59
TRANSIENCE AND EDUCATION: ACADEMIC AND PSYCHOSOCIAL EFFECTS OF GEOGRAPH IC MOBILITY ON CElILDREN Susan M. Smith Koschmider A t hesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the Degree of Master o f Arts Depanment o f Human Developrnent and Applied Psychology Ontario Institute for Studies in Education o f the University o f Toronto @Copyright by Susan M. Smith Koschmider 1997

TRANSIENCE EDUCATION: ACADEMIC AND PSYCHOSOCIAL EFFECTS …€¦ · TRANSIENCE AND EDUCATION: ACADEMIC AND PSYCHOSOCIAL EFFECTS OF GEOGRAPHIC MOBILITY ON CHILDREN BACKGROUND AND INTRODUCTION

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • TRANSIENCE AND EDUCATION: ACADEMIC AND PSYCHOSOCIAL EFFECTS OF GEOGRAPH IC MOBILITY ON CElILDREN

    Susan M. Smith Koschmider

    A t hesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the Degree of Master o f Arts

    Depanment o f Human Developrnent and Applied Psychology Ontario Institute for Studies in Education o f the

    University o f Toronto

    @Copyright by Susan M. Smith Koschmider 1997

  • National Library 1*1 of Canada Bibliothbque nationale du Canada Acquisitions and Acquisitions et Bibliographie Seivices services bibliographiques 395 Wellington Street 395, nie Wellington Ottawa ON KIA ON4 OttawaON K1AON4 Canada Canada

    The author has ganted a non- exclusive licence allowing the National Library of Canada to reproduce, loan, distribute or seiî copies of this thesis in microfonn, paper or electronic formats.

    The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantial extracts fiom it may be printed or otherwise reproduced without the author's permission.

    L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive permettant à la Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou vendre des copies de cette thèse sous la forme de microfiche/fYm, de reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique.

    L'auteur conserve la propriété du droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés ou autrement reproduits sans son autorisation.

  • TRANSIENCE AND EDUCATION:

    ACADEMIC AND PSYCHOSOCIAL EFFECTS

    OF GEOGRAPHIC MOBILITY ON CHILDREN

    by

    Susan M. Smith Koschmider

    Degree of Master of Arts

    1997

    Departmeut of Human Development and Applied Psychology

    Ontario Institute for Studies in Education of the

    University of Toronto

    ABSTRACT

    This review attempts to examine the question: 1s rnobility a risk factor for high risk

    school children? Mobility and risk terms are defined, and mobility research is

    reviewed and analyzed. The research to date does not provide definitive answers to

    the questions posed, mainly for a variety of methodological reasons. The review

    concludes with some suggestions for future research directions.

  • ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    I would like to acknowledge al1 those who helped and supported me in the completion

    of my degree requirements:

    Dr. Solveiga Miezitis, my Thesis Supervisor, who guided and redirected me throughout

    this project, giving outstanding encouragement and support.

    Dr. Otto Weininger, m y second Committee Member.

    Cecily Jennings and Linda Booty who read the drafts and made helpful suggestions.

    Family and friends, near and far, who inspire with their optimism.

    My children, Kai and Melissa, who have cheered me on and helped with photocopying,

    computer gli tches and household duties.

    Horst, partner and friend, who steadfastly and loyally has supported and charnpioned

    me throughout many years of education.

    Thank-you very much

    Home. Home was arbitrary, the place she had designared as home, the house on Tenrh Street, rather than the farm in Connecticut ... odd, how the heort will put down roofs in what was, at the time, only a ternporary shelter.

    Madeleine L'Engle, A Severed Wasv

    iii

  • This thesis is dedicated to Dorot h y Isabel Fraser Smith (1903-1983)

    who taught me the importance of investing time in the lives of children.

  • TRANSIENCE A N D EDUCATION: ACADEMIC A N D PSYCHOSOCIAL EFFECTS O F GEOGRAPHIC MOBILITY ON CHILDREN

    TABLE OF CONTENTS

    INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND

    CRITERIA FOR INCLUSION O F STUDIES

    DEFINITIONS OF CRITERIA USED IN MOBILITY RESEARCH Mobility Criteria Risk and Protective Factors

    OVERVIEW OF THE LITERATURE REVIEW

    CRITIQUE OF MOBILITY STUDIES Reason for Move Pre- and Post-Move Data Mobility and Risk

    LIMITATIONS AND DIRECTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH

  • LISTS OF TABLES

    TABLE 1: Mobility Studies Reviewed in Summary

    TABLE 2: Major Characteristics of Studies Reviewed

    TABLE 3: Reason for Move

    TABLE 4: Research Controls

  • TRANSIENCE AND EDUCATION: ACADEMIC A N D PSYCHOSOCIAL EFFECTS

    OF GEOGRAPHIC MOBILITY ON CHILDREN

    BACKGROUND AND INTRODUCTION

    A review of studies on the ef fects of transieace on the educational progress of

    the child is vital at this time given the high mobility of school children in the greater

    Toronto area, as well as in other parts of Canada and North America. The current

    literatu re reveals contradictions regarding the e l fects of transience on children's

    academic achievement, social-emotional adaptation and behaviour. Where some studies

    show reduced academic achievement, less positive social experiences, and greater

    adolescent alienation for mobile groups (Calabrese, 1989; Ingersoll, Scamman and

    Eckerling, 1989; Vernberg, 19900, other studies find insignificant effects, positive

    effects for bright students, and higher levels of child and social competence among the

    mobile groups (Marchant and Medway, 1987; Stroh and Brett, 1990b; Whalen and Fried,

    1973). Some of the discrepancy may be due to di fferences in definitions. Words such

    as mobility, highly mobile, nonmobile, and transient are used to describe di f ferent

    groups i n this literature, but without common criteria of selection. Other problems

    noted in this body of literature are poor design, insufficient control, and loosely

    defined dependent variables. Studies are performed with a variety of rubjects, often

    wi thout controlling for such vital dif ferences as, for example, socioeconomic status

    (SES) or reason for move. Other studies assess different aspects of adaptation and

    adjustment or lack thereof. Thus arise the contradictions detailed above. Contradictions

    lead to confusion on the subject and threaten the legitimacy of transience and mobility

    as an important issue relative to other pressing issues in education today. In addition,

    this leads to uncertainty about the necessity for interventions appropriate to the

    accommodation of new students. It is the purpose of this review to contribute to an

    awareness of the complexity of the interaction between transience and education, and

    to suggest directions for future research on the subject. It is also the purpose of this

  • 2

    review to suggest criteria by which to assess potentia1 strengths and risks for

    individual children who move.

    Life transitions require adaptation. Moving from one school to another is a

    challenging transition in which children must adapt. Circumstances surrounding a

    move have a bearing on the child's ability to adapt or adjust. For example, a move due

    to family breakdown or the death of a parent may be accompanied by symptoms of

    stress from the trauma associated with multiple losses, A child in this situation could

    exhibit academic difficulties or behavioural problems such as acting out or social

    withdrawal (Ribordy, 1989). In al1 transitions, ability of an individual to adjust after

    a move is thought to be related to a variety of factors including personality,

    adaptability, level of motivation, socioeconomic status (SES), intelligence, age, social

    skills, stress levels, amount of input into the decision to move, amount of difference

    between the schools' curricula, level of emotional adjustment, and stability of and

    support from the family andlor other relationships (AIlan and Bardsley, 1984; Jason,

    Weine, Johnson, Warren-Sohlberg, Filippelli, Turner and Lardon, 1992). The current

    review will evaluate studies based, in part, on the inclusion of such variables in the

    design.

  • 3

    CRITERIA FOR INCLUSION OF STUDIES

    The scope of this review is English-language articles dealing with any aspect of

    mobility related to chiIdren. Searches were made using CD Rom software (ERIC and

    PsvchLit), manual searches of recent journals, and the reference pages of books and

    articles on the subject of mobility and children's education. In addition to peer-

    reviewed articles, non-reviewed and unpublished articles have also been included

    because there is some evidence suggesting that refereed journals may have a bias

    towards publishing mainly statistically significant results, overlooking other research

    with statistically insignificant, but possibly valid results (Light & Pillemer, 1984;

    White, 1982).

    Not included in this paper are studies of adult movers, with the foIlowing

    exceptions: 1) where the study includes both adults and children as the subject group;

    and 2) a study which retrospectively examined adaptation to university based upon

    mobility in childhood.

  • DEFINITIONS O F CRITERIA USED IN MOBILITY RESEARCH

    MOBILITY CRITERIA

    Mobility or being mobile refers to geographic mobility, the change of residence,

    school, andlor location. For the purpose of this study mobility refers to changes in

    schools with o r without a change of residence, but not changes in residence without a

    change in school. While there may be important consequences to changes in residence

    without a change of school, these particular types of moves will not be considered at

    this time as they tend to go unnoticed both in schools and for research purposes.

    In the articles reviewed, specific de finitions of mobility used by researchers

    tended to be arbitrary, varying from study to study. Difficulties arise in cornparing

    studies because the definitions are sometimes unclear and because the different

    definitions don? always concur well enough to compare one with another. Part of the

    confusion is due to overlap of conditions. There is a good variety of combinations in

    the studies due, for example, to numbers of moves, types of moves and subject groups.

    This review will look at several definitions andlor types of definitions of mobility,

    describe them, discuss the dif ferences, and highlight the strengths and weaknesses of

    each.

    a) NUMBER OF MOVES

    Several theorists use the number of moves a child has made as a criterion for

    definition of mobility. Typically researchers assign subjects to two o r more groups.

