23
Engli sh Teaching, Vo l. 67, No . 3, Autumn 20 12 Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition * 3 Sung-Soo Jang (Inje U ni versity) Jang , Sung-Soo. (2012). Types of information in written corrective feedback and its efficacy on L2 acquisition. English Teachillg, 67(3), 3-25. This study in ves ti gates wheth er th e effect of written corrective feedback (CF) on learners' acq ui sition of Eng li sh art icl e usage is mediated by the type of information CF provides. To tap into this iss ue, four types of information on error were id ent ifi ed: its existence, locati on, correct form and nature. Written CF was catego ri zed into three types according to th e so rt s of in for mat ion it contained: indirect CF that supplies inf ormat ion on existence and location, direct CF that provides information on existence, locati on an d correct form, a nd metalinguisti c CF th at includes metalinguistic explanati on on error nature a nd a ll the other infornlation ty pe s. Three CF gro ups were accord in gly formed along with a cont ro l gro up in a quasi-experiment, where the gro up s engaged in three narrative-writing tasks. Error co rr ection and narrati ve writing tests were administered to measure th e acquisition of explicit and implic it knowledge of Eng li sh articles respecti vel y. The results revealed that written CF was beneficial to acquisition of both L2 exp li cit and im pli cit knowledge a nd that such positive effects emerged onl y in the metalingui stic CF group. The findings added counterevide nc e to Truscott's (1996) argument aga in st written CF utility and suggested th at metalinguisti c explanation on er ror may playa pivotal role in written CF-triggered L2 acq ui siti on. I. INTRODUCTION Language acqui sition th eori es have not taken a unif o rm view of learners' errors and their corrections. BehaviOJists, who para ll eled habit formation with learning, claimed that errors are detri ment al to learning processes and correcting them is necessary. In contrast, tho se wld er the influence of nativist views such as Uni ve rsal Gra mmar did n ot be li eve that • This work was supported by th e 20 II Inj e U ni versi ty research grant.

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    9

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

English Teaching, Vol. 67, No . 3, Autumn 20 12

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition *

3

Sung-Soo Jang

(Inje University)

Jang, Sung-Soo. (2012). Types of information in written corrective feedback

and its efficacy on L2 acquisition. English Teachillg, 67(3), 3-25.

This study investi gates whether the effect of written corrective feedback (CF) on

learners' acquisition of English art icle usage is mediated by the type of information

CF provides. To tap into this issue, four types of informat ion on error were

identifi ed: its existence, location, correct form and nature. Written CF was

categorized into three types according to the sorts of informat ion it contained:

indirect CF that supplies informat ion on existence and location, direct CF that

provides information on ex istence, location and correct form, and metalinguistic CF

that includes metalinguistic explanation on error nature and all the other infornlation

types. Three CF groups were accord ingly formed along with a control group in a

quasi-experiment, where the groups engaged in three narrative-writing tasks . Error

correction and narrati ve writing tests were administered to measure the acquisition

of explicit and implicit knowledge of English articles respectively. The results

revealed that written CF was beneficial to acquisition of both L2 exp licit and

im plicit knowledge and that such positive effects emerged only in the metalinguistic

CF group. The findings added counterevidence to Truscott' s (1996) argument

against written CF utility and suggested that metalinguistic explanation on error may

playa pivotal role in written CF-triggered L2 acqui sition.

I. INTRODUCTION

Language acquisition theories have not taken a uniform view of learners' errors and

their corrections. BehaviOJists, who paralle led habit formation with learning, claimed that

errors are detrimental to learn ing processes and correcting them is necessary. In contrast,

those wlder the influence of nativist v iews such as Universal Grammar did not believe that

• Thi s work was supported by the 20 II Inj e Uni versity research grant.

Page 2: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

4 Sung-Soo Jang

error correction is essential as acquisition is fundamentally driven by positive evidence.

lnteractionists emphasized that errors can be corrected through more competent

interlocutors' feedback arising in their interaction with learners. They focused on this

particular type of corrective feedback (CF) and its va lue of facilitating acquisition.

CF can be provided in oral or written fonn. Since Chaudron's (1977) seminal work,

however, a majority of CF studies in the SLA literature have empirically and theoretically

focused on learners' oral errors. Overall, empirical research has shown that oral CF plays a

positive role in L2 acquisition although its efficacy is mediated by various factors such as

research settings, target features, and individual learner differences including protlciency

and anxiety (Ammar & Spada, 2006; Egi, 2007; Leeman, 2003; Long, lnakagi, & Ortega,

1998; Lyster & Mori, 2006; Mackey & Philp, 1998; Sheen, 2006, 2008). On the

theoretical front, such beneficial effectiveness of oral CF was supported by several SLA

theories such as noticing hypothesis (Schmidt, 1995), comprehensible output hypothesis

(Swain, 1995), interaction hypothesis (Long, 1996) and ski ll learning theory (DeKeyser,

1998, 2007).

Theories, however, differ in terms of what type of CF is optimal. Interaction hypothesis

prioritizes CF providing correct form while comprehensible output hypothesis places more

emphasis on CF withholding it. Sociocultural theory denies the existence of a single best

type, claiming that CF efficacy depends on how finely it is tuned to the learner's stage of

development. Some researchers even took a counter view of CF benefits, arguing that CF

is harmful, rather than helpful, to L2 acquisition as it raises the learner's affective filter

(Krashen, 2003).

Research into the effect of written CF on L2 acquisition has recently increased but is

sti ll scarce in comparison with oral CF. It is partially because, until recently, writing skill

was not a major target area of SLA research and CF did not constitute an important

component of L2 writing instruction. This study is one attempt to alleviate current

imbalance between oral and written CF research by focusing on written CF and comparing

the efficacy of its different types. To contextualize the present study in SLA research

paradigm, details are given in the fo llowing section of how written CF is compared with

oral CF in terms of characteristics and classification and of how they were differentially

treated in L2 acquisition research.

II. BACKGROUND

1. Distinction between Oral and Written CF

Oral and written CF contrast strikingly in terms of how they are characterized, classified,

Page 3: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition 5

and researched.

1) Differences in Characteristics

Oral CF is provided online in that it constitutes an immediate response to the leamer's

erroneous utterance during the interactional exchanges. Written CF, on the other hand, is

given offline. Its provision is delayed as it requires some time to review and correct the

leamer's writing. This difference leads to oral CF typically imposing more cognitive load

on the learner's short-term memory than written CF as the former forces the learner to

engage in making a spontaneous cognitive comparison between hislher ill-formed output

and information delivered in CF (Sheen, 2010).

