9
Acta Neuropsychiatrica 2011: 23: 3–11 All rights reserved DOI: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2010.00508.x © 2011 John Wiley & Sons A/S ACTA NEUROPSYCHIATRICA Review article Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer? Soh NL, Walter G. Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer? Objective: To compare the scientific content of recent general media articles on tryptophan, diet and depression, with recent empirical research into dietary manipulation of tryptophan published in the scientific literature. Method: A review of the recent empirical research into the role of tryptophan in depression, focusing on dietary methods to influence tryptophan levels. In parallel, a review of recent articles in the general English language media regarding tryptophan and mood. Results: Empirical evidence for improving mood through dietary manipulation of tryptophan is lacking, and it is difficult to change plasma tryptophan levels through diet alone. Tryptophan supplementation and depletion studies suggest that altering tryptophan levels may only benefit certain groups of patients who have a personal or family history of depression. Scientific studies also focus on elucidating mechanisms in depression, rather than treating depression by changing tryptophan levels. However, general media articles often recommend diets and foods to increase blood tryptophan levels and raise brain serotonin levels. Such recommendations are not supported by scientific studies. Conclusion: It is very difficult to alter blood tryptophan levels through dietary methods alone, outside of a laboratory or research setting. Only a small number of lay articles provide sound advice, with general media reports on tryptophan often being hyperbolic and misleading. A clinician should be aware of the type of (mis)information a patient may have accessed and have the scientific knowledge to explain the impracticalities of influencing tryptophan levels through diet alone. Nerissa L. Soh 1 , Garry Walter 1,2,3 1 Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services, Northern Sydney Central Coast Area Health, North Ryde New South Wales, Australia; 2 Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia; and 3 Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada Keywords: depression; diet; media; serotonin; tryptophan Dr Nerissa L. Soh, Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services, Northern Sydney Central Coast Area Health, Coral Tree Family Service, PO Box 142, North Ryde NSW 1670, Australia. Tel: +61 2 8877 5371; Fax: +61 2 9887 2941; E-mail: [email protected] Introduction A patient with symptoms of depression is assessed by a psychiatrist and, when treatment is discussed, mentions reading about tryptophan being of potential benefit ‘because it turns into serotonin in the brain’. The patient wants to try a tryptophan-rich diet and asks for the psychiatrist’s overall opinion about the matter and advice on how to achieve such a diet. What advice should the psychiatrist give? This review focuses on dietary methods to influence tryp- tophan levels, the clinical applications this may have and the empirical research supporting the role of tryp- tophan in depression, particularly studies published in the last 10 years and also earlier relevant studies. The article also examines the information provided by the general English language media regarding tryptophan and mood over the past few years, but does not focus on the role of tryptophan in sleep disorders, behaviour, anxiety, attention or cognition. Tryptophan’s role in mood Tryptophan’s potential as a treatment for depression has been given much attention by the general media. A search using ‘tryptophan’ and ‘mood’ as keywords in Factiva, a search engine for general media publications, particularly newspapers and magazines, yielded 1461 citations from 1986 to February 2010. The Factiva search was for English language articles 3

Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Acta Neuropsychiatrica 2011: 23: 3–11All rights reservedDOI: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2010.00508.x

© 2011 John Wiley & Sons A/S

ACTA NEUROPSYCHIATRICA

Review article

Tryptophan and depression: can diet alonebe the answer?

Soh NL, Walter G. Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be theanswer?

Objective: To compare the scientific content of recent general mediaarticles on tryptophan, diet and depression, with recent empirical researchinto dietary manipulation of tryptophan published in the scientificliterature.Method: A review of the recent empirical research into the role oftryptophan in depression, focusing on dietary methods to influencetryptophan levels. In parallel, a review of recent articles in the generalEnglish language media regarding tryptophan and mood.Results: Empirical evidence for improving mood through dietarymanipulation of tryptophan is lacking, and it is difficult to change plasmatryptophan levels through diet alone. Tryptophan supplementation anddepletion studies suggest that altering tryptophan levels may only benefitcertain groups of patients who have a personal or family history ofdepression. Scientific studies also focus on elucidating mechanisms indepression, rather than treating depression by changing tryptophan levels.However, general media articles often recommend diets and foods toincrease blood tryptophan levels and raise brain serotonin levels. Suchrecommendations are not supported by scientific studies.Conclusion: It is very difficult to alter blood tryptophan levels throughdietary methods alone, outside of a laboratory or research setting. Only asmall number of lay articles provide sound advice, with general mediareports on tryptophan often being hyperbolic and misleading. A clinicianshould be aware of the type of (mis)information a patient may haveaccessed and have the scientific knowledge to explain the impracticalitiesof influencing tryptophan levels through diet alone.

Nerissa L. Soh1,Garry Walter1,2,3

1Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services,Northern Sydney Central Coast Area Health, NorthRyde New South Wales, Australia; 2Child andAdolescent Psychiatry, University of Sydney, NewSouth Wales, Australia; and 3Department ofPsychiatry, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada

Keywords: depression; diet; media;serotonin; tryptophan

Dr Nerissa L. Soh,Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services,Northern Sydney Central Coast Area Health, CoralTree Family Service, PO Box 142, North Ryde NSW1670, Australia.Tel: +61 2 8877 5371;Fax: +61 2 9887 2941;E-mail: [email protected]

Introduction

A patient with symptoms of depression is assessedby a psychiatrist and, when treatment is discussed,mentions reading about tryptophan being of potentialbenefit ‘because it turns into serotonin in the brain’.The patient wants to try a tryptophan-rich diet andasks for the psychiatrist’s overall opinion about thematter and advice on how to achieve such a diet.

