101
The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: From Tense to Movement 37-975-01 Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University

The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: From Tense to Movement

  • Upload
    lorne

  • View
    40

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: From Tense to Movement. 37-975-01 Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University. Topics. Passive Binding WH-Questions Relative clauses. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Citation preview

Page 1: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: From Tense to Movement

37-975-01

Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment

Dr. Sharon Armon-LotemBar Ilan University

Page 2: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Topics

Passive Binding WH-Questions Relative clauses

Page 3: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: The Passive

Page 4: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Passive

Maryi was kissed ti by John

Passive is A-movement rather than A’-movement The subject is the patient (no necessary agent) The transitive verb has unique morphology (with or without

an auxiliary verb) which makes it intransitive The passive derives n-place predicate from n+1-place

predicate Not all languages permit an agent-phrase (by phrase), and

the same agent-phase can occur with non-passive verbs Verbal vs. adjectival passive

Page 5: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Verbal vs. adjectival

The girl is covered (by the boy)

The covered girl (*by the boy)

Ha-yalda mexusa (al yedey ha-yeled)

the-girl cover-pass (on hands the-boy)

‘The girl is covered (by the boy)’

Page 6: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Issues in acquisition

Reversible vs. non-reversible Actional vs. non-actional Adjectival vs. verbal Do children understand the by-phrase? Comprehension vs. production

Page 7: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

How do children with SLI interpret the passive?

Children with SLI consistently interpret reversible passive using SVO strategy (Bishop 1982)

Children with SLI show a mixture of correct interpretation and a reversal interpretation (Van der Lely & Harris 1990)

Children with SLI perform better on short passive than on long Passive (Van der Lely 1994)

Children with SLI adopt an adjectival interpretation (Van der Lely 1996)

Page 8: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Van der Lely, H. 1996. Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: Verbal passive vs. adjectival passive interpretation. Lingua, 98, 243–272.

Page 9: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Subjects

Page 10: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Method – Picture selection task

6 verbs: wash, mend, paint, eat, cut, hit

Page 11: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement
Page 12: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Results (p.258)

Reversal

Adjectival

Passive

Page 13: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

p. 259

Page 14: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

p. 260

Page 15: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

D. V. M. Bishop, P. Bright, C. James, S. J. Bishop, and H. K. J. Van der lely. 2000. Grammatical SLI: A distinct subtype of developmental language impairment? Applied Psycholinguistics 21, 159–181

Page 16: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Subjects

Sample A - LI - 46 children out of 37 same-sex twin pairs selected for the presence of language impairment in one or both twins

Sample B - LN- 32 children out of an unselected sample of 104 twin pairs from the general population

All children were 7 - 13.

Page 17: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

TAPS (Van der Lely 1996)

(a) reversible active SVO (e.g., “the man eats the fish”);

(b) reversible full passive (e.g., “the man is eaten by the fish”);

(c) short progressive passive (e.g., “the fish is being eaten”); and

(d) short passive with potentially adjectival passive interpretation (e.g., “the fish is eaten”).

12 items x 4 sentence types = 48 sentences

Page 18: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Results

There was a significant difference between groups: mean correct (out of 48) for group LI = 40.4 (SD = 3.96) and for group LN = 45.3 (SD = 2.29), F(1, 76) = 39.8, p < .001.

Age was not significantly correlated with TAPS performance, r(76) = −.047

Nonverbal ability was significantly correlated with TAPS : r(76) = .420 for Raven’s Matrices and .445 for PIQ (both p < .001

Page 19: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Results by sentence type

*

*

Page 20: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

SLI Children's Delayed Acquisition of Passive

Mabel L. Rice, Kenneth Wexler, & Jennifer Francois

Paper Presented at the BU Conference on Language Development

Boston, MA, November 1-4, 2001

Page 21: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Subjects Study 1

19 10-year-old children 17 age-equivalent controls 16 8-year-old lexically-equivalent controls (PPVT raw

scores)

Study 2 17 5-year-old SLI children 17 age-equivalent controls 16 3-year-old lexically-equivalent controls (PPVT raw

scores)

Page 22: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Method

Stromswold’s 32-item task for reversible full passives, with toy animals.

Examiner: “The goal kicked the horse.”