    WhiIe the number of groups is arbitrary, the simplest would be two groups, one

    consisting of students who have not moved, and the other of students who have moved

    one or more times (Pinder, 1989; Stroh & Brett, 1990alb; Vernberg, 1990). A variation

    of this category is number of schools attended (Morris, Pestaner & Nelson, 1967;

    Whalen & Fried, 1973). For example, Harter, Whitesell & Kowalski (1992) defined

    transitions for the purpose of their study as changes to a new school. In their study of

    student mobili ty and achievement, lngersoll et ai., (1989) described five mobilit y

  • 5

    groups, based upon student enrolment patterns for the duration of their two year study.

    These included three groups of continuing students and two groups of students new to

    the system in the second year of the study, divided as follows: 1) no moves; 2) only one

    move; 3) more than one move; 4) new entry to the school but no moves dur ing the

    course of the study; 5) new entry and with one or more moves and/or exit from system.

    This allowed the researchers to compare mobile with nonmobile students and then to

    compare the effects of one move versus more than one move.

    In some descriptive articles, writers merely note that the subject has moved, or

    mention the average number of moves (Shaw, 1987).

    b) DISTANCE OF MOVE

    Some researchers have defined mobility on the basis of the distance of a move

    or the cross from one area to another. Barrett & Noble (1973) studied movers who

    relocated a distance of over 50 miles; Johnson & Lindblad (1991) compared intercity

    moves, intracity moves and nonmobility; and Vernberg, Ewell, Beery & Abwender

    (2994) described an intercommunity move.

    Neither the number of moves nor the distance of move fully capture the whole

    scope of the impact of a move. For example, a move within Canada can involve a

    dif ferent climate, region, culture, and /o r language (Pinder, 1989).

    c) MULTIPLE FACTORS

    Kroger (1980), in a study of mobility and seIf-concept in adolescents, defined

    mobility using a formula calcuiating the "distance" o f a move based upon the sum of

    the number of moves multiplied by the weight of a move. The weighting was based

    upon a scale developed by Oakay (1972) where the type of move is given numerical

    weight (i.e., intracommunity = 1; intrastate = 2; intraregion [contiguous state] = 3;

    intraregion [non-contiguous statel = 4; intranation [contiguous state] = 5; intranation

    [non-contiguous state] = 6; and international = 7). For the subjects who had moved a t

  • 6

    least once Kroger used recency of last move and age span of the greatest number of

    moves as variables. While it may be possible to improve and refine it, Kroger's formula

    nevertheless provides an example of an attempt at the quantification of some of the

    dif ferences and complexities inherent in mobility.

    d) SCHEDULED VERSUS UNSCHEDULED MOVES

    The definition of mobility includes the important distinction of whether a move

    is schcduled (Le., a move from elementary to senior school) or unscheduled, such as a

    school change due to job transfer, eviction, etc. A key factor involved in these two

    types of moves is that with a scheduled move, groups of students usually move

    together, w hereas an unscheduled move usually affects an individual student alone or

    with siblings (Bogat, Liang, Caldwell, Davidson, Bristor, Phillips & Suu rme yer, 1993;

    Jason et al., 1992). As Danner, Jason & Kurasaki (1993) note,

    al1 children change schools during their educational careers. Many school transfers are considered routine, such as transferring from middle to high school. Other children experieace unscheduled school transfers, which are due to situations not considered part of the usual educational experience (p. 1).

    Scheduled moves, being highly predictable, lend themselves well to research study and,

    as will be discussed later, have provided valuable mobility information. The chief

    drawback is that research of scheduled transitions does not necessarily generalize well

    to the entire mobile population, since scheduled transitions lack the potential

    unpredictability, isolation and some of the risk factors inherent in other types of

    moves.

    SUMMARY

    WhiIe researchers have been fairly consistent about some criteria (i.e., change

    of school), other criteria are inconsistent ranging from a simple haslhas not moved to

    complicated formulae. To be useful for research a basic definition of mobility will

    likely need to incIude the number of moves, the rate of moves (number of moves in

  • 7

    what time span), the distance moved, and whether the move is a normal scheduled

    transition as opposed to an individual move outside of the usual school experience.

    RlSK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS

    There is a growing interest in establishing the risk and protective factors for

    children at different stages and in different circumstances. Some children appear to

    bc at greater risk for school failure, underachievement, mental health problems and

    psychosocial di f ficulties than others. Discussing risk and protective factors, Garmezy

    (1991) States that,

    protectiveness is not the obverse of vulnerability ... whereas risk mechanisms tend to lead directly to disordcr, protective processes operate indirectly, their effects partly a function of their interaction with and their modification of the risk variable (p. 428).

    Risk and protective factors are important to consider in a study of transient

    children because a move requires adaptation, and the circumstances surrounding a

    move may have a bearing upon the child's ability to adapt. It is thus hypothesized that

    mobility is likely to be a risk factor for certain children.

    In order to identify areas that need to be considered when examining mobility

    and children, some of the common risk and protective factors for school-age children

    will be identified. Following this, the mobility studies will be evaluated in light of

    the importance of these risk factors,

    a) SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS (SES)

    Low socioeconomic status (SES) is cited by researchers as a significant risk

    factor af fecting academic achievement, adaptive behaviour, mental health, and future

    socioeconomic status. I t is difficuIt to know exactly what is meant by the term low

    socioeconomic status, however, because various researchers use dif ferent variables to

    describe it. For example, in a meta-analytic review of studies comparing sociometric

    status (SES) with academic achievement, White (1982) found that SES measures varied

    and also that the relationship between SES and school achievement varied depending

  • 8

    upon which factors were used to measure SES. White iists types of SES measured in the

    studies he reviewed, including: income of family; education o f parents; occupation o f

    head of house; home atmosphere (e.g., parents' a t t i tude toward education; parents'

    aspirations for their children; cultural and intellectual activities of the family);

    dwelling value; school resources; subjective judgement; and a miscellaneous category

    (e.g., number of siblings, ethnicity, and mobility of family). He identi fied over seventy

    di f ferent variables which were used eitber alone o r in some combination as indicators

    of SES.

    White (1982) found a positive, but weaker correlation between SES and

    academic achievement than is normally assumed. Of the traditional SES indicators,

    income was the highest single correlate of academic achievement. He found it

    surprising, however, that measures of home atmosphere had a much higher correlation

    with academic achievement than any single o r combined group of the traditional

    indicators of SES. White recommended that when using SES as a research tool, specific

    labels for the variables be used with a precise definition of how the variable was

    measu red.

    Family income, specifically poverty, was consistently found to be the strongest

    predictor of academic failure, stress difficulties, developmental disabilities, emotional

    disorder and other types of risk, of several variables studied (Caldas, 1993; Fergusson,

    Horwoad, & Lynsky, 1994; Garmezy, 1991; Offord, 1989; Patterson, Vaden &

    Kupersmidt, 1991; Reynolds, Weissberg, & Kasprow, 1992; Sameroff, 1975; Walker &

    Boyle, unpublished; Wyman, Cowen, Work & Parker, 1991).

    While there may be controversy regarding the actual relationship between SES

    and factors such as child well-being and academic achievement, i t is an important

    variable to include. l n some cases SES is implicit in the research question. For example,

    in literature surrounding job transfers of mainly managerial, professional and

    technical employees, concerns relate to cost-effectiveness of job transfers, employee

    satisfaction, employee change and development, and parents' concerns regarding

  • 9

    children's happiness, adjustment, and academic achievement (Pinder, 1981; Pinder and

    Walter, 1984; Stroh & Brett, 1990a/b). This is contrasted to mobitity studies involving

    movers generally considered to be less advantaged when the research question concerns

    student success or lack of success in academic subjects or programs (Manaster, Chan &

    Safady, 2992).

    The role of socioeconomic status i n chitdren's adaptation to transience will best

    be evaluated by ideatifying specific variables and defining them precisely. Generally,

    while poverty was consistently the strongest predictor of academic and other

    difficulties, home atmosphere was also found to play an important role in predicting

    academic success.

    b) INTELLIGENCE

    Intelligence is cited as a risk or protective factor i n a variety of studies

    including those of Kandell, Mednick, Kirkegaard-Sorensen, Hutchings, Knop,

    Rosenberg, & Schufsinger, (1988), Luthar, (1991) and Whalen & Fried, (1973).

    IntelIectual assessment of children, however, is at best a challenging procedure, and is

    complicated by factors associated with mobility. Gaps in schooling, inconsistencies in

    education, and assessment procedures which discriminate against foreign-born and

    other language children (Cummins, 1986), are some complicating factors. It is thus

    difficult to establish causality by examining the relationship between intelligence and

    risk factors. In addition, parent, teacher, and school expectations of students can be

    powerful confounding variables (Weinstein, Soule, Collins, Cone, Mehlhorn and

    Simon tacchi, 1991).

    Alternatively, the perception that one is of Iower intelligence or has less ability

    is fikely in itself to be debilitating, as lngraham (1985) found when he highlighted the

    importance of accurate self-concept and logical information processing for self-esteem,

    school motivation and achievernent.