In typical instructional settings, learners are likely exposed to oral CF on other learners'

errors, let alone on their own errors. Such exposure is generally not available in the written

mode unless CF is intentionally induced through a particular activity such as peer

correction (Sheen, 2010).

Due to the differences in delivery of and exposure to CF, its efficacy may vary across

oral and written modes. However, rarely has SLA research empirically addressed this issue

with the exception of Sheen (2010), who failed to find any mediating impact of CF mode.

Further research is defmitely in order before reaching any compelling argument regarding

the issue.

2) Differences in Classification

There are several ways to classifY oral CF strategies. One common way is

explicit/implicit distinction with explicit type being more overt in showing its corrective

nature . Another criterion is whether correct form of error is contained in CF, which serves

to distinguish input-providing from output-prompting CF. The former offers error-related

information (e.g., its correct fonn) while the latter intends to supply opportunities for

learners to self-correct their own errors. What is notable is that the two classifYing

dimensions are not exclusive to each other and a CF strategy can be described in both

dimensions. For instance, recasts are a CF strategy which is implicit and input-providing

type whi le elicitation can be categorized as explicit and output-prompting. An additional

dimension involves focused/unfocused division according to how many linguistic features

CF targets. Focused CF responds to only one or a limited number of pre-determined

specific types of errors whereas unfocused CF is free of such restrictions.

Research has overall shown that exp licit CF is more effective than implicit CF (Carroll

& Swain, 1993 ; Carroll, Swain, & Roberge, 1992; Ellis, Loewen, & Erlam, 2006).

However, findings are mixed over the relative efficacy of input-providing vs. output-

Page 4: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

6 Sung-Soo Jang

prompting CF. Some researchers argued for the superiority of input-providing CF

paliicularly in the fonn of recasts (Doughty, 2003 ; Long, 2007) while others fOlmd more

benefits in output-prompting type (Ammar & Spada, 2006; Lyster, 2004).

With regard to written CF, focused/unfocused division can be made while

explicit/implicit distinction is not feasib le as written CF is intrinsically explicit. No matter

how written CF is delivered, its corrective intention is most likely to be perceived by the

leamer due to its visual and sustained nature. Written CF is most commonly categorized as

two types, direct and indirect. Direct CF is one that provides correct foml, corresponding

to oral recasts in this regard. In contrast, indirect CF withholds correct from and, in this

respect, is similar to oral output-prompting CF. However, the fonner is more limited in its

capacity to prompt the leamer's output than the latter due to the inherent nature of written

CF to delay the delivery of corrections.

Written CF studies produced mixed findings about the relative effectiveness of direct

and indirect CF (Chandler, 2003; Ferris, 2006; Robb, Ross, & Shortreed, 1986; Semke,

1984). Ferris (2006) contended that indirect CF is more beneficial as it facilitates forming

hypotheses about L2 foml and mles. On the contrary, Chandler (2003) argued for the

superiority of direct CF, asserting that it helps confirm previously formed hypotheses.

Recent studies showed that focused written CF promotes leamers ' linguistic accuracy

(Bitchener & Knoch, 2008; Ellis, Sheen, Murakami, & Takashima, 2008; Sheen, 2007).

Ellis et al. (2008) further compared focused with unfocused CF but found no differential

effectiveness.

3) Differences in Research Purposes and Design

Oral and written CF differ in several ways conceming how they have been investigated.

One difference emerges in terms of who conducted research with what purposes. Oral CF

research has predominantly been conducted by SLA researchers while written CF has

mostly been studied by L2 writing researchers and practitioners.

SLA researchers initially concentrated on developing taxonomies of oral CF strategies

and later shifted their focus to investigating the effectiveness of different types of

identified CF strategies on L2 grammatical development. On the other hand, granlmatical

error correction received little attention in L2 writing instruction where the process­

oriented perspective was prevalent. In this process-oriented approach, pedagogical priority

was given to improving the quality of the leamer's original writing through recursive

processes of drafting, receiving feedback, and redrafting over developing linguistic

accuracy. Accordingly, written CF was primarily given on a variety of writing aspects

such as content, organization, rhetoric, and mechanics rather than on linguistic accuracy.

However, some researchers claimed that linguistic accuracy constitutes a component ofL2

Page 5: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types of Information in Wrinen Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition 7

writing perfonnance and should be addressed in L2 writing instruction (Eskey, 1983;

Horowitz, 1986; Johns, 1995; Leki, 1990). Along this line, Ferris (2010) and other L2

writing specialists (Lane & Lange, 1999; Frodesen, 1991; Reid, 1994) made suggestions

of how written CF should be provided for increasing linguistic accuracy in the process­

oriented L2 writing class. To be specific, it was maintained that written CF should be

"contextualized within the recursive writing process, prioritized to focus on serious and

frequent patterns of written eITor, and personalized to the specific needs of the individual

student writer" (Ferris, 2010, p. 185). Most L2 writing researchers and teachers have

recently come to at least agree that "there is some role for written CF in L2 writing

instruction" (Ferris, 2010, p. 184) that considers linguistic accuracy improvement.

In short, SLA researchers were mainly concerned with the effect of oral CF strategies on

L2 acquisition while L2 writing experts mostly focused on examining the value of written

CF in improving overall quality of the leamer's revised writing text. These different

concerns of the two CF research fields are reflected in the ways that they each design CF

studies (FeITis, 2010; Sheen, 2007). With goals to examine the effectiveness of CF on

acquisition, SLA research typically has control and treatment groups and pretest-posttest

designs. CF generally targets one or a few chosen error types and is consistently provided.

In contrast, such SLA-specific design features are not necessarily required in L2 writing

studies whose primary concern is whether written CF assists in producing the revised text

of a higher quality by the same student writer. Thus, until recently, it remained to be

resolved whether written CF could lead to L2 acquisition.

2. Integration of Written CF in SLA Research

The issue of whether and how written CF is related to L2 acquisition saw its turning

point in Truscott's (1996) influential study. Truscott (1996) claimed that written CF should

be abandoned in L2 writing instruction as it is not only ineffective but even harmful to

development of L2 implicit knowledge. His study drew earnest attention to the issue

among researchers of SLA and L2 writing alike, sparking heated debate and leading to

more subsequent studies. Change in research methodology and scope occurred in both

fields to empirically tackle Truscott's (1996) argument. Many L2 Wliting experts began to

adopt SLA research design (Ashwell, 2000; Chandler, 2003; Ferris, 2006) while SLA

researchers expanded their acquisition studies beyond oral to written CF (Bitchener &

Knoch, 2008; Ellis et al. , 2008; Sheen, 2007). A majority of subsequent studies

demonstrated that written CF is effective in L2 acquisition, casting doubt over Truscott 's

(1996) strong skepticism of written CF.