What advice should the psychiatrist give? Thisreview focuses on dietary methods to influence tryp-tophan levels, the clinical applications this may haveand the empirical research supporting the role of tryp-tophan in depression, particularly studies publishedin the last 10 years and also earlier relevant studies.

The article also examines the information providedby the general English language media regardingtryptophan and mood over the past few years, butdoes not focus on the role of tryptophan in sleepdisorders, behaviour, anxiety, attention or cognition.

Tryptophan’s role in mood

Tryptophan’s potential as a treatment for depressionhas been given much attention by the general media.A search using ‘tryptophan’ and ‘mood’ as keywordsin Factiva, a search engine for general mediapublications, particularly newspapers and magazines,yielded 1461 citations from 1986 to February 2010.The Factiva search was for English language articles

3

Page 2: Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Soh and Walter

and these were published in newspapers from a widerange of countries and regions, including the UnitedKingdom, United States, Canada, the Philippines,India, Pakistan, Middle-East, Singapore, Australiaand New Zealand.

Tryptophan is an essential amino acid; a sub-unit in protein molecules and a precursor to sero-tonin. It cannot be synthesised by the body andmust be obtained only through diet; the averagedaily requirement for adult humans being 4 mg/kgbody weight (1). A Cochrane Review of the efficacyof oral tryptophan and 5-hydroxytryptophan (5-HT)supplementation as treatments for depression con-cluded that there was insufficient evidence that eitheris more effective than placebo in unipolar depres-sion (2). However, because of the strictness of theinclusion criteria, there were only two studies, pub-lished in 1972 and 1982, in this meta-analysis. Allthe studies reviewed, including those rejected, werequite old and were published from 1964 to 1999,despite a search update in 2004.

Altering dietary intakes of protein and carbohy-drate can theoretically modify mood by manipulat-ing tryptophan uptake by the brain. The Wurtmanhypothesis states that a carbohydrate-rich meal stim-ulates insulin secretion and causes most of the aminoacids in the bloodstream, other than tryptophan, tobe taken up by the peripheral tissues (3–5). Theresulting higher ratio of tryptophan to other largeneutral amino acids (LNAAs) (valine, isoleucine,leucine, tyrosine, phenylalanine, methionine and his-tidine) (6) leads to a greater transport of tryptophanto the brain.

Changing tryptophan levels by diet

There are very few published studies in humanswhich manipulate plasma tryptophan levels bydietary means alone. Merens et al.’s 2007 reviewof empirical studies that manipulated tryptophan lev-els through supplementation or depletion techniquesdid not report any studies that used solely dietarymethods (7). The study by Markus et al. (8) is oneof the few to use dietary techniques. Plasma tryp-tophan:LNAA ratios rose significantly on admin-istering high-carbohydrate-low-protein diets (pro-tein content 3.9–5% of caloric intake), by anaverage of 42% above the ratio when on high-protein-low-carbohydrate diets. Inclination to stresswas measured through the Inadequacy Scale ofthe Dutch Personality Inventory, which assessesneuroticism, and in individuals highly prone tostress, high-protein-low-carbohydrate diets increaseddepression when stress was induced. However, inindividuals less prone to stress, both high-protein-low-carbohydrate and low-protein-high-carbohydrate

diets led to increased severity of depression. It is notknown if the test diets influenced the fasting aminoacid profiles as baseline plasma amino acid ratioswere not measured, and also the test meals wereadministered only on the test day rather than as long-term diets. Wurtman et al.’s more recent study (9)showed that the protein and carbohydrate content of ameal can significantly alter plasma tryptophan:LNAAratios in healthy adults. In this 2003 randomisedcrossover trial, a high-carbohydrate breakfast (69.9 gcarbohydrate, 5.2 g protein) initially lowered tryp-tophan:LNAA ratios below baseline, with the ratiothen increasing over the 3 h of the test, albeit notsignificantly. In contrast, the high-protein breakfast(15.4 g carbohydrate, 46.8 g protein) significantlylowered plasma tryptophan:LNAA ratios from base-line, by 35% at 3 h. Wurtman et al.’s test meals weredesigned to reflect typical breakfasts consumed inthe United States, but the study had a small sample(n = 9) and did not include psychological measures;it is not known if the changes in amino acid pro-file were associated with mood changes. The authorshighlighted that while the combination of variousfoods in a meal would mitigate changes to serotoninsynthesis, snacks may be composed of a single fooditem and if it is very high or low in protein, its impacton brain serotonin would be mostly unmoderated.However, Wurtman et al. also noted that the effectswould be confounded by the composition of priormeals (9). In this vein, the composition of subsequentmeals, snacks or beverages and the time betweenconsumption would also confound the effects of a‘single-item snack’ on serotonin levels. It is question-able how long the changes in brain serotonin wouldbe sustained in daily life outside of the laboratory.

Energy restriction also lowers plasma tryptophan,but its impact on mood is not consistent. Attenburrowet al.’s study of 50 healthy women (10) found lowcalorie diets (1000 kcal/day) followed over 3 weekssignificantly lowered total plasma tryptophan levelsand total plasma tryptophan:branched chain aminoacid ratios, but did not change levels of depression oranxiety symptoms. Further, in a randomised, double-blind placebo-controlled method, the women weresupplemented with 1.8 g tryptophan/day derivedfrom lactalbumin (10). The supplementation did notcircumvent the fall in plasma tryptophan induced bythe weight-loss diet. Attenburrow’s group hypothe-sised that food restriction may upregulate tryptophanmetabolism for which the tryptophan supplementscould not compensate. In contrast, a much older studyby Schweiger et al. (11) into weight-loss diets foundthat plasma tryptophan:LNAA ratios correlated withoverall mood. However, unlike Attenburrow’s studywhere the diet had a fixed macronutrient composi-tion, participants in Schweiger’s study were advised

4

Page 3: Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Tryptophan, diet and depression

only on maintaining a ‘mixed’ or a ‘vegetarian’ dietand so the proportions of protein and carbohydrateintake were not controlled.