    I t is also possible that the differences Found in the literature may be related to

  • 10

    the method used to measure intelligence. Several authors refer to higher IO as a

    protective factor (Garmezy, Masten & Tellegen, 1984; Kandel, et ai., 1988; Masten, Best

    & Garmezy, 1990; Rutter, 1989; Werner, 1989) while others associate intelligence with

    increased risk and vulnerability (Luthar, 1991; Yoshi kawa, f 994). 1 t appears, however,

    that the conclusions rnay have been based upon different types of intelligence

    measures. Luthar used a nonverbal measure of intelligence, and while not specifying

    the type of measure, Yoshikawa points out that low verbal ability is a disadvantage i n

    school. I t rnay be that verbal intelligence and language abilities are a protective factor

    (Werner, 1989) while non verbal intelligence does not function as a protective factor,

    and rnay even increase vulnerability in stressful situations (Luthar, 1991). In iiner-

    grained examinations, interactions between a child's abilities and farnily variables rnay

    moderate risk factors (Christenson, 1990). Further, stressfui situations, and especially

    traumatic events, are known to affect concentration and distractibility, and rnay

    confound the predictive value of measures of intelligence for school achievement.

    Nevertheless, in spite of these limitations, it is important to consider whether

    intelligence is a risk factor in general, and more specifically, in relation to mobility.

    Whalen and Fried (1973), found that mobility magnifies existing differences between

    children in that those mobile children considered more intelligent showed higher

    academic achievement than their non-mobile counterparts, while less intelligent mobile

    children showed lower academic achievement than their non-mobile peers. I t rnay be,

    as Jason, Betts, Johnson, Weine, Warren-Sohlburg, Shinaver, Neuson, Filipelli and

    Lardon, (1990) point out, that difficulty in mastering new skills rnay compound

    challenges for weaker students who rnay then choose to give up rather than continue

    to experience the humiliation of failure. It woutd appear that some children

    transferring schools are a t a greater risk for experiencing school difficulties and that

    these high risk kids tend to have fewer interna1 resources, including lower measured

    intelligence.

  • C) LEARNING DISABILITIES AND LEARNING DIFFICULTIES

    Scme researchers bave found that learning d i f ficulties and disabili ties seem

    especially noticeable for children who move a great deal. Foreign children were over-

    represen ted in special education classes in European countries (Organisation for

    Economic Co-operation and Development, 1987). Children in shelters and hostels were

    found to score lower on a standardized reading test and on a test of attention span

    (Moore & Pepler, unpublished). Jason et al. (1990) found that academic vulnerabilities

    add to other stressors, such as mobility, leaving a child more likely to experience

    maladjustment and failure. Other researchers, however, have not found that mobile

    children di f fer from their less mobile peers in terms of learning di f ficulties (Barrett

    & Noble, 1973). One important di fference between these various bodies of work may

    be SES. The latter subjects were drawn from families using a major interstate moving

    Company, a luxury perhaps not used by some of the other subject groups. Thus, the

    relationship between moving and learning difficulties may be mediated by SES or

    family income factors,

    Risk factors associated with learning difficulties, school failure and/or

    underachievement may include high absences, difficulties with concentration and the

    inability to solve interpersonal problems (Kellam, Werthamer-Larsson, Dolan, Brown,

    Mayer, Rebok, Anthony, Laudolff, Edelsohn & WheeIer, 1991; Leonard & Elias, 1993;

    Reyes & Hedeker, 1993).

    d) PEER SUPPORT AND ADJUSTMENT

    Support of peers &as been examined as a variable in research about emotional

    adjustment, academic achievement, behaviour, stress, protection and resiliency. A

    further question about the importance of peer support is raised when one asks if peer

    support helps protect a mobile child against later persona1 risk and/or contributes to

    later personal adjustment. In other words, is peer support a buffer factor in general,

    and specifically in transience?

  • 12

    The support of peers is thought to be related to emotional adjustment. ChiIdren

    with pour peer relationships, especially those with low acceptance coupled with

    aggressiveness, are at risk for difficulties in adolescence and adulthood including

    dropping out of schoul, criminal activity and externalizing di f ficulties (Hyrnel, Rubin,

    Rowden & LeMare, 1990; Parker & Asher, 1987; Rubin & Mills, 1988). Further, social

    skills and social resources have been found to protect against stress and to influence

    well-being (Cohen & Wills, 1985; Luthar, 1991; Hirsch & DuBois, 1992). Some

    researchers, however, have not found consistent relationships between peer support and

    adjustment (Barone, Aguirre-Deandreis & Trickett, 1991; and Wolchik, Ruehlman,

    Braver, and Sandler, 1989). Barone et ai. speculated that this may be due to the

    measurement procedures or because the effect of peer support may differ according to

    peer group characteristics. For example, support from delinquent peers may have

    different consequences for an individual's adaptation than support from nondelinquent

    peers, Walker and Greene (1987) found that gender was a factor in whether peer

    support was helpful. Their research showed that peer support protected adolescent

    males, but not adolescent females from the stress associated with negative l i fe events.

    For females, regardless of the frequency or valence of life events, low peer support was

    associated with high levels of symptoms.

    Other researchers have found that peer support has mixed effects on personal

    adjustment. Cauce, Hannan & Sargeant (1992) inferred potential stress-buffering

    effects of social support from a study that showed that peer support was positively

    related to peer competence and anxiety, but negatively related to school competence.

    Asher, Hyrnel & Renshaw (1984) reported a positive correlation between low

    peer support and the reporting of loneliness and social dissatisfaction, but also found

    that satisfactory home relationships may be a mediating factor. Other researchers

    found that those children who were actively rejected were more likely to remain

    rejected when they moved into a new group or class, while children who were neglected

    were more likely to improve their social status (Coie & Kupersmidt, 1983).

  • Academic achievement, often considered a protective factor, is related to social

    skills and peer support. Leonard & Elias (1993) found that grade point average (GPA),

    is related to the skills needed to solve interpersonal problems. Further, French (1990),

    in a study of peer-rejected girls, found a relationship between low self-control, low

    academic task orientation and high peer-rated aggression, social withdrawal, overall

    behaviour problems, anxiety, hostile isolation, academic disability, and peer rejection.

    Vernberg et al. (1994) found that social perspective coordination was helpful in

    establishing and maintaining Friendships and, further, that establishment and

    maintenance of close friendships is important in both short- and long-term adaptation.

    Social perspective coordination refers to the ability to

    di f ferentiate and integrate the self's perspective with the perspectives of others. This requires an understanding of the relationships among one's own thoughts, feelings and wishes and those of other people (Selman & Schultz, 1989, p. 6).

    Asher (1983), Putallaz (1983) and Crick & Dodge (1994) found that socially competent

    children are able to read and understand social situations more accurately and thus to

    both initiate and respond positively and relevantly to others. Asher also fouad that

    socially competent children reaIize that building relationships takes time and are able

    to use indirect as well as direct approaches in goal-attainment. However, the self-

    perception that one is socially competent was not necessarily accurate and could not be

    viewed as a basis for social or behavioural cornpetence (Patterson, Kupersmidt &

    Griesler, 1990).

    Cultural or environmental factors, generally outside of the individual (and over

    which he o r she may have less direct control) also affect behaviour and peer status.

    These may include attractiveness and reputation (Dodge, 1983), cultural characteristics

    (Schneider, 1993), high levels of stress, both long-term and acute (Patterson et ai., 1991),

    the classroom achievement environment (Werthamer-Larsson, Kellam & Wheeler, 1991),

    age (Barrett & Noble, 1973), and mobility (Byrnes, 1985).

    Thus, as Sameroff (1975) suggests, mere causality may not be supportable. The

    child and his or her environment must be examined in combination because complex

  • 14

    reciprocal interactions ameliorate or worsen behaviour problems, Requirements of good

    peer relationships include intra-personal, inter-personal and cultural/environmental

    factors. Although researchers have found that peer support is positively related to an

    individual's adjustment, some inconsistency in the literature remains in relation to

    peer group factors and to the area of adaptation being examined. Many factors may

    contribute to modify the intensity of the relationship between social abiIities and later

    persona1 adjustment, but there is generally strong support for this relationship and

    thus, it is important to consider the role of peer support in a study of mobile children.

    e) ADULT SUPPORT AND ADJUSTMENT

    Researchers also need to account for the role of adult support of children in

    mobility studies because relationships with aduIts are critical in a child's life. Thus the

    interpersonal and intrapersonal factors which affect these relationships, including

    availability and type of family support, are essential to an evaluation of risk and

    protectiveness.

    1) INTRA- AND INTERPERSONAL FACTORS

    Among the intrapersonal factors likely to play a part in exacerbating or

    ameliorating risk are a child's behaviour or adaptive functioning, which is related to

    the child's ability to get along with adults.

    Having a temperamental style or coping style that is a poor match for the

    caretaking environment increases the challenges that a child must cope with and thus

    increases risk of failure or maladjustment (Compas, 1987). For example, a very active

    child living in a family that values peace and tranquillity rnay find it diff icult to

    conform to the family's standards of quiet and inactivity, and may thus experience a

    sense of failure.

    Some intrapersonal traits and interpersonal factors are linked with g r m e r stress

    and poor relationships with adults. Timko, Moos & Michelson (1993) found a

  • 15

    relationship between chronic stressors and such intrapersonal factors as an emotional

    disposition and low self-worth, as well as environmental and interpersonal factors such

    as negative l i fe events and limited social resources. Furthermore, longitudinal studies

    show that aggressive behaviour, especially for males, is stable over time and that

    aggressive behaviour in young children is highly predictive of serious adult antisocial

    behaviour (Huesman, Eron, Lefkowitz & Walder, 1984; Lochman & Lampron, 1985).

