2 represents the random effect of curvature.

4.3.1.2. Conditional model. Once the intercept, slope, and curvature were identified

through the unconditional models, conditional models were run where variables of interest

were included to see if they explained the between-person and within-person variance. In

Study 3, the first conditional model, Model 3, involved the addition of the need satisfaction

variables: relatedness with colleagues, relatedness with students, competence, and

autonomy.5 Namely, participants’ scores on the need satisfaction variables at Time 1 were

entered as a time-invariant predictor. The aim of this model was to assess the effect of need

satisfaction on participants’ well-being and self-efficacy. The final model, Model 4, only

included the need satisfaction variables that were significant. The rationale for excluding the

non-significant need satisfaction variables was for parsimony given that they did not

significantly add to the explanation of the model.

4.3.2. Results for Multilevel Findings

In order to conduct the multilevel analyses, I used Mplus Version 7.11 (Muthén &

Muthén, 1998-2012). The method of estimation used was MLR, which involves maximum

likelihood estimation with robust standard errors and a robust chi-square test. This method of

estimation is robust in the face of non-normality and missing data. In order to assess model

fit, a chi-square difference test based on the loglikelihood values was used. This method is

appropriate for MLR estimation and incorporates the scaling correction factor of each model

5 Covariates of age, teaching experience, teaching load, perceptions of workshop (i.e., ‘perceived usefulness’), number of workshops, sex, and identified minority were included in preliminary conditional models; however, none were significant. Thus, these models were not reported in the results and the covariates were excluded so that they did not compromise power given the small sample size.

165

Page 178: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

(Muthén & Muthén, 2005). The df value used in this method is the difference in the number

of parameters between the two models. In order to aid interpretability, time was centred so

that Time 1 = 0, and the need satisfaction variables were centred around the mean at Time 1.

4.3.2.1. Workload well-being. The first outcome examined was workload well-

being. Table 4.10 shows the results. As detailed above, the first model tested was the

unconditional means model (Model 1, Table 4.10). The results revealed that workload well-

being varied significantly among participants (as seen in the significant intercept in Table

4.10, p < .001). The variance estimated for the mean intercept (τ00) was 0.803 (p < .001) and

that for the within-person random effect (σ2) was 0.208 (p < .001). This highlights that there

was more variance between participants than within participants.

Model 2a, an unconditional growth model, involved the addition of time as a fixed

effect to see if it explained the two variance components. It was not significant (p = .088)

revealing that there was no significant change in workload well-being over time. Given this,

there was no need to run an unconditional model with time as a random effect (Model 2b

described above). The curvature of the trajectories was also tested (Model 2c described

above) to assess whether there was a significant curvilinear trend that was not captured by

the time variable. To that end, time2 was entered in the model, but it was not significant

confirming that there was no change over time (results not presented).

Despite there being no significant changes in workload well-being over time,

conditional models were run to help explain the between-person variance. In order to provide

contextual timing of the data, time was left in the model despite being non-significant. The

first conditional model involved the addition of the need satisfaction variables (see Model 3,

Table 4.10). The results showed that autonomy was a significant predictor of workload well-

166

Page 179: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

being (p = .002). The final conditional model (Model 4) excluded the non-significant need

satisfaction variables and confirmed that autonomy was still a significant predictor (p <

.001). As teachers’ reports of autonomy increased, so did their workload well-being. This

finding highlights that individual differences in workload well-being were explained in part

by autonomy. This was mirrored in the between-person variance (τ00), which was reduced

from 0.789 (p < .001) in Model 2a to 0.568 (p < .001) in Model 3. Put another way, the

addition of autonomy into the model explained 28% of the between-person variance. The fact

that the between-person variance remained significant suggests that there are other variables

that explain differences in workload well-being between participants (e.g., perhaps school

climate or autonomy support). The within-person variance (σ2) was not greatly reduced by

the addition of factors into the model (0.208 [p < .001] in Model 1 to 0.193 in Model 4 [p <

.001]), which is not surprising given that time (the within-person variable) was not

significant. A chi-square difference test showed that the addition of autonomy significantly

improved the fit of model: χ2 (1) = 71.86, p < .001.

In order to graph the predicted values of workload well-being, the following equation

was used:

Yti = π00 + π01Timeti + π05Autonomyi

This equation was solved for three different levels of autonomy: teachers reporting low

autonomy (one standard deviation below the mean), average autonomy, and high autonomy

(one standard deviation above the mean). Figure 4.1 shows the relationship between

workload well-being and autonomy based upon this equation. Time is not considered in the

graph given that it was not a significant factor in the model. As the graph shows, teachers

167

Page 180: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

who reported high autonomy experienced significantly greater workload well-being than

teachers reporting average or low autonomy.

Table 4.10

Multilevel Models for Examining Change in Workload Well-Being Over Time

Model 1 Model 2a Model 3 Model 4

Fixed effects

Intercept (π00) 4.039*** 3.975*** 3.878*** 3.880***

Timeti (π01) 0.127 0.100 0.101

Rel. with colleagues (π02)

-0.103†

Rel. with students (π03) -0.254

Competence (π04) 0.097

Autonomy (π05) 0.702** 0.637***

Random effects

τ00 0.803*** 0.789*** 0.555*** 0.568***

σ2 0.208*** 0.198*** 0.192*** 0.193***

Fit statistics

-2LL -131.951 -130.198 -97.538 -98.186

AIC 269.902 268.396 211.076 206.373 †p = .055, *p < .050, ** p < .010, *** p < .001.

168

Page 181: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Figure 4.1. Predicted values of workload well-being for different levels of autonomy.

3

3.5

4

4.5

5

5.5

6

Low Average High

Wor

kloa

d W

ell-B

eing

Autonomy

169

Page 182: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.3.2.2. Organisational well-being. The second variable to be examined was

organisational well-being. The results are shown in Table 4.11. The unconditional means

model (Model 1) revealed that organisational well-being varied statistically significantly

among participants (p < .001). In addition, the between-person variance (τ00 = 0.870, p <

.001) and the within-person variance (σ2 = 0.250, p < .001) were statistically significant, with

more variance occurring at the between-level than the within-level. The unconditional

growth model (Model 2a) revealed that time as a fixed effect did not add significantly to the

explanation of the outcome variable (p = .302). The addition of time2 to test the curvature of

the model was also non-significant, confirming that there was no change over time (results

not presented).

As with workload well-being, the time variable was left in subsequent conditional

models to contextualise the data. The first conditional model involved the addition of the

four need satisfaction variables (Model 3). Two need satisfaction variables were significant

and so the final model (Model 4) was run with only the two significant need satisfaction

variables in the model. Relatedness with colleagues (p = .001) and autonomy (p < .001) were

the significant predictors and they both had positive relationships with the outcome variable.

As teachers’ reports of relatedness with colleagues and autonomy increased, so did their

organisational well-being. A chi-square test revealed that the final model provided

significantly greater fit than Model 2a: χ2 (2) = 117.82, p < .001. The two need satisfaction

variables explained 72% of the between-person variance (τ00 was reduced from 0.887 in

Model 2a to 0.247 in Model 4).

170

Page 183: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

The following equation was used to graph the predicted values of organisational well-

being:

Yti = π00 + π01Timeti + π02Relatedness with Colleaguesi + π05Autonomyi

This equation was solved for three different levels of need satisfaction: low, average, and

high levels of relatedness with colleagues and autonomy. Figure 4.2 shows the relationship

between organisational well-being and need satisfaction. Teachers who reported high

relatedness with colleagues and autonomy experienced markedly greater organisational well-

being than teachers reporting average or low relatedness with colleagues or autonomy.

171

Page 184: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.11

Multilevel Models for Examining Change in Organisational Well-Being Over Time

Model 1 Model 2a Model 3 Model 4

Fixed effects

Intercept (π00) 5.025*** 5.062*** 4.958*** 4.959***

Timeti (π01) -0.075 -0.048 -0.051

Rel. with teachers (π02) 0.348*** 0.347**

Rel. with students (π03) -0.173

Competence (π04) 0.256

Autonomy (π05) 0.845*** 0.907***

Random effects

τ00 0.870*** 0.877*** 0.227** 0.247**

σ2 0.250*** 0.243*** 0.232*** 0.229***

Fit statistics

-2LL -144.415 -143.900 -86.393 -87.466

AIC 294.830 295.800 188.787 186.931 *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

172

Page 185: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Figure 4.2. Predicted values of organisational well-being for different levels of relatedness

with colleagues and autonomy.

3

3.5

4

4.5

5

5.5

6

Low Average High

Org

anis

atio

nal W

ell-B

eing

Relatedness with Colleagues and Autonomy

173

Page 186: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.3.2.3. Student interaction well-being. The results of the multilevel models

predicting change in student interaction well-being are shown in Table 4.12. The

unconditional means model (Model 1) revealed that student interaction well-being varied

significantly among participants (p < .001). The between-person (τ00 = 0.661, p < .001) and

within-person (σ2 = 0.268, p < .001) variance components were both significant, with more

variance occurring between participants. Model 2a revealed that the addition of time as a

fixed effect was not significant (p = .867), nor was the inclusion of time2 (results not

presented). We can conclude that student interaction well-being did not change significantly

over time.

Model 3 revealed that autonomy was a significant factor in the model. Model 4

confirmed autonomy as a significant and positive predictor (p < .001) while excluding the

non-significant need satisfaction variables. As teachers’ reports of autonomy increased, so

did their student interaction well-being. Furthermore, autonomy explained 27% of the

between-person variance (τ00 was reduced from 0.658 in Model 2a to 0.479 in Model 4). The

chi-square difference test revealed that Model 4 significantly improved model fit: χ2 (1) =

44.61, p < .001.

The following equation was used to graph the predicted values of student interaction

well-being:

Yti = π00 + π01Timeti + π05Autonomyi

Figure 4.3 shows the relationship between student interaction well-being and autonomy.

Teachers who reported high levels of autonomy experienced greater student interaction well-

being than teachers reporting low or average autonomy.

174

Page 187: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.12

Multilevel Models for Examining Change in Student Interaction Well-Being Over Time

Model 1 Model 2a Model 3 Model 4

Fixed effects

Intercept (π00) 5.272*** 5.266*** 5.180*** 5.185***

Timeti (π01) 0.012 0.004 -0.001

Rel. with colleagues (π02)

-0.083

Rel. with students (π03) 0.093

Competence (π04) 0.397

Autonomy (π05) 0.523* 0.646***

Random effects

τ00 0.661*** 0.658*** 0.426** 0.479**

σ2 0.268*** 0.269*** 0.239*** 0.238***

Fit statistics

-2LL -140.780 -140.767 -98.529 -100.690

AIC 287.560 289.534 213.058 211.380 *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

175

Page 188: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Figure 4.3. Predicted values of student interaction well-being for different levels of

autonomy.

3

3.5

4

4.5

5

5.5

6

Low Average High

Stud

ent I

nter

actio

n W

ell-B

eing

Autonomy

176

Page 189: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.3.2.4. Self-efficacy for student engagement. The first self-efficacy variable tested

was self-efficacy for student engagement. The results of the multilevel models are shown in

Table 4.13. Self-efficacy for student engagement varied significantly (p < .001) among

participants as shown in the unconditional means model (Model 1). The two variance

components were significant (τ00 = 0.616, p = .001; σ2 = 0.427, p < .001). Once again, there

was more between-person variance than within-person variance. The addition of time as a

fixed effect in Model 2a was not significant (p = .238), nor was the addition of time2 (results

not presented).

The conditional models (Models 3 and 4) revealed that relatedness with students was

a significant and positive predictor (p = .003) of the outcome variable. As teachers’ reports of

relatedness with students increased, so did their self-efficacy for student engagement.

Relatedness with students explained 31% of the between-person variance (τ00 was reduced

from 0.616 in Model 2a to 0.424 in Model 4). The chi-square difference test revealed that

Model 4 significantly improved model fit: χ2 (1) = 57.79, p < .001. The equation used to

graph the predicted values of self-efficacy for student engagement was as follows:

Yti = π00 + π01Timeti + π03Relatedness with Studentsi

Figure 4.4 shows the relationship between self-efficacy for student engagement and

relatedness with students. Teachers who reported high levels of relatedness with students

experienced the greatest self-efficacy for student engagement.