Some studies further looked into what types of written CF are more beneficial than

others (Ellis et al. , 2008; Sheen, 2007). This is closely linked to a question of what and

Page 6: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

8 Sung-Soo Jang

how much information should be required in CF for its optimal efficacy. Written CF can

be categorized as direct, indirect, or metalinguistic CF according to the SOltS of

infonnation it delivers, which include the existence, location, correct form, and nature of

the error. As written CF is inherently explicit, its presence alone is sufficient to signal the

occurrence of error irrespective of its type. Indirect CF indicates where error occurred

while direct CF shows not only the error's location but its corresponding correct form.

Metalinguistic CF even helps the learner understand the nature of error by providing

metalinguistic explanation of it.

Ellis et al. (2008) proved that focused direct CF was beneficial in the acquisition of

English articles by L2 learners. Sheen (2007) compared direct CF with and without

metalinguistic explanation and found that both were effective in the short-term period but

only the former had a long-tenn effect. However, no study has compared all three types of

written CF so far in terms of their acquisition-facilitating value. The present study

addresses this limitation and, in so doing, aims to sort out the information that is required

in written CF for its optimal effectiveness in L2 acquisition. This goal can only be pursued

on the premise that written CF can enhance L2 learning and, as noted above, a variety of

previoLls studies revealed that written CF plays an acquisition-promoting role. Nonetheless,

the present study additionally seeks to confmn such CF benefits to consolidate the validity

of comparing CF types containing different sorts of information. The following research

questions are investigated to bring about the study's purposes:

1. Is written CF beneficial to L2 acquisition?

2. What sort of information in written CF is most conducive to its efficacy on L2

acquisition?

III. METHOD

1. DeSign

An experiment with adult EFL learners was conducted to address the research questions.

As this study aims to examine the effect of written CF on L2 acquisition, it employed

methodological features that have typically characterized SLA research. Thus, a control

group was formed to be compared with the treatment groups and a specific linguistic

feature was chosen as the CF target. Materials for the experiment consisted of three sets of

feedback treatment tasks and three sets of tests . The tasks were designed to produce

writing productions in frequent use of the target form. The tests included the pretest, the

immediate posttest, and the delayed posttest to trace changes in the learners' L2

Page 7: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition 9

knowledge. Each test was composed of two versions to examine different levels of

knowledge-declarative and procedural knowledge. Pretests were administered before the

treatment tasks, inunediate posttests right after their completion, and delayed posttests two

weeks later.

2. Participants

The battery of participants was composed of a teacher and fifty four Korean EFL

students who completed al\ the components of the experiment including three treatment

tasks and three testing sessions. They were all sophomores of a university in Southeast

Korea and were taking one of the courses by the researcher, who was also their teacher, in

the semester that the experiment was carried out. The participants were assigned to one of

three treatment groups or a control group. The treatment groups, which were labeled direct,

metalinguistic, and indirect, were distinguished respectively according to the sorts of

infornlation in CF that they received during the treatment sessions. Further details of the

distinctions in this regard are provided in a subsequent section. The control group took part

in the treatment tasks but did not receive any CF. The group composition for the study is

summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1

Participant Numbers by Group

Group Direct Group (DG)

Metalinguistic Group (MG) Indirect Group (IG) Control Group (CG)

3. Target Structure

Number 13 13 14 14

English alticles were selected as the target feature. The decision was made as a result of

considering methodological and pedagogical advantages the feature could have when

written CF is examined in an EFL context involving Korean learners. On the

methodological front, Korean language does not have a structure equivalent to English

articles, which causes learners to frequently make errors. Nonetheless, they are seldom

corrected in typical instructional settings in Korea as articles are neither salient graphically

or phonologically nor carry high communicative value. In fact, correcting article error did

not constitute part of instruction for the English course that the participants of the study

were taking, which made it possible to isolate the effect of CF offered in the experiment.

From a pedagogical perspective, targeting articles has a high validity as they turned out to

Page 8: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

10 Sung-Soo Jang

be one ofthe most frequently used structures in English (Sinclair, 1991).

English article rules are extremely complicated in nature and hard to figure out

particularly for learners whose L 1 does not have an equivalent structure such as those in

the current study. In this light, written CF was given only on errors involving two major

functions of the articles: indefmite article a used to refer to someone or something for the

first time and definite article the with a function of indicating that its following noun was

previously mentioned (i.e. , anaphoric reference). Example I is a sentence that includes

both functions.

Example 1: Article functions offrrst and second mention

I recently got a new computer and I will use the computer mostly for surfing the

Internet.

4. Sorts of Acquisition for Measurement

Although L2 writing experts and SLA researchers sought to clarifY the impact of CF on

L2 ' learning outcome', they had a different view of what it meant. As noted early, it was

considered an equivalent of acquisition for SLA researchers. In contrast, L2 writing

specialists evaluated it on the basis of improvements in overall quality of the leamer's

revised writing text. Yet some L2 writing practitioners recently adopted the SLA view and

written CF is increasingly investigated within SLA research framework.

The notion of 'acquisition' also has yet to be well defined in the literature. Frequently,

acquisition is connected with two different levels of knowledge. For example, skill

learning theory makes distinction between declarative and procedural knowledge

(DeKeyser, 1998,2001). The former refers to knowledge about the facts while the latter

represents ability to use declarative knowledge. Declarative knowledge may be retrieved

from short-term memory, which demands controlled processing that requires a lot of

attentional resources and conscious awareness. For this reason, declarative knowledge is

viewed as more of the explicit type. Procedural knowledge, on the other hand, is

established through practicing declarative knowledge and is retrieved from long-term

memory, which involves automatic processing that does not heavily rely on attention and

awareness. Procedural and automatized knowledge is therefore more of the implicit type.

In skill learning theory, acquisition is defined as proceduralizing and automatizing

declarative knowledge. Some researchers took quite a different view of acquisition,

claiming it to be progress along a sequence of developmental stages for chosen target

features (Mackey & Philp, 1998; Pienemann, Johnston, & Brindley, 1988).