    Werner (1989) in a 1955 birth cohort s tudy on the island of Kauai, Hawaii

    discovered that the relative impact of risk and protective factors changed at various

    life phases. For example, Fenzel (1989) suggests that simply being a young adolescent

    student brings role strain, while Rutter (1989), found that key patterns of interaction

    between d i f ferent mediating factors (i.e., genetic, biological, environmental) exist and

    that some of these persist across development whereas others are age specific.

    Cauce et al. (1992) found that both family and school support moderated the

    relationship between negative events and school com petence. Interactive e f fects were

    also detected in that school support buffered a number of negative events best for

    those indiv iduah with an interna1 locus of control for success,

    Wertlieb, Weigel, & Feldstein (1987) note a distinction between stress associated

    with major li fe events and that associated with normal dail y problems. Additionally,

    they mention the possible modifying influence of social support. Undesirable life

    events had the strongest association with behaviour problems. Ribord y (1989) concurs

    that children's reactions to serious l i fe losses and stressors may show themselves as

    behaviours a t school and that the form of the manifestation depends upon the type of

    loss (i.e., death, separation/divorce).

    Again i t appears that while there is a relationship, the influences that affect

    resiliency may not be merely causal, but are likely interactional and may need to be

    accounted for in mobility studies.

  • 2) FAMILY SUPPORT AND FAMILY COHESION

    The most important adults in a child's l i fe are the parents or caretakers who

    make up the child's family. Family support has been found to be a buffer against

    negative life situations, and is, in itself a protective factor, while a lack of family

    cohesion has been found to be a risk factor.

    Family support was consistently found to be helpful and stress-buffering in

    several domains related to school adjustment including attitude toward school, anxiety,

    and adapting to new school social tasks (Barone et al., 1991; Cauce et al., 1992). In

    addition, there is a significan t positive relationship between parental involvement in

    schools, especially the perceived quality of the parent involvement, and academic

    achievement, social cornpetence and attendance (Haynes, Corner & Hamilton-Lee, 1989;

    Reynolds et al., 1992). As well, a strong inverse relationship exists between a family's

    social support and child symptomatology, including levels of substance (i.e., cigarette,

    alcohol, marijuana) use (WertIieb et al., 1987; Wills, Vaccaro & McNamara, 1992).

    Lack of family cohesion was found to be a possible stressor for adolescents

    (Walker and Greene, 1987) and is related to adolescent suicidality (Kurtz &

    Derevensky, 1993). When family stress increases there is a corresponding increase in

    behaviour problems for children and adolescents (Fergusson et al., 1994; Shaw, Vondra,

    Hommerding, Keenan & Dunn, 1994).

    Some researchers, however, have been unable to find support for the hypothesis

    that parental stress or even serious mental ilIness is a predictor of long-term pathology

    or incompetence in children (Cohen, Burt & Bjork, 1987; Garmezy et al., 1984).

    Nevertheless, research generally supports the notion that strong family support and

    good cohesion are beneficial for children's adaptation, academic achievement and Iong-

    term outlook. Not only were parental attitudes significantly linked to those of their

    children (Stroh & Brett 1990), but in some cases it was found that parents could be

    proactive in aiding their children. For example, in a study of mobile children, parents

    used friendship facilitation strategies which were beneficial in helping adolescents

  • 17

    develop closer and more intimate friendships after moving (Vernberg, Beery, Ewell, &

    Abwender, 1993).

    A most extreme and distressing example of disruption of family and consequent

    loss of support can occur with the death of a parent. Particularly i f the death was

    sudden, unnatural or violent, i t can be considered a traumatic stressor. Ribordy (1989)

    notes that children o f al1 ages will usually exhibit changes in behaviour as well as have

    some interference in academic progress for at least the short term, and possibly for

    several years after the traumatic event. Wolfelt (1996) suggests that children grieve i n

    what he refers to as "doses" and that they can be expected to feel and/or express their

    grief long after the death of a parent, especially at later developmental milestones such

    as graduation, rnarriage, childbearing, etc. In addition, the death of a parent can be

    accompanied by several other changes previously discussed as potential risk factors,

    including change in family income, less availability of family support, and/or possible

    move to another location. Thus, the death of a parent and the accompanying

    disruptions create significant change in a child's life and can be considered a

    significant stressor or risk for dif ficulties and/or maladjustrnent.

    Poor parental support, whether due to poor family cohesion, mismatch of

    parent-child temperament style, poor intrapersonal and interpersonal skills, or the

    death of a parent can be considered a risk factor in a child's life. Without positive

    adult support and strong intrapersonal skills a child is likely to be at greater risk if

    subjected to f requent disruptions such as moves.

    SUMMARY

    Several factors can be considered risk factors for school-aged children. These

    include, but are not limited to, socioeconomic status, intelligence, learning dif ficulties,

    peer support, and adult support. These risk factors are complex and interactional and

    need to be examined in studies of mobility.

  • 18

    OVERVIEW OF THE LITERATURE REVIEW

    Thirty-eight studies were chosen according to the criteria for inclusion

    described earlier i n this paper. Ideally, to explore the effects of transience and

    evaluate the risks invoIved, researchers need to clearly define the type mobility being

    studied as well as describe the subject population fully in terms of the risk factors.

    The mobility studies reviewed here have been presented in Table 1. A

    description of the subjects, the independent and dependent variabIes, and a brief

    summary of the findings is provided for each study, Furthermore, the studies have

    been grouped according to four major categories: three based upon the major type of

    dependent variable (both Academic and Psychosocial Ef fects; Academic Ef fects;

    Psychosocial Effects) and a fourth comprises qualitative studies.

  • TABLE 1 - MOBILITY STUDIES REVIEWED IN SUMMARY STUDY SUBJECTS INDEPENDENT DEPENDENT FINDINGS

    (age, etc) VARIABLE VARIABLE

    Mobility and Both Academic & Psychosocial Effects

    Filippelli 3-5 years; & Jason n=132 (1992) U.S.

    Felner et al. (1982) US.

    Pepler & Moore (unpub- lished 1991) Canada

    Barone et ai. (1991) U.S.

    Reyes & Hedeker

    Grade 9; scheduled transition n=172

    6-12 years; n=265

    Grade 9; scheduled transit ion n=82

    Grade 9: high risk

    (1993) U.S. inner city n=154

    Hatzi- 13-15 years; christou & n=784 Hop f(1992) Greece

    Mobility + negative events

    Prevention project

    Homelessness

    Mobility + means-end problem solving skills; l i fe stress; social support

    Transition; preventative program

    Remigration to Greece from Germany

    Sel f-concept; achievement scores; academic grades

    Sel f-concept; perceptions of school; grades

    Academic achievement; attention; locus of control

    Adjustment:

    Transfer students with more negative life events had sig. lower w riting grades & self-concept scores, & lower achievement scores.

    Project participants had sig. better grades, attendance & more stable sel f-concepts.

    Homeless child ren sig. lower on reading & math, more behaviour probiems, and sig. more external in control orientation.

    Transition resulted in grades; quality of sig. decrease in GPA life; attendance; & attendance; Females dif ficulty of posttransition tasks; state anxiety

    Absence, academic achievement

    Achievement, language com petence, adjustment (interpersonal, intrapersonal)

    did not decrease GPA's as much as males.

    Sig. decrease in GPA, increase in failure rate & drop in rankings for all; higher absenteeism Gr 8 linked to academic di f ficulties & higher absenteeism Gr 9.

    Remigrants had di f ficulty in language/learning domains; newer remigrants sig. more dif ficulty in class adaptation & learning than local students; sig. lower achievement language, history & math.

  • 20

    TABLE 1 (continued)

    STUDY SUBJECTS INDEPENDENT DEPENDENT FINDINGS (age, etc) VARIABLE VARIABLE

    Mobility and Academic Ef fects

    Johnson & Grade 6; Mobility Lindblad city; (in tracity, (1991) U.S. n=1686 ext racity,

    nonmobile)

    Warren- Grades 3-5; Reason for move Sohlberg & n=451 Jason (1992) U.S.

    Marchant Grades 2,4,6; Mobility & Medway military; (1987) U S . n=170

    Whalen & Grade I l ; Fried n=874 (1973) US.

    Morris et Grade 5; al. (1967) n=410 us.

    Mobility, IQ, & SES (3-way ANOVA)

    MobilityISES; IQ con t rolled

    Mancaster 14-20 years; Acculturation; et al. migrant urbanization; (1992) U.S. n=l51 SES

    Levine et Grades K-6; Pupil turnover al. (1966) inner city U.S. n=574

    fngersoll et Grades 1-12 Mobility al. (1989) n=41,735 (geographic U.S. instability)

    Academic achievement

    Adjustment to new school; (grades)

    Adjust ment; achievement

    Achievement scores

    Reading/ arithmetic achievement

    Intracity mobile groups had sig. lower academic achievement scores than others

    Reason for move sig. di f ferentiated academic achievemcnt in reading, spelling and math; Grades dropped for al1 students regardless of reason for move

    Sig. correlation betwcen total li fe moves & achievement (more moves = higher achievement)

    High mobility1IQ group had sig. higher achievement scores than low mobilityl high IQ or high mobilityllow IQ groups

    Mobility af fected reading but not arithmetic scores

    Academic success AcculturaIization & urbanization finked with academic success

    Academic # of moves sig. performance associated with poorer

    academic achievement

    Student Achievement levels of achievement stable groups & sig.

    higher than mobile groups; Impact of mobility diminishes in higher grades.