177

Page 190: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.13

Multilevel Models for Examining Change in Self-Efficacy for Student Engagement Over Time

Model 1 Model 2a Model 3 Model 4

Fixed effects

Intercept (π00) 6.895*** 6.830*** 6.800*** 6.793***

Timeti (π01) 0.115 0.104 0.107

Rel. with colleagues (π02)

-0.096

Rel. with students (π03) 0.555† 0.787**

Competence (π04) 0.208

Autonomy (π05) 0.313

Random effects

τ00 0.616** 0.616*** 0.370** 0.424**

σ2 0.427*** 0.417*** 0.473*** 0.480***

Fit statistics

-2LL 136.487 -135.766 -110.723 -113.168

AIC 278.975 279.532 237.446 236.336

† p = .05, *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

178

Page 191: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Figure 4.4. Predicted values of self-efficacy for student engagement for different levels of

relatedness with students.

6.4

6.6

6.8

7

7.2

7.4

7.6

7.8

Low Average High

Self-

Effic

acy

for S

tude

nt E

ngag

emen

t

Relatedness with Students

179

Page 192: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.3.2.5. Self-efficacy for classroom management. Table 4.14 shows the results for

self-efficacy for classroom management. Model 1 revealed that participants’ scores on the

outcome variable varied significantly (p < .001) and that there was significant between-

person (τ00 = 0.575, p < .001) and within-person variance (σ2 = 0.478, p = .001). Unlike

previous models, the two variance components were close in size suggesting that there were

almost equal amounts of variance to explain at the two levels.

In Model 2a, time was entered as a fixed effect. It was significant (p = .035),

suggesting that there was variation in participants’ scores on the outcome variable over time.

The fixed time effect explained 6% of the within-person variance (σ2 was reduced from

0.478 in Model 2a to 0.451 in Model 2a). Furthermore, the chi-square difference test

revealed that the inclusion of time as a fixed variable significantly improved the fit of the

model: χ2 (1) = 5.48, p = .019. In two subsequent models, time was allowed to vary randomly

and time2 was entered as a fixed predictor, but neither effects were significant (results not

presented). It was concluded that self-efficacy for classroom management changed linearly

over time and that there was no variation in the trend across participants.

The conditional models (Models 3 and 4) revealed two significant need satisfaction

variables. Relatedness with students (p = .001) and autonomy (p = .002) were both

significant and positive predictors. The addition of the two predictors in Model 4

significantly improved model fit (χ2 [2] = 51.28, p < .001) and explained 56% of the

between-person variance (τ00 was reduced from 0.554 in Model 2a to 0.244 in Model 4). The

equation used to graph the predicted values of self-efficacy for classroom management over

time was as follows:

Yti = π00 + π01Timeti + π03Relatedness with Studentsi + π05Autonomyi

180

Page 193: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

where the time coefficient (π 01) was 0.233 as specified in Model 4. Figure 4.5 shows the

trajectories for teachers with low, average, and high levels of relatedness with students and

autonomy. This is similar to the graphs shown for the other outcome variables in that need

satisfaction was positively correlated with the outcome; however, where the other graphs did

not include time, this one did. It shows how self-efficacy for classroom management changed

over time for teachers reporting low, average, and high levels of relatedness with students

and autonomy. The trajectories show that teachers reported greater self-efficacy over time.

Moreover, the results show that the starting point for teachers reporting high relatedness with

students and autonomy was greater than the final point for teachers reporting low and

average levels of these two needs, suggesting that need satisfaction plays a major role in self-

efficacy for classroom management.

181

Page 194: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.14

Multilevel Models for Examining Change in Self-Efficacy for Classroom Management Over

Time

Model 1 Model 2a Model 3 Model 4

Fixed effects

Intercept (π00) 7.223*** 7.094*** 7.062*** 7.063***

Timeti (π01) 0.223* 0.230* 0.233*

Rel. with colleagues (π02)

-0.089

Rel. with students (π03) 0.737** 0.751**

Competence (π04) 0.048

Autonomy (π05) 0.473** 0.440**

Random effects

τ00 0.575*** 0.554*** 0.243 0.244

σ2 0.478** 0.451*** 0.495** 0.499***

Fit Statistics

-2LL -138.498 -135.970 -106.857 -107.094

AIC 282.996 279.939 229.714 226.188

*p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

182

Page 195: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Figure 4.5. Predicted values of self-efficacy for classroom management for different levels

of relatedness with students and autonomy.

6

6.5

7

7.5

8

8.5

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Self-

Effic

acy

for C

lass

room

Man

agem

ent

Low Relatedness with Students and AutonomyAverage Relatedness with Students and AutonomyHigh Relatedness with Students and Autonomy

183

Page 196: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.3.2.6. Self-efficacy for instructional strategies. Table 4.15 shows the results of

the multilevel models for self-efficacy for instructional strategies. Model 1 showed that

scores on the outcome varied significantly between participants (p < .001), and that the

between-person variance was significant (τ00 = 0.854, p < .001), as was the within-person

variance (σ2 = 0.291, p = .006). The addition of time as a fixed effect in Model 2a was not

significant (p = .830), nor was the addition of time2 (results not presented).

The conditional models (Models 3 and 4) revealed that relatedness with students was

a significant and positive predictor (p = .001) of the outcome variable. The addition of

relatedness with students in the final model explained 27% of the between-person variance

(τ00 was reduced from 0.854 in Model 2a to 0.620 in Model 4). The chi-square difference test

confirmed that the addition of relatedness with students in the model significantly improved

model fit: χ2 (1) = 52.65, p < .001. The equation used to graph the predicted values of self-

efficacy for instructional strategies was as follows:

Yti = π00 + π01Timeti + π03Relatedness with Studentsi

Figure 4.6 shows the relationship between self-efficacy for instructional strategies and

relatedness with students. Teachers reporting greater relatedness with students also reported

greater self-efficacy for instructional strategies.

184

Page 197: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.15

Multilevel Models for Examining Change in Self-Efficacy for Instructional Strategies Over

Time

Model 1 Model 2a Model 3 Model 4

Fixed effects

Intercept (π00) 7.200*** 7.184*** 7.221*** 7.213***

Timeti (π01) -0.014 -0.008 -0.009

Rel. with colleagues (π02)

0.172

Rel. with students (π03) 0.853** 0.982**

Competence (π04) 0.048

Autonomy (π05) 0.102

Random effects

τ00 0.854*** 0.854*** 0.567** 0.620**

σ2 0.291** 0.291** 0.325* 0.319*

Fit statistics

-2LL -133.511 -133.497 -107.958 -109.178

AIC 273.022 274.994 231.917 228.356 *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

185

Page 198: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Figure 4.6. Predicted values of self-efficacy for instructional strategies for different levels of

relatedness with students.

4.3.2.7. Summary of need satisfaction effects. Table 4.16 summarises the results of

the multilevel models showing the observed predictors, along with the original hypotheses.

As shown, Hypotheses 2a to 2e were partially supported. In contrast, no support was found

for Hypothesis 2f. Autonomy and relatedness with students had the most consistent effect,

having a significant relationship with the outcome variables in four and three models,

respectively. Relatedness with colleagues had a significant effect in one model (i.e.,

organisational well-being). The results for competence were unexpected. Although it was

hypothesised as having a significant relationship with all of the outcome variables, it was not

a significant effect in any of the models.

6.4

6.6

6.8

7

7.2

7.4

7.6

7.8

Low Average High

Self-

Effic

acy

for I

nstru

ctio

nal S

trate

gies

Relatedness with Students

186

Page 199: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Table 4.16

Hypothesised and Observed Significant Need Satisfaction Effects on the Outcome Variables

Hypothesis number

Outcome Hypothesised predictors Observed predictors

2a Workload well-being Competence Autonomy

Autonomy

2b Organisational well-being Relatedness with colleagues Competence Autonomy

Relatedness with colleagues Autonomy

2c Student interaction well-being Relatedness with students Competence Autonomy

Autonomy

2d Self-efficacy for student engagement Relatedness with students Competence

Relatedness with students

2e Self-efficacy for classroom management Relatedness with students Competence Autonomy

Relatedness with students Autonomy

2f Self-efficacy for instructional strategies Competence Autonomy

Relatedness with students

187

Page 200: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.4. Discussion

The need to improve our understanding of how teacher well-being and motivation

function over time is a key priority regularly highlighted in the literature (e.g., Klassen et al.,

2011). Study 3 aimed to address this issue by conducting growth curve modelling. Results

showed that self-reported well-being was stable (Hypothesis 1a), whereas self-efficacy for

classroom management showed a positive linear trend over time (Hypothesis 1b; self-

efficacy for student engagement and instructional strategies were both stable over time). In

addition, results showed that both well-being and self-efficacy were predicted by need

satisfaction (Hypotheses 2a through 2f). Key findings from the study are discussed below.

4.4.1. Self-efficacy for Classroom Management

The findings of the study showed that self-efficacy for classroom management

changed over time among the teachers in the sample and that it showed a positive linear trend

increasing over the three time points. There are several reasons why this may have occurred,

the first of which is a natural growth in teachers’ confidence as the school year progresses

and as they get to know their students better (e.g., they have more understanding of how to

best interact with individual students). Hence, the passing of two to three months between the

first and third time point of data collection may have been enough to precipitate this change

in self-efficacy for classroom management. Collie and colleagues’ (2012) research showing

that positive perceptions of student behaviour and motivation among teachers predicted

greater self-efficacy for teaching supports this interpretation, as does longitudinal research

showing positive changes in self-efficacy for teaching over the school year (e.g., Pas et al.,

2012).

188

Page 201: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Previous research examining the stability of self-efficacy over time has also

suggested that professional development can have a positive effect on self-efficacy (e.g.,

Carleton et al., 2008). Based on the reasons discussed earlier, however, the interpretations of

Carleton and colleagues’ (2008) research is hampered by the conceptually troubled

instrument that was used. Nonetheless, another possible explanation for the change in self-

efficacy found in Study 3 is teachers’ experiences of professional development during the

course of data collection. This may include the anti-oppression workshop in which they

participated as well as other professional development sessions they likely attended. In order

to determine whether professional development influences self-efficacy for teaching, there is

a need for intervention research involving multiple forms of data collection (e.g.,

questionnaires, observations, interviews). This will help us to identify whether and how

professional development influences self-efficacy for teaching.

Notwithstanding the fact that we can only speculate why the change occurred, the

finding broadens understanding of self-efficacy for classroom management in a couple of

key ways. First, it extends previous research that has looked at changes in self-efficacy for

teaching among early career teachers (e.g., Woolfolk Hoy & Spero, 2005) by revealing that

changes among more experienced teachers are possible. In that sense, it supports Klassen and

Chiu’s (2010) cross-sectional research showing a positive relationship between self-efficacy

for teaching and years of experiences up to the mid-point in the career, while extending this

to consider multiple time points. Second, this finding extends previous research that looked

at changes across longer time frames (e.g., Pas et al., 2012) by suggesting that self-efficacy

for classroom management can change over short periods of time. Furthermore, by using a

189

Page 202: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

conceptually sound instrument it provides much needed support for similar findings in

Carleton and colleagues’ (2008) study.

The implications of this finding extend to students, educators, and administrators.

Self-efficacy for teaching has been associated with several important outcomes for teachers

(e.g., work engagement; Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004) and students (e.g., student achievement;

Caprara et al., 2006). More specifically, self-efficacy for classroom management has been

negatively associated with teachers’ reports of stress, burnout (Brouwers & Tomic, 1999,

2000), and the use of punitive strategies for classroom management (Morin & Battalio,

2004). Consequently, efforts that can help to improve teacher self-efficacy affect not only the

teachers themselves, but their students’ learning and achievement. In addition, Collie and

colleagues (2012) showed that self-efficacy played a mediating role in the relationship

between teacher stress from student behaviour and job satisfaction. They concluded that

stress appears to have a detrimental effect on teachers when it is accompanied by low self-

efficacy. Efforts that can help to bolster teacher confidence should be considered by

administrators to help offset damage caused by teacher stress levels. This is particularly

important given that teaching is recognised as a highly stressful career (e.g., Kyriacou, 2000,

2001) and that this stress results in lower work engagement among teachers (Hakanen et al.,

2006) and lower learning motivation among students (Pakarinen et al., 2010).