Child: act out action with toy animals

[Verbal item set: Kiss, slap, touch, hug, kick, lick, tickle, push]

Page 23: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Results - Study 1Passive Comprehension

0

0.15

0.3

0.45

0.6

0.75

0.9

SLI Lexically Matched Age Matched

Per

cen

t C

orre

ct

By 10 years of age, children in the SLI group comprehended reversible full verbal passives, showing knowledge of movement (A-chains)

Page 24: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Results - Study 2Passive Comprehension: Identification of Agent

0

0.15

0.3

0.45

0.6

0.75

0.9

SLI Lexically Matched Age Matched

Per

cen

t C

orre

ct

At 5 years of age, children in the SLI group were below age peers in their comprehension of reversible full verbal passives, and similar to their younger lexically-equivalent peers

Page 25: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The Acquisition of Passive Constructions in Russian Children with SLI

Maria Babyonyshev, Lesley Hart, & Elena Grigorenko. 2005. Paper presented at Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics - The Princeton Meeting

Page 26: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Subjects A medium-sized village (population of

approximately 900) in Arkhangelsk region where the incidence of language disorders is far greater than in the general population.

14 monolingual Russian children aged between 6;3 and 9;10 (mean age 7; 10), non-verbal IQ above 70: seven TD children (mean age 8;3 ) and seven children with SLI (mean age 7;5).

Children were grouped based on: clinical impressions, and either MLU, or syntactic complexity (the proportion of syntactically complex structures to all structures produced)

Page 27: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Method

A picture selection task with reversible passive sentences in the perfective form.

20 passive sentences with pairs of pictures: 10 based on actional verbs (a), 5 based on psychological predicates (b), and 5 based on perception verbs (c).

a. Petux byl oščipan gusem. ‘A rooster was plucked by a goose.’

b. Lisa byla utešena korovoj. ‘A fox was consoled by a cow.’

c. Žiraf byl obnyuxan obez’janoj. ‘A giraffe was smelled by a monkey.’

Page 28: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Results - percentage of success

ActionalPsychologicalPerceptionTotal

TD*77%71%80%76%

SLI71%57%40%56%

* Younger TD do not distinguish the three types of passives, performing at chance level on all of them (see Babyonyshev & Brun 2003).

Page 29: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Is this universal?

Leonard, L. B., Wong, A. M. Y, Deevy, P., Stokes, S. F., and P. Fletcher .2006. The production of passives by children with specific language impairment: Acquiring English or Cantonese. Applied Psycholinguistics 27, 267–299

English – movement, one-to-many often reduced morpheme, adjectival/verbal confusion,

Cantonese – movement, no morphology, bei with a contrastive tone which is unique to passive

Page 30: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement
Page 31: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

English

Cantonese

Page 32: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement
Page 33: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

“The findings necessitate a modification of the assumptions of the sparse morphology hypothesis, and provide only partial support for the surface account. The English get-passives and the Cantonese passives employed in this study differ in their structure but both require some type of movement. However,we found no evidence that movement was at the heart of the children’s difficulties. If optional movement is a correct characterization, then we must assume that our tasks increased the likelihood that an available but optional movement operation was selected by the children with SLI."

Page 34: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: Binding

Page 35: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Johni shaved himselfi

1. John likes himself2. John likes him3. He likes John4. *Himself likes John

5. John thinks that Bill likes him6. He thinks that Bill likes John7. John thinks that Bill likes himself

Page 36: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Binding conditions

A: anaphors must be bound in their local domain

B: pronouns must be free in their local domain

C: R-expressions are always free

The coindexation resembles A-movement, but no theta role transmission is involved

The binding local domain varies across languages

Page 37: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Issues in acquisition

Which words are pronouns and which are reflexives.

What the local domain is. Principle A vs. principle B. Comprehension vs. production

Page 38: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Solan (1987 – (Act-out task, 37 children, ages 4-7.