The majority of CF research has examined acquisition only in terms of procedural

knowledge. The present study, however, looked into both declarative and procedural

Page 9: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition II

knowledge. Accordingly, two different versions of tests were developed and administered

at each testing session: an error correction test for assessing the learners' declarative

knowledge of the target structure and a narrative writing test for procedural knowledge.

5. Tests for Assessing Acquisition

1) Error Correction Test

An error correction test contained 25 items. Each item had two sentences and one of

them was underlined, indicating that it was grammatically wrong. The learners were asked

to correct and rewrite the underlined sentences. Twenty items involved misuse of English

articles. Example 2 below is one item from the test.

Example 2: An item of the error correction test

There may be restaurant around here. I want to eat something soon because I am really

hungry now.

~ ------------------------------------------------------

The other five items were distracters and their lmgrammaticality was caused by errors in

various grammar features such as tense, word order, and verb f0011. The article-targeting

items only were scored by giving 1 point to each correctly changed sentence, the perfect

score being 20 points. As the test imposed no time limit, the learners were able to review

the items as many times as they wanted and to mobilize as much attentional resources as

they needed to retrieve knowledge about article-related rules to complete the test. In this

sense, the test was regarded as measuring the learners' declarative/explicit knowledge of

the two major functions of English articles.

2) Narrative Writing Test

This test was adapted from those used in Muranoi (2000) and Sheen (2007). It consisted

of two sets of four sequential pictures with a few word prompts next to each picture. For

each set, the learners were asked to write a coherent story by making reference to the

pictures and accompanying words. A learner's score was calculated as a percentage by

dividing the number of articles correctly supplied by the number of contexts where articles

should be obligatorily used and contexts where articles are not obligatory but used by the

learner. As the constrained free writing, like ones in this study, does not guarantee that

articles are only usable in obligatory contexts, the following coding guidelines were

adopted as presented in Sheen (2007).

Page 10: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

12 Sung-Soo Jang

1. When one article was followed by two noun phrases as in 'a shirt and skirt' , only the

first noun phrase was coded as it was not clear whether the second noun was the

result of elision.

2. When an article and a detelminer coexisted as in 'a his dog ', the noun phrase was not

coded.

3. Idiomatic expressions containing at1icles as with 'at the moment' were not coded.

There was a five-minute time limit to write a story for each set. This pushed the learners

to rely on the kind of knowledge retrievable through subconscious and automatic

processing. In addition, creating a coherent story required them to focus preferentially on

words of high communicative value. Given a low communicative value involved in

articles, the learners were not likely to consciously pay attention to article usage with such

a time-limit. For this reason, scores of this test were regarded as reflecting the learners'

level of procedural/implicit knowledge of the target structure.

6. Treatment Tasks

Three treatment sessions continued over a two-week period, at each of which the

learners performed a narrative writing task. Three stories each of 78 words with 24

indefinite articles and 30 definite articles in total were created. In each session, the

learner's task was to read a story and write it as accurately as possible. Specifically, the

teacher distributed a story with a writing sheet to each leamer, who then read it for 3

minutes. After collecting the story, the teacher read it aloud. The learners then wrote the

story on the writing sheet as closely to the original one as they could remember for 5

minutes. The sheet had a few key words from the story. The teacher's act of reading the

story aloud and provision of key words were intended to reduce the learner' s processing

load on retrieving the story. Lastly, the teacher collected the learners' written stories and

later corrected article errors only.

When correcting errors, the teacher gave different types of CF to the treatment groups.

Direct CF was provided to the direct group in the way that article error was underlined and

its correct form was written above it. For the metalinguistic group, metalinguistic feedback

was supplied that indicated error with a coding symbol and then added metalinguistic

explanation the symbol represented. Indirect CF was presented to the indirect group in the

way that marked error by only underlining it (see Appendix for CF samples).

The learners received their writings with corrections in the following treatment session

and reviewed their errors and corrections for 5 minutes. For the metalinguistic group, an

additional sheet with metalinguistic explanations was provided. After reviewing, the

learners engaged in the next task that followed the same procedure as the previous one

Page 11: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition 13

with a different story. Stories written in the third session were given back with con'ections

to the learners prior to the first posttest that was administered two days later. The control

group took part in the same writing tasks as the treatment groups but did not receive any

CF.

7. Procedure

The learners were assigned to one of the treatment groups or a control group before the

experiment began. Each group constituted one of four classes the researcher was teaching

during the period of the experiment, meaning that the learners in each group were from the

same class . This decision was made on the ground that the classes were similarly at low­

intennediate English proficiency level according to the university's placement test results.

The learners completed the pretests in the first week of the experiment to make sure that

levels of knowledge about English article usage were comparable across all groups and to

obtain base data to be compared with those of the posttests. The learners then engaged in

three treatment tasks over the next two weeks. The immediate posttests were administered

right after the learners reviewed their third narratives and corrections, which occurred two

days after the last writing task. The learners completed the delayed posttests two weeks

later. The whole procedure is summarized in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1

Whole Procedure of the Experiment I- Pretest

1 - Treatment sess ion I

- Treatment groups: Narrative writing I - Control group: Narrative writing I

1 - Treatment sess ion 2

- Treatment g roups: Receiving C F on narrative writing I / Narrati ve writing 2 - Control group: Narrative writing 2

1 - Treatment session 3

- Treatment groups: Receiving C F on narrati ve writing 2 / Narrati ve writing 3 - Control group: Narrative writing 3

1 - Immediate posttests

(Treatment groups: Receiving C F on narrati ve writing 3 prior to tests)

I- Delayed posttests

Page 12: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

14 Sung-Sao Jang

8. Data Analysis

Percentage scores were computed from the learners' performance on the narrative

writing tests. However, raw scores were used for the error con'ection tests as the possible

score range was the same across all groups, making it unnecessary to convert raw scores

into percentages. SPSS was used to analyze al l scores, performing one-way ANOYAs,

posthoc pairwise comparison tests using Tukey, and two-way ANOY As. The alpha level

determining significance was set at p < .05 for inferential statistics.