4.4.2. Teacher Well-Being

The three types of teacher well-being showed no changes over time. For this finding,

it is possible that no changes occurred because teacher well-being is relatively stable over

short time periods. This is supported by the literature on subjective well-being (e.g.,

Schimmack & Lucas, 2010; Suh et al., 1996), which has shown that unless individuals are

190

Page 203: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

regularly engaged in positive activities designed to improve well-being it remains relatively

stable and returns to baseline levels within three months of any major life changes

(Lyubomirsky et al., 2011). An alternative explanation for why no changes in well-being

were observed over time relates to the level of statistical power in Study 3. Given the small

sample size, it is possible that non-significant findings were due to inadequate power. Future

research with larger sample sizes is needed to examine teacher well-being over time to see

whether similar results are found or whether time becomes significant with an increase in

power. The inclusion of questions asking teachers about any recent changes in work or life

circumstances is also a fruitful avenue for future research as it would help to improve our

understanding of whether and how life events affect teacher well-being in ways that are

similar to subjective well-being (i.e., minimally).

In spite of the concerns about statistical power, this finding is important for research

and practice. It extends our understanding of teacher well-being in two key ways. First, it

suggests that it acts differently from stress and burnout, at least in the short-term. Research

has revealed that stress and burnout change over the course of a school year (e.g., Pas et al.,

2012) and across even shorter time frames in the case of an intervention (e.g., Roeser et al.,

2013). This differential functioning between stress/burnout and well-being over time

provides support for the examination of teacher well-being as a distinct construct and not just

as the absence of stress or burnout. Second, this finding suggests that teacher well-being may

function in similar ways to individuals’ global assessments of their subjective well-being. In

other words, it may be a stable construct that is largely unaffected by everyday occurrences.

For practice, this finding suggests that in the short-term teacher well-being may be somewhat

robust in the face of negative events that occur in teaching (e.g., sudden increases in

191

Page 204: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

disruptive behaviour among students). The reverse of this, however, is that teachers reporting

low well-being may not experience a long-term or significant increase in well-being when

positive events occur (e.g., an increase in salary). This raises concerns about how to improve

teacher well-being over the long term and, thus, highlights the importance of considering

more stable, contextual factors like need satisfaction so as to promote lasting improvements

in teacher well-being.

Another implication of these findings is that they provide further support for the three

factors of teacher well-being developed in Study 1 with a different sample of teachers.

Briefly, scores on workload well-being were much lower than scores on organisational and

student interaction well-being. This corroborates similar findings from Study 1 and provides

further evidence for the conceptual meaning of the factors. In addition, it reiterates the fact

the workload generally affects teacher well-being less positively than organisational issues

and student interaction.

4.4.3. Need Satisfaction

The inclusion of the need satisfaction variables as time-invariant predictors in the

multilevel models explained significant amounts of between-level variance for all models. In

all cases, need satisfaction predicted higher levels of well-being and self-efficacy. In

particular, satisfaction of the needs for autonomy and relatedness with students had the most

consistent effect on the outcome variables. Autonomy had a significant relationship with

workload well-being, organisational well-being, student interaction well-being, and self-

efficacy for classroom management. These relationships were expected and likely occurred

because autonomy allows teachers to feel a sense of choice and control over their workload,

their role in the school, how they interact with students, and how they manage their

192

Page 205: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

classroom. These findings are supported by previous research showing links between

autonomy and other measures of well-being (i.e., a negative association with stress; Collie et

al., 2012) and motivation (i.e., a positive association with work engagement; Klassen, Perry,

et al., 2012).

Relatedness with students had, as expected, a significant relationship with self-

efficacy for student engagement and classroom management. Contrary to expectations, it

also had a significant relationship with self-efficacy for instructional strategies. For these

findings, it is possible that a positive connection with students allows teachers to feel more

confident in their ability to engage, manage, and effectively instruct those students. Indeed,

several scholars (Hamre & Pianta, 2006; Jennings & Greenberg, 2009; Martin & Dowson,

2009) have highlighted the importance of positive teacher-student relationships for student

engagement, effective classroom management, and effective teaching and learning, which

likely explains the unexpected relationship between relatedness with students and self-

efficacy for instructional strategies.

These findings extend our understanding of how autonomy and relatedness with

students are associated with teacher well-being and self-efficacy. They support Klassen,

Perry, et al.’s (2012) assertion of the relative importance of relatedness with students over

relatedness with colleagues for explaining teacher outcomes (i.e., relatedness with colleagues

only predicted one outcome—organisational well-being). For well-being, the findings

corroborate results from Study 2 that linked autonomy with a composite teacher well-being

variable, while also extending our understanding by revealing relationships from autonomy

to each of the three separate factors of teacher well-being. For self-efficacy, the findings add

193

Page 206: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

to our understanding of the needs for autonomy and relatedness with students by showing

that they are relevant for self-efficacy.

These findings are also important for administrators and policy-makers who should

be aware of the significant role that autonomy and relatedness with students play in teacher

well-being and self-efficacy. In particular, the findings support the need for efforts at the

school-level to create autonomy-supportive school climates. Furthermore, they suggest that

administrators and policy-makers should pay greater attention to the importance of positive

teacher-student relationships. The final need satisfaction variable to be discussed briefly is

competence. Contrary to expectations it was not significantly related to any of the outcomes.

This is intriguing given the significant relationship found in Study 2 for well-being. Perhaps

it speaks to the small sample size or some uniqueness about the sample of teachers in Study

3.

For self-efficacy, perhaps competence was not related because it is more aligned with

self-concept and ability based on past/current experiences (e.g., I am competent at my job),

rather than judgements of future ability that are context-specific (e.g., I believe can do a lot

to control disruptive behaviour in my classroom). For example, if a teacher has worked for

many years, he or she may feel competent in the job. However, if the teacher happens to have

a particularly difficult class in one year, his or her confidence about how much can be done

to engage that group of students or manage the classroom may be quite low. Future research

is needed that examines this relationship among larger samples and in more detail, including

investigation of whether a reversed relationship between these variables is more relevant.

194

Page 207: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

4.5. Limitations and Future Directions

Study 3 has several limitations that must be discussed. The first concerns the sample

size. Although significant effects were found, a larger sample size would have provided more

power and given stronger assurances that non-significant effects were not due to low power.

In order to provide further support for the findings, samples involving a larger number of

teachers are a necessary step in future research. This will provide more conclusive evidence

of whether teacher well-being is a stable construct (like subjective well-being) or whether

changes can be expected over time.

The second limitation concerns the time frame. Although the short time frame did not

affect the findings per se, it did not allow me to examine the impact of the different stages of

a school year (beginning, end, report writing, etc.). Given that different points in the school

year are associated with unique and changing demands on teachers, there is a need for

research that examines these variables over the course of a full school year (and beyond)

with more waves of data collection. This would provide a greater understanding of whether

and how teacher well-being and motivation fluctuate across (and beyond) a school year,

along with the influence of the changing contextual and personal experiences of teachers. In

spite of this limitation, the current findings do provide important understanding about the

flexibility of these constructs in the short term.

Another limitation is that I was not able to ascertain why teachers dropped out of

Study 3 and whether they were identifiable by a confounding variable (e.g., whether they

were too busy to fill out the questionnaire which could affect workload well-being scores).

The findings may be conservative, therefore, in that only teachers functioning reasonably

well were able to continue participating in the study. The statistical comparisons of teachers

195

Page 208: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

who remained in the study with those who dropped out provide evidence that there were no

differences between the groups at Time 1; however, we do not know if any relevant changes

occurred after this point (e.g., because of a negative experience with the workshop or major

changes in teachers well-being or motivation). In the future, efforts that attempt to track why

participants drop out will be helpful to rule out and/or help understand reasons for attrition.

This will also provide greater understanding of the representativeness of the data provided by

participants involved in all waves of data collection.

The final limitation to be discussed is that the participants were recruited through the

booking process of a professional development program. As such, the sample may have been

unique in some way compared with the broader population. The fact that reports of well-

being and self-efficacy were comparable with previous research (e.g., Study 1 and 2 in this

dissertation; Collie et al., 2012) provides support for the generalisability of the findings.

Nonetheless, there is a need for research that uses different samples of teachers in order to

corroborate the findings of Study 3. In addition, it would be interesting to involve groups of

teachers at different careers stages so as to understand how years of teaching experience

influence changes in well-being and self-efficacy for teaching (e.g., whether changes in these

constructs occur more readily among early career teachers than more experienced teachers).

4.6. Conclusions

In summary, Study 3 has involved examining how reports of teacher well-being and

self-efficacy for teaching functioned over two to three months. This type of research provides

an important addition to the understanding we have gained from studies conducted at one

point in time. By using multilevel modelling, results revealed a positive change in self-

efficacy for classroom management over time; however, there were no changes over time for

196

Page 209: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

the remaining variables. Also investigated was the influence of basic psychological need

satisfaction on teachers’ initial reports of well-being and self-efficacy. The results showed

that teachers’ sense of autonomy, relatedness with students, and relatedness with colleagues

predicted greater well-being and self-efficacy among the participants. Combined, these

findings extend our understanding of the flexibility of self-efficacy for classroom

management over short time periods. In addition, they indicate that well-being is a stable

construct as measured by the TWBS. Study 3, along with the other two studies, provides

important implications for research and practice. These are discussed in the General

Discussion below.

197

Page 210: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

CHAPTER 5. GENERAL DISCUSSION

The aim of the dissertation research was to provide a comprehensive examination of

teacher well-being and motivation. Three related studies were conducted that developed a

measure of well-being, tested an explanatory model of teachers’ experiences of well-being,

motivation, job satisfaction, and organisational commitment, and examined teachers’ reports

of well-being and motivation over time. More specifically, the first study involved

developing and testing a measure of teacher well-being based on teachers’ experiences at

work. Results revealed three factors of teacher well-being. The first was workload well-

being, which refers to teachers’ perceptions of the amount of marking, teaching, and

administrative work they have, the work/meetings they have to do after school hours, and

their ability to fit everything into the allotted time. The second was organisational well-

being, which refers to teachers’ perceptions of support offered by and relations with school

leadership, the culture towards teachers and teaching, and the level of participation they have

in decision making. The third was student interaction well-being, which refers to teachers’

perceptions of student motivation, student behaviour, classroom management, and their

relations with students. Comparisons showed that demographic characteristics did not play a

substantial role in determining scores on the three factors of well-being, indicating that the

measure functions similarly across the different demographic groups that were present in the

sample of teachers. Furthermore, correlational analyses provided support for the three factors

of well-being showing that they were related as expected with three other measures of

teachers’ experiences: stress, job satisfaction, and flourishing (i.e., general life well-being).

The second study involved describing and testing an explanatory model of teacher

well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment. The

198

Page 211: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

results provided support for the model. They showed that the relationships established by

SDT were supported: perceptions of autonomy support predicted need satisfaction that, in

turn, predicted well-being and motivation. Furthermore, the relationships involving well-

being, motivation, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment were generally

supported. Namely, teachers’ reports of autonomous motivation (measured as identified

regulation) and well-being were positively associated with job satisfaction and affective

organisational commitment. There were some unexpected relationships, however, involving

autonomy and controlled motivation (measured as introjected regulation and external

regulation), as well as between controlled motivation, job satisfaction, and affective

organisational commitment.

The third study involved an examination of teachers’ reports of well-being and self-

efficacy for teaching over time. Results showed that self-efficacy for classroom management

changed over time, whereas the three types of teacher well-being (i.e., workload,

organisational, and student interaction well-being) and the other two types of self-efficacy

(i.e., self-efficacy for student engagement and instructional strategies) did not. Findings also

revealed the significance of basic psychological need satisfaction in predicting teacher well-

being and self-efficacy. In particular, need satisfaction of autonomy, relatedness with

students, and relatedness with colleagues positively predicted greater well-being and self-

efficacy for teaching.

Taken together the findings from these three studies advance knowledge in the field

of teacher well-being and motivation. Before discussing the major contributions of the

dissertation research, however, there are several findings that refer to the dissertation as a

whole and warrant discussion first. The first was support found for the Teacher Well-Being

199

Page 212: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Scale (TWBS) developed in Study 1. The factor analyses and correlations in Study 1, along

with the SEM in Study 2, provided validity evidence based on internal structure and relations

to other variables. Furthermore, the multilevel analyses in Study 3 provided further support

for the three factors by revealing their stability over time and how they relate to need

satisfaction. Taken together, these findings provide important initial evidence of validity,

including reliability, to support the interpretations of scores on the TWBS as a measure of

teacher well-being. Future research is needed to examine the measure among different

samples and in relation to other constructs to provide further evidence of validity.