Sentences % correct 95%

49%

82%

1. The dog said that the horse hit himself

2. The dog said that the horse hit him

3. The dog told the horse to hit himself

4. The dog told the horse to hit him 36%

85% 5. The dog found the horse’s picture of himself

6. The dog found the horse’s picture of him 1%

86%

38%

68%

7. The dog said that the horse found the picture of himself

8. The dog said that the horse found the picture of him

9. The dog told the horse to find the picture of himself

10. The dog told the horse to find the picture of him

23%

Page 39: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Chien & Wexler (1990) – Pictures selection, 150 children, ages 2;6-6;6

This is Goldilocks; this is Mama bear.

Is Mama bear touching herself/her?

Children older than 5 obey principle A. Younger children allow non-local antecedent: Goldilocks = herself

Children seem to violate principle B even after 6;6

Page 40: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

But Children obey principle B at the same age that

they obey principle A, but violate a pragmatic principle which governs the choice of reference (Reinhart 1983, 1986).

Coreference is possible without coindexing on a pragmatic basis (contrastive stress). Children who are not sensitive to contrastive stress would seem to violate principle B ( McDaniel 1992)

Grice’s principles of cooperation (maxim of manner) – use the most precise way to say what you want to say - use him only when you do not mean himself. This is hard for children (Grodzinsky & Reinhart 1993)

Page 41: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Binding in SLI

Franks, S. L., Connell, P. J. 1996. Knowledge of Binding in Normal and SLI Children. Journal of Child Language, 23, 431-64

Reflexives NL - pass through a long-distance binding stage LI - behave like very young NL requiring the

nearest available noun phrase to be the antecedent.

Page 42: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Bishop et al. 2000. Grammatical SLI: A distinct subtype of developmental language impairment? Applied Psycholinguistics 21, 159–181

Advanced Syntactic Test of Pronominal Reference (Figure 2, A)

Page 43: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

ResultsLI 18.72 (SD=2.90)LN 21.41 (SD=2.53)(t = 5.61, p < .001).

“Baloo Bear says Mowgli is tickling him” “Baloo Bear says Mowgli is tickling himself” (X) “Mowgli says Baloo Bear is tickling him” (S)

Page 44: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement
Page 45: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: WH-Questions

Page 46: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Questions in English Yes/no questions are marked only by subject-auxiliary

inversion, i.e., an overt syntactic change in word order in which the auxiliary is raised into C. Do-support operates when there is no auxiliary is the declarative.

[Spec, CP] is the target for overt Wh-movement both in matrix and embedded clauses, with subject-auxiliary inversion in matrix clauses, but not in embedded clause. Do-support operates when there is no auxiliary is the declarative. a. What did the child see?

b. The teacher wondered what the child saw.

Page 47: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

TD Acquisition

Phase I Children use neither modals nor auxiliaries Yes/no questions are marked only with rising intonation Wh-word appears sentence initially in wh-questions

without inversion. A limited set, ‘what,’ ‘where’ and ‘why,’ ( ‘where NP go?,’

‘what NP doing?’) Children do not seem to understand wh-questions and

their responses are often inappropriate (Radford 1990)

Page 48: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Phase II Auxiliary verbs are used in subject auxiliary

inversion for yes/no questions Auxiliary verbs are not used for wh-questions. Wh-questions involve productive use of an

extended set of wh-words, but no inversion.

Phase III Children make adult use of question formation,

which involves subject-auxiliary inversion.

Page 49: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

What determines the order in which questions are acquired?

Wh in-situ hypothesis (WISH) – universally wh in-situ with no overt movement is allowed by UG. Subject questions can be interpreted as in-situ, while objects require movement.

Vacuous movement hypothesis (VMH) – the wh-parameter can be either + or – movement, but we should not have both options within one language. In English all questions involve movement, only it is invisible for subjects

Proper government hypothesis (PGH) - traces (of movement) must be properly governed. Object traces are theta-governed by the verb, while subject (and adjunct) traces must be antecedent governed (cf. complements are obligatory, everything else is optional).

Page 50: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Predictions:

WISH – subject questions first

VMH – subject and object questions at the same time

PGH – object questions first

Page 51: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Stromswold, K. 1995. The acquisition of subject and object wh-questions.

Longitudinal study of 12 children in CHILDES.

Who and what are acquired almost simultaneously, around age 2;5. Object questions are acquired at the same age or earlier than subject questions.

All children asked at least one long distance object question (mean age 2;10), but only one child asked a long distance subject question (at 5;0).

