24
This article was downloaded by: [UNSW Library] On: 28 October 2012, At: 18:28 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pecp20 The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom setting Jacquelyn Cranney a , Mihyun Ahn a , Rachel McKinnon a , Sue Morris b & Kaaren Watts a a School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia b Learning and Teaching at UNSW, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia Version of record first published: 10 Aug 2009. To cite this article: Jacquelyn Cranney, Mihyun Ahn, Rachel McKinnon, Sue Morris & Kaaren Watts (2009): The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom setting, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21:6, 919-940 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440802413505 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub- licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

This article was downloaded by: [UNSW Library]On: 28 October 2012, At: 18:28Publisher: Psychology PressInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

European Journal of CognitivePsychologyPublication details, including instructions for authorsand subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pecp20

The testing effect, collaborativelearning, and retrieval-inducedfacilitation in a classroomsettingJacquelyn Cranney a , Mihyun Ahn a , Rachel McKinnona , Sue Morris b & Kaaren Watts aa School of Psychology, University of New South Wales,Sydney, Australiab Learning and Teaching at UNSW, University of NewSouth Wales, Sydney, Australia

Version of record first published: 10 Aug 2009.

To cite this article: Jacquelyn Cranney, Mihyun Ahn, Rachel McKinnon, Sue Morris &Kaaren Watts (2009): The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-inducedfacilitation in a classroom setting, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21:6,919-940

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440802413505

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expresslyforbidden.

Page 2: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make anyrepresentation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up todate. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should beindependently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liablefor any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damageswhatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connectionwith or arising out of the use of this material.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 3: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

The testing effect, collaborative learning, and

retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom setting

Jacquelyn Cranney, Mihyun Ahn, and Rachel McKinnon

School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia

Sue Morris

Learning and Teaching at UNSW, University of New South Wales, Sydney,

Australia

Kaaren Watts

School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia

Two studies were conducted to investigate aspects of the test effect in a tertiaryeducation setting. During weekly tutorial sessions first year psychology studentswatched a psychobiology video (Phase 1), followed by different video-relatedactivities (Phase 2). In the tutorial 1 week later, students took an unexpected test(Phase 3). In Phase 2 of Study 1, students completed a quiz in small groups (groupquiz) or individually (individual quiz), highlighted the video transcript (re-study),or did nothing further (no-activity). Group quiz performance was superior toindividual quiz in both Phase 2 and Phase 3. In Phase 3 individual quiz studentsperformed better than no-activity students, but not better than restudy students. Inexploring the individual testing effect further, Phase 2 of Study 2 included quiz(individual), restudy, and no-activity conditions. Quiz participants were presentedwith one (target) of two sets of questions, whereas restudy participants werepresented with equivalent statements. During Phase 3, all participants answeredboth sets of questions (target and related). Quiz performance was superior torestudy and no-activity performance on both target and related material,supporting the retrieval-induced facilitation explanation of the testing effect.Implications of the current research for assessment practices in classroom settingsare discussed, and directions for future research are indicated.

Keywords: Testing effect; Collaborative testing; Retrieval-induced facilitation;

Educational setting.

Correspondence should be addressed to Jacquelyn Cranney, School of Psychology, University

of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia. E-mail: [email protected]

Study 1 in this paper was presented at the 29th annual conference of the National Institute

on the Teaching of Psychology, St. Petersburg Beach, Florida, January 2007, as well as at the

Australasian Experimental Psychology Conference, Canberra, April, 2007. The authors would

like to acknowledge the participation and cooperation of all the PSYC1011 students and tutors

in 2006 and 2007, as well as the financial support of the School of Psychology, UNSW.

EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY

2009, 21 (6), 919�940

# 2008 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business

http://www.psypress.com/ecp DOI: 10.1080/09541440802413505

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 4: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

THE TESTING EFFECT

Knowledge of an encroaching assessment usually prompts students to study;

however, they do not always study in an effective manner. For example,

‘‘study’’ in the form of simple rereading of the material may not be the

optimal means for enhancing memory for that material. Many studies within

the cognitive learning literature suggest that repeated testing leads to greater

retention of information than does repeated reading, a finding termed the

‘‘testing effect’’ (Glover, 1989; Spitzer, 1939). In particular, research has

shown that repeated testing leads to greater retention of information than

repeated rereading sessions when there is a delay of 24 hours or more

between the retrieval practice and final recall stages (Glover, 1989; Karpicke

& Roediger, 2007; Kuo & Hirshman, 1996; Spitzer, 1939).

The testing effect has been examined primarily in laboratory settings and

the learning materials administered have commonly been dissimilar to real

educational tasks (e.g., word lists or pairs of words; Roediger & Karpicke,

2006a). Recent research by Roediger and Karpicke (2006b), however,

provides a demonstration of the testing effect using educational prose

material covering general scientific topics. In their Experiment 2, all of the

participants studied one of two prose passages in the initial learning block.

In the subsequent three 5 minute blocks, participants in the repeated study

condition (SSSS) reread the passage; participants in the SSST condition

reread the passage for the next two periods, and then completed a free-recall

test related to the studied information; and participants in the repeated test

condition (STTT) took three consecutive free recall tests. Participants then

received a final recall test either 5 min after the final learning block or 1 week

later. As predicted, at the 5-min retention interval, participants in the SSSS

condition had the highest rate of recall, but at the 1-week retention interval,

participants in the STTT condition showed the greatest recall. In other

words, although massed reading had short-term efficiency, repeated testing

was superior to repeated reading in promoting long-term retention. A

separate study by Butler and Roediger (2007) found that the testing effect

can be generalised to different methods of presentation of the original

learning material, including video stimuli.

A number of different direct and indirect mechanisms have been proposed

to underlie the testing effect, and although some have been discarded (Fitch,

Drucker, & Norton, 1951; Roediger & Karpicke, 2006a; Thompson, Wegner,

& Bartling, 1978), it is likely that more than one mechanism involving both

Transfer Appropriate Processing (TAP; Karpicke & Roediger, 2007;

McDaniel, Friedman, & Bourne, 1978; Morris, Bransford, & Franks,

1977) and retrieval induced facilitation (e.g., Dempster, 1996; Roediger &

Karpicke, 2006a) is operative. The retrieval hypothesis, for example, states

that the level of retrieval effort can impact upon memory retention, such that

920 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 5: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

elaboration during the retrieval process can increase the strength of a

memory trace and increase the number of retrieval routes (Anderson, Bjork,

& Bjork, 1994; Butler & Roediger, 2007; Roediger & Karpicke, 2006a),

enabling the information to be better remembered at a later time. A test

administered following the initial encoding stage is a more effortful process

of continued learning than rereading because testing involves cognitive

engagement, elaboration, and manipulation of concepts rather than

relatively passive reprocessing of information. Thus, practice testing

increases later memory performance (Roediger & Karpicke, 2006a).