The second finding relevant across the research as a whole was the relationship that

sense of need satisfaction for autonomy had with reported motivation. The results of Study 2

revealed a double-sided view of autonomy. It was positively associated with autonomous

motivation and controlled motivation suggesting that high autonomy can be reported by

teachers acting with very different motivations (e.g., “I do this job for the paycheque”, which

is associated with external regulation, or “because this job fits my personal values”, which is

associated with identified regulation; Gagné et al., 2010). The findings of Study 3 revealed a

positive relationship between experiences of autonomy and self-efficacy: autonomy

predicted greater self-efficacy for student engagement, classroom management, and

instructional strategies. Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of examining

all three needs simultaneously in both research and practice. That is, if we focus only on

promoting autonomy we may be supporting not only identified regulation and self-efficacy

for teaching, but also external regulation (i.e., because autonomy positively predicted both

positive and negative forms of motivation). Given that external regulation was negatively

associated with the needs for relatedness and competence, a focus on promoting all three

200

Page 213: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

needs simultaneously will help to ensure that we are encouraging teachers to move towards

positive forms of motivation and away from external regulation.

This finding also highlights the need for future research using person-centred

approaches to examine teachers’ experiences. Person-centred approaches examine

differences among individuals in how variables relate to one another (Laursen & Hoff,

2006). Latent profile analysis and cluster analysis are two means for providing understanding

of the different groups of teachers in the population and how they experience the different

constructs. For example, these techniques would allow for the identification of groups of

teachers, such as those reporting high autonomy but also high external regulation, in order to

understand predictors and outcomes that we can expect among this group.

The third common finding reflected across the three studies was that contextual

factors need to be considered when attempting to understand teachers’ experiences at work.

For the development of the TWBS in Study 1, contextual factors made up the bulk of all

items in the final subscales (e.g., “Support offered by school leadership”). In Study 2,

contextual factors of perceived autonomy support and need satisfaction were central in

teachers’ reports of their well-being and motivation. Finally, in Study 3 the contextual factors

of need satisfaction provided important depth to our understanding of teacher well-being and

self-efficacy. This extends previous research that has highlighted the importance of school

climate for teacher outcomes (e.g., Collie et al., 2012; Grayson & Alvarez, 2008) and

indicates the importance of considering a wide range of contextual experiences—at both the

classroom and school level—when attempting to understand and promote teacher well-being

and motivation.

201

Page 214: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

5.1. Conclusions and Implications

The overall purpose of the dissertation research was to address several significant

gaps in the literature through a three-part examination of teacher well-being and motivation.

Various researchers have indicated the need for educational psychology research to focus on

advancing our scientific understanding, while also providing useful and practical applications

that are accessible to schools and relevant stakeholders (e.g., Martin, 2007; Pintrich, 2000).

This dissertation has aimed to address both of these goals and they are discussed below in

turn, along with implications for research and practice.

5.1.1. Contributions for Scientific Understanding

The main contribution of this dissertation research is that it has extended our

understanding of the constructs of teacher well-being and motivation in several key ways. In

particular, the findings have provided support for a multidimensional construct of teacher

well-being that is distinct from related variables such as stress, job satisfaction, and

flourishing (i.e., general life well-being). This is important because it highlights how

different aspects of teachers’ work have varying polarities of influence on their well-being.

In particular, the findings have highlighted the negative influence of workload, and the

positive influence of organisational-level and student-related aspects of teachers’ work on

their well-being. An implication of this contribution is that it has highlighted the value of

allowing participants to respond in ways that consider the full range of influences (both the

positive and negative). By allowing this, the TWBS provided a more complete picture of

participants’ experiences than would have been possible if considering only the negative or

positive influence of the items.

202

Page 215: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

In relation to motivation, the findings have provided corroborating evidence to

support the relevance of the different types of regulation from SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985,

2002) for understanding teacher motivation, while at the same time raising questions about

the psychometric differences between intrinsic regulation and job satisfaction. Indeed, the

findings indicate the need for more nuanced methods for assessing the differences between

these constructs (e.g., interviews, more sensitive measures).

The dissertation has also provided evidence of how teacher well-being and self-

efficacy function over time. Given that teacher well-being appeared to be stable over time, it

may not only be robust to negative events, but also to positive events or efforts to improve it

(practical implications of this are discussed below). This indicates the need to examine

teacher well-being in greater detail to understand whether other factors can help to improve it

over the long term. Future research examining need satisfaction as a time-varying covariate

would help to extend our understanding in this area as it would provide an indication of

whether and how changes in need satisfaction influence changes in well-being. Additional

avenues of research that would be revealing include attempts to control for individual base-

line levels of well-being and examinations of whether regular well-being exercises can help

to improve teacher well-being (as per Roeser et al., 2013).

For self-efficacy, the findings have extended our understanding about the flexibility

of self-efficacy for classroom management over a relatively short time frame. This finding is

important given that self-efficacy appears to buffer the negative effects of stress (e.g., Collie

et al., 2012) and given that it is positively associated with student achievement (e.g., Caprara

et al., 2006). Moving forward, research that examines why this occurred (e.g., by conducting

203

Page 216: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

interviews with participants) and whether this increase holds over a longer time frame would

further advance our understanding.

Another contribution of the dissertation research is that it has extended our

understanding of how teachers’ experiences of well-being and motivation relate to other

important constructs (e.g., job satisfaction). Where previous research has revealed that these

constructs are related (e.g., Hakanen et al., 2006; Klassen & Chui, 2011), this dissertation

research has shown how these constructs are related via the explanatory model. In addition,

the dissertation has extended previous research on job satisfaction by highlighting that

motivation may be an important determinant in whether or not job satisfaction is relevant for

certain teachers. Furthermore, the findings provide a greater understanding of autonomy, the

relationship this can have with different types of motivation, and the need for more sensitive

measures that differentiate between the positive and negative reasons underlying autonomy.

Taken together, these findings have underscored the complexity of teachers’ work and the

value of considering multiple constructs simultaneously in order to gain a more complete

picture of teachers’ experiences (Collie et al., 2012).

This dissertation research has also provided evidence of the relevance of existing

theory for understanding teachers’ experiences at work. In particular, the findings have

provided corroborating evidence for previous research using SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985,

2002) to examine teachers’ experiences at work (e.g., Klassen, Perry, et al., 2012; Roth et al.,

2007; Taylor et al., 2008). Moreover, it has extended the literature by suggesting that SDT is

an appropriate framework for examining how well-being and motivation relate to job

satisfaction and affective organisational commitment. In addition, the study raises several

204

Page 217: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

questions for future research about the causes and correlates of introjected regulation among

teachers.

Another area in which this dissertation provides research contributions is in relation

to contextual factors. In particular, the findings have deepened our understanding of the

contexts within which teachers work and how these contexts can influence teacher well-

being and motivation. The dissertation research has revealed that different contextual factors

have varying degrees of importance for teachers’ reports of well-being and motivation. It has

revealed that contextual factors such as perceptions of principals’ autonomy support and

interactions with students played substantial roles in teachers’ experiences of well-being and

motivation, as well as other outcomes (e.g., need satisfaction, job satisfaction).

The findings have also revealed that the examined demographic characteristics did

not play a major role in determining teachers’ reports of well-being. In fact, the findings

indicate that researchers may want to consider other demographic characteristics (e.g.,

teaching positions) and other factors (e.g., beliefs) when attempting to understand what

drives differences in teacher well-being. There is a need to conduct research among different

samples of teachers to understand whether demographic variables play a more substantial

role among more diverse populations. Notwithstanding this, an important contribution of the

dissertation research is that it suggests that efforts by administrators and policy makers to

improve teacher well-being should have a broad influence among teachers who are

demographically similar to the participants who were involved in this research. Additional

practical contributions of the dissertation are discussed below.

205

Page 218: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

5.1.2. Contributions for Practice

The findings of this dissertation research provide several important contributions for

practice. Overall, it is my hope that the findings will help to cultivate awareness of the

importance of teacher well-being and motivation and, in turn, promote supportive and

positive work experiences for teachers. The first contribution relevant to practice concerns

the development of the TWBS. The practical nature of the instrument—involving the

examination of core aspects of teachers’ work—means that it can be used to inform practice.

Potentially, it could be used by school administrators, policy-makers, and even educators

themselves in order to understand what aspects of teaching work positively or negatively

influence teacher well-being. Thus, while extending our understanding of teacher well-being,

possible uses of the TWBS also extend to diagnosis and intervention.

The second contribution relevant to practice concerns the explanatory model of

teacher well-being, motivation, job satisfaction, and affective organisational commitment.

The model provides a framework through which school administrators and policy-makers

can better understand the experiences of well-being and motivation among teachers. It also

describes how these experiences are affected by and, in turn, affect other experiences such as

need satisfaction and job satisfaction. Consequently, the model has the potential to help

guide efforts to promote positive work experiences among teachers. Given the links between

teacher well-being/motivation and student outcomes (e.g., achievement; Caprara et al.,

2006), such efforts also have the potential to positively impact students.

The findings concerning changes in teachers’ reports of well-being and self-efficacy

over time, although more removed from direct practical application, do provide relevant

contributions. In particular, they provide an understanding of what types of changes can be

206

Page 219: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

expected in teacher well-being and self-efficacy across relatively short time frames. This is

important for school administrators and policy-makers who want to ensure that professional

development efforts have the potential to affect change among teachers. More specifically,

the positive change over time observed in self-efficacy for classroom management suggests

that interventions designed to improve teachers’ skills in this area can realistically expect to

result in changes in teachers’ confidence over short periods of time. Considering that self-

efficacy for teaching is associated with teaching effectiveness (e.g., Cho & Shim, 2013), this

is a worthwhile construct to target in the attempt to improve teaching quality.

In contrast, the findings for well-being suggest that perhaps changes over a short

period of time cannot be expected. This has positive and negative ramifications for practice

given that teacher well-being may not drop significantly when troubling events occur, but

that it may not rise when efforts are made to improve work circumstances for teachers. The

small sample size may have been partially responsible for no significant changes in well-

being. Additional research with larger sample sizes is needed, therefore, along with research

that investigates whether increases in teacher well-being can be fostered over the short- and

long-term through other methods (e.g., basic psychological need support; regular well-being

exercises as per Roeser et al., 2013).

5.1.3. Final Conclusions

Taken together, this dissertation research has important implications for research and

practice regarding teacher well-being and motivation. However, it is also important to

mention that many, if not all, of these implications ultimately relate to students as well. The

contributions of the dissertation to our understanding of teacher well-being and motivation

extend to students given that these variables are related to teaching effectiveness (Duckworth

207

Page 220: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

et al., 2009), resiliency in teaching (Klusmann et al., 2008), students’ motivation (Pakarinen

et al., 2010; Taylor & Ntoumanis, 2007) and students’ achievement (Caprara et al., 2006).

Furthermore, the practical implications and applications that this dissertation research can

help promote and guide also have the potential to affect students in positive ways. Future

research that examines links between the constructs examined in this dissertation (i.e.,

teacher well-being, autonomous motivation, self-efficacy for teaching, job satisfaction, and

organisational commitment) and student outcomes more directly will help to extend our

knowledge of how teachers’ experiences relate to students’ outcomes and, ultimately, how

we can best support students’ academic, social, and emotional development.

208

Page 221: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

REFERENCES

Albuquerque, I., Pedroso de Lima, M., Figueiredo, C., & Matos, M. (2011). Subjective well-being structure: Confirmatory factor analysis in a teachers’ Portuguese sample. Social Indicators Research, Advance online publication. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9789-6

Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1996). Affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization: An examination of construct validity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 49, 252-276. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.1996.0043

American Psychological Association, American Educational Research Association, and National Council on Measurement in Education. (1999). Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2005) Multivariate statistical modeling with survey data. Proceedings of the Federal Committee on Statistical Methodology Research Conference. Retrieved from http://www.fcsm.gov/05papers/Asparouhov_Muthen_IIA.pdf

Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Intrinsic need satisfaction: A motivational basis of performance and well-being in two work settings. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 2045-2068.

Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122-147. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.37.2.122

Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44, 1175-1184. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.9.1175

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.

Barnabé, C., & Burns, M. (1994). Teachers' job characteristics and motivation. Educational Research, 36, 171.

Beretvas, S. N., Meyers, J. L., & Leite, W. L. (2002). A reliability generalization study of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62, 570-589.

Billingsley, B. S., & Cross, L. H. (1992). Predictors of commitment, job satisfaction, and intent to stay in teaching: A comparison of general and special educators. Journal of Special Education, 25, 453-471.

Black, A. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of instructors' autonomy support and students' autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: A self-determination theory perspective. Science Education, 84, 740-756.