IV. RESULTS

Table 2 presents the mean scores and standard deviations for the four groups over three

error correction tests. A two-way ANOY A was perfonned with mean scores as the

dependent variable and four groups and three testing times as independent variables. The

results revealed that there were both the main effect for group differences, F(3, 150) =

8.561, P < .001, and the main effect for time, F(2, 150) = 59.999, p < .001. A one-way

repeated measures ANOY A showed that the gains the control group made were significant,

F(2, 26) = 10.129, p = .001. Given that all the groups produced significant longitudinal

gains, it is instrumental to examine whether there was a time-group interaction. Results of

a two-way ANOY A showed that the time-group interaction did not reach a statistically

significant level, F(6, 150) = 2.024, p = .066, ns.

One-way ANOY As were performed to see how the groups performed differently at

each testing time. An analysis of mean scores on the pretest showed no statistical

significance, F(3, 50) = .023, p = .995, ns, confirming that all groups were at the

comparable level of explicit knowledge regarding article usage when the experiment

began. Analyses of posttest means revealed that the groups significantly differed on both

the immediate and delayed tests, F(3, 50) = 5.387,p = .003; F(3, 50) = 4.521,p = .007,

respectively. Tukey's posthoc pairwise comparions, which were carried out to locate

where the significant difference lies, revealed that the metalinguistic group performed

better than the indirect and control groups on the immediate test. The metalinguistic

group's superiority over the control group was sustained on the delayed test.

Page 13: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition 15

TABLE 2

Descriptive Statistics for Error Correction Tests

Group Pretest Immediate Posttest Delayed Posttest

M SD M SD M SD DG 3.92 3.23 13.62 5.68 14.46 5.04 MG 4.15 3.74 15.00 2.35 15.77 2.20 IG 4.00 2.51 9.07 5.55 11 .07 5.40 CG 3.86 2.63 8.57 5.85 9.50 6.33

Table 3 displays the mean scores and standard deviations for the narrative writing tests

over time. A two-way ANOV A with mean scores as the dependent variable and four

groups and three testing times as independent variables revealed that there were the main

effect for group differences, F(3, 150) = 4.975, p = .003 , and the main effect for time, F(2 ,

150) = 63 .917, p < .001. As in error correction tests, a one-way repeated measures

ANOVA showed that even the contro l group made significant progress, F(2 , 26) = 15.674,

p < .001. As aU the groups increased test scores over time, a check over time-group

interaction was in need. Results of a two-way ANOV A revealed that the statistica lly

significant time-group interaction did not exist, F( 6, 150) = 1.807, p = .101 , ns .

A one-way ANOVA ana lysis of the groups' pretest mean scores produced no statistical

significance, F(3 , 50) = .073 , p = .974, ns, indicating that all groups were comparable in

their implicit knowledge of English article usage at the beginning of the experiment.

Additional one-way ANOV As for the immediate and delayed posttest scores revealed that

the group differences were statistically significant on both tests, F(3 , 50) = 4.536, p = .007

for the immediate posttest; F(3 , 50) = 3.375, p = .025 for the delayed posttest. Tukey's

posthoc pairwise analyses found that the significant difference in mean scores emerged

between the metalinguistic and control groups. The former group remarkably

outperfonned the latter group on both the immediate and delayed tests.

TABLE 3

Descriptive Statistics for Narrative Writing Tests

Group Pretest Immediate Posttest Delayed Posttest

M SD M SD M SD DG 42.28 20.60 83.21 21.77 82.92 23 .33 MG 41.65 21.77 9 1.30 6.36 94.7 1 6.33 IG 43.33 13.14 75 .75 20.90 81.22 17.63 CG 44 .80 19.04 63 .18 26.96 70.1 9 26.40

V. DISCUSSION

Research question 1 was concerned with whether written CF IS facilitative of L2

Page 14: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

16 Sung-Sao Jang

acquisition. To answer the question, the study measured the learners' abilities to monitor

and use two functions of English articles (i.e., explicit and implicit knowledge of articles),

using elTor cOlTection tests and narrative writing tests respectively. The results revealed

that the metalinguistic group performed better than the control group on both types of

posttests. This means that written CF was beneficial to explicitly developing knowledge of

article usage and to implicitly increasing accuracy of article use. Moreover, such positive

effects continued for long as the meta linguistic group outperformed the control group not

only on the immediate tests but also on the delayed tests. Thus, this study provides another

empirical counterevidence to Truscott's (1996) claim about the ineffectiveness of written

CF on the development ofL2 implicit knowledge.

Research question 2 asked what information should be contained in written CF to

maximize its efficacy in L2 acquisition. As described previously, CF offered to the

treatment groups differed in tenns of the four aspects of information-existence, location,

COlTect form, and nature of the leamer's error. CF for the indirect group contained

information on existence and location of error whi le CF for the direct group provided

COlTect form of elTor in addition to information on its existence and location. The

metalinguistic group 's CF had all the four aspects of information including infonnation on

error nature (i.e., metalinguistic rule explanations).

The results of the experiment showed that only the metalinguistic group outperfonned

the control group. Performances among the other treatment groups and the control group

were not significantly differentiated. This raises two intelTelated issues: why the

metalinguistic group made progress and why the direct and indirect groups perfOlmed

similarly and were not distinguishable from the control group. These questions can be

answered within the theoretical framework of Schmidt's (1993, 1995, 2001) noticing

hypothesis, whose main argument is that attention to L2 grammatical feattu'es is a

necessary path to their acquisition. Attention involves conscious awareness which occurs

at two different levels (Schmidt, 2001). One is the level at which the learner 'notices ' a

new fOlm and the gap between hislher interlanguage output and target language fonn . The

other level refers to 'understanding' rules and patterns involving an L2 fonn. It is argued

that understanding requires more complex cognitive processing and leads to greater L2

learning in comparison with noticing (Schmidt, 2001 ; Sheen, 2010).

It should be noted that as for CF, perceiving its cOlTective intention is critical for its

efficacy on L2 acquisition. This was aptly pointed out in CatToll's (2001) autonomous

induction theory, which claimed that CF cannot work for L2 learning if learners do not

recognize its cOlTective function. It is because, without its recognition, CF is unlikely to

lead to noticing and understanding. Thus, upon receiving CF, the leamer' s attention needs

to be paid preferentially to its cOlTective force to trigger further cognitive processes

necessary for L2 acquisition. In short, three facets of cognitive processing need to be

Page 15: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition 17

sequentially involved in CF-facilitated L2 acquisition: recognizing corrective force of CF,

noticing correct form of error, and lmderstanding rules that govern the form.