An important issue that has arisen in this research is the appropriate

comparison condition for the testing effect. In Roediger and Karpicke

(2006b, Exp. 1), the testing effect was demonstrated by the superior

performance of the test condition compared to the restudy condition on

the final (delayed) criterion test. It is argued that this is a strict criterion

testing effect because it occurs between two groups of participants who

are given the same context and time frame in which to learn the material

and only the method of review varies: testing or rereading. A lenient

criterion testing effect would be that the test condition leads to better

performance than a no-activity condition where there is no exposure to

the material during the review phase (that is, neither rereading nor

testing). Although the lenient criterion (cf. the strict criterion) application

is of less interest theoretically, it is practically interesting to examine

whether the test condition leads to better, and not worse, performance

than no study at all.

The role of feedback in promoting the testing effect is yet to be defined.

The testing effect has been demonstrated in many studies without feedback

(e.g., Roediger & Karpicke, 2006b; Wheeler & Roediger, 1992). Nevertheless,

feedback is generally considered to enhance the testing effect (e.g., Kulhavy

& Stock, 1989; McDaniel & Fisher, 1991), and immediate feedback is widely

recognised as optimal for maximising students’ memory for the tested

information in applied classroom settings (see Kulik & Kulik, 1988). Hence,

in the current classroom research, corrective feedback immediately after

testing was provided.

COLLABORATIVE TESTING

The benefits of collaborative learning in classroom settings have begun to be

empirically documented, particularly in undergraduate psychology. Colla-

borative learning situations, for instance, promote ‘‘in-depth, process-

oriented research’’ (Miyake & Shirouzu, 2006, p. 102). In traditional top-

down teaching approaches, teachers or lecturers impart knowledge and are

perceived as the ‘‘knowledge authorities’’. In contrast, collaborative learning

THE TESTING EFFECT 921

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 6: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

encourages the development of ‘‘learning communities’’ and promotes the

perception among novice learners that ‘‘knowledge is common propertyrather than primarily the property of authorities’’ (Cockrell, Caplow, &

Donaldson, 2000, p. 359). Most importantly, collaborative learning pro-

motes the development of strategies (e.g., cooperation, communication, and

negotiation), which closely approximate skills required in the ‘‘real world’’

(p. 348). The current research will explore the effectiveness of collaborative

test taking on later individual test performance.

In laboratory experiments it has been shown that during initial group

recall of study material, group performance is superior to individual but notto nominal group performance; however, the subsequent individual memory

performance is better for those who initially recalled in a collaborative than

in a nominal group (e.g., Basden, Basden, & Henry, 2000; Weldon &

Bellinger, 1997). Between- rather than within-session collaborative test-

taking effects have not been rigorously explored.

The primary aim of the current quasi-experimental research was to

examine the generalisability of the testing effect to classroom settings. The

preliminary Study 1 focused on collaborative testing effects, whereas Study2 improved the methodology in search of stronger individual testing

effects on memory for the whole learning episode. In Study 1 it was

predicted that memory performance for ‘‘old’’ test items (presented in the

initial test, i.e., Phase 2) would be superior for those participants who

completed the initial test compared to those who did not (lenient

criterion). It was also expected that memory performance for ‘‘old’’ test

items would be better for those who reviewed the material in an initial

test compared to participants who simply restudied the material (strictcriterion). It was anticipated that collaborative testing would produce

superior memory retention for ‘‘old’’ test items relative to individual

testing. In addition, an exploratory analysis of ‘‘new’’ test items, that is,

test items from the study material but not included in the initial test, was

also undertaken.

STUDY 1

Method

Participants and design

Seventy-five students (25 males, 50 females), who were enrolled in an

introductory psychology course at the University of New South Wales,

participated in this study. The age range was 15 to 28 years (M�19.42,

SD�1.90). This introductory course was comprised of 4 hours of large-classlectures and 1 hour of a smaller group tutorial (which focused on lecture-

922 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 7: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

related information and activities) each week. Data was analysed only from

students who attended tutorials on Mondays because it was reasoned that

students with tutorials on subsequent days may have become privy to

information about the procedure from other students, potentially contam-

inating the results (i.e., the communication problem; Meltzoff, 1998).

There were three experimental phases: initial learning, review, and final

memory test. The first two phases were presented consecutively in the first

tutorial, and the final phase was presented a week later in the next tutorial.

In a quasi-experimental approach, tutorials were randomly allocated to

one of four conditions (one tutorial group per condition). The conditions

were differentiated by the activity students engaged in during the review

phase, after viewing a video recording in the initial phase. The four

activities were: group quiz, individual quiz, restudy, and no-activity

control. The dependent variable was percentage of items correct on the

Phase 3 final memory test.

Materials

The video viewed by participants was called Discovering Psychology: The

Behaving Brain (WGBH Boston & American Psychological Association,

2001). The content of the video was an introduction to the way in which

electrical and chemical messages are transmitted in the brain, and the basic

functions of several of the major brain structures.

Two quizzes were developed based upon the information presented in the

video. The quiz (Phase 2) consisted of 10 questions (5 multiple choice and 5

fill-in-the-blanks, tapping into different concept units from the video). The

final memory test or pop quiz (Phase 3) consisted of 13 items: Three of these

items were replicated exactly from Quiz 1 (‘‘old’’) (two multiple choice, one

fill-in-the-blank); seven were new questions, derived from the video shown

during the first phase (‘‘new’’) (three multiple choice, four fill-in-the-blanks).

Two of the ‘‘new’’ multiple choice questions partly overlapped with the

concept units of two of the Phase 2 short answer questions (e.g., Phase 2:

‘‘the hippocampus is part of the [limbic] system’’; Phase 3: ‘‘Which

structures are part of the limbic system?’’; correct answer: amygdala,

hippocampus, hypothalamus, and thalamus). In addition, there were three

new multiple choice questions based on information presented during the

lectures (‘‘unrelated material’’). It is important to note that only participants

in the group quiz and individual quiz groups completed the quiz and,

therefore, participants in the restudy and no-activity groups were not

exposed to the ‘‘old’’ questions prior to completing the final memory test.