Bogler, R., & Somech, A. (2004). Influence of teacher empowerment on teachers’ organisational commitment, professional commitment and organisational citizenship

209

Page 222: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

behaviour in schools. Teaching and Teacher Education, 20, 277-289. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2004.02.003

Borg, M. G., & Riding, R. J. (1991). Occupational stress and satisfaction in teaching. British Educational Research Journal, 17, 263.

Bowling, N., Eschleman, K. J., & Wang, Q. (2010). A meta-analytic examination of the relationship between job satisfaction and subjective well-being. Journal of Occupational and Organisational Psychology, 83, 915-934. doi: 10.1348/096317909X478557

Boyle, G. J., Borg, M. G., Falzon, J. M., & Baglioni, A. J. (1995). A structural model of the dimensions of teacher stress. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 49-67.

Brackett, M., Palomera, R., Mojsa-Kaja, J., Reyes, M. R., & Salovey, P. (2010). Emotion-regulation ability, burnout, and job satisfaction among British secondary-school teachers. Psychology in the Schools, 47, 406-417. doi: 10.1002/pits.20478

Brenner, C. A., Perry, N. E., & Collie, R. J. (2012, September). Student teachers' developing practices that promote self-regulated learning: Linking efficacy and utility beliefs to effectiveness. In N. Perry & B. Kramarski (Co-chairs), Metacognition and self-regulation in developing professionals. Symposium presented at the biennial meeting of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, Milan, Italy.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1978). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1994). Ecological models of human development. International Encyclopedia of Education (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 1643-1647). Oxford, England: Elsevier Sciences.

Brouwers, A., & Tomic, W. (1999). Teacher burnout, perceived self-efficacy in classroom management, and student disruptive behaviour in secondary education. Curriculum and Teaching, 14, 7-26.

Brouwers, A., & Tomic, W. (2000). A longitudinal study of teacher burnout and perceived self-efficacy in classroom management. Teaching and Teacher Education, 16, 239-253. doi: 10.1016/S0742-051X(99)00057-8

Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York, NY: The Guildford Press.

Bryk, A. S., & Schneider, B. (2003). Trust in schools: A core resource for school reform. Educational Leadership, 60, 40-44.

Burke, R. J., & Greenglass, E. (1995). A longitudinal study of psychological burnout in teachers. Human Relations, 48, 187-202. doi: 10.1177/001872679504800205

210

Page 223: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Butler, R. (2007). Teachers' achievement goal orientations and associations with teachers' help seeking: Examination of a novel approach to teacher motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 241-252. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.241

Butler, R., & Shibaz, L. (2008). Achievement goals for teaching as predictors of students' perceptions of instructional practices and students' help seeking and cheating. Learning and Instruction, 18, 453-467. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.06.004

Butt, G., Lance, A., Fielding, A., Gunter, H., Rayner, S., & Thomas, H. (2005). Teacher job satisfaction: Lessons from the TSW pathfinder project. School Leadership and Management, 25, 455-471. doi: 10.1080/13634230500340807

Canrinus, E. T., Helms-Lorenz, M., Beijaard, D., Buitink, J., & Hofman, A. (2012). Self-efficacy, job satisfaction, motivation and commitment: Exploring the relationships between indicators of teachers’ professional identity. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 27, 115-132. doi: 10.1007/s10212-011-0069-2

Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Borgogni, L., & Steca, P. (2003). Efficacy beliefs as determinants of teachers' job satisfaction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 821-832. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.821

Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Steca, P., & Malone, P. S. (2006). Teachers' self-efficacy beliefs as determinants of job satisfaction and students' academic achievement: A study at the school level. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 473-490. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.09.001

Carleton, L. E., Fitch, J. C., & Krockover, G. H. (2008). An in-service teacher education program's effect on teacher efficacy and attitudes. Educational Forum, 72, 46-62. doi: 10.1080/00131720701603628

Chan, D. W. (1998). Stress, coping strategies, and psychological distress among secondary school teachers in Hong Kong. American Educational Research Journal, 35, 145-163.

Chaplain, R. P. (2008). Stress and psychological distress among trainee secondary teachers in England. Educational Psychology, 28, 195-209.

Cho, Y., & Shim, S. S. (2013). Predicting teachers’ achievement goals for teaching: The role of perceived school goal structure and teachers’ sense of efficacy. Teaching and Teacher Education, 32, 12-21.

Clunies-Ross, P., Little, E., & Kienhuis, M. (2008). Self-reported and actual use of proactive and reactive classroom management strategies and their relationship with teacher stress and student behaviour. Educational Psychology, 28, 693-710. doi: 10.1080/01443410802206700

Collie, R. J. (2010). Social and emotional learning and school climate: Predictors of teacher stress, job satisfaction, and sense of efficacy (Unpublished master’s thesis). University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada.

211

Page 224: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Collie, R. J., Perry, N. E., & Brenner, C. A. (2013, August). Does stress have to be bad? Examining stress and coping among early career teachers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, Munich, Germany.

Collie, R. J., Shapka, J. D., & Perry, N. E. (2011). Predicting teacher commitment: The impact of school climate and social-emotional learning. Psychology in the Schools, 48, 1034-1048.

Collie, R. J., Shapka, J. D., & Perry, N. E. (2012). School climate and social–emotional learning: Predicting teacher stress, job satisfaction, and teaching efficacy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 1189-1204. doi: 10.1037/a0029356

Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 98-104.

Curran, P. J., Obeidat, K., & Losardo, D. (2010). Twelve frequently asked questions about growth curve modeling. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 121-136. doi:10.1080/15248371003699969

Darling-Hammond, L. (2000). Solving the demands of teacher supply, demand and standards: How we can ensure a competent, caring, and qualified teacher for every child. New York: National Commission on Teaching and America's Future.

Day, C., Elliot, B., & Kington, A. (2005). Reform, standards and teacher identity: Challenges of sustaining commitment. Teaching and Teacher Education, 21, 563-577. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2005.03.001

Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994). Facilitating internalisation: The self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119-142.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behaviour. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (Eds.). (2002). Handbook of self-determination research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008a). Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well-being across life's domains. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 49, 14-23. doi: 10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.14

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008b). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: An introduction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 1-11. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008c). Self-determination theory: A macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 49, 182-185. doi: 10.1037/a0012801

212

Page 225: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Motivation, personality, and development within embedded social contexts: An overview of self-determination theory. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The oxford handbook of human motivation (pp. 85-110). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagné, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., & Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satisfaction, motivation, and well-being in the work organisations of a former eastern bloc country: A cross-cultural study of self-determination. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 930-942. doi: 10.1177/0146167201278002

Deci, E. L., Schwartz, A. J., Sheinman, L., & Ryan, R. M. (1981). An instrument to assess adults' orientations toward control versus autonomy with children: Reflections on intrinsic motivation and perceived competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 642-650. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.73.5.642

Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). Motivation and education: The self-determination perspective. Educational Psychologist, 26, 325.

Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2001). The job demands-resources model of burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 499-512. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.499

Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542-575. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542

Diener, E. (2009). Conclusion―The well-being science needed now. Social Indicators Research Series, 37, 267-271. doi: 10.1007/978-90-481-2350-6 1

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71-75.

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276-302. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276

Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2010). New well-being measures: Short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Social Indicators Research, 97, 143-156. doi: 10.1007/s11205-009-9493-y

Dinham, S., & Scott, C. (1998). A three domain model of teacher and school executive career satisfaction. Journal of Educational Administration, 36, 319-331.

Duckworth, A. L., Quinn, P. D., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2009). Positive predictors of teacher effectiveness. Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 540-547.

213

Page 226: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Feather, N. T., & Rauter, K. A. (2004). Organizational citizenship behaviours in relation to job status, job insecurity, organizational commitment and identification, job satisfaction and work values. Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology, 77, 81-94.

Fernet, C., Guay, F., & Senécal, C. (2004). Adjusting to job demands: The role of work self-determination and job control in predicting burnout. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 65, 39-56. doi: 10.1016/S0001-8791(03)00098-8

Filak, V. F., & Sheldon, K. M. (2008). Teacher support, student motivation, student need satisfaction, and college teacher course evaluations: Testing a sequential path model. Educational Psychology, 28, 711-724. doi: 10.1080/01443410802337794

Fischer, D. G., & Fick, C. (1993). Measuring social desirability: Short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53, 417-424. doi: 10.1177/0013164493053002011

Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Guay, F. (1995). Academic motivation and school performance: Toward a structural model. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 257-274.

Friedman, I. A., & Kass, E. (2002). Teacher self-efficacy: A classroom-organization conceptualization. Teaching and Teacher Education, 18, 675-686.

Gagné, M., Chemolli, E., Forest, J., & Koestner, R. (2008). A temporal analysis of the relation between organisational commitment and work motivation. Psychologica Belgica, 48, 219-241.

Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination theory and work motivation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 331-362. doi: 10.1002/job.322

Gagné, M., Forest, J., Gilbert, M., Aubé, C., Morin, E., & Malorni, A. (2010). The motivation at work scale: Validation evidence in two languages. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 70, 628-646. doi: 10.1177/0013164409355698

Gagné, M., Ryan, R. M., & Bargmann, K. (2003). Autonomy support and need satisfaction in the motivation and well-being of gymnasts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15, 372-390. doi: 10.1080/714044203

Geving, A. M. (2007). Identifying the types of student and teacher behaviours associated with teacher stress. Teaching and Teacher Education, 23, 624-640. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2007.02.006

Gibson, S., & Dembo, M. H. (1984). Teacher efficacy: A construct validation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 569-582. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.76.4.569

214

Page 227: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Gillison, F., Osborn, M., Standage, M., & Skevington, S. (2009). Exploring the experience of introjected regulation for exercise across gender in adolescence. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 309-319. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2008.10.004

Grayson, J. L., & Alvarez, H. K. (2008). School climate factors relating to teacher burnout: A mediator model. Teaching and Teacher Education, 24, 1349-1363. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2007.06.005

Hakanen, J. J., Bakker, A. B., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2006). Burnout and work engagement among teachers. Journal of School Psychology, 43, 495-513. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2005.11.001

Hakanen, J. J., Schaufeli, W. B., & Ahola, K. (2008). The job demands-resources model: A three-year cross-lagged study of burnout, depression, commitment, and work engagement. Work & Stress, 22, 224-241. doi:10.1080/02678370802379432

Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2006). Student-teacher relationships. In G. G. Bear, & K. Minke (Eds.), Children's needs III: Development, prevention, and intervention (pp. 59-71). Bethesda, MD: NASP.

Hayton, J. C., Allen, D. G., & Scarpello, V. (2004). Factor retention decisions in exploratory factor analysis: A tutorial on parallel analysis. Organizational Research Methods, 7, 191-205. doi: 10.1177/1094428104263675

Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. (1959). The motivation to work. New York: John Wiley.

Hobfall, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. American Psychologist, 44, 513-524.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 55.

Huppert, F. A., & So, T. T. (2013). Flourishing across Europe: Application of a new conceptual framework for defining well-being. Social Indices Research, 110, 837-861.

Ilardi, B. C., Leone, D., Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). Employee and supervisor ratings of motivation: Main effects and discrepancies associated with job satisfaction and adjustment in a factory setting. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1789-1805. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01066.x

Ingersoll, R. M. (2002). Holes in the teacher supply bucket. School Administrator, 59, 42-43.

Ingersoll, R. M., & May, H. (2012). The magnitude, destinations, and determinants of mathematics and science teacher turnover. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 34, 435-464. doi:10.3102/0162373712454326

215

Page 228: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Jackson, L. T. B., Rothmann, S., & van de Vijver (2006). A model of work-related well-being for educators in South Africa. Stress & Health: Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress, 22, 263-274. doi: 10.1002/smi.1098

Jamal, M. (1990). Relationship of job stress and type-A behavior to employees' job satisfaction, organizational commitment, psychosomatic health problems, and turnover motivation. Human Relations, 43 727-738. doi: 10.1177/001872679004300802

Jang, H., Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (2010). Engaging students in learning activities: It is not autonomy support or structure but autonomy support and structure. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 588-600. doi: 10.1037/a0019682

Jennings, P. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (2009). The prosocial classroom: Teacher social and emotional competence in relation to student and classroom outcomes. Review of Educational Research, 79, 491-525. doi: 10.3102/0034654308325693

Jepson, E., & Forrest, S. (2006). Individual contributory factors in teacher stress: The role of achievement striving and occupational commitment. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 183-197. doi: 10.1348/000709905X37299

Joinson, A. (1999). Social desirability, anonymity, and internet-based questionnaires. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 31, 433-438.