Changing tryptophan levels by supplementationand depletion methods

Most empirical studies use supplementation, intra-venous infusions or tryptophan depletion techniquesto alter tryptophan and serotonin levels. Supple-mentation methods include high-tryptophan proteindietary supplements, such as α-lactalbumin, andamino acid supplements. Acute tryptophan depletionis effected through administering amino acid mix-tures without tryptophan, which stimulates proteinsynthesis by the liver and subsequently lowers thebody’s endogenous pool of plasma tryptophan (12);it is a popular research technique for modifyingplasma tryptophan levels.

As tryptophan is a limiting amino acid in mostfood proteins (3), there are few foods that canbe used as a source of tryptophan. One ‘food’that has been used as a supplement in studiesis α-lactalbumin, a tryptophan-rich whey protein.To investigate the cognitive and mood effects ofα-lactalbumin, Booij et al. (13) supplemented low-protein-high-carbohydrate diets with α-lactalbuminin individuals recovered from depression and incontrols. Although the plasma tryptophan:LNAAratio was 71.5% higher in those supplemented withα-lactalbumin compared with the casein placeboand was 77.5% higher than baseline, there wasno significant improvement in mood from base-line or compared with the placebo group. Firk andMarkus (14) found that individuals supplementedwith α-lactalbumin had improved mood but this didnot occur with casein supplementation; however,this study did not measure plasma amino acid pro-files. Merens et al. (15) significantly increased theplasma tryptophan:LNAA ratio in individuals witha history of depression and in healthy participantsby 21% from baseline, also using α-lactalbumin.α-Lactalbumin did not elicit a significant differencein mood compared with those taking casein sup-plements as a placebo, despite casein promoting asignificant decrease in tryptophan:LNAA, and therewere minimal differences when the participants wereplaced under stress. It is possible that a larger loadof α-lactalbumin is required to influence mood (15).

An older study by Beulens et al. (16) raisedplasma tryptophan:LNAA ratios in young healthymen by 16% by supplementing a normal diet withα-lactalbumin. The researchers also found no signif-icant difference in mood between supplement andplacebo groups an hour after consumption. All trialswere randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled

and crossover, and, like the studies by Markus et al.and Wurtman et al. (8,9), the diets and supplementswere administered only on test days and not as long-term dietary regimes. Thus, the findings can onlyrepresent acute effects. It is not clear whether thehealthy status and/or youth of the participants ren-dered them insensitive to such changes in the tryp-tophan:LNAA ratio or whether a greater elevationof the ratio would be required in order for dif-ferences in mood to be detected. Murphy et al.’splacebo-controlled trial is notable in that it supple-mented healthy participants with 3 g tryptophan/dayfor 14 days (17). This trial of 38 participants foundthat tryptophan induced a positive bias in women’sprocessing of emotional material after 14 days, butnot men’s, indicating women may be more sensi-tive to changes in serotonin level. In a similar vein,Moreno et al.’s study of 59 individuals in remissionfor depressive symptoms found women had signifi-cantly greater depressive responses to acute trypto-phan depletion compared with men (18). However,neither study measured plasma or cerebrospinal fluid(CSF) tryptophan levels, and the background diet wasnot controlled for, which may be particularly perti-nent in Murphy’s lengthier trial.

The form in which tryptophan is administeredalso influences its in vivo effects. Markus et al. (19)investigated the differences in administering trypto-phan as intact α-lactalbumin, a protein hydrolysate,pure tryptophan and as a tryptophan dipeptide.Despite providing the same quantities of tryptophan,mood in healthy individuals improved significantlyonly with the protein hydrolysate and pure trypto-phan supplements but not with the casein controlor other forms of tryptophan. The plasma trypto-phan:LNAA ratio increased significantly from base-line with all four versions of tryptophan (peakincreases being 263% for the peptide, 255% forthe hydrolysate, 191% for pure tryptophan and 67%for α-lactalbumin), although with α-lactalbumin theincrease was significantly lower than the proteinhydrolysate and pure tryptophan (19). The source oftryptophan may have implications when trying toincrease plasma tryptophan through dietary meansalone.

Whether or not an individual has a history ofmood disorders also influences their susceptibility tochanges in tryptophan levels. Meren’s 2007 reviewnoted that in healthy subjects, tryptophan manipula-tion had minimal effect on mood and while mooddid decrease in patients with a past or family historyof depression, the results were inconsistent (7). Evenin older individuals in remission and on medication,acute tryptophan depletion had no significant effecton mood and it was hypothesised that longer remis-sion times may be protective (20). Moreno et al.’s