All materials in Study 1 and 2 are available from the first author upon

request.

THE TESTING EFFECT 923

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 8: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

Procedure

The study was integrated into the normal activities of the first yearpsychology tutorials; therefore, the time and place for each of the phases was

the same throughout the experiment. There were three phases in this study,

conducted over two sessions.

Phase 1: Initial learning. Psychobiology was introduced as a new topicin lectures during the week of Phase 1. In their tutorials, all students viewed

an 8-min portion of the video about psychobiology. Students were instructed

not to take notes because they would receive a transcript of the video prior

to the exam.

Phase 2: Review. Immediately following the video, group quiz studentscollaborated with their team of 4 to 5 people (teams were formed during the

previous week of tutorials) to complete the quiz. Individual quiz students

completed the same quiz individually and without discussion. Students were

given 10 min to complete the quiz; the quizzes were then collected. The

quizzes were then randomly allocated to another group/individual to mark.

Feedback was provided such that the answers were presented one at a time

and at a reasonably rapid rate on an overhead projector, and students could

ask questions. The quiz was then collected again, and in the group condition,

the winning (highest score) group was announced. This feedback process

took 5 min. Although students could check their score, they could not take

their quiz away.

Restudy students read through a transcript of the video for 8 min and

were instructed to ‘‘highlight or underline all the words and phrases that you

consider to be important or relevant to this course’’. Then, they had 2 min to

ask any questions about the material. Their transcripts were then collected.

Students in the no-activity condition did not re-engage with the video

information. In order to ensure equity of information access for further

study within the course, the video transcript was made available to all

students on the WebCT Vista course site 1 week after the completion of

testing in this study (i.e., after Phase 3).

Phase 3: Final memory test. In tutorials exactly 1 week after Phase 2, all

students were presented with an unexpected psychobiology ‘‘pop’’ quiz.

They were told that although this quiz would not count toward their final

grade, it would be an indication of how much they knew in preparation for a

test in 3 weeks’ time. Prior to completing the final memory test, participants

judged the grade they thought they might receive (1�‘‘fail’’ to 5�‘‘high

distinction’’), and also indicated the number of hours they had spent

independently studying the topic of psychobiology during the previous week.

924 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 9: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

Results and discussion

All of the analyses conducted were one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs),

unless otherwise specified. The significance level throughout was an alpha of

.05.

Sample characteristics. The uniformity of conditions in terms of their

demographic composition was checked using chi-square analyses, which

indicated that there were no differences between conditions in terms of

gender, x2(3, N�75)�6.298, p�.098, phi�.29, or English/non-English

native language, x2(3, N�75)�2.184, p�.535, phi�.17.

Testing effect. Evidence for the testing effect in this education setting

was determined by responses to ‘‘old’’ items, that is, those previously seen by

group quiz and individual quiz participants in the Phase 2 quiz. The mean

percentage correct (old items), as a function of postvideo condition, is

provided in Figure 1.

A one-way ANOVA on ‘‘old’’ items confirmed that there was a significant

difference in test performance between groups, F(3, 71)�8.67,

MSE�995.10, pB.05, ph2�.27. Individual quiz participants performed

more accurately on old items than did no-activity participants, t(71)�2.12,

SEM�10.41, pB.05, d�.50. This means that completion of an individual

quiz, with feedback, facilitated more accurate responding for items that were

presented on the Phase 2 quiz. There was, however, no difference between

individual quiz and restudy conditions on old items, t(71)�1.63,

SEM�10.82, p�.11. Thus, the strict testing effect criterion, which allows

a conclusion that the testing process produced gains over simply revisiting

the material, was not met.

Figure 1. Mean percentage of ‘‘old’’ items correct on the Phase 3 test as a function of postvideo

condition for Study 1. The error bars indicate the standard error of the mean.

THE TESTING EFFECT 925

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 10: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

Collaborative testing. Group quiz scores on the Phase 2 quiz (M�94%,

SD�5.48%) were higher than the individual quiz scores (M�47.4%,

SD�20.83%), F(1, 24)�23.93, MSE�366.71, ph2�.50. This means that

collaborative input during the testing process resulted in greater accuracy.

This is likely to be due to the benefits of pooled information.

Consistent with the expectation for the benefits of collaborative testing,

those who completed the group quiz performed better than those who had

completed the individual quiz on old items in Phase 3, t(71)�2.29,

SEM�10.29, pB.05, d�.54. This suggests that a collaborative testing

effect was maintained following a 1 week interval and reflects the impact of

good performance in Phase 2 on the strength of the testing effect.

New items. An exploratory one-way ANOVA on new items revealed that

there were significant differences in test performance between groups, F(3,

71)�6.39, MSE�381.94, pB.05, ph2�.21. The mean percentage correct

on the new items, as a function of postvideo condition, is presented in Figure

2. It appears that the group quiz condition showed facilitated performance

compared to all other conditions. No differences were observed between the

individual quiz and restudy conditions, t(71)�0.25, SEM�6.70, p�.80, or

between the individual quiz and No-activity conditions, t(71)��0.89,

SEM�6.45, p�.38. That is, completion of the individual quiz did not

lead to a significant enhancement of or detriment to memory for the new

items. However, group quiz participants scored higher on new items than

individual quiz participants, t(71)�3.57, SEM�6.38, pB.05, d�.85.

Overall, the group quiz participants demonstrated superior test perfor-

mance on new items compared to the other three groups (individual quiz,

restudy, and no-activity), which provides strong evidence of a facilitation

effect resulting from the collaborative testing procedure. This is likely to be

Figure 2. Mean percentage of ‘‘new’’ items correct on the Phase 3 test as a function of postvideo

condition for Study 1. The error bars indicate the standard error of the mean.

926 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 11: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

the result of broader engagement with the video information as a result of

group discussion in Phase 2.

Alternative explanations. The possibility of preexisting academic differ-

ences (such as superior general test performance or superior general

comprehension of the psychobiology material) between the groups, or of

some kind of tutor effect, was checked by comparing performance on the

unrelated lecture-based questions. No differences were observed amongst the

four conditions (no-activity, M�43.3%, SD�28.8%; restudy, M�47.1%,

SD�35.5%; individual quiz, M�47.1%, SD�35.5%; group quiz,

M�49.2%, SD�22.7%), F(3, 71)�0.13, MSE�933.86, p�.94. The

possibility of differences between groups on reported hours spent studying

between Phases 2 and 3 was examined, and no significant differences existed

amongst the conditions (no-activity, M�1.45, SD�1.61; restudy, M�3.24,

SD�3.42; individual, M�1.41, SD�1.57; group, M�1.93, SD�2.69),

F(3, 71)�2.14, MSE�5.92, p�.10.