Karabenick, S. A., Woolley, M. E., Friedel, J. M., Ammon, B. V., Blazevski, J., Rhee Bonney, C., … & Kelly, K. L. (2007). Cognitive processing of self-report items in educational research: Do they think what we mean? Educational Psychologist, 42, 139-151.

Klassen, R. M., Aldhafri, S., Mansfield, C., Purwanto, E., Siu, a. F. Y., Wong, M. W., Woods-McConnery, A. (2012). Teachers’ engagement at work: An internationals validation study. The Journal of Experimental Education, 80, 317-337. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2012.678409

Klassen, R. M., Bong, M., Usher, E. L., Chong, W. H., Huan, V. S., Wong, I. Y. F., & Georgiou, T. (2009). Exploring the validity of a teachers’ self-efficacy scale in five countries. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 67-76. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2008.08.001

Klassen, R. M., & Chiu, M. M. (2010). Effects on teachers’ self-efficacy and job satisfaction: Teacher gender, years of experience, and job stress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 741-756. doi: 10.1037/a0019237

Klassen, R. M., & Chiu, M. M. (2011). The occupational commitment and intention to quit of practicing and pre-service teachers: Influence of self-efficacy, job stress, and teaching context. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 114-129. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.01.002

216

Page 229: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Klassen, R. M., Perry, N. E., & Frenzel, A. C. (2012). Teachers' relatedness with students: An underemphasized component of teachers' basic psychological needs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 150-165. doi: 10.1037/a0026253

Klassen, R. M., Tze, V. M. C., Betts, S. M., & Gordon, K. A. (2011). Teacher efficacy research 1998–2009: Signs of progress or unfulfilled promise? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 21-43.

Klassen, R. M., Usher, E. L., & Bong, M. (2010). Teachers’ collective efficacy, job satisfaction, and job stress in cross-cultural context. Journal of Experimental Education, 78, 464-486. doi: 10.1080/00220970903292975

Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: The Guildford Press.

Klusmann, U., Kunter, M., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., & Baumert, J. (2008). Engagement and emotional exhaustion in teachers: Does the school context make a difference? Applied Psychology: An International Review, 57, 127-151. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00358.x

Konu, A., Viitanen, E., & Lintonen, T. (2010). Teachers' wellbeing and perceptions of leadership practices. International Journal of Workplace Health Management, 7, 44-57. doi: 10.1108/17538351011031939

Kyriacou, C. (2000). Stressbusting for teachers. London, England: Nelson Thornes.

Kyriacou, C. (2001). Teacher stress: Directions for future research. Educational Review, 53, 27-35. doi: 10.1080/00131910120033628

Kyriacou, C., & Sutcliffe, J. (1977). The prevalence of stress among teachers in medium-sized mixed comprehensive schools. Research in Education, 18, 75-79.

Laursen, B., & Hoff, E. (2006). Person-centered and variable-centered approaches to longitudinal data. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 377-389.

Lavian, R. H. (2012). The impact of organizational climate on burnout among homeroom teachers and special education teachers (full classes/individual pupils) in mainstream schools. Teachers and Teaching: Theory and Practice, 18, 233-247.

Leiter, M. P., & Bakker, A. B. (2010). Work engagement: Introduction. In A. B. Bakker & M. P. Leiter (Eds.), Work engagement: A handbook of essential theory and research (pp. 1-9). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Lent, R. W., Nota, L., Soresi, S., Ginevra, M. C., Duffy, R. D., & Brown, S. D. (2011). Predicting the job and life satisfaction of Italian teachers: Test of a social cognitive model. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 79, 91-97. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2010.12.006

217

Page 230: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Wood, A. M., Osborne, G., & Hurling, R. (2009). Measuring happiness: The higher order factor structure of subjective and psychological well-being measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 878-884. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.010

Locke, E. A. (1969). What is job satisfaction? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 4, 309-336.

Loo, R., & Thorpe, K. (2000). Confirmatory factor analyses of the full and short versions of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. Journal of Social Psychology, 140, 628-635.

Lynch, M. F. J., Plant, R. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2005). Psychological needs and threat to safety: Implications for staff and patients in a psychiatric hospital for youth. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 36, 415-425. doi: 10.1037/0735-7028.36.4.415

Lyubomirsky, S., Dickerhoof, R., Boehm, J. K., & Sheldon, K. M. (2011). Becoming happier takes both a will and a proper way: An experimental longitudinal intervention to boost well-being. Emotion, 11, 391-402. doi: 10.1037/a0022575

MacNeil, A. J., Prater, D. L., & Busch, S. (2009). The effects of school culture and climate on student achievement. International Journal of Leadership in Education, 12, 73-84. doi: 10.1080/13603120701576241

Manthei, R., & Gilmore, A., Tuck, B., & Adair, V. (1996). Teacher stress in intermediate schools. Educational Research, 38, 3-19.

Martin, A. J. (2007). Examining a multidimensional model of student motivation and engagement using a construct validation approach. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 413-440. doi:10.1348/000709906X118036

Martin, A. J., & Dowson, M. (2009). Interpersonal relationships, motivation, engagement, and achievement: Yields for theory, current issues, and educational practice. Review of Educational Research, 79, 327-365. doi: 10.3102/0034654308325583

Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1981). The measurement of experienced burnout. Journal of Occupational Behavior, 2, 99-113.

McCarthy, C. J., Lambert, R. C., O'Donnell, M., & Melendres, L. T. (2009). The relation of elementary teachers' experience, stress, and coping resources to burnout symptoms. Elementary School Journal, 109, 282-300.

Mertler, C. A. (2002). Job satisfaction and perception of motivation among middle and high school teachers. American Secondary Education, 31, 43-53.

218

Page 231: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Mertler, C. A. (2003). Patterns of response and nonresponse from teachers to traditional and web surveys. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 8. Retrieved from http://PAREonline.net/getvn.asp?v=8&n=22

Meyer, J. P., Becker, T. E., & Vandenberghe, C. (2004). Employee commitment and motivation: A conceptual analysis and integrative model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 991-1007.

Meyer, J. P., & Herscovitch, L. (2001). Commitment in the workplace: Toward a general model. Human Resource Management Review, 11, 299-326.

Meyer, J. P., & Maltin, E. R. (2010). Employee commitment and well-being: A critical review, theoretical framework and research agenda. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 77, 323-337. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2010.04.007

Milkie, M. A., & Warner, C. H. (2011). Classroom learning environments and the mental health of first grade children. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 52, 4-22.

Miller, L. A., McIntire, S. A., & Lovler, R. L. (2011). Foundations of psychological testing: A practical approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.

Ministry of Education (2001). BC Performance Standards: Social Responsibility – A Framework. Retrieved from http://www.bced.gov.bc.ca/perf_stands/social_resp.htm

Ministry of Education (2012). School calendar regulation. Retrieved from http://www.bced.gov.bc.ca/legislation/schoollaw/d/bcreg_114-02.pdf

Moè, A., Pazzaglia, F., & Ronconi, L. (2010). When being able is not enough. the combined value of positive affect and self-efficacy for job satisfaction in teaching. Teaching and Teacher Education, 26, 1145-1153. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2010.02.010

Morin, J., & Battalio, R. (2004). Construing misbehaviour: The efficacy connection in responding to misbehaviour. Journal of Positive Behaviour Interventions, 6, 251-254.kay

Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus User’s Guide. Seventh Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. O. (2005) Chi-square difference testing using the Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square. Retrieved from http://www.statmodel.com/chidiff.shtml

Naylor, C. (2001). Teacher workload and stress: An international perspective on human costs and systemic failure. Vancouver, BC: British Columbia Teachers' Federation.

Pakarinen, E., Kiuru, N., Lerkkanen, M., Poikkeus, A., Siekkien, M., & Nurmi, J. (2010). Classroom organisation and teacher stress predict learning motivation in kindergarten children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 25, 281-300.

219

Page 232: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Pas, E. T., Bradshaw, C. P., & Hershfeldt, P. A. (2012). Teacher- and school-level predictors of teacher efficacy and burnout: Identifying potential areas for support. Journal of School Psychology, 50, 129-145. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2011.07.003

Patrick, B. C., Hisley, J., Kempler, T., & College, G. (2000). `What's everybody so excited about?': The effects of teacher enthusiasm on student intrinsic motivation and vitality. Journal of Experimental Education, 68, 217.

Pearson, L. C., & Moomaw, W. (2005). The relationship between teacher autonomy and stress, work satisfaction, empowerment, and professionalism. Educational Research Quarterly, 29, 38-54.

Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Brière, N. M. (2001). Associations among perceived autonomy support, forms of self-regulation, and persistence: A prospective study. Motivation & Emotion, 25, 279-306.

Perry, N. E., & Winne, P. H. (2006). Learning from learning kits: gStudy traces of students’ self-regulated engagements with computerized content. Educational Psychology Review, 18, 211-228.

Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Educational psychological at the millennium: A look back and a look forward. Educational Psychologist, 35, 221-226.

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879-903. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

Rath, T., & Harter, J. (2010). Well being: The five essential elements. New York, NY: Gallup Press.

Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J. (2004). Enhancing students’ engagement by increasing teachers’ autonomy support. Motivation and Emotion, 28, 147-169.

Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, J., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). Daily well-being: The role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 419-435. doi: 10.1177/0146167200266002

Retelsdorf, J., Butler, R., Streblow, L., & Schiefele, U. (2010). Teachers' goal orientations for teaching: Associations with instructional practices, interest in teaching, and burnout. Learning and Instruction, 20, 30-46. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.01.001

Richardson, P. W., & Watt, H. M. G. (2010). Current and future directions in teacher motivation research. In T. C. Urdan, & S. A. Karabenick (Eds.), The decade ahead: Applications and contexts of motivation and achievement (pp. 139-173). Emerald Group Publishing Limited.

220

Page 233: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Roeser, R. W., Schonert-Reichl, K., Jha, A., Cullen, M., Wallace, L., Wilensky, R., ... & Harrison, J. (2013). Mindfulness training and reductions in teacher stress and burnout: Results from two randomized, waitlist-control field trials. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 787-804. doi: 10.1037/a0032093

Ronfeldt, M., Loeb, S., & Wyckoff, J. (2013). How teacher turnover harms student achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 50, 4-36. doi:10.3102/0002831212463813

Ross, J. A. (1994). The impact of an inservice to promote cooperative learning on the stability of teacher efficacy. Teaching and Teacher Education, 10, 381-394. doi:10.1016/0742-051X(94)90020-5

Roth, G., Assor, A., Kanat-Maymon, Y., & Kaplan, H. (2007). Autonomous motivation for teaching: How self-determined teaching may lead to self-determined learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 762-774. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.4.761

Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749-761. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.749

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68-78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141-166.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2011). A self-determination theory perspective on social, institutional, cultural, and economic supports for autonomy and their importance for well-being. In V. I. Chirkov, R. M. Ryan & K. M. Sheldon (Eds.), Human autonomy in cross-cultural context (pp. 45-64). Netherlands: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-90-481-9667-8_3

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069-1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. (2004). Job demands, job resources, and their relationship with burnout and engagement: A multi-sample study. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25, 293-315. doi: 10.1002/job.248

Schimmack, U., & Lucas, R. E. (2010). Environmental influences on well-being: A dyadic latent panel analysis of spousal similarity. Social Indices Research, 98, 1-21. doi: 10.1007/s11205-009-9516-8

221

Page 234: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Schleicher, D. J., Hansen, S. D., & Fox, K. E. (2011). Job attitudes and work values. In S. Zedeck, & et al. (Eds.), APA handbook of industrial and organizational psychology: Vol. 3. maintaining, expanding, and contracting the organization (pp. 137-189). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Seligman, M. E. P. (2012). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being. New York, NY: Free Press.

Selye, H. (1974). Stress without distress. Philadelphia: Lippincott.

Shann, M. H. (1998). Professional commitment and satisfaction among teachers in urban middle schools. Journal of Educational Research, 92, 67-73.

Simms, L. J. (2008). Classical and modern methods of psychological scale construction. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 414-433.

Sinclair, C. (2008). Initial and changing student teacher motivation and commitment to teaching. Asia-Pacific Journal of Teacher Education, 36, 79-104.