5

Page 4: Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Soh and Walter

prospective study (21) observed that individuals weremore likely to experience a depressive episode whenfollowed up over the course of a year, if they hada depressive response to acute tryptophan deple-tion. Moreno included individuals in remission formajor depression and healthy controls and found nocorrelation between the number of previous majordepression episodes and the occurrence of episodeson follow-up, although it was noted that the samplesize was small (n = 24). The inconsistent impact oftryptophan depletion on the mood of those with ahistory of depression was also highlighted by Booijet al.’s meta-analysis (22), where chronic depression,recurrent depressive episodes, being female, previoustreatment with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors(SSRIs) and a history of serious suicidal thoughtsor attempts were independent predictors of a moodresponse in patients. In Klaassen et al.’s study ofhealthy individuals with and without a family his-tory of depression, tryptophan depletion significantlylowered plasma tryptophan and tryptophan:LNAAratios after 6 h compared with placebo, which wererestored after 24 h, but there were no differencesat baseline (23). There was no difference in moodat baseline between placebo and tryptophan-depletedgroups, but mood significantly lowered in the latter at6 h only in those with a family history of depression.Klaassen et al.’s study is notable as it also controlledbackground protein intake using low-protein diet forthe 24 h of the study.

As highlighted by Merens et al. (7), there is a lackof studies in participants with current depression.Booij et al. (24) studied acute tryptophan depletionin patients who were depressed and receiving antide-pressant medication. Depletion of tryptophan and sig-nificantly lowering of tryptophan:LNAA ratios wereassociated with improved mood in patients receivingselective serotonin–noradrenaline reuptake inhibitors(SSNRIs) but there was no change in mood in thosereceiving other types of medication. The reason givenfor the improvement was that long-term treatmentwith SSNRIs desensitises and downregulates sero-tonin receptors 5-HT1, 1A and 2, but in patientswho have not been medicated for very long, deple-tion of tryptophan would not yield the usual decreasein serotonin because of the SSNRIs, and thus mayincrease post-synaptive 5-HT receptor activity. How-ever, the sample size was small (n = 14), withthe authors acknowledging the difficulty of recruit-ing depressed participants. Delgado et al.’s 1994study (25), although older, is remarkable for its largesample of currently depressed patients who were noton medication (n = 43). This double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover trial found that acute trypto-phan depletion significantly reduced plasma-free andtotal tryptophan levels by 83%, but there was no

significant change in depressive symptoms. A lagtime was observed, where there were significant andheterogeneous changes in depressive symptom sever-ity the following day, when plasma tryptophan levelshad recovered. Individuals who had responded to pre-vious antidepressant treatments reported improvedmood according to the Hamilton Depression Rat-ing Scale when compared with placebo, while thosewho had not responded to prior antidepressant treat-ments scored worse (increase by 5–9 points) ormuch worse (increase by 10 or more points) in theirdepressive symptomatology (25). It was speculatedthat acute tryptophan depletion and repletion mayhave sensitised some of the patients’ serotonin func-tion, hence their improved mood, while those whoreported worse mood may have impaired serotoninmetabolism and thus were less able to compensatefor tryptophan depletion. The authors suggested thatserotonergic antidepressants may not be efficaciousin improving the mood of patients with impairedserotonin metabolism.

Plasma tryptophan versus brain tryptophan and serotonin

Most human studies measure plasma tryptophan lev-els, and thus do not directly show whether the tech-niques which alter tryptophan levels in the bloodtranslate to changes in the uptake and metabolismof tryptophan by the brain. Roiser et al. (26) investi-gated regional blood flow in the brain following acutetryptophan depletion, but found no significant changein mood either in healthy participants or in patientswith remitted major depressive disorder. There weredifferences in hemodynamic responses between thetwo groups depending on the treatment. However,the results could not confirm if the hemodynamic dif-ferences were mediated by serotonin and tryptophan.Blomstrand et al. (27) inferred tryptophan uptake bythe brain in human subjects through the difference inblood concentration of tryptophan in the radial arterycompared with that of the jugular vein, although thisstudy’s focus was on the impact of carbohydrateintake on amino acid uptake during exercise, ratherthan mood.

Radio-labelled 3-(11)C-amino-4(2-dimethylamino-methylphenylsulfanyl)benzonitrile has been used tostudy the binding potential of the serotonin trans-porter 5-HTT in the brains of healthy humans (28).Binding potential is an index of the expression andaffinity of 5-HTT and this study found that there wasno difference in 5-HTT binding potential betweencontrol and depleted states in any region of thebrain assessed. This was despite significantly low-ering plasma tryptophan by 90% through tryptophandepletion, indicating that the degree of transport ofserotonin in the brain was not altered. Mood was not

6

Page 5: Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Tryptophan, diet and depression

assessed but the absence of change in binding poten-tial is congruent with the frequent observation thathealthy individuals are protected against the mood-lowering effects of tryptophan depletion (28). Itwould be interesting to see this study repeated in indi-viduals with depression or a history of depression.

Most studies in humans extrapolate changes inplasma tryptophan to changes in brain levels andvery few studies assess CSF levels of serotonin ortryptophan. The validity of CSF serotonin levels asan index of changes in brain serotonin has alsobeen questioned, with neuroimaging (using radioli-gands) offered as a more sensitive method of assess-ing serotonin metabolism in the brain (29). Salomonet al. (30) measured CSF levels of tryptophan and5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), a metaboliteof tryptophan, during tryptophan depletion in individ-uals being treated with sertraline or bupropion. Tryp-tophan depletion elicited a relapse in mood in thosewhose CSF tryptophan fell below 40 nM and deple-tion led to a significant decrease in CSF tryptophanand 5-HIAA and in plasma tryptophan. There wasa strong and significant inverse correlation betweenmood and CSF tryptophan levels during tryptophandepletion, but mood did not correlate significantlywith CSF 5-HIAA or plasma-free tryptophan levels.The association between plasma and CSF levels wasnot consistent: at baseline and before commencingsertraline or bupropion, there was a significant andstrong correlation between CSF and free plasma tryp-tophan, but no significant correlation between CSFtryptophan and 5-HIAA. Following 5 weeks of med-ication, both correlations were not significant. Theresearchers acknowledge that their sample size wassmall (n = 12), making it inappropriate to draw gen-eralisations (30), and this also reflects the difficultiesof recruiting participants for a study with such ademanding protocol.