An additional analysis assessed the potential benefit of the ‘‘study’’

engaged in by the restudy participants. Old and new items were combined in

this analysis since neither restudy nor no-activity conditions had been

previously exposed to the quiz questions. It was found that there was no

difference in accuracy in the Phase 3 test between participants who

highlighted the video material (M�44.1%, SD�12.7%) and the no-activity

group (M�48%, SD�19.6%), t(35)��0.70, SEM�5.54, p�.49. Thus, the

short session of highlighting produced no gains in memory that were

maintained over the 1 week interval.

In summary, Study 1 demonstrated the testing effect in a real educational

setting: that is, students who were tested in the initial phase performed better

on ‘‘old’’ items in the final test than did those who did not spend any more

time on the material. The effect, however, did not generalise to related ‘‘new’’

questions. The benefits of collaborative testing were demonstrated. Colla-

borative testing enhanced accuracy during an initial test and improved

retention for both ‘‘old’’ and ‘‘new’’ questions on a final test; these effects

are considered further in the General Discussion. It is unclear, however, why

the individual testing condition did not yield better performance than for the

restudy condition. Only one other study (McDaniel, Anderson, Derbish, &

Morrisette, 2007), using a somewhat different methodology, has demon-

strated a strict-criterion testing effect in a real educational setting. Is it

possible that there are significant limitations to the generalisation of the

testing effect from the laboratory to the applied setting? If so, then we need

to acknowledge the limited practical value of such laboratory work, or

determine the conditions under which findings reliably generalise to the

classroom setting.

THE TESTING EFFECT 927

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 12: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

Indeed, the lack of replication of the strong testing effect to the

educational setting in this study may be related to a number of methodo-

logical factors. First, there were very few ‘‘old’’ items, and so the measure of

memory on the final test was limited in its range. Second, the ‘‘old’’ and

‘‘new’’ questions were not matched for difficulty, and it would appear from a

comparison within the no-activity condition that the ‘‘new’’ questions were

easier than the ‘‘old’’ questions, t(19)�6.31, SEM�5.20, pB.05. Third,

some of the ‘‘new’’ questions tested similar concepts or information to some

of the items in Phase 2. Fourth, there simply may not have been enough

participants in each of the conditions to provide the power to detect what

appears to be the superior performance of the individual quiz students

compared to the restudy students (see Figure 1). Given the generalisabilty

considerations discussed earlier, it was thought important to address these

methodological limitations in Study 2 through careful selection and

matching of test items. A related important educational issue that was not

intentionally and directly tested in Study 1 is the opposing evidence in the

literature that taking an initial test may either enhance memory for new,

related material, termed ‘‘retrieval-induced facilitation’’ (Chan, McDermott,

& Roediger, 2006), or, conversely, inhibit later memory for new, related

material, termed ‘‘retrieval-induced forgetting’’ (Anderson, 2003). Study 2

addressed this issue within the context of the further exploration of the test

effect with the individual condition.

STUDY 2

A retrieval induced facilitation effect is based on associative memory

theories, such that activation of one concept produces facilitative effects

for related concepts. For example, when a semantic node is activated, it also

activates semantically related nodes through an associative network, a

process called spreading activation (Collins & Quillian, 1972). This theory

has also been applied to episodic memory (Roediger & McDermott, 1995).

The adaptive control of thought�rational (ACT-R) and search of associative

memory (SAM) theories have also been used to explain the underlying

effects of retrieval induced facilitation. Both theories suggest that retrieval of

a particular subset of studied material facilitates retrieval of the unstudied

material when associative links exist between them (Anderson, 1996;

Raaijmakers, 2003). Moreover, the stronger the associative links are between

the two sets of material, the higher the probability of retrieval of the

unstudied material. These theories, therefore, postulate that because the

associative links between the tested and related materials are strengthened

through repeated testing sessions, the retrieval of related material in a final

memory test will be more likely than that of unrelated material.

928 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 13: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

Two studies by Chan et al. (2006) demonstrated that initial testing

enhanced later memory for untested, related material. In Study 1,participants were allocated to one of three groups based on three different

review conditions: test, restudy, and no-activity. In the review phase, the test

group completed two tests on a subset of a target prose article while the

restudy condition reread a subset of the target article, twice. The no-activity

group did not receive any additional exposure to the material. The final

memory test on the target article was administered 24 hours later. Consistent

with the strict criterion, the test group had significantly better recall for the

untested, related items on the final test than both the restudy and the no-activity groups.

In Chan et al.’s (2006) Study 2, participants were exposed to two sets of

prose material (Stories A and B) in the initial learning stage in a within

subjects design. This design did not allow for a test of the strict criterion. All

participants received a test on a subset (A1) of one of the original stories as

practice for the final memory test. A retrieval-induced facilitation effect was

obtained such that performance on the related material (A2) was signifi-

cantly better than that on the unrelated material (B). This provided aconceptual replication of their Study 1; testing significantly increased

performance on a later memory test compared to no extra study. Hence,

the testing effect was obtained in both experiments despite participants

receiving no feedback about their performance.

In contrast to retrieval induced facilitation, retrieval induced forgetting is

a phenomenon whereby remembering information can cause impairment in

the ability to remember unpractised related information (Carroll, Campbell-

Ratcliffe, Murnane, & Perfect, 2007). It is based upon the notion that whentwo or more items of information are to be retrieved, we unconsciously

forget the information considered irrelevant, in the process of retrieving the

desired information (Anderson et al., 1994). This theory is consistent with

the retrieval induced facilitation hypothesis that for the repeatedly retrieved

target material (A1), the retrieval process itself is strengthened, leading to

better ‘‘final’’ recall. However, during retrieval there is competition for

activation so that semantically related material (A2) is unconsciously

inhibited in order for the target material (A1) to be better recalled.Therefore, the more closely the material is related, the more likely a

forgetting effect is to occur for the related material. Carroll et al. (2007),

however, found a retrieval-induced forgetting effect only if the final test was

immediately, rather than 24 hours after, the review session. In summary, the

retrieval induced facilitation and forgetting effects appear to operate as

opposing influences on learning through repeated retrieval of information.