Page 52: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

By the age of 2;6

TD children use wh-movement properly

TD children do not show problem with wh-non-local dependency

TD children have no problem with theta-government

Page 53: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Elicited ProductionThe nurse feeds someone. Burney knows who. Ask Burney.

Page 54: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Comprehension: Picture selection task

Michal Cohen

Subject vs. Object

Who is pushimng the girl?

Who is the girl pushing?

Page 55: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Wh-movement in children with grammatical SLI: A test of the RDDR hypothesis

Van der Lely HKJ and Battell J (2003), Language 79: 153-181

SLI subjects fail to master the syntax of the two types of movement operation involved in wh-questions (preposing a wh-expression and preposing an auxiliary).

This is the result of difficulties they have in processing non-local dependencies.

Page 56: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Subjects

15 SLI subjects aged from 11;3 to 18;2 12 TD (typically developing) grammar-

matched children aged from 5;3 to 7;4 12 TD (typically developing) vocabulary-

matched children aged from 7;4 to 9;1

Page 57: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Method

Wh-questions containing who, what and which by getting the subjects to play a version of the board game Cluedo:

Prompt Target response Mrs Peacock saw someone in the lounge. Ask me who Who did Mrs Peacock see in the lounge? Mrs Brown placed something in the library. Ask me what What did Mrs Brown place in the library? Professor Plum wore a coat. Ask me which one Which coat did Professor Plum wear?

Page 58: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Findings

Types of error produced by the subjects SLI subjects Younger TD controls Older TD controls No errors 0/15 (0%) 6/12 (50%) 12/12 (100%) AUX errors only 3/15 (20%) 4/12(33%) 0/12 (0%) WH errors only 0/15 (0%) 1/12 (8%) 0/12 (0%) WH and AUX errors 12/15 (80%) 1/12 (8%) 0/12 (0%)

Page 59: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Wh-errors(a) Who Miss Scarlett saw somebody? (Response to ‘Miss

Scarlet saw someone in the lounge. Ask me who’ – the target response being Who did Miss Scarlet see in the lounge?)

(b) Which Reverend Green open a door? (Response to ‘Reverend Green opened a door. Ask me which one’ – the target response being Which door did Rev. Green open?).

(c) What did Colonel Mustard had something in his pocket? (Response to ‘Something was in Colonel Mustard’s pocket. Ask me what’ – the target response being What was in Colonel Mustard’s pocket?).

Page 60: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Summary of findings

SLI subjects have far more problems with the syntax of wh-questions than language-matched TD controls.

The pattern of errors made by the SLI subjects differs from the pattern of errors made by the TD subjects: Most SLI subjects have problems with both auxiliaries

and wh-expressions Most TD subjects have problems with neither, or only

with auxiliary inversion.

Page 61: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Can it account for auxiliary inversion errors?

1. What cat Mrs White stroked?

2. What did they drank?

3. Who Mrs Brown see?

Page 62: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Wh-Errors in Leonard Corpus

1 Which one I can do? (C ‘Which one can I do?) 2. What Kent’s gonna play with? (C ‘What’s Kent gonna play with?)3. How you knowed? (E ‘How did you know?’) 4. What he did? (F ‘What did he do?’) 5. What you doing? (E ‘What are you doing?’) 6. What this for? (G ‘What is this for?’) 7. How much we got to do? (J ‘How much have we got to do?’) 8. How you get this out? (A ‘How d’you get this out?’) 9. What this do? (A ‘What’s this do?/What does this do’) 10. How open it up? (B ‘How d’you open it up?’) 11. What say? (B ‘What d’you say?’) 12. Where go on? (B ‘Where’s it go on/Where does it go on?’) 13. How much long gonna be? (A ‘How much longer’s it gonna be?’) 14. These do? (C ‘What do these do?’) 15. What is this is? (H ‘What is this?’)

Page 63: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The Uninterpretable Feature Deficit Model (Tsimpli and Stavrakaki 1991) SLI children have problems with movement operations,

because these are driven by uninterpretable features. Chomsky (2006) argues that wh-movement is driven by an

interpretable edge feature on C which (in an interrogative clause) attracts an interrogative wh-expression to move to the edge of CP

Pesetsky and Torrego (2001) argue that auxiliary inversion is driven by an uninterpretable tense feature on C which attracts a tensed auxiliary to move from T into C.