With regard to perceiving the corrective force ofCF, it is unlikely that there was notable

difference among the treatment groups because written CF of any type is invariably

explicit in nature as noted previously. This facet of processing therefore does not account

for the metalinguistic group's greater gains in knowledge. In terms of the two levels of

conscious awareness (i.e., noticing and understanding), Sheen (2010) contrasted written

direct CF with written metalinguistic CF. She argued that both types of CF can raise

awareness at the level of noticing due to correct fornl they provide. Visual juxtaposition of

error with its correct form in written mode is most likely to lead to learners' noticing and

noticing-the-gap with little difficulty. However, with an aid of metalinguistic explanation,

metalinguistic CF can further enhance learners' awareness at the level of understanding.

The increased understanding was most likely a main source of the metalinguistic group's

improvement. In contrast, direct CF, in the absence of such metalinguistic explanation, can

only promote understanding "if learners reflect on their errors and work out why they were

incorrect and what the rule is" (Sheen, 20 I 0, p. 226). Then, it is highly probable that

although the learners in the direct group noticed correct form, they did not go so far as to

try to figure out the rules that govern the article functions, which in tum may have brought

about little understanding and, consequently, little progress in their knowledge.

Given that indirect CF did not even provide correct form, the indirect group may not

have been able to engage in noticing let alone understanding. And this could be at least

partially responsible for the indirect group 's relatively poor posttest performance. It is not

clear, however, whether this group 's weak performance was mainly due to the learners'

failure to work out (i.e. , understand) the rule, or to notice, or the combination of both.

One point to be made here is that the control group made significant gains over time,

indicating that there was a test practice effect. It arose in part from the same tests being

administered over three testing periods. Given that all the groups went through the same

testing procedure, it is estimated that the test practice effect also accOlmted for much of the

improvement the treatment groups exhibited over time. However, the fact that only the

meta linguistic group did better than the control group suggested that metalinguistic CF had

a unique efficacy beyond such test practice effect.

It should be noted that metalinguistic CF facilitated not only the learners ' explicit

knowledge but also implicit knowledge. This is paJticulariy significant in rejecting

Truscott 's (1996) argument against written CF effectiveness because he looked into the

effect of CF only in relation with implicit knowledge. Receiving metalinguistic comments

on their errors, the leamers in the metalinguistic group may have had little difficulty in

developing explicit/declarative knowledge when CF targeted only two ftmctions of articles.

However, receiving such CF was probably not sufficient for implicit/procedural

Page 16: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

18 Sung-Soo Jang

knowledge development, considering the limited number of possible CF instances in the

treatment tasks. How then did this group get to develop implicit knowledge? One account

is plausible within skill learning theory, according to which, as mentioned early, practicing

explicit knowledge is what transfonns explicit into implicit knowledge. The metalinguistic

group members may have estab lished explicit knowledge through CF on their writings in

early treatment tasks and utilized the ensuing writing tasks as opportunities to practice and

proceduralize the explicit knowledge, resulting in ultimately developing implicit

knowledge.

The second issue raised above was about non-differentiation in posttest performances

among the direct, indirect and control groups. The non-differentiation may be taken as

evidence that information delivered by CF for the direct and indirect groups (e.g. ,

existence, location, correct form of error) is not sufficient for written CF to contribute to

L2 acquisition. However, such conclusion is probably too simplistic and premature when

learning process is viewed as involving two separate ways-deductive and inductive. The

metalinguistic group was able to engage in deductive learning as their CF provided what

was needed for such learning (i.e., metalinguistic accounts of rules). In contrast, the direct

and indirect groups did not receive information to enable deductive learning. This,

however, does not necessarily justify their inferior test perfomlance. With infomlation

offered in CF, the direct and indirect groups could have at least tried inductive learning,

struggling to work out the rules of articles as noted earlier (Sheen, 2010). The

ineffectiveness of CF seen in the two groups thus demonstrates that they did not engage in

inductive learning.

What then prevented the direct and indirect groups from making inductive endeavors to

make out the target rules? One plausible explanation is that they did not get enough

instances of correction to inductively come up with the article rules. There were only three

treatment tasks and a relatively short story of 78 words was used in each task. The number

of errors and corrections avai lable may have been too limited to make inductive learning

possible. Another explanation is the possibility that the learners did not appreciate CF so

much. Articles are quite low in communicative value but highly complex in their rule

application as their choice is constrained by consideration of both linguistic and pragmatic

environments. This means that misuse of articles rarely disturbed conveying meaning of

the story while the rules for their correct use were not easy to capture. Thus, it can be

argued that even though the learners were explicitly given CF on articles, they may not

have felt a need to focus on such form and discover its related rules on their own as it was

deemed more crucial to retain and convey an original storyline in completing their writing

task at hand.

The findings of this study could help assure language teachers in general and writing

instructors in particular that the effect of written CF is not limited to improving linguistic

Page 17: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types o f In fo rmation in Wri tten Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition 19

accuracy of a revised writing text but it also works to further interlanguage development.

The study also demonstrated that the posi tive effect does not manifest unifonnly across CF

types. In providing written CF, a teacher might want to make sure that it contains

meta linguistic explanation relating to the leamer's error as the study showed that those

receiving CF with such aid made the greatest progress in their knowledge.

It should be noted that choosing a particular pedagogical practice for L2 writing class

based upon findings of research conducted in SLA framework could raise a thorny

question of practical applicability. SLA-oriented research that produced compelling

evidence for the positive role of written CF in L2 acquisition has targeted only a single

linguistic feature. However, in a real writing class, there are various writing aspects of

content, organization, rhetoric, and mechanics that CF can and should target aside from

linguistic errors. Moreover, even when focusing CF exclusively on linguistic accuracy,

few writing teachers, in reality, have the luxury of being able to correct every linguistic

error with its metaJinguistic explanation as implemented in this study and others (Ellis et

aI. , 2008; Sheen, 2007, 2010). Thus, L2 writing practitioners may legitimately doubt the

feasibility of app lying SLA-oriented research findings to a real Wliting class (Fenis, 20 I 0).

One potential solution to the applicability problem is what is called dynamic written CF

advanced by Hartshorn, Evans, Menill, Sudweeks, Strong-Krause, and Anderson (20 10).

They suggested that the alternative approach is to limit the length of the student writing for

which en·or correction is intensively supplied. This way, feedback becomes practically

manageable because "with a shorter piece of writing, teachers can identify all linguistic

errors produced by their students, without overwhelming themselves or their students"

(Hartshorn et a!., 20 I 0, p. 89).