Tryptophan, mood and the popular press

Much of the scientific research into tryptophan andmood has focused on elucidating mechanisms formood disturbances, rather than trialling tryptophanmanipulation as a clinical treatment. A complicat-ing factor in the interpretation of these studies is thatnot all controlled the composition of the participants’background or baseline diets. The effects of low-ering tryptophan levels may not be purely becauseof serotonin metabolism and may also be medi-ated through other neurotransmitter pathways (31).Further, as tryptophan depletion induces or wors-ens depressive symptoms only in certain groups,depression may not be mediated solely by reducedserotonin (32). In addition, a large epidemiologicalstudy of 29 133 men found no association between

dietary tryptophan intake and self-reported depressedmood, hospital admission for depressive disorders orsuicide (33). In contrast, articles in the lay mediaemphasise ‘treating’ mood through altering trypto-phan levels and thus serotonin levels.

A pure carbohydrate load in fasting individ-uals will significantly increase the plasma tryp-tophan:LNAA ratio compared with placebo (34).Despite scientific commentary and empirical researchshowing the impracticality of using the Wurtmaneffect to alter tryptophan and serotonin levels, theWurtman effect is constantly invoked by the generalmedia. Implementing the Wurtman effect in practiceis problematic. Benton and Donohoe (3) state that amere 5% of calories as protein is sufficient to curb theincrease in the tryptophan:LNAA ratio and that even‘high-carbohydrate’ foods typically still contain suf-ficient protein to prevent this increase. Young (6) putthe protein threshold at 4% of total energy. Bentonand Donohoe (3) note that there is a clear increasein the plasma tryptophan:LNAA ratio when the mealcontains less than 2% of its calories as protein, butthere is little evidence that carbohydrate specificallybenefits depressed individuals.

General media reports regarding the effects oftryptophan are often hyperbolic, misleading andambiguous, with much of their advice and informa-tion being inconsistent with Wurtman’s hypothesisand even basic biochemistry. The list of ‘tryptophan-rich foods’ extolled by lay publications is wide-ranging and includes bananas, turkey, chocolate,dates, papaya, poultry, milk, oats, various nuts andbeans, chickpeas, sunflower seeds, dairy foods, avo-cado, eggs, red meat, soybeans and soy foods, tuna,shellfish, brown rice, lentils, lobster, seafood, whole-grains, hummus, pineapple, asparagus, beetroot andchicken soup (33–52). Protein-rich foods such asmeat, poultry, dairy products, nuts, beans and eggscontain tryptophan (Table 1), but tryptophan is thelimiting amino acid in most protein sources (1,3,6),meaning that it is the essential amino acid which ispresent in the lowest quantity in that food source. Ascan be seen in Table 1, fruits and vegetables are gen-erally poor protein sources, including those recom-mended by the general media, and cannot be viewedas concentrated sources of tryptophan. Another con-sideration is that even if a food is high in proteinand therefore high in tryptophan, it may not be nor-mally eaten in sufficient quantities or frequency tobe a significant dietary source.

While a few lay articles provide sound andscientific information (54–56), many reports areambiguous or misleading. Carbohydrate foods arealso recommended to increase serotonin levels,although the way these foods do this is not alwaysexplained (45,50). Recommending milk as a source

7

Page 6: Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Soh and Walter

Table 1. Total protein and tryptophan levels in various ‘tryptophan-rich foods’commonly cited by the general media

Food

Protein(g/100 g edible

portion)

Tryptophan(mg/100 g

edible portion)

Asparagus (boiled)∗ 2.9Avocado 2.0 26Bananas 1.7 28Beans (baked, canned) 4.6 66Beetroot (canned, drained) 1.3 9Brown rice 2.9 38Cashew nuts (roasted) 16.7 319Cheese (cheddar) 25.9 357Chicken breast (baked, lean) 29.0 394Chickpeas (canned, drained)∗ 6.3Cream of chicken soup (canned, reconstituted)∗ 1.8Dates (dried)∗ 2.0Eggs (hard boiled) 13.0 209Hazelnuts (raw)∗ 14.8King prawns (cooked) 23.7 178Lamb leg (roasted, lean) 29.7 400Lentils (boiled) 6.8 49Lobster (steamed or boiled)∗ 22.0Milk (full cream) 3.3 42Milk chocolate 8.2 112Oats (rolled, boiled as porridge) 2.0 25Pawpaw (papaya)∗ 0.4Peanuts (roasted) 25.1 285Pineapple (raw, peeled)∗ 0.6Soy beverage (regular fat, unflavoured, unfortified) 2.6 39Soybeans (boiled) 13.5 203Sunflower seeds 22.7 347Tofu (soft)∗ 8.9Tuna (canned in brine, drained) 23.8 208Turkey breast (baked, lean) 29.4 372Wholemeal bread 8.8 112

Source: NUTTAB 2006 (53).∗Tryptophan levels were not available for these foods but their protein contents arelisted here for readers’ interest (53).