  • TABLE 1 (continued)

    STUDY SUBJECTS INDEPENDENT DEPENDENT FINDINGS (age, etc) VARIABLE VARIABLE

    Mobility and Psychosocial Effects

    Calabrese Grade 10- Mobility Alienation Early moves no sig. (1989) U.S. 12; urban; (Dean Alienation ef fect on alienation;

    n=239 Scale) greater alienation for older adolescents; less alienation for adolescents who plan to go to college; sig. effects for race & alienation.

    Goebel sophomores, Mobility SES; sex; Higher mobility (1981) U.S. juniors & patterns: age a t intelligence; 3 during preschool &

    seniors; rnove; intra- vs way relationship lower rates during n=586 inter-community adolescence associated

    Stroh & Brett (1990a) U .S.

    Stroh & Brett (1990b) US.

    Fenzel

    moves.

    6-18 years; Short term & Social, corporation cumulative behavioural & trans fers; ef fects of school n=309 mobility; before adjustment;

    after data physical health; sel f-con fidence

    6-18 years; Mobility Premove & corporation postmove transfers; attitudes and n=56 activities.

    with higher SES, greater intelligence.

    No short-term e f fects of moving or negative ef fects o f frequent moves; coping ability linked to previous coping ability.

    3 months af ter move fewer best f riends, less time in sports, lessons and reading; no Sig. interactions for age, sexy p removd postmove activities except rank order constant; postmove attitude related to premove at t i tude and mother's well-being.

    Grade 6; Scheduled school Role strain Development of Early (1989) U.S. n=120 transition ~ d o l e s c e n t Role

    Strain Inventory (EASRI); concept of role strain has merit in studying adolescent stress.

  • 22

    TABLE 1 (continued)

    STUDY SUBJECTS INDEPENDENT DEPENDENT FINDINGS (age, etc) VARIABLE VARIABLE

    Mobility and Psychosocial Ef fects (continued)

    Field 2-5 years; (1984) U.S. n=28

    Vernberg Grade 7-8; (1990) U.S. n=73

    Mann Under- (1972) U.S. graduates

    n=69

    Brett 1-18 years; (1982) U.S. n=373

    School transfer Separation stress Increased agitated behaviour before the move for movers; post-move, agi tated behaviour diminished but increased in children who stayed.

    Mobility Experiences with Fewer contacts with peers friends & less

    intimacy with best f riends; Mobile boys had more rejection than peers.

    Highllow Adaptation: OP1 High mobile group mobility & scores; anxiety had less anxiety than upward/no social ratings; low mobile group; mobility classroom social but not

    pre ferences; residential mobility ANOVA af fected classroom

    preferences; mobile males more intellectually oriented, value autonom y, independ. & more adaptive; no dif ferences for fernales.

    Rate of mobility Well-being Mobile adolescents (moves/years in had more health work force; problems; mobile boys rnoves/age for less persistent on children) tasks; mobile girls

    more frequent behaviour problems; # of moves related to quality of peer relations for children, not adolescents; missing friends & making new friends harder for adolescents than children.

  • TABLE 1 (continued)

    STUDY SUBJECTS INDEPENDENT DEPENDENT FINDINGS (age, etc) VARIABLE VARIABLE

    Mobility and Psychosocial Ef fects (continued)

    Hirsch & Grade 6-8; Peer social Psy chological Sig. cross-sectional DuBois scheduled support symptomology correlates bet ween (1992) U.S. transition; peer support & mental

    n=143 health.

    Soussignan 5-6 years; Changing school Behavioural & Grade 1's more off - et al. n=17 environment ( K - cardiovascuIar task time than in (1988) U K Grade 1) changes Kindergarten; less

    time with peers; no sig. di f f. in heart rate.

    Gilchrist Grade 6; Move from Student's Entry to high school et al , middle & elementary to perception of potentially disturbing (1988) US. lower SES; junior high change; beliefs; in normal adolescents;

    77% white; school readiness worried more about 23% non- social than academic white; n=606 matters.

    Dowling 10-21 years; Trans fer & Adjustmeat (1986) U K scheduled predictor factors (behaviour;

    transition; (verbal reason; attitude; n=503 sentence reading; attendance);

    personality before & after inventory; da ta behaviour; attendance; gender

    Best predictor of secondary adaptation is behaviour in primary; Attitude to school least predictable; Best predictor of secondary attendance was primary attendance .

    Kroger 16-17 years; Distance, # & Sel f-concept No sig. correlations (1980) U.S. middle class; recency of between # of moves,

    n=242 moves; greatest # recency of last move, of moves from 4 or age of greatest age spans moves & self-concept;

    negative correlation between distance of moves & self-concept.

    Jason et al. Grades 3-5; Parent-based High-risk (1993) U.S. n=147 intervention children

    50% of high-intensity intervention group moved from poor to average/good coping compared with 30% of low-intensi ty group.

  • TABLE 1 (continued)

    STUDY SUBJECTS INDEPENDENT DEPENDENT FINDINGS (age, etc) VARIABLE VARIABLE

    Mobility and Psychosocial Ef fects (continued)

    Walker & 4-16 years; Residential Boyle (un- n=3294 mobility: published) frequency of Chedoke- moves, recency McMaste r of move hospital; Child Health Study 1983 Canada

    Gerner et High schoo1; International al. (1992) a=1076 mobility U.S.

    Causey & Grade 7; Coping strategies Dubow scheduled & perceptions of (1993) U.S. move; n=162 school

    environment

    Barone et Grade 12; Mainstream o r al. (1993) scheduled special education U.S. move; n=234 status

    Child psychiatric # of moves associated disorders with emotional

    disorder; risk (29%) associated with low income; reccncy of move predictor of conduct disorder & hyperactivity (no temporal information) high rate of mobility associated with increase in child psychiatric disorders (not straight forward); single move not same resu l ts.

    Interest in travel, Internationally mobile languages; adolescents more ratings of interest in crave1 & cultural learning languages; accep tance; more culturally orientation to accepting & oriented international toward international li festyle lifestyle in future.

    Adaptation Approach coping strategies, favourable perceptions of school environment tied to higher levels of perceived coping ef fectiveness & general adaptation; avoidance strategies with lower levels; initial levels of coping strategies predictive of later perceived ef fectiveness.

    Education & Spec. ed. group higher employment risk of disengagement; outcornes Respondents relying

    on informa1 rather than forma1 resources; sig. changes in social networks 6 months ~ost- transi t ion.

  • TABLE 1 (continued)

    STUDY SUBJECTS INDEPENDENT DEPENDENT FINDINGS (age, etc) VARIABLE VARIABLE

    Mobility and Psychosocial Ef fects (continued)

    Vernberg 12-14 years; Ratings of Intimacy & Social perspective et al. new sophistication companionship in coordination (1994) U.S. enrollees;

    n=74 close f riendships

    Barrett & 3-18 years; Long distance Emotional Noble mobile, non- moves adjustment (1973) U.S. military; (scores on

    n=318 questionnaire & LBCL; mot hers' anxieties)

    Har tere t Grades7-8; Mobility al. (1992) scheduled US. move; n=463

    Academic self- concepts, motivation

    important influence on f riendship formation.

    No dif ference for Total Disability, Aggression, Inhibition or LD; somewhat more disturbed post-move but still below total population & dissipated quickly; more d i f ficulties making new friends af ter age Il.

    Changes in perceived competence (50% felt either more or less competent) related to changes in motivation & to school-related affect & anxiety after transition.

  • TABLE 1 (continued)

    STUDY SUBJECTS INDEPENDENT DEPENDENT FINDINGS (age, etc) VARIABLE VARIABLE

    Qualitative

    Brown & 11 years; Transition to Children's Armstrong S.W. London; secondary school worries & (1986) UK n=220 positive feelings

    Murdoch 8-12 years; (1986) U K n=42

    Transition to Individual and upper school; social meanings reading ages & of t r am fer m a t h scores; events social class; position in family; other schools attended

    Wide individual variations & idiosyncratic nominations of worry; real, if transitory fears; general patterning & structure of both positive & negative expectations changes over time; no sig. di f f , bet ween girls' & boys' worries; evidence of degree of mismatch between teachers' assessments & children's reported feelings; 3 distinct types of transfer experience anxicty (transi tory, residual & debilitating).

    Social class may have in fluenced expectations of transfer as may have previous school size and # of post-move schools group went to.

  • 27

    Table 2 shows an overview of the subjects in the studies. Of the thirty-eight

    studies reviewed, ha1 f targeted adolescents and over ha1 f (t wenty-t wo) studied

    subjects in middle school, junior high school or high school. Subjects in twenty-five

    of the studies were divided fairly evenly between males and females, although

    twelve of the studies failed to specify the gender mix.

    TABLE 2 Major Characteristics of Studies Reviewed

    n = 38

    Study Characteristics # of studies

    Age of Subjects c 5 years 6-11 years 12-19 years Mixed Unspeci f ied Total

    Grade Range of Subjects Preschool Elementary school Middle school/junior high High school Post-secondary Mixed Unspeci fied Total

    Gender % male % female < 40 > 60 40-60 40-60 > 60 c 40

    Unspeci fied Total

  • CRITIQUE OF MOBILITY STUDIES

    REASON FOR MOVE

    Of the studies reviewed, most contained serious flaws. One major problem

    with much of the mobility literature is failure to differentiate between types of

    moves or reasons for moves. Generalization of results of a specific group of movers

    (Le., children of military personnel) to the whole population, or failure to

    differentiate between reasons and/or types of moves can invalidate results. Jason er

    al. (1992) stress the importance of reason for a move as a factor influencing a child's

    adjustment to a school transfer. Table 3 shows the studies which controlled for o r

    faited to control for reason and type of move.