VI. CONCLUSION

The study looked into whether written CF is effective in L2 acquisition and, if so, what

information on the leamer's elTor should be contained in CF to boost its effectiveness. It

was f01Uld that written CF is beneficial to improving both explicit and implicit knowledge

of Engli sh article rules, refuting Truscott's (1996) argument against the utility of written

CF in developing implicit L2 knowledge. However, such positive efficacy surfaced only

when CF provided metalinguistic explanation, whkh shows that only deductive learning,

rather than inductive learn ing, was triggered through CF.

Jt, however, would be premature to generalize the dependence of written CF on the

presence of meta linguistic explanation for its effectiveness based solely upon what the

CUlTent study found. The study has at least two limitations in this regard . As noted above,

the direct and indirect groups perhaps fa iled to produce gains in knowledge due to

Page 18: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

20 Sung-Soo Jang

inadequate supplies of error correction over three task sessions. A longer treatment might

have enabled inductive leaIlling and produced beneficial effects of non-metalinguistic CF

(i.e., direct and indirect CF). Another limitation is that the study did not isolate the effect

of pure metalinguistic explaI1ation as metalinguistic CF included other information such as

ex istence, location, and COlTect form of error. It is therefore still unclear whether or not the

effectiveness of metalinguistic CF CaI11e exclusively from the workings of metalinguistic

explanation.

Future research then should be designed with CF that is given in more treatments over a

longer period and that isolates metalinguistic explanation from other error-related

information. This way, the question of what constitutes optimal infonnation required in

written CF for L2 acquisition will be more adequately addressed. And this in tum can

more infonnatively help L2 writing teachers to implement written CF in real writing

classrooms.

There are two additional issues that merit further SLA-oriented writing investigation.

Written CF studies adopting SLA methodology have mostly chosen English articles as a

target lingui stic feature (Bitchener & Knoch, 2008; Bitchener, Young, & Cameron, 2005;

Ellis et a\., 2008; Sheen, 2007) and the present study was no exception. Inspecting the

efficacy of written CF targeting a variety of different grammatical fonns and structures is

definitely in order. The other issue is concerned with the value of revision. Traditionally,

the leamer's capability of revising his/her original writing text to produce the same text of

higher quality was taken as a measure for the writing class 's success. Revision was the

purpose and object of L2 writing class and treated as a dependent variable in L2 writing

studies. Recently, SLA researchers took a different perspective of revision (Ferris, 2004,

2006, 2010; Sachs & Polio, 2007). FelTis (2010) claimed that "revision and editing of a

text after receiving written CF may be a helpful and perhaps necessary intermediate step

between expert feedback about a target feature and long-term acquisition of that feature"

(p . 189). The feasibility and value of treating revision as a pedagogical means rather than

goal has yet to be empirically proven. To answer this issue, future research should take

revision as an independent variable and examine its efficacy on L2 acquisition.

Despite having a long way to go before getting any clear picture of how written CF is

connected with L2 acquisition, research in this line has its unique value. Methodologically,

written CF research is relatively easy to conduct in that written errors are quite easy to

identifY and cOlTections are not so hard to manipulate in comparison with its oral

counterparts or other pedagogical interventions. More importantly, as Sheen (2010)

pointed out, CF research is directly and closely relevant to language pedagogy as CF is a

pedagogical technique that teachers frequently use in their classrooms. This close

association signifies that CF research can inform language teachers of effective ways to

COlTect learners' elTors. In a similar vein, a classroom rich in errors and cOlTections may

Page 19: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition 2 1

serve as an appropriate site for conducting CF research. In this sense, "CF is an ideal

object of inquiry for researcher-teacher collaboration and constitutes an area of inquiry that

can connect theory, research, and practice" (Sheen, 20 10, p. 177). As written CF is

invariably explicit, it is less likely to be missed out by learners than oral CF. Thus, the

relevance and connection of CF with pedagogy are most likely stronger when it is

provided in written than oral mode. This leaves a certain legitimacy to call for continued

and enriched written CF studies, counterbalancing the predominance of oral CF studies to

date in SLA literature.

REFERENCES

Ammar, A., & Spada, N. (2006) . One size fits all? Recasts, prompts, and L2 learning.

Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 543-574.

Ashwell, T. (2000). Patterns of teacher response to student writing in a multiple-draft

composition classroom: Is content feedback fo llowed by forn1 feedback the best

method? Journal of Second Language Writing, 9, 227-258.

Bitchener, J. , & Knoch, U. (2008) . The value of written corrective feedback for migrant

and international students. Language Teaching Research, 12, 409-431 .

Bitchener, 1. , Young, S. , & Cameron, D. (2005). The effect of different types of corrective

feedback on ESL student writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 9, 227-258

Carroll, S. (200 I ). input and evidence: The raw material of second language acquisition.

Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Carroll , S., & Swain, M. (1993). Explicit and implicit negative feedback: An empirical

study of the learning of linguistic generalizations. Studies in Second Language

Acquisition, 15. 357-386.

Carroll , S., Swain, M., & Roberge, Y. ( 1992). The ro le of feedback in adult second

language acquisition: Error correction and morphological generalizations. Applied

Psycholinguistics, 13, 173-1 98.

Chandler, 1. (2003). The efficacy of various kinds of error feedback for improvement in

the accuracy and fluency of L2 student writing. Journal of Second Language

Writing, 12, 267-296.

Chaudron, C. (1977). A descriptive model of discourse in the corrective treatment of

learners' errors. Language Learning, 27, 29-46.

DeKeyser, R. ( 1998). Beyond focus on fom1: Cognitive perspectives on learning and

practicing second language grammar. In C. 1. Doughty & 1. Williams (Eds.), Focus

on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 42-63). New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Page 20: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

22 Sung-Soo Jang

DeKeyser, R. (2001). Automaticity and automatization. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition

and second language instruction (pp. 125-151). Cambridge: Cambridge University

Press.

DeKeyser, R. (2007). Skill acquisition theory. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.),

Theories in second language acquisition (pp. 97-113). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum .

Doughty, C. 1. (2003). Instmcted SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In C.

1. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition

(pp. 256-310). Oxford: Blackwell.

Egi, T. (2007). Interpreting recasts as linguistic evidence: The roles oflength and degree of

change. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 511-537.

Elli s, R., Loewen, S., & Erlam, R. (2006). Implicit and explicit con-ective feedback and the

acquisition ofL2 grammar. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 339-368.

Ellis, R ., Sheen, Y., Murakami, M. , & Takashima, H. (2008) . The effects of focused and

unfocused written con-ective feedback in an English as a foreign language context.