of both protein (and thus tryptophan) and carbohy-drate to increase serotonin (57) shows a lack ofunderstanding of the mechanism behind the Wurt-man effect. One magazine article went so far asto say ‘brains can only make serotonin when weeat starchy carbs. . .without protein’, only to thenadd ‘The more tryptophan you eat with carbo-hydrates, the cheerier you become. . . Tryptophanis enhanced with carbohydrates, so the key is toeat something that contains tryptophan with com-plex carbohydrates. . .’ (58). Unfortunately, the arti-cle did not explain how eating tryptophan couldbe achieved without eating protein in the dailydiet. Some newspaper columnists advise eating pro-tein and carbohydrate foods separately (59) or toeat low-protein-high-carbohydrate meals to improvemood (60), but neglect to explain that a protein foodcontains tryptophan along with other amino acids andthat ‘carbohydrate foods’ such as bread also contain

protein. In the case of white bread, protein formsabout 14% of its caloric content. General mediaarticles also show a lack of understanding in bio-chemistry, in that tryptophan, as an amino acid, isa component of protein; they describe proteins asstimulatory and tryptophan as calming (61). Somepublished statements are profoundly misleading, suchas ‘. . .tryptophan, which is plentiful in carbohydrate-rich foods, such as chocolate’ (62) and ‘starch- andsugar-rich breakfast cereals. . .are high in the aminoacid tryptophan. . .’ (63). Tryptophan is an aminoacid, not a carbohydrate, and although chocolatedoes contain an appreciable amount of protein alongwith fat and carbohydrate, the protein content wouldreduce rather than increase brain uptake of trypto-phan (3). As described in the studies reviewed above,the whey protein α-lactalbumin is rich in tryptophanbut needs to be consumed as a supplement and notas an intact food and thus is still an artificial methodfor influencing plasma tryptophan levels. Lastly, pub-lished personal testimonials detailing success in treat-ing depression by dietary manipulation often do notreport what the pre-existing diet was like (35,64).

Toxicity, side-effects and caveats

The outbreak of eosinophilia-myalgia syndrome(EMS) in 1989 in individuals using over-the-counterl-tryptophan supplements resulted in these supple-ments being banned in the United States (65,66).EMS is characterised by elevated blood eosinophilcount, myalgia and sometimes neurological and pul-monary complications (65,66). The syndrome wasthought to be caused by contaminants, although itcould not be convincingly established whether thesecompounds were responsible (65).

Supplementation with tryptophan as an amino acidcan have side-effects, including nausea, vomiting,headaches and drowsiness (67); such side-effectsalso occur with consuming amino acid loads intryptophan depletion studies (12,23). A 6 g oral loadof tryptophan in healthy adults significantly increasedlipid peroxidation and tryptophan metabolites ofthe kynureinine pathway after 5 and 7 h comparedwith baseline. This potentially promotes cellularoxidative stress, damage or death, which may haveclinical implications in individuals either takingtryptophan supplements or following popular high-protein diets (68).

Mood changes may not occur until plasma tryp-tophan has dropped below a certain level, anda 60% decrease has been suggested as a thresh-old (31). Also, individuals with a family or per-sonal history of depression or who are chronicallystressed may be more sensitive to tryptophan changesinduced through dietary means (19,23), while healthy

8

Page 7: Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Tryptophan, diet and depression

young individuals may be less susceptible (16,19).Robinson and Sahakian (69) found that in healthywomen, depletion of plasma tryptophan in itselfwas not associated with negative mood. However,when negative mood was induced during tryptophandepletion, this was associated with more pronouncednegative mood on a subsequent episode of trypto-phan depletion. Thus, individuals with concurrentdepressed mood and reduced serotonin may be sen-sitive to changes in tryptophan levels.

Conclusion

Research into tryptophan must continue to elucidateits role in mood and mood disorders, and in themechanisms behind SSRNIs. Most of the studies todate have been on small to modest sample sizes,and hence more studies with larger sample sizes areneeded, along with studies in individuals with currentdepression. Empirical studies have mostly shownthat healthy individuals are insensitive to changesin tryptophan levels and for those who do have apersonal or family history of depression, the resultsare inconsistent.

It is unlikely that the highly artificial dietaryregimens used in studies such as Markus et al. (8)would be popular in the community and sustainedfor any length of time, if diet alone were usedto alter brain serotonin levels. As Benton andDonohoe (3) state, ‘No normal meal will contain solittle protein that the uptake of tryptophan will beincreased’ (p404). However, many general mediaarticles give contradictory and impractical advice.The articles convey the impression that mood can beimproved with ‘tryptophan-rich foods’ or by eatingcarbohydrate to raise serotonin levels, but do notconsider whether this would be of any use to thosewithout depression. It is questionable whether dietarychanges to manipulate tryptophan levels, even ifachievable, would be of advantage to the generalpopulation. This does not detract from the importanceof a nutritious diet for individuals with depression,as well as the general population, for overall health.

Acknowledgement

Over the past 48 months, Nerissa Soh and Garry Walter have hadno competing interests.

References

1. Elango R, Ball R, Pencharz P. Amino acid require-ments in humans: with a special emphasis on the metabolicavailability of amino acids. Amino Acids 2009;37:19–27.

2. Shaw KA, Turner J, Del Mar C. Tryptophan and5-hydroxytryptophan for depression. Cochrane DatabaseSyst Rev 2002;1:Art No CD003198.

3. Benton D, Donohoe RT. The effects of nutrients onmood. Public Health Nutr 1999;2:403–409.

4. Bellisle F, Blundell JE, Dye L et al. Functional foodscience and behaviour and psychological functions. BrJ Nutr 1998;80:S173–S193.