    Of the thirty-eight studies, eighteen presented mixed or unspecified reasons

    for moves. Of those, one study (Warren-Sohlberg & Jason, 1992) looked at the

    correlation between the reason for a move and academic achievement, and found

    that the reason for a move may have a dif ferential ef fect upon academic

    achievement in readiog, spelling and math. In this study Warren-Sohlberg and Jason

    found that children moving for "household considerations" or "transferring from

    public school" experienced a drop in marks, while those chitdren who transferred

    because their old school was closing scored consistently highest on al1 subtests. This

    important finding indicates that the question is relevant and should be included as

    background information in mobility studies.

  • TABLE 3 - Reason for Move

    STUDY corporate military family scheduled migrant mixed breakupl transition otherjnot financial speci fied hardship

    Johnson & X Lindblad (1991)

    Calabrese (1989) X

    Stroh & Brett X (1990a)

    Stroh & Brett X (1990b)

    Fenzel (1989)

    Field (1984)

    Warren-Sohlberg & Jason (1992)

    Marchant & Medway (1987)

    Filippelli & Jason (1992)

    Whalen & Fried (1973)

    Felner et al. (1982)

    Vernberg (1990)

    Mann (1972)

    Brett (1982)

    Morris et al. (1967)

    Hirsch & DuBois (1992)

  • TABLE 3 (continued)

    STUDY corporate military family scheduled migrant mixed breakupl transition otherfnot f inancial speci f ied hardship

    Pepler & Moore (unpublished - 1991)

    Levine et al. (1966)

    Barone, et al. (1991)

    Manaster et al. (1992)

    Gerner et al. (1992)

    Jason et al. (1993)

    Causey & Dubow (1993)

    Reyes & Hedeker (1993)

    Barone et al. (1993)

    Ingetsoll et al. (1989)

    Vernberg et al. (1994)

    Barrett & Noble (1973)

    Hatzichristou & Hop f (1992)

    Harter et al. (1992)

    Soussignan et al. (1988)

  • TABLE 3 (continued)

    STUDY corporate military famiiy scheduled migrant mixed breakupl transition o t h e r h o t financial speci fied hardship

    Gilchrist et ai. (1988)

    Brown & Armstrong (1986)

    Murdoch (1986)

    Dowling (1986)

    K roger (1980)

    Walker & Boyle (unpublished - 1983)

    TOTAL (n=38)

    Of the remaining seventeen studies, three adequately describe reasons for

    moving and give arguments for the homogeneity of the population (Barrett & Noble,

    1973; Field, 1984; Gerner, Perry, Moselle & Archbold, 1992) while the other fifteen

    overlook the question. Why this important factor has been ignored can be speculated

    upon. I t may be that researchers do not ask the question or that the school

    administration does not gather such information at the time of a move. It may also

    be that for some parents, revealing the true reason for a move is embarrassing,

    especially if that reason inciudes financial hardship or a family break-up.

    Nevertheless, to truly understand how moving exacerbates or ameliorates certain

    known risk factors, the information must be more rigorously collected and

    examined.

  • PRE- A N D POST-MOVE DATA

    Another common omission in studies was failure to provide da ta both before

    and af ter a move. AI1 studies reported data a f t e r the move but only seventeen of the

    reviewed thirty-eight studies included full o r partial da ta from before the move as

    Table 4 shows. Neglecting to include data from both before and af ter the move

    compromises the validity of results. Without this important compatison, it is

    diff icult to ascertain whether results found are effects of the move or are due to

    factors present before and possibly unretated to the move. Granted, the mobile

    population is diff icult to research well. The unpredictable nature of some moves

    because of, for example, short notice for job transfers, and sudden fami1y crises

    (marital separation or job loss) make it diff icult to anticipate subject pools and

    track mobile groups. While i t is fairly simple to Iocate a sample of children who

    have moved into an area, it can be difficult to obtain pre-move data for such a

    group. The studies whose authors were successful in overcoming at least some of

    these difficulties by including pre- and post-move data as part of their design are

    described in the next section. Some of these studies included subjects whose moving

    patterns were easier to track than others due to the reason for and nature of the

    move, for example, subjects who moved at scheduled transition points.

  • TABLE 4 - Research Controls STUDY pre- & SES IQ learning peer adult

    post- problems support support move

    Johnson & Lindblad (1991)

    Calabrese (1989)

    Stroh & Brett X (1990a)

    Stroh & Brett X (1990b)

    Fenzel (1989) X

    Field (1984) X

    Warren-Sohlberg X & Jason (1992)

    Marchant & Medway (1987)

    FilippeIli & Jason (1992)

    Whalen & Fried (1973)

    Felner et al. (1982)

    Vernberg (1990)

    Mann (1972)

    Brett (1982)

    Morris et al. (1967)

    Hirsch & DuBois X (1992)

  • TABLE 4 (continued)

    STUDY pre- & SES IQ Iearning peer adult post- problems support support move

    Pepler & Moore (unpublished - 1991)

    Levine et al. (1966)

    Barone, et al. (199 1)

    Manaster et al. (1992)

    Gerner et al. (1992)

    Jason et al. (1993)

    Causey & Dubow (1993)

    Reyes & Hedeker (1993)

    Barone et al, (1993)

    Ingersoll et al. (1989)

    Vernberg et al. (1994)

    Barrett & Noble (1973)

    Hatzichristou & Hop f (1992)

    Harter et al. (1992)

    Soussignan et al. (1988)

  • TABLE 4 (continued)

    STUDY pre- & SES IQ learning peer adult post- problems support support move

    Gilchrist et ut. (1988)

    Brown & X Armstrong (1986)

    Murdoch (1986) X

    Dowling (1986) X

    Kroger (1980)

    Walker & Boyle (unpublished - 1983)

    TOTAL (n=38)

    a) PRE- AND POST- DATA FOR MOVES MADE AT SCHEDULED

    TRANSITION POINTS

    The groups of researchers most consistently successful in gathering both pre-

    and post-move data were those studying subjects at normal transition times (i.e.,

    preschool to elementary school; elementary to school; junior to senior high school).

    Of the thirteen transition studies twelve include both pre- and post-move data,

    providing valuable information about the effects of normal transitions. Studies of

    scheduled traasitions allow easier and more standardized collection of pre-and post-

    move data because the sampIe group generally moves a t one time from one or more

    feeder schoo1s to the next level. The moves are predictable and easy to document

    and thus have afforded useful information about students at normal transition

    times.

    Of these studies, three examined the ef fect of mobility on academic

  • achievement (Barone et al., 1991; Felner, Ginter & Primavera, 1982; Reyes &

    Hedeker, 1993). Two of the studies found that when pre-move grade point averages

    (GPA) were compared with those post-move, there was a significant decrease in

    GPA's af ter the transition. The third study (Felner et al., 1982) examined the

    e f ficacy of an intervention project, comparing control subjects wit h the project

    subjects. The project subjects had signi ficantly better grades than the control

    subjects post-move, even though the subjects were well-matched pre-move. While it

    cannot be known from this data whether the third group also had lower post-move

    grades, it is interesting to note that in this study extra intervention made a

    difference in academic achievement af ter a move. Thus, looking at the general

    trends in these three studies there may be some deleterious academic effects even in

    normal transitions which may need to be addressed.

    These three studies plus the remaining nine addressed issues chat migh t be

    loosely grouped under the heading of psychosocial effects and included self-

    concept, role strain, worries, peer support, mental health, coping strategies and

    absenteeism. Al1 studies found some changes between pre- and post-move measures,

    predominantly negative, such as greater distress or worries, higher absenteeism, or

    lower perceived effectiveness. Five of the twelve found that while al1 subjects

    experienced drops on the dependent variable, there was a link between pre- and

    post-move abilities. For example, higher absenteeism pre-move predicted higher

    absen teeism rates post-move.

    It is indicated in these studies, albeit haphazardly, that even a normal time

    of transition may be a stressor for children. For example, in one study Fenzel (1989)

    developed an assessrnent tool (Early Adolescent School Role Strain Inventory) for

    measuring role strain in young adolescents moving from elementary to middle

    school, and analyzed its validity by administering known and normed measures at

    one point in t h e before, and two points in time after, the school transition. He

    found that the concept of role strain is likely to be useful in "investigating stress

  • 37

    related to the student role in early adolescencen (p.29). His conclusion is that simply

    being a student undergoing a transition is likely in itself a stressor.

    b) PRE- A N D POST- DATA IN UNSCHEDULED MOVES

    Studies of students at scheduled transition periods provide valuable

    information about students' reactions to scheduled moves, but can not necessarily be

    generalized to unscheduled moves d u e to changes in family circumstances, job

    transfer, and so on. It is thus useful to find some studies, albeit iimited in numbers,

    which employ pre- and post-move data from studics of mobility at other than

    normal scheduIed transition points (Field, 1984; Jason, Johnson, Danner, Taylor &

    Kurasaki, 1993; Stroh & Brett, 1990a & 1990b; Warren-Sohlberg and Jason, 1992).