System, 36, 353-37l.

Eskey, D. E. (1983). Meanwhile, back in the real world ... : Accuracy and fluency in second

language teaching. TESOL Quarterly, 17, 315-323.

Ferris, D. R. (2004). The "grammar con-ection" debate in L2 writing: Where are we, and

where do we go from here? (and what do we do in the meantime .. . ?). Journal of

Second Language Writing, 13, 49-62.

Ferris, D. R. (2006). Does error feedback help student writers: New evidence on the short­

and long-term effects of written error con-ection. In K. Hyland & F. Hyland (Eds.),

Feedback in second language writing: Contexts and issues (pp. 81 -104).

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ferris, D. R. (2010). Second language writing research and written corrective feedback in

SLA: Intersections and practical applications. Studies in Second Language

Acquisition, 32, 181-20l.

Frodesen, J. (1991). Grammar in writing. In M. Celce-Murcia (Ed.), Teaching English as a

second or foreign language (2nd ed.) (pp. 264-276). Boston: Heinle & Heinle.

Hartshorn, K. 1., Evans, N. W., Merrill, P. F., Sudweeks, R. R., Strong-Krause, D. , &

Anderson, N. 1. (2010). Effects of dynamic corrective feedback on ESL writing

accuracy. TESOL Quarterly, 44, 84-109.

Horowitz. D . (1986). Process not product: Less than meets the eye. TESOL Quarterly, 20,

141-144.

Jolms, A. M. (1995). Genre and pedagogical purposes. Journal of Second Language

Writing, 4, 181-190.

Krashen, S. (2003). Explorations in language acquisition and use. Protsmouth, NH:

Heinemann.

Page 21: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Efficacy on L2 Acquisition 23

Lane, J., & Lange, E. ( 1999). Writing clearly: An editing guide (2nd ed.). Boston: Heinle

& Heinle.

Leeman, 1. (2003). Recasts and second language development: Beyond negative evidence.

Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25(1),37-63 .

Leki , 1. (1990). Coaching from the margins: Issues in written response. In B. F. Kroll (Ed.),

Second language writing: Research insights fo r the classroom (pp. 57-68). New

York: Cambridge University Press.

Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition.

In W. Ritchie & T. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp.

413-468). New York: Academic Press.

Long, M. H. (2007). Recasts in SLA: The story so far. In M. H. Long (Ed.), Problems in

SLA (pp. 75-1 16). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Long, M. H., Inagaki, S., & Ortega, L. (1998). The ro le of implicit negative feedback in

SLA: Models and recasts in Japanese and Spanish. Modern Language Journal, 82,

357-371.

Lyster, R. (2004). Differential effects of prompts and recasts in fonn-focused instruction.

Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 399-432.

Lyster, R. , & Mori, H. (2006). Interactional feedback and instructional counterbalance.

Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 269-300.

Mackey, A., & Phi lp, J. (1998). Conversational interaction and second language

development: Recasts, responses, and red herrings? Modern Language Journal, 82,

338-356.

Muranoi , H. (2000). Focus on fonn through interaction enhancement: Integrating fo rnl a I

instruction into a communicative task in EFL classrooms. Language Learning, 50,

617-673.

Pienemann, M., Jolmston, M., & Brindley, G. (1988). Constructing an acquisition-based

procedure for second language assessment. Studies in Second Language

Acquisition, 10, 2 17-243.

Reid, 1. (1994). Responding to ESL students' texts: The myths of appropriation. TESOL

Quarterly, 28, 273-292.

Robb, T., Ross, S., & Shortreed, 1. (1986). Salience offeedback on error and its effects on

EFL writing quality . TESOL Quarterly, 20, 83-93 .

Sachs, R., & Polio, C. (2007). Learners' uses of two types of written feedback on a L2

writing revision task. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 67-100.

Sclunidt, R. (1993). Awareness and second language acquisition. Annual Review of

Applied Linguistics, 13, 206-226.

Sclmlidt, R. ( 1995). Attention and awareness in foreign language learning. Honolulu:

Ulliversity ofHawai'i Press.

Page 22: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

24 Sung-Soo Jang

Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language

instruction (pp. 3-32). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Semke, H . (1984). The effects of the red pen. Foreign Language Annals, 17, 195-202.

Sheen, Y. (2006). Exploring the relationship between characteristics of recasts and learner

uptake. Language Teaching Research, 11 , 361 -392.

Sheen, Y. (2007). The effect offocused written corrective feedback and language aptitude

on ESL learners' acquisition of articles. TESOL Quarterly, 41, 255-283.

Sheen, Y . (2008). Recasts, language anxiety, modified output and L2 learning. Language

Learning, 58, 835-874.

Sheen, Y. (2010). Differential effects of oral and written corrective feedback in the ESL

classroom. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32, 203-234.

Sinclair, M. (1991). Corpus, concordance, collocation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Swain, M . (1995). Three functions of output in second language learning. In G. Cook & B.

Seidhofer (Eds.), Principle and practice in applied linguistics: Studies in honor of

H G. Widdowson (pp. 125- [44). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Truscott, J. (1996). The case against grammar correction in L2 writing classes. Language

Learning, 46, 327-369.

APPENDIX Written Corrective Feedback Samples

1. Direct CF

a

A moment later, he saw the dog biting the bag.

2. Indirect CF

He looked into _ bag but f! sandwich was not in it.

3. Metalinguistic CF

A man was traveling in _(*) small town near f!(+) Chicago. He went into a fast food

restaurant in f!(#) town.

(*): a is needed before a singular noun that is first mentioned.

(+): a is not used before a city name.

(#): the is needed before a noun that is previously mentioned.

Page 23: Types of Information in Written Corrective Feedback and

Types ofInfo rmation in Written Corrective Feedback and Its Effi cacy on L2 Acquis ition 25

note. 1) All samples are from this study' s data.

2) Metalinguistic explanation was provided in Korean to the learners.

App li cable leve ls: tertiary education

Key words: written corrective feed back, consc ious awareness, noticing, understanding, exp licit knowledge,

implicit knowledge

Sung-Soo Jang

College of General Education

Inje Uni versity

607 Obang-dong, Gimhae

Gyeongsangnam-do, 62 1-749, Korea

Fax: (055) 339-3734

Email : elans ing@han mail.net

Rece ived in June, 201 2

Rev iewed in July, 201 2

Revised vers ion received in August, 20 12