5. Wurtman RJ, Wurtman JJ. Carbohydrates and depres-sion. Science 1989;260:50–57.

6. Young SN. Some effects of dietary components (aminoacids, carbohydrate, folic acid) on brain serotonin synthesis,mood, and behavior. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 1991;69:893–903.

7. Merens W, Willem Van der Does AJ, Spinhoven P.The effects of serotonin manipulations on emotional infor-mation processing and mood. J Affect Disord 2007;103:43–62.

8. Markus CR, Panhuysen G, Tuiten A, Koppeschaar H,Fekkes D, Peters ML. Does carbohydrate-rich, protein-poor food prevent a deterioration of mood and cognitiveperformance of stress-prone subjects when subjected to astressful task? Appetite 1998;31:49–65.

9. Wurtman RJ, Wurtman JJ, Regan MM, McDermottJM, Tsay RH, Breu JJ. Effects of normal meals rich incarbohydrates or proteins on plasma tryptophan and tyrosineratios. Am J Clin Nutr 2003;77:128–132.

10. Attenburrow MJ, Williams C, Odontiadis J et al. Theeffect of a nutritional source of tryptophan on dieting-induced changes in brain 5-HT function. Psychol Med2003;33:1381–1386.

11. Schweiger U, Laessle R, Kittl S, Dickhaut B,Schweiger M, Pirke KM. Macronutrient intake, plasmalarge neutral amino acids and mood during weight-reducingdiets. J Neural Transm 1986;67:77–86.

12. Dougherty DM, Marsh-Richard DM, Mathias CWet al. Comparison of 50- and 100-g L-tryptophan depletionand loading formulations for altering 5-HT synthesis: phar-macokinetics, side effects, and mood states. Psychopharma-cology 2008;198:431–445.

13. Booij L, Merens W, Markus CR, Van der Does AJ.Diet rich in alpha-lactalbumin improves memory in unmed-icated recovered depressed patients and matched controls.J Psychopharmacol 2006;20:526–535.

14. Firk C, Markus CR. Mood and cortisol responses follow-ing tryptophan-rich hydrolyzed protein and acute stress inhealthy subjects with high and low cognitive reactivity todepression. Clin Nutr 2009;28:266–271.

15. Merens W, Booij L, Markus R, Zitman FG,Onkenhout W, Van der Does AJ. The effects of adiet enriched with alpha-lactalbumin on mood and corti-sol response in unmedicated recovered depressed subjectsand controls. Br J Nutr 2005;94:415–422.

16. Beulens JW, Bindels JG, de GC, Alles MS, Wouters-Wesseling W. Alpha-lactalbumin combined with a regulardiet increases plasma Trp-LNAA ratio. Physiol Behav2004;81:585–593.

17. Murphy SE, Longhitano C, Ayres RE, Cowen PJ,Harmer CJ. Tryptophan supplementation induces a pos-itive bias in the processing of emotional material inhealthy female volunteers. Psychopharmacology 2006;187:121–130.

18. Moreno FA, McGahuey CA, Freeman MP, Delgado PL.Sex differences in depressive response during monoaminedepletions in remitted depressive subjects. J Clin Psychiatry2006;67:1618–1623.

9

Page 8: Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Soh and Walter

19. Markus CR, Firk C, Gerhardt C, Kloek J, SmoldersGF. Effect of different tryptophan sources on amino acidsavailability to the brain and mood in healthy volunteers.Psychopharmacology 2008;201:107–114.

20. Porter RJ, Phipps AJ, Gallagher P, Scott A, Steven-son PS, O’Brien JT. Effects of acute tryptophan deple-tion on mood and cognitive functioning in older recov-ered depressed subjects. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005;13:607–615.

21. Moreno FA, Heninger GR, McGahuey CA, DelgadoPL. Tryptophan depletion and risk of depression relapse:a prospective study of tryptophan depletion as a poten-tial predictor of depressive episodes. Biol Psychiatry2000;48:327–329.

22. Booij L, Van der DW, Benkelfat C et al. Predictors ofmood response to acute tryptophan depletion. A reanalysis.Neuropsychopharmacology 2002;27:852–861.

23. Klaassen T, Riedel WJ, van SA, Deutz NE, Honig A,van Praag HM. Mood effects of 24-hour tryptophandepletion in healthy first-degree relatives of patients withaffective disorders. Biol Psychiatry 1999;46:489–497.

24. Booij L, Van der Does AJ, Haffmans PM, Riedel WJ.Acute tryptophan depletion in depressed patients treatedwith a selective serotonin-noradrenalin reuptake inhibitor:augmentation of antidepressant response? J Affect Disord2005;86:305–311.

25. Delgado PL, Price LH, Miller HL et al. Serotonin andthe neurobiology of depression. Effects of tryptophan deple-tion in drug-free depressed patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry1994;51:865–874.

26. Roiser JP, Levy J, Fromm SJ et al. The effects oftryptophan depletion on neural responses to emotionalwords in remitted depression. Biol Psychiatry 2009;66:441–450.

27. Blomstrand E, Møller K, Secher NH, Nybo L. Effectof carbohydrate ingestion on brain exchange of amino acidsduring sustained exercise in human subjects. Acta PhysiolScand 2005;185:203–209.

28. Praschak-Rieder N, Wilson AA, Hussey D et al. Effectsof tryptophan depletion on the serotonin transporter inhealthy humans. Biol Psychiatry 2005;58:825–830.