Skaalvik, E. M., & Skaalvik, S. (2009). Does school context matter? Relations with teacher burnout and job satisfaction. Teaching and Teacher Education, 25, 518-524. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2008.12.006

Skaalvik, E. M., & Skaalvik, S. (2011). Teacher job satisfaction and motivation to leave the teaching profession: Relations with school context, feeling of belonging, and emotional exhaustion. Teaching and Teacher Education, 27, 1038. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2011.04.001

Skinner, E., & Edge, K. (2002). Self-determination, coping, and development. In E. L. Deci, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 297-337). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Solinger, O. N., van Olffen, W., & Roe, R. A. (2008). Beyond the three-component model of organizational commitment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 70-83. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.70

Spector, P. E. (1997). Job satisfaction: Application, assessment, causes and consequences. London: Sage Publications.

Spector, P. E., & Brannick, M. T. (2009) Common method variance or measurement bias? The problem and possible solutions. In D. A. Buchanan & A. Bryman (Eds.), The Sage Handbook of Organization Research Methods (346-362). London, UK: Sage Publications.

Standage, M., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2005). A test of self-determination theory in school physical education. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 411-433.

222

Page 235: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Steinhardt, M. A., Smith Jaggers, S. E., Faulk, K. E., & Gloria, C. T. (2011). Chronic work stress and depressive symptoms: Assessing the mediating role of teacher burnout. Stress and Health, 27, 420-429.

Suh, E., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being: Only recent events matter. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70, 1091-1102. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.1091

Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education.

Taylor, I. M., & Ntoumanis, N. (2007). Teacher motivational strategies and student self-determination in physical education. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 747-760. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.4.747

Taylor, I. M., Ntoumanis, N., & Standage, M. (2008). A self-determination theory approach to understanding the antecedents of teachers’ motivational strategies in physical education. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30, 75-94.

Thomas, N., Clark, V., & Lavery, J. (2003). Self-reported work and family stress of female primary teachers. Australian Journal of Education, 47, 73-87.

Thoresen, C. J., Kaplan, S. A., Barsky, A. P., Warren, C. R., & de Chermont, K. (2003). The affective underpinnings of job perceptions and attitudes: A meta-analytic review and integration. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 914-945. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.914

Thornton, B., Peltier, G., Medina, R. (2007). Reducing the special education teacher shortage. The Clearing House, 80, 233-238.

Tov, W., & Diener, E. (2009). The well-being of nations: Linking together trust, cooperation, and democracy. In E. Diener (Ed.), The science of well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener, Social Indicators Research Series, 37, 155-173. doi: 10.1007/978-90-481-2350-6 7

Tschannen-Moran, M., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. E. (2001). Teacher efficacy: Capturing an elusive construct. Teaching and Teacher Education, 17, 783-805. doi: 10.1016/S0742-051X(01)00036-1

Tschannen-Moran, M., Woolfolk Hoy, A. E., & Hoy, W. K. (1998). Teacher efficacy: Its meaning and measure. Review of Educational Research, 68, 202-248.

Uziel, L. (2010). Rethinking social desirability scales: From impression management to interpersonally oriented self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 243-262. doi:10.1177/1745691610369465

Vallerand, R. J., Fortier, M. S., & Guay, F. (1997). Self-determination and persistence in a real-life setting: Toward a motivational model of high school dropout. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1161-1176.

223

Page 236: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Van den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., De Witte, H., Soenens, B., & Lens, W. (2010). Capturing autonomy, competence, and relatedness at work: Construction and initial validation of the work-related basic need satisfaction scale. Journal of Occupational and Organisational Psychology, 83, 981-1002. doi: 10.1348/096317909X481382

Van Horn, J. E., Schaufeli, W. B., & Taris, T. W. (2001). Lack of reciprocity among Dutch teachers: Validation of reciprocity indices and their relation to stress and well-being. Work & Stress, 15, 191-213. doi: 10.1080/02678370110066571

Van Horn, J. E., Taris, T. W., Schaufeli, W. B., & Schreurs, P. J. G. (2004). The structure of occupational well-being: A study among Dutch teachers. Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology, 77, 365-375.

Vandenberghe, C., & Bentein, K. (2009). A closer look at the relationship between affective commitment to supervisors and organisations and turnover. Journal of Occupational & Organisational Psychology, 82, 331-348.

Vandenberghe, C., Bentein, K., Michon, R., Chebat, J., Tremblay, M., & Fils, J. (2007). An examination of the role of perceived support and employee commitment in employee-customer encounters. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1177-1187. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.1177

Velicer, W. F., Eaton, C. A., & Fava, J. L. (2000). Construct explication through factor of component analysis: A review and evaluation of alternative procedures for determining the number of factors or components. In Goffin, R. D., & Helmes, E. (Eds.), Problems and solutions in human assessment: Honoring Douglas N. Jackson at seventy (pp. 41-71). Boston, MA: Kluwer.

Vivoll Straume, L. V., & Vittersø, J. (2012). Happiness, inspiration and the fully functioning person: Separating hedonic and eudaimonic well-being in the workplace. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 7, 387-398. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2012.711348

Watt, H. M. G., & Richardson, P. W. (2007). Motivational factors influencing teaching as a career choice: Development and validation of the FIT-choice scale. Journal of Experimental Education, 75, 167-202. doi: 10.3200/JEXE.75.3.167-202

Wei, H., & Chen, J. (2010). School attachment among Taiwanese adolescents: the roles of individual characteristics, peer relationships, and teacher well-being. Social Indicators Research, 95, 241-436. doi: 10.1007/s11205-009-9529-3

Weiqi, C. (2007). The structure of secondary school teacher job satisfaction and its relationship with attrition and work enthusiasm. Chinese Education and Society, 40, 17-31. doi: 10.2753/CED 1061-1932400503

Winne, P. H., Jamieson-Noel, D. L., & Muis, K. (2002). Methodological issues and advances in researching tactics, strategies, and self-regulated learning. In P. R. Pintrich and M. L.

224

Page 237: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: New directions in measures and methods (Vol. 12, pp. 121-155). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Wolpin, J., Burke, R. J., & Greenglass, E. R. (1991). Is job satisfaction an antecedent or a consequence of psychological burnout? Human Relations, 44, 193-209. doi: 10.1177/001872679104400205

Woolfolk Hoy, A. E., & Spero, R. B. (2005). Changes in teacher efficacy during the early years of teaching: A comparison of four measures. Teaching and Teacher Education, 21, 343-356. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2005.01.007

225

Page 238: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

APPENDICES

Appendix A: Invitation Letter for Studies 1 and 2

Dear Teachers, Did you know your well-being as a teacher is important for you and your students? Would you like to help us learn more about teacher well-being with the aim of improving working conditions for teachers? Do you want to win an iPad? My name is Rebecca Collie and I am a doctoral student at the University of British Columbia. Under the supervision of Dr. Jennifer Shapka, I am currently conducting an online research study entitled, “Well-Being and its Influence on Teachers,” and I am writing to request your professional assistance in this research. Teacher well-being is not only critically important for teachers' health and teaching effectiveness, but also the motivation and achievement of students. Despite the clear importance of this issue, relatively little is known about what aspects of teaching work impact teachers’ well-being. My research study aims to uncover more about this with the long-term aim of helping to improve the profession for teachers and students alike. With this email, I am inviting you to participate in my research. Your participation will involve only 15-20 minutes of your time by completing an online questionnaire. If you agree to participate, you can access the questionnaire at this link: [URL] How does this research benefit teachers? By participating, you will be helping to provide important information for how best to support your well-being. It is my hope that this research will help to provide tangible strategies for schools, districts, and policy-makers to help improve teachers’ experiences of well-being. I hope that these, in turn, will also help to improve the experiences of students, which are clearly linked with teacher well-being. The results of the study will be made available to your school district (without identifying any schools or individuals) with the aim of promoting working conditions that support teacher well-being.

Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, and Special Education The University of British Columbia Faculty of Education 2125 Main Mall Vancouver BC Canada V6T 1Z4 Tel 604-822-0242 Fax 604-822-3302 www.ecps.educ.ubc.ca

226

Page 239: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Please be assured that your responses will be anonymous, and that your participation in this study is voluntary. To ensure confidentiality, your completed survey will be stored on a secure and password protected server in the Lower Mainland of British Columbia. Please be assured that your decision to participate or not participate in this study will have no impact on your relationship with the [NAME OF TEACHERS’ ASSOCIATION] or your job standing. In addition, your teachers’ association will not know who did/did not participate in the research. By completing and submitting the survey, you are giving your consent to participate. It is recommended that you keep a copy of this letter for your own records. If you do not wish to participate, simply disregard this message. If you have any questions regarding the study, please contact me at [EMAIL]. Let me thank you in advance for your valuable time and assistance in this study, which aims to advance our understanding of teacher well-being. As a thank you for your interest in our study, click below and enter your name in a draw to win an iPad. Your eligibility to enter the draw is not linked to your participation. [URL] Please note: If you have any questions or concerns about this questionnaire, please contact Dr. Jennifer Shapka, the principal investigator and the supervisor of this study (CONTACT DETAILS). You may also contact the Research Subject Information Line in the UBC Office of Research Services at [PHONE NUMBER]. Best of luck in the remainder of the school year. Sincerely, Rebecca Collie

227

Page 240: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Appendix B: Questionnaire for Studies 1 and 2

Section 1: Demographics

1. What school level are you teaching at?

Elementary

Middle

Secondary

Both Elementary and Middle

Both Elementary and Secondary

Other:

2. What is your current position?

Pre-service teacher

Regular teacher

Resource teacher

Special education teacher

Administrator (e.g., principal, vice- principal, director, school head)

Teacher librarian

Teacher counsellor

Other:

3. How much of your time at work is spent teaching students or planning for teaching in a week?

0%

1-25%

26-50%

51-75%

76-100%

4. What are the major subjects that you teach?

5. What is your highest degree?

6. What is your age?

7. What is your gender?

F

M

8. What is the zipcode(s) of the school(s) you teach at?

9. What was your country of birth?

10. What is your ethnicity?

Northern and Western European origins (e.g., British, Scottish, German, Swedish, Danish, Norwegian, Dutch)

Eastern and Southern European origins (e.g., Polish, Russian, Ukranian, Italian, Greek, Spanish)

Aboriginal origins (e.g., First Nations, Inuit, Metis)

South Asian origins (e.g., East Indian, Punjabi, Pakistani)

East Asian origins (e.g., Chinese, Japanese, Korean)

Southeast Asian origins (e.g., Filipino, Thai, Vietnamese)

Other (please list):

11. How many years have you been teaching (including as a TOC)?

228

Page 241: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

12. What is the socio-economic status of students in your school (e.g., average family income level compared to most people in your province)?

1 Low

2 3 Average

4 5 High

13. What is the academic ability of students in your school (e.g., average ability compared to other schools in your district/province)?

1 Low

2 3 Average

4 5 High

14. What is your school location?

Urban Suburban Rural Small Town

Section 2: Flourishing, Autonomy Support, and Commitment

Please indicate the degree to which you agree with the following statements currently in your life.

1 Strongly Disagree

2

3

4 Mixed or Neither

Agree nor Disagree

5

6

7 Strongly Agree

1. My principal listens to how I would like to do things

2. I feel that my principal provides me with choices and options

3. I feel understood by my principal 4. My principal conveys confidence in my

ability to do well at my job

5. My principal encourages me to ask questions

6. My principal tries to understand how I see things before suggesting a new way of doing things

7. I lead a purposeful and meaningful life 8. My social relationships are supportive and

rewarding

9. I am engaged and interested in my daily activities

10. I actively contribute to the happiness and well-being of others

11. I am competent and capable in the activities that are important to me

12. I am a good person and live a good life 13. I am optimistic about my future 14. People respect me

229

Page 242: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

15. This school has a great deal of personal meaning for me

16. I really feel a sense of 'belonging' to the school(s) at which I work

17. I am proud to belong to this school community

18. I do not feel emotionally attached to my school

19. I really feel as if my school’s problems are my own

20. I do not feel like 'part of the family at my school

Section 3: Need Satisfaction

The following statements aim to tap your personal experiences at work. Would you please indicate the degree to which you currently agree with these statements?