The initial objective of Study 2 was to improve aspects of the Study 1

methodology and test again whether the individual test condition wouldproduce better final memory test performance than the restudy condition,

THE TESTING EFFECT 929

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 14: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

that is, the strict testing effect criterion not met in Study 1. A second

objective of Study 2 was to determine whether recall for nontested related

material was facilitated or inhibited in a real educational setting. Three study

conditions were utilised: quiz (individual), restudy, and no-activity. Two sets

of questions were developed to test discrete but related concepts from the

video material. One set was utilised during Phase 2 (target material), while

both sets (target, related) were administered during Phase 3. It was predicted

that the testing effect would be demonstrated for the target material such

that the quiz group would perform better than the control group (lenient

criterion). It was also expected that the quiz group would outperform the

restudy group (strict criterion). Given that the final test was 1 week after the

review session, it was expected that a retrieval induced facilitation rather

than forgetting effect would be found for the related material; specifically,

the quiz group would perform better than the restudy and no-activity

groups.

Method

Participants and design

Two hundred and eighteen first year psychology students (126 femalesand 92 males), enrolled in the Psychology 1B classes at the University of

New South Wales participated in this experiment. The mean age was 19.9

years (range�17�51). Data was analysed only for those students present for

both sessions of the study.As in Study 1, there were three experimental phases: initial learning,

review, and the final memory test. The first two phases were presented

consecutively in the one tutorial, and the final phase was presented 1 week

later in the next tutorial. In a quasiexperimental approach, tutorials were

randomly allocated to one of three conditions: quiz, restudy, and no-activity

conditions. Data was analysed from the first six (two per condition) tutorials

of the week where there were no procedural issues such as equipment

malfunction. These occurred from Monday to Wednesday. The independent

variable was the type of review method, which was operationalised by the

presentation of the practice quiz, summary highlight sheet or nothing,

during the review phase. The dependent variable was memory recall, which

was measured in the final memory test.

Materials

The video learning material was identical to that presented in Study 1.

All quizzes and summaries were presented on A4 sheets of paper as

handouts. Two practice quizzes (I, II) were created for this experiment and

930 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 15: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

each consisted of 10 cued recall item questions. During Phase 2 half of the

quiz participants received Quiz I and half of them received Quiz II*this

was the target material (A1). During Phase 3 all of the Quiz I participants

received both the Quiz I questions (target A1) and the Quiz II questions

(related A2). Similarly, two summary sets, which consisted of 10 summary

points that were the completed items of the cued recall items, were also

created. The final memory test (pop quiz) consisted of all the practice quiz

questions (target and related), presented in item-by-item alternating order,

and there were two counterbalanced orders. On the first page of the pop

quiz students were asked to (a) indicate how many hours of psychobiology

study they had undertaken in the past week and (b) make a percentage

prediction of their own performance on the subsequent pop quiz (data not

reported here).

Procedure

The study was integrated into the normal activities of the first year

psychology tutorials; therefore, the time and place for each of the phases was

the same throughout the experiment.

Phase 1: Initial learning. Participants were presented with the video

segment in the first part of the tutorial. As in Study 1, they were instructed

not to take notes during the video, as a transcript would be available later

from the course website.

Phase 2: Review. Immediately after the video, participants received their

specific review task, depending upon which tutorial group they were in. For

the quiz group, one of the two practice tests (I, II; counterbalanced for each

condition) was given to be completed within 8 min, in examination

conditions. After the time had elapsed, the papers were collected and

redistributed to another student in the same class for marking. Correct

answers were briefly presented on an overhead projector by the tutor.

Students marked the paper out of 12 (two questions required two answers

each) before handing back the paper to the tutor. This process took

approximately 5 min. If participants wanted to know their specific mark,

they were instructed to ask the tutor at the end of the class. For the restudy

group, participants were administered a summary set to be read for 8 min.

Participants were instructed to highlight or underline what they considered

the key points of each summary sentence, and that no additional writing was

to be made. There was then a 2-min period where students were invited to

ask any questions about the material. At the end of this phase of the

experiment, their summary sets were handed back to the tutor. The no-

activity group was given no task for this phase.

THE TESTING EFFECT 931

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 16: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

Phase 3: Memory test. During the following week’s tutorial, partici-

pants were given a pop quiz, of 8 min duration, under examinationconditions. Participants were unaware that this quiz was to be administered,

to reduce the chances of additional external study of the material. When the

time had elapsed, participants handed in their papers which were then

redistributed across the class for marking. The answers were briefly

presented on the overhead projector by the tutor. A final mark out of 24

was calculated by the student marker before the papers were collected by the

tutor.

Results and discussion

A significance level of .05 was used for all statistical analyses. Pairwise

comparisons were Bonferroni-corrected to the .05 level.

Sample characteristics and manipulation check. The uniformity of

conditions in terms of their demographic composition was checked using

chi-square analyses, which indicated that there were no differences betweenconditions in terms of gender, x2(2, N�218)�218, p�.329, phi�.101, or

English/non-English native language, x2(2, N�212)�3.351, p�.187,

phi�.126.

As two different sets of quiz/restudy items were used in this experiment

for the target and related materials, quiz difficulty levels could have varied.

To determine if this was the case, an initial omnibus 3 (group: test, highlight,

control)�(2) (test material: target, related)�2 (quiz type: I vs. II) mixed

ANOVA was conducted. There was a significant Test material�Quiz typeinteraction, F(1, 212)�10.94, MSE�138.35, pB.05, ph2�.05, and there-

fore subsequent analyses included quiz type as a factor.

Testing effect. It was hypothesised that the quiz group would perform

better than both the restudy and no-activity groups on the target material in

the pop quiz. Figure 3 suggests that as expected, for the target material, the

quiz group performed better than both the no-activity and restudy groups,

and there was little difference between the latter two groups. A 3 (group)�2(quiz type: I, II) ANOVA indicated a main effect for group,

F(2, 212)�50.79, MSE�449.89, pB.05, ph2�.32. Pairwise comparisons

confirmed that the quiz group performed better than did both the no-

activity group, t(215)�9.72, SEM�3.48, pB.05, d�1.33, and the restudy

group, t(215)�7.00, SEM�3.45, pB.05, d�.95. The restudy group also

performed better than the no-activity group, t(215)�2.69, SEM�3.59,

pB.05, d�.37. There were no significant main or interaction effects for quiz

type. In summary, with the increased power afforded by more participants in

932 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 17: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

each group, there was evidence for the individual testing effect according to

both the lenient and strict criteria.