29. Nishizawa S, Benkelfat C, Young SN et al. Differ-ences between males and females in rates of serotoninsynthesis in human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A1997;94:5308–5313.

30. Salomon RM, Kennedy JS, Johnson BW et al. Asso-ciation of a critical CSF tryptophan threshold level withdepressive relapse. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003;28:956–960.

31. Hood SD, Bell CJ, Nutt DJ. Acute tryptophan depletion.Part I: rationale and methodology. Aust N Z J Psychiatry2005;39:558–564.

32. Bell CJ, Hood SD, Nutt DJ. Acute tryptophan depletion.Part II: clinical effects and implications. Aust N Z JPsychiatry 2005;39:565–574.

33. Hakkarainen R, Partonen T, Haukka J, Virtamo J,Albanes D, Lonnqvist J. Association of dietary aminoacids with low mood. Depress Anxiety 2003;18:89–94.

34. Markus CR. Effects of carbohydrates on brain trypto-phan availability and stress performance. Biol Psychol2007;76:83–90.

35. Mercieca T. The natural way to get happy. SundayTelegraph 2010:4.

36. Feed your desire. The Toronto 12 February 2010.

37. Priesmeyer M. Winter dragging you down? Here’s acomplete menu for beating the winter blues. Star Tribune4 June 2009.

38. Food that fills you up and keeps you slim. Prevention (India)1 February 2010.

39. Krishnan PR. Nutrition for those grey cells. The Hindu28 January 2010.

40. Food and your mood. Tufts University Health and NutritionLetter 2010;27:4–5.

41. Khicha P. Don’t count sheep to sleep. The Hindu22 November 2009.

42. Mood boosters: foods that reduce stress. Marie ClaireMagazine 4 November 2009.

43. Jones C. Eat yourself HAPPY! Mirror 26 October 2009.44. Pierleoni A. No gray, author says: keep your brain

exercised. The Sacramento Bee 22 October 2009.45. Caradas A. Eat your way to optimum brain function.

Business Day (South Africa) 14 October 2009.46. Fischer K. The seven. The Mail on Sunday 6 September

2009.47. Roberts L. What’s your FAT FATE? You don’t need a

crystal ball to discover whether you’re going to struggleto stay in your skinny jeans – just take our quiz instead.The News of the World 14 July 2009.

48. Winter harvest. The West Australian 4 June 2009.49. Shaheen E. Bananas add tropical aura. Gulf Daily News

22 March 2009.50. Gan E. Happy food for a lousy mood; certain kinds of

food do make you feel and sleep better. Today (Singapore)11 November 2008.

51. Fuller B. Darker days. Illawarra mercury, Section: You2008: 16–17.

52. Highfield R. Chocolate unlocks good feelings; a healthyreason to indulge: a key ingredient stimulates serotonin,which regulates emotions. Daily Telegraph, Section: Bodyand Health 7 June 2008.

53. Food Standards Australia and New Zealand. NUTTAB2006 Online version: foods that contain tryptophan. URLwww.foodstandards.gov.au, http://www.foodstandards.gov.au/consumerinformation/nuttab2006/onlineversionintroduction/onlineversion.cfm?&action=nutrientFoods&nutrientID=TRYPFD,2010 [Last updated on 8 January 2010; accessedon 18 January 2010].

54. Food myths. Northern Territory News/Sunday Territorian,Section: BBQ Brainiac 2010: 14.

55. Aubrey A. A myth examined: can Turkey make you tired?National Public Radio: Morning Edition 27 November2008.

56. Rajiv M. When food makes you feel good. The Hindu5 January 2008.

57. Stanely N, Milroy P, Govindji A. I’m so tired I’mfalling asleep at work: when I sit still I fall asleep, whichis embarrassing, especially during meetings at work. I onlydrink alcohol at weekends and have eight hours’ sleep anight. What can I do? The Observer Magazine 2008:91.

58. Bailey J. Happy meals. Allure 2009;19:113.59. Filipinos on holiday food mood advised to watch their diet,

mood swings. Philippines News Agency 23 December 2009.60. Time to bust-a-mood about food. Daily Camera 14 August

2009.61. No ordinary bedtime story. Manila Bulletin, Section: Well-

Being 24 November 2009.62. Taylor V. The science of food cravings; or why we never

binge on broccoli. The Hamilton Spectator 3 April 2009.

10

Page 9: Tryptophan and depression: can diet alone be the answer?

Tryptophan, diet and depression

63. Jacobs D. Fairness, not size, matters most; the desire tobe treated fairly rules with most workers. Vancouver Sun,Section: Weekend Review 19 July 2008.

64. King J. How food instead of pills lifted my mood. The Mailon Sunday 27 April 2008.

65. Fernstrom JD. Can nutrient supplements modify brainfunction? Am J Clin Nutr 2000;71:1669S–1675S.

66. Barrett S. Notes on the tryptophan disaster. Quackwatch,URL http://www.quackwatch.org/01QuackeryRelatedTopics/DSH/trypto.html, 2010 [Last updated on 22 December2001; accessed on 18 January 2010].

67. Leathwood PDP Pollet P. Diet-induced mood changesin normal populations. J Psychiatr Res 1982;17:147–154.

68. Forrest CM, Mackay GM, Stoy N et al. Tryptophanloading induces oxidative stress. Free Radic Res 2004;38:1167–1171.

69. Robinson OJ, Sahakian BJ. Acute tryptophan depletionevokes negative mood in healthy females who have previ-ously experienced concurrent negative mood and tryptophandepletion. Psychopharmacology 2009;205:227–235.

11