1

Totally disagree

2 Disagree

3 Somewhat disagree / somewhat

agree

4 Agree

5 Totally agree

1. I don’t really feel connected with other people at my job

2. At work, I feel part of a group 3. I don’t really mix with other people at my

job

4. At work, I can talk with people about things that really matter to me

5. I often feel alone when I am with my colleagues

6. Some people I work with are close friends of mine

7. I don’t really feel competent in my job. 8. I really master my tasks at my job 9. I feel competent at my job 10. I doubt whether I am able to execute my job

properly

11. I am good at the things I do in my job 12. I have the feeling that I can even

accomplish the most difficult tasks at work

13. I feel like I can be myself at my job 14. At work, I often feel like I have to follow

other people’s commands

15. If I could choose, I would do things at work differently

16. The tasks I have to do at work are in line with what I really want to do

230

Page 243: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

17. I feel free to do my job the way I think it could best be done

18. In my job, I feel forced to do things I do not want to do

19. I am very committed to my students 20. Connecting with my students is an essential

part of the job

21. I value the relationships I build with my students

22. I feel connected to my students Section 4: Motivation at Work

Using the scales below, please indicate for each of the following statements to what degree they presently correspond to one of the reasons for which you are doing this specific job. Currently, why are you doing your current job?

1 Not at

all

2 Very little

3 A

little

4 Moderately

5 Strongly

6 Very

strongly

7 Exactl

y

1. Because I enjoy this work very much

2. Because I have fun doing my job

3. For the moments of pleasure that this job brings me

4. I chose this job because it allows me to reach my life goals

5. Because this job fulfils my career plans

6. Because this job fits my personal values

7. Because I have to be the best in my job, I have to be a “winner”

8. Because my work is my life and I don’t want to fail

9. Because my reputation depends on it

10. Because this job affords me a certain standard of living

11. Because it allows me to make a lot of money

12. I do this job for the paycheque

231

Page 244: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Section 5: Job Satisfaction, Stress, and Social Desirability

Please indicate the degree to which the following statements are part of your experiences with your job.

1 Never

3 Almost never

3 Rarely

4 Sometimes

5 Often

6 Very often

7 Always

1. I am satisfied with my job 2. I am happy with the way my

colleagues and superiors treat me

3. I am satisfied with what I achieve at work

4. I feel good at work 5. In general, how stressful do you find being a teacher?

0 Not at all stressful

1 Mildly stressful

2 Moderately

stressful

3 Very stressful

4 Extremely stressful

Listed below are a number of statements concerning personal attitudes and traits. Read each item and decide whether the statement is true or false as it pertains to you personally.

True False 6. I like to gossip at times 7. There have been occasions when I took advantage of someone 8. I’m always willing to admit it when I make a mistake 9. I sometimes try to get even rather than forgive and forget 10. At time I have really insisted on having things my own way 11. I have never been irked when people expressed ideas very different from my

own

12. I have never deliberately said something that hurt someone’s feelings

232

Page 245: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Section 6: Teacher Well-Being

Currently, how do the following aspects of being a teacher affect your well-being as a teacher? Well-being refers to open, engaged, and healthy functioning as a teacher. For example, in your current working environment how do your relations with fellow teachers influence your well-being? Do your relations have a positive or negative influence on your well-being as a teacher? If the current job action means that you are not currently engaging in some of the different items below, please answer based on your experiences prior to job action, or if you are a beginning teacher, please leave these items blank. 1

Negatively 2

Mostly Negatively

3 More

Negatively than

Positively

4 Neither

Positively nor

Negatively

5 More

Positively than

Negatively

6 Mostly

Positively

7 Positively

1. Relations with fellow teachers

2. Relations with students in my class

3. Relations with students not in my class (e.g., in hallways, etc.)

4. Relations with leadership at my school

5. Relations with administrators at my school

6. Relations with my students’ parents

7. Relations with my own family at home

8. Marking work 9. Lesson planning 10. Knowledge of

subject-matter

11. Interruptions to my teaching in my classroom (e.g., from people from outside of my class)

12. Fitting everything in to the allotted time (e.g., required curriculum, extra-curricular activities)

13. Implementing educational

233

Page 246: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

innovations or new programs

14. Using technology for administrative work (e.g., report cards, class lists)

15. Using technology for teaching (e.g., the Internet, electronic whiteboards, etc.)

16. The challenge of teaching work (e.g., being intellectually challenged)

17. The nature of teaching work (e.g., the meaningfulness of the job)

18. Satisfaction with my current job

19. Meeting learning goals

20. Student behaviour 21. Student motivation 22. My salary and

benefits

23. Responsibility for student success

24. Rest periods/breaks during the day

25. Noise(s) around the classroom/school

26. Administrative work related to teaching

27. Classroom management

28. Expectations of parents

29. Expectations of leadership or district

30. Support offered by parents

31. Support offered by leadership

32. The amount of time to spend with individual students

234

Page 247: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

33. The availability of equipment, facilities, or materials for teaching

34. Number of students in my class(es)

35. Recognition for my teaching

36. Opportunities for promotion

37. School rules and procedures that are in place

38. Follow-through by staff and leadership of school rules

39. Communication between members of the school

40. Work I complete outside of school hours for teaching

41. The community or neighbourhood in which I live

42. My commute to school

43. My physical health 44. The state of affairs

in the world

45. Working to finish my teaching tasks

46. The ability to control what/how I teach

47. Instances of bullying between students and/or staff

48. Student/staff use of social media/Internet

49. Using the proscribed curriculum and syllabi

50. Participation in school-level decision making

235

Page 248: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

51. The health of my family and/or friends

52. Uncontrolled events outside of school (e.g., car breaking down)

53. My or my families extracurricular activities outside of school (e.g., going to the gym, taking children to activities)

54. Looking after family members

55. Inclusion of students with special needs

56. Availability of support for students with special needs

57. Staying late after work for meetings and activities

58. Standardised testing (e.g., FSA)

59. Expectations of the provincial government

60. Communication between all members of my school

236

Page 249: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Appendix C: Invitation Letter for Study 3

Dear Teachers, In the next few weeks, you will be participating in the YouthMADE curriculum―a program that uses short films made by youth to explore issues of racism and discrimination. At the same time, you are being invited to participate in a UBC-based evaluation research project about YouthMADE, which aims to examine the impact of the YouthMADE curriculum on your well-being and development. This research is being conducted by Dr. Jennifer Shapka, who is an Associate Professor in the Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, and Special Education at the University of British Columbia (her contact information is below). To participate, all you will need to do is fill out an online questionnaire before you take part in the YouthMADE workshops. You can fill it out right now―details for accessing it are in the accompanying email. This questionnaire will ask questions about your well-being, as well as how you feel about your school and your experiences as a teacher. Demographic questions such as age, gender and ethnicity will also be asked. It will take about 15 minutes to complete. If you consent, please complete the first online questionnaire as honestly and openly as you can. After you have the YouthMADE curriculum in your classroom, we will contact you with details about two follow-up online questionnaires. These questionnaires will be spread over two months and will include similar questions to the first questionnaire. There are no known risks for your participation in this study, and you have the right to withdraw from the study at any time. Also, the study is voluntary and it will not impact your job security. Please be assured that your responses will be kept confidential. Contact for information about the study: If you have any questions about this study, please contact Dr. Jennifer Shapka at PHONE NUMBER, or her research associate, Rebecca Collie at PHONE NUMBER. Contact for concerns about the rights of research subjects: If you have any concerns about your child’s treatment or rights as a research subject, you may contact the Research Subject Information Line in the UBC Office of Research Services at PHONE NUMBER. Thank you for your time and support! Sincerely, Jennifer Shapka (on behalf of the YouthMADE research team)

Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, and Special Education The University of British Columbia Faculty of Education 2125 Main Mall Vancouver BC Canada V6T 1Z4 Tel 604-822-0242 Fax 604-822-3302 www.ecps.educ.ubc.ca

237

Page 250: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Appendix D: Questionnaire for Study 3

Section 1: Demographics

1. What is your age?

2. What is your gender?

F

M

3. What is your ethnicity?

Northern and Western European origins (e.g., British, Scottish, German, Swedish, Danish, Norwegian, Dutch)

Eastern and Southern European origins (e.g., Polish, Russian, Ukranian, Italian, Greek, Spanish)

Aboriginal origins (e.g., First Nations, Inuit, Metis)

South Asian origins (e.g., East Indian, Punjabi, Pakistani)

East Asian origins (e.g., Chinese, Japanese, Korean)

Southeast Asian origins (e.g., Filipino, Thai, Vietnamese)

Other (please list):

4. Do you consider yourself to be a member of a minority group (e.g., based on race, gender, ethnicity, class, ability, and/or sexual orientation)?

Yes

No

I don’t know / No answer 5. What grades do you teach? Check all that apply.

Kinder

Grade 1

Grade 2

Grade 3

Grade 4

Grade 5

Grade 6

Grade 7

Grade 8

Grade 9

Grade 10

Grade 11

Grade 12

6. What is your current position?

Pre-service teacher

Regular teacher

Resource teacher

Special education teacher

Administrator (e.g., principal, vice- principal, director, school head)

Teacher librarian

Teacher counsellor

Other:

7. How many years have you been teaching (including as a TOC)?

238

Page 251: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

Section 2: Well-being

Currently, how do the following aspects of being a teacher affect your well-being as a teacher? Well-being refers to open, engaged, and healthy functioning as a teacher. For example, in your current working environment how do your relations with fellow teachers influence your well-being? Do your relations have a positive or negative influence on your well-being as a teacher? If the current job action means that you are not currently engaging in some of the different items below, please answer based on your experiences prior to job action, or if you are a beginning teacher, please leave these items blank.

1 Negatively

2 Mostly

Negatively 3

More Negatively

than Positively

4 Neither

Positively nor

Negatively

5 More

Positively than

Negatively

6 Mostly

Positively 7

Positively

1. Relations with students in my class

2. Relations with administrators at my school

3. Marking work 4. Fitting everything

in to the allotted time (e.g., required curriculum, extra-curricular activities)

5. Student behaviour 6. Student

motivation 7. Administrative

work related to teaching

8. Classroom management

9. Support offered by leadership

10. Recognition for my teaching

11. School rules and procedures that are in place

12. Communication between members of the school

13. Work I complete outside of school hours for teaching

239

Page 252: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

14. Working to finish my teaching tasks

15. Participation in school-level decision making

16. Staying late after work for meetings and activities

Section 3: Need Satisfaction

The following statements aim to tap your personal experiences at work. Would you please indicate the degree to which you currently agree with these statements?

1

Totally disagree

2 Disagree

3 Somewhat disagree / somewhat

agree

4 Agree

5 Totally agree

1. I don’t really feel connected with other people at my job

2. At work, I feel part of a group 3. I don’t really mix with other people at my

job 4. At work, I can talk with people about things

that really matter to me 5. I often feel alone when I am with my

colleagues 6. Some people I work with are close friends of

mine 7. I don’t really feel competent in my job. 8. I really master my tasks at my job 9. I feel competent at my job 10. I doubt whether I am able to execute my job

properly 11. I am good at the things I do in my job 12. I have the feeling that I can even accomplish

the most difficult tasks at work 13. I feel like I can be myself at my job 14. At work, I often feel like I have to follow

other people’s commands 15. If I could choose, I would do things at work

differently 16. The tasks I have to do at work are in line

with what I really want to do 17. I feel free to do my job the way I think it

could best be done 18. In my job, I feel forced to do things I do not

want to do 19. I am very committed to my students

240

Page 253: UNDERSTANDING TEACHER WELL-BEING AND MOTIVATION

20. Connecting with my students is an essential part of the job

21. At work, I often feel like I have to follow other peoples’ commands

22. I feel connected to my students Section 4: Self-efficacy for Teaching

Please indicate your opinion about each of the questions below by marking any one of the nine responses in the columns on the right side, ranging from (1) “Nothing” to (9) “A Great Deal”. Please respond to each of the questions by considering the combination of your current ability, resources, and opportunity to do each of the following in your teaching position.

1 Nothing

2

3 Very Little

4 5 Some

Degree

6 7 Quite a Bit

8 9 A

Great Deal

1. How much can you do to control disruptive behaviour in the classroom?

2. How much can you do to motivate students who show low interest in school work?

3. How much can you do to calm a student who is disruptive or noisy?

4. How much can you do to help your students value learning?

5. To what extent can you craft good questions for your students?

6. How much can you do to get children to follow classroom rules?

7. How much can you do to get students to believe they can do well in school work?

8. How well can you establish a classroom management system with each group of students?

9. To what extent can you use a variety of assessment strategies?

10. To what extent can you provide an alternative explanation or example when students are confused?

11. How much can you assist families in helping their children do well in school?

12. How well can you implement alternative teaching strategies in your classroom?

241