Retrieval induced facilitation versus retrieval induced forgetting. In line

with the findings of Chan et al. (2006) a retrieval induced facilitation effect

would be evident if performance on the related material is significantly better

for the quiz group than both the restudy and no-activity groups with no

difference between the latter two groups. Figure 3 suggests that, as expected

for the related material, the quiz group performed better than the no-activity

and restudy groups, with little difference between the latter two. A 3

(group)�2 (quiz type: I, II) ANOVA on the related material indicated a

main effect for group, F(2, 212)�9.40, MSE�374.80, pB.05, ph2�.08.

Pairwise comparisons confirmed that the quiz group performed better than

did the no-activity group, t(215)�3.72, SEM�3.23, pB.05, d�.51, and

restudy group, t(215)�3.56, SEM�3.21, pB.05, d�.49, and there was no

significant difference between the latter two groups. There was a main effect

of quiz type, F(1, 212)�4.89, MSE�374.80, pB.05, ph2�.02 indicating

that Quiz I performance was superior for related items. There was no

significant interaction. These consistent patterns of findings suggest that the

nonrandom assignment of participants to tutorial (and tutor) did not

confound the results.

Alternative explanations. As for Study 1, it was possible that partici-pants undertook additional study between Phases 2 and 3, which may have

influenced final memory performance. A one-way ANOVA yielded no group

differences (quiz M�2.02, SD�2.67; restudy M�2.61, SD�3.71; no-

activity M�2.63, SD�4.63). Nevertheless, subsequent analyses of percen-

tage items correct on the final test were also run with additional study hours

Figure 3. Mean percentage of target and related items correct on the Phase 3 test as a function of

postvideo condition for Study 2. The error bars indicate the standard error of the mean.

THE TESTING EFFECT 933

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 18: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

as a covariate. All analyses yielded the same findings with or without the

covariate included, so the covariate analyses are not reported here.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Study 1 demonstrated the testing effect in a classroom setting in that

individual quiz students performed better on the final test than did the no-

activity group, who did not complete a Phase 2 quiz. However, the strict

criterion for the testing effect was not met as the individual quiz students did

not perform better than the restudy students. Given the importance of

determining the potential limitations of generalisability of the test effect,

Study 2 was designed to further investigate this issue by improving aspects of

the methodology. In Study 1, the testing effect did not generalise to new

(previously unseen) test items in that the individual quiz students did not

perform better than the restudy or the no-activity students. Group quiz

scores were superior to the individual quiz scores for both old and new items,

as well as for the Phase 2 quiz, thus demonstrating a significant collaborative

testing benefit.

Study 2 yielded strong evidence for the testing effect in the classroom

setting in that individual quiz students performed better than both the

restudy and no-activity students on the target (previously tested) items in the

final test, suggesting that the lack of a strict criterion testing effect in Study 1

was related to lack of power. Moreover, the retrieval induced facilitation

hypothesis was supported. Memory for related (untested) material was

significantly enhanced as a result of testing (quiz) but not following simple

rereading of the material (restudy). This finding contrasts with the Study 1

result of no difference in performance on new items for restudy and

individual quiz groups. In Study 1, old (conceptually equivalent to target)

and new (conceptually equivalent to related) quiz items were not counter-

balanced, and new items were easier to answer than old items. Further

research is required to investigate whether item difficulty interacts with

facilitation of memory performance with related (untested) material. The

memory performance of the restudy group did not differ significantly from

the no-activity group, who did not engage in any revision of the video

material.

The current research demonstrated the benefits of testing on later

memory retention of both target and related material that was presented

in an authentic tertiary educational setting. Additional hours of study

between review and the final test did not influence the results in either study,

suggesting that the findings are consistent with ‘‘direct’’ theoretical accounts

of the testing effect such as the retrieval hypothesis. This theory states that

repeated elaborative retrieval of the target material (e.g., testing) produces

934 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 19: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

better retention. The proposed mechanism is that the effortful retrieval

processes used at the time of encoding increase the strength of a memory

trace and increase the number of available retrieval routes (Anderson et al.,

1994; Butler & Roediger, 2007; Roediger & Karpicke, 2006a). Therefore, it is

predicted that the more effortful or elaborative retrieval processes that are

engaged in during the review phase, the better the information will be

remembered at a later time. A test administered during the review phase is a

more effortful form of learning than rereading (e.g., passive rereading or

highlighting of the information). As such, the elaborative processes used in a

practice test increase later memory performance (Roediger & Karpicke,

2006a).Study 2 yielded results in favour of the retrieval induced facilitation

hypothesis. These findings suggest that as a direct result of testing the target

material, thematically related information retention is also enhanced after a

delay between the retrieval practice and final retrieval phases (Chan et al.,

2006). The retrieval induced facilitation effect obtained in Study 2 is in direct

opposition to the retrieval induced forgetting effects obtained by other

researchers (e.g., Anderson et al., 1994; Carroll et al., 2007; Macrae &

MacLeod, 1999). For example, the current findings diverge from those of

Carroll et al. (2007), who obtained a forgetting effect using similar prose

materials in two studies. There was, however, an important difference

between Study 2 in the current research and Carroll et al.’s studies: the

length of the delay between the review phase and the final test. This delay is

one of several boundary conditions influencing the likelihood of retrieval

induced forgetting. Other studies support the postulate that retrieval induced

forgetting appears to decay relatively quickly such that it is eliminated at

longer delays (e.g., 24 hours; MacLeod & Macrae, 2001).Theoretically this suggests that practice tests not only significantly

increase learning of the tested information, but memory for related

information is also increased when practice tests are taken a week before

the final exam. It appears that in relation to the testing effect, elaborative

retrieval processes activate schemas and semantic processes for related

information as well as for the target information. Therefore, the related

information does not appear to compete for activation, as previous retrieval

induced forgetting research suggests (Anderson et al., 1994; Carroll et al.,

2007). In contrast, a spreading of the activation of the semantic nodes and

processes appears to yield a retrieval induced facilitation effect over a longer

interval (Chan et al., 2006; Collins & Quillian, 1972). Since Study 2 included

a 1 week delay between Phase 2 and the final test, the relative influence of

retrieval induced facilitation and retrieval induced forgetting could not be

examined in the ‘‘immediate’’ time period. Future research should incorpo-

rate an immediate condition in a real world educational setting to examine

THE TESTING EFFECT 935

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 20: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

any differences in performance on the final memory test as a function of

time between the retrieval practice and final recall phases.The current research represents an extension of the work of Chan et al.