Can UFDM account for why the SLI children in the Leonard corpus show perfect performance on wh-movement but perform much more poorly on auxiliary inversion.

Page 64: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Controlled naturalistic sample (Michal Cohen 2008) A twenty centimeter square box is presented

which contains different objects: The investigator tells the child that there is a

surprise in the box. If the child wants to open the box, she has to find out what is in the box by asking questions.

Once a relevant question regarding the content of the box is asked, she received one object from the box.

Page 65: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Other topics: From Singleton to Exhaustive: the Acquisition of Wh-

Roeper, T., Schulz, P., Pearson, B. Z. & Reckling, I. (2006).   From singleton to exhaustive: The acquisition of wh-. Proceedings of SULA 2005 Conference (Semantics of Understudied Languages), Buffalo NY.

Page 66: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Who is eating what?

Double wh-question - Paired answer

Page 67: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Who is wearing a hat ?

Exhaustive answer, singleton answer, plural answer

Page 68: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The [+variable] Feature

Necessary in order to recognize exhaustivity

Specificity: relating to pre-established elements in the discourse +Specific = - variable = singleton, -Specific = +variable = exhaustive/paired.

Child’s initial default assumption: Questions are specific in nature

Page 69: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Results

All children pass through a singleton stage around age 4-5.

Singleton readings in four-year-olds: English 79%, German 52%

Exhaustive responses Age 5: German 80%, English 27% Age 6: German 85%, English 75% Age 7: German 84%, English 74%

Plural responses: 6%

Page 70: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:Relative Clauses

Page 71: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Types of complex clauses

Complement clauses – I want to drink, I know that she is late

Coordinate clauses – I like juice and she likes water

Adverbial clauses – I went to sleep when we got home

Relative clauses – The man who Mary saw was funny

Page 72: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Relative clauses

The girli that John kissed ti is nice

Relative clauses involve an A'-movement which yields coindexation of an NP in the main clause with a gap in the embedded clause, through an operator.

The operator carries the theta-role of its trace/gap

subject vs. object

Page 73: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Some languages have resumptive pronouns in RCs

ha-yalda she dani nishek ota nexmada

the-girl that Dani kissed her nice

'The girl that Dani kissed is nice'

Page 74: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Issues in acquisition

Production vs. comprehension Resumptive NPs Subject vs. object

Page 75: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Types of relative clauses Subject RC

The man who _ reads the book is my friend I saw the man who _ read my book האיש ש_קרא את הספר הוא ידידיפגשתי את האיש ש_קרא את הספר

Object RC The man who David saw _ is my friend I met the man who David saw _ האיש שדויד ראה _ הוא ידידיפגשתי את האיש שדויד ראה _

Page 76: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Resumptive pronouns

) קרא את הספר הוא ידידיהואהאיש ש(* ) הוא ידידיאותוהאיש שפגשתי ( מתנה הוא ידידילוהאיש שנתתי The man who I gave a present to (*him) is

my friend הוא ידידילידוהאיש שישבתי The man who I sat next to (*him) is my

friend

Page 77: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Pied piping

נתתי _ מתנה הוא ידידישלוהאיש The man to whom I gave a present _ is my

friend

ישבתי _ הוא ידידישלידוהאיש The man next to whom I sat _ is my friend

Page 78: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The head external analysis (Chomsky 1977, Jackendoff 1977, Partee 1975) The man [CP whoi [C 0 ] [IP Mary loves ti]] is my friend The man [CP Opi [C that] [IP Mary loves ti]] is my friend The man [CP Opi [C 0 ] [IP Mary loves ti]] is my friend

The head noun is base-generated outside CP The operator undergoes A'-movement to [Spec CP] The relative clause is right adjoined to the head noun The head noun and CP are combined via predication Resumptive pronouns are either base generated (a non-

movement analysis) or traces spell out (a movement analysis).

Page 79: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Comprehension of relative clauses by monolingual TD children (Sheldon 1974) Act-out task

[The dog that __ jumps over the pig] bumps into the lion [SS]

The dog stands on [the horse that the giraffe jumps over __] [OO]

[The lion that the horse bumps into __ ] jumps over the giraffe [SO]

The pig bumps into [the horse that __ jumps over the giraffe] [OS]

Page 80: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Findings

SS & OO are easier than SO & OS

Error in OS: The pig bumps into the horse and __ jumps over the giraffe (49%)

Why?

Page 81: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Nonadult competence (Tavakolian 1981) Linear rather than hierarchical reading Problems: Continuity. Negative Evidence (How do children unlearn

the structure? )

Adult competence + processing complexity (Goodluck & Tavakolian 1982) The errors are the outcome of the complexity of the relative clause 6. d’. The pig bumps into [the horse that __ hops up and down] [OS] Correct in 76%

Adult competence + pragmatic factors (Hamburger & Crain 1982) Felicity conditions - What is said should be appropriate for the goals of

the conversation (Grice 1989) Relative clauses should be used only when there is a choice between

two identical objects. A change in the original experiment: two horses 95% at age 5, 69%

at age 3

Page 82: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Comprehension

Michal Cohen

Show me the boy who is pushing the girl.

Show me the boy who the girl is pushing?

Page 83: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Production of relative clauses by TD children Children produce preconjunctional relative clauses even before the

age of 2:

a. *ze regel koevet laxthis foot-fm hurts-fm you'This is the foot that hurts you' [Lior 1;10;08]

b. *ze shaon ose tuktuk this clock does ticktock'This is a clock that goes ticktock' [Leor 2;1]

The complementizer appears around 2-2;6

aviron she la-shamayim [Lior 2;01;27]airplane that to-the-sky'an airplane that flies to the sky'

Page 84: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Which dog is happy?

Reem Bshara

Page 85: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Resumptive pronouns and resumptive NPs

Children initially use resumptive pronouns in French, English an other languages

Children use resumptive NPs The zebra who the man sat next to the zebra.

The non-movement approach (cf. Labelle 1988, 1990, 1996, Goodluck & Stojanoviç 1996)

The movement approach (cf. Law 1992, Pérez-Leroux 1995, Guasti & Shlonsky 1995, McDaniel, Bernstein & McKee 1997, Varlokosta 1997a).

Page 86: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Resumptives and Wh-Movement in the Acquisition of Relative Clauses in Modern Greek and Hebrew Varlokosta & Armon-Lotem (1998)

Dependency Number +cl/pronoun -cl RNP S 55 *13 (24%) 42 (76%)

DO 44 41 (93%) 2 (5%) *1 (2%) IO 38 38 (100%) PP 40 33 (83%) *3 (7%) *4 (10%)

24 monolingual Hebrew-speaking children from 2;8 to 5;5

Page 87: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Comprehension and Production of relative clauses by children with SLI Stavrakaki, S. 2001. Comprehension of reversible

relative clauses in specifically language impaired and normally developing Greek children. Brain and Language, 77, 419-31.

Novogrodsky, R., & Friedmann, N. 2006. The production of relative clauses in SLI: A window to the nature of the impairment. Advances in Speech-Language pathology, 8(4).

Page 88: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Stavrakaki (2001): Hypotheses

The parser shows a locally preference (Frazier & Fodor, 1978; Gibson, 1998). Thus, long distance associations are expected to be more difficult than local ones.

When there is an option of a gap or a lexical NP, the parser will prefer a gap (Crain, 1999). Thus, if there is a NP instead of a gap, some processing difficulty should arise.

Sentence parsing strategies are sensitive to language specific properties. Theta-roles in Greek are indicated through case or agreement markers (morphological suffixes) and not through word order, as in English. Default associations between nominative case and agent theta-role, and accusative case and the patient theta-role may be made.

The more the preferences of the parser are violated the more difficult the processing of the structures will be.

Page 89: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

The Greek relative clauses

Relatives in modern Greek (MG) are introduced either with the relative pronoun "who" (o opios, I opia, to opio), or with the complementizer "that" (pu). In this research only pu relatives were tested.

The properties of relative clauses in MG: In object gap relatives with an object head and in object gap relatives

with a subject head the subject of the relative clause obligatorily occupies the postverbal position.

MG allows resumptive strategies, particularly the presence of a clitic, in relative clauses, which is coindexed with the head of the relative.

In this study, relatives with subject gap as well as with object gap

were presented. Seven types of relatives were tested, including relatives with resumptive clitics that marked for the same case as the head of the relative.

Page 90: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Methods

8 children with SLI (age: 5.4-9.4. mean age: 7.38), and two control groups of normally developed children: age matched and language matched

7 sentence types, 4 tokens of each sentence type (28 sentences), , all semantically reversible.

An acting out task

Page 91: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Correct performance

Sentence typeSLI groupLM controlsAM controls

O-O18/32(56.25%)47/64(73.43%)31/32(96.87%)

O-O- cl10/32(31.25%)36/64(56.25%)28/32(87.5%)

O-O-cl-case12/32(37.5%)51/64(79.68%)30/32(93.75%)

S-O7/32(21.87%)23/64(35.93%)22/32(68.75%)

S-S13/32(40.6%)47/64(73.43%)30/32(93.75%)

O-S16/32(50%)37/64(57.81%)28/32(87.5%)

O-O-preverb.subj13/32(40.62%)46/64(71.87%)31/32(96.87)

Totals 89/224(39.73%)287/448(64.06%)200/224(89.28%)

Page 92: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Theta-roles Reversal

SLI childrenLM controlsAM controls

Proportion 65/135(48.15%)27/161(16.77%)10/24(41.66%)

# of children8/8(100%)15/16(93.75%)5/8(62.65%)

# of sentence types7/7(100%)6/7(85.7%)5/7(71.42%)

Page 93: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Discussion The presence of the clitic reduces only SLI

children's performance. Case marking increases only performance of LM

controls. For the AM controls, neither a clitic effect nor a

case marking effect was found.

>>> SLI children's deficit cannot be interpreted in terms of a general delay in language development (Rice et al., 1995)

Page 94: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Table 3: The Grammatical and Processing Properties of the Test Sentences and the

Subject Group for which Each Sentence Type is Difficult Sentence typeGrammatical propertiesProcessing propertiesgroup

0-0 relativesPostverbal subject Marked case

Compatible with the parser's preferencesNone

O-O-clPostverbal subjectUnmarked caseResumptive clitic

Filled gap effectSLI children

O-O-cl-casePostverbal subjectMarked caseResumptive clitic

Filled gap effectSLI children

S-OPostverbal subjectMarked case

Long-distance association between the verb of the main clause and the subject NP; nominative case is associated with the patient theta role

All

S-SMarked caseLong-distance associations between the verb of the main clause and the subject NP

SLI children

O-SMarked caseCompatible with the parser's preferences None

O-O-preverb.subj

Marked casePreverbal subject

Filled gap effectSLI children

Page 95: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Novogrodsky & Friedmann (2006)

18 Hebrew-speaking children with SLI, aged 9;3 to 14;6 years (mean =12;6).

28 TD children divided into three subgroups: 8 (7 years old), 13 (9 years old), and 7 (10 years old).

13 participated in the preference task, and 16 participated in the picture description task (11 participated in both tasks).

Page 96: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Picture selection

Page 97: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Preference (Adif) - SR

Page 98: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Preference (Adif) - OR

Page 99: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Findings

Page 100: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement
Page 101: The syntactic abilities of children with SLI:  From Tense to Movement

Children with S-SLI have difficulties in the production of relative clauses, especially in object relatives that were mainly related to thematic role assignment.

Age is not a factor in the production of relative clauses in that age range. Their production was either identical or virtually the same with no significant difference in both tasks.

In the preference task the S-SLI children produced significantly fewer target object relatives than the control group (60% compared to 94%), and significantly fewer subject relatives (94%compared to 99%).

In the picture description task the S-SLI children produced significantly fewer target object relatives than the control group (46% compared to 94%), and significantly fewer subject relatives (83%compared to 98%). (See figure 3).

The children used a variety of structures in order to provide a task-appropriate response without using the impaired syntactic abilities. The non target responses in both tasks included thematic errors and reduction of thematic roles, avoidance of movement from object position, relative head doubling and production of simple sentences without a relative clause.

No complementizers were omitted.