    Stroh and Brett (1990a & 1990b) succeeded in gathering pre- and post-move

    information by interviewing families of employees of estabtished Fortune 500

    corporations at two time periods 18 months apart. The companies' records provided

    the information necessary to track the families effectively. The sample of subjects

    consisted of those families who were transferred between the two data collection

    points. Families of those employees not transferred during this period served as the

    control group. Stroh and Brett found that the effects of the move itself were not

    significant fo r the children of corporate movers. Rather, they found that an

    important predictor in children's adjustment to a move were their pre-move

    activities. In addition, the mother's adjustment to the move was found to be

    significantly related to children's attitudes to moving, although no causal

    relationship was found. Stroh and Brett point out that the results of these corporate

    move studies can only be generalized to other corporate moves. While insigni ficant

    effects were found in this study, significant results may be found in studies of

    other populations with fewer resources.

    Warren-Sohlberg and Jason, (1992), obtained partial pre-and post-move data

    by using school grades as one of the measures contributing to total adjustment.

  • Report card grades in math and reading were used from the quarter before the

    move and from the quarter af ter the move. However, report card grades are not

    standardized from teacher to teacher, o r from school to school and thus cannot be

    considered reliable sources for comparing pre- and post-move adjustment.

    Nevertheless, using the before and af ter grades, Warren-Sohlberg and Jason found

    that no rnatter what the reason for moving, grades dropped for al1 the t r am fer

    students w hen they changed schools.

    Jason et al. (1993) compared the impact of high-intensity and low-intensity

    parent-based interventions on groups of high-risk children after a move and found

    that the high-intensity interventions helped 50% of the students move from poor to

    average or good coping compared with 30% of the low-intensity group. Like the

    study by Felner et al., (1982), this study suggests that not only are some students at

    risk for academic difficulties, but that some interventions at the time of a move

    may help lessen that risk, thus reinforcing the need for better, more complete

    examinations of the nature of the interaction between mobility and risk.

    In a study using multiple methods of data collection (naturalistic

    observation, physiological measures, child interviews and parent and teacher

    ratings) Field, (19841, followed two groups of preschoolers as some moved to new

    schools, while others remained in the original school. The subjects were preschoolers

    attending an all-day nursery school-kindergarten and the moves took place at the

    close of a summer school session. Timing of the moves to coincide with the end of

    term, and the short distance moved helped to facilitate tracking of the subjects in

    this relatively small sample (N = 28). The children who moved were found to have

    physioiogical and behavioural symptoms of agitation prior to the move, while the

    children who remained in the original school experienced these physiological and

    behavioural agitation symptoms after the other children had left. While these

    findings cannot be generalized to include movers of al1 ages o r even al1 preschool

    moves, they make a worthwhile contribution to the body of preschooler mobility

  • 39

    literature and point towards coping responses common to at least some preschoolers

    in transition.

    The studies employing pre- and post-move data provide important

    information about the impact of normal transition points, but they represent a

    minority among the mobility studies. Twenty-one of the thirty-eight studies

    reviewed failed to provide data pre- and post-move. This may explain, in part, the

    wide variety of results and some of the contradictions inherent in the body of

    literature examining mobility. There is no doubt that serious problems exist in

    attempting ta gather pre- and post-move information that is valid and worthwhile,

    due to the nature of the subject group, the unpredictability of many moves, and the

    difficulty in tracking subjects from one geographical or administrative area to

    another. Still, the questions remain and perhaps in an era relying more and more on

    computer access to information, there may be some hope that some of these

    technical limitations may be overcome by obtaining more precise pre- and post-

    move data which provides more exact information. Such information would

    facilitate the planning of better, more accurately-targeted interventions.

    MOBILITY AND RISK

    Studies which examined the interaction between specific potential strengths

    and risks for children moving were less evident, as Table 4 shows. Fewer than half

    of the studies (eighteen) accounted or controlled for socioeconomic status. Two

    unpublished Canadian studies are worth examining in relation to this question

    (Pepler & Moore, unpublished 1991; Walker & Boyle, unpublished 1983). Pepler and

    Moore compared homeless children residing in shelters with children living a t home

    in both one- and two-parent families. The children living in shelters scored

    significantly lower on achievement tests of reading and arithmetic, had higher

    checklist scores fo r behaviour problems, and exhibited a more external locus of

    control than did the children living in the family home. Walker and Boyles' study

  • 40

    found that for children, moving two or more times was linked with emotional

    disorder and that part of the risk (29%) was linked with poverty. While i t woufd be

    premature to draw conclusions about causality from either o f these studies, both

    raise important questions which have not been adequately addressed by the mobility

    literature to date. Both of these studies begin to examine the link between mobiIity

    and some aspects of SES.

    Even fewer studies cont rolled for intelligence (IQ - fivc), learning probIems

    (four), peer support (four), or adult support (four). These omissions present serious

    limitations in view of the profound impact that each of these factors has on the

    lives of children. Since this type of data appears crucial to an examination of the

    risk factors associated with mobility, its omission may account for the genera1 lack

    of data useful for practitioners in much of the mobility Iiterature.

  • LIMITATIONS A N D DIRECTIONS FOR F U T U R E RESEARCH

    Future studies investigating effects of geographic mobility on children

    should provide more precise descriptions of al1 subjects, controls, variables and

    measures in order to address some of the major flaws noted in the mobility

    literature described above: failure to control for risk variables known to affect

    school adjustment, (i.e., SES, IQ, peer relationships, family di f ficulties); failure to

    differentiate between types of moves or reasons for moves; and failure to include

    both pre- and post-move data. While much has been hypothesized and written about

    the effects of geographic mobility on the academic and psychosocial developrnent of

    children and adolescents, the findings to date tend to be contradictory. Researchers

    have made valiant attempts to document the effects of mobility on children, but

    subjects are mobile and therefore diff icult to track effectively. Consequently the

    research does not yield the type of information practitioners are looking for.

    Research to date is sketchy and has serious gaps. The studies reviewed lack

    information which may be predictive of difficulties for children. None commented

    upon the amount o f input a child had in the decision to move. Information is

    lacking about differences between previous and receiving schools' curricula. Clear,

    consistent definitions of socioeconomic status are rare. Information about the

    differences due to maturity or developmental stages at the time of a move is also

    lacking. The questions still remain: does i t make a dif ference why children move; is

    the reason for a move important?

    Nevertheless, researchers developing a more disciplined approach to research

    in this field rnay more clearly delineate risks for mobile children. As suggested by

    Vernberg and Field (1990), the delineation of major tasks or challenges posed by a

    transition is a necessary step in understanding how children may be affected by a

    move, a change of school and so forth. If we know what is involved in a move for

    any particular individual child, we might then be better equipped to help that child

    negotiate the move successfully.

  • BIBLIOCRAPHY

    Allan, J.A.B. and Bardsley, P. (1984). Children who move. Toronto, ON: Faculty of Education, University of Toronto,

    Asher, S.R. (1983). Social competence and peer status: Recent advances and future directions. Child Deveio~ment, 54, 1427-1434.

    Asher, S.R., Hymel, S. and Renshaw, P.D. (1984). Loneliness in children. Child Develo~ment, 55, 1456-1464.

    Barone, C., Aquirre-Deandreis, A.I. and Trickett, E.J. (1991). Means-end problem- solving skills, life stress and social support as mediators of adjustment in the normative transition to high school. American Journal of Comm u n i tv Psvcholoa~, 19(2), 207-225.

    Barone, C., Trickett, E.J., Schmidt, K.D. and Leone, P.E. (1993). Transition tasks and resources: An ecological approach to life after high school. In L.A. Jason, K.E. Danner and K.S. Kurasaki (Eds). Prevention and school transitions. New York: The Haworth Press, Inc.

    Barrett, C.L. and Noble, H. (1973). Mothers' anxieties versus the effects of long distance move on children. Journal of Marriane and the Familv. 35(2), 181-188.

    Bogat, G.A., Liang, B., Caldwell, R.A., Davidson, W., Bristor, M., Phillips, M., and Suurmeyer, M. (1993). Alternative schools: A school transition for adolescent mothers. In L.A. Jason, K.E. Danner and K.S. Kurasaki (Eds). Prevention and school transitions. New York: The Haworth Press, Inc.

    Byrnes, D.A. (1985). "Ciphern in the classroom: the invisible child. Childhood Education, 62(2), 91-97.

    Calabrese, R.L. (1989). The ef fects of mobiIity on adolescent alienation. Hinh School Journal, 73(1), 41-46.

    Caldas, S.J. (1993). Reexamination of input and process factor effects on public school achievement. Journal of Educational Research, 86(4), 206-214.

    Cauce, A.M., Hannan, K. and Sargeant, M. (1992). Life stress, social support, and locus of control during early adolescence: Interactive effects. American Journal o f Commuai ty Psvcholony, 20(6), 787-798.

    Causey, D.L. and Dubow, E.F. (1993). Negotiating the transition to junior high school: The contribution of the school environment. In L.A. Jason, K.E. Danner and K.S. Kurasaki (Eds). Prevention and school transitions. New York: The Haworth Press, Inc.

    Christenson, S.L. (1990). Differences in students' home environments: The need to work with families. School Psvcholoav Review, 19(4), 505-517.

    Cohen, L.H., Burt, C.E. and Bjork, J.P. (1987). Life stress and adjustment: Effects of li f e even ts experienced b y young adolescents and their parents. Deveio~men ta1 Psvcholoav 23(4), 583-592.

  • Cohen, S. and Wills, T.A. (1985). Stress, social support and the bu f fering hypothesis. Psvcholoaical Bulletin, 99(2), 310-357.

    Coie, J.D. and Kupersmidt, J.B. (1983). A behavioral analysis of emerging social status in boys' groups. Child Deveio~ment, 54, 1400-1416.

    Compas, B.E. (1987). Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence. Psvcholonical Bulletin, 101(3), 393-403.

    Crick, N.R. and Dodge, K.A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psvcholonical Bulletin, 115(1), 74-101.

    Cummins, J. (1986). Psychological assessment of minority students: Out of context, out of focus, out of control? Journal of Reading. Writinn. and Learnina Disabilities, 2(1), 9-19.

    Danner, K.E., Jason, L.A. and Kurasaki, K.S. (1993). Introduction. In L.A. Jason, K.E. Danner and K.S. Kurasaki (Eds.) Prevention and school transitions. New York: The Haworth Press, Inc.

    Dodge, K.A. (1983). Behavioral antecedents of peer social status. CbiId Deveio~ment , 54, 1386-1399.

    Felner, R.D., Ginter, M. and Primavera, J. (1982). Primary prevention during school transitions: Social support and environmental structure. American Journal of Communi tv Psvcholonv, 10, 277-290.

    Fenzel, L.M. (1989). Role strain in early adolescence: A mode1 for investigating school transition stress. Journal of Earlv Adolescence, 9(1-2), 13-33.

    Fergusson, D.M., Horwood, L.J. and Lynsky, M. (1994). The childhoods of multiple problem adolescents: A 15-year longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psvcholonv and Psvchiatrv, 35(6), 1123-1 140,

    Field, T. (1984). Separation stress of young children trans ferring to new schools. Deveiobmentaî Psvcholo Y. 20(5), 786-792.

    French, D.C. (1990). Heterogeneity of peer-rejected girls. Child Develo~ment, 61, 2028-203 1.

    Garmezy, N. (1991). Resiliency and vulnerability to adverse developmental outcomes associated with poverty. American Behavioral Scientist, 34(4) 416-430.

    Garmezy, N., Masten, AS. and Tellegen, A. (1984). The study of stress and competence in children: A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Develomnent, 55, 97-111.

    Gerner, M., Perry, F., Moselle, M.A. and Archbold, M. (1992). Characteristics of internationally mobile adolescents. Journal of School Psvcholoqy, 30(2), 197-214.

    Goebel, B.L. (1981). Mobile children: An American tragedy? PsvchoIoaical R e ~ o r t s 48, 15-18.

  • Harter, S., Whitesell, N.R. and Kowalski, P. (1992). Individual differences in the ef fects of educational transitions on young adolescents' perceptions of competence and motivational orientation. American Educational Research Journa1 29(4), 777- 807.

    Haynes, N.M., Comer,J.P. and Hamilton-Lee, M. (1989). School climate enhancernent through parental invoIvement. Journal of School Psvcholonv, 27, 87-90.

    Hirsch, B.J. and Du Bois, D.L. (1992). The relation of peer social support and psychological symptomatology during the transition to junior high school: A two- year longitudinal analysis. American Journal of Communitv Psvcholoae 20(3), 333- 347,

    Huesman, L.W., Eron, L.D., Lefkowitz, M.M. and Walder, L.O. (1984). Stability of aggression over time and generations. Develo~menta l Psvcholo~v, 20(6), 1120- 1134.

    Hymel, S., Rubin, K.H., Rowden, L. and LeMare, L. (1990). Children's peer relationships: Longitudinal prediction of internalizing and externalizing problems from middle to late childhood. ChiId Development, 61, 2004-2021.

    Ingersoll, G.M., Scamman, J.P., and Eckerling, W.D. (1989). Geographic mobility and student achievement in an urban setting. Educational Evaluation and Policv Analvsis, 11(2), 143-149.

    Ingraham, C.L. (1985). Cognitive-affective dynamics of crisis intervention for school entry, school transition and school failure. School Psvcho lo~v Review, 14(3), 266- 279.

    Jason, L.A., Betts, D., Johnson, J.H., Weine, A.M., Warren-Sohlburg, M.L., Shinaver, C.S., Neuson, L., Filippelli, L. and Lardon, C. (1990). Promoting cornpetencies in high-risk transfer children. Special Services in the Schools, 6(1/2), 21-36.

    Jason, L.A., Johnson, J.H., Danner, K.E., Taylor, S. and Kurasaki, K.S. (1993). A comprehensive, preventive, parent-based intervention for high-risk t r a m fer students. In L.A. Jason, K.E. Danner and K.S. Kurasaki (Eds). Prevention and school transitions. New York: The Haworth Press, Inc.

    Jason, L.A., Weine, AM., Johnson, J.H., Warren-Sohlberg, L., Filippelli, L.A., Turner, E.Y. and Lardon, C. (1992). Helpinn transfer students: Stratenies for educational and social readiustment. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers.

    Johnson, R.A. and Lindblad, A.H. (1991). Effect of mobility on academic performance of sixth grade students. P e r c e ~ t u a l and Motor Skills, 72, 547-552.

    Kandell, E., Mednick, S.A., Kirkegaard-Sorenson, L., Hutchings, B., Knop, J., Rosenberg, R. and Schulsinger, F. (1988). IQ as a protective factor for subjects at high risk for antisocial behavior. Journal of Consultinn and Clinical Psvcholo~v, 56(2), 224-226.

    Kroger, J.E. (1980). Residential mobility and self concept in adolescence. Adolescence, 15(60), 967-977.

    Kurtz, L. and Derevensky, J.L. (1993). Stress and coping in adoIescents: the ef fects of family configuration and environment on suicidality. Canadian Journal of School Psvcholonv, 9(2) 204-216.

  • Leonard, C.P. and Elias, M.J. (1993). Entry into middle school: Student factors predicting adaptation to an ecoIogica1 transition. In L.A. Jason, K.E. Danner and K.S. Ku rasaki (Eds). Prevention and school transitions. New York: The Haworth Press, Inc.

    Light, R.J. and Pillemer, D.B. (1984). Summina UD: The science of reviewinq research. Cambridge, MS:Harvard University Press.

    Lochman, S.E. and Lampron, L.B. (1985). The usefulness of peer ratings of aggression and social acceptance in the identification of behavioral and subjective difficulties in aggressive boys. Journal of A ~ ~ l i e d Deveio~menta l Psvcholonv, 6, 187-198,

    Luthar, S.S. (1991) Vulnerability and resilience: A study of high-risk adolescents. Child Develo~ment, 62, 600-616.

    Manaster, G.J., Chan, J.C. and Safady, R. (1992). Mexican-American migrant students' academic success: Sociological and psychological acculturation. Adolescence, 27(105), 123-136.

    Marchant, K.H. and Medway, F.J. (1987). Adjustment and achievement associated with mobility in military families. Psvcholonv in the Schools, 24, 289-294.

    Masten, A.S., Best, K.M. and Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: contributions from the study of children who overcame adversity. Deve lo~ment and Psvcho~atholonv, 2, 425-444.

    Moore, T.E. and Pepler, D.J. (unpublished). Academic deficits in shelter and hostel children. A workshop presented by the York Family ProbIem-solving Project.

    Morris, J.L., Pestaner, M. and Nelson, A. (1967). Mobility and achievement. The Journal of ExDerimental Education. 35(4), 74-80.

    Offord, D. (1989). Ontario chiId health studv: Summarv of findinns. Toronto: Queen's Printer.

    Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (1987) Migrants' children at school. The OECD Observer, 146, 16-18 & 23.

    Parker, J.G. and Asher, S.R. (1987). Peer relations and later persona1 adjustment: Are low-accepted children a t risk? Psvcholoai,cal Bulletin, 102(3), 357-389.

    Patterson, C.J., Kupersmidt, J.B. and Griesler, P.C. (1990). Children's perceptions of self and of relationships with others as a function of sociometric status. Child Deveio~ment , 61, 1335-1349.

    Patterson, C.J., Vaden, N.A. and Kupersrnidt, J.B. (1991). Family background, recent l i fe events, and peer rejection dur iag childhood. Journal of Social and Persona1 Relat ionshi~s, 8(3) 347-36 1.

    Pepler, D.J. and Moore, T.E. (1991). Children of the homeless: Living in the shadows. Paper presented by the first author as a keynote address a t the Annual General Meeting of the Defence for Children International, Canada, September 25, 1991.

    Pinder, CC. (1981). The role of transfers and mobiIity experiences in employee motivation and control. In W. Meltzer (Ed.). Makinn Oraanizations Humane and Productive: A Handbook for Practitioners. Toronto, ON: Wiley & Sons, Inc.

  • Pinder, C.C. (1989). The dark side of executive relocation. Ornanizational Dvnamics. 17(4) 48-58.

    Pinder, C.C. and Walter, G.A. (1984). Personnel transfers and employee development. Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management, 2, 187-218.

    Putallaz, M. (1983). Predicting children's sociometric status from their behavior. Child Development, 54, 1417-1426.

    Reyes, 0. and Hedeker, D. (1993). Identifying high-risk students during school transition. In L.A. Jason, K.E. Danner and K.S. Kurasaki (Eds.) Prevention and school transitions. New York: The Howarth Press, Inc.

    Reynolds, A.J., Weissberg, R.P. and Kasprow, W.J. (1992). Prediction of early social and academic adjustment of children from the inner city. Arnerican Journal of Communitv Psvcholonv, 20(5), 599-624.

    Ribordy, S. (1989). Children a t risk due to experienced trauma and loss. In J.M. Lakebrink (Ed.). Children a