(2006) because the testing effect and retrieval induced facilitation were

examined in conjunction with corrective feedback. That is, in both studies,

corrective feedback was provided after the practice quiz. This raises the

possibility of different outcomes if no feedback is provided. Previous studies

on the role of feedback suggest that it can influence the testing effect both

directly and indirectly (Black & Wiliam, 1998). One possible indirect effect

of feedback is that participants may undertake additional study because theydid not perform optimally on the practice test, and want to improve (Fitch et

al., 1951; Roediger & Karpicke, 2006a). This, however, does not appear to be

the case in the current research because hours of (self-reported) additional

study did not differ significantly between the groups. A potential direct effect

is that feedback could function as another retrieval practice opportunity,

thus further enhancing memory retention (Black & Wiliam; 1998; Roediger

& Karpicke, 2006a). In contrast, without feedback, incorrect responses

during the initial test may be assumed to be correct by the respondent, andthis effect of misinformation may appear in the final memory test (Roediger

& Marsh, 2005; Rogers, Goldstein, & Benassi, 2006). To explore these

notions further, future research on the testing effect in a classroom setting

should directly compare feedback and no-feedback conditions (Butterfield &

Metcalfe, 2006; Kang, McDermont, & Roediger, 2007).

The current research also explored collaborative testing effects in the

classroom. A previous classroom study by Cortright, Collins, Rodenbaugh,

and DiCarlo (2003) reported a positive collaborative testing effect onsubsequent individual student memory of course material; however, the

collaborative manipulation was confounded with retrieval opportunity (i.e.,

the individual testing comparison group had not received an equivalent

number of test exposures). One way in which to conceptualise the

collaborative testing experience is in terms of more elaborative retrieval

processing afforded by the group interaction, thus leading to the better later

retrieval of both old and new items in Study 1. This notion needs to be tested

more rigorously with better controlled materials and conditions, such asthose employed in Study 2.

An alternative approach is from the social cognition literature, which has

reported the collaborative inhibition effect, whereby collaborative group

performance is worse than nominal group performance (Basden, Basden,

Bryner, & Thomas, 1997; Weldon & Bellinger, 1997; Wright & Klumpp,

2004). Studies by Meudell and colleagues (e.g., Meudell, Hitch, & Boyle,

1995) suggest that the collaborative inhibition effect is reduced if individuals

first respond individually immediately prior to discussing and respondingwithin a group testing situation. These and other issues, such as the inclusion

936 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 21: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

of misinformation (Clark, Abbe, & Larson, 2006; Maki, Weigold, &

Arellano, 2008), deserve further rigorous testing within the classroomsetting. Overall, however, it can be concluded that the individual usually

benefits in the long term from the pooling of information during an interim

collaborative testing episode.

Practical and educational implications

The current research provides evidence that a practice test yields betterperformance on a later test than no additional study (Studies 1 and 2) and

also compared to restudy of the original material (Study 2). Importantly, the

benefits of testing in the review phase generalised to semantically related

(but untested) material in Study 2. In other words, contrary to the retrieval

induced forgetting hypothesis (Anderson, 2003; Anderson et al., 1994;

Carroll et al., 2007), the findings of Study 2 suggest that studying does not

adversely affect memory for the unstudied material when there is a

significant delay between review and the final test. The findings of Carrollet al. (2007) suggest that forgetting is most likely to occur for the related,

unstudied information when the final exam is taken immediately after

retrieval practice. Hence, early, spaced (rather than massed) testing during

review appears most likely to facilitate (rather than inhibit) memory for

related material. Taken together, the findings highlight the efficacy of

repeated testing as a learning tool and not just as a means of assessment (see

also McDaniel et al., 2007). This indicates that regular testing would be

beneficial to learning in undergraduate university courses.The presence of feedback may aid in the learning process, but further

research is needed to disentangle the effects of feedback and the testing

effect. The current findings suggest that formative assessments should be

used as practice tests with immediate corrective feedback in the lead up to a

final summative exam. Moreover, the level of motivation of the students

during a practice quiz may be important for their learning and subsequent

performance on a final test (Velan & Kumar, 2007), such that higher

motivation produces better performance on a final test. The practice tests,therefore, should appear necessary to students’ learning and future final test

performance in order to increase motivation to perform well in the practice

tests and to learn from the feedback provided.

The benefits of collaborative testing compared to individual testing on

later memory retention were evident in Study 1. Collaborative testing

produced higher scores on the practice test, but more importantly, it

produced superior performance on both ‘‘old’’ and ‘‘new’’ items in the final

test. To our knowledge, this research is the first to examine the impact on thetesting effect of collaborative testing. The findings are consistent with

THE TESTING EFFECT 937

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 22: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

previous research that has demonstrated that collaborative learning situa-

tions enhance students’ understanding of and later memory for psycholo-gical concepts (e.g., Miyake & Shirouzu, 2006). In a group situation,

students have the opportunity to collaboratively read, explain, exchange, and

discuss the material. This pooled information may enhance the integration

of the material in memory and assist students to form a cohesive and

abstracted view of the key concepts (Miyake & Shirouzu, 2006). The current

findings highlight that substantial collaborative learning opportunities in

school and university curricula will encourage in-depth, process-oriented

learning. A fruitful avenue for future research will be to examine the

mechanisms underlying the beneficial effects of collaborative learning onlater memory retention.

CONCLUSIONS

The present study found evidence in support of the testing effect in a

classroom setting. The substantial benefits of collaborative learning on later

memory retention were also demonstrated. It appears that as a result of

effortful retrieval processes from a practice test on a target subset of

information, performance on a final memory test is improved not only for

the target subset, but also for semantically related information. Corrective

feedback appears to be important although future research is required to

examine precisely how feedback influences memory retention. Moreresearch is required to further examine the retrieval processes operating

during these different retrieval practice opportunities and how they influence

performance on a subsequent final memory test.

Original manuscript received December 2007

Revised manuscript received May 2008

First published online October 2008

REFERENCES

Anderson, J. R. (1996). ACT: A simple theory of complex cognition. The American Psychologist,

51, 355�365.

Anderson, M. C. (2003). Rethinking interference theory: Executive control and the mechanisms of

forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 415�445.

Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval

dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and

Cognition, 20, 1063�1087.

Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., & Henry, S. (2000). Costs and benefits of collaborative remembering.

Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 497�507.

938 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 23: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., Bryner, S., & Thomas, R. L., III. (1997). A comparison of group and

individual remembering: Does collaboration disrupt retrieval strategies? Journal of Experi-

mental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 1176�1189.

Black, P., & Wiliam, D. (1998). Assessment and classroom learning. Assessment in Education:

Principles, Policy and Practice, 5, 7�74.

Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). Testing improves long-term retention in a simulated

classroom setting. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 514�527.

Butterfield, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2006). The correction of errors committed with high confidence.

Metacognition and Learning, 1, 69�84.

Carroll, M., Campbell-Ratcliffe, J., Murnane, H., & Perfect, T. (2007). Retrieval-induced forgetting

in educational contexts: Monitoring, expertise, text integration, and test format. European

Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 580�606.

Chan, J. C. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2006). Retrieval-induced facilitation:

Initially nontested material can benefit from prior testing of related material. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 553�571.

Clark, S. E., Abbe, A., & Larson, R. P. (2006). Collaboration in associative recognition memory:

Using recalled information to defend ‘‘new’’ judgements. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Learning, Memory and Cognition, 6, 1266�1273.

Cockrell, K. S., Caplow, J. H., & Donaldson, J. F. (2000). A context for learning: Collaborative

groups in the problem-based learning environment. Review of Higher Education, 23, 347�363.

Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1972). Experiments on semantic memory and language

comprehension. In L. W. Gregg (Ed.), Cognition in learning and memory (pp. 117�137). New

York: Wiley.

Cortright, R. N., Collins, H. L., Rodenbaugh, D. W., & DiCarlo, S. E. (2003). Student retention of

course content is improved by collaborative-group testing. Advances in Physiological Education,

27, 102�108.

Dempster, F. N. (1996). Distributing and managing the conditions of encoding and practice. In E.

C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman ( Series Eds.) and E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Vol. Eds.),

Handbook of perception and cognition: Vol. 10. Memory (2nd ed., pp. 317�344). New York:

Academic Press.

Fitch, M. L., Drucker, A. J., & Norton, J. A. (1951). Frequent testing as a motivating factor in large

lecture classes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 42, 1�20.

Glover, J. A. (1989). The ‘‘testing’’ phenomenon: Not gone but nearly forgotten. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 81, 392�399.

Kang, S. H. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). Test format and corrective

feedback modify the effect of testing on long-term retention. European Journal of Cognitive

Psychology, 19, 528�558.

Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). Repeated retrieval during learning is the key to long-

term retention. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 151�162.

Kulhavy, R. W., & Stock, W. A. (1989). Feedback in written instruction: The place of response

certitude. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 279�308.

Kulik, J. A., & Kulik, C. C. (1988). Timing of feedback and verbal learning. Review of Educational

Research, 58, 79�97.

Kuo, T.-M., & Hirshman, E. (1996). Investigations of the testing effect. American Journal of

Psychology, 109, 451�464.

MacLeod, M. D., & Macrae, C. N. (2001). Gone but not forgotten: The transient nature of

retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 12, 148�152.

Macrae, C. N., & MacLeod, M. D. (1999). On recollections lost: When practice makes imperfect.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 463�473.

Maki, R. H., Weigold, A., & Arellano, A. (2008). False memory for associated word lists in

individuals and collaborating groups. Memory and Cognition, 36, 598�603.

THE TESTING EFFECT 939

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2

Page 24: The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced … · 2012-10-29 · The testing effect, collaborative learning, and retrieval-induced facilitation in a classroom

McDaniel, M. A., Anderson, J. L., Derbish, M. H., & Morrisette, N. (2007). Testing the testing

effect in the classroom. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 494�513.

McDaniel, M. A., & Fisher, R. P. (1991). Tests and test feedback as learning sources. Contemporary

Educational Psychology, 16, 192�201.

McDaniel, M. A., Friedman, A., & Bourne, L. E. (1978). Remembering the levels of information in

words. Memory and Cognition, 6, 156�164.

Meltzoff, J. (1998). Critical thinking about research: Psychology and related fields. Washington, DC:

American Psychological Association.

Meudell, P. R., Hitch, G. J., & Boyle, M. M. (1995). Collaboration in recall: Do pairs of people

cross-cue each other to produce new memories? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Human Experimental Psychology, 48A, 141�152.

Miyake, N., & Shirouzu, H. (2006). A collaborative approach to teaching cognitive science to

undergraduates: The learning sciences as a means to study and enhance college student

learning. Psychologia, 49, 101�113.

Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer

appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 16, 519�533.

Raaijmakers, J. G. (2003). Spacing and repetition effects in human memory: Application of the

SAM model. Cognitive Science, 27, 431�452.

Roediger, H. L., III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006a). The power of testing memory: Basic research and

implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181�210.

Roediger, H. L., III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006b). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests

improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17, 249�255.

Roediger, H. L., III, & Marsh, E. J. (2005). The positive and negative consequences of multiple-

choice testing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 31, 1155�1159.

Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K.B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not

presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21,

803�814.

Rogers, R. A. ,Goldstein, G. D., & Benassi, V. A. (2006). Using tests to promote retention of

information acquired while watching a video. Paper presented at the 29th annual conference of

the National Institute on the Teaching of Psychology, St. Petersburg Beach, FL.

Spitzer, H. F. (1939). Studies in retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 30, 641�656.

Thompson, C. P., Wenger, S. K., & Bartling, C. A. (1978). How recall facilitates subsequent recall:

A reappraisal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 210�221.

Velan, G., & Kumar, R. (2007). Integrated online formative assessment in the biomedical sciences.

Paper presented at the International Society for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning,

Sydney, Australia.

Weldon, M. S., & Bellinger, K. D. (1997). Collective memory: Collaborative and individual

processes in remembering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and

Cognition, 23, 1160�1175.

WGBH Boston (Producer), in association with the American Psychological Association. (2001).

Discovering psychology: The behaving brain [Videotape]. Boston: American Psychological

Association.

Wheeler, M. A., & Roediger, H. L., III. (1992). Disparate effects of repeated testing: Reconciling

Ballard’s (1913) and Bartlett’s (1932) results. Psychological Science, 3, 240�245.

Wright, D. B., & Klumpp, A. (2004). Collaborative inhibition is due to the product, not the

process, of recalling in groups. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 1080�1083.

940 CRANNEY ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

UN

SW L

ibra

ry]

at 1

8:28

28

Oct

ober

201

2