15
RESEARCH ARTICLE The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism Diagnostic Instruments Karoline Alexandra Havdahl , Somer L. Bishop, Pa ˚l Sur en, Anne-Siri Øyen, Catherine Lord, Andrew Pickles, Stephen von Tetzchner, Synnve Schjølberg, Nina Gunnes, Mady Hornig, W. Ian Lipkin, Ezra Susser, Michaeline Bresnahan, Per Magnus, Nina Stenberg, Ted Reichborn-Kjennerud, and Camilla Stoltenberg The parental report-based Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and the clinician observation-based Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) have been validated primarily in U.S. clinics specialized in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in which most children are referred by their parents because of ASD concern. This study assessed diag- nostic agreement of the ADOS-2 and ADI-R toddler algorithms in a more broadly based sample of 679 toddlers (age 35–47 months) from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort. We also examined whether parental concern about ASD influenced instrument performance, comparing toddlers identified based on parental ASD concern (n 5 48) and parent-reported signs of developmental problems (screening) without a specific concern about ASD (n 5 400). The ADOS cutoffs showed consistently well-balanced sensitivity and specificity. The ADI-R cutoffs demonstrated good spe- cificity, but reduced sensitivity, missing 43% of toddlers whose parents were not specifically concerned about ASD. The ADI-R and ADOS dimensional scores agreed well with clinical diagnoses (area under the curve 0.85), contribut- ing additively to their prediction. On the ADI-R, different cutoffs were needed according to presence or absence of parental ASD concern, in order to achieve comparable balance of sensitivity and specificity. These results highlight the importance of taking parental concern about ASD into account when interpreting scores from parental report- based instruments such as the ADI-R. While the ADOS cutoffs performed consistently well, the additive contributions of ADI-R and ADOS scores to the prediction of ASD diagnosis underscore the value of combining instruments based on parent accounts and clinician observation in evaluation of ASD. Autism Res 2017, 0: 000–000. V C 2017 Interna- tional Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Keywords: Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; early diagnosis; screening Background Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is important given that interventions in young children are associated with considerable improvements in symptoms and functioning [Zwaigenbaum et al., 2015]. However, the time lag from first evaluation to ASD diagnosis can be long, often more than a year [Crane, Chester, Goddard, Henry, & Hill, 2016; Wiggins, Baio, & Rice, 2006; Zuckerman, Lindly, & Sinche, 2015]. Therefore, reliable and valid instruments are crucial to aid clinicians in making timely and appropriate diagno- ses of ASD in toddlers. Among the most widely used assessment instruments for ASD are the Autism Diag- nostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R) [Rutter, Le Couteur, & Lord, 2003] and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) [Lord et al., 2000]. The ADI-R is a semistructured caregiver interview, in which a trained interviewer asks questions to elicit detailed descriptions of the child’s social-communication and repetitive behaviors. The ADOS is a standardized, semistructured observational assessment of social communication and repetitive behaviors and interest, which is administered and scored by a trained examiner. The ADI-R and ADOS have demonstrated good agreement with clinical diag- nosis of ASD, especially when used in combination [see From the Nic Waals Institute, Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway (K.A.H., A.-S.Ø.); Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway (K.A.H., P.S., A.-S.Ø., S.S., N.G., P.M., T.R.-K., C.S.); MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK (K.A.H.); Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco (S.L.B.); Center for Autism and the Developing Brain, Weill Cornell Medical College, White Plains, New York, New York (C.L.); Department of Biostatistics, King’s College London, Lon- don, UK (A.P.); Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway (S.v.T.); Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, New York, New York (M.H., W.I.L., E.S., M.B.); Neuropsychiatric Unit, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway (N.S.); Institute of Clinical Medicine, Uni- versity of Oslo, Oslo, Norway (T.-R.K.); Department of Global Public Health and Primary Care, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (C.S.); New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, New York (M.H., E.S.) Received July 17, 2016; accepted for publication May 04, 2017 Address for correspondence and reprints: Alexandra Havdahl, Department of Children’s Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Post Box 4404 Nydalen, N-0403 Oslo, Norway. E-mail: [email protected] Published online 00 Month 2017 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/aur.1817 V C 2017 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. INSAR Autism Research 00: 00–00, 2017 1

The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

RESEARCH ARTICLE

The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of AutismDiagnostic Instruments

Karoline Alexandra Havdahl , Somer L. Bishop, Pal Sur�en, Anne-Siri Øyen, Catherine Lord,Andrew Pickles, Stephen von Tetzchner, Synnve Schjølberg, Nina Gunnes, Mady Hornig, W. Ian Lipkin,Ezra Susser, Michaeline Bresnahan, Per Magnus, Nina Stenberg, Ted Reichborn-Kjennerud, andCamilla Stoltenberg

The parental report-based Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and the clinician observation-based AutismDiagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) have been validated primarily in U.S. clinics specialized in autism spectrumdisorder (ASD), in which most children are referred by their parents because of ASD concern. This study assessed diag-nostic agreement of the ADOS-2 and ADI-R toddler algorithms in a more broadly based sample of 679 toddlers (age35–47 months) from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort. We also examined whether parental concern aboutASD influenced instrument performance, comparing toddlers identified based on parental ASD concern (n 5 48) andparent-reported signs of developmental problems (screening) without a specific concern about ASD (n 5 400). TheADOS cutoffs showed consistently well-balanced sensitivity and specificity. The ADI-R cutoffs demonstrated good spe-cificity, but reduced sensitivity, missing 43% of toddlers whose parents were not specifically concerned about ASD.The ADI-R and ADOS dimensional scores agreed well with clinical diagnoses (area under the curve�0.85), contribut-ing additively to their prediction. On the ADI-R, different cutoffs were needed according to presence or absence ofparental ASD concern, in order to achieve comparable balance of sensitivity and specificity. These results highlightthe importance of taking parental concern about ASD into account when interpreting scores from parental report-based instruments such as the ADI-R. While the ADOS cutoffs performed consistently well, the additive contributionsof ADI-R and ADOS scores to the prediction of ASD diagnosis underscore the value of combining instruments basedon parent accounts and clinician observation in evaluation of ASD. Autism Res 2017, 0: 000–000. VC 2017 Interna-tional Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Keywords: Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; early diagnosis; screening

Background

Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is

important given that interventions in young children

are associated with considerable improvements in

symptoms and functioning [Zwaigenbaum et al., 2015].

However, the time lag from first evaluation to ASD

diagnosis can be long, often more than a year [Crane,

Chester, Goddard, Henry, & Hill, 2016; Wiggins, Baio,

& Rice, 2006; Zuckerman, Lindly, & Sinche, 2015].

Therefore, reliable and valid instruments are crucial to

aid clinicians in making timely and appropriate diagno-

ses of ASD in toddlers. Among the most widely used

assessment instruments for ASD are the Autism Diag-

nostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R) [Rutter, Le Couteur, &

Lord, 2003] and the Autism Diagnostic Observation

Schedule (ADOS) [Lord et al., 2000]. The ADI-R is a

semistructured caregiver interview, in which a trained

interviewer asks questions to elicit detailed descriptions

of the child’s social-communication and repetitive

behaviors. The ADOS is a standardized, semistructured

observational assessment of social communication and

repetitive behaviors and interest, which is administered

and scored by a trained examiner. The ADI-R and ADOS

have demonstrated good agreement with clinical diag-

nosis of ASD, especially when used in combination [see

From the Nic Waals Institute, Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway (K.A.H., A.-S.Ø.); Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway

(K.A.H., P.S., A.-S.Ø., S.S., N.G., P.M., T.R.-K., C.S.); MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of

Bristol, Bristol, UK (K.A.H.); Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco (S.L.B.); Center for Autism and the

Developing Brain, Weill Cornell Medical College, White Plains, New York, New York (C.L.); Department of Biostatistics, King’s College London, Lon-

don, UK (A.P.); Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway (S.v.T.); Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, New

York, New York (M.H., W.I.L., E.S., M.B.); Neuropsychiatric Unit, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway (N.S.); Institute of Clinical Medicine, Uni-

versity of Oslo, Oslo, Norway (T.-R.K.); Department of Global Public Health and Primary Care, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (C.S.); New

York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, New York (M.H., E.S.)

Received July 17, 2016; accepted for publication May 04, 2017

Address for correspondence and reprints: Alexandra Havdahl, Department of Children’s Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Post Box

4404 Nydalen, N-0403 Oslo, Norway. E-mail: [email protected]

Published online 00 Month 2017 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com)

DOI: 10.1002/aur.1817VC 2017 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

INSAR Autism Research 00: 00–00, 2017 1

Page 2: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

systematic review: Charman & Gotham, 2013; Falkmer,

Anderson, Falkmer, & Horlin, 2013]. Due to findings of

reduced diagnostic validity in certain groups (e.g., tod-

dlers, individuals with very low IQ), the ADI-R and

ADOS algorithms have recently been revised to better

account for influences of age and language level on

ASD symptom ratings [Kim & Lord, 2012b; Lord et al.,

2012]. The new ADI-R Toddler and ADOS-2 algorithms

have shown improved diagnostic agreement compared

to the previous algorithms [Kim & Lord, 2012a].

The ADI-R and ADOS are heavily relied upon for diag-

nostic evaluations of ASD across a range of clinical and

research settings worldwide, yet only a few studies have

examined their validity outside of ASD specialty clinics

in the United States (U.S.). In a Swedish sample of tod-

dlers, Zander, Sturm, and B€olte [2015] found that the

ADOS performed similarly as in the U.S. validation

studies, whereas the ADI-R performed differently (i.e.,

generally lower scores, resulting in increased specificity

and reduced sensitivity). The ADI-R algorithms also

showed reduced diagnostic agreement in another study

of toddlers in Europe and Israel [de Bildt et al., 2015].

ADI-R sensitivity was especially low among toddlers

with ASD using phrase-speech; nearly half of these tod-

dlers scored in the little-to-no concern range. These find-

ings warrant further study given that both the ADI-R

and ADOS are widely used in Europe [e.g., in more

than 80% of ASD diagnostic evaluations of toddlers and

preschoolers in Norway; Larsen, 2015]. Multiple cultural

and contextual factors could contribute to the inconsis-

tent results between U.S. and non-U.S. studies. For

example, parent awareness of ASD symptoms and/or

inclination to report problematic behavior in their tod-

dlers might be generally lower in European countries

compared to in the United States [Zander et al., 2015].

Additionally, parental report could be influenced by

health system differences (e.g., whether an ASD diagno-

sis is required to be eligible for services).

A notable difference between the U.S. and European

studies was sampling from ASD specialized clinics, in

which most children are referred by their parents

because of concern about ASD, compared to from non-

specialized neuropsychiatric clinics or via screening. It

is possible that, among toddlers with ASD, those whose

parents are concerned about ASD may be more severely

impaired than those whose parents have nonspecific

concerns. However, this would be expected to also

result in differences in the performance of the ADOS

(not only the ADI-R); yet the ADOS demonstrated good

diagnostic validity in Swedish toddlers referred to a

nonspecialized neuropsychiatric clinic [Zander et al.,

2015] as well as in a U.S. sample of toddlers identified

based on community screening for signs of develop-

mental delay [Guthrie, Swineford, Nottke, & Wetherby,

2013]. It is also possible that parents who are concerned

about ASD may be more aware of and/or more inclined

to report autism-related behaviors than parents who do

not suspect ASD, thereby affecting the performance of

parent report-based instruments such as the ADI-R.

Information about the role of parental concern in

influencing the performance of the ADI-R and/or ADOS

is needed given widespread use of these instruments

outside of ASD clinics [Molloy, Murray, Akers, Mitchell,

& Manning-Courtney, 2011]. Current best practice

guidelines recommend routine screening for ASD in

general child psychiatry settings followed by diagnostic

assessment if signs of ASD are detected [Volkmar et al.,

2014]. Therefore, use of the ADI-R and ADOS with chil-

dren initially brought for assessment due to nonspecific

developmental and behavioral concerns is increasingly

common.

To date, no study has compared the psychometric

properties of ASD diagnostic instruments among chil-

dren identified for evaluation of ASD in different ways.

This study examined agreement between scores and

cutoffs from the ADI-R and ADOS and clinical diagno-

ses among toddlers recruited from a population-based

study that employed multiple methods for identifica-

tion. First, we aimed to examine diagnostic agreement

in this broadly based Norwegian sample and compare

these estimates with diagnostic agreement reported in

U.S. validation studies carried out in ASD clinics. Sec-

ond, we examined whether parental concern about ASD

influenced the performance of the instruments. This

was possible given that a subgroup of toddlers was

recruited because their parents were concerned about

ASD, whereas other toddlers were recruited because

their parents reported behavioral signs associated with

ASD (e.g., language delay) without a specific concern

about ASD. In particular, we were interested in whether

parental ASD concern influenced the discriminative

utility of the standardized cutoffs and/or the dimen-

sional scores from the ASD assessment instruments.

Methods

The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study

A sample from the Norwegian Mother and Child

Cohort Study (MoBa) received diagnostic evaluations

for ASD as part of a substudy, the Autism Birth Cohort

(ABC) [Stoltenberg et al., 2010; Sur�en et al., 2014].

MoBa is a prospective pregnancy cohort study estab-

lished by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health in

1999, with nationwide recruitment of mothers in asso-

ciation with routine ultrasound examinations (41%

consented) [see Magnus et al., 2006, 2016; Schreuder &

Alsaker, 2014]. The current data were derived from

quality-assured MoBa data files released in 2014 (v7).

The children (n 5 114,500) were born August 1999

2 Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments INSAR

Page 3: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

through July 2009. The participants are largely of Nor-

wegian or Scandinavian ethnicity (95%) [Myhre, Thore-

sen, Grogaard, & Dyb, 2012].

Multiple strategies were used to identify children

with possible ASD. One strategy was based on parental

concern about ASD, defined as the parent responding

yes to the question of whether the child has autistic

traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and

7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional referral

(parental agreement was a prerequisite) to the ABC

research clinic for suspected ASD. Another strategy,

which did not require parental concern about ASD, was

screening for behavioral signs associated with ASD in

the 3-year MoBa questionnaire (response rate: 58.6%).

The screening consisted of questions about language

and social development, as well as the Social Commu-

nication Questionnaire (SCQ) [Rutter, Bailey, & Lord,

2003] (see criteria in Appendix 1). The participation

rate for assessment based on the 3-year-questionnaire

was approximately 50%.

Identification routes also included registered ASD

diagnoses in the Norwegian Patient Registry, siblings of

referred/screen-positive children, and random selection

of controls (flow chart in Appendix 2). The clinical

assessments were undertaken in 2005–2012 at Lovisen-

berg Diaconal Hospital in Oslo, in collaboration with

the Norwegian Institute of Public Health and Columbia

University.

Participant Flow

Of the 114,500 children in MoBa, 1,033 children were

clinically assessed for ASD (not invited n 5 112,255;

declined assessment n 5 1,212). Since the present study

focused on the ADI-R and ADOS in toddlers (age <48

months), children assessed at older ages (n 5 201) or

who did not complete the ADI-R/ADOS (n 5 153) were

excluded (see flow chart in Appendix 2). Hence, the ini-

tial sample consisted of 679 toddlers (aged 35–47

months). Children with severe sensory (sight/hearing)

and/or motor impairments and/or nonverbal mental

age below 10 months (n 5 14) were excluded, as the

instruments have not been validated for children with

such impairments [Kim & Lord, 2012a; Lord et al.,

2012].

ASD was defined as clinical diagnoses of Autistic Disor-

der (n 5 41), Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Oth-

erwise Specified (n 5 24), and Asperger’s Disorder (n 5 1)

(Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,

Fourth Edition, Text Revision, DSM-IV-TR; American Psy-

chiatric Association, 2000). These three DSM-IV-TR sub-

categories of ASD have been found to be well accounted

for by a single ASD category [Frazier et al., 2012]. For

comparability with previous studies, Rett’s Disorder and

Childhood Disintegrative Disorder were not included in

the ASD case definition (excluded n 5 2). Non-ASD diag-

noses were assigned to 303 toddlers, primarily language

disorders (n 5 204), intellectual disability (n 5 38), and

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (n 5 20). The

remaining 294 children did not meet criteria for any

DSM-IV-TR diagnosis (as shown in the flow chart in

Appendix 2, the majority of these were recruited ran-

domly as controls). The sample characteristics are pre-

sented in Table I.

Measures

The ADI-R (Norwegian translation) was administered by

trained research assistants who had demonstrated

research reliability in using the instrument [Rutter, Le

Couteur, et al., 2003]. The original ADI-R algorithm

used in the assessments provides a classification for

Autistic Disorder. The ADI-R Toddler algorithms, con-

sisting of ADI-R items that have demonstrated high sen-

sitivity and specificity in toddlers of three separate

language levels (nonverbal, single words, phrase-speech)

[Kim & Lord, 2012b], were retroactively calculated.

Each algorithm provides a cutoff prioritizing sensitivity

(clinical cutoff), a cutoff prioritizing specificity (research

cutoff), as well as ranges of concern about ASD (the

clinical cutoff differentiates “mild-to-moderate” from

“little-to-no” concern). The Toddler algorithms were

used when analyzing dimensional ADI-R scores. When

small subgroup sample sizes necessitated collapsing

across the language levels, we used the 10 items appli-

cable to children of all language levels.

The ADOS (Norwegian translation) was administered

by licensed clinical psychologists who had demon-

strated research reliability in using the instrument [Lord

et al., 2000]. Revised algorithms from the ADOS-2 [Lord

et al., 2012] and calibrated severity scores [CSS, range

1–10; Gotham, Pickles, & Lord, 2009] were calculated

retrospectively. The CSS was used when analyzing

dimensional ADOS scores.

Age-equivalent scores derived from the Vineland Adap-

tive Behavior Scales were used to measure expressive

language ability [Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984].

Language level was defined by ADI-R item 30 (i.e., non-

verbal, single words, phrase-speech or better). IQ was

measured with standard scores from the Stanford–Binet

Intelligence Scales-5th Edition for most participants

(SB5; full version:n 5 401, abbreviated version:n 5 200)

[Roid, 2003]. Toddlers with lower developmental levels

than required for the SB5 received the Mullen Scales of

Early Learning (MSEL;n 5 56) [Mullen, 1995]. To avoid

floor effects on the MSEL, the ratio full scale IQ was

derived from age-equivalent scores (mental age/chrono-

logical age*100) [Bishop, Guthrie, Coffing, & Lord, 2011].

Behavior problems not specific to ASD (attention

problems/hyperactivity, aggressive behavior, and

INSAR Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments 3

Page 4: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

Table

I.To

tal

Sam

ple

Char

acte

rist

ics

by

Expre

ssiv

eLa

ngu

age

Leve

lan

dD

iagn

osi

s

Phra

se-s

pee

chSi

ngl

ew

ord

sNonve

rbal

ASD

dia

gnose

sOth

erdia

gnos

esNo

dia

gnose

sASD

dia

gnose

sOth

erdia

gnos

esASD

dia

gnos

esOth

erdia

gnose

s

NM

(SD)

NM

(SD)

NM

(SD)

NM

(SD)

NM

(SD)

NM

(SD)

NM

(SD)

Age

inm

onth

s35

41.2

(2.6

)231

41.5

(2.1

)294

42.1

(2.2

)20

41.1

(3.3

)58

41.0

(2.4

)11

41.4

(3.2

)14

41.8

(2.6

)

Mal

ese

x[%

]32

[91.

4]

167

[72.

3]

165

[56.1

]17

[85.0

]48

[82.8

]6

[54.5

]10

[71.4

]

IQ34

81.3

(20.

5)

230

92.5

(15.0

)294

103

.6(1

2.2)

18

64.0

(19.

9)

57

72.0

(15.

9)

11

37.7

(11.

2)

13

52.

7(1

9.9)

Langua

ge

age

33

29.5

(7.6

)222

33.2

(9.1

)283

43.2

(8.9

)20

16.9

(3.4

)58

18.8

(4.4

)11

7.5

(4.3

)14

11.9

(4.6

)

Beh

avio

rpro

ble

ms

(ite

mm

ean)

Par

ent-

report

ed35

0.6

(0.2

)223

0.6

(0.3

)286

0.5

(0.2

)20

0.8

(0.3

)56

0.6

(0.3

)11

0.6

(0.2

)13

0.5

(0.2

)

Clin

icia

n-o

bse

rved

35

0.5

(0.4

)231

0.2

(0.3

)294

0.2

(0.2

)20

0.4

(0.4

)58

0.3

(0.4

)11

0.5

(0.5

)14

0.2

(0.4

)

ADOS-

CSS

35

5.9

(2.0

)231

1.8

(1.6

)294

1.3

(1.0

)20

6.1

(2.4

)58

2.3

(1.7

)11

7.8

(1.5

)14

2.6

(2.3

)

ADI-

R(f

ull

dia

gnost

ical

gori

thm

)35

13.6

(7.0

)231

4.0

(4.1

)294

1.7

(2.7

)20

12.2

(5.3

)58

4.8

(4.8

)11

15.1

(7.0

)14

5.0

(4.3

)

ADI-

R(c

om

mon

item

s)35

7.1

(4.1

)231

1.9

(2.2

)294

1.0

(1.6

)20

8.1

(3.5

)58

3.3

(3.1

)11

11.6

(5.2

)14

4.4

(3.7

)

Iden

tifica

tion

route

,n

[%]

Par

enta

lco

nce

rnfo

rASD

13

[37%

]14

[6%

]2

[1%

]9

[45%

]4

[7%

]4

[36%

]2

[14%

]

Par

ent-

report

edASD

signs

wit

hout

conce

rnab

out

ASD

21

[60%

]187

[81%

]112

[38%

]11

[55%

]52

[90%

]5

[46%

]12

[86%

]

Contr

ols

/oth

er1

[3%

]30

[13%

]180

[61%

]0

2[3

%]

2[1

8%]

0

Mat

ernal

educa

tion

�12

year

s,n

[%]a

9[2

6%]

73

[33%

]79

[28%

]10

[50%

]23

[40%

]3

[38%

]5

[39%

]

Not

e.La

nguag

ele

vel

stra

taar

ebas

edon

ADI-

Rit

em30

(ove

rall

leve

lof

languag

e).

aM

issi

ng

on

mat

ernal

educa

tion

for

n5

54,

per

centa

ge

of

non

-mis

sing

isre

port

ed.

ASD

,au

tism

spec

trum

dis

order

;ADOS-

CSS,

stan

dar

diz

edco

mpar

ison

scor

esfr

om

the

auti

smdia

gnos

tic

obse

rvat

ion

sched

ule

;ADI-

RTo

ddle

r(c

om

mon

item

s),

sum

of

the

10

ADI-

Rdia

gnos

tic

item

s

com

mon

acro

ssla

ngua

ge

leve

ls(a

tten

tion

tovo

ice,

dir

ect

gaz

e,se

ekin

gto

shar

een

joym

ent,

range

of

faci

alex

pres

sions,

appro

pri

aten

ess

of

soci

alre

sponse

,in

tere

stin

childre

n,

resp

onse

to

appro

aches

of

childre

n,

han

dan

dfinger

man

ner

ism

s,oth

erco

mple

xm

anner

ism

s,unusu

alse

nso

ryin

tere

sts)

.

4 Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments INSAR

Page 5: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

anxiety; hereafter “behavior problems”) were measured

by parental report in the MoBa 3-year-questionnaire

[scale derived from the Child Behavior Checklist,

Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000] [see validity data in Biele,

Zeiner, & Aase, 2014; Zachrisson, Dearing, Lekhal, &

Toppelberg, 2013], and by clinician observation in the

ADOS section for “Other Abnormal Behaviors,” which

is not included in ASD algorithms [Havdahl, Hus Bal,

et al., 2016a].

Parental concern about ASD was defined as the

parent answering yes to the question of whether the

child has autistic traits in the MoBa 3-year-

questionnaire and/or by self-referral or professional

referral (parental agreement was a prerequisite) to the

ABC research clinic for suspected ASD (n 5 48). Most

toddlers in the ASD concern group also met the screen-

ing criteria based on parent-reported behavioral signs

(n 5 39 of 47, missing information for one child).

Behavioral signs without concern about ASD was opera-

tionalized as meeting the ABC Study screening criteria

for parent-reported behaviors associated with ASD while

answering no to the question of whether the child has

autistic traits (n 5 400). The screening criteria (see Appen-

dix 1) required parent concern about some aspect of the

child’s behavior or development (although not necessar-

ily about ASD).

Procedure

Informed consent was obtained from caregivers, using

forms approved by the Regional Committee for Medical

and Health Research Ethics in South-Eastern Norway

and the Columbia University Medical Center Institu-

tional Review Board. Participants underwent a compre-

hensive multidisciplinary clinical evaluation during the

course of one or two days. The assessment team did not

have access to information from MoBa questionnaires

or previous evaluations. Separate examiners adminis-

tered the ADOS and ADI-R, without knowledge of

results from the other instrument. Inter-rater reliability

on both instruments was continuously monitored.

There were multiple other sources of information. A

child psychiatrist or other physician administered a

parental interview of developmental history and cur-

rent concerns, a physical exam, and a standardized

observation of mother-child play interaction. Parents

and daycare staff completed questionnaires covering

signs of ASD and other psychiatric disorders, language

abilities, and executive functioning [see Sur�en et al.,

2014]. Psychiatric symptom assessment included the

Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment [PAPA; Egger

et al., 2006] or the Early Childhood Inventory-4th Edi-

tion [ECI-4; Gadow & Sprafkin, 2000]. Following the

assessment, the team met to review all available infor-

mation and discuss clinical impressions. In accordance

with the current gold standard for diagnosing ASD

[Falkmer et al., 2013], licensed and experienced clini-

cians used clinical judgment informed by the full multi-

disciplinary evaluation to assign a consensus best-

estimate diagnosis (DSM-IV-TR criteria).

Data Analysis

All analyses were performed in SPSS 22.0 (IBM Corp.,

USA) or STATA 13 (StataCorp LP, USA). Significance

level was set at 0.05. Group differences were examined

with two-samples t-tests and chi-square tests or Fisher’s

exact tests. Effect sizes are reported as Cohen’s d (d;

small:0.20–0.49, medium: 0.50–0.79, large: �0.80), or

Cramer’s V (V; small: 0.10–0.29, moderate: 0.30–0.49,

large: �0.50).

Agreement between instrument scores and clinical

diagnoses was estimated using area under the curve

(AUC) from receiver operating characteristic (ROC) anal-

yses, a well-established method for assessing the overall

discriminative performance of a dimensionally scored

instrument compared against a dichotomously defined

reference standard (e.g., clinical diagnosis) [Janes, Long-

ton, & Pepe, 2009]. The ROC curve is a plot of the true

positive rate (the proportion of children with ASD diag-

nosis correctly classified as ASD) against the false positive

rate (the proportion of children without ASD diagnosis

misclassified by the instrument as ASD), across the com-

plete range of possible cutoffs. The Stata procedure roc-

comp was used to compare AUCs. Logistic regression was

used to examine whether scores from the ADOS and ADI-

R contributed independently to prediction of ASD diag-

noses (odds ratios [ORs] are reported).

Agreement between ADI-R/ADOS classifications and

clinical diagnoses was examined by calculating sensitiv-

ity (the proportion of children with ASD diagnoses clas-

sified as ASD) and specificity (the proportion of

children without ASD diagnoses classified as non-ASD).

Likelihood ratios (LR) are also reported. LR1 is the ratio

of the probability of scoring above the cutoff in chil-

dren with ASD to the probability in children without

ASD (sensitivity/1-specificity), and estimates >1 indicate

increased probability of ASD (small:2–4, moderate:5–10,

large:>10). LR– is the ratio of the probability of scoring

below the cutoff in children with ASD to the probabil-

ity in children without ASD (1-sensitivity/specificity),

and estimates <1 indicate reduced probability of ASD

(small:0.5–0.3, moderate:0.2–0.1, large:<0.1). For com-

parability with the U.S. validation study by Kim and

Lord [2012a], sensitivity and specificity is reported for

the ADOS-2 ASD cutoff and the ADI-R Toddler clinical

cutoffs, and by language level as defined by ADI-R item

30 (i.e., no words, single words, phrase-speech or

higher).

INSAR Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments 5

Page 6: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

Prior to assessing the influence of parental ASD con-

cern on instrument performance, we examined how

parental ASD concern was associated with other rele-

vant child and parent characteristics (i.e., age, language

and cognitive abilities, behavior problems, ASD diagno-

sis, maternal education). To examine the influence on

ADI-R and ADOS performance, we employed ROC

regression methods [Janes & Pepe, 2008; Janes et al.,

2009] (Stata procedure rocreg with linear covariate

adjustment and 1,000 bootstrap resamples). This

approach allowed assessment of the influence of paren-

tal ASD concern on (1) threshold (cutoff) performance,

and (2) overall scale performance (the ROC curve). In

the first step, parental ASD concern was entered as a

predictor of ADI-R/ADOS scores among children with-

out ASD (linear regression coefficients are reported). As

measures of the effect size of the influence of parental

ASD concern on threshold performance, we report the

sensitivity and specificity of the cutoffs in toddlers

with parental ASD concern compared to those without.

In the second step, parental concern about ASD was

assessed as a predictor of the capacity of ADI-R/ADOS

dimensional scores to differentiate between toddlers

with and without ASD diagnosis. Probit regression coef-

ficients and AUC’s are reported. The ROC regression

analyses were also carried out with adjustment for age,

language abilities, nonverbal IQ and behavior

problems.

Results

Instrument Performance in the Total Sample

Discriminative performance of the cutoffs. Among

children without any clinical diagnosis (all of whom

were using phrase-speech), specificity of the ASD cutoff

was 96% for the ADOS and 99% for the ADI-R (98% and

99% when restricting to children recruited randomly as

controls). To facilitate comparison with previous studies

and because the ADI-R and ADOS are intended for differ-

entiation between ASD and other clinical disorders, diag-

nostic agreement is detailed below and in Table II for the

comparison of children with ASD versus non-ASD diag-

noses. In this subsample, the ADI-R (clinical) cutoff

showed good specificity, but sensitivity was modest.

Children meeting the ADI-R cutoff were 4 to 10 times

more likely to be diagnosed with ASD than with a non-

ASD disorder (see estimates of LR in Table II, which are

calculated from the ratio of sensitivity and specificity

estimates). Scoring below the ADI-R cutoff reduced the

probability of ASD diagnosis somewhat among children

with single words or less (LR–<0.3), but only modestly

among children with phrase-speech (LR– 5 0.5).

The ADOS (ASD cutoff) showed well-balanced sensi-

tivity and specificity (89% and 83%, respectively). Meet-

ing the ADOS cutoff increased the probability of ASD

diagnosis by a factor of 3–4 in children with single

words or less, and by 7 in children with phrase-speech.

Table II. Agreement of the Instrument Classifications With Clinical Diagnoses of ASD Versus Other Disorders

N ASD

N Other

disordersSensitivity

95% CI

Specificity

95% CI LR1 LR–TP FN TN FP

Total

ADOS 59 7 252 51 89%, 79–96 83%, 79–87 5 0.13

ADI-R 44 22 275 28 67%, 54–78 91%, 87–94 7 0.37

ADI-R & ADOS 39 27 294 9 59%, 46–71 97%, 94–99 20 0.42

ADI-R or ADOS 64 2 233 70 97%, 90–100 77%, 72–82 4 0.04

Phrase-speech (PS)

ADOS 31 4 200 31 89%, 73–97 87%, 82–91 7 0.13

ADI-R 20 15 216 15 57%, 39–74 94%, 90–96 9 0.46

ADI-R & ADOS 17 18 225 6 49%, 31–66 97%, 94–99 19 0.53

ADI-R or ADOS 34 1 191 40 97%, 85–100 83%, 77–87 6 0.03

Single words (SW)

ADOS 17 3 42 16 85%, 62–97 72%, 59–83 3 0.21

ADI-R 16 4 46 12 80%, 56–94 79%, 67–89 4 0.25

ADI-R & ADOS 14 6 55 3 70%, 46–88 95%, 86–99 14 0.32

ADI-R or ADOS 19 1 33 25 95%, 75–100 57%, 43–70 2 0.09

Nonverbal (NV)

ADOS 11 0 10 4 100%, 72–100 71%, 42–92 4 <0.01

ADI-R 8 3 13 1 73%, 39–94 93%, 66–100 10 0.29

ADI-R & ADOS 8 3 14 0 73%, 39–94 100%, 77–100 na 0.27

ADI-R or ADOS 11 0 9 5 100%, 72–100 64%, 35–87 3 <0.01

Note. ADI-R (clinical) cutoff: NV 5 11, SW 5 8, PS 5 13. ADOS-2 (ASD) cutoff: Module 1-no words 5 11, Module 1-some words 5 8, Module 2-

phrase-speech 5 7. Language level is defined by ADI-R item 30 (overall level of language).

ADI-R, autism diagnostic interview-revised; ADOS, autism diagnostic observation schedule; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; LR1, positive likeli-

hood ratio; LR–, negative likelihood ratio; TP, true positives; TN, true negatives; FP, false positives; FN, false negatives; CI, confidence interval.

6 Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments INSAR

Page 7: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

Conversely, not meeting the ADOS cutoff was associ-

ated with greatly reduced probability of ASD diagnosis

across language levels (LR–�0.2).

Requiring toddlers to meet both the ADOS (ASD) and

ADI-R (clinical) cutoffs resulted in excellent specificity

(95–100%), and greatly increased the probability of ASD

diagnosis across language levels (LR1�14). For children

with single words or less, not meeting the combined cri-

terion was associated with moderately reduced probabil-

ity of ASD diagnosis (LR–�0.3). Given that sensitivity

was low among children with phrase-speech (49%), not

meeting the combined criterion was associated with lit-

tle reduction in probability of ASD diagnosis in this

subgroup (LR– 5 0.5). The least restrictive criterion,

requiring children to meet cutoff on either ADOS or

ADI-R, resulted in very high sensitivities (95–100%).

Not meeting this criterion was associated with greatly

reduced probability of ASD diagnosis across language

levels (LR–<0.1). Among children with single words or

less, the tradeoff was low specificity (57–64%), whereas

among children with phrase-speech, specificity was

good (83%).

Characteristics of the misclassified children are shown

in Appendix 4. Among children ultimately diagnosed

with ASD, those missed by the ADI-R (clinical) cutoff

(false negatives) had higher intellectual (d 5 0.63,

P 5 0.02) and language abilities (d 5 0.76, P<0.01).

Among children diagnosed with disorders other than

ASD, those meeting the ADI-R cutoff (false positives)

had lower intellectual (d 5 0.75, P<0.001) and language

abilities (d 5 0.68, P<0.01) and more parent-reported

behavior problems (d 5 0.77, P < 0.001). Although no

statistically significant differences were found between

children with ASD who received ADOS classifications of

non-ASD compared to ASD, the subgroup of false nega-

tives was small (n 5 7). Among children with diagnoses

other than ASD, those meeting the ADOS cutoff (false

positives) had lower intellectual (d 5 0.62, P<0.001)

and language abilities (d 5 0.37, P 5 0.02) and more

clinician-observed behavior problems during the ADOS

administration (d 5 0.79, P<0.001).

Discriminative performance of the dimensional

instrument scores. The Pearson’s correlation

between scores on the ADI-R and ADOS was 0.63 (using

the full ADI-R Toddler algorithm: phrase-speech r 5 0.56,

single words r 5 0.52, nonverbal r 5 0.77). Dimensional

scores from both instruments differentiated well between

children with and without ASD. AUC was 0.93 for the

ADI-R (95% confidence interval [CI] 5 0.91–0.96) (ASD

versus other disorder: AUC 5 0.90) and 0.95 for the

ADOS (95% CI 5 0.92–0.97) (ASD versus other disorder:

AUC 5 0.92). There was no statistically significant differ-

ence between ADOS and ADI-R AUC scores (v2 5 0.47,

P 5 0.49). Scores from the ADI-R and ADOS contributed

independently to the prediction of ASD diagnoses in

logistic regression (ADOS: OR 5 1.96, P<0.001, ADI-R:

OR 5 1.42, P<0.001, v2(2, N 5 663) 5 254.44, P<0.001),

with independent contributions from the social commu-

nication (P<0.001) and repetitive behavior domains

(P<0.04) of both instruments.

The Role of Parental ASD Concern

As described in the methods, the study groups for these

analyses were: (1) parental ASD concern (referral to the

ABC research clinic for suspected ASD and/or parental

endorsement of autistic traits during screening; n 5 48),

and (2) parent-reported behavioral signs of ASD without

a specific concern about ASD (n 5 400). Parental ASD

concern was associated with diagnosis of ASD (54% ver-

sus 9% of toddlers were diagnosed with ASD in the

group with and without parental ASD concern, respec-

tively). Within diagnostic group (ASD and non-ASD),

toddlers with and without parental ASD concern were

largely comparable with regard to age, IQ, and language

abilities (see Table III). Among toddlers ultimately diag-

nosed with ASD, those whose parents had concern

about ASD had more parent-reported behavior problems

(d 5 0.56).

Influence on Cutoff Performance

The first step of the ROC regression showed that among

toddlers without ASD, parental ASD concern was signifi-

cantly associated with higher ADI-R scores (B 5 2.82

[95% CI 5 1.79–3.85], P<0.001), but not with ADOS

scores (B 5 0.25 [95% CI 5 20.45–0.94], P 5 0.49). The

association with elevated ADI-R scores remained

(B 5 2.37, P<0.001) after adjusting for IQ, language

abilities, and parent-reported behavior problems (all

P-values<0.01).

The substantial effect size of the influence of parental

concern about ASD on ADI-R cutoff performance is

shown by the differences in sensitivity and specificity

of the ADI-R cutoffs (Fig. 1). Among toddlers with

parental ASD concern, the ADI-R clinical cutoff had

85% sensitivity (95% CI 5 65–96%) and 68% specificity

(95% CI 5 45–86%). These estimates are largely consis-

tent with those reported in the algorithm development

study [Kim & Lord, 2012b: sensitivity 80–94%, specific-

ity 70–81%]. In contrast, the ADI-R classifications

showed considerably lower sensitivity and higher spe-

cificity among toddlers without parental concern about

ASD (Fig. 1). A sizable proportion of the toddlers ulti-

mately diagnosed with ASD scored below the ADI-R

clinical cutoff (43%). Sensitivity and specificity of the

ADOS cutoffs were similar in toddlers with and without

parental concern about ASD (Fig. 1).

INSAR Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments 7

Page 8: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

Influence on the Discriminative Performance of theDimensional Instrument Scores

Parental ASD concern was associated not only with ASD

diagnosis, but also with increased ADI-R scores indepen-

dent of ASD diagnosis (i.e., among the toddlers without

ASD). Hence, the presence or absence of parental ASD

concern could affect estimates of the agreement

between ADI-R scores and ASD diagnosis (parental con-

cern about ASD may in itself drive a sizable portion of the

agreement between ADI-R scores and ASD diagnosis).

After adjusting for the influence of parental ASD concern,

agreement between ADI-R scores and clinical diagnoses

was still good, resulting in AUC 5 0.85 (95% CI 5 0.80–

0.91), and parental ASD concern had no statistically

significant effect on the ROC curve (i.e., the ability of

ADI-R scores to distinguish between toddlers with and

without ASD) (coefficient 5 20.28 [95% CI 5 21.14–

0.57], P 5 0.51). Furthermore, ADI-R scores contributed

independently to the prediction of ASD diagnoses also in

the subgroup of toddlers without parental concern about

ASD (ADI-R: OR 5 1.37, P<0.001, ADOS: OR 5 1.99,

P<0.001, v2(2, N 5 400) 5 130.08, P<0.001). Still, as

shown by plotting the balance between sensitivity and

specificity across all possible ADI-R cutoffs (Fig. 2), differ-

ent cutoffs were required for optimal balance in toddlers

Figure 1. Sensitivity and specificity of ADI-R (clinical) and ADOS (ASD) cutoffs in children with and without parental concernabout ASD. Note. ASD, autism spectrum disorder; ADI-R, Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (toddler algorithms); ADOS, AutismDiagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2 algorithms). V, Cramer�s V. ***P< 0.001; **P< 0.01; *P< 0.05.

Table III. Characteristics of Toddlers Ultimately Diagnosed With and Without ASD by Level of Concern About ASD

Diagnosed with ASD Not diagnosed with ASD

Parental concern about ASD Parental concern about ASD

Yes No Yes No

N M (SD) N M (SD) P N M (SD) N M (SD) P

Demographics

Age, months 26 41.5 (3.2) 37 41.2 (2.7) 0.64 22 41.4 (3.0) 363 41.6 (2.1) 0.71

Male sex, [%] 23 [88.5] 29 [78.4] 0.30 14 [63.6] 275 [75.8] 0.20

Maternal education �12yb 10 [41.7] 12 [33.3] 0.51 7 [41.2] 129 [36.3] 0.69

Developmental level

IQa 25 70.5 (24.7) 35 68.8 (25.6) 0.80 21 84.4 (17.1) 361 90.3 (18.7) 0.17

Language agea, months 25 21.6 (10.3) 36 22.3 (10.2) 0.82 19 27.9 (14.1) 351 31.6 (10.5) 0.15

Phrase-speech, [%] 13 [50.0] 21 [56.8] 0.60 16 [72.7] 299 [82.4] 0.26

Behavior problems

Parent-reporteda (range:0–2) 26 0.7 (0.3) 37 0.6 (0.2) 0.03 20 0.6 (0.4) 356 0.6 (0.3) 0.35

Clinician-observed (range:0–2) 26 0.4 (0.4) 37 0.5 (0.5) 0.32 22 0.2 (0.3) 363 0.2 (0.3) 0.61

ASD diagnostic instruments

ADOS CSS (range:1–10) 26 6.2 (2.3) 37 6.3 (2.0) 0.87 22 2.1 (1.7) 363 1.8 (1.6) 0.49

ADI-R (common items) (range:0–20) 26 9.9 (4.6) 37 6.9 (3.6) <0.01 22 4.8 (3.3) 363 2.0 (2.3) <0.01

a A few cases had missing information (see N).b Missing by subgroup from left to right: n 5 2, n 5 1, n 5 5, and n 5 8. Proportions are calculated based on non-missing n.

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; ADOS-CSS, standardized comparison scores from the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ADI-Diag10, sum of

the 10 items common across the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised Toddler algorithms [Kim & Lord, 2012b].

8 Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments INSAR

Page 9: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

with and without parental concern about ASD (shown in

blue and red, respectively).

Since ADOS scores were not significantly influenced

by parental ASD concern, there was no need to adjust

for this when estimating the ROC curve (AUC 5 0.93

[95% CI 5 0.90–0.96]). There was also no statistically

significant influence of parental ASD concern on the

ability of ADOS scores to distinguish between toddlers

with and without ASD (coefficient 5 0.18 [95%

CI 5 21.30–0.94], P 5 0.76.

Sensitivity Analyses

In contrast to most previous studies, a few of the chil-

dren who received clinical diagnoses had been recruited

based on random selection (n 5 1 ASD, n 5 29 other

non-ASD disorder). The results were very similar when

excluding randomly selected children (Appendix 3).

Given that the rate of participant exclusion was higher

in the group with parental ASD concern compared to

the group without, we also carried out the analyses

with no exclusions and found essentially unchanged

results.

Results were similar when using the unrevised instru-

ment algorithms. The ADI-R Autism criteria performed

similarly as the ADI-R Toddler Research cutoff, and the

previously proposed relaxed ADI-R ASD criteria [Risi

et al., 2006] performed similarly as the ADI-R Toddler

Clinical cutoff. Due to the small sample sizes in sub-

groups stratified by language level, diagnosis, and

parental ASD concern, the analyses of parental ASD

concern were performed with collapsed language levels.

The ADOS CSS already takes language level into

account, and for the ADI-R we used only the algorithm

items applicable to all children across language levels.

The results were similar when using a mean item score

from the full language-specific algorithms.

Discussion

ASD diagnostic instruments have been validated pri-

marily in children referred for suspected ASD to special-

ized clinics in the United States, resulting in

standardized thresholds that may not be appropriate for

children identified in other ways (e.g., through general

population screening). Accordingly, the first aim of this

study was to examine diagnostic agreement in a

broadly based Norwegian sample. Compared with diag-

nostic agreement reported in validation studies carried

out in ASD clinics in the United States, we found simi-

lar estimates for the ADOS cutoffs (85–100% sensitivity

and 71–87% specificity). The ADI-R cutoffs showed

reduced sensitivity (57–80%) and increased specificity

(79–94%). Toddlers with ASD diagnoses who were

missed by the ADOS/ADI-R cutoffs had relatively strong

intellectual and language abilities. The reverse was

found for toddlers with other diagnoses who were mis-

classified as false positives by the instruments. False

positives also tended to have more behavior problems

not specific to ASD, such as hyperactivity, irritability,

and anxiety. These associations appeared to be rela-

tively informant-specific, with parent-reported behavior

problems significantly associated with ADI-R misclassifi-

cations, and clinician-observed behavior problems sig-

nificantly associated with ADOS misclassifications. This

pattern of informant-specific associations between rat-

ings of ASD symptoms and behavior problems was also

found in a recent study of primarily school-aged chil-

dren from the United States [Havdahl, et al., 2016a].

Our second aim was to examine whether parental

ASD concern influences the performance of the ASD

diagnostic instruments. The low sensitivity of the ADI-

R cutoffs, especially in toddlers using phrase-speech, is

consistent with studies of toddlers recruited primarily

through screening and/or referral for general concerns

(rather than self-referred due to suspected ASD) [de

Bildt et al., 2015; Zander et al., 2015]. Comparing tod-

dlers identified by parental ASD concern versus parent-

reported ASD signs (screening) without concern about

ASD, the ADOS cutoffs had consistently high sensitivity

and specificity. In contrast, the ADI-R cutoffs showed

reduced sensitivity and increased specificity among tod-

dlers whose parents did not have a specific concern

about ASD. The influence of parental ASD concern on

ADI-R scores appeared to be independent of other fac-

tors that typically affect ADI-R scores, such as IQ and

language level, and of clinician-observed ASD features

(ADOS scores). These findings indicate that in addition

to child features of ASD, scores on the parental report-

based ADI-R are also affected by parents’ concern about

ASD per se. Even though trained interviewers rate the

ADI-R items after eliciting detailed parent descriptions

Figure 2. Balance of sensitivity and specificity across ADI-Rscores in children with and without parental concern aboutASD. Note. ASD, autism spectrum disorder; ADI-R scores, sum ofthe 10 items common across the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised Toddler algorithms.

INSAR Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments 9

Page 10: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

of the child’s actual behaviors, parents who are con-

cerned that their child has ASD are likely to be more

aware of behavioral features associated with ASD, give

more examples and/or provide clearer descriptions of

these features. Accordingly, cutoffs derived from spe-

cialized ASD clinic settings may miss many children

whose parents do not have a specific concern about

ASD.

Although the categorical ADI-R cutoffs performed

below expectations in parents who were not concerned

about ASD, the dimensional ADI-R scores were useful

for detecting toddlers with ASD even when parents

were not concerned about ASD (AUC�0.85). When tak-

ing parental concern about ASD into account (i.e., only

comparing children with parental ASD concern and

children without parental ASD concern) ADI-R scores

had comparable accuracy in detecting toddlers with

ASD across the two groups (see Fig. 2). This suggests

that parents who do not have a specific concern about

ASD are able to report as diagnostically useful informa-

tion about their child’s social communication and

repetitive behaviors as those who are concerned about

ASD, but the scores need to be interpreted in light of

the lack of ASD concern. Moreover, although the ADOS

cutoffs performed well alone, ADI-R scores contributed

independently to the prediction of ASD diagnosis over

and above the ADOS scores. Thus, parental report

remains an important source of information about past

and current ASD features beyond what can be observed

during clinic visits. The instruments’ additive contribu-

tions underscore the value of combining direct observa-

tion and parent accounts in diagnostic evaluations of

ASD [Kim & Lord, 2012a; Zander et al., 2015].

The findings illustrate that sensitivity and specificity

of instrument cutoffs can vary widely depending on the

characteristics of the sample. The ADI-R cutoffs, which

have been derived from samples of children seen in spe-

cialized ASD clinics, missed nearly half of toddlers with

ASD whose parents did not have a specific concern

about ASD. This demonstrates the importance of con-

sidering factors that may influence the performance of

instruments used in ASD assessment. While the present

study focused on the influence of parent concern about

ASD, which may affect the performance of parental

report based instruments in particular, direct observa-

tion based instruments may be influenced by other fac-

tors (e.g., child behavior problems in that context).

When cutoffs are relied upon without critical clinical

judgment, instrument misclassifications can lead to

false reassurance, loss of valuable time for appropriate

interventions, and/or inappropriate interventions [Hav-

dahl, von Tetzchner, Huerta, Lord, & Bishop, 2016b].

Interventions for ASD in young children are associated

with clinically significant improvements in symptoms,

skills and functioning [Zwaigenbaum et al., 2015]. Loss

of time for ASD interventions could potentially increase

the likelihood of more significant impairment and sup-

port needs later, as brain maturation and behavioral

patterns may become more fixed and less plastic at

older ages.

The results have significant implications for instru-

ment development and validation efforts. Studies of

samples in ASD specialty clinics have been valuable in

forming a primary base for the development, valida-

tion, and refinement of diagnostic instruments. While

this has positively influenced assessment practices in

such clinics, caution must be exercised when applying

identical practices to other settings. Parents who are

not specifically concerned about ASD cannot necessarily

be expected to respond to questions about their child’s

behavior in the same way as parents who have sought

an ASD evaluation for their child. Therefore, to achieve

an optimal balance of sensitivity and specificity, modi-

fied criteria may be required (e.g., alternative cutoffs

and/or item combinations). Due to the complexity and

ramifications of adjusting algorithms and cutoffs, such

changes are not warranted on the basis of a single sam-

ple. Replication studies are needed to determine

whether and how parental concern about ASD should

be taken into account at the level of instrument design.

In the meantime, it behooves clinicians and researchers

to consider parental concern about ASD when interpret-

ing scores based on parental report of ASD features. For

example, it would be useful to keep in mind that the

standard cutoffs on the ADI-R may be too high for tod-

dlers whose parents are not specifically concerned about

ASD, and that it is important to integrate parent

accounts with information from other sources (e.g.,

direct observation, daycare staff).

Strengths and Limitations

Strengths of the present study included the indepen-

dent administration of the ADOS and ADI-R by separate

clinicians blinded to information from the other instru-

ment, developmental history, and previous evaluations.

Nevertheless, as in the majority of studies of ADOS/

ADI-R validity, the assessment team was not blinded to

information from these instruments in determining

clinical diagnoses [Kim & Lord, 2012a; Zander et al.,

2015]. Currently, the most widely accepted gold stan-

dard for ASD diagnosis is expert clinical judgment

informed by all available information from a multidisci-

plinary evaluation that includes multiple sources of

information and standardized instruments [Falkmer

et al., 2013; Kim, Macari, Koller, & Chawarska, 2015].

Beyond the ADOS and ADI-R, the assessment comprised

several other sources of information about ASD symp-

toms, including observation of play interaction, parent

10 Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments INSAR

Page 11: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

interview, and questionnaires completed by parents

and daycare staff. Additionally, the assessment team

included a specialist clinician who had conducted par-

ent interview and direct child observation, but who was

not involved in the administration or scoring of the

ADOS or ADI-R. Finally, even though the availability of

information from these instruments could have contrib-

uted to overestimation of diagnostic agreement, this

would be expected both for children with and without

parental ASD concern.

DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria were used in this study

and while findings suggest that the vast majority of

children diagnosed under DSM-IV can be expected to

receive the same classification (ASD/non-ASD) under

DSM-5 [Huerta, Bishop, Duncan, Hus, & Lord, 2012] it

is possible that use of DSM-5 criteria could have

affected the results.

Whereas this was a relatively large sample of toddlers,

sample sizes were small when stratifying by diagnosis

and identification method. Therefore, the results should

be interpreted with caution and with consideration of

their confidence intervals. In addition, the study was

conducted in a single culture and generalization to

other countries cannot necessarily be assumed. Selec-

tion bias could also potentially limit the generalizability

of the results. MoBa is population-based, but compari-

son with the general Norwegian population indicates

underrepresentation of young, single, and less educated

mothers, as well as lower prevalence of certain expo-

sures (e.g., smoking in pregnancy) [Nilsen et al., 2013;

Statistics Norway, 2016]. Self-selection bias may also

have been associated with participation in the clinical

assessment. Nonetheless, the participation rate for invi-

tations based on the 3-year-questionnaire was relatively

high (parental ASD concern: 52.7%, parent-reported

behavioral signs but no concern about ASD: 50.4%).

Conclusion

In this broadly based sample of toddlers, the ADOS cut-

offs showed consistent and well-balanced sensitivity and

specificity. However, the ADI-R cutoffs demonstrated low

sensitivity, particularly in identifying toddlers with ASD

in the absence of specific parental concern about ASD.

Different ADI-R cutoffs were needed according to pres-

ence or absence of parental ASD concern in order to

achieve comparable balance of sensitivity and specificity.

When using these different cutoffs, ADI-R scores differ-

entiated toddlers with and without ASD equally well in

the presence and absence of parental ASD concern. These

results highlight the importance of taking parental con-

cern about ASD into account when interpreting scores

from parental report-based instruments such as the ADI-

R. Although the ADOS cutoffs performed consistently

well, the additive contributions of ADI-R and ADOS

scores to the prediction of ASD diagnosis underscore the

value of combining instruments based on parent

accounts and clinician observation in evaluation of ASD.

Future studies should examine the influence of parental

ASD concern on the ADI-R and other parental report-

based instruments in other samples, and the utility of

adjusting cutoffs and/or algorithms based on whether

parents are concerned specifically about ASD.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the contribution of

the families that participated in the study. We thank

Kari Kveim Lie, Britt Kveim Svendsen, Elin Tandberg

and the Autism Birth Cohort Study clinic and research

staff for their invaluable contributions to the data col-

lection. This work was supported by the South-Eastern

Norway Regional Health Authority (Grant number:

2012101) and National Institutes of Health/National

Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (Grant

number: U01 NS047537). Somer L. Bishop, Catherine

Lord, and Andrew Pickles receive royalties for instru-

ments they have co-authored (ADOS, ADI-R, SCQ). The

other authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix

Appendix 1: 36-Month Screening Criteria in theAutism Birth Cohort (ABC) Study

Criteria:

1. Parent reports that the child has autistic traits or has

been referred to a specialist for autistic traits

2. SCQ-33 score of �12

3. Repetitive behavior subdomain on the SCQ-33 5 9

(full score)

4. Parent reports that the child has been referred to a

specialist for language delay

5. Parent reports that the child shows very little interest

in playing with other children

6. Parent reports that others (well-baby nurse, teacher,

family member) have expressed concern about the

child�s development

36-month screening algorithm:

� Meeting criterion 1

� Meeting any of criteria 2–5 AND criterion 6

Note. SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire

(Rutter et al., 2003); SCQ-33, the 33 of the 40 items

which are applicable to both verbal and nonverbal chil-

dren. Criteria 2 and 3 were based on SCQ scores, while

the remaining criteria required the parent to tick off for

“yes” when asked about the particular concern.

INSAR Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments 11

Page 12: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

Appendix 2: Participant Flow

Note. Colored boxes mark stratification used in analyses of the influence of parental concern for ASD.

ADI-R, autism diagnostic interview-revised; ADOS, autism diagnostic observation schedule; NVMA, nonverbal mental age; Severesens/motor imp., severe sensory/motor impairment with profound intellectual disability and autistic traits; CDD, childhood disinte-grative disorder.; ASD, autism spectrum disorder diagnoses; dx, diagnoses.

12 Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments INSAR

Page 13: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

Appendix 3: Agreement of the Instrument Classifications with Clinical Diagnoses of ASD Versus OtherDisorders (Excluding Children Recruited Randomly as Controls)

N ASD

N Other

disordersSensitivity

95% CI

Specificity

95% CI LR1 LR–TP FN TN FP

Total

ADOS 59 6 226 48 91%, 81–97 83%, 78–87 5 0.11

ADI-research (res) 30 35 262 12 46%, 34–59 96%, 93–98 11 0.56

ADI-clinical (clin) 44 21 247 27 68%, 55–79 90%, 86–93 7 0.36

ADI-clin & ADOS 39 26 265 9 60%, 47–72 97%, 94–99 18 0.41

ADI-clin or ADOS 64 1 208 66 99%, 92–100 76%, 70–81 4 0.02

Phrase-speech (PS)

ADOS 31 3 176 28 91%, 76–98 86%, 81–91 7 0.10

ADI-res 11 23 198 6 32%, 17–51 97%, 94–99 11 0.70

ADI-clin 20 14 190 14 59%, 41–75 93%, 89–96 9 0.44

ADI-clin & ADOS 17 17 198 6 50%, 32–68 97%, 94–99 17 0.52

ADI-clin or ADOS 34 0 168 36 100%, 90–100 82%, 76–87 6 <0.01

Single words (SW)

ADOS 17 3 40 16 85%, 62–97 71%, 58–83 3 0.21

ADI-res 11 9 51 5 55%, 32–77 91%, 80–97 6 0.49

ADI-clin 16 4 44 12 80%, 56–94 79%, 66–88 4 0.25

ADI-clin & ADOS 14 6 53 3 70%, 46–88 95%, 85–99 13 0.32

ADI-clin or ADOS 19 1 31 25 95%, 75–100 55%, 42–69 2 0.09

Nonverbal (NV)

ADOS 11 0 10 4 100%, 72–100 71%, 42–92 4 <0.01

ADI-res 8 3 13 1 73%, 39–94 93%, 66–100 10 0.29

ADI-clin 8 3 13 1 73%, 39–94 93%, 66–100 10 0.29

ADI-clin & ADOS 8 3 14 0 73%, 39–94 100%, 77–100 na 0.27

ADI-clin or ADOS 11 0 9 5 100%, 72–100 64%, 35–87 3 <0.01

ADI, autism diagnostic interview-revised; ADOS, autism diagnostic observation schedule; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; LR1, positive likelihood

ratio; LR–, negative likelihood ratio. TP5true positives, TN5true negatives, FP, false positives; FN, false negatives; CI, confidence interval.

ADI-research cutoffs: NV 5 13, SW 5 13, PS 5 16. ADI-clinical cutoffs: NV 5 11, SW 5 8, PS 5 13. ADOS-2 ASD cutoff: Module 1-no words 5 11,

Module 1-some words58, Module 2-phrase-speech 5 7.

Age IQ Language age

Behavior problems not specific to ASD

Parent reported Clinician observed

N M (SD) N M (SD) N M (SD) N M (SD) N M (SD)

ADI-RASD diagnoses – True positive 44 41.1 (3.0) 43 64.0 (25.0) 42 19.2 (9.9) 44 0.7 (0.2) 44 0.5 (0.4)

ASD diagnoses – False negative 22 41.4 (2.6) 20 79.1 (21.5)* 22 26.7 (10.0)** 22 0.6 (0.2) 22 0.5 (0.5)

Other diagnoses – False positive 28 40.9 (2.4) 28 74.3 (19.0)*** 27 22.8 (10.3)** 27 0.8 (0.3)*** 28 0.4 (0.3)a

Other diagnoses – True negative 275 41.5 (2.1) 272 88.2 (18.3) 267 30.0 (10.6) 265 0.5 (0.3) 275 0.2 (0.3)

ADOSASD diagnoses – True positive 59 41.3 (2.9) 56 67.9 (25.3) 57 21.5 (10.7) 59 0.6 (0.2) 59 0.5 (0.4)

ASD diagnoses – False negative 7 40.5 (2.7) 7 75.6 (20.9) 7 24.1 (8.7) 7 0.6 (0.2) 7 0.3 (0.3)

Other diagnoses – False positive 51 41.3 (2.2) 49 77.5 (21.6)*** 49 26.0 (11.2)* 48 0.6 (0.2) 51 0.4 (0.4)***

Other diagnoses – True negative 252 41.5 (2.2) 251 88.8 (17.7) 245 30.0 (10.5) 244 0.6 (0.3) 252 0.2 (0.3)

a P 5 0.050

***P< 0.001;**P< 0.01; *P< 0.05.

ADI-R, autism diagnostic interview-revised; ADOS, autism diagnostic observation schedule; ASD, autism spectrum disorder. Cutoffs: ADI-R Clinical

[Kim & Lord, 2012], ADOS ASD [Lord et al., 2012].

Appendix 4: Characteristics of Misclassified Children

INSAR Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments 13

Page 14: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

References

Achenbach, T.M., & Rescorla, L.A. (2000). Manual for the

ASEBA preschool forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: Univer-

sity of Vermont.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statis-

tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Wash-

ington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Biele, G., Zeiner, P., & Aase, H. (2014). Convergent and dis-

criminant validity of psychiatric symptoms reported in The

Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study at age 3 years

with independent clinical assessment in the Longitudinal

ADHD Cohort Study. Norsk Epidemiologi 5 Norwegian

Journal of Epidemiology, 24, 169–176.

Bishop, S.L., Guthrie, W., Coffing, M., & Lord, C. (2011). Con-

vergent validity of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning and

the Differential Ability Scales in children with autism spec-

trum disorders. American Journal on Intellectual and Devel-

opmental Disabilities, 116, 331–343.

Charman, T., & Gotham, K. (2013). Measurement issues:

Screening and diagnostic instruments for autism spectrum

disorders - lessons from research and practice. Child and

Adolescent Mental Health, 18, 52–63.

Crane, L., Chester, J.W., Goddard, L., Henry, L.A., & Hill, E.

(2016). Experiences of autism diagnosis: A survey of over

1000 parents in the United Kingdom. Autism, 20, 153–162.

de Bildt, A., Sytema, S., Zander, E., Bolte, S., Sturm, H.,

Yirmiya, N., . . . Oosterling, I.J. (2015). Autism Diagnostic

Interview-Revised (ADI-R) algorithms for toddlers and

young preschoolers: Application in a non-US sample of

1,104 children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-

orders, 45, 2076–2091.

Egger, H.L., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., Potts, E., Walter, B.K., &

Angold, A. (2006). Test-retest reliability of the Preschool

Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA). Journal of the American

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 538–549.

Falkmer, T., Anderson, K., Falkmer, M., & Horlin, C. (2013).

Diagnostic procedures in autism spectrum disorders: A sys-

tematic literature review. European Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 22, 329–340.

Frazier, T.W., Youngstrom, E.A., Speer, L., Embacher, R., Law,

P., Constantino, J., . . . Eng, C. (2012). Validation of pro-

posed DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Journal

of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia-

try, 51, 28–40. e23.

Gadow, K., & Sprafkin, J. (2000). Early Childhood Inventory-4

screening manual. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus.

Gotham, K., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2009). Standardizing

ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum

disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

39, 693–705.

Guthrie, W., Swineford, L.B., Nottke, C., & Wetherby, A.M.

(2013). Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: Stabil-

ity and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presenta-

tion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 582–

590.

Havdahl, K.A., Hus Bal, V., Huerta, M., Pickles, A., Øyen, A.S.,

Stoltenberg, C., . . . Bishop, S.L. (2016a). Multidimensional

influences on autism symptom measures: Implications for

use in etiological research. Journal of the American

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55, 1054–

1063. e1053.

Havdahl, K.A., von Tetzchner, S., Huerta, M., Lord, C., &

Bishop, S.L. (2016b). Utility of the Child Behavior Checklist

as a screener for autism spectrum disorder. Autism

Research, 9, 33–42.

Huerta, M., Bishop, S.L., Duncan, A., Hus, V., & Lord, C.

(2012). Application of DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum

disorder to three samples of children with DSM-IV diagno-

ses of pervasive developmental disorders. The American

Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 1056–1064.

Janes, H., Longton, G., & Pepe, M. (2009). Accommodating

covariates in ROC analysis. The Stata Journal, 9, 17–39.

Janes, H., & Pepe, M.S. (2008). Adjusting for covariates in stud-

ies of diagnostic, screening, or prognostic markers: An old

concept in a new setting. American Journal of Epidemiol-

ogy, 168, 89–97.

Kim, S.H., & Lord, C. (2012a). Combining information from

multiple sources for the diagnosis of autism spectrum disor-

ders for toddlers and young preschoolers from 12 to 47

months of age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

53, 143–151.

Kim, S.H., & Lord, C. (2012b). New autism diagnostic

interview-revised algorithms for toddlers and young pre-

schoolers from 12 to 47 months of age. Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders, 42, 82–93.

Kim, S.H., Macari, S., Koller, J., & Chawarska, K. (2015). Exam-

ining the phenotypic heterogeneity of early autism spec-

trum disorder: Subtypes and short-term outcomes. Journal

of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 93–102.

Larsen, K. (2015). The early diagnosis of preschool children with

autism spectrum disorder in Norway: A study of diagnostic

age and its associated factors. Scandinavian Journal of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology, 3, 136–145.

Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E.H., Jr., Leventhal,

B.L., DiLavore, P.C., . . . Rutter, M. (2000). The Autism Diag-

nostic Observation Schedule-Generic: A standard measure

of social and communication deficits associated with the

spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 30, 205–223.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P.C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., &

Bishop, S.L. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Sched-

ule, second edition (ADOS-2) modules 1–4. Los Angeles,

CA: Western Psychological Services.

Magnus, P., Birke, C., Vejrup, K., Haugan, A., Alsaker, E.,

Daltveit, A.K., . . . Stoltenberg, C. (2016). Cohort profile

update: The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study

(MoBa). International Journal of Epidemiology, 45, 382–

388.

Magnus, P., Irgens, L.M., Haug, K., Nystad, W., Skjaerven, R.,

Stoltenberg, C., MoBa Study Group. (2006). Cohort profile:

The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa).

International Journal of Epidemiology, 35, 1146–1150.

Molloy, C.A., Murray, D.S., Akers, R., Mitchell, T., & Manning-

Courtney, P. (2011). Use of the Autism Diagnostic Observation

Schedule (ADOS) in a clinical setting. Autism, 15, 143–162.

Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Minneapolis,

MN: Pearson Assessments.

Myhre, M.C., Thoresen, S., Grogaard, J.B., & Dyb, G. (2012).

Familial factors and child characteristics as predictors of

14 Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments INSAR

Page 15: The Influence of Parental Concern on the Utility of Autism ......Jul 17, 2016  · traits/autism in the MoBa questionnaires (ages 3, 5, and 7 years) and/or by self-referral or professional

injuries in toddlers: A prospective cohort study. BMJ Open,

2, e000740.

Nilsen, R.M., Suren, P., Gunnes, N., Alsaker, E.R., Bresnahan, M.,

Hirtz, D., . . . Stoltenberg, C. (2013). Analysis of self-selection

bias in a population-based cohort study of autism spectrum

disorders. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 27, 553–563.

Risi, S., Lord, C., Gotham, K., Corsello, C., Chrysler, C.,

Szatmari, P., . . . Pickles, A. (2006). Combining information

from multiple sources in the diagnosis of autism spectrum

disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 1094–1103.

Roid, G.H. (2003). Stanford-Binet intelligence scales (5th ed.).

Itasca, IL: Riverside.

Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communica-

tion Questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological

Services.

Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism Diagnos-

tic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles, CA: Western

Psychological Services.

Schreuder, P., & Alsaker, E. (2014). The Norwegian Mother and

Child Cohort Study (MoBa)–MoBa recruitment and logistics.

Norsk Epidemiologi 5 Norwegian Journal of Epidemiology, 24,

23–27.

Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland Adaptive

Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.

Statistics Norway. (2016). Population’s level of education.

Retrieved 20th November 2016 from https://http://www.

ssb.no/statistikkbanken/selecttable/hovedtabellHjem.asp?

KortNavnWeb5utniv&CMSSubjectArea5utdanning&

PLanguage51&checked5true

Stoltenberg, C., Schjolberg, S., Bresnahan, M., Hornig, M.,

Hirtz, D., Dahl, C., ABC Study Group. (2010). The Autism

Birth Cohort: A paradigm for gene-environment-timing

research. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 676–680.

Sur�en, P., Schjølberg, S., Øyen, A.-S., Lie, K.K., Hornig, M.,

Bresnahan, M., . . . Stoltenberg, C. (2014). The Autism Birth

Cohort (ABC): A study of autism spectrum disorders in

MoBa. Norsk Epidemiologi 5 Norwegian Journal of Epidemi-

ology, 24, 39–50.

Volkmar, F., Siegel, M., Woodbury-Smith, M., King, B.,

McCracken, J., State, M., . . . American Academy of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry (AACAP) Committee on Quality

Issues (CQI). (2014). Practice parameter for the assessment

and treatment of children and adolescents with autism

spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 53, 237–257.

Wiggins, L.D., Baio, J., & Rice, C. (2006). Examination of the

time between first evaluation and first autism spectrum

diagnosis in a population-based sample. Journal of Develop-

mental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, S79–S87.

Zachrisson, H.D., Dearing, E., Lekhal, R., & Toppelberg, C.O.

(2013). Little evidence that time in child care causes exter-

nalizing problems during early childhood in Norway. Child

Development, 84, 1152–1170.

Zander, E., Sturm, H., & B€olte, S. (2015). The added value of

the combined use of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-

Revised and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule:

Diagnostic validity in a clinical Swedish sample of toddlers

and young preschoolers. Autism, 19, 187–199.

Zuckerman, K.E., Lindly, O.J., & Sinche, B.K. (2015). Parental

concerns, provider response, and timeliness of autism spec-

trum disorder diagnosis. The Journal of Pediatrics, 166,

1431–1439. e1431.

Zwaigenbaum, L., Bauman, M.L., Choueiri, R., Kasari, C.,

Carter, A., Granpeesheh, D., . . . Natowicz, M.R. (2015).

Early intervention for children with autism spectrum disor-

der under 3 years of age: Recommendations for practice

and research. Pediatrics, 136(Suppl 1), S60–S81.

INSAR Havdahl et al./Influence of parental concern on ASD